Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Topical Article

Teaching of Psychology
2015, Vol. 42(1) 26-33
The Effect of Immersion Scheduling on ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Academic Performance and Students’ DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562675
top.sagepub.com
Ratings of Instructors

Aaron S. Richmond1, Bridget C. Murphy1, Layton S. Curl1,


and Kristin A. Broussard1

Abstract
During the past decades, little research has investigated the effects of immersion scheduling on the psychology classroom.
Therefore, we sought to compare academic performance of students in 2-week immersion psychology courses to that of
students in traditional 16-week courses. In Study 1, students who received instruction in a 2-week immersion course signifi-
cantly outperformed their cohorts in a traditional 16-week course. In order to address potential limitations in the first study, in
Study 2, we controlled for individual differences variables (e.g., cumulative grade point average), and results indicated significantly
higher academic performance for students in the 2-week immersion course. In both studies, students in the immersion courses
consistently evaluated the courses and their instructors significantly higher than those students in the 16-week courses. In light
of our results and in contrast to critics, immersion courses may be useful and effective when teaching psychology.

Keywords
immersion learning, student learning, course evaluations

Between 1995 and 1999, a number of researchers and national Currently, there have been only two studies that have spe-
committees set to work on efforts to address scheduling and cifically investigated the effects of the immersion scheduling
retention issues in secondary as well as higher education in the psychology classroom. Ray and Kirkpatrick (1983)
(Boyer Commission on Educating Undergraduates in the investigated the effects on knowledge and anxiety in two
Research University, 1998; Kojaku, Nunez, & Malizio, 1998; course lengths (15 vs. 3 weeks) for a human sexuality course;
Muraskin, 1998; U.S. Department of Education, 2001). As a interestingly, they found no academic advantage between the
result, there was a large push for immersion scheduling in both two course lengths. In contrast, Anastasi (2007) found across
secondary and higher education (Lawerence & McPherson, different psychology courses (e.g., memory, research meth-
2000). The immersion course is defined as an educational ods, and effective thinking), students in 2- to 5-week immer-
schedule, in which students experience one course at a time sion courses sometimes had higher academic performance
by engaging in learning activities within extended blocks of compared to students in traditional 16-week courses. Specifi-
time (Petrowsky, 1996; van Scyoc & Gleason, 1993). However, cally, Anastasi found that the academic performance in the
with this push came a backlash by many others who claimed summer immersion sections only existed in certain courses
that immersive courses are less effective than traditional for- and certain assignments (e.g., exam scores in a critical think-
mats (e.g., Daniel, 2000; Scott, 2003). ing course and research methods course). Unfortunately, this
Although immersion scheduling has a history of selective use research was aggregated across different immersion course
in higher education and is becoming increasingly popular in formats (e.g., 2, 3, and 5 weeks) and is difficult to assess
some universities (Davies, 2006), a review of the literature which type of immersion course improved academic perfor-
revealed a startling absence of rigorous empirical studies addres- mance. Consequently, we sought to investigate the effect
sing academic achievement outcomes, particularly in psychol- immersion scheduling had across courses, across levels of
ogy. As studies have demonstrated that immersion scheduling
may increase college retention (e.g., Soldner, Lee, & Duby,
1
2000), academic self-concept (e.g., Richmond & Krank, 2007), Metropolitan State University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
and critical thinking (Burton & Nesbit, 2008; Jonas, Weimer,
Corresponding Author:
& Herzer, 2004; Mims, 1983; Richmond & Krank, 2007), we Aaron S. Richmond, Department of Psychology, Campus Box 54, PO Box
thought it is important to investigate the effects immersion sche- 173362, Denver, CO 80217, USA.
duling has on the teaching and learning of psychology. Email: arichmo3@msudenver.edu

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


Richmond et al. 27

courses (e.g., first year to senior), and within only one type of and 94 students in traditional 16-week courses. Participants
immersion format (i.e., 2-week long course with no other were selected from six sections of three different sophomore-
course at the same time). and junior-level psychology courses (cognitive development
Beyond student academic performance, student ratings of and learning, human development, and social psychology).
instructors (SRIs) are widely used to determine instructor and Three instructors each taught one specific course in the
course effectiveness (Heckert, Latier, Ringwald, & Silvey, 2-week immersion format and the same course in the tradi-
2006) and can be influenced in part by student engagement tional 16-week format. The sample consisted of 69.7% females.
(Handlesman, Briggs, Sullivan, & Towler, 2005), professor– Although not directly measured, psychology students in our
student rapport (Berglund et al., 2013; Ryan, Wilson, & Pugh, department typically consist of 1.2% American Indian, 2.7%
2011), course size (Heckert et al., 2006), and course objectives Asian or Pacific Islander, 5.3% African American, 13.2% His-
(Gump, 2004). In the immersion literature, Anastasi (2007) is the panic, 70.8% White, and 6.1% declaring other.
only study to investigate the impact immersion scheduling may Both course formats were virtually the same, with the
have on one aspect of SRIs. Anastasi found that students per- exception of distribution of course instructional time. Both
ceived the rigor of both immersion and traditional course formats course formats required a total of 45 hr of instructional time.
as the same. Unfortunately, Anastasi’s measure of SRIs was a sin- The traditional 16-week course format was delivered twice a
gle measure, and the reliability and validity were not reported. week, for 1 hr and 15 min per meeting for a total of 45 instruc-
Because SRIs may provide an assessment of the intended vari- tional hours. Traditional 16-week courses were in the fall or
ables (i.e., course and instructor effectiveness) as well as identify spring traditional semester only. The 2-week immersion
possible mediating and moderating variables such as student courses met 5 days per week for 4.5 hr per day, for a total of
engagement, which is important to study within the context of 45 instructional hours, and occurred between the spring and fall
comparing immersion and traditional scheduling formats, we semesters during the university’s winter break. Students were
investigated the effects of immersion scheduling on SRIs. only allowed to take one course over this time period. Conse-
Given the limited research in this area, in two separate stud- quently, both course formats were not offered simultaneously.
ies, we sought to improve this area of research in several ways. Additionally, all students select to enroll in either format based
First, in the few studies in psychology (e.g., Anastasi, 2007; Ray on their own personal needs and are not required to take an
& Kirkpatrick, 1983), there seems to be conflicting evidence of immersion course. In both course formats, students often take
the efficacy of immersion scheduling, and we sought to clarify the course for a required major course, minor course, and/or
these inconsistencies by initially comparing the two schedule elective. Because this is a matched-pair design (i.e., each
formats on academic performance (Study 1) and then replicating instructor taught both in the immersion and 16-week format),
this study to check for consistent results (Study 2). Second, in each instructor used the same teaching method, covered the
past studies (Anastasi, 2007; Burton & Nesbit, 2008; Richmond same material, and used the same assessments for both the
& Krank, 2007), 2- to 5-week and 3-week immersion courses 2-week immersion and traditional 16-week courses. For
were combined. In Studies 1 and 2, we were interested to see example, both course formats were required to do quizzes,
whether academic gains could be made in only a 2-week immer- exams, and writing assignments (e.g., term papers and theory
sion course. Third, Anastasi reported no difference in perceived synthesis papers). Additionally, students in both types of
course rigor but did not report additional ratings from SRIs, so courses engaged in collaborative writing and presentation
for both Studies 1 and 2, we sought to test whether course format assignments (e.g., a group theory paper and presentation of
differentially changed SRIs. Finally, in Study 2, we choose to how cognitive development applies to K–12 classroom).
replicate the findings of Study 1 on the effects immersion sche-
duling had on academic performance.
Based on our review of literature, for both Studies 1 and 2, we
Materials and Measures
were interested in assessing these overarching research ques- To assess our research question, each instructor collected
tions: (1) Are there academic performance benefits to teaching course quizzes and test scores for each of their courses for
in a 2-week immersion course over that of teaching in a tradi- research purposes after the course ended. Questions were
tional 16-week course? (2) Are instructors viewed more or less multiple-choice, true/false, or fill in the blank. For each instruc-
favorably (as measured by SRIs) in a 2-week immersion course tor, the test items for the 2-week immersion and traditional 16-
when compared to a traditional 16-week course? week courses were identical. Averaged over the six courses,
there were 150 questions (range ¼ 55–207). The proportion for
Study 1 total correct answers of test and quiz questions was computed
for each participant in each course, resulting in one measure
of student academic performance for the course. To assess
Method SRIs, we used the formal policy and procedures outlined by our
university. Our university’s measure for SRIs loosely follows
Participants the guidelines set forth by Seldin (2006). These were four ques-
There were 155 undergraduate psychology students who parti- tions using a 1 (very poor) to 6 (excellent) response range. The
cipated in this study; 61 students in 2-week immersion courses exact wording of all four questions appears in Table 1.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


28 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Inferential Statistics of Course increased participation, which may lead to higher academic
Evaluations for Study 1. performance. However, we contend that students academically
Two-Week 16-Week
perform almost 10% higher because of reduced interference
Immersion Traditional effects (see Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; Tulving & Psotka,
Coursesa Coursesa 1971). Interference occurs either prospectively or retrospec-
Questions Mean (SD) Mean (SD) t (d) p tively and occurs when new learning interferes with the recall
of old learning or when old learning interferes with encoding of
The course as a whole 5.61 (0.55) 4.93 (0.89) 5.61 (0.92) <.001
new learning. In the context of Study 1, we suggest that in a
was:
The course content 5.44 (0.67) 5.02 (0.82) 3.23 (0.56) .03 normal 16-week semester, often full-time students take four
was: to five courses, which creates greater cognitive load, whereas
The instructor’s 5.82 (0.44) 5.19 (0.90) 4.92 (0.88) .011 in our 2-week immersion course, they typically only take one
contribution to the course. Thus, the amount of cognitive load is reduced, and
course was: there is far less potential for interference of any type.
The instructor’s 5.84 (.045) 5.19 (0.83) 5.49 (0.98) .002 Second, we assessed whether instructors were viewed more
effectiveness in
or less favorably (as measured by SRIs) in a two-week immer-
teaching the subject
matter was: sion course when compared to a traditional 16-week course.
For all four SRI questions (Table 1), students consistently per-
Note. aThere were different sample sizes for both the immersion courses (N ¼ ceived instructors and the course in the 2-week immersion
59) and the 16-week courses (N ¼ 85) on the four student evaluation ques-
course as more positive than the same instructor in the tradi-
tions. The Likert-type scale was anchored on 1 ¼ very poor to 6 ¼ excellent.
tional 16-week course. It is our opinion that students may rate
instructors who teach the 2-week immersion courses higher
Procedures. This study was a post facto assessment of six because the immediate longer exposure (5–6 hr per day) over
psychology courses. As stated previously, each instructor a short period of time (2 weeks) allows for a concentrated
taught the same course in the 2-week immersion and traditional interaction between students and instructors that may increase
16-week course. After each semester ended, each instructor student–teacher rapport.
collected quiz and exam items and computed the percentage Even though the difference between the two course formats
of correct overall for each student. had a large effect size, Study 1 was not without limitations that
needed to be addressed, as they may restrict the generalization
of results. First, selection effects could be at play in the Study 1
Results sample (see Beins & McCarthy, 2012; Campbell & Stanley,
To examine student performance, we conducted an indepen- 1963; Nesselroade & Thompson, 1995). For example, course
dent t-test between the immersion and 16-week courses. size, individual instructor differences, demographic differ-
Results indicate that students in the 2-week immersion ences, student collegiate grade point average (GPA), and
courses (M ¼ 84.33%, SD ¼ 9.53%) performed significantly higher education experience (e.g., total credits completed at
higher than students in the traditional 16-week courses this level) may impact student learning. Second, in light of the
(M ¼ 75.58%, SD ¼ 10.26%), t(153) ¼ 5.33, p <. 001, d ¼ inconsistent findings in previous studies (e.g., Anastasi, 2007;
.88. For SRIs, four independent t-tests were conducted. On all Ray & Kirkpatrick, 1983), even though the results of Study 1
four questions, students in the immersion courses rated their begin to establish a pattern indicating immersion courses may
courses and instructors significantly higher than the tradi- have positive effects on academic performance, we sought to
tional courses (see Table 1 for descriptive data). As indicated replicate Study 1’s findings with an even larger sample size
in Table 1, all four t-tests produced large effect sizes. across several different types of psychology courses at several
levels of the psychology curriculum while controlling for indi-
vidual differences. In addition to improving Study 1 based on
Brief Discussion the limitations, we decided to add a more in-depth measure
We sought to answer two research questions. First, we were of SRIs with a particular focus on how students compared the
interested in investigating whether there were academic perfor- course to other courses they have completed. For example, we
mance benefits to teaching in a 2-week immersion course over assessed their perceived effort, intellectual challenge, grade
that of teaching in a traditional 16-week course. The results expectations, and course involvement when compared to other
indicate that there was significantly higher student academic courses and, as such, we were able to gain a deeper insight into
performance in the immersion courses. As Anastasi (2007) sug- the perceptions of these two course formats.
gests, there may be several explanations for findings such as Based on strengthening Study 1, we sought to answer the
ours, in that students in immersion courses may be at an unjust following research questions: (1) After controlling for course
advantage. Specifically, it may be argued that our results are a size, overall students’ collegiate GPA, and education experi-
product of course size (i.e., immersion courses were smaller ence (e.g., total credits completed), is there a difference in stu-
than the traditional courses). Becker, Sommer, Bee, and Oxley dents’ academic performance between 2-week immersion and
(1973) suggested that when courses are smaller, there is traditional 16-week course?

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


Richmond et al. 29

Table 2. Demographic and Descriptive Data for Study 2. for descriptive data pertaining to the student participants and
courses.
Two-Week 16-Week
Immersion Traditional
Courses Courses Procedure and Measures
Variable (N ¼ 177) (N ¼ 780)
This study was a post facto program assessment of our depart-
Gender ment’s psychology courses taught in both the immersion
Female 138 (78.00%) 540 (69.20%) (2 weeks) and traditional (16 weeks) format. We obtained
Male 39 (22.00%) 240 (30.80%) anonymous data on the variables described subsequently from
Ethnicity
our university’s Office of Institutional Research (OIR). There
African American 8 (4.50%) 23 (2.90%)
American Indian 3 (1.70%) 13 (1.70%) were four main variables (see Table 2 for descriptive demo-
Asian 7 (4.00%) 33 (4.20%) graphic data). First, we requested and received standard
Caucasian 121 (68.40%) 550 (70.50%) demographic data (e.g., gender, ethnicity, and age) from OIR.
Hispanic 23 (130%) 97 (12.40%) Second, to assess academic performance, we received the
International Student 1 (0.60%) 4 (0.50%) final course grades for each student aggregated by format of
Other 14 (7.90%) 60 (7.70%) course length (i.e., 2 vs. 16 weeks). Third, we collected indi-
Course
vidual differences variables to use as covariate measures to
Child psychology 17 (9.6%) 45 (5.9%)
Cognitive development 15 (8.5%) 51 (6.5%) control for possible confounds. Specifically, based on work
and learning by Olani (2009), we collected data on university cumulative
Introductory psychology 5 (2.8%) 52 (6.7%) GPA as a measure of prior academic performance and by proxy
Psychology of human 70 (39.5%) 166 (21.3%) a measure of performance goal motivation. Additionally, Rau
development and Durand (2000) suggest that when students progress in their
Social psychology 66 (37.3%) 461 (59.1%) higher education, they tend to select courses either personally or
Age of student (years) M ¼ 27.80 M ¼ 25.40
academically relevant to them, which is positively related to
SD ¼ 7.00 SD ¼ 6.66
Cumulative GPA M ¼ 3.11, M ¼ 2.79, their GPA. We attempted to control for this selection bias by col-
SD ¼ 0.61 SD ¼ 0.79 lecting data on the total credits that the students completed at our
Academic performance M ¼ 3.26, M ¼ 2.46, institution and using it as a covariate. As class size has been sug-
SD ¼ 0.82 SD ¼ 1.14 gested as a mediator of student learning (Gilmore, Swerdik, &
Total credits completed M ¼ 78.43, M ¼ 52.90, Beehr, 1980), we collected data on the number of students
SD ¼ 30.95 SD ¼ 30.41 enrolled in each course.
Course size M ¼ 20.16, M ¼ 30.55,
Finally, to assess course evaluations and teacher effective-
SD ¼ 6.13 SD ¼ 7.76
ness, we used the university’s SRI form. This form is divided
Note. GPA ¼ grade point average. into two sections. The first section consisted of the same ques-
tions used in Study 1 (see Table 3), which is loosely based on
Selvin (2006). The second section of the form used a 7-point
(2) Is there a difference in students’ perception of teacher Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 ¼ much lower to 7 ¼ much
effectiveness, student involvement in the course, and students’ higher to assess students’ perception of expected course
course ratings between 2-week immersion and traditional grade, the amount of effort put into the course, the amount
16-week courses? of effort to succeed in the course, and the student’s involve-
ment in the course (e.g., doing assignments and attending
class) when compared to other courses. Refer to Table 3 for
Study 2 a complete list of the questions and descriptive data.
Method
Participants Results
From the fall semester of 2006 to the spring semester of To examine academic performance differences between the
2010, 957 undergraduate student participants enrolled in 12 two course types, while controlling for the effect of cumula-
sections of five different lower and upper division courses tive GPA, course size, and total credits completed at the uni-
of psychology. Six instructors each taught one course in the versity, we first ran three independent samples t-tests1 on
2-week immersion format and the same course in the 16- each of the three covariates. Results indicate significant dif-
week traditional format. Both course formats required the ferences for all three covariates (i.e., course size, cumulative
same amount of instructional time (45 hr) and covered the GPA, and total credits completed); specifically, students in
same material. As was the case in Study 1, both course for- the 2-week immersion courses had significantly higher
mats were identical in course structure (e.g., writing assign- academic performance over that of traditional 16-week
ments, tests, quizzes, collaborative projects, instructional course students, t(927) ¼ 4.86 p < .001, d ¼ 0.44; signifi-
time, etc.) with the exception of course length. See Table 2 cantly smaller course size than traditional 16-week courses,

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


30 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)

Table 3. Descriptive Data for SRIs for Study 2.

2-Week Immersion 16-Week Traditional


Courses Courses
M (SD) M (SD)

Instructor effectiveness and course ratings questions


a
The course as a whole was: 5.46 (0.69) 5.19 (0.83)
a
The course content was: 5.35 (0.69) 5.17 (0.78)
a
The instructor’s contribution to the course was: 5.67 (0.61) 5.44 (0.78)
a
The instructor’s effectiveness in teaching the subject matter was: 5.62 (0.66) 5.35 (0.81)
Comparative Course Questions
b
Compared to other courses you have taken, do you expect your grade in this course to be 5.34 (1.22) 4.93 (1.06)
(higher/lower):
b
Compared to other courses you have taken, the intellectual challenge presented was (higher/ 5.62 (0.98) 5.41 (0.94)
lower):
b
Compared to other courses you have taken, the amount of effort you put into this course was 5.68 (1.17) 5.13 (1.08)
(higher/lower):
b
Compared to other courses you have taken, your involvement in this course (doing 5.96 (1.18) 5.39 (1.08)
assignments, attending class, etc.) was (higher/lower):
Note. SRIs ¼ student ratings of instructors. aThere were different sample sizes for both the immersion courses (N ¼109) and the 16-week courses (N ¼478) on
the four student evaluation questions. bThere were different sample sizes for both the immersion courses (N ¼ 74) and the 16-week courses (N ¼ 204) on the
four-student evaluation questions. The Likert-type scale was anchored on 1 ¼ very poor to 6 ¼ excellent.

t(927) ¼ 16.46, p < .001, d ¼ 1.49; and significantly more total the courses higher than students in the traditional 16-week
credits completed than traditional 16-week course students, course on ratings of the course as a whole, F(1, 585) ¼
t(927) ¼ 9.76, p <.001, d ¼ 0.83. 10.10, p ¼ .002, Zp2 ¼ .02, ratings of course content F(1,
To control for individual differences between students in 585) ¼ 4.85, p ¼ .028, Zp2 ¼ .01, ratings of the instructor’s
the 2-week immersion course and the traditional 16-week contribution of the course, F(1, 585) ¼ 8.00, p¼ .005, Zp2
course, we conducted an analysis of covariance on student ¼ .01, and ratings of instructor effectiveness in teaching the
academic performance, and results indicated a significant subject matter, F(1, 585) ¼ 10.75, p ¼ .001, Zp2 ¼ .02. See
main effect of the between-factor course type, F(1, 898) ¼ Table 3 for descriptive data.
41.53 p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .044, after covariates were considered. To examine whether scheduling type had an effect on
Even after adjusting for the covariates, students in the 2-week evaluations of the course in comparison to other courses the
immersion courses had significantly higher academic per- student had taken, a one-way MANOVA was conducted.
formance than students in the traditional 16-week courses, Specifically, questions on the student rating form included
F(4, 898) ¼ 299.76, p <. 001, Zp2 ¼ .57. The covariate of the amount of effort put into the course relative to other
cumulative GPA and total credits completed significantly courses; do you expect your grade to be (higher than/lower
influenced the dependent variable of academic performance, than other courses); the intellectual challenge presented was
F(1, 898) ¼ 903.79 and 7.87, p <. 001, Zp2 ¼ .50 and .01, (higher/lower than other courses); and the effort to succeed in
respectively. However, the covariate of course size did not the course was (higher/lower than other courses). A one-way
influence academic performance, F(1, 898) ¼ .34, p >.05, MANOVA was conducted to examine the effect of 2-week
Zp2 < .001. immersion versus traditional 16-week courses on students’
Two one-way multivariate analyses of variance (MANO- course evaluations for a sample of 2782 students. The results
VAs) were conducted to examine the effect of scheduling type of the MANOVA show an overall significant difference
on students’ perceptions of (1) teacher effectiveness and between scheduling type in instructor evaluations, Wilks’
course content; and (2) students’ perceptions of effort and l ¼ F(5, 272) ¼ 3.79, p ¼ .002. The results of the ANOVA
involvement in the course, for a sample of 587 students. For indicate that students in the 2-week immersion courses
the first MANOVA, the dependent variables included the stu- expected higher grades, F(1, 276) ¼ 7.58, p ¼.006, Zp2 ¼
dents’ rating of the course as a whole, the course content, the .03, perceived a greater amount of effort put into the course
instructor’s contribution to the course, and the instructor’s compared to other courses, F(1, 276) ¼ 13.96, p < .001, Zp2 ¼
effectiveness in teaching the subject matter. This gave us a .05, and the effort required to succeed in the course was
total of four dependent variables. The results of the MAN- greater compared to other courses, F(1, 276) ¼ 13.18, p ¼
OVA show an overall significant difference between 2-week .012, Zp2 ¼ .02, over that of students in traditional 16-week
immersion courses and traditional 16-week courses in instruc- course. However, there was not a significant difference between
tor evaluations, Wilks’ l ¼ F(4, 582) ¼ 3.24, p ¼ .012. Spe- the two course formats in the intellectual challenge of the
cifically, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that course, F(1, 276) ¼ 2.39, p ¼ .10, Zp2 ¼ .01. See Table 3 for
students in the 2-week immersion courses significantly rated descriptive data.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


Richmond et al. 31

Brief Discussion overall course ratings. Furthermore, since students are only
enrolled in one course during immersion scheduling, most
To assess our research question centered on the effects immer-
of their time and effort is devoted to this single course, which
sion scheduling has on academic performance, the data suggest
may make their involvement in the course more salient when
that after controlling for selection effects (i.e., Nesselroade &
completing the student evaluations of course and instructor.
Thompson, 1995) of cumulative GPA and total credits com-
pleted, students in immersion courses can perform just as well
or better than students in standard length courses. Again, coun- General Discussion and Conclusions
ter to Ray and Kirkpatrick (1983) and in support of Anastasi
Throughout both studies, we consistently found students in the
(2007), we found consistent results across various psychology
immersion format significantly academically outperformed their
courses supporting the use of immersion courses. As in Study 1,
16-week cohort who received instruction from that same
students may perform better in immersion courses primarily
instructor with the same course content. As we have argued,
because of the lack of interference effects as previously dis-
the immersion instructional format may be beneficial because
cussed (see Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; Tulving & Psotka,
there are fewer competing cognitive demands, and we suggest
1971). That is, in a normal semester, often full-time students
that, when possible, psychology departments may want to
take four to five courses, whereas in our 2-week immersion
consider incorporating this instructional format more often.
course, they can take only one course. Thus, there is far less
We also argue that when students academically perform
potential for interference and competing cognitive demand.
higher in the immersion format, it may be due to developing
SRIs were utilized as a measure of course and instructor
a quicker and more established rapport with their instructors,
effectiveness per the suggestion of Heckert, Latier, Ringwald,
which has demonstrated to increase academic performance
and Silvey (2006). A large body of research has shown several
(Wilson et al., 2010). Tentatively, based on our findings, psy-
factors may mediate or moderate the ratings students give of
chology departments may consider using more immersion
teachers and courses, such as engagement, student–teacher
courses to increase student academic performance.
rapport, course size, and course objectives (Berglund et al.,
Additionally, throughout both studies, we consistently
2013; Gump, 2004; Handlesman et al., 2005; Heckert et al.,
observed that students in the 2-week immersion course pro-
2006). Inconsistent with Anastasi’s (2007) finding of no dif-
duced higher SRIs over that of the traditional 16-week
ference in 1 SRI item (i.e., course rigor) between the two
course. Because of course length and time spent per day
course types, we found that students in the 2-week immersion
in class—everyday—we contend that students and instruc-
courses rated the course and instructor more highly than stu-
tors develop a heightened rapport. And as student–instructor
dents in the traditional 16-week courses even with the same
rapport is purported as significantly contributing to student
instructor. Specifically, students in the 2-week immersion
evaluations of instructor and course, the immersion format,
courses rated their instructors as more effective and as contri-
when applicable, may be more beneficial to developing stu-
buting more to the course than students in traditional 16-week
dent–instructor rapport.
courses. These students also rated the course as better, and
There are several instructional implications to our findings.
in comparison to other courses. In the 2-week immersion
First, our results counterclaims that immersion courses provide
courses, students expected a higher grade, put in more effort
only ‘‘superficial’’ coverage of material and cannot provide the
to succeed, and felt that more of the time per week they spent
same content as traditional courses due to the reduced time
on the course was valuable to their education. However, stu-
period in which the course takes place (Wlodkowski, 2003).
dents did indicate that the 2-week immersion course was no
Second, if—as a psychology instructor—the course objective
different than a 16-week course in intellectual demand. This
is to promote students’ basic knowledge and comprehension
last point is important to note because it provides some evi-
of course material and to have students think critically about
dence that the courses were equivalent.
course material, which Ormrod (2006) and Shell and Kleen
Overall, we contend that students may rate the 2-week
(1992) suggest is critical in higher education, then it seems that
immersion course and instructors more highly than students
immersion course may be an important modality of teaching.
in the traditional 16-week course because the condensed for-
Finally, our Study 2 findings replicate the results of Study 1,
mat allows for a large amount of interaction between students
implying that immersion courses are a practical option for both
and instructors in a very short time span (2 weeks), which may
college administrators and students who may face obstacles
bolster student–teacher rapport. Berglund et al. (2013) found
such as decreased resources and course scheduling.
the rapport between teachers and their students was the stron-
Alongside the educational implications of Studies 1 and 2,
gest predictor of teacher effectiveness ratings on SRIs, such
there are a few limitations. First, we did not study long-term
that teachers were perceived to be more effective when they
retention (e.g., 6-month delay) of academic performance. Some
had better rapport with their students (see Benson, Cohen,
may argue that retention rates are lower in immersion courses;
& Buskist, 2005; Wilson, Ryan, & Pugh, 2010 for further
however, Seamon (2004) suggests that most researchers do not
examples). Our results indicate that immersion courses may
track performance over time, regardless of course format, due
promote, expedite, and -solidify student–teacher rapport, thus
to the difficulties associated with this process. If possible,
improving students’ perception of teacher effectiveness and
future researchers would do well to investigate the long-term

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


32 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)

effects on academic performance of immersion courses. Second, Benson, T. A., Cohen, A. L., & Buskist, W. (2005). Rapport: Its rela-
both studies lacked a randomized double-blind control design. tion to student attitudes and behaviors toward teachers and
However, we attempted to diminish this limitation by controlling classes. Teaching of Psychology, 32, 237–270. doi:10.1207/
for teacher characteristics, using a matched-instructor design, s15328023top3204_8
and an extremely large sample size (i.e., Study 2). Finally, in Berglund, M., Richmond, A. S., Epelbaum, V. B., Oakman, S. D.,
accordance with Dunn’s (2008) argument for vigor over rigor, Pullano, K., Klein, E. M., . . . Pisel, A. L. (2013, April). Influence
we present an ecologically valid study that may be helpful to of humor, student rapport, and engagement on teaching effective-
psychology instructors. Specifically, Dunn argues that random ness . . . no joke! Poster presentation at the annual meeting of the
assignment is not always ethical or practical in classroom Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, Denver, CO.
research. Also, Dunn argues that again the ethical implication Boyer Commission on Educating Undergraduates in the Research
of having a true control group is problematic. University. (1998). Reinventing undergraduate education: A blue-
As Davies (2006) states, print for America’s Research Universities. Carnegie Foundation
for the Advancement of Teaching, Princeton, NJ.
Advantages of intensive formats [immersion] can be both ped- Burton, S., & Nesbit, P. L. (2008). Block or traditional? An analysis of
agogical and logistical. They can accrue to both student and student choice of teaching format. Journal of Management and
instructor. They revolve around increased motivation, commit- Organization, 14, 4–19. doi:10.5172/jmo.2008.14.1.4
ment, and concentration, diversity of teaching methods, stimu- Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-
lation and enthusiasm, stronger relations among students, and experimental designs for research. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
flexibility. (p. 19) Dalai, N. P. (1994). Higher order thinking in MIS. The Journal of
Computer Information Systems, 34, 26–30.
We suggest that, in addition, the immersion course may pro- Daniel, E. L. (2000). A review of time-shortened courses across disci-
vide the benefit of increasing students’ academic performance. plines. College Student Journal, 34, 298–308.
Although not always practical for every psychology depart- Davies, W. M. (2006). Intensive teaching formats: A review. Issues in
ment, this type of course infused with the traditional course Educational Research, 16, 1–20. Retrieved from http://www.iier.
may best enhance student performance. org.au/iier16/davies.html
Dunn, D. (2008). Another view: In defense of vigor over rigor in class-
Authors’ Note
room demonstrations. Teaching of Psychology, 34, 349–352. doi:
The results of Study 1 were presented at the 2010 annual meeting of
10.1080/00986280802374039
the American Psychological Association, San Diego, CA, USA. The
Gilmore, D., Swerdik, M. E., & Beehr, T. A. (1980). Effects of
results of Study 2 were presented at the 2011 annual meeting of the
American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C, USA. class size and college major on student ratings of psychology
courses. Teaching of Psychology, 7, 210–214. doi:10.1207/
Declaration of Conflicting Interests s15328023top0704_3
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to Gump, S. E. (2004). Daily class objectives and instructor’s effec-
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. tiveness as perceived by students’. Psychological Reports, 94,
1250–1252.
Funding Handlesman, M. M., Briggs, W. L., Sullivan, N., & Towler, A. (2005).
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, A measure of college student course engagement. Journal of Edu-
and/or publication of this article. cational Research, 98, 184–191. doi:10.3200/JOER.98.3.184-192
Heckert, T. M., Latier, A., Ringwald, A., & Silvey, B. (2006). Relation
Notes of course, instructor, and student characteristics to dimensions of
1. Twenty-eight students were removed from the analyses because student ratings and teaching effectiveness. College Student Jour-
they either received an ‘‘Incomplete’’ in the course or a ‘‘No nal, 40, 195–203.
Credit.’’ Jonas, P. M., Weimer, D., & Herzer, K. (2004). Comparison of tradi-
2. This sample is smaller than the sample used for main analysis of tional and nontraditional undergraduate business degree pro-
scheduling effects on academic performance (e.g., N ¼ 586) grams—Adult education. Journal of Instructional Psychology,
because student instructor and course data were optional for stu- 28, 161–168.
dents to complete. Kojaku, L. K., Nunez, A. M., & Malizio, A. G. (1998). Descriptive
summary of 1995–1996 beginning postsecondary students with
References profiles of students entering 2- and 4- year institutions U.S.
Anastasi, J. S. (2007). Full-semester and abbreviated summer courses: Department of Education, Washington, DC. (NCES 1999-030)
An evaluation of student performance. Teaching of Psychology, Lawerence, W. W., & McPherson, D. D. (2000). A comparative study
34, 19–22. doi:10.1080/00986280709336643 of block scheduling and traditional scheduling on academic
Becker, F. D., Sommer, R., Bee, J., & Oxley, B. (1973). College achievement. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 27, 178–182.
classroom ecology. Sociometry, 36, 230–231. Mims, S. K. (1983). The impact of time on art learning: Intensive vs.
Beins, B. C., & McCarthy, M. A. (2012). Research methods and concurrent scheduling in higher education. Studies in Art Educa-
statistics. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. tion, 24, 118–125.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015


Richmond et al. 33

Muraskin, L. D. (1998). A structured freshman year for at-risk Scott, P. A. (2003). Attributes of high-quality intensive courses. New
students. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Directions for Adult and Continuing Education, 97, 29–38. doi:10.
Nesselroade, J. R., & Thompson, W. W. (1995). Selection and 1002/ace.86
related threats to group comparisons: An example comparing fac- Seamon, M. (2004). Short-and long-term differences in instructional
torial structures of higher and lower ability groups of adult twins. effectiveness between intensive and semester-length courses.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 271–284. doi:10.1037/0033-2909. Teachers College Record, 106, 852–874.
117.2.271 Seldin, P. (2006). Evaluating faculty performance: A practical guide
Olani, A. (2009). Predicting first year university students’ academic to assessing teaching, research, and service. Bolton, MA: Anker
success. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychol- Publishing Company.
ogy, 7, 1696–2095. Retrieved from http://www.investigacion-psi- Shell, W. L., & Kleen, B. A. (1992). Innovative pedagogies for out-
copedagogica.com/revista/articulos/19/english/Art_19_376.pdf comes based teaching in the core MIS course. The Journal of
Ormrod, J. E. (2006). Educational psychology: Developing learners Computer Information Systems, 33, 3–7.
(5th ed.).Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall. Soldner, L., Lee, Y., & Duby, P. (2000). Welcome to the block:
Petrowsky, M. C. (1996). The two-week summer microeconomics Development freshman learning communities that work. Journal
course: Success or failure? (ERIC Document Reproduction Ser- of College Student Retention: Research, Theory, and Practice, 1,
vice, ED 396779). Glendale Community College, Glendale, AZ. 115–129. doi:10.2190/QL13-7QWA-VDXF-PJHL
Rau, W., & Durand, A. (2000). The academic ethic and college Tulving, E., & Psotka, J. (1971). Retroactive inhibition in free recall:
grades: Does hard work help students to ‘‘make the grade’’? Inaccessibility of information available in memory store. Journal
Sociology of Education, 73, 19–38. doi:147.153.177.2 of Experimental Psychology, 87, 1–8. doi:10.1037/h0030185
Ray, R., & Kirkpatrick, D. (1983). Two time formats to teaching U.S. Department of Education. (2001). The condition of education
human sexuality. Teaching of Psychology, 10, 84–88. doi:10. 2001 (NCES 1999-030). U.S. Department of Education, Washing-
1207/s15328023top1002_6 ton, DC.
Richmond, A. S., & Krank, H. M. (April, 2007). Immersion schedul- van Scyoc, L., & Gleason, J. (1993). Traditional or intensive
ing, academic self-concept, and college student success. Paper course lengths? A comparison of outcomes in economics learn-
presented at the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association; ing. Journal of Economics Education, 24, 15–22.
Denver, CO. Wilson, J. H., Ryan, R. G., & Pugh, J. L. (2010). Professor-student
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing rapport scale predicts student outcomes. Teaching of Psychology,
memory: Basic research and implications for educational prac- 37, 246–251. doi:10.1080/00986283.2010.510976
tice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210. doi: Wlodkowski, R. J. (2003). Accelerated learning in colleges and
10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x universities. In R. J. Wlodkowskii & C. Kasworm (Eds.), Accel-
Ryan, R. G., Wilson, J. H., & Pugh, J. L. (2011). Psychometric char- erated learning for adults: The promise and practice of inten-
acteristics of the professor–student rapport scale. Teaching of sive educational formats (Vol. 97, pp. 5–15). San Francisco,
Psychology, 38, 135–141. doi:10.1177/0098628311411894 CA: Jossey Bass.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com by guest on March 22, 2015

You might also like