Examining Executive Functioning in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Typical Development

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210 – 222


www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum


disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
and typical development
Blythe A. Corbett a,b,⁎, Laura J. Constantine b , Robert Hendren a,b ,
David Rocke c , Sally Ozonoff a,b
a
Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA
b
M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA
c
Division of Biostatistics, University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA
Received 27 July 2007; received in revised form 30 January 2008; accepted 8 February 2008

Abstract

Executive functioning (EF) is an overarching term that refers to neuropsychological processes that enable physical, cognitive,
and emotional self-control. Deficits in EF are often present in neurodevelopmental disorders, but examinations of the specificity of
EF deficits and direct comparisons across disorders are rare. The current study investigated EF in 7- to 12-year-old children with
autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and typical development using a comprehensive
battery of measures assessing EF, including response inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, planning, fluency and
vigilance. The ADHD group exhibited deficits in vigilance, inhibition and working memory relative to the typical group; however,
they did not consistently demonstrate problems on the remaining EF measures. Children with ASD showed significant deficits in
vigilance compared with the typical group, and significant differences in response inhibition, cognitive flexibility/switching, and
working memory compared with both groups. These results lend support for previous findings that show children with autism
demonstrate generalized and profound impairment in EF. In addition, the observed deficits in vigilance and inhibitory control
suggest that a significant number of children with ASD present with cognitive profiles consistent with ADHD.
© 2008 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Attention; Inhibition; Comorbidity; Working memory; CANTAB; Neuropsychology; Vigilance

1. Introduction

Executive function (EF) is an overarching term that


⁎ Corresponding author. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral refers to mental control processes that enable physical,
Sciences, M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California, Davis, 2825 cognitive, and emotional self-control (Denckla, 1996;
50th Street, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA. Tel.: +1 916 703 0232;
fax: +1 916 703 0244. Lezak, 1995; Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996) and are
E-mail address: blythe.corbett@ucdmc.ucdavis.edu necessary to maintain effective goal-directed behavior
(B.A. Corbett). (Welsh and Pennington, 1988). Executive functions
0165-1781/$ - see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 211

generally include response inhibition, working memory, distinct EF profiles, a notion that has received some
cognitive flexibility (set-shifting), planning and fluency foundational support. Specifically, impaired motor and
(Ozonoff and Strayer, 1997; Pennington and Ozonoff, response inhibition in ADHD is well supported (e.g.,
1996). Deficits in EF are frequently observed in (Barkley et al., 1992; Geurts et al., 2004; Pennington and
neurodevelopmental disorders, including autism and Ozonoff, 1996). Deficits in planning and set-shifting
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). have been shown to be more pronounced in individuals
Autism is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder with HFA than ADHD and typical development (Geurts
characterized by impairment in communication, recipro- et al., 2004; Ozonoff et al., 2004; Ozonoff and Strayer,
cal social interaction, and a markedly restricted reper- 1997; Sergeant et al., 2002). Similarly, more impairment
toire of activities and interests (American Psychiatric in HFA as compared with ADHD has also been de-
Association, 2000). The symptoms of autism fall on a monstrated with verbal working memory (Pennington
continuum of severity referred to as autism spectrum and Ozonoff, 1996) and spatial working memory
disorder (ASD), which include autistic disorder, Asper- (Goldberg et al., 2005; Landa and Goldberg, 2005).
ger syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder— Studies are beginning to emerge directly comparing
not otherwise specified (PDD–NOS). Autism is often autism and ADHD groups with their typically develop-
further divided into those with mental retardation and ing counterparts (Corbett and Constantine, 2006; Gold-
those functioning in the average or above average range berg et al., 2005; Ozonoff and Jensen, 1999; Verte et al.,
of intelligence (often called high functioning autism or 2006). These studies have generally found EF deficits
HFA). In addition to the core features, deficits in EF have across both diagnostic groups, with ostensibly more
been widely reported (e.g., (Geurts et al., 2004; Goldberg severe and global deficits in ASD. Deficits within the
et al., 2005; Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff et al., 1991; ADHD groups tend to be more consistently restricted
Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996)). The primacy of EF to behavioral disinhibition and vigilance (Corbett and
deficits in 57 autism (Russell, 1997), especially in terms Constantine, 2006; Goldberg et al., 2005; Ozonoff
of planning, cognitive flexibility and working memory, and Jensen, 1999; Verte et al., 2006). However, there is
remains an ongoing debate (for a review, see Hill, 2004). evidence that individuals with ADHD may also have
ADHD is also a neurologically mediated disorder deficits in planning, set-shifting, and spatial working
that exists on a continuum. ADHD is characterized by memory (e.g., Kempton et al., 1999).
varying degrees of inattention, impulsivity and hyper- Recently, Goldberg et al. (2005) examined inhibition,
active behavior (American Psychiatric Association, planning, set-shifting and working memory in a sample
2000). ADHD is further divided into those individuals of children 8 to 12 years of age with HFA, ADHD and
meeting symptom criteria in all the aforementioned typical development. Participants were carefully
areas (Combined type), those primarily evidencing assessed to screen out comorbid impulsivity or hyper-
attention problems (Predominantly inattentive type) and activity in autism. Using a computerized battery
those with mostly hyperactive and impulsive symptoms (CANTAB®), the study reported that response inhibi-
(Predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type). Significant tion, planning, and set-shifting were similar across the
EF deficits in individuals with ADHD have been three groups of ASD, ADHD and typical development
reported; however, there is still some inconsistency and only impaired spatial working memory in the ADHD
regarding particular impairments in domains of function- and HFA groups were reported (Goldberg et al., 2005).
ing. In a comprehensive meta-analysis, Willcutt et al., However, age and level of functioning on this measure
2005 found that studies most consistently report response may explain the limited sensitivity (Goldberg et al.,
inhibition and vigilance deficits in ADHD. Other 2005; Landa and Goldberg, 2005).
impairments have been found in working memory (e.g., Since deficits in EF are present in several neurode-
(Kempton et al., 1999; Rhodes et al., 2005)), planning velopmental disorders, the issue of discriminant validity
(e.g., (Kempton et al., 1999; Rhodes et al., 2005)), and must be considered as to how disorders with different
flexibility (Vance et al., 2003). behavioral phenotype can share similar neuropathologi-
Few studies have directly compared EF across ADHD cal substrates (Ozonoff and Jensen, 1999). Geurts et al.
and ASD groups and studies investigating these groups (2004) expanded on this notion using a comprehensive
separately report inconsistent findings. Some have neuropsychological battery in children between 6 and
proposed that EF deficits are core to ASD (Russell, 12 years and reported that children with ADHD de-
1997) and ADHD (Barkley, 1997). Pennington and monstrated EF deficits in inhibition and verbal fluency
Ozonoff (1996) suggested that deficits in domains of EF while children with HFA showed deficits across most of
could be disassociated across disorders resulting in the EF measures.
212 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

Verte et al. (2006) recently reported significant on population-based investigation, it has been shown
differences for children with ADHD and HFA in inhibition that children diagnosed with ADHD may show autistic
and response variability compared to children with traits (Reiersen et al., 2007), which punctuates the
Tourette Syndrome or typical development. Further, importance of investigating comorbid features.
poorer inhibition and more response variability were Although we recognize the value of elucidating EF in
associated with symptoms of ADHD, while poor working clearly defined prototypical cases of autism and ADHD,
memory was associated with more symptoms of autism. this may not be representative or generalizable to many
Taken together, the majority of the studies conclude that children on the spectrums of autism or ADHD. Thus, we
children with ASD exhibit more pronounced deficits in EF conducted a comprehensive neuropsychological study
than children with ADHD. to compare and contrast six domains of EF (response
Converging evidence from a variety of methods inhibition, working memory, flexibility/shifting, plan-
including chart review (Goldstein and Schwebach, ning, fluency and vigilance), in children with ASD,
2004), parent and teacher questionnaires (Gadow ADHD and typical development deliberately allowing
et al., 2004) and neuropsychological measures (Corbett comorbid ADHD features in the children with ASD. We
and Constantine, 2006) conclude that a high percentage hypothesized that children with ASD would demon-
of children with ASD evidence symptoms of ADHD, strate greater impairment across a broad range of EF
some warranting a comorbid diagnosis. The etiology of tasks. Simultaneously, we investigated the performance
both neurodevelopmental disorders has a strong genetic of EF and related it to the level of ADHD symptoms
basis with heritability estimates for autism to be 0.9 across these groups. We predicted that specific measures
(Baron-Cohen and Belmonte, 2005; Zafeiriou et al., of vigilance and behavioral inhibition would be
2007) and estimates for ADHD to be 0.7 (Faraone et al., associated with ADHD symptoms across the groups.
2005). Furthermore, there are some preliminary findings
of a genetic linkage between these disorders at 2. Methods
chromosomal locations 2q24 and 16p13 (Fisher et al.,
2002; Ogdie et al., 2003; Smalley et al., 2005). Even 2.1. Participants
without consideration of comorbid features, various
neuroscientific models have highlighted the common The participants in this study included 18 children
behavioral features, biological pathways and neuroana- with high functioning (IQ N 70) ASD (autism = 12,
tomical correlates between ASD and ADHD implicating Asperger = 3, PDD-NOS = 3); 18 children with ADHD
the frontostriatal system including the frontal lobes and (combined = 16, primarily inattentive = 1, primarily
basal ganglia (Damasio and Maurer, 1978; Ozonoff and hyperactive/impulsive = 1); and 18 typically developing
Jensen, 1999; Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996; Stuss and children (TYP). The demographic information for the
Benson, 1984). Structural and functional neuroimaging groups is presented in Table 1. Regarding medication,
studies show frontal lobe dysfunction in autism (e.g., seven ADHD participants were receiving stimulant
(Carper and Courchesne, 2000, 2005; Courchesne et al., medication, one of whom was also being treated with
2001; McAlonan et al., 2005; Muller et al., 2001), and clonidine, and two of whom were receiving hormone
ADHD (e.g., (Faraone and Biederman, 1998; Kates medications. Of the ASD participants, six were being
et al., 2002; Lou et al., 1984; Mostofsky et al., 2002; treated with stimulants, neuroleptics, selective serotonin
Smith et al., 2006; Sowell et al., 2003; Zang et al., reuptake inhibitors, or a combination of these. While
2005)). These brain regions are important in EF, and, as stimulant medication was withheld for 24 h prior to
discussed, both disorders have been associated with testing (Greenhill, 1998), other longer term medications
deficits in EF (e.g., (Barkley, 1997; Goldberg et al., were not stopped for ethical reasons. Inclusion criteria for
2005; Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996; Russell, 1997)). all participants consisted of having an IQN 70, an absence
Furthermore, many children with ASD display of Fragile X or other serious neurological (e.g., seizures),
ADHD symptomatology, suggesting that the disorders psychiatric (e.g., bipolar disorder) or medical conditions.
may share similar traits or are often comorbid (Corbett The University of California, Davis Institutional
and Constantine, 2006; Gadow et al., 2004; Geurts et al., Review Board (IRB) approved the study. The child's
2004; Ghaziuddin et al., 1992; Goldberg et al., 2005; parent completed written informed consent and the
Goldstein and Schwebach, 2004; Verte et al., 2006). Yet, child assented to participate in the study. Approximately
the limited studies that have compared these two one third of the children participated in a previous
disorders generally exclude comorbid features (Geurts investigation (Corbett and Constantine, 2006). The
et al., 2004; Goldberg et al., 2005). Conversely, based diagnostic groups were recruited from the University
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 213

Table 1
Means and standard deviations for the demographic variables.
Variable TYP ADHD ASD
M (S.D.) M (S.D.) M (S.D.) F P Eta-squared
Age 9.56 (1.81) 9.40 (1.98) 9.44 (1.96) 0.04 0.966 0.00
IQ 112.22 (14.84) 105.17 (12.82) 94.17 (17.79) 6.38 0.003 0.20
Note. TYP = 18 (12 males, 6 females) typically developing children; ADHD = 18 (12 males, 6 females) children with attention deficit/hyperactivity
disorder; and ASD = 18 (17 males, 1 female) children with autism spectrum disorders.

of California, Davis M.I.N.D. (Medical Investigation of the assessment results from the published, standardized
Neurodevelopmental Disorders) Institute. Thus, many of measures.
the participants already had a confirmed ASD diagnosis
with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord 2.3. Instruments
et al., 1999) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview (Lord
et al., 1994). For children who were not already Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)
evaluated, the following diagnostic procedures were (Lord et al., 1999). The ADOS comprises semi-struc-
conducted. The diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder tured interactive activities designed to assess current
(i.e., Autistic Disorder, PDD-NOS, Asperger) was based behaviors indicative of autism involving social beha-
on DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, vior, communicative functioning, and restricted activ-
2000) and established by 1) a previous diagnosis by ities (Lord et al., 1999).
either a psychologist, psychiatrist or behavioral pedia- Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI;
trician, 2) clinical judgment by a licensed clinical (Wechsler, 1999)). The WASI is a measure of general
psychologist (bac), and 3) confirmation by a score on intelligence used to obtain an estimated IQ for inclusion/
the social-communication scale of the Autism Diagnos- exclusion into the study.
tic Observation Schedule within or above the autism The following dependent neuropsychological mea-
spectrum threshold (Lord et al., 1999). sures are conceptualized based on a previous theoretical
The diagnosis of ADHD was based on DSM-IV criteria model (Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996) and grouped
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994) established by into six EF domains (inhibition, working memory,
1) a previous diagnosis of ADHD by either a psychologist, flexibility, planning, and fluency, vigilance). In addition,
psychiatrist or behavioral pediatrician, 2) clinical judgment a measure of ADHD symptoms was included.
by a licensed clinical psychologist (bac), and 3) a semi- Response Inhibition was measured using the Inte-
structured parent interview extracted from the Diagnostic grated Visual and Auditory response control quotients
Interview Schedule for Children (DISC) (Shaffer et al., and the D-KEFS Color Word Interference Test.
1996). The presence of autism symptoms was an exclusion The Integrated Visual and Auditory (IVA) Continuous
for children who had a primary diagnosis of ADHD but Performance Test (CPT) (Sandford and Turner, 2000)
none of the ADHD children had to be excluded based on was designed to help in the diagnosis and quantification of
this criteria. The typically developing children were the symptoms of ADHD, but it has also been used across a
selected based on age and gender and recruited from variety of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions.
area schools and recreation centers, then screened via The IVA combines inattention (vigilance) and impulsivity
parent interview for the absence of neurodevelopmental in a counter-balanced design across both visual and
disorders, including ASD and ADHD, using the DISC. auditory modalities. The Visual Response Control Quo-
tient (VRCQ) and Auditory Response Control Quotient
2.2. Procedures (ARCQ) are the primary dependent variables.
The Dellis-Kaplan Executive Function System
Diagnostic and neuropsychological measures were (D-KEFS) (Dellis et al., 2001) consists of nine
completed in one visit using standardized procedures. tests that measure a variety of EFs. The D-KEFS Color
A few of the participants were unable to complete every Word Interference Test consists of four conditions
measure. Thus, analyses were completed with only those including: color naming (word finding), word reading
subjects who had data for the measures. Participants (reading speed), and inhibition (verbal inhibition) that
received minimal financial compensation and toys, and expose the child to different reading conditions (fourth
their parents/guardians were sent a letter summarizing condition see Cognitive Flexibility/Switching below).
214 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

The task is analogous to a Stroop test and performance stimuli, and the formerly incorrect shape becomes the
is based on speed of completion. The inhibition por- correct stimuli (intra-dimensional shift). Through Stage 7,
tion requires the ability to verbally inhibit the more it is the shape that determines which picture is correct.
salient response of reading words in order to name the However, beginning with Stage 8, the line identifies the
color of the discordant ink. These tasks are designed correct picture, (extra-dimensional shift) and cognitive
for children 8 years and older. As such, 34 of the 54 flexibility is required. As this shift is the key measure of
participants in the study were administered or able to cognitive flexibility and many subjects were unable to
complete these measures. complete Stage 8, only the number of errors committed in
Working Memory was measured using the CANTAB Stage 8 was used as the dependent variable. As directed in
Spatial Span and Spatial Working Memory subtests. the CANTAB manual, if a subject did not complete Stage
The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated 8, 25 errors were assigned.
Battery (CANTABexpedio) assesses cognitive domains Children's Color Trails Test 1 and 2 (CCTT-1 & 2);
including attention, executive function, memory, proces- (Llorente et al., 1998) are used to measure alternating
sing speed, and visuospatial ability. and sustained visual attention, sequencing ability, psy-
Spatial Span (SSP) measures both forward and chomotor speed, cognitive flexibility and inhibition. It
reverse spatial memory span. At the onset, white is analogous to Trail Making tests for adults but modi-
squares are displayed, some of which momentarily fied for children. The CCTT-1 requires the individual to
change color in an unpredictable pattern. The individual rapidly connect different colored circles in the correct
is required to touch the boxes in the same sequence numerical order (1-2-3…). The CCTT-2 requires con-
order as they changed color. Throughout the task, the necting circles numerically while switching between
number of boxes in the sequence is increased, and the colored numbers. An Interference Index is obtained that
order and color change with each sequence to minimize rates the effect of interference in processing time
interference. The total raw score was used as the depen- needed to complete one task (CCTT2) that is more
dent variable. complex than another (CCTT1). The CCTT2 inter-
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) measures the ference score was the primary dependent variable used
ability to maintain spatial information and to subse- for this measure.
quently manipulate the presented items in working Planning was measured using the CANTAB SOC.
memory. The objective is to find a blue “token” in each Stockings of Cambridge (SOC). The SOC is based on
of the boxes, then select and place them in an empty the “Tower of London” test and is a spatial planning task.
column. The individual must resist returning to a box Two views are shown, each with three colored balls
where a token was previously found, which constitutes which appear to be in “stockings” or stacks. The
an error. The number of boxes is progressively individual must repeat the pattern shown in the example
increased. For each trial, the color and position of the view by touching the ball and then touching where the
boxes are changed. The total spatial working memory ball is to be moved. The individual's planning ability is
between search errors (SWM Btwn Error) and strategy based on how quickly and accurately the pattern is
scores (SWM Strat) were used as dependent variables. imitated. The SOC number of problems solved in
Cognitive Flexibility/Switching was measured using minimum number of moves (SOC Min Moves), initial
the D-KEFS Total Switching Accuracy, CANTAB ID/ thinking (SOC Initial Thinking) and subsequent thinking
ED Set Shifting, and Children's Color Trails Test 2. (SOC Sub Thinking) were the dependent variables used.
The D-KEFS Category Switching (DK T-Switch); Fluency was measured using the D-KEFS Letter
(Dellis et al., 2001) condition is a measure of cognitive Fluency and Category Fluency (see D-KEFS above).
flexibility that requires the individual to shift between The D-KEFS Letter Fluency and Category Fluency
color naming, word reading and inhibition (see Color Tests (Dellis et al., 2001) provide information regarding
Word Interference Test above), and was used as a de- the individual's ability to fluently retrieve words beginning
pendent variable for flexibility. with the same letter, and ability to retrieve lexical items
Intra–Extra Dimensional Set Shift (ID/ED). The ID/ED from a designated category, respectively. The DK Letter
is a test of rule acquisition and reversal that measures and DK Category were the dependent variables used.
shifting and flexibility of attention, visual discrimination, Vigilance was measured using the IVAVAQ and AAQ.
attention set formation, and maintenance. The ID/ED task The IVA Visual Attention Quotient (VAQ) and Auditory
consists of colored shapes and white lines that increase in Attention Quotient (AAQ) (Sandford and Turner, 2000)
complexity throughout the test. Following 6 consecutive are based on equal weights of Vigilance (inattention),
correct responses, the correct shape becomes the incorrect Focus (speed of mental processing), and Speed (reaction
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 215

time). The VAQ and AAQ were the primary dependent 3. Results
variables used for this measure (see IVA above).
ADHD Symptomology was measured using the Con- Descriptive statistics for the 54 participants across the
ners' Parent Rating Scale-Revised (Short) (CPRS-R:S) three groups are presented in Table 1. Chi square analysis
(Conners, 2001). The CPRS-R:S provides information demonstrated that the three groups did not differ relative
about behaviors associated with attention and/or hyper- to gender, χ2 (2, N = 54) = 14.52, P N 0.05. Univariate
activity as well as oppositional behavior. The CPRS ANOVA demonstrated that the three groups did not
was used as an index of ADHD symptoms rather than differ relative to age, F(2,51) = 0.04 P N 0.10. There was
for diagnostic purposes. The four domain Standard a significant difference in Full Scale IQ, F(2,51) = 6.38,
Scores were used as dependent measures and the P b 0.005. Post hoc planned comparisons revealed
ADHD Index (C-ADHD) was used as an index of that this difference was due to the ASD group being
symptomology. significantly lower than both the ADHD, t = (1,34) =
2.13, P b 0.05, and TYP, t = (1,34) = 3.31, P b 0.01,
2.4. Statistical analysis groups. Subsequently, IQ was used as a covariate in
the analyses.
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS® The means and standard deviations and post hoc
(Norusis, 1993). As a first step, we did a multivariate multiple pairwise comparisons (Tukey HSD) for the
analysis of variance, while covarying for IQ (MAN- Conners Parent Rating Scale are reported in Table 2. The
COVA) on the six EF domains. Secondly, of those average range on this measure is defined by T scores
results that were significant, independent Analyses of from 40 to 60. ADHD was defined by a score greater
Variance (ANOVAs) were conducted on the dependent than 1.5 standard deviations above the mean (Conners,
measures. The partial eta square was reported as an 2001). Based on this criterion, all of the ADHD children
index of effect size. Subsequently, we conducted post met criteria, eight of the ASD children, and none of the
hoc multiple pairwise comparisons using the Tukey typical children qualified for a diagnosis of ADHD.
HSD to control for overall Type 1 error and to determine The results of the MANCOVAs for each domain are
what group comparisons were significant across the presented below. The means and standard deviations and
variables. the results of the ANCOVAS of the EF measures grouped
Simultaneously, we investigated the extent to which by domains are presented in Table 3. Post hoc analyses
ADHD symptoms predict attention and EF performance for each domain are presented below. Due to concerns
across these groups using linear and stepwise multiple about the possible effects of medication, analyses were
regression modeling. The predictor variables entered conducted comparing the clinical groups between those
into the model were C-ADHD, diagnosis, IQ and age and with and without medication and there were no
the criterion variables were the EF variables previously significant differences (all F's b 1.0 and P N 0.05).
shown to be statistically significant using ANOVA. We
predicted that measures of vigilance and behavioral 3.1. Response inhibition
inhibition would be more predictive of ADHD across
the groups. Exploratory analyses were conducted with Based on MANCOVA, there was a significant differ-
MANCOVA excluding children with combined ASD ence between the groups regarding Inhibition: F(6,56) =
and ADHD. 3.99, P b 0.005; Wilks' lambda = 0.548. Post hoc

Table 2
Means and standard deviations for the Conners' diagnostic measure.
Variable TYP ADHD ASD
M (S.D.) M (S.D.) M (S.D.) F P Eta-squared
C-OPP 48.78 (7.57) 65.89 (13.85) 55.78 (9.36) 11.87 0.000 0.32
C-INA 46.06 (6.83) 72.33 (9.27) 61.44 (8.45) 46.18 0.000 0.64
C-HYP 47.56 (5.70) 79.33 (10.53) 61.11 (12.94) 44.16 0.000 0.63
C-ADHD 45.72 (5.02) 76.22 (5.22) 64.78 (6.86) 128.96 0.000 0.83
Note. TYP = 18 (12 males, 6 females), ADHD = 18 (12 males, 6 females), ASD = 18 (17 males, 1 female), C = Conners Parent Rating Scale-Revise,
C-OPP = Oppositional, C-INA = Inattention, C-HYP = Hyperactivity, ADHD = Conners' ADHD Index.
216 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

Table 3
Means and standard deviations and analysis of variance for the executive functioning and attention measures.
Measure TYP ADHD ASD
M (S.D.) M (S.D.) M (S.D.) P
Eta-squared
Response inhibition
VRCQ 94.50 18.37 83.28 21.96 65.33 24.90 a, c 0.33
ARCQ 91.56 19.29 72.89 18.38 67.44 19.75 a, b 0.21
DK INH 10.31 3.66 11.27 2.53 5.80 2.20 a 0.29
Working memory
SSP 5.59 1.37 4.67 1.14 3.94 1.39 a, b 0.16
SWM Btwn Errors 41.88 19.94 45.94 21.59 63.18 10.57 a, c 0.14
SWM Strategy 34.35 5.77 35.28 4.11 38.59 2.67 a, c 0.15
Cognitive flexibility
ID/ED-Shift 25.08 10.90 25.25 11.88 23.44 10.58 ns 0.01
DK T-Switch 11.83 2.73 12.75 3.36 7.22 4.18 a, c 0.34
CCTT2 96.67 15.13 86.58 18.72 74.56 18.19 ns 0.14
Planning
SOC Min Moves 6.94 1.92 6.28 2.42 5.06 2.21 ns 0.06
SOC Initial Thinking 0.85 0.38 1.11 0.55 0.67 0.48 ns 0.13
SOC Sub Thinking − 0.15 0.75 − 0.26 1.06 −0.49 0.64 ns 0.01
Fluency
DK Letter 10.92 1.98 10.83 3.95 6.22 4.30 a, c 0.23
DK Category 12.08 2.72 11.83 4.24 8.44 2.65 ns 0.14
Vigilance
VAQ 91.56 22.15 63.00 22.16 61.67 24.75 a, b 0.18
AAQ 94.39 21.23 65.12 19.17 64.06 20.04 a, b 0.25
Note: aASD vs. Typical, P b 0.05; bADHD vs. Typical, P b 0.05; cASD vs. ADHD, P b 0.05; ns, P N 0.05. ⁎N varies depending on test.
VRCQ = Visual Response Control Quotient, ARCQ = Auditory Response Control Quotient, DK = D-KEFS, DK INH = Inhibition, SSP = Spatial Span,
SWM = Spatial Working Memory, ID/ED-Shift = Extradimensional Shift errors, DK T-Switch = Total Switching, CCTT2 = Children's Color Trails
Trial 2, SOC Min Moves = Stockings of Cambridge Minimum Moves, SOC Initial Thinking = Stockings of Cambridge Initial Thinking Time; SOC Sub
Thinking = Stockings of Cambridge Subsequent Thinking Time; DK Letter = Letter Fluency, DK Category = Category Fluency, VAQ = Visual
Attention Quotient, AAQ = Auditory Attention Quotient.

multiple pairwise comparisons (Tukey HSD) for the significant differences between the ADHD and ASD
VRCQ showed significant differences between the ASD children for SWM Btwn Errors, F(2,48)= 3.95, P b 0.01,
and TYP group and ASD and ADHD: F(2,51)= 5.33, and SWM Strategy, F(2,48)= 3.97, P b 0.05, with the ASD
P b 0.01. The ARCQ revealed significant differences be- group performing more poorly. Regarding the SSP, there
tween the ADHD and TYP groups, F(2,51) = 4.59, was a significant difference between the ADHD and TYP
P b 0.01, and between the ASD and TYP groups, group, F(2,51) = 4.72, P b 0.05.
F (2,51)= 5.36, P b 0.01. The ASD group demonstrated
the lowest performance, followed by the ADHD group 3.3. Flexibility/switching
and then the TYP group. There were no differences
between the ADHD and TYP groups on the DK INH, but There were significant differences across the groups for
the ASD group performed significantly lower than the Switching, F(6,54)= 3.18, P b 0.01; Wilks' lambda =0.546.
TYP group, F(2,34) = 6.20, P b 0.01. For DK T-Switch, significant differences were found
between the ADHD and ASD groups, F(2,33)= 7.56,
3.2. Working memory P b 0.01, and between the ASD and TYP groups, P b 0.001.
There were no significant differences for the CCTT2 or the
The MANCOVAwas significant between the groups for ID/ED tasks across the groups.
Working Memory, F(6,92) = 2.67, P b 0.05, Wilks'
lambda= 0.726. There were significant differences between 3.4. Planning
the ASD and TYP groups for SSP, F(2,48)= 4.72, P b 0.05,
and for SWM Btwn Errors, F(2,48) = 3.95, P b 0.05, and There were no significant differences between the
SWM Strategy, F(2,48) = 3.97, P b 0.05. There were groups based on MANCOVA for Planning, F(6,56) =
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 217

1.73, P N 0.05, Wilks' lambda = 0.79, which included 3.6. Vigilance


SOC Min Moves, SOC Initial Thinking and SOC
Subsequent Thinking. Based on MANCOVA, there were significant dif-
ferences in Vigilance between the groups, F(4,98) =
3.5. Fluency 4.63, P b 0.01; Wilks' lambda = 0.707. Subsequently,
significant differences were found between the ADHD
There were no significant differences between the and TYP groups for AAQ, F(2,50) = 8.28, P b 0.001,
ASD and TYP groups for Fluency, F(4,58) = 2.38, and VAQ F(2,50) = 5.58, P b 0.01, with the ADHD
P N 0.05; Wilks' lambda 0.738, which included the DK group performing more poorly. There were significant
Letter and DK Category measures. differences between the ASD and TYP groups on the

Fig. 1. a and b. Scatterplots of the relationship between the C-ADHD (Conners ADHD Index) and the 1a. ARCQ (Auditory Response Control
Quotient) and 1b. AAQ (Auditory Attention Quotient) across the groups: TYP = Typical, ADHD = ADHD, ASD = Autism Spectrum Disorder, and
ASD/ADHD = Autism Spectrum Disorder with ADHD.
218 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

IVA for AAQ, F(2,50) = 8.28, P b 0.001, and VAQ, ASD and ADHD, compared with children with typical
F (2,50) =5.58, P b 0.01. development while not controlling for ADHD symp-
toms. The initial aim was accomplished by assessing
3.7. Prediction and exploratory analysis performance using a comprehensive neuropsychological
battery of EF measures across six domains (response
Next, we used stepwise multiple regression to examine inhibition, vigilance, working memory, flexibility/shift-
the relationship between the dependent variables and ing, planning and fluency). We confirmed our hypothesis
behavioral indices of ADHD symptoms. Stepwise multiple that children with ASD demonstrate pervasive impair-
regression showed that the ADHD index (C-ADHD) was ment across a broad range of EF tasks. Specifically,
the first to enter the equation for AAQ explaining 28.5% of children with ASD showed poor performance relative to
the variance (t = 10.09, P b 0.001); when IQ was also the typical group in inhibition, working memory,
entered into the model it predicted 40.7% of the variance flexibility/shifting and vigilance. The ASD performed
(t = 3.24, P b 0.01). For VAQ, the C-ADHD explained more poorly than the ADHD group in regards to
27.5% of the variance individually (t = 9.41, P b 0.05) and inhibition, working memory, and flexibility. There
when IQ (t = 4.57) and age (t= 3.96) were entered into the were no significant differences observed on measures
equation, 56.3% of the variance was explained (both of planning and fluency across the groups. As can be
P b 0.05). For D-KEFS Inhibition, diagnosis (t = 13.30) seen in Table 3, the ASD group consistently showed
along with the ADHD index (t = 2.47) explained 35.4% of more impairment than the control or ADHD group on all
the variance (P b 0.05). Using linear regression analysis, of the aforementioned EF measures. Thus, the current
the ADHD index predicted 16.1% and 11.1% of the investigation supports previous findings that children
variance for ARCQ (t = 9.27, P b 0.001) and VRCQ with ASD demonstrate generalized and profound
(t = 7.96, P b 0.05), respectively. The ADHD index was impairment in EF skills (Geurts et al., 2004; Goldberg
not predictive for the remaining variables. et al., 2005). It is also consistent with recent studies in
Using an exploratory approach, we investigated the autism reporting working memory deficits (Goldberg
influence of ADHD within the ASD group by removing et al., 2005; Landa and Goldberg, 2005; Pennington and
these subjects. The ASD group was divided into those Ozonoff, 1996; Verte et al., 2006), as well as set-shifting
without ADHD (N =10) and with ADHD (N = 8) based deficits (Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff et al., 2004;
on the ADHD index (N 65), and MANCOVAs were Ozonoff and Strayer, 1997; Sergeant et al., 2002). The
conducted excluding the ASD/ADHD participants on finding that children with ASD performed more poorly
domains which more consistently discriminated the ADHD than children with ADHD on measures of flexibility has
group; namely Inhibition (ARCQ, VRCQ) and Vigilance also been previously reported (Geurts et al., 2004).
(AAQ, VAQ). The MANCOVA with all subjects included Conversely, it has been shown that some ASD subjects
was highly significant for Inhibition (F(4,98) = 3.81, have less severe and persistent EF deficits than ADHD
P =0.006, Wilks' lambda = 0.75). However, the exploratory children (Happe et al., 2006). It appears, however, that
MANCOVA without the ASD/ADHD group fell to trend the majority of the ASD subjects had Asperger syndrome
level F(4, 82) = 2.24, P = 0.07; Wilks' lambda= 0.81 (see (81%) compared to the current study in which the
ARCQ, Fig. 1a), suggesting that the ASD/ADHD majority had autism and few ASD participants had
comorbid group significantly contributed to differences in Asperger syndrome (17%). Thus, developmental and
these domains. The original Vigilance MANCOVA, F diagnostic issues likely serve as important distinctions in
(4,98) =4.63, P b 0.0001, Wilks' lambda = 0.71, conducted EF within ASD.
without the ASD/ADHD group was reduced but remained The ADHD group exhibited deficits in vigilance
statistically significant, F(4,82)=4.241, P=0.004, Wilks' and response inhibition when compared to the TYP
lambda=0.687, suggesting that the results could not entirely group corroborating our previous findings (Corbett and
be explained by ADHD symptoms (see AAQ, Fig. 1b). It is Constantine, 2006) and consistent with a meta-analytic
important to note, however, that this approach resulted in review showing these as the most consistently reported
unequal groups and a smaller sample size, which reduced domains of executive dysfunction in ADHD (Willcutt
the power to detect differences. et al., 2005). Other comparative investigations report
similar and specific deficits in inhibition (Geurts et al.,
4. Discussion 2004; Happe et al., 2006; Pennington and Ozonoff,
1996; Verte et al., 2006). Our ADHD group also showed
The overarching goal of this investigation was to some impairment in working memory, but they did not
profile EF deficits for two major childhood disorders, show statistically significant deficits in the remaining
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 219

areas of EF. The current findings are in contrast to as being able to assist in reliably capturing and classi-
studies, which found impairments across working fying children with symptoms of ADHD in ASD.
memory, planning, and attentional set-shifting simulta- The observed deficits in vigilance and inhibition in
neously (Kempton et al., 1999; Rhodes et al., 2005, our ASD group provide additional evidence that a
2006; Vance et al., 2003). Although the sensitivity of significant number of children with ASD present
some of the measures may be called into question with ADHD-like cognitive impairments (Corbett and
(Goldberg et al., 2005), the results in the current inves- Constantine, 2006; Goldstein and Schwebach, 2004;
tigation are consistent with the notion that children with Happe et al., 2006). Our results differ from some
ADHD demonstrate variable deficits on neuropsycholo- previous investigations (Goldberg et al., 2005) that do
gical measures of EF (Doyle et al., 2000; Pennington and not report significant differences in vigilance and
Ozonoff, 1996; Rhodes et al., 2005, 2006; Vance et al., inhibition in ASD. The lack of replication may be due
2003; Willcutt et al., 2005). Further, it was shown that to distinctions in subject inclusion criteria. Goldberg
such variability, as in our own investigation, is not et al. (2005) excluded subjects with ASD who had
attributed to medication (Doyle et al., 2000). ADHD features, while we did not. This suggests that
While EF deficits are associated with ADHD, they these deficits in our ASD group were due to comorbid
appear to be merely part of the etiology contributing to the ADHD symptoms, rather than fundamental deficits of
complex cognitive and behavioral profile (Willcutt et al., autism. This notion is supported by the relatively high
2005). In consideration of the heterogeneity, more recent CPRS scores of our ASD subjects, two-thirds of whom
neuropsychological models are emerging suggesting that fell in the at risk range or above on this measure.
there may be additive or interactive effects arising from Whether symptoms of ADHD seen in children with
multiple neural networks contributing to the complexity ASD represent an “ADHD-like” disorder unique to
of the symptom profile of ADHD (e.g., Nigg et al., 2005; autism or represent a distinct co-occurring ADHD may
Sergeant et al., 2003; Sonuga-Barke, 2005). Further, it has have important treatment implications. It has been
been suggested that studies of both ADHD and autism proposed that children with both sets of symptoms are
need to take into account the overlapping symptoms of more impaired functionally and may respond differently
these neurodevelopmental disorders (Verte et al., 2006); to treatment (Arnold et al., 2006; Ghaziuddin et al.,
thus, a more dimensional (symptom profile) rather than a 1992; Goldstein and Schwebach, 2004; Kadesjo and
categorical approach (diagnostic grouping) may be Gillberg, 2001; Posey et al., 2006). Pharmacologic
warranted (Frazier et al., 2007). treatment reports for ADHD symptoms in children with
Thus, the next aim of the study was to examine the ASD are mixed, with older studies suggesting poor
relationship of ADHD symptoms to the neuropsycho- results (Quintana et al., 1995) and more recent studies
logical measures. We hypothesized that ADHD symp- suggesting benefit in a subgroup of children (Stigler
toms would predict task performance on measures of et al., 2004). Thus, the identification of subtype
vigilance and behavioral inhibition across the groups, treatment predictors for medications and for cognitive
and we confirmed our hypothesis. The results suggest and behavioral interventions may be helpful in treatment
that symptoms of ADHD are associated with poor effectiveness and side effect avoidance. Future studies
performance on measures of visual and auditory should determine if there is a subgroup of children who
vigilance and response inhibition as previously reported have ASD with ADHD symptoms that are more likely to
(Corbett and Constantine, 2006). Further, exploratory respond to particular interventions.
analysis excluding the ASD/ADHD children from the
analysis provided support that symptoms of ADHD are 4.1. Limitations
especially associated with deficits in inhibitory control.
Taken together, the results indicate that symptoms of Despite these findings, there are important limita-
ADHD are associated with inattention and inhibition tions to report. It is unclear if the current sample of
deficits across the groups (Verte et al., 2006). The subjects is truly representative of most children with
finding supports the utility of the IVA (Sandford and ASD or ADHD. It is possible that some families
Turner, 2000) as a neuropsychological tool in identify- enrolled the children with ASD in the study knowing
ing symptoms of poor vigilance and inhibitory control that an investigation of ADHD was underway. As such,
within and across neurodevelopmental disorders, we may have enrolled a higher proportion of children
including ADHD and ASD. These results also support with ADHD symptoms within ASD. Even so, recent
the inclusion of a diagnostic parent report measure, such reports showing the notable preponderance of ADHD
as the Conners (Conners, 2001) in an assessment battery symptoms in ASD (Corbett and Constantine, 2006;
220 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

Gadow et al., 2004; Ghaziuddin et al., 1992; Goldberg requires further study. Neuropsychological models
et al., 2005; Goldstein and Schwebach, 2004; Happe are beginning to emerge that could account for the
et al., 2006) enhance the generalizability of our findings. heterogeneity of ASD and ADHD. Across both
In addition, our investigation included a rather small disorders there may be an additive or interactive effect
sample size and multiple data points. In particular, the stemming from dysfunction from various neural net-
exploratory analysis was conducted with very few sub- works that contributes to the heterogeneous profile (e.g.,
jects and uneven groups; thus, it must be interpreted (Castellanos et al., 2006; Nigg et al., 2005; Sergeant
with extreme caution until the findings are replicated in et al., 2003; Sonuga-Barke, 2005). It may also be the
larger samples. For clinical utility, we chose to use case that a more dimensional approach to understanding
standardized instruments rather than conducting a factor neurodevelopmental disorders is warranted (Frazier
analytic study merging domains of functioning; thus, et al., 2007). It is our hope that elucidating the overlap
there is an increased chance of Type 1 error. Our com- and distinctions between ASD and ADHD profiles will
promise was to conduct multivariate analysis followed enable clinicians and researchers to better assess,
up by analysis of variance on individual subtests. characterize and treat these complex disorders.
Further, we were unable to collect a medication naïve
sample for this investigation. In an attempt to control for Acknowledgments
this, stimulant medication was withheld for 24 h, which
is a sufficient washout period (Greenhill, 1998). Some Funding was provided by a NIH Career Develop-
investigations have shown benefit from stimulant ment Award to Blythe Corbett (5 K08NMHO72958).
medication on EF measures e.g., (Kempton et al., Additionally, the authors thank the Perry Family
1999; Vance et al., 2003), while other recent studies Foundation and the Debber Family Foundation for
show modest or a lack of benefit on EF measures their generous support of our research. We express our
following stimulant use (Coghill et al., 2007; Rhodes sincere gratitude to Josh Day and Meridith Brandt for
et al., 2006). Other medications were few (ADHD = 3, the participant recruitment.
ASD = 4) and evenly distributed across the two disorder
groups. Even so, we conducted analyses across the References
clinical groups comparing those with and without
medication and the results remained the same. Despite American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical
these efforts, we acknowledge that there may have been Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. APA, Washington, DC.
American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical
some modest effects on the results attributed to Manual of Mental Disorders. (Fourth edition text revision). APA,
medication use in some of the participants. Finally, Washington, DC.
contradictory findings across studies may be, in part, Arnold, L.E., Aman, M.G., Cook, A.M., Witwer, A.N., Hall, K.L.,
explained by differences in inclusion criteria, age, level Thompson, S., et al., 2006. Atomoxetine for hyperactivity in
of functioning and task demands, which inadvertently autism spectrum disorders: placebo-controlled crossover pilot trial.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
contribute to inconsistencies in the literature. Psychiatry 45 (10), 1196–1205.
Barkley, R.A., 1997. ADHD and the Nature of Self-control. Guilford
4.2. Conclusion Press, New York.
Barkley, R.A., 2003. Issues in the diagnosis of attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder in children. Brain Development 25 (2), 77–83.
Due to the converging evidence reporting a high
Barkley, R.A., Grodzinsky, G., DuPaul, G.J., 1992. Frontal lobe
prevalence of ADHD in ASD, a serious reconsideration functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyper-
is necessary regarding the current diagnostic practice of activity: a review and research report. Journal of Abnormal Child
not providing a diagnosis of ADHD within a pervasive Psychology 20 (2), 163–188.
developmental disorder when appropriate (APA, 2000). Baron-Cohen, S., Belmonte, M.K., 2005. Autism: a window onto the
The issues and challenges of diagnosing ADHD, development of the social and the analytic brain. Annual Review of
Neuroscience 28, 109–126.
especially within special populations, must be carefully Carper, R.A., Courchesne, E., 2000. Inverse correlation between
considered (Barkley, 2003). The presence of symptoms frontal lobe and cerebellum sizes in children with autism. Brain
from more than one neurodevelopmental disorder 123 (Pt 4), 836–844.
likely leads to exponential risks and greater treatment Carper, R.A., Courchesne, E., 2005. Localized enlargement of the
challenges. Additional research is needed to help frontal cortex in early autism. Biological Psychiatry 57 (2),
126–133.
delineate patterns of performance within and between Castellanos, F.X., Sonuga-Barke, E.J., Milham, M.P., Tannock, R., 2006.
ADHD and ASD. This comorbid profile of ADHD in Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction.
ASD may represent a distinct phenotype in autism that Trends in Cognitive Science 10 (3), 117–123.
B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222 221

Coghill, D.R., Rhodes, S.M., Matthews, K., 2007. The neuropsycho- Happe, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., 2006. Executive
logical effects of chronic methylphenidate on drug-naive boys with function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 62 hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and
(9), 954–962. ages. Brain and Cognition 61 (1), 25–39.
Conners, K.C., 2001. Conners' Rating Scales Revised Manual. MHS, Hill, E.L., 2004. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive
Tonawanda, New York. Science 8 (1), 26–32.
Corbett, B.A., Constantine, L.J., 2006. Autism and attention deficit Hughes, C., Russell, J., Robbins, T.W., 1994. Evidence for executive
hyperactivity disorder: assessing attention and response control dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 32, 477–492.
with the integrated visual and auditory continuous performance Kadesjo, B., Gillberg, C., 2001. The comorbidity of ADHD in the general
test. Child Neuropsychology 12 (4–5), 335–348. population of Swedish school-age children. Journal of Child
Courchesne, E., Karns, C.M., Davis, H.R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R.A., Psychology and 42 (4), 487–492.
Tigue, Z.D., Chisum, H.J., Moses, P., Pierce, K., Lord, C., Lincoln, Kates, W.R., Frederikse, M., Mostofsky, S.H., Folley, B.S., Cooper, K.,
A.J., Pizzo, S., Schreibman, L., Haas, R.H., Akshoomoff, N.A., Mazur-Hopkins, P., et al., 2002. MRI parcellation of the frontal lobe
Courchesne, R.Y., 2001. Unusual brain growth patterns in early in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or Tourette
life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology 57 syndrome. Psychiatry Resarch: Neuroimaging 116 (1–2), 63–81.
(2), 245–254. Kempton, S., Vance, A., Maruff, P., Luk, E., Costin, J., Pantelis, C., 1999.
Damasio, A.R., Maurer, R.G., 1978. A neurological model for Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:
childhood autism. Archives of Neurology 35 (12), 777–786. stimulant medication and better executive function performance in
Dellis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H., 2001. Delis-Kaplan Executive children. Psychological Medicine 29 (3), 527–538.
Function System. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. Landa, R.J., Goldberg, M.C., 2005. Language, social, and executive
Denckla, M.B., 1996. Biological correlates of learning and attention: functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance.
what is relevant to learning disability and attention-deficit Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35 (5), 557–573.
hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Lezak, M., 1995. Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd ed. Oxford
Pediatrics 17 (2), 114–119. University Press, New York.
Doyle, A.E., Biederman, J., Seidman, L.J., Weber, W., Faraone, S.V., 2000. Llorente, A.M., Williams, J., Satz, P., D'Elia, L., 1998. Children's Color
Diagnostic efficiency of neuropsychological test scores for discrimi- Trails Test (CCTT). Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles.
nating boys with and without attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., 1994. Autism Diagnostic Interview-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68 (3), 477–488. Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of
Faraone, S.V., Biederman, J., 1998. Neurobiology of attention-deficit individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal
hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 44 (10), 951–958. of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24 (5), 659–685.
Faraone, S.V., Perlis, R.H., Doyle, A.E., Smoller, J.W., Goralnick, J.J., Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., 1999. Autism Diagnostic
Holmgren, M.A., et al., 2005. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/ Observation Schedule. Western Psychological Services, Los
hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 57 (11), 1313–1323. Angeles, CA.
Fisher, S.E., Francks, C., McCracken, J.T., McGough, J.J., Marlow, A.J., Lou, H.C., Henriksen, L., Bruhn, P., 1984. Focal cerebral hypoperfusion
MacPhie, I.L., et al., 2002. A genomewide scan for loci involved in in children with dysphasia and/or attention deficit disorder. Archives
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Human of Neurology 41 (8), 825–829.
Genetics 70 (5), 1183–1196. McAlonan, G.M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Suckling, J., Lam, G.Y., Tai,
Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E.A., Naugle, R.I., 2007. The latent K.S., et al., 2005. Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MRI
structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic- study of volumetric differences and intercorrelations in autism.
referred sample. Neuropsychology 21 (1), 45–64. Brain 128 (Pt 2), 268–276.
Gadow, K.D., DeVincent, C.J., Pomeroy, J., Azizian, A., 2004. Mostofsky, S.H., Cooper, K.L., Kates, W.R., Denckla, M.B., Kaufmann,
Psychiatric symptoms in preschool children with PDD and clinic W.E., 2002. Smaller prefrontal and premotor volumes in boys with
and comparison samples. Journal of Autism and Developmental attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 52 (8),
Disorders 34 (4), 379–393. 785–794.
Geurts, H.M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Sergeant, J.A., Muller, R.A., Pierce, K., Ambrose, J.B., Allen, G., Courchesne, E., 2001.
2004. How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention Atypical patterns of cerebral motor activation in autism: a functional
deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child magnetic resonance study. Biological Psychiatry 49 (8), 665–676.
Psychology and 45 (4), 836–854. Norusis, M.J., 1993. SPSS for Windows. SPSS, Inc, Chicago, IL.
Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., Alessi, N., 1992. ADHD and PDD. Journal of the Nigg, J.T., Willcutt, E.G., Doyle, A.E., Sonuga-Barke, E.J., 2005.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31 (3), 567. Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: do
Goldberg, M.C., Mostofsky, S.H., Cutting, L.E., Mahone, E.M., Astor, we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological
B.C., Denckla, M.B., et al., 2005. Subtle executive impairment in Psychiatry 57 (11), 1224–1230.
children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism Ogdie, M.N., Macphie, I.L., Minassian, S.L., Yang, M., Fisher, S.E.,
and Developmental Disorders 35 (3), 279–293. Francks, C., et al., 2003. A genomewide scan for attention-deficit/
Goldstein, S., Schwebach, A.J., 2004. The comorbidity of pervasive hyperactivity disorder in an extended sample: suggestive linkage on
developmental disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: 17p11. American Journal of Human Genetics 72 (5), 1268–1279.
results of a retrospective chart review. Journal of Autism and Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D.L., 1997. Inhibitory function in nonretarded
Developmental Disorders 34 (3), 329–339. children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Greenhill, L.L., 1998. Childhood attention deficit hyperactivity Disorders 27 (1), 59–77.
disorder: pharmacological treatments. In: Gorman, J. (Ed.), A Ozonoff, S., Jensen, J., 1999. Brief report: specific executive function
Guide to Treatments That Work. Oxford University Press, New profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism
York, pp. 42–64. and Developmental Disorders 29 (2), 171–177.
222 B.A. Corbett et al. / Psychiatry Research 166 (2009) 210–222

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B.F., Rogers, S.J., 1991. Executive function tion, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA
deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to Study. Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent
theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32 (7), Mental Disorders Study. Journal of the American Academy of
1081–1105. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 35 (7), 865–877.
Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R.M., Klin, A., Smalley, S.L., Loo, S.K., Yang, M.H., Cantor, R.M., 2005. Toward
McMahon, W.M., Minshew, N., Munson, J.A., Pennington, B.F., localizing genes underlying cerebral asymmetry and mental health.
Rogers, S.J., Spence, M.A., Tager-Flusberg, H., Volkmar, F.R., American Journal of Medical Genetics. B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics
Wrathall, D., 2004. Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychologi- 135 (1), 79–84.
cal Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe Smith, A.B., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Toone, B., Rubia, K., 2006. Task-
function in people with autistic disorder: evidence from the specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and temporoparietal brain
Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. Journal regions during motor inhibition and task switching in medication-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34 (2), 139–150. naive children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity
Pennington, B.F., Ozonoff, S., 1996. Executive functions and develop- disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 163 (6), 1044–1051.
mental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- Sonuga-Barke, E.J., 2005. Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
try 37 (1), 51–87. disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental
Posey, D.J., Wiegand, R.E., Wilkerson, J., Maynard, M., Stigler, K.A., pathways. Biological Psychiatry 57 (11), 1231–1238.
McDougle, C.J., 2006. Open-label atomoxetine for attention- Sowell, E.R., Thompson, P.M., Welcome, S.E., Henkenius, A.L.,
deficit/ hyperactivity disorder symptoms associated with high- Toga, A.W., Peterson, B.S., 2003. Cortical abnormalities in
functioning pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity
and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 16 (5), 599–610. disorder. Lancet 362 (9397), 1699–1707.
Quintana, H., Birmaher, B., Stedge, D., Lennon, S., Freed, J., Bridge, Stigler, K.A., Desmond, L.A., Posey, D.J., Wiegand, R.E., McDougle,
J., et al., 1995. Use of methylphenidate in the treatment of children C.J., 2004. A naturalistic retrospective analysis of psychostimu-
with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental lants in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child and
Disorders 25 (3), 283–294. Adolescent Psychopharmacology 14 (1), 49–56.
Reiersen, A.M., Constantino, J.N., Volk, H.E., Todd, R.D., 2007. Stuss, D.T., Benson, D.F., 1984. Neuropsychological studies of the
Autistic traits in a population-based ADHD twin sample. Journal frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin 95 (1), 3–28.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48 (5), 464–472. Vance, A.L., Maruff, P., Barnett, R., 2003. Attention deficit
Rhodes, S.M., Coghill, D.R., Matthews, K., 2005. Neuropsychological hyperactivity disorder, combined type: better executive function
functioning in stimulant-naive boys with hyperkinetic disorder. performance with longer-term psychostimulant medication. Aus-
Psychological Medicine 35 (8), 1109–1120. tralian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 37 (5), 570–576.
Rhodes, S.M., Coghill, D.R., Matthews, K., 2006. Acute neuropsy- Verte, S., Geurts, H.M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J.A., 2006.
chological effects of methylphenidate in stimulant drug-naive boys The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response
with ADHD II-broader executive and non-executive domains. variability in child psychopathology. Journal of Neuroscience
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47 (11), 1184–1194. Methods 151 (1), 5–14.
Russell, J., 1997. Autism as an Executive Disorder. Oxford University Wechsler, D., 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.
Press, New York. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.
Sandford, J.A., Turner, A., 2000. Integrated Visual and Auditory Welsh, M.C., Pennington, B.F., 1988. Assessing frontal lobe
Continuous Performance Test Manual. Brain Train, Richmond, VA. functioning in children: view from developmental psychology.
Sergeant, J.A., Geurts, H., Oosterlaan, J., 2002. How specific is a Developmental Neuropsychology 4, 199–230.
deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity Willcutt, E.G., Doyle, A.E., Nigg, J.T., Faraone, S.V., Pennington, B.F.,
disorder? Behavioral Brain Research 130 (1–2), 3–28. 2005. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/
Sergeant, J.A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry
2003. The top and the bottom of ADHD: a neuropsychological 57 (11), 1336–1346.
perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review 27 (7), Zafeiriou, D.I., Ververi, A., Vargiami, E., 2007. Childhood autism and
583–592. associated comorbidities. Brain Development 29, 257–272.
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Dulcan, M.K., Davies, M., Piacentini, J., Zang, Y.F., Jin, Z., Weng, X.C., Zhang, L., Zeng, Y.W., Yang, L., et al.,
Schwab-Stone, M.E., Lahey, B.B., Bourdon, K., Jensen, P.S., Bird, 2005. Functional MRI in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:
H.R., Canino, G., Regier, D.A., 1996. The NIMH Diagnostic evidence for hypofrontality. Brain Development 27 (8), 544–550.
Interview Schedule for Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): descrip-

You might also like