Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/278144932

Coverage of the Stanford Prison Experiment in Introductory Social Psychology


Textbooks

Article  in  Teaching of Psychology · October 2014


DOI: 10.1177/0098628314549703

CITATIONS READS

13 1,192

2 authors:

Richard Griggs George I Whitehead


University of Florida Salisbury University
119 PUBLICATIONS   2,211 CITATIONS    42 PUBLICATIONS   267 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs on 20 July 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Teaching of Psychology
http://top.sagepub.com/

Coverage of the Stanford Prison Experiment in Introductory Social Psychology Textbooks


Richard A. Griggs and George I. Whitehead III
Teaching of Psychology 2014 41: 318
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314549703

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://top.sagepub.com/content/41/4/318

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for the Teaching of Psychology

Additional services and information for Teaching of Psychology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://top.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://top.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Oct 5, 2014

What is This?

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


Topical Article
Teaching of Psychology
2014, Vol. 41(4) 318-324
Coverage of the Stanford Prison ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Experiment in Introductory Social DOI: 10.1177/0098628314549703
top.sagepub.com
Psychology Textbooks

Richard A. Griggs1 and George I. Whitehead III2

Abstract
This study is concerned with the nature of the coverage in introductory social psychology textbooks of the Stanford prison
experiment (SPE), given the many criticisms, especially recently, of the SPE. These criticisms concern both the study’s metho-
dology and the situationist explanation of the outcome. Ten textbooks were analyzed for coverage of the SPE and its ensuing
criticisms. Coverage of the SPE was found to be slightly less than that in introductory psychology textbooks, and the majority of
the texts providing coverage either provided no or minimal coverage of the criticisms. Contributing factors to and possible
explanations for such coverage are discussed. One explanation is applied to textbook coverage of the Kitty Genovese story and a
critique exposing its inaccuracies.

Keywords
Stanford prison experiment, social psychology texts, textbook analysis

The Stanford prison experiment (SPE; Haney, Banks, & Zimbardo himself about his active leadership in the SPE
Zimbardo, 1973a, 1973b; Zimbardo, 2007) is one of the most (Zimbardo, 2007) disclosed that these participants were largely
famous experiments, and arguably the most famous experi- told how they were supposed to behave (Banyard, 2007; Gray,
ment, in the history of psychology (Gregoire, 2013). However, 2013; Haslam & Reicher, 2003). This is because in his role as
since the SPE was conducted in 1971, there have been numer- prison superintendent, Zimbardo (2007) gave the guards an
ous criticisms of both the study’s methodology and the orientation that seems to have provided clear guidance about
researchers’ situationist explanation of its outcome. Because how they should behave (p. 55). As Banyard (2007; see also
Griggs (2014) has recently provided a detailed account of these Haslam & Reicher, 2003) pointed out, in his orientation,
criticisms in this journal and this study is a direct continuation Zimbardo aligned himself with the guards by using the pronoun
of the Griggs study, we will only briefly highlight the evidence ‘‘we’’ and gave clear instructions for the hostile environment
for the most damning criticism of the SPE, the presence of that ‘‘we’’ are going to create for ‘‘them’’ (the prisoners). For
strong demand characteristics, and then provide a summary example, he said, ‘‘They will be able to do nothing and say
listing of the other major criticisms before describing the pur- nothing that we don’t permit’’ (p. 55). Thus, Zimbardo’s orien-
pose of this study. tation served to legitimize oppression in the SPE.
Not long after the SPE had been conducted, Banuazizi and In addition, when some of the guards became abusive
Movahedi (1975) provided data that indicated that the SPE was toward the prisoners, Zimbardo, via his silence, provided tacit
likely confounded by strong demand characteristics. Banuazizi approval of the guards’ behavior, thereby confirming to these
and Movahedi solicited student responses to a questionnaire guards that they were behaving as they should. He also
that included a brief description of the procedures followed instructed his prison warden (one of his research assistants)
in the SPE and some open-ended questions to determine the to chastise the guards who were not behaving like these abusive
respondents’ awareness of the experimental hypothesis and guards and to make them more assertive (see Zimbardo, 2007,
their expectancies regarding the outcomes of the experiment. p. 65, for more details). Furthermore, Carlo Prescott, an ex-con
The vast majority of the 150 students responding determined
the experiment’s hypothesis (80%) and predicted that the beha-
1
vior of the guards would be oppressive (80.9%). Thus, it seems University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
2
that most of the participants in the SPE would also have Salisbury University, Salisbury, MD, USA
guessed how they were supposed to behave in the experiment. Corresponding Author:
However, the participants, especially those who were assigned Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville, FL 32225, USA.
to be guards, did not have to guess. Subsequent revelations by Email: rgriggs@ufl.edu

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


Griggs and Whitehead 319

who was the SPE’s chief consultant on real prisons (Zimbardo, decision-making process, the behavior of the guards and pris-
2007), claimed that he provided Zimbardo and his colleagues oners was rather benign and very dissimilar from the dramatic
with information that enabled them to make the experiment behavioral outcomes observed in the SPE. In addition, in 2002,
more similar to real prison life (Prescott, 2005). In his 2005 let- social psychologists Alexander Haslam and Stephen Reicher,
ter in the Stanford Daily titled ‘‘The lie of the Stanford Prison in collaboration with the British Broadcasting Corporation
Experiment’’ (available at http://valtinsblog.blogspot.com.au/ (BBC), conducted a prison study (known as the BBC Prison
2012/03/lie-of-the-stanford-prison-experiment.html), Prescott Study) that followed the same basic paradigm as the SPE but
expressed great regret for his involvement in the SPE and dis- had a very different outcome. For our purposes, it is only
closed that it was he who came up with the abusive and humi- important to know that, unlike Zimbardo, Haslam and Reicher
liating behaviors displayed by the guards (e.g., having did not instruct their guards to subjugate the prisoners to their
prisoners use buckets in place of toilets). He also said that he will in the way that Zimbardo did and that the guards’ and pris-
thought that ‘‘Zimbardo began with a preformed blockbuster oners’ behavior diverged markedly from that in the SPE,
conclusion and designed an experiment to ‘prove’ that thereby bolstering the argument that demand characteristics
conclusion.’’ (and Zimbardo’s leadership) were responsible for the outcome
Gray (2013; see Addendum, February 14, 2014) also of the SPE. More information about the BBC Prison Study and
reported some remarks by John Mark, one of the guards in the its results can be found in Haslam and Reicher (2005, 2012)
SPE, that mesh well with Prescott’s admissions about the SPE. and Reicher and Haslam (2006) and at the study’s Web site,
Mark’s comments appeared in ‘‘The Menace Within,’’ in the www.bbcprisonstudy.org.
July/August 2011 issue of the Stanford Alumni magazine In addition to the presence of strong demand characteristics,
(available at http://alumni.stanford.edu/get/page/magazine/ the SPE has been criticized for the lack of generalizability and
article/?article_id¼40741). Mark said: ecological validity (e.g., Banuazizi & Movahedi, 1975;
Fromm, 1973), the possibility of participant selection bias
I didn’t think it was ever meant to go the full 2 weeks. I think (Carnahan & McFarland, 2007; McFarland & Carnahan,
Zimbardo wanted to create a dramatic crescendo, and then end 2009), for breaching research ethics (e.g., Savin, 1973), for pro-
it as quickly as possible. I felt that throughout the experiment, viding no satisfactory account of the individual differences
he knew what he wanted and then tried to shape the experi- observed (e.g., McGreal, 2013), and for being a pseudoscienti-
ment—by how it was constructed, and how it played out—to fit fic experiment that is more aptly described as Kafkaesque
the conclusion that he had already worked out. He wanted to be absurdist performance (Ribkoff, 2013).
able to say that college students, people from middle-class This extensive body of criticism creates doubt about the
backgrounds—people [sic] will turn on each other just because accuracy of the coverage of the SPE in psychology textbooks.
they’re given a role and given power. Based on my experience, According to Morawski (1992), textbooks are the key transmit-
and what I saw and what I felt, I think that was a real stretch. I ters of psychological knowledge both to potential new mem-
don’t think the actual events match up with the bold headline. I bers of the discipline and to those outside of the discipline
never did, and I haven’t changed my opinion. (giving psychology away), and therefore it is essential that text-
book information be accurate. Thus, it is important to the psy-
Given Mark’s comments, Prescott’s admissions, Zimbardo’s chological teaching community to identify inaccuracies in our
description of his guard orientation along with his tacit textbooks, so that they can be corrected and we as textbook
approval of the bad guards’ later abusive behavior, and the authors and teachers do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false
prison warden’s chastising talks with some of the nonabusive information about our discipline. Given that the SPE is one
guards, it certainly seems likely that the guards’ oppressive of the most famous studies in psychology with far-reaching
behavior was largely a function of demand characteristics— impact outside of psychology, it is especially important that
their doing what they believed they were supposed to do. It coverage of it in our textbooks be accurate. Hence, Griggs
is important to note that despite these strong demand character- (2014) analyzed the accuracy of the coverage of the SPE and
istics, not all of the guards (only about a third of them) became the criticisms of it in a sample of 13 current introductory psy-
sadistic ‘‘bad guards.’’ Some were tough but fair ‘‘by the book chology textbooks. He found that the coverage was inaccurate
guards,’’ and others were ‘‘good guards’’ (Zimbardo, 2007). because of the very limited coverage of the criticisms of the
This variance in guard behavior is important because it argues SPE. For example, 5 of the 11 texts that discussed the SPE did
against a strict situationist interpretation of the SPE results. not include any discussion of the criticisms of the SPE, and the
That Zimbardo and his research associates’ guidance may other 6 included only very minimal coverage of them. He pro-
have been critical to the SPE outcome is also supported by posed some possible explanations for this limited coverage,
some findings of a study of simulated prison environments con- principally: (a) text authors’ lack of awareness of the extent
ducted in Australia by Lovibond, Mithiran, and Adams (1979). of the criticisms and (b) space constraints in introductory psy-
For example, in both a more liberal prison condition in which chology textbooks.
security was maintained in a manner that allowed prisoners to Although these two explanations seem plausible for the
retain their self-respect and in a participatory condition in inadequate coverage in introductory psychology textbooks,
which prisoners were treated as individuals and included in the they would not be as applicable to introductory social

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


320 Teaching of Psychology 41(4)

psychology textbooks. Because introductory social psychol- Results


ogy texts are exclusively on social psychology and written
First, 3 (30%) of the 10 texts did not include any discussion of
by social psychologists, lack of awareness of the extent of the
the SPE. It should be noted that one of these texts included the
criticisms should not be as problematic, and space issues
citation of Haney and Zimbardo (2009) in a sentence in a para-
would not be as constraining as they are in introductory psy-
graph on the fundamental attribution error that claimed that this
chology texts in which social psychology is typically covered
error often resulted in a gross underestimation of the inherent
in just one chapter (Griggs & Jackson, 2013). Thus, in this
power of the situation. We decided that this one sentence, com-
study, we examined a sample of contemporary introductory
prising at best a tangential reference to the SPE, did not consti-
social psychology textbooks to check their coverage of the
tute coverage of the SPE. Coverage in the other seven
SPE and the criticisms of it. We hypothesized that on average,
textbooks varied from 1 to 6 paragraphs, with mean and median
these texts would provide lengthier and more balanced cover-
coverage of 3.0 and 2.0 paragraphs, respectively. Mean and
age of the SPE and the criticisms of it. If this hypothesis is
median coverage for the entire sample of 10 textbooks were
supported, then the extent and nature of the coverage will
2.1 and 1.5 paragraphs, respectively, with a range of 0–6.
be of interest; if it is not supported, the reasons for the lack
To put these coverage statistics into perspective, we will
of more adequate coverage than in introductory psychology
compare them to the comparable statistics observed by Griggs
textbooks will be of interest.
(2014) for a set of 13 contemporary introductory psychology
textbooks. Griggs found that 2 (15%) of the 13 texts did not
Method include any discussion of the SPE and that SPE coverage in the
other 11 textbooks varied from 1 to 7 paragraphs, with mean
We used the most recent editions of 10 introductory social psy-
and median coverage of 3.6 and 4.0 paragraphs, respectively.
chology textbooks as the text sample. We include complete ref-
Mean and median coverage for the entire sample of 13 text-
erence information for all of these texts in the References
books were 3.1 and 3.0 paragraphs, respectively, with a range
section, and each reference is denoted by an asterisk. These
of 0 to 7 paragraphs. Thus, contrary to our hypothesis, the typ-
10 texts comprise almost the population of introductory social
ical SPE coverage in introductory social psychology textbooks
psychology textbooks if briefer versions of two of these texts
was slightly less (but not significantly so, p ¼ .15, t-test) than
and texts authored by sociologists are excluded.1
that observed in introductory psychology textbooks.
The methodology was similar to that used in Griggs (2014).
In the seven introductory social psychology textbooks that
To determine whether coverage of the SPE was included in a
included SPE coverage, this coverage appeared in six different
text, the Name Index was checked for Zimbardo and the Subject
content chapters—twice in an opening chapter on the nature of
Index was checked for Stanford prison experiment or any possi-
social psychology and once each in chapters on research meth-
ble variants, such as prison study. If coverage was identified, the
ods, group processes, social influence, behavior and attitudes,
location of the coverage was recorded and the extent of the cov-
and the law, respectively. The coverage in the two opening
erage was measured in terms of the number of paragraphs
chapters mainly involved using the SPE as an example of social
devoted to it. The extent of coverage of SPE criticisms was also
psychology research explaining behavior, especially the power
measured by the number of paragraphs devoted to them. This
of the situation, and the coverage in the research methods chap-
measure of length (number of paragraphs) was used primarily
ter centered on ethical issues in social psychology research.
because the actual amount of text on a page varies greatly among
The coverage in the other four chapters revolved around social
texts. This variance is caused by the number of columns of text,
roles in two texts and conformity and real prison experience in
how extensive the art program is, the font size employed, and so
the other two. The text that included SPE coverage in the law
on. In addition, this length measure allowed us to compare our
chapter (under the subtopic of real prison experience) also
coverage findings to those of Griggs (2014) for introductory psy-
included very brief SPE discussions in a section on ethics in
chology textbooks. The number of photographs of the SPE and
research in a chapter on doing social psychology research and
their content was also recorded for each text, and whether or not
a section on roles in a chapter on group processes.
the SPE was related to the Iraqi prisoner abuse at Abu Ghraib
Three of the seven texts providing SPE coverage did not
was noted.
include any coverage of the SPE criticisms. Of the four that did,
As in Griggs (2014), the nature of the coverage of the SPE
two provided rather minimal coverage in one paragraph. One
was determined by the chapter topic and subsection heading of
discussed only the ethical issues associated with the SPE and
the location in the text. If coverage was spread across multiple
the other discussed the individual differences present in situa-
chapters, chapter topics and subsection heading information
tions such as the SPE and very briefly, alluded to the possibility
were noted for all of the locations. The completeness of cover-
of participant self-selection in the SPE. The other two texts pro-
age was determined by identifying what other SPE-related
vided more nuanced, lengthier coverage of the SPE criticisms.
studies or critiques were cited and how much space was
One text focused mainly on the BBC Prison Study and the
devoted to coverage of the critiques. The cited references were
implications of its findings with respect to the SPE results,
recorded for the purpose of determining which SPE references
leading to a discussion of social norms and how they arise. The
were cited most often and whether references for SPE critiques
other text very briefly alluded to two SPE criticisms, then
and the BBC Prison Study were included.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


Griggs and Whitehead 321

discussed the BBC Prison Study and its findings, and ended texts in the present study demonstrated that such coverage was
with a discussion of the possible participant self-selection crit- clearly doable. Lack of knowledge of the criticism also does
icism proposed by Carnahan and McFarland (2007). Neither of not seem very likely, given the sheer amount of criticism that
these two texts explicitly mentioned the strong demand charac- has been published and the prominence of the journals in which
teristics present in the SPE, but one did cite the Banuazizi and it has appeared. Nevertheless, it may be the case that the some
Movahedi (1975) reference as a sample methodological criti- text authors are not aware of the extent of the criticism, and if
cism. In sum, only two of the seven texts including coverage they were, they would likely revise their coverage of the SPE or
of the SPE approached providing the type of coverage pre- possibly even omit it. If this is the case and the text authors are
dicted by our hypothesis. not aware of the entirety of the criticism of the SPE, then this
The mean and median numbers of SPE citations per text article should help to rectify that situation.
were 2.3 and 2.0, respectively, with a range of 1–5. The specific Another possibility for the lack of coverage of the SPE criti-
SPE references cited varied considerably across the seven texts. cisms stems from the pressure on text authors to be up to date
There were seven different references cited and a total of 16 and include coverage of important new studies and topics but
citations provided for the SPE. Two of these seven references keep the text length about the same (Blumenthal, 1990–
were cited only once, two twice, and two 3 times. One (Haney 1991). In order to accommodate these conflicting demands,
et al., 1973b) was cited 4 times. This citation variance is likely expansions of coverage for all of the existing topics required
at least partially due to the shotgun publication approach used by new developments for those topics obviously cannot make
to initially report the SPE results in the early 1970s. With it into the new edition. Sometimes older classic studies are even
respect to citations to SPE criticisms, Banuazizi and Movahedi deleted from texts (Griggs & Jackson, 2007). But, given the
(1975), Carnahan and McFarland (2007), McFarland and Car- notoriety of the SPE, it would seem too important to delete
nahan (2009), Savin (1973), and Turner (2006) were all cited or not to update its coverage. However, 3 of the 10 textbooks
once. Citations for the BBC Prison Study included three (twice examined did not even include coverage of the SPE. This lack
in one text) to Reicher and Haslam (2006) and one each to of any coverage could be due to the age of the SPE, but it could
Haslam and Reicher (2007) and Haslam and Reicher (2012). also be due to the authors’ decision not to include coverage
Providing evidence of the SPE’s compelling iconography, because the extensive criticism of the SPE has successfully
five of the seven texts providing SPE coverage included at least negated its findings (e.g., see Gray, 2013, who details why he
one photo from the SPE. The mean and median numbers of has never included SPE coverage in his introductory psychol-
photos were 1.1 and 1.0, respectively, with a range of 1–4. All ogy textbook).
of the photos were of the guards or prisoners or both. In addi- Another factor that may have partially contributed to the
tion, six of the seven textbooks discussed the Iraqi prisoner brief coverage of the SPE and the lack of coverage of the SPE
abuse at Abu Ghraib and related it to the SPE. criticisms is the authors’ desire to link the SPE to the recent
Iraqi prisoner abuse at Abu Ghraib, thereby demonstrating the
relevance of social psychology research to real-world behavior.
Discussion Such authors would need to provide at least minimal coverage
Our hypothesis that introductory social psychology textbooks of the SPE in order to develop the link to Abu Ghraib in their
would provide lengthier, more balanced coverage of the SPE text. In support of this possibility, six of the seven texts that
and its criticisms than that found in introductory psychology provided coverage of the SPE linked the SPE results to prisoner
textbooks was not supported. The current findings indicated abuse at Abu Ghraib. Interestingly, the seventh text linked the
that introductory social psychology textbooks do not devote Abu Ghraib prisoner abuse to Milgram’s obedience experi-
much space to coverage of the SPE and even less to its criti- ments (Milgram, 1974) instead of the SPE.
cisms. Thirty percent did not even include coverage of the SPE, One other possible reason for the lack of sufficient coverage
and the remainder only devoted an average of three paragraphs of the SPE criticisms is what we term the ‘‘story line’’ explana-
to it, slightly less than the coverage in introductory psychology tion. Without inclusion of any criticism that would muddle the
textbooks. Coverage of the SPE criticisms was also rather story line, the SPE ‘‘story’’ can be told in two or three paragraphs
scarce in most of the textbooks. Of the seven texts that provided and provides a compelling tale of the power of social roles and
coverage of the SPE, three provided no coverage of the criti- the situation, likely leading students to grasp this singular point.
cisms, two provided rather minimal critical discussions, and As McConnell (1988) pointed out in his article on textbook writ-
two provided coverage that approached the predicted lengthier, ing, ‘‘If you want to capture the imaginations of young people,
more balanced coverage of the SPE and its criticisms. you have to tell them stories!’’ (p. 160). But what happens if the
First, we will consider the question of why, given the abun- story line is muddled by infusing the various SPE criticisms into
dance of SPE criticism available, would most introductory the story? Simplicity becomes complexity, likely resulting in
social psychology text authors provide such limited coverage student confusion and disengagement (Harris, 1979). This expla-
of it? Two possibilities are (a) lack of space for inclusion and nation is also consistent with the transportation theory of narra-
(b) lack of knowledge of the criticism. Lack of sufficient space tive impact on persuasion (Green & Brock, 2000, 2002).
for inclusion would not seem very likely, given that such cov- According to this theory, a story impacts persuasion via the
erage would only entail a few paragraphs of text. Two of the reader being transported into the world of the narrative (being

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


322 Teaching of Psychology 41(4)

engrossed in the story) and hence, cognitively and affectively Manning et al., and three of the five that cited it did so in a
impacted by the narrative. It certainly seems likely that the SPE rather perfunctory manner in one sentence, somewhat defusing
narrative sans criticisms would lead to such ‘‘transportation.’’ its importance (e.g., ‘‘Although some of the facts have been
This story line explanation could reasonably apply to the two disputed, . . . ’’). In one of these texts, essentially the same sen-
texts that positioned SPE coverage in the opening chapter on the tence citing Manning et al. appeared in two different chapters
nature of social psychology and another that positioned coverage (methodology and prosocial behavior). One of the two remain-
in its discussion of social roles. None of these texts included any ing texts provided a paragraph describing Manning et al.’s criti-
discussion of SPE criticism to complicate the SPE story line. The cisms but separated the Kitty Genovese story and the Manning
story line explanation can also be applied to one of the two texts et al. coverage with about five pages of text, thereby lessening
that provided rather minimal discussion of the SPE criticisms. its impact on the Genovese story line. Although three of these
This text positioned the SPE coverage in the research methods four texts did not provide much coverage of the corrections, it
chapter and only briefly mentioned the ethical issues created is important to note that they did provide qualification of the
by the experiment, thereby not muddling the SPE story line. The story. The fifth text did describe the corrections and integrated
obvious downside of such coverage is that students will be mis- them within its coverage of the Genovese story. In sum, cover-
led into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that stu- age of the Kitty Genovese story is consistent with the story line
dents seldom question the textbook stories that they are told explanation in that half of the texts did not even cite the Manning
(Burton, 2001). Thus, it becomes critical for social psychology et al. critique and some of those that did, did so in a somewhat
teachers who are using textbooks lacking sufficient coverage defusing manner. These findings for coverage of the Kitty Gen-
of the SPE criticisms to provide such coverage in their lectures. ovese story thus provide support, albeit indirect, for the story line
Coverage of these methodological criticisms will also serve to explanation as a contributing factor to the lack of satisfactory
reinforce students’ knowledge of research methods and their coverage of the SPE criticisms observed in this study.
importance in psychological research. Given the story line explanation’s plausibility as a contribut-
To pursue this explanation further, we will consider another ing factor to the lack of coverage of SPE criticisms and the
story developed by Gansberg (1964) and Rosenthal (1964) that Manning et al. (2007) study, the coverage of Milgram’s obedi-
is included in almost all, if not all, introductory social psychol- ence experiments (which provide another compelling story line
ogy textbooks—the story of the 38 witnesses who watched the for textbook authors) in future editions of introductory social
attack but remained inactive during the brutal murder of Kitty psychology textbooks should prove interesting because
Genovese. Providing coverage of both this story and the recent recently the methodology of these experiments has received
evidence-based criticisms of the accuracy of the story by substantial criticism (Gibson, 2013; Nicholson, 2011; Perry,
Manning, Levine, and Collins (2007) poses a problem for 2013). For our purposes here, we will only briefly describe
textbook authors akin to that of providing coverage of the some of Perry’s criticisms.
SPE story and its subsequent criticisms.2 Similar to the SPE After spending months locating and interviewing some of
story, the story of the 38 witnesses is compelling and can be told the participants in the Milgram experiments, relatives of the
in a few paragraphs. However, just as coverage of the SPE men who served as experimenter and learners in the experi-
criticisms would muck up the SPE story line, the Manning ments, and Milgram’s former students in addition to poring
et al. criticisms would do so for the Kitty Genovese story. For over the articles, notebooks, and recordings in the Milgram
example, there were likely only 3 to 6 witnesses, not 38; there archives, Perry (2013) discovered some serious methodological
were only two attacks, not three; there is clear evidence of inter- problems in the obedience experiments. It seems that Milgram
vention disrupting the first attack, even calls to the police; and deliberately misrepresented his postexperimental debriefing
given the location of the second and fatal attack, it was impossi- procedures in his published work (see also Nicholson, 2011).
ble for all but one of the known witnesses to observe it. The Kitty Perry found that most of the participants at the end of the
Genovese story is a good test of the story line explanation experiment were not completely debriefed and not told that the
because according to Manning et al., it is resistant to change shock machine was a fake, that the learner was an actor, and
because it has come to function as a kind of modern parable—the that the learner was not actually shocked. It turns out that it was
antonym of the parable of the Good Samaritan. Hence, to almost a year before these people received a complete explana-
examine further the plausibility of the story line explanation for tion. In addition, the man posing as the experimenter did not
the lack of coverage of SPE criticisms that we observed, we stick to the experimental protocol and only use the four speci-
analyzed the coverage of the Kitty Genovese story in our sample fied prods in the script and then stop the experiment after these
of 10 introductory social psychology textbooks. prods had been exhausted. At various times, he strayed from
All 10 textbooks provided coverage of the Kitty Genovese the script and didn’t stop at four prods but invented more coer-
story, on average in three paragraphs. This finding is consistent cive prods and continued on. For example, in one experimental
with Manning et al.’s (2007) finding that the typical Kitty condition, he insisted that a woman participant continue 26
Genovese story was present in all of the 10 introductory social times. In addition, the extent and nature of the experimenter’s
psychology texts (copyright dates ranging from 1995 to 2005) deviation from protocol varied across experimental conditions.
that they examined. In support of the story line explanation, Even the infamous ‘‘65% obedience rate’’ is brought into ques-
five (50%) of the texts examined in this study did not cite tion by the strong possibility of participant expectations. For

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


Griggs and Whitehead 323

example, Perry argues that only about half of the participants in Banuazizi, A., & Movahedi, S. (1975). Interpersonal dynamics in a
the experiments fully believed that the learner was being simulated prison: A methodological analysis. American Psycholo-
shocked, and about two thirds of these participants disobeyed gist, 30, 152–160.
the experimenter. Banyard, P. (2007). Tyranny and the tyrant: From Stanford to Abu
Given Perry’s criticisms (and those of Gibson, 2013, and Ghraib. [Review of the book The Lucifer effect: Understanding
Nicholson, 2011), it will be interesting to see how introductory how good people turn evil, by P. G. Zimbardo]. The Psychologist,
social psychology textbook authors present the Milgram obedi- 20, 494–495.
ence ‘‘story’’ in the next editions of their textbooks. Will these *Baron, R. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Byrne, D. (2012). Social psy-
criticisms be ignored as many text authors have ignored the chology (13th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
SPE criticisms and Manning et al.’s (2007) criticism of the *Baumeister, R. F., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Social psychology and
accuracy of the Kitty Genovese story? We would hope not, but human nature (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
the present results indicate that this is a strong possibility. Blumenthal, A. L. (1990-1991, Winter). The introductory psychol-
It is likely that all of the factors that we have discussed as ogy textbook. The International Journal of Social Education, 5,
well as others that we are not aware of played a role in the text- 11–28.
book authors’ decisions about the extent and nature of SPE Burton, G. (2011). The tenacity of historical misinformation: Titch-
coverage to be included in their texts. Making such decisions ener did not invent the Titchener illusion. History of Psychology,
is not an easy task so it is important to realize that authoring 4, 228–244.
a textbook is a very complex, challenging, and arduous task. Carnahan, T., & McFarland, S. (2007). Revisiting the Stanford prison
McConnell’s (1988) discussion of this task for introductory experiment: Could participant self-selection have led to the
psychology authors explicates the problems facing textbook cruelty? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 603–614.
authors. He pointed out that textbook authors have to satisfy *Franzoi, S. (2013). Social psychology (6th ed.). Redding, CA: BVT
five different audiences—students, instructors, peers and col- Publishing.
leagues, publishers, and one’s inner feelings and needs—that Fromm, E. (1973). The anatomy of human destructiveness. New York,
make different and sometimes conflicting demands on the text- NY: Fawcett Crest.
book author. As McConnell concluded: ‘‘Satisfying them all is Gansberg, M. (1964, March 27). 37 who saw murder didn’t call the
something of an impossibility’’ (p. 167). police. New York Times, p. 1.
Gibson, S. (2013). ‘‘The last possible resort’’: A forgotten prod and the
in situ standardization of Stanley Milgram’s voice-feedback condi-
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
tion. History of Psychology, 16, 177–194.
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
*Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., Chen, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2013). Social
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
psychology (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Norton.
Gray, P. (2013, October 18). Why Zimbardo’s prison experiment isn’t
Funding in my textbook [Web log comment]. Retrieved from http://www.
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn/201310/why-zimbardo-
and/or publication of this article. s-prison-experiment-isn-t-in-my-textbook
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the
Notes persuasiveness of pubic narratives. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79, 701–721.
1. Because we did not include briefer versions of regular introductory
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye:
social psychology textbooks, it is important to point out that
Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C.
Aronson (2012) is not a briefer version of Aronson, Wilson, and
Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social
Akert (2013).
and cognitive foundations (pp. 315–341). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
2. As Manning et al. (2007) carefully pointed out, there is a clear dis-
Erlbaum.
tinction between the Kitty Genovese story itself and the bystander
Gregoire, C. (2013, December 18). 10 psychological studies that will
intervention research tradition that emerged in response to it,
change what you think you know about yourself. The Huffington
resulting in our knowledge of the bystander effect (e.g., Latané
Post. Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2013/10/18/
& Darley, 1968, 1970). They also make it clear that challenges
20-psychological-studies-_n_4098779.html
to the accuracy of the story in no way invalidate the canon of
Griggs, R. A. (2014). Coverage of the Stanford prison experiment in
bystander research.
introductory psychology textbooks. Teaching of Psychology, 41,
195–203.
References Griggs, R. A., & Jackson, S. L. (2007). Classic studies as primary
An * indicates a textbook examined in the present study. source reading in introductory psychology. Teaching of Psychol-
*Aronson, E. (2012). The social animal (11th ed.). New York, NY: ogy, 34, 181–186.
Worth. Griggs, R. A., & Jackson, S. L. (2013). Introductory psychology text-
*Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (2013). Social psychol- books: An objective analysis update. Teaching of Psychology, 40,
ogy (8th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. 163–168.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014


324 Teaching of Psychology 41(4)

Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1973a). A study of prisoners and and Zimbardo. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35,
guards in a simulated prison. Naval Research Reviews, 9, 1–17. 815–818.
Washington, DC: Office of Naval Research. McGreal, S. (2013, September 10). Individual differences in the Stan-
Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1973b). Interpersonal ford Prison Experiment [Web log comment]. Retrieved from http://
dynamics in a simulated prison. International Journal of Criminol- www.psychologytoday.com/blog/unique-everybody-else/201309/
ogy and Penology, 1, 69–97. individual-differences-in-the-stanford-prison-experiment
Haney, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2009). Persistent dispositionalism in inter- Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view.
actionist clothing: Fundamental attribution error in explaining prison New York, NY: Harper & Row.
abuse. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 807–814. Morawski, J. G. (1992). There is more to our history of giving: The
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to Little Albert? American place of introductory psychology textbooks in American psychol-
Psychologist, 34, 151–160. ogy. American Psychologist, 47, 161–169.
Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2003). Beyond Stanford: Questioning a *Myers, D. G. (2012). Social psychology (11th ed.). New York, NY:
role-based explanation of tyranny. Dialogue, 18, 22–25. McGraw-Hill.
Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2005, October). The psychology of Nicholson, I. (2011). ‘‘Torture at Yale’’: Experimental subjects,
tyranny. Scientific American MIND, 6, 41–51. laboratory torment and the ‘‘rehabilitation’’ of Milgram’s ‘‘obedi-
Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2007). Beyond the banality of evil: ence to authority.’’ Theory & Psychology, 21, 737–761.
Three dynamics of an interactionist social psychology of tyranny. Perry, G. (2013). Behind the shock machine: The untold story of the
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 615–622. notorious Milgram psychology experiments. New York, NY: The
Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2012). Tyranny: Revisiting Zimbardo’s New Press.
Stanford prison experiment. In J. R. Smith & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), Prescott, C. (2005, April 28). The lie of the Stanford prison
Social psychology: Revisiting the classic studies (pp. 126–141). experiment. Stanford Daily, in News section. Posted at: http://
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. valtinsblog.blogspot.com.au/2012/03/lie-of-the-stanford-prison-
*Kassin, D. T., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2013). Social psychology experiment.html
(9th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2006). Rethinking the psychology of
*Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., & Cialdini, R. B. (2010). Social psy- tyranny: The BBC prison study. British Journal of Social Psycho-
chology: Goals in interaction (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. logy, 45, 1–40.
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander Ribkoff, F. (2013, February). Unheeded post-traumatic unpredictabil-
intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social ity: Philip G. Zimbardo’s Stanford Prison Experiment as absurdist
Psychology, 10, 215–221. performance. Liminalities: A Journal of Performance Studies, 9.
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why Retrieved from http://liminalities.net/9-1/unheeded.pdf
doesn’t he help? New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rosenthal, A. M. (1964). Thirty-eight witnesses: The Kitty Genovese
Lovibond, S. H., Mithiran, X., & Adams, W. G. (1979). The effects of case. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
three experimental prison environments on the behavior of non- *Sanderson, C. A. (2010). Social psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John
convict volunteer subjects. Australian Psychologist, 14, 273–287. Wiley.
Manning, R., Levine, M., & Collins, A. (2007). The Kitty Genovese Savin, H. B. (1973). Professors and psychological researchers:
murder and the social psychology of helping: The parable of the Conflicting values in conflicting roles. Cognition, 2, 147–149.
38 witnesses. American Psychologist, 62, 555–562. Turner, J. C. (2006). Tyranny, freedom and social structure: Escaping
McConnell, J. V. (1988). Confessions of a textbook writer. American our theoretical prisons. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25,
Psychologist, 33, 159–169. 41–46.
McFarland, S., & Carnahan, T. (2009). A situation’s first powers are Zimbardo, P. G. (2007). The Lucifer effect: Understanding how good
attracting volunteers and selecting participants: A reply to Haney people turn evil. New York, NY: Random House.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on October 7, 2014

View publication stats

You might also like