Emotional Design Perspective

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

This article was downloaded by: [University of Central Florida]

On: 22 November 2014, At: 11:04


Publisher: Taylor & Francis
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hihc20

A Multimethod Evaluation of Online Trust and Its


Interaction with Metacognitive Awareness: An
Emotional Design Perspective
a b
Supavich Pengnate & Pavlo Antonenko
a
Oklahoma State University , Stillwater , Oklahoma , USA
b
University of Florida , Gainesville , Florida , USA
Accepted author version posted online: 02 Nov 2012.Published online: 14 Jun 2013.

To cite this article: Supavich Pengnate & Pavlo Antonenko (2013) A Multimethod Evaluation of Online Trust and Its Interaction
with Metacognitive Awareness: An Emotional Design Perspective, International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 29:9,
582-593, DOI: 10.1080/10447318.2012.735185

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2012.735185

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Intl. Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 29: 582–593, 2013
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1044-7318 print / 1532-7590 online
DOI: 10.1080/10447318.2012.735185

A Multimethod Evaluation of Online Trust and Its Interaction


with Metacognitive Awareness: An Emotional Design Perspective
Supavich Pengnate1 and Pavlo Antonenko2
1
Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, Oklahoma, USA
2
University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA

Meek, 2004; Garbarino & Strahilevitz, 2004; Rodgers & Harris,


Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

Despite the rapidly increasing numbers of informational 2003), dispositions (Li, Sarathy, & Xu, 2011; McKnight &
websites and learners using the World Wide Web to research Chervany, 2001), and familiarity (Casaló, Flavián, & Guinalíu,
topics, empirical evidence on the relationships between website 2008; Gefen, 2000). One area that has received little attention
design elements and website trustworthiness is scarce. This study in the empirical literature is a systematic examination of how
used self-reported and behavioral screen-capture data to investi-
gate the impact of Don Norman’s (2003) emotional design levels
website users’ emotional, cognitive, and metacognitive pro-
and metacognitive awareness on website trustworthiness during an cesses influence the users’ evaluation and judgment of website
information search learning task. The results suggest that the inter- trustworthiness, which is the objective of this study.
action effects of website visual appeal (visceral level) and website Multiple human–computer interaction (HCI) frameworks
usability (behavioral level) can override the effects of the quality have been developed, tested, and employed in practice to
or relevance of the information (reflective level) on website evalua-
tion. In addition, in the context of limited time to find the answers,
explain how user interface design improves user experience.
these effects on the evaluation of website trustworthiness are not Most of these frameworks focus either on the affective aspects
moderated by users’ metacognitive awareness. of user experience or on the (meta)cognitive aspects. For exam-
ple, Jordan’s (2000) pleasure model focuses on the pleasurable
aspects of users’ experience with the system, and McCarthy
and Wright’s (2004) affective model describes how sense-
1. INTRODUCTION
making takes place in the development of experience via four
The World Wide Web has evolved and become an important
core types of processes: compositional, sensual, emotional, and
source of information for the global society. With the increasing
spatio-temporal.
use of the World Wide Web as an information source, the need
Don Norman’s (2003) emotional design model is another
to understand how online users develop information literacy and
framework that explains and predicts user experience from
evaluate the trustworthiness of websites has never been greater.
an affective perspective, describing how emotion and cog-
Multiple studies have provided suggestions on how to enhance
nition are processed by different functional areas of the
the trustworthiness and credibility of websites, pointing out that
human brain and should therefore both receive attention of
the major determinant of website trust is the overall design of
user interface designers. Norman’s emotional design frame-
the website (Cyr, 2008; Cyr, Head, & Larios, 2010; Fogg et al.,
work is comprehensive because encompasses visceral, behav-
2003; Jarvenpaa, Tractinsky, & Vitale, 2000; Schlosser, White,
ioral, and reflective aspects of users’ information processing
& Lloyd, 2006).
and overall experience. This emotional design framework fit
Although the influence of various website design features on
the goals of this study because it afforded categorization of
user behavior has been explored extensively in the past decade,
website components to match users’ emotional, cognitive, and
most studies tend to focus on the website design manipula-
metacognitive processing—reflecting the full spectrum of user
tions and do not take into account user characteristics such
characteristics that are crucial for determining website user’s
as differences in navigation preferences, cognitive style, or
experience.
metacognitive awareness. Most studies of website design that do
The primary objective of this study was to investigate the
investigate the moderating effects of user characteristics focus
impact of website design components at each of the three lev-
on cultural differences (Cyr, 2008; Cyr et al., 2010; Gage, 1999;
els of Norman’s (2003) emotional design framework on users’
Simon, 2000), gender (Cyr & Bonanni, 2005; Dittmer, Long, &
evaluation of website trustworthiness as part of an information
Address correspondence to Supavich Pengnate, 204 Hanner Hall, search task that is typical for any formal or informal learning
Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74078. E-mail: fone. context. Although Norman’s taxonomy of visceral, behavioral,
pengnate@okstate.edu and reflective emotional design levels has been widely adopted
582
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 583

in website design practice, the impact of each level on trust involves active, task-driven use of the website and cognitive
development has not been validated empirically. evaluation of its usability. Finally, the reflective level is at the
In addition, the second objective of this study was to examine top of Norman’s emotional design model. This level is associ-
the extent to which online users’ evaluation of website trust- ated with metacognitive, reflective processing of the website’s
worthiness depends on their metacognitive assessment of the usefulness and deals mostly with the evaluation of informa-
relevance of information presented on the website—in the con- tion presented in the website. Norman’s emotional design
text of limited time to complete an information search task and model is a useful framework for designing and evaluating
a range of websites to be evaluated. Metacognition refers to websites—particularly because it reflects the three recognized
knowledge about when and how individuals plan and monitor levels of human information processing—affective, cognitive,
their information processing and use such planning, monitor- and metacognitive.
ing, and reflection strategies for learning or for solving prob-
lems (Metcalfe & Shimamura, 1994). Metacognition has been
explored extensively in educational contexts (Ally, 2004; Artino 2.2. Initial Trust in the Online Context
& Jones Ii, 2012; Azevedo & Jacobson, 2008; Garrison, 2003; An important predictor of website use is the user’s per-
Garrison & Cleveland-Innes, 2005; Liu, Chen, Sun, Wible, ceived trust (Jarvenpaa et al., 2000). Information processing
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

& Kuo, 2010). As for user interface design, user characteris- at the visceral, behavioral, and reflective level contributes to
tics such as metacognitive awareness and metacognitive skills the development of online trust. McKnight Cumming, and
are considered as a major predictor of users’ evaluations and Chervany (1998) theorized that initial trust—that is, trust that
their actual web browsing behavior (Tractinsky & Lowengart, first-time website visitors have in a website without an estab-
2007). Thus, this study was concerned with exploring the medi- lished reputation—has three antecedents: individual disposi-
ating effects of users’ metacognition on their perception of tion to trust, institution-based trust, and cognitive-based trust.
trustworthiness of a number of websites with variable design Although individual disposition to trust is a personality char-
features. It is an initial attempt to determine the influence of acteristic that develops during childhood, institution-based trust
metacognition on website trust and provide new insights on how refers to the security one feels about a situation because guar-
user characteristics interact with website design features at the antees, safety nets, or other regulatory or legal structures are
visceral, behavioral, and reflective level, impacting the users’ constituted. Cognitive-based trust is developed based on rapid,
evaluation of websites. cognitive cues or first impressions. In accordance with this
conceptualization of initial trust, interface design of websites
2. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK AND HYPOTHESES should be fundamental to the development of cognitive-based
trust, impacting online users’ evaluation of website trustwor-
2.1. Emotional Design thiness. Several studies have suggested that website trust is
The HCI literature provides a broad range of models explain- an important antecedent of user responses and a critical fac-
ing how emotional responses impact users’ evaluation of inter- tor of success in online business impacting such variables as
active products (Cyr et al., 2010; Deng & Poole, 2010; Éthier, user satisfaction (Flavián et al., 2006; Kim, Ferrin, & Rao,
Hadaya, Talbot, & Cadieux, 2006; Flavián, Guinalíu, & Gurrea, 2009), intention to purchase (Jarvenpaa et al., 2000; Lindgaard,
2006; Li et al., 2011; van der Heijden, 2004). This study Dudek, Sen, Sumegi, & Noonan, 2011; Pennington, Wilcox, &
adopted Norman’s (2003) emotional design model, which has Grover, 2003; Wells, Valacich, & Hess, 2011), actual purchas-
been well received in the HCI domain and widely adopted by ing behavior (Lim, Sia, Lee, & Benbasat, 2006), and e-loyalty
website designers. This model proposes that human emotion (Cyr, 2008).
and behavior when interacting with an object result from three
different levels of information processing—the visceral level,
the behavioral level, and the reflective level. This framework is 2.3. Website Interface, Emotional Design, and Online
consistent with prior conceptual and empirical research in cog- Trust
nitive science (e.g., Sweller, van Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998), Conceptual and empirical research on online trust and
which typically distinguishes between three distinct levels of website design has been performed by scholars from sev-
information processing—affective (cf. visceral), cognitive (cf. eral disciplines, ranging from art to travel and tourism, with
behavioral), and metacognitive (cf. reflective). According to topics focused on website aesthetics and gender differences
Norman’s model, the first level of emotional design is visceral. (Moss, Gunn, & Heller, 2006), effects of culture on percep-
At this level, visual design features of a system (e.g., graphics, tions of website design (Clemmensen, 2011; Cyr, 2008), effects
cleanliness, perceived attractiveness) dominate human percep- of cognitive style (imagers vs. verbalizers) users preferences
tion and, in the website design context, this is when the user of usability and aesthetics (Lee & Koubek, 2011), or website
makes rapid (millisecond) judgments of the website based on design characteristics and hotels’ marketing practices (Baloglu
the initial emotional responses to the color scheme and over- & Pekcan, 2006). However, no empirical research exists differ-
all layout. The second level of processing is behavioral, which entiating the impacts of Norman’s (2003) emotional levels of
584 S. PENGNATE AND P. ANTONENKO

website design with the development of website users’ initial quality include accessibility, interpretability, and integrity
online trust. (Bovee, 2004), which are considered as control variables in this
With regard to the visceral level, the first level of emo- study. The quality of information presented on the website is
tional design, several studies have identified the important role an important predictor of website trustworthiness in such con-
of visual design features on website users’ emotional states, texts as online shopping (Nicolaou & McKnight, 2006), mobile
such as liking, enjoyment, and perception of the design as commerce (Siau & Shen, 2003), and supply chain management
visually pleasing (Éthier et al., 2006; Éthier, Hadaya, Talbot, (Li & Lin, 2006). As suggested by Norman (2003), the effect
& Cadieux, 2008). Schwarz’s (1986) framework of affect- of the reflective level (i.e., information quality) on the over-
as-information can be used to support the view that emo- all evaluation of artifacts overrides the effects of the visceral
tions have the ability to impact user psychological reactions. (visual appeal) and behavioral (usability) levels. This study was
According to this framework, the halo effect of emotional designed to conduct an empirical investigation of the validity of
response toward an object influences the evaluation of object this proposition. Specifically, we hypothesized the following:
characteristics (Deng & Poole, 2010; Dunn & Schweitzer, 2005;
Schwarz, 1986). In information systems context, Cyr and col- H1: The effects of relevant information (i.e., the reflective
leagues found that visual appeal evoked enjoyment, which in level) will override the visceral and behavioral effects on
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

turn increased website trust (Cyr, 2008). Van der Heijden’s participants’ website trust.
(2004) study revealed that perceived enjoyment influenced
online users’ intention to use websites. Porat and Tractinsky
(2008) suggested that visual appeal affected users’ attitudes 2.4. Metacognition and Online Trust
toward online stores. Tractinsky and Lowengart (2007) also Because Norman’s (2003) emotional design model empha-
found the influence of visual appeal on website trustworthiness sizes the role of the reflective level as the top level of this
in their study. Furthermore, based on the work of Hassanein framework, users’ metacognitive skills—that is, the ability to
and Head (Hassanein & Milena, 2007), Shin and Shin (2011) monitor and regulate, or reflect on one’s cognitive processing
provided empirical evidence that perceived social presence can (Flavell, 1979)—should be paramount to determining online
influence user perception of security, which is a key antecedent trust. In this study, we followed the definition of metacognition
of trust. The findings of this line of research suggest that proposed by Metcalfe and Shimamura (1994) as “knowing
website’s visceral design elements elicit emotional responses in about knowing”, which refers to knowledge about when and
online users, which affect the users’ evaluation of websites and how individuals can use particular strategies for learning or
online trust. for solving problems. Generally, metacognition contains two
The second level of emotional design—that is, the behav- distinct components, knowledge of cognition and regulation
ioral level—is reflected in website’s usability. Usability refers of cognition (Garner, 1987). Knowledge of cognition includes
to the features of website design that help users accomplish three different types of metacognitive awareness: declarative,
their website browsing goals (Sharp, Rogers, & Preece, 2007). procedural, and conditional knowledge (Schraw, 1998; Schraw
Several studies have suggested that website usability is a deter- & Moshman, 1995). Regulation of cognition refers to knowing
minant of the initial establishment of trust (e.g. Kim & Moon, when and why to use specific knowledge, which helps individ-
1998). Based on the technology acceptance model (Davis, uals control their learning or solving problems (Garner, 1990;
1989), Gefen, Karahanna, and Straub (2003) provided empir- Schraw, 1998).
ical evidence demonstrating that website usability influenced Metacognitive awareness has been mostly studied in online
both website trust and perceived usefulness in the online shop- learning context, and it is suggested that it is a critical fac-
ping context. Schlosser et al. (2006) found that website usability tor in determining success of online learners (Ally, 2004;
signaled the capability of website providers to provide prod- Garrison, 2003; Garrison & Cleveland-Innes, 2005; Salmon,
ucts and services, which resulted in trust and intention to 2002). However, it is also important to understand the roles of
use websites. Flavian et al. (2006) found that the level of metacognitive awareness on online users’ evaluation of website
website trust increased when users perceived that the website trust. Thus, we proposed that metacognitive awareness would
was usable. In addition, in diverse cultural groups (Canada, influence online users’ evaluation of website trust. Specifically,
Germany, and Japan), Cyr (2008) demonstrated that website we expected that online users with relatively high metacognitive
usability was a determinant of website trust and the level of awareness would rely on information quality (reflective level)
significance was determined by cultural effects. rather than visual appeal (visceral level) or usability (behav-
With respect to the reflective level, the third level of ioral level) in determining website trust. This is also consistent
emotional design, information quality has been described as with Norman’s (2003) model of emotional design, which pre-
an assessment of website providers’ trustworthiness (Kelton, dicts that the effects of the reflective level override the impacts
Fleischmann, & Wallace, 2008). In this study, information qual- of the visceral and behavioral level. In addition, we also
ity refers to users’ judgment of information relevance, which anticipated that the level of metacognitive awareness would
is one of the four criteria of information quality proposed influence online user performance in performing a learning task,
by Bovee (2004). The other three dimensions of information especially the time spent on searching for information.
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 585

Specifically, we hypothesized the following: website brands were removed to avoid branding effects and to
ensure that the websites would be unfamiliar to participants.
H2: Participants selecting mostly relevant concepts on the test Thumbnails of the eight experimental websites are shown in
of learning will have higher metacognitive awareness than Figure 1.
participants who use mostly irrelevant information in their Experimental websites were created by a website design
test responses. expert. The independent variables were manipulated in the
H3: Participants with higher metacognitive awareness will following way. Visual appeal (visceral level) manipulations
spend less time searching for answers to the learning task included the color scheme, page layout, and quality of graph-
than participants with low metacognitive awareness. ics. For example, less visual appeal websites contained low-
resolution images, inconsistent color scheme, and asymmetry
layout. Usability (behavioral level) was primarily manipulated
3. METHODOLOGY by changing the readability of the text. For example, less usable
3.1. Participants websites contained long paragraphs of text with distracting text
formatting including horizontal and vertical scroll bars. The last
A total of 151 undergraduate students from education and
factor, the relevance of the information (reflective level), was
business programs at a major midwestern university partici-
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

manipulated by providing concepts that were mostly relevant


pated in a laboratory experiment (n = 60 of business majors
to solving the learning task on half of the websites and mostly
and n = 91 of education majors). Almost all students from both
irrelevant concepts on the other half of the websites. All ver-
majors were junior and senior students. Most participants from
sions of the text presented on each website were reviewed and
the business program were male (n = 40), and most educa-
edited by 10 proofreaders to ensure appropriate content and face
tion students were female (n = 69). Most of the participants
validity. The level of manipulation for visual appeal, usability,
were White (n = 123). An independent-samples T test showed
and information relevance was consistent across the website
that the participants’ differences in computer usage and the use
conditions.
of web-based resources to study was not significant. A total
of 133 usable responses (88%) were produced by this study’s
sample. 3.3. Pilot Study and Manipulation Checks
A pilot study was conducted with 41 undergraduate busi-
ness majors. Manipulation checks were administered to ensure
3.2. Research Design
that the eight website conditions were manipulated sufficiently
The laboratory experiment was conducted in a controlled
and appropriately. Participants were asked to make comments
setting in a computer laboratory where participants worked indi-
on the differences between the website visual appeal, usability,
vidually on completing a learning task. The study was designed
and quality of information across all website conditions. Data
as a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial experiment with two levels (low vs.
from the pilot study were then used to revise the website design
high) for each of three factors (visual appeal – visceral level,
manipulations for the main experiment.
usability – behavioral level, and relevance of the information –
reflective level). Thus, eight website conditions were created
for the experiment as presented in Table 1. Each experimental 3.4. Procedure
website was designed to closely mimic the design of legitimate Data collection took place in a computer lab with one
organizations’ websites to increase the fidelity of treatment. The computer per participant to control exogenous variance relating

TABLE 1
Eight Experimental Website Conditions
Website Characteristic

Website Condition Website Code Visual Appeal Usability Information Relevance


1 V1B0R1 High Low Relevant
2 V1B1R1 High High Relevant
3 V0B1R0 Low High Irrelevant
4 V0B1R1 Low High Relevant
5 V0B0R0 Low Low Irrelevant
6 V1B1R0 High High Irrelevant
7 V0B0R1 Low Low Relevant
8 V1B0R0 High Low Irrelevant
586 S. PENGNATE AND P. ANTONENKO
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

FIG. 1. Thumbnails of the eight experimental websites.

to hardware and software issues, such as display size, monitor by the participants to find the answers to the learning task.
color configuration, network response time, web browser, and An average experimental session lasted approximately 45 min.
operating systems. Participants individually arrived at the com- Participants received up to 10 points (2.5%) extra credit toward
puter lab during a regular class time and were given a brief their course grade as compensation for their participation. The
written introduction to the study including the consent form. extra credits were divided into 5 points for participation and
Screen recording software, CamStudioTM (2010), was installed 5 points based on the correctness of participants’ answers. One
on all computers in the lab to record participants’ website point was earned for each answer relevant to learning task.
browsing behavior. Before the data were collected, a trial task
was administered to acquaint the participants with CamStudio’s
screen recording interface (the Record and Stop buttons) and
3.5. A Multimethod Approach
the nature of the information search task. The experimental task
In this study, all data came from two types of sources—
instruction was then provided to participants as follows:
self-reports and screen capture. The two methods were com-
You will now be presented with eight websites, which can be plementary in that they offered different insights on the users’
accessed via the WindowsTM task bar. You need to choose a website navigation and decision making. On one hand, self-reported
to answer this question: “What are the 5 most promising technolo- data afforded the ability to statistically test the proposed hypoth-
gies?” You have four minutes to browse information on the websites
and six more minutes to record your answers in your answer sheet.
esis dealing with participants’ perception of trust and their
metacognitive awareness. On the other hand, the screen-capture
The experimental websites were presented on the monitor as approach allowed collection of subconscious and behavioral
different browser windows in the Microsoft Windows task bar data (that might not be reported accurately by participants;
in random order to avoid an order effect. The participants had Clark, 2010) as to how the participants browsed the websites
4 min to find the answers to encourage rapid selection of the best to complete the experimental task, how they interacted with
website for the purpose. Time duration for finding the answers the websites, which websites they viewed, in what sequence,
was limited to four minutes according to website evaluation and for how long (in milliseconds). The screen-capture data
time suggested by Lindgaard, Fernandes, Dudek, and Brown collection method is also beneficial because it does not inter-
(2006) to enforce rapid selection of trustworthy websites. After fere with primary cognitive processing like, for instance, the
participants wrote down the answers, a paper-based survey was commonly used think-aloud protocols (e.g., Antonenko, Toy, &
administered, and then they were shown a screenshot of each Niederhauser, 2012).
website and asked to rate the trustworthiness of each website. Although eye-tracking can be used to capture useful data
At the conclusion of the experimental session, the researcher (Murata, 2006) regarding the website elements that users
administered an open-ended question on the strategies used examine on a web page per the visceral level of Norman’s
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 587

(2003) user interface design model, it is not applicable at the in a self-reported using Likert-scale responses from 1 (least
behavioral level because eye tracking cannot capture the actual trustworthy) to 9 (most trustworthy). In addition, participants
on-screen interactions of users with website design elements, were then asked to provide a response to the following open-
such as using the mouse to control scroll bars or switching ended question: “What strategies did you use to evaluate the
between multiple websites. Therefore, screen capture was more websites?” On average, each participant provided approxi-
appropriate in this context in the sense that it afforded recording mately eight to 10 lines of the text responding to the open-ended
of participants’ interactions with the experimental websites via question. Finally, participants were asked to answer questions
a computer mouse. regarding their experience with the Internet and technology and
This dual methodology contributes to enhancing online demographic information.
trust research, which so far has relied heavily on either self-
reported or behavioral data (Williams, Cote, & Buckley, 1989).
4. RESULTS
Furthermore, the qualitative, open-ended question included in
the survey was used to afford a better understanding of “why” H1: The effects of relevant information (i.e., the reflective
and “how” participants evaluate the websites as evidenced by level) will override the visceral and behavioral effects on
the screen capture data. participants’ website trust.
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

Participants’ answers to the question regarding the five most


3.6. Measurement and Construct Validity promising technology used in the experimental task were ana-
The paper-based survey administered contained ques- lyzed for the effects of the three website design levels on trust
tions regarding metacognitive awareness, learning perfor- (Figure 2). Based on the participants’ answer sheet, the top three
mance, trust, website browsing strategies, experience with websites conditions that were most trusted include Website
the Internet and technology, and demographic information. Condition 2 (V1B1R1), 4 (V0B1R1), and 6 (V1B1R0), which
Participants’ metacognitive awareness was measured by using represent 35.3%, 27.8%, and 23.3% of the total responses,
the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) developed by respectively, and account for 86.4% of the total participants.
Schraw and Dennison (1994). The items in MAI reflect the A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted
knowledge of cognition and regulation of cognition dimen- to investigate the differences in website trustworthiness across
sions of metacognitive awareness and divide them further website conditions based on the self-reported rating of trust rat-
into eight distinct factors: procedural knowledge, declarative ing for each website. Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference
knowledge, conditional knowledge, information management post hoc comparison was then run to examine specific dif-
strategies, debugging strategies, planning, comprehension mon- ferences between website conditions. The results of Tukey’s
itoring, and evaluation). All items of the MAI were rated on a Honestly Significant Difference post hoc comparison and the
6-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly mean scores of trust across websites are presented in Table 2.
agree). Construct validity of the MAI measures was assessed Overall, Hypothesis 1 was not supported, because trustworthi-
using Cronbach’s alpha and resulted in the value of .95 indi- ness scores for Website Condition 6 (low information relevance,
cating excellent internal consistency (Rivard & Huff, 1988). high visual appeal, and high usability) and Website Condition
In addition to establishing the construct validity, the MAI scores 8 (low information relevance, high visual appeal, and low
were examined for their relationship with academic achieve- usability) were significantly higher than website conditions
ment as suggested by several prior studies (Coutinho, 2007; containing relevant information, F(7, 1055) = 26.25, p < .001.
Nietfeld, Cao, & Jason, 2005; Schraw & Dennison, 1994;
Young & Fry, 2008). Pearson’s correlations were computed
for participants’ metacognitive awareness scores, grade point
average, and performance on the experimental task. The results
showed that there were significant positive correlations between
metacognitive awareness, grade point average (r = .190), and
task performance (r = .182). Even though the correlations
are relatively low, these results are consistent with the litera-
ture and demonstrate that metacognitive awareness is positively
correlated with academic achievement.
Learning performance was measured by computing the num-
ber of relevant responses to the question (maximum score was
10). After participants submitted their responses on the learn-
ing task, they were asked to view a brief presentation that
demonstrated screenshots of each of the eight websites and
then rate their perceptions of trustworthiness for each website FIG. 2. Summary of website trustworthiness survey.
588 S. PENGNATE AND P. ANTONENKO

TABLE 2
Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference (HSD) Post Hoc Comparison Results
Website Condition N M SD Tukey HSD Group
#2 – V1B1R1 133 7.01 2.33 a
#6 – V1B1R0 133 6.69 2.55 a
#8 – V1B0R0 132 6.31 2.30 a
#4 – V0B1R1 133 6.18 2.54 a
#7 – V0B0R1 133 5.13 2.80 b
#1 – V1B0R1 133 5.03 2.40 b
#3 – V0B1R0 133 4.53 2.40 b, c
#5 – V0B0R0 133 4.02 2.08 c
Note. The Tukey HSD Group column represents Tukey’s HSD groupings of means. Different
letters represent significant differences of the means among website conditions.
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

H2: Participants reporting relevant concepts on the test of evaluation). A one-way ANOVA analysis showed that there
learning will have higher metacognitive awareness than were no significant differences in the subdimensions of
participants who report mostly irrelevant information in metacognitive awareness across the top three website condi-
their test answers. tions. The means and standard deviations for each subdimension
are presented in Table 4. Participants selecting Website
To assess the relationship between metacognitive awareness Condition 4 (V0B1R1) demonstrated the highest average rating
and website trust behavior, a one-way ANOVA was conducted of procedural knowledge, declarative knowledge, conditional
using the top three trusted website conditions (2, 4, and 6) as knowledge, information management strategies, debugging
an independent variable and metacognitive awareness score as
the dependent variable. The ANOVA results indicated that the
metacognitive awareness of individuals selecting each of the TABLE 3
three website conditions was not significantly different across Means and Standard Deviation of
website conditions. Thus, Hypothesis 2 was not supported. Metacognitive Awareness of the Top Three
The means and standard deviations of metacognitive awareness Websites
across website conditions are presented in Table 3.
Website Condition N M SD
We then further investigated the participants’ responses
related to trust and the eight subdimensions of metacognitive #2 – V1B1R1 47 37.53 10.10
awareness (procedural knowledge, declarative knowledge, con- #4 – V0B1R1 37 38.97 5.83
ditional knowledge, information management strategies, debug- #6 – V1B1R0 31 36.54 8.83
ging strategies, planning, comprehension monitoring, and

TABLE 4
Descriptive Statistics for Metacognitive Awareness Subdimensions
#2a #4b #6c
V1B1R1 V0B1R1 V1B1R0

M SD M SD M SD
Procedural knowledge 3.10 .98 3.51 .86 3.19 .98
Declarative knowledge 6.31 1.77 6.45 1.55 6.25 1.61
Conditional knowledge 3.89 1.14 4.27 .83 3.87 1.23
Information management strategies 7.53 2.07 7.86 1.63 7.22 2.04
Debugging strategies 4.00 1.26 4.45 .55 4.16 1.03
Planning 4.74 1.82 4.67 1.66 4.35 1.79
Comprehension monitoring 4.44 1.94 4.27 1.71 4.25 1.93
Evaluation 3.65 1.63 3.59 1.32 3.32 1.79
Note. The values in boldface represent the highest means among the three website conditions.
a
n = 47. a n = 37. a n = 31.
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 589

strategies, whereas participants selecting Website Condition


2 (V1B1R1) showed the highest average rating of planning,
comprehension monitoring, and evaluation.

H3: Participants with high metacognitive awareness will spend


less time searching for answers to the learning task than
participants with low metacognitive awareness.

Screen-capture data were analyzed for the time spent search-


ing for the answers from the eight website conditions. Video
player software programs like Windows Media Player provide
an effective means to analyze screen-capture data in the video
file format because they display a time line and allow raters to
calculate the total time participants spent browsing the websites FIG. 3. Summary of coded data.
until they located the website that they trusted. Three indepen-
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

dent raters reviewed screen-capture videos for each participant (Denzin & Lincoln, 2005). Figure 3 contains a summary of the
and coded the data indicating the amount of time spent by coded data.
each participant on each of the eight experimental websites and According to the open-ended data, the results revealed that
the sequence of accessing these websites. Interrater reliability 48% of the participants started browsing the websites by consid-
was computed using Pearson product–moment correlation coef- ering website usability (i.e., the behavioral level), especially the
ficient, and this analysis confirmed high interrater agreement layout and readability of the content as the first factor to deter-
(r = .92) within a 2-s margin of error. Participants could switch mine if the websites were trustworthy. Approximately 28% of
between websites several times. However, they had to specify the participants used the visual appearance and attractiveness
only one website to answer the question in the answer sheet. of the websites as the main indicator of website trustworthi-
Analysis of the screen-capture data confirmed that all the par- ness. Finally, approximately 17% of the participants evaluated
ticipants spent most time reading information from the website the content of the information to determine website trustworthi-
that they trusted as indicated in the answer sheet. ness. Almost 7% of the participants could not identify or recall
To test Hypothesis 3, we ranked the participants based on strategies they used in the website evaluation.
metacognitive awareness and then divided the data set into In addition to the three emotional design levels highlighted
terciles by metacognitive awareness scores. The top, mid- in the experimental websites, analysis of open-ended response
dle, and bottom terciles represent group of participants with data revealed two prominent themes for website design ele-
high, moderate, and low metacognitive awareness, respectively. ments that influence online trust:
In this analysis, we were interested in only the low and high
metacognitive awareness groups, so participants in the middle Visual rhetoric. Several participants believe that images dis-
tercile were excluded from the analysis. played on websites convey important messages. For exam-
Cases with the highest and the lowest scores were then used ple, several participants made comments that the most
in a T test that compared means based on the average time trustworthy website was selected because the website con-
spent searching for answers as the independent variable. The tained technology-related images that were relevant to the
results suggested that the effect of metacognitive awareness on question asked in the experimental task, and based on that
time spent searching for the answers was in the correct direction they assumed that website content was relevant.
as expected in the hypothesis—that is, participants with higher Design appropriateness. Participants made comments regard-
metacognitive awareness tended to find the answers faster (137 s ing how appropriate website design matched purposes
on average) than those with lower metacognitive awareness of websites. A number of participants reported that sev-
(147 s). However, the difference in metacognitive awareness eral of the experimental websites looked like forum-based
for these two participant groups was not statistically significant. websites or had layouts resembling blogs so the informa-
Therefore, Hypothesis 3 was not supported. tion on those websites was perceived as opinion rather
than fact. Therefore, online trust in these situations was
diminished.
4.1. Results of Qualitative Analysis In general, open-ended data supported results from the quan-
To better understand the influence of the three emotional titative analysis and provided some insights into the screen
design levels highlighted in the experimental websites, open- capture analysis. Several participants reported that the major
ended data were analyzed by coding and then categorizing par- reason why they started browsing the websites by focusing on
ticipants’ website browsing behavior, followed by the creation usability first was because it could help them find the answers
of emerging themes about website design and trust evaluation faster within the limited time constraints of the experiment.
590 S. PENGNATE AND P. ANTONENKO

In some cases, participants mentioned that they felt websites usability (behavioral level) can override the effect of informa-
with an abundance of visual design elements had negative affec- tion quality (reflective level) on trust. This finding extends work
tive influences (i.e., “too gaudy” or “extravagant”) on website on website credibility (Fogg et al., 2003) and on website design
evaluation. (Cyr, 2008; Flavián et al., 2006). This study is also the first study
to extend Norman’s (2003) emotional design framework into
the realm of website design with subsequent consequences on
online trust.
5. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS Third, this study theorized that the moderation effects
This study clarifies the impacts of emotional levels of of individuals’ metacognitive awareness on their perceptions
website design and metacognitive awareness on website trust- regarding website trust would be significant. However, this
worthiness during a learning task. Our research suggests that was not the case in the context of this study. Even though
when individuals have limited time and a high number of we offered rewards for each correct answer in the experimen-
websites to choose from, website trustworthiness is influenced tal tasks, it seems that participants were not intensively using
by affective (visceral) and usability (behavioral) design levels their metacognitive awareness in evaluating the experimental
rather than the relevance of information (the reflective level). websites when pressed for time. Specifically, they did not likely
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

Results reveal that the interaction effects of website visual have the time or motivation to process the websites at a deeper
appeal and website usability can override the effects of the qual- reflective level but judged website trustworthiness based on
ity or relevance of the information. In addition, results also their feelings instead. Consistent with findings in the cognitive
suggest that in the context of limited time to find the answers, science literature (Nelson, 1996; Nelson, Kruglanski, & Jost,
these effects of the website evaluation on trust are not moder- 1998), one factor that stimulates metacognition is motivation
ated by users’ metacognitive awareness. However, even though for accuracy. The elaboration likelihood model (ELM; Petty &
the results of statistical tests were not significant, our findings Cacioppo, 1986) can explain why this occurred. ELM proposes
suggest that individuals with higher metacognitive awareness that there are two major factors that influence human informa-
tend to perform better on an information search task by spend- tion processing: motivation and ability. Without deep motiva-
ing shorter time searching for relevant information. To our tion, even though individuals may have the ability to process
knowledge, this is the first study conducted to compare the information, they are likely to rely on a peripheral route, such as
effects of the levels of emotional website design and effects visual appearance, in their assessment (Fogg et al., 2003; Petty
of metacognitive awareness on website trustworthiness when & Cacioppo, 1986). Consequently, if this study’s participants
online users have limited time and are exposed to a range of had high motivation to examine the experimental websites care-
websites. fully, ELM would predict that the participants would use the
From a theoretical perspective, this study provides the fol- central route, such as more intensive use of metacognition, and,
lowing contributions. First, website design elements are con- therefore, the moderation effects of metacognitive awareness
firmed as important predictors of online trust. Although these would play a more significant role. In addition to the effects
relationships have been supported by previous studies (Cyr, on trust, metacognitive awareness did not impact time spent
2008; Cyr, Head, Larios, & Pan, 2009; Flavián et al., 2006; Fogg searching for the answers. Although, as expected, the partici-
et al., 2003), this study validated these results by testing the pants with high metacognitive awareness on average spent less
impacts of various website design elements using experimen- time searching for the answers, this effect was not statistically
tal manipulations of emotional design—visual appeal, usability, significant.
and information quality. According to our results, the experi- The fourth theoretical contribution of this study is the intro-
mental website condition that was trusted most by participants duction of two themes that emerged from the analysis of
was Website Condition 4, which contained low visual appeal responses to the open-ended question. Both visual rhetoric and
but high usability and provided high-quality information. The design appropriateness have been investigated in the online
number of participants trusted this website condition is higher shopping context as impacting the overall perception of the
than Website Condition 2, which had been designed to be the purpose of the website (Cyr et al., 2009) and relevance of infor-
“best” website where all visual appeal, usability, and quality mation presentation format (Deng & Poole, 2010; Hong, Thong,
information were high. One of the explanations for this unex- & Tam, 2004; Nadkarni & Gupta, 2007), respectively. However,
pected finding may be that processing of the multiple elements the roles of both concepts are still not well understood and may
presented on some of the websites, such as those found in result in different user responses in the educational website con-
Website Condition 2, is compromised due to a competition for text as online shopping websites and educational websites have
participants’ limited working memory resources, and such com- different goals and result in different motivations and patterns
petition hinders evaluation of website trust in the context of of HCI. Hence, it is important to determine the applicability of
limited time for completing the task. these concepts in online learning, as well as other domains.
Second, another significant finding of this study is that the From a practical perspective, there are several contributions
interaction effect of website visual appeal (visceral level) and that arise from this study that will be of interest to online users
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 591

and website designers. In particular, website users are advised Azevedo, R., & Jacobson, M. J. (2008). Advances in scaffolding learning with
to pay attention to information quality when evaluating informa- hypertext and hypermedia: A summary and critical analysis. Educational
Technology Research and Development, 56, 93–100.
tion presented on websites, as there is a potential for halo effect, Baloglu, S., & Pekcan, Y. A. (2006). The website design and Internet site
where the judgment of website’s visual appeal can override marketing practices of upscale and luxury hotels in Turkey. Tourism
the assessment of information quality, as found in the exper- Management, 27(1), 171–176. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2004.07.003
Bovee, M. W. (2004). A Conceptual Framework and Empirical Validation.
imental Website Condition 6, which was rated as high visual
Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS.
appeal and usability, but it actually did not present high-quality Casaló, L., Flavián, C., & Guinalíu, M. (2008). The role of perceived
information. usability, reputation, satisfaction and consumer familiarity on the website
Further, website designers and managers should consider loyalty formation process. Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 325–345.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2007.01.017
the impact of website elements related to trust. Websites that Clark, R. E. (2010). Cognitive and neuroscience research on learning and
carry high-quality information but are perceived as inferior to instruction: recent insights about the impact of non-conscious knowledge
those with higher visual appeal and usability can cause distrust on problem solving, higher order thinking skills and inter- active cyber-
among users because they perceive such websites as unap- learning environments. Paper presented at the International Conference on
Education Research, October, Seoul, Korea.
pealing. Finally, the results of this study can provide guidance Clemmensen, T. (2011). Templates for cross-cultural and culturally specific
as to which types of website elements should be used in the usability testing: results from field studies and ethnographic interviewing
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

website interface to suit particular website audiences’ prefer- in three countries. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction,
ences. For example, if target audiences of a website tend to 27, 634–669. doi:10.1080/10447318.2011.555303
Coutinho, S. A. (2007). The relationship between goals, metacognition, and
have relatively low metacognition, such as children, the website academic success. The Journal of Doctoral Research in Education, 7,
designers should design the interfaces that help its users achieve 39–47.
their goals with minimum reflective effort. Cyr, D. (2008). Modeling web site design across cultures: Relationships to trust,
As with any study, the findings should be considered in light satisfaction, and e-Loyalty. Journal of Management Information Systems,
24(4), 47–72. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/MIS0742-1222240402
of their limitations. First, the 10-point extra credit inducement Cyr, D., & Bonanni, C. (2005). Gender and website design in e-business.
may not have been sufficient to encourage the participants to International Journal of Electronic Business, 3, 565–582.
engage in deep reflective processes performing the experimen- Cyr, D., Head, M., & Larios, H. (2010). Colour appeal in website design within
and across cultures: A multi-method evaluation. International Journal of
tal task. Additional research can compare results obtained in a
Human–Computer Studies, 68, 1–21. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.08.005
real setting with a greater stake in the outcome of the search task Cyr, D., Head, M., Larios, H., & Pan, B. (2009). Exploring human images in
so that participants are more motivated to look for indicators of Website design: a multi-method approach. MIS Quarterly, 33, 539–566.
website quality. Second, experimental websites were manipu- Davis, F. D. (1989). Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user
acceptance of information technology. MIS Quarterly, 13(3), 319.
lated for visual appearance, usability, and information quality. Deng, L., & Poole, M. S. (2010). Affect in web interfaces: A study of the
However, as noted by some participants, the layout of several impacts of web page visual complexity and order. MIS Quarterly, 34,
websites appeared inappropriate because it reminded them of a 711–730.
discussion forum or blog. Other studies can, therefore, explore Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). Introduction: The discipline and practice
of qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The sage
the effects of website layout perceptions either in this context or handbook of qualitative research (2 ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
in other contexts. Dittmer, H., Long, K., & Meek, R. (2004). Buying on the internet: Gender
To conclude, it is hoped that this study will inform future difference in on-line and conventional buying motivations. Sex Roles, 50,
research to expand our understanding of the role of website 423–444.
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and Believing: The Influence
design features and learners’ individual differences in website of Emotion on Trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5),
evaluation. This research also has the potential to provide 736–748. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736
website users and learners with enhanced knowledge on how Éthier, J., Hadaya, P., Talbot, J., & Cadieux, J. (2006). B2C web site quality and
to evaluate website trustworthiness and credibility, which is emotions during online shopping episodes: An empirical study. Information
& Management, 43, 627–639.
critical to the development of information literacy in the 21st Éthier, J., Hadaya, P., Talbot, J., & Cadieux, J. (2008). Interface
century, especially in the educational domain. design and emotions experienced on B2C Web sites: Empirical test-
ing of a research model. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(6),
2771–2791.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new
REFERENCES area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10),
Ally, M. (2004). Foundations of educational theory for online learning. In R. 906–911.
McGreal (Ed.), Designing effective learning objects for distance education Flavián, C., Guinalíu, M., & Gurrea, R. (2006). The role played by perceived
(pp. 87–97). London, UK: Routledge Falmer. usability, satisfaction and consumer trust on website loyalty. Information &
Antonenko, P., Toy, S., & Niederhauser, D. (2012). Using cluster analysis for Management, 43(1), 1–14. doi:10.1016/j.im.2005.01.002
data mining in educational technology research. Educational Technology Fogg, B. J., Soohoo, C., Danielson, D. R., Marable, L., Stanford, J., & Tauber, E.
Research and Development, 60, 363–398. doi:10.1007/s11423-012-9235-8 R. (2003). How do users evaluate the credibility of Web sites?: A study with
Artino, A. R., Jr., & Jones Ii, K. D. (2012). Exploring the complex rela- over 2,500 participants. Proceedings of the 2003 Conference on Designing
tions between achievement emotions and self-regulated learning behaviors for User Experiences, 1–15.
in online learning. The Internet and Higher Education, 15, 170–175. Gage, J. (1999). Color and culture: Practice and meaning from antiquity to
doi:10.1016/j.iheduc.2012.01.006 abstraction. Berkeley: The University of California Press.
592 S. PENGNATE AND P. ANTONENKO

Garbarino, E., & Strahilevitz, M. (2004). Gender differences in the perceived McKnight, D. H., & Chervany, N. L. (2001). What trust means in
risk of buying online and the effects of receiving a site recommen- e-commerce customer relationships: An interdisciplinary conceptual typol-
dation. Journal of Business Research, 57, 768–775. doi:10.1016/s0148- ogy. International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 6(2), 35–59.
2963(02)00363-6 Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Metacognition: Knowing about
Garner, R. (1987). Metacognition and reading comprehension. Cognition and knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
literacy. Westport, CT: Ablex. Moss, G., Gunn, R., & Heller, J. (2006). Some men like it black, some
Garner, R. (1990). When children and adults do not use learning strategies: women like it pink: consumer implications of differences in male
Toward a theory of settings. Review of Educational Research, 60, 517–529. and female website design. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 5(4),
Garrison, D. R. (2003). Cognitive presence for effective asynchronous online 328–341.
learning: The role of reflective inquiry, self-direction and metacognition. Murata, A. (2006). Eye-gaze input versus mouse: Cursor control as a function
In J. Bourne & J. C. Moore (Eds.), Elements of quality online education: of age. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 21, 1–14.
Practice and direction (pp. 47–58). Needham, MA: Sloan-C. doi:10.1080/10447310609526168
Garrison, D. R., & Cleveland-Innes, M. (2005). Facilitating cognitive presence Nadkarni, S., & Gupta, R. (2007). A task-based model of perceived website
in online learning: Interaction is not enough. American Journal of Distance complexity. MIS Quarterly, 31, 501–524.
Education, 19, 133–148. doi:10.1207/s15389286ajde1903_2 Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American
Gefen, D. (2000). E-commerce: the role of familiarity and trust. Omega, 28, Psychologist, 51, 102–116. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.51.2.102
725–737. doi:10.1016/s0305-0483(00)00021-9 Nelson, T. O., Kruglanski, A. W., & Jost, J. T. (1998). Knowing thyself and
Gefen, D., Karahanna, E., & Straub, D. W. (2003). Trust and TAM in Online others: Progress in metacognitive social psychology. In V. Y. Yzerbyt,
Shopping: An Integrated Model. MIS Quarterly, 27(1), 51–90. G. Lories, & B. Dardenne (Eds.), Metacognition: Cognitive and social
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

Hassanein, K. S., & Milena, H. M. (2007). Manipulating perceived social pres- dimensions (pp. 69–89). London, UK: Sage.
ence through the web interface and its impact on attitude towards online Nicolaou, A. I., & McKnight, D. H. (2006). Perceived Information Quality in
shopping. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 65, 689–708. Data Exchanges: Effects on Risk, Trust, and Intention to Use. [Article].
Hong, W., Thong, J. Y. L., & Tam, K. Y. (2004). The effects of informa- Information Systems Research, 17(4), 332–351.
tion format and shopping task on consumers’ online shopping behavior: A Nietfeld, J. L., Cao, L., & Jason, W. O. (2005). Metacognitive monitoring accu-
cognitive fit perspective. Journal of Management Information Systems, 21, racy and student performance in the postsecondary classroom. Journal of
149–184. Experimental Education, 74(1), 7–28.
Jarvenpaa, S., Tractinsky, N., & Vitale, M. (2000). Consumer trust in an Internet Norman, D. A. (2003). Emotional design: Why we love (or hate) every things.
store. Information Technology and Management, 1, 45–71. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Jordan, W. P. (2000). Designing pleasurable products. New York, NY: Taylor Pennington, R., Wilcox, H. D., & Grover, V. (2003). The role of system trust
& Francis. in business-to-consumer transactions. Journal of Management Information
Kelton, K., Fleischmann, K. R., & Wallace, W. A. (2008). Trust in digital Systems, 20, 197–226.
information. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood
Technology, 59(3), 363–374. Model of persuasion. In B. Leonard (Ed.), Advances in experimental
Kim, J., & Moon, J. Y. (1998). Designing towards emotional usability in social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York, NY: Academic
customer interfaces–trustworthiness of cyber-banking system interfaces. Press.
Interacting with Computers, 10(1), 1–29. Porat, T., & Tractinsky, N. (2008). Affect as a Mediator between Web-Store
Kim, D. J., Ferrin, D. L., & Rao, H. R. (2009). Trust and satisfaction, two Design and Consumers’ Attitudes toward the Store. In C. Peter & R. Beale
stepping stones for successful e-commerce relationships: A longitudinal (Eds.), Affect and Emotion in Human-Computer Interaction (Vol. 4868,
exploration. Information Systems Research, 20(2), 237–257. pp. 142–153): Berlin, Germany: Springer Berlin / Heidelberg.
Lee, S., & Koubek, R. J. (2011). The impact of cognitive style on user Rivard, S., & Huff, S. L. (1988). Factors of success for end-user computing.
preference based on usability and aesthetics for computer-based systems. Communication of ACM, 31, 552–561. doi:10.1145/42411.42418
International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 27, 1083–1114. Rodgers, S., & Harris, M. A. (2003). Gender and e-commerce: An exploratory
doi:10.1080/10447318.2011.555320 study. Journal of Advertising Research, 43, 322–329.
Li, S., & Lin, B. (2006). Accessing information sharing and information quality Salmon, G. (2002). E-tivities: The key to active online learning. London:
in supply chain management. Decision Support Systems, 42(3), 1641–1656. Routledge Falmer.
Li, H., Sarathy, R., & Xu, H. (2011). The role of affect and cogni- Schlosser, A. E., White, T. B., & Lloyd, S. M. (2006). Converting web site
tion on online consumers’ decision to disclose personal information visitors into buyers: how web site investment increases consumer trust-
to unfamiliar online vendors. Decision Support Systems, 51, 434–445. ing beliefs and online purchase intentions. Journal of Marketing, 70,
doi:10.1016/j.dss.2011.01.017 133–148.
Lim, K., Sia, C., Lee, M., & Benbasat, I. (2006). Do I trust you online, and if Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instructional
so, will I buy? An empirical study of two trust-building strategies. Journal Science, 26(1), 113–125. doi:10.1023/a:1003044231033
of Management Information Systems, 23(2), 233–266. Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive
Lindgaard, G., Dudek, C., Sen, D., Sumegi, L., & Noonan, P. (2011). An awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 460–475.
exploration of relations between visual appeal, trustworthiness and per- doi:10.1006/ceps.1994.1033
ceived usability of homepages. ACM Transactions on Computer–Human Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational
Interaction, 18, 1–30. doi:10.1145/1959022.1959023 Psychology Review, 7, 351.
Lindgaard, G., Fernandes, G., Dudek, C., & Brown, J. (2006). Attention web Schwarz, N. (1986). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational
designers: You have 50 milliseconds to make a good first impression! functions of affective states. In T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
Behaviour & Information Technology, 25, 115–126. Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York:
Liu, I. F., Chen, M. C., Sun, Y. S., Wible, D., & Kuo, C.-H. (2010). The Guilford Press.
Extending the TAM model to explore the factors that affect intention to Sharp, H., Rogers, Y., & Preece, J. (2007). Interaction design: Beyond human-
use an online learning community. Computers & Education, 54, 600–610. computer interaction. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley.
doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.09.009 Shin, D.-H., & Shin, Y.-J. (2011). Consumers’ trust in virtual mall
McCarthy, J., & Wright, P. (2004). Technology as experience. Cambridge, MA: shopping: The role of social presence and perceived security.
MIT Press. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 27, 450–475.
McKnight, D. H., Cummings, L. L., & Chervany, N. L. (1998). Initial trust doi:10.1080/10447318.2011.552060
formation in new organizational relationships. The Academy of Management Siau, K., & Shen, Z. (2003). Building customer trust in mobile commerce.
Review, 23(3), 473–490. Commun. ACM, 46(4), 91–94.
EMOTIONAL DESIGN, ONLINE TRUST, AND METACOGNITION 593

Simon, S. J. (2000). The impact of culture and gender on web sites: An empiri- ABOUT THE AUTHORS
cal study. SIGMIS Database, 32, 18–37. doi:10.1145/506740.506744
Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and
Supavich (Fone) Pengnate is a doctoral candidate at Oklahoma
instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296. State University. He completed his Master of Science at
Tractinsky, N., & Lowengart, O. (2007). Web-Store aesthetics in e-retailing: University of North Carolina-Greensboro. Fone’s research
A conceptual framework and some theoretical implications. Academy of focuses on the influence of user interface design on users’
Marketing Science Review, 11(1).
Van der Heijden, H. (2004). User acceptance of hedonic information systems. perceptions and responses in various computer platforms.
MIS Quarterly, 28, 695–704. Pavlo (Pasha) Antonenko is an Assistant Professor of
Wells, J. D., Valacich, J. S., & Hess, T. J. (2011). What signal are you sending? Educational Technology at the University of Florida. Pavlo
How website quality influences perceptions of product quality and purchase holds a PhD in Education and Human Computer Interaction
intentions. MIS Quarterly, 35, 373–396.
Williams, L. J., Cote, J. A., & Buckley, M. R. (1989). Lack of method variance from Iowa State University. He teaches educational technology
in self-reported affect and perceptions at work: Reality or artifact? Journal graduate courses and conducts research in problem-based learn-
of Applied Psychology, 74(3), 462–468. ing, psychophysiology and cognition, and design of learner- and
Young, A., & Fry, J. D. (2008). Metacognitive awareness and academic achieve-
teacher-friendly interfaces.
ment in college students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and
Learning, 8(2), 1–10.
Downloaded by [University of Central Florida] at 11:04 22 November 2014

You might also like