Shock

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/4733661

Does It Pay to Shock? Reactions to Shocking and Nonshocking Advertising


Content among University Students

Article  in  Journal of Advertising Research · September 2003


DOI: 10.1017/S0021849903030332 · Source: RePEc

CITATIONS READS

170 10,574

3 authors, including:

Darren Dahl Rajesh Manchanda


University of British Columbia - Vancouver University of Manitoba
109 PUBLICATIONS   5,576 CITATIONS    20 PUBLICATIONS   1,142 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

“Our” Brand's Failure Leads to “Their” Product Derogation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Darren Dahl on 10 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Does It Pay to Shock? Reactions to
Shocking and Nonshocking Advertising
Content among University Students

DARREN W. DAHL Although the use of shocking content in advertising appeals has been widely adopted,
University of British
the effectiveness of such communication strategies has not been empirically
Columbia
darren.dahl@sauder. investigated. In two laboratory studies, conducted in the context of HIV/AIDS
ubc.ca prevention, we examine the effectiveness of shock advertising in comparison to the

KRISTINA D. commonly used appeals of fear and information. Our findings suggest that shocking
FRANKENBERGER content in an advertisement significantly increases attention, benefits memory, and
Western Oregon
positively influences behavior among a group of university students.
University
frankek@wou.edu.

RAJESH V. MANCHANDA IN AN AGE when consumers are exposed to an and driving, condom usage. Whether shock ap-
University of Manitoba estimated 3,000 advertisements per day (Lasn, peals actually produce these effects, and produce
raj_manchanda 1999), what does it take to get an advertisement them better than other appeals, is the main re-
@umanitoba.ca. noticed? For many advertisers, the answer is shock. search question addressed in this article.
Although opinions vary on whether shock is a
legitimate creative technique (Shannon, 1995; Van DEFINING SHOCK ADVERTISING
Munching, 1998) or a gratuitous attention-grabbing A shock advertising appeal is generally regarded
gimmick (Horovitz, 1992; Van Munching, 1998), as one that deliberately, rather than inadvertently,
press accounts make it clear that shock is a widely startles and offends its audience (Gustafson and
used advertising appeal (Cosper, 1997; Schloss- Yssel, 1994; Venkat and Abi-Hanna, 1995). Offense
berg, 1991; Shannon, 1995; Vagnoni, 1999; Van is elicited through the process of norm violation,
Munching, 1998; Wald, 1999). The trend is so encompassing transgressions of law or custom
prolific in the United Kingdom that it even has its (e.g., indecent sexual references, obscenity), breaches
own name, yobbo advertising, which is “roughly of a moral or social code (e.g., profanity, vulgar-
translated as the desire to shock the audience ity), or things that outrage the moral or physical
into taking notice by whatever means possible” senses (e.g., gratuitous violence, disgusting images).
(Cooper, 1996). There is little doubt that some advertising pur-
The purpose of this article is to investigate the posely breaches social norms with the intent to
effectiveness of shocking advertisement content in shock. Probably the most widely publicized cases
the context of a public service message. Presum- include the advertising campaigns produced by
The authors would like to thank ably, shocking messages are used in a bid to draw clothing makers Benetton and Calvin Klein. Over
Jennifer Argo for her assistance attention to an advertisement with the expecta- the years Benetton’s advertisements have featured
in running the studies and tion that further processing will take place if the photographs of a slain soldier’s bloodied uni-
McKim Communications for advertisement is noticed. Further, companies and form, a dying AIDS patient, and a white infant
their assistance in creating the organizations employing these techniques often nursing at a black woman’s breast. The advertise-
advertising stimuli. The financial do so thinking that a shock appeal will influence ments have won awards for heightening public
support from the Social Sciences behavior. Particularly in public service announce- awareness of social issues but have also provoked
and Humanities Research Coun- ments, it is hoped that shock appeals will gain public outrage and consumer complaints (Graham,
cil of Canada is gratefully attention, encourage cognitive processing, and have 1989; Kalish, 1990; Tinic, 1997). Calvin Klein’s
acknowledged. an immediate impact on behavior, e.g., drinking advertisements, whose “deliberately shocking

268 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003 DOI: 10.1017/S0021849903030332


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

graphics” (Goodrum and Dalrymple, 1990) groups, they come to learn the rules that tising messages (e.g., Waller, 1999). Fur-
are typically of a sexual nature, were tar- those groups define for acceptable, and thermore, because much of this research
geted by government and political groups unacceptable, behavior (Baron and Byrne, is descriptive rather than causal, we know
for their use of “pornographic” images 1977). The range of acceptable behaviors only that individuals consider broad types
(Sloan and DeCoursey, 1995). defined by norms is then used to evaluate of content offensive (e.g., racist, sexist,
Public health campaigns have also been objects, persons, actions, and ideas (Sherif indecent language; Waller, 1999) but not
the object of public scrutiny (Eisner, 2001). and Sherif, 1969). how they respond to a given advertise-
An organization called The Breast Cancer Advertising, as a social object, is eval- ment. Similarly, conclusions from re-
Fund recently employed a poster cam- uated by norms and is considered offen- search on irritating advertising indicate
paign that mimics sexy lingerie advertise- sive when its content breaches norms for that some product categories (e.g., hem-
ments, featuring attractive models in bras decency, good taste, aesthetic propriety, orrhoid preparations) are intrinsically
and panties, but with one difference— and/or personal moral standards (Day, irritating and that some advertising execu-
these women reveal mastectomy scars in- 1991). Sony Entertainment violated norms tion characteristics (e.g., contrived situa-
stead of breasts (Associated Press, 2000). for decency and good taste by using dis- tions and personal put-downs) increase
The advertisements were criticized by the gusting and violent images in a poster irritation (Aaker, 1985), but there is
media for their shocking nature, to the depicting a dismembered body on a mor- nothing that suggests whether irritating
extent that one media company refused tuary slab (Marketing Week, 1999). Sexual advertising is offensive or what kind
to place them. The Fund’s founder, how- references that violate norms of morality of impact it might have on consumer
ever, defended the advertisements, argu- are consistently used by the clothing com- behavior.
ing that without the shock element, people pany F.C.U.K. in billboard advertisements
would “lapse into acceptance” of breast that have urged consumers to F.C.U.K. Shocking advertising content and
cancer. In a public health context, shock “all night all long” and F.C.U.K. “think cognitive processing
appeals have also been used to discour- my clothes off” (Lippert, 1999). Similarly, Advertisers typically justify shock ap-
age alcohol abuse, encourage seat belt Bad Frog Brewery breached norms by peals in advertising for their ability to
safety, and promote AIDS awareness, do- using profanity in an advertisement that “[break] through the clutter,” “get no-
mestic violence awareness, colorectal featured an obscene gesture made by a ticed,” and “get people’s attention” (Vagno-
cancer screening, and STD prevention frog that has its middle finger extended ni, 1999). Although there is no academic
(Bainbridge, 1996; Campaign, 1995; Mar- (Advertising Age, 1996). Other examples of literature that covers responses to shock-
keting Week, 1995; Schlossberg, 1991; Tylee, shocking advertisements that have been ing advertising per se, the notion that
1998). criticized for breaking societal norms are shocking stimuli attract attention and fa-
provided in Table 1. cilitate other elements of cognition is out-
LITERATURE REVIEW We note that previous research has iden- lined by models of advertising information
tified the usage of sexual, irritating, and processing (e.g., McGuire, 1978). Indeed,
The role of norms offensive advertising content but has not after attention, according to information-
Shocking advertising content is that which fully investigated the role of inherent norm processing models, shocking stimuli should
attempts to surprise an audience by delib- violation in these approaches on con- facilitate message comprehension and elab-
erately violating norms for societal values sumer response. Research on sex in ad- oration, enhance message retention, and
and personal ideals. It is the norm viola- vertising, for example, often only assumes influence behavior. Figure 1 illustrates this
tion aspect of the shock appeal that is that the content is offensive or obscene; it process. In the figure, exposure to the
assumed to underlie its ability to break rarely tests the full implications of how a advertisement serves as the starting point.
through advertising clutter and capture “sexy” advertisement impacts the con- Initial processing of advertising informa-
the attention of a target audience who sumer (cf. Belch, Belch, and Villarreal, 1987; tion involves a cognitive appraisal that
then listens and acts on the related mes- Courtney and Whipple, 1983). Research determines whether the advertisement vi-
sage. Norm violation derives from the on offensive advertising more often fo- olates a social and/or personal norm. Any
violation of shared expectations that peo- cuses on potentially offensive products (e.g., object or event (such as an advertisement)
ple develop through the process of social advertising for feminine hygiene, alcohol, that contradicts an established expecta-
learning. As people interact with social and underwear) than on offensive adver- tion or schema (such as a social or per-

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 269


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

TABLE 1
Types of Shock Appeals
Offense Elicitor Description Advertising Exemplars Published Reactions
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Disgusting images References to blood, body parts Playstation, United States, 2000 Bob Garfield, Advertising Age adver-
or secretions, orifices, especially Television commercial wherein chef tising critic, coins new term for such
urinary/fecal, gases, odors, wipes steak around toilet bowl rim advertising: “advertrocities”
disease, parasites, bodily harm and adds rolled up nose pickings as (Garfield, 2000)
(e.g., dismemberment), death garnish in preparation for serving.
and decay
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Sexual references References to masturbation, Reebok, United States, 2000 CBS rejected the advertisement, which
implied sexual acts, sexually A guy sucks snake venom out of “struck some adults as repulsive”
suggestive nudity or partial his friend’s leg in a position that is (Vranica, 2000).
nudity mistaken for felatio by an onlooker.
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Profanity/obscenity Swear words, obscene gestures, Holsten Pils, United Kingdom, 1996 Advertisement deemed part of
racial epitaphs Print advertisement depicts smashed “yobbo advertising” trend—the
bottles of the product accompanied desire to shock an audience to
by the word “shit.” attention by whatever means possible
(Cooper, 1996).
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Vulgarity References to crude or distasteful Smartbeep, United States, 1999 Pushes the limits of bad taste
acts by humans or animals, such Woman “lets rip a frat-house blast” (Vagnoni, 1999).
as nose picking, farting, licking, of gas while waiting for date to join
humping, or drinking from the toilet her in car.
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Impropriety Violations of social conventions Adidas, Canada, 1993 Sports Illustrated rejected the adver-
for dress, manners, etc. Magazine advertisement depicts tisement because the male nudity was
all-nude male soccer team. considered offensive by the magazine
(Advertising Age International, 1993).
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Moral offensiveness Harming innocent people/animals, Diesel Jeans, Italy, 1998 Written complaints to British
gratuitous violence or sex, alluding Television commercial features a Advertising Standards Authority;
to people or objects that provoke soldier who is shot in the back by the press tags the campaign
violence (e.g., Hitler), violating a German sniper while protecting “controversial” (Financial Post, 1998).
standards for fair behavior (e.g., a friend wearing Diesel jeans.
shooting a person in the back),
putting children in provocative Acurist, United Kingdom, 1997 Press calls campaign “startling” and
situations (e.g., sexual, violent), Depicts “skeletal” model wearing “controversial” (Hall, 1997).
victim exploitation watch on upper arm with tagline
“Watch your weight”
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Religious taboos Inappropriate use of spiritual or Benetton, 1994 Campaign designed to shock and
religious symbols and/or rituals Depicts a priest and nun kissing provoke (Marketing Week, 1994)

British Safety Council, 1994 The British Advertising Standards


Leaflet campaign for national condom Authority received 1,192 complaints
week depicts the Pope wearing a (Marketing, 1996).
safety helmet with the line, “The
11 th commandment—thou shalt
wear a condom.”
..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

270 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

application. We chose to use student sub-


jects in this context because they repre-
sent a realistic target market for HIV/
AIDS prevention initiatives. Indeed, among
the cases of positive HIV infection re-
ported in North America through 2001
approximately one-third occurred among
Figure 1 A Preliminary Model of Consumer Reactions to people age 29 and younger (Centers for
Disease Control [CDC], 2001; Health Can-
Shock Appeals
ada, 2002). Risk factors are generally higher
among this group because of their in-
creased incidence of sexual activity and
sonal norm) causes surprise (Stiensmeier- as well documented (Vakratsas and Am- drug-related behaviors (CDC, 2002), and
Pelster, Martini, and Reisenzein, 1995). bler, 1999), but the presumption is that both the CDC and Health Canada have
Surprise is a significant part of the pro- shocking advertising content, because it identified their nations’ youth as repre-
cess because it initializes the processing attracts attention and elicits cognitive pro- senting the key to preventing the spread
of advertising information; it attracts at- cessing, stands a better chance of eliciting of HIV/AIDS in their countries.
tention to the novel stimulus or event appropriate behavior compared to ad- One-hundred-and-five undergraduate
(Meyer, Niepel, Rudolph, and Schötz- vertisements that have less success at students participated in the study for
wohl, 1991). By focusing attention on the moving people through the stages of in- course credit. Subjects were randomly as-
stimulus, surprise encourages additional formation processing. signed to one of the three experimental
processing of advertising content, an idea Our first study tests the process just conditions and run individually. Subjects
supported by research on expectancy- described by comparing three print adver- ranged in age from 18 to 27.
disconfirmation theory. Empirical find- tisements, one of which is deliberately
ings in this area demonstrate that designed to provoke offense—the shock Stimuli. To prepare stimulus materials,
individuals engage in higher levels of appeal. we first examined a convenience sample
attributional thought for unexpected (sur- of HIV/AIDS prevention advertisements
prising) compared to expected events STUDY 1 targeted toward our subject population
(Pyszczynski and Greenberg, 1981). Sur- that had appeared in print in the United
prise, therefore, encourages further cog- Method States or Canada during the preceding
nitive activity as individuals seek to three years. Three print advertisements,
understand the source of their surprise. Subjects and setting. A between-subjects one for each type of appeal (i.e., informa-
The additional processing of advertising experimental design employing three dif- tion, fear, and shock), were selected from
information takes the form of comprehen- ferent advertising appeals (shock, fear, and the sample to serve as frames for stimu-
sion, a literal understanding of the mes- information) was used to test for effects lus development. None of these advertise-
sage, and elaboration, the production of on advertising attention, recall, and rec- ments ran in the geographical region in
product/message-related thoughts (Green- ognition in an HIV/AIDS prevention con- which the study was conducted. Each ad-
wald and Leavitt, 1984). Indeed, empiri- text. We chose a HIV/AIDS context, as vertisement promoted condom usage as
cal research investigating unexpectedness, prevention communications employed by an aspect of a healthy sexual lifestyle.
incongruity, and other dimensions of stim- public health organizations have a history Borrowing headlines and graphical themes
ulus novelty indicates that these charac- of using “shock value” messages to com- from the selected advertisements, three
teristics encourage cognitive processes that bat ambivalence toward this particular dis- otherwise original print advertisements
produce robust effects on memory (Heck- ease (Schlossberg, 1991). were developed by an advertising agency.
ler and Childers, 1992; Lynch and Srull, In defining our experimental sample, it The shock advertisement’s headline read
1982; MacInnis, Moorman, and Jaworski, was important to center the communica- “Don’t be a F—ing Idiot” and featured a
1991). The effects of cognitive processing tion in a population in which an AIDS- hazy but recognizable visual of a nude
on behavior are generally weaker and not prevention message had relevance and couple in an intimate embrace (see Fig-

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 271


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

ure 2). The fear advertisement showed a vertisement was the tagline “Use a Con- 11 inch mockups of the test advertise-
driver’s license with the expiry date cir- dom!” in large letters, with smaller text ments (shock n 5 23, fear n 5 28, in-
cled in red and the headline “If you get below that read “Latex condoms, when formation n 5 26). Subjects rated the
the AIDS virus now, you and your license used correctly and consistently, signifi- advertisement on 7-point scales (Disagree/
could expire at the same time” (see Fig- cantly reduce the chance of acquiring Agree) indicating the extent to which ad-
ure 3). The information advertisement fea- HIV/AIDS.” vertisements were considered shocking,
tured a graphic of the AIDS acronym with Pretesting was conducted to ensure that scary/frightening (r 5 .815), educational/
the words “Acquired Immunodeficiency the advertisements were perceived as con- factual (r 5 .713), understandable, and
Syndrome” appearing in brackets imme- veying shock, fear, or information, and likable.
diately below. The subhead read “First that they were equally understandable and As expected, the shock advertisement
Identified May 11, 1982 in New York City” likable. A sample of undergraduate stu- (mean 5 4.41) was shown to be more
(see Figure 4). At the bottom of each ad- dents (n 5 77) viewed one of three 9 _12 by shocking than the fear advertisement
(mean 5 3.55) and the information adver-
tisement (mean 5 2.98). Further, the fear
advertisement was deemed more fright-
ening (mean 5 4.20) than the shock
advertisement (mean 5 3.20) or the infor-
mational advertisement (mean 5 3.17).
Finally, the information advertisement
(mean 5 4.63) was rated more educational/
factual than either the shock (mean 5
3.85) or fear advertisement (mean 5 3.77).
The three advertisements were rated to be
equally likable and understandable. Table 2
provides the relevant test statistics for these
comparisons.

Procedure. Upon arriving at the lab re-


ception area, subjects were told they would
undertake the study in an alternate room,
as the experimental lab was being reno-
vated. The alternate room was described
as a “student clubroom” that had been
temporarily vacated and loaned to the
experimenters for the week. The club-
room was in fact a controlled experimen-
tal environment consisting of a table, chair,
the stimulus poster, and four decoy post-
ers (scenery poster, Safewalk poster, Pepsi
poster, and student crossing poster). The
table and chair were situated such that
subjects had an equal opportunity to view
each poster. Subjects were asked to sit at
the table and complete a pricing survey (a
decoy). At this point the experimenter
indicated that they had forgotten to bring
Figure 2 Shock Appeal the first part of the survey and asked the

272 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

dent variables detailed below. Subjects


who completed the instrument were then
fully debriefed and thanked for their
participation.

Measures. The survey instrument first


measured unaided recall of the posters by
asking the subjects to write down the
names or themes of the posters they re-
membered seeing in the student club-
room. Subjects were then asked to indicate
which of these posters attracted their at-
tention the most. They were then asked to
explain why the posters attracted their
attention. Responses to this question were
coded by two independent research assis-
tants, blind to the purpose of the experi-
ment, who identified if subjects mentioned
a specific norm violation as a cause of
their attention toward the advertisement.
Initial agreement between the coders was
96.4 percent, and disputes were resolved
through discussion with one of the au-
thors. Following the cognitive response
question, a list of the eight posters (the
five that were in the room and an addi-
tional three that were not) was provided
and subjects were asked to indicate which
posters they recognized. A second recog-
nition measure provided five statements
from which subjects were to select the
statement they remembered seeing in the
Figure 3 Fear Appeal copy of the target poster.
Only those subjects that remembered
the “Use a Condom” poster were in-
subject to wait while the missing section istered to determine if subjects suspected structed to complete the manipulation
was retrieved. This delay provided the that the posters were the experimental check measures. These consisted of a se-
subject an opportunity to be exposed to stimuli. This probe involved asking the ries of 7-point scales (Disagree/Agree) that
the posters in the room. The experimenter subject what they thought the purpose of asked subjects how they felt after viewing
returned after 1.5 minutes, provided the the study was. Subjects were deemed sus- the target advertisement (shocked, scared,
decoy survey, and instructed the subject picious if they mentioned the posters or educated). Additional measures asked sub-
to return to the reception area for debrief- indicated that the experimenters had ul- jects to indicate whether the advertise-
ing after completing the survey. terior motives beyond the completion of ment was obscene, startling, likable, and
After arriving at the reception area, the the pricing survey. Subjects who were not understandable. All subjects then com-
subject was debriefed about the purpose identified to be suspicious (n 5 96; 32 per pleted an open-ended question that asked
of the decoy study. As part of this debrief- experimental cell) were asked to com- them what they thought the study was
ing, a verbal suspicion probe was admin- plete an instrument containing the depen- about (a second suspicion probe). They

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 273


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

likable and understandable manipulation


check measures. Table 2 provides the rel-
evant test statistics for these comparisons.
To test our shock definition, we also com-
pared ratings of the obscene and startling
nature of the three advertisements. As ex-
pected, the shock advertisement (mean 5
3.25) was perceived to be more obscene than
the fear advertisement (mean 5 1.73) and
the information advertisement (mean 5
2.23). The shock advertisement (mean 5
3.91) was also shown to be more startling
than the fear advertisement (mean 5 3.00)
and the information advertisement (mean 5
2.62). These results validate our concep-
tualization that shocking advertising both
startles audiences and violates societal
norms in its execution.
Responses to the question related to the
perceived purpose of the study indicated
that none of the subjects was aware of the
experimental hypotheses.

Main analyses. The primary dependent


measures in this study focused on the
attention, recall, recognition, and process-
ing of the three experimental advertise-
ments (see Table 2). When asked to indicate
which poster drew their attention the most,
a greater percentage of subjects in the
shock advertisement condition chose the
experimental advertisement (84.4 percent)
compared to the fear (40.6 percent) and
Figure 4 Information Appeal information conditions (46.9 percent). Anal-
ysis using logistic regression, with atten-
tion as the criterion variable and the three
were then asked to indicate their age, imental conditions. Paralleling the pre- advertising appeals as dummy-predictor
gender, relationship status, and usage of test, the shock advertisement was perceived variables reinforces the previous results
condoms. Responses to these items had to be more shocking (shock mean 5 4.00, by indicating a higher probability for sub-
no effect on the results reported below fear mean 5 2.77, inform mean 5 2.27), jects in the shock condition to identify the
and therefore are not discussed further. the fear advertisement more frightening experimental advertisement as being able
(fear mean 5 3.81, shock mean 5 2.28, to capture their attention most.
Findings inform mean 5 2.92), and the information The coded cognitive responses indi-
advertisement more educational (inform cated that among subjects naming the
Preliminary analyses. Planned contrasts mean 5 5.69, shock mean 5 4.72, fear shock advertisement as that which at-
were used to test for the effectiveness of mean 5 4.58). No differences were found tracted their attention the most, 77.8 per-
our manipulations across the three exper- across the three advertisements for the cent mentioned a specific norm violation

274 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

TABLE 2
Stimulus Means, Sample Sizes, and Test Statistics
Advertising Type Statistical Comparisons
................................................................................. ........................................................
Shock Fear Information Test Statistic p<
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Pretest means n = 23 n = 28 n = 26
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Shocking 4.41 3.55 2.98 a. t(75) = 2.02 .05
c. t(75) = 3.31 .01
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Frightening 3.20 4.20 3.17 a. t(75) = 2.25 .05
b. t(75) = 2.38 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Educational/factual 3.85 3.77 4.63 b. t(75) = 2.29 .05
c. t(75) = 1.98 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Study 1—Preliminary Analysis Means n = 32 n = 32 n = 32
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Shocking 4.00 2.77 2.27 a. t(94) = 2.37 .05
c. t(94) = 3.78 .001
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Frightening 2.28 3.81 2.92 a. t(94) = 3.08 .01
b. t(94) = 1.70 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Educational/factual 4.72 4.58 5.69 b. t(94) = 2.20 .05
c. t(94) = 2.02 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Obscene 3.25 1.73 2.23 a. t(94) = 3.82 .001
c. t(94) = 2.56 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Startling 3.91 3.00 2.62 a. t(94) = 1.78 .05
c. t(94) = 2.54 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Study 1—Main Analysis Percentages
Percent who claim test advertisement drew most attention 84.4 40.6 46.9 x 2 (2) = 14.65 .01
Wald t (1) = 9.05 .01
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Percent who mention norm violation as attention draw 77.8 7.7 6.7 x 2 (2) = 28.14 .001
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Percent who recalled test advertisement 96.9 78.1 78.1 x 2 (2) = 5.69 .10
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Percent who recognized test advertisement 100.0 81.3 81.3 x 2 (2) = 6.86 .05
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Percent who correctly recognized test advertising copy 90.6 59.4 46.9 x 2 (2) = 14.41 .01
Wald t (1) = 7.18 .01
................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Note: a 5 Shock versus Fear, b 5 Fear versus Information, c 5 Shock versus Information

as a factor in drawing their attention. The . . . norm violation is the key to heightened awareness of
percentages of people identifying norm
violations in the other conditions were shocking advertising content.
much lower (information, 6.7 percent; fear,
7.7 percent). This result validates our con-
ceptualization that norm violation is the On recall, individuals in the shock con- percent) than those in either the informa-
key to heightened awareness of shocking dition recalled the experimental advertise- tion or fear conditions (78.1 percent each).
advertising content. ment at a significantly higher level (96.9 Analysis on the recognition measure indi-

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 275


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

cated that all subjects in the shock condi- intended behavior. In contrast, it is also Measures. On the table were eight items
tion were able to identify the advertisement possible that due to the norm violation in sets of five each: (1) pamphlets by a
when cued, whereas those in the fear and inherent in shock appeals, the resulting public mental health organization on sui-
information conditions realized only an outcome effect of shocking advertising con- cide prevention; (2) pamphlets on alcohol
81.3 percent recognition rate. Further, a tent may be unintended negative behav- and drug consumption; (3) information
greater proportion of subjects in the shock ior. To investigate these differing positions, on United Way charities; (4) personal bor-
condition were able to identify the correct a second study was conducted in which rowing information from a local bank;
advertising copy (90.6 percent) compared the subsequent behavior of subjects ex- (5) sunscreen advice from the Cancer
to the fear (59.4 percent) and information posed to shocking advertising content was Society; (6) Condom Knowledge from
conditions (46.9 percent). Logistic regres- measured. Durex; (7) AIDS ribbons; and (8) a busi-
sion, with recognition of the advertising Study 2 employed the same stimuli and ness card with the HIV/AIDS hotline from
copy serving as the criterion variable between-subjects experimental design that a local medical clinic. Of the eight items,
and the advertising appeals acting as was used in study 1 but added a control the last three were used to measure the
dummy-variable predictors, indicates a condition in which the “Use a Condom” dependent variable.
higher probability for subjects in the shock poster was absent. One-hundred-and-
condition to identify the correct advertis- forty undergraduate students partici-
Findings
ing copy. pated in the study for course credit.
The dependent variable was measured by
Subjects were randomly assigned to one
observing which of the HIV/AIDS-related
Summary of the four experimental conditions and
materials the subject picked up when the
As expected, the shock appeal outper- run individually.
experimenter was absent from the room.
formed the fear and information appeals
Table 3 gives the percentages of item
on measures of attention, recall, and rec- Procedure. The same cover story and pro-
pickup for each experimental condition.
ognition. Importantly, our results showed cedure that were used in study 1 were
Differences across conditions were shown
that subjects felt the shock advertisement followed with the exception that quali-
to be statistically significant, with subjects
violated social norms, and this finding is fied (i.e., nonsuspicious) subjects (n 5
in the shock (47.1 percent) and fear (52.9
interpreted as the cause of heightened 136; 34 per cell) did not respond to a
percent) conditions more likely to pick up
awareness for the shock appeal. The evi- paper and pencil measure. Instead, sub-
materials than in the information (23.5
dence from this initial study supports our jects were directed to a table in the re-
percent) and control (20.6 percent) condi-
contention that shocking advertising con- ception area. Subjects were told that on
tions. Analysis using logistic regression,
tent is superior to nonshocking content in this table were some items left over from
with the observation variable serving as
its ability to attract attention and facilitate cleaning out the clubroom and that the
the criterion and the three advertising and
memory for the advertisement. club no longer wanted to keep these items.
control conditions acting as dummy-
Subjects were informed they could look
variable predictors, indicates a higher prob-
STUDY 2 at the items and keep any that they
ability for subjects in both the shock
wanted. The experimenter left the recep-
condition and the fear condition to pickup
Method tion area at this point on the pretext of
HIV/AIDS-related materials.
getting the extra credit roster from an-
Subjects and setting. The first study other room so that the subject could sign
showed positive outcomes for shocking it. This allowed subjects to be alone to Summary
advertising content with respect to mea- select and take any of the items. The Approximately one-half of the subjects in
sures of attention and memory. However, experimenter returned to the reception the shock and fear conditions picked up
it remains unclear about how shocking area after two minutes, obtained the sub- an AIDS-related item, compared to ap-
advertising content will impact subsequent ject’s signature, and thanked the subject proximately 20 percent in the information
behavior. On one hand, given the strong for participating. Once the subject left and control conditions. Beyond benefits
positive effects of shock advertising on the reception area, the experimenter for attention and memory, shocking ad-
memory, it is likely that these positive counted the items and made a note of vertising content was able to motivate
effects will in turn translate into actual which items had been picked up. subjects to acknowledge the risk of AIDS

276 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

TABLE 3 tisements, need not be considered to be


necessarily negative and ineffective. In a
Percentages of People Who Picked up HIV/AIDS Items
public-policy context we have shown that,
Experimental Condition % Who Select an Item although a shock advertisement generates
.............................................................................................................................................................
an acknowledgement of norm violation
Control 20.6
............................................................................................................................................................. among viewers, it also ensures that sub-
Fear 52.9
............................................................................................................................................................. jects remember the message and engage
Information 23.5
............................................................................................................................................................. in message-relevant behavior. In a clut-
Shock 47.1 tered advertising environment, shocking
.............................................................................................................................................................
advertising content ensures that the mes-
n = 34 subjects per cell x 2 (3) = 11.836, p < .01
sage will be heard.
Shock—Wald t (1) = 5.10, p < .05
We temper our endorsement for shock
Fear—Wald t (1) = 7.23, p < .01
............................................................................................................................................................. tactics by noting critical characteristics of

acquisition and obtain information regard- The shock appeal . . . was effective at encouraging sub-
ing safe sex behavior.
Interestingly, we noted similar positive jects to remember advertising information and to engage
effects for the fear advertisement appeal.
Given the findings from our two studies, in message-relevant behaviors.
it seems that although shock and fear are
equally effective in producing message
consistent behaviors, these appeals may this than other types of appeals (e.g., fear the research context we employed. First,
be operating in different ways. Shock seems and information). Furthermore, the two the subject population (university stu-
to be effective because its norm violation studies demonstrate that it is possible for dents) we chose to expose to the shock
surprises the subject and produces addi- a shock appeal to have positive effects appeal is a specific at-risk population for
tional cognitive processing. The effective- beyond initial attention. The shock appeal AIDS transmission. It has been recom-
ness of the fear appeal may be due to the investigated in this study was effective mended that shock appeals should be tar-
fact that it solicits and taps into a more at encouraging subjects to remember ad- geted to specific populations that will be
affective response from the subject. Fu- vertising information and to engage in open to the message conveyed (Schloss-
ture research that explores the potential message-relevant behaviors. This article berg, 1991; Tylee, 1998), and this seems
differences in the processes resulting from also makes an important contribution by probable for the audience we selected.
these two forms of appeal would be help- conceptualizing shock as a combination Subject populations drawn from different
ful in further understanding their similar- of norm violation and surprise. This as- age cohorts, who are less educated or
ities and differences. sists in the development of future re- who reside in different countries, may
search on shocking communications by have responded differently to our shock
CONCLUSIONS providing a meaningful conceptual defi- message. Second, our research was con-
This research tested the effectiveness of a nition from which to work. ducted in a public health context through
shock appeal against two other com- Our research would suggest that con- a specific public service message. It re-
monly used appeals in the context of HIV/ trary to recent skepticism and concern mains to be seen whether the shock ap-
AIDS prevention and, in doing so, makes regarding the negative effects of using peal effects shown in this research translate
the following contributions. First, study 1 shocking advertising content (e.g., Eads, into success in other types of advertising
confirmed what has until now been only 1999), this type of communications strat- situations. It is possible that there is a
an intuition that shock is very good at egy can be effective. The publicity that is greater toleration for norm violation in a
attracting attention. Second, it was dem- often generated, as a consequence of the public-policy context because viewers may
onstrated that a shock appeal is better at norm-violating nature of shocking adver- agree that “the ends justify the means”

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 277


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

(Thacker, 1993). Finally, the shock tactic ate advertising situations, brand and possible, however, for a shocking adver-
we employed is relevant to the subject products offerings, and advertising medi- tisement that is potentially disliked to be
matter of the communicated message. In ums suited for shock tactics. As noted effective? The negative repercussions re-
the shock appeal tested, we matched the previously, in our research we chose a alized by Benetton for using death row
subject matter of the communication (i.e., public policy context as it seemed that inmates in a recent advertising campaign
safe sex) to relevant shock graphics (i.e., shock tactics can be judged to be appro- provide some anecdotal evidence that a
naked couple embracing) and language priate in these types of advertising situ- strongly disliked shock appeal may not
(i.e., f--k) execution. The importance of ations (Schlossberg, 1991; Thacker, 1993). provide tangible benefits (de Chenecey,
the fit between the message topic and Shock tactics also seem commonplace in 2000; Lippert, 2000). However, other dis-
shock tactic employed has been previ- advertising situations that involve a new- liked shock advertisements (e.g., Calvin
ously recognized (Gustafson and Yssel, product or brand introduction (Lauro, Klein’s 1995 child exploitive advertise-
1994), and studies show that communica- 1999; Sellar, 1999). For example, internet ments) have been cited as casual factors
tions can be rendered less effective when companies like Outpost.com, Ecampus. in an organization’s subsequent profitabil-
their executions involve a mismatch be- com, and Beyond.com utilized shock tac- ity (Rees, 1995). Future research should
tween either the product message (Court- tics when first entering the ecommerce investigate the boundaries and strength
ney and Whipple, 1983) or execution marketplace. In these instances, the of the shocking aspect of an appeal to
message (Venkat and Abi-Hanna, 1995). attention-grabbing ability of a shock ap- better define what leads to shock adver-
peal was used to gain top-of-mind aware- tising effectiveness.
Limitations and future research ness (Lauro, 1999). We also note that a Our findings suggest that advertising
There are a number of limitations of this large number of the shock advertise- practitioners as well as public policy mak-
research that provide direction for future ments we encountered advertised prod- ers should consider the use of shocking
investigation. While this study makes a ucts and brands targeted to younger age content in their advertising campaigns.
significant contribution in reporting ac- cohorts. Shock tactics map well onto the Skeptics have criticized the effectiveness
tual rather than intended behaviors, the stereotypical youth need for rebellion and of shocking advertising, but our research
behaviors we tested are product-use re- societal norm challenging behavior. A indicates that shocking communications
lated (e.g., awareness/concern through shock appeal targeted to a youth audi- can have positive effects on attention,
picking up an HIV/AIDS ribbon or bro- ence has the potential to be deemed “cool” memory, and behavior. Still, we believe
chure) rather than actual product (e.g., and relevant to this audience’s specific that direct norm violation in advertising
condom) use. The current study also mea- needs. Indeed, some research suggests should be used responsibly. This would
sured memory after only a short delay, that this particular audience is prone to entail careful target audience selection and
leaving future research to discover any like and accept potentially offensive ma- savvy creative execution. Just as some
long-term effects on memory and behav- terial (Oppliger and Zillman, 1997). While people can enjoy a vulgar or tasteless
ior for shocking stimuli. Further, it re- somewhat intuitive, these ideas regard- joke, certain advertising audiences can like
mains to be seen whether the positive ing the application of shock are derived and respond to a shocking advertisement
benefits in attention we attribute to shock from anecdotal and cursory evidence. Fu- even while judging it obscene or vulgar.
appeals are mitigated by a faster rate of ture research is needed to better define Furthermore, shocking advertisements
advertising wearout for shock appeals. the specific advertising characteristics should reveal some benefit to using the
The blunt execution of norm violation that are appropriate for an effective shock product, rather than using shocking con-
and surprise inherent in shock advertise- appeal. tent as an unrelated attention-getting
ments may quickly lose its “edginess” as Our research used a shock advertise- gimmick. Despite these caveats and the
the audience becomes familiarized with ment that was rated as likable by the limitations described above, our research
the appeal. Future research should probe subject population participating in our suggests that skepticism regarding the ef-
for discrepancies in wearout rates for shock study. Our findings that showed the ef- fectiveness of shocking advertising might
advertisements vis-à-vis other advertising fectiveness of the tested shock appeal thus be put aside until additional investiga-
appeals and test for relevant implications. fit well with previous research linking tions are made into the appropriate use of
Future research should also be di- advertising effectiveness with advertising norm violations in different advertising
rected toward identifying the appropri- liking (Haley and Baldinger, 1991). Is it contexts.

278 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

................................................................................................ Centers for Disease Control and Preven- Greenwald, Anthony G., and Clark Lea-
DARREN W. DAHL is associate professor of marketing tion (CDC). HIV/AIDS Surveillance Report 13, 1 vitt. “Audience Involvement in Advertising:
at the Sauder School of Business at the University of (2001). Four Levels.” Journal of Consumer Research 11, 1
British Columbia in Vancouver, British Columbia. (1984): 581–92.

................................................................................................ –——. Fact Sheet —Young People at Risk: HIV/


KRISTINA D. FRANKENBERGER is associate professor of AIDS Among America’s Youth. [URL: http:// Gustafson, Bob, and Johan Yssel. “Are Ad-

marketing in the Division of Business and Economics www.cdc.gov/hiv/pubs/facts/youth.htm] , vertisers Practicing Safe Sex?” Marketing News,

at Western Oregon University in Monmouth, Oregon. 2002. March 14, 1994.

................................................................................................
Cooper, Ann. “The Shock of the Crude.” Ad- Haley, Russell I., and Allan L. Baldinger.
RAJESH V. MANCHANDA is associate professor of mar-
week, February 5, 1996. “The ARF Copy Research Validity Project.” Jour-
keting at the I.H. Asper School of Business at the
nal of Advertising Research 31, 2 (1991): 11–32.
University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba.
Cosper, Darcy. “Shock Value.” Print 51, 6 (1997):
38–40. Hall, Emma. “TBWA Simons Uses a Skeletal
Model in Shock Accurist Ads.” Campaign-
REFERENCES
London, November 7, 1997.
Courtney, Alice E., and Thomas W. Whipple.
Aaker, David A. “Causes of Irritation in Ad- Sex Stereotyping in Advertising. Lexington, MA:
D.C. Health and Company, 1983. Health Canada. HIV and AIDS in Canada.
vertising.” Journal of Marketing 49, 2 (1985):
Surveillance Report to December 31, 2001. Divi-
47–57.
sion of HIV/AIDS Epidemiology and Surveil-
Day, Louis A. Ethics in Media Communications.
lance, Centre for Infectious Disease Prevention
Advertising Age. “Bad Frog Beer Triggers an Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 1991.
and Control, Health Canada, 2002.
Amphibious Assault.” April 1, 1996.

de Chenecey, Sean Pillot. “When is it Right


Heckler, Susan E., and Terry L. Childers.
Advertising Age International. “Adidas to Use Shock Ad Strategies?” Marketing, March
“The Role of Expectancy and Relevancy in
Finds ‘SI’ Ad Cancellation Difficult to Bare.” 30, 2000.
Memory for Verbal and Visual Information:
July 5, 1993.
What is Incongruity?” Journal of Consumer Re-
Eads, Stefani. “Eighty-Six the Flying Gerbils.”
search 18, 4 (1992): 475–92.
Associated Press. “Breast Cancer Ads Cause
Business Week, December 6, 1999.
Stir.” The New York Times on the Web, January
27, 2000. Horovitz, Bruce. “Shock Ads: New Rage that
Eisner, Jane. “Nonprofit ‘Shock’ Ads Overdo Spawns Rage.” Los Angeles Times, March 22,
It.” The Philadelphia Inquirer, February 12, 2001. 1992.
Bainbridge A. “’Offensive Ads at All-Time High
Warns the ASA.” Marketing, April 4, 1996.
Financial Post. “Diesel Defends War Ads.” Kalish, David. “Doing Well by Doing Good.”
November 9, 1998. Marketing and Media Decisions 25, 1 (1990): 22–23.
Baron, Robert A., and Donn Byrne. Social
Psychology: Understanding Human Interaction. Bos-
ton, MA: Allyn and Bacon, Inc., 1977. Garfield, Bob. Advertrocities. [video]. New York: Lasn, Kalle. Culture Jam: The Uncooling of Amer-
London International Advertising Awards, ica. New York: Eagle Brook, 1999.
2000.
Belch, George E., Michael A. Belch, and
Angelina Villarreal. “Effects of Advertising Lauro, Patricia Winters. “Dot-com Compa-
Communications: Review of Research.” In Re- Goodrum, Charles, and Helen Dalrymple. nies Have Built Their Brands by Using More
search in Marketing, Jagdish Sheth, ed. New Advertising in America: The First 200 Years. New Ads, in Good Taste or Not. Often Not.” The
York: JAI Press, 1987. York: Harry N. Abrams, Inc., 1990. New York Times, September 30, 1999.

Campaign. “LTA Bans Anti-Drink Poster.” Feb- Graham, Judith. “Benetton ‘Colors’ the Race Lippert, Barbara. “All FCUKed Up.” Adweek,
ruary 10, 1995. Issue.” Advertising Age, September 11, 1989. October 11, 1999.

September 2003 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH 279


SHOCKING ADVERTISEMENTS

–——. “Capital Offense.” Adweek, February 28, Pyszczynski, Thomas A., and Jeff Greenberg. Tinic, Serra A. “United Colors and Untied
2000. “Role of Disconfirmed Expectancies in the In- Meanings: Benetton and the Commodification
stigation of Attributional Processing.” Journal of Social Issues.” Journal of Communication 47, 3
Lynch, John G., Jr., and Thomas K. Srull. of Personality and Social Psychology 40, 1 (1981): (1997): 3–25.
“Memory and Attentional Factors in Consumer 31–38.
Choice: Concepts and Research Methods.” Jour-
Tylee, John. “AMV Ad Aims to Shock Viewers
nal of Consumer Research 9, 1 (1982): 18–37.
Rees, Jon. “Calvin Klein Makes ‘Offensive’ Ads into Belting Up.” Campaign, July 10, 1998.
Pay.” Marketing Week, September 8, 1995.
MacInnis, Deborah J., Christine Moorman,
and Bernard J. Jaworski. “Enhancing and Mea- Vagnoni, Anthony. “’Something About’ This

suring Consumers’ Motivation, Opportunity, and Schlossberg, Howard. “AIDS-Prevention Ads Advertising.” Advertising Age, February 8,

Ability to Process Brand Information from Ads.” Spark Battle Over What’s Proper.” Marketing 1999.

Journal of Marketing 55, 4 (1991): 32–53. News, April 29, 1991.


Vakratsas, Demetrios, and Tim Ambler. “How
Marketing. “‘Offensive’ Ads at All-Time High Advertising Really Works: What Do We
Sellar, Sara. “Pop Goes the Gerbil.” Sales and
Warns the ASA.” April 4, 1996. Really Know?” Journal of Marketing 63, 1 (1999):
Marketing Management 151, 5 (1999): 15.
26–43.
Marketing Week. “Blood Sweaters and Tears.”
June 3, 1994. Shannon, John. “Why Shock Ads Have Their
Van Munching, Philip. “The Devil’s Ad-
Place.” Marketing Week, November 10, 1995.
man.” Brandweek, August 24, 1998.
–——. “NSPCC to Hit Cinemas with ‘Shock’
Ad.” 1995.
Sherif, Muzafer, and Carolyn W. Sherif.
Venkat, Ramesh, and Noba Abi-Hanna. “Ef-
Social Psychology. New York: Harper & Row,
–——. “ASA Blasts Sony for ‘Offensive’ Mor- fectiveness of Visually Shocking Advertise-
1969.
tuary Ad.” February 11, 1999. ments: Is It Context Dependent?” In
Administrative Science Association of Canada Pro-
McGuire, William J. “An Information Process- Sloan, Pat, and DeCoursey, Jennifer. “Klein’s ceedings, 1995.
ing Model of Advertising Effectiveness.” In Risque Jeans Ads Dodge Kiddie Porn Rap
Behavioral and Management Science in Marketing, from the Feds.” Advertising Age, November 20, Vranica, Suzanne. “Hard Sell: In 2000, Taste
Harry J. Davis and Alvin J. Silk, eds. New 1995. was Optional.” Wall Street Journal, December 5,
York: Ronald Press, 1978. 2000.
Stiensmeier-Pelster, Joachim, Alice Mar-
Meyer, Wulf-Uwe, Michael Niepel, Udo Ru-
tini, and Rainer Reisenzein. “The Role of Wald, Matthew L. “Shock to Replace Dum-
dolph, and Achim Schötzwohl. “An Experi-
Surprise in the Attribution Process.” Cognition mies in TV Ads on Seat Belt Use.” The New
mental Analysis of Surprise.” Cognition and
and Emotion 9, 1 (1995): 5–31. York Times, January 27, 1999.
Emotion 5, 4 (1991): 295–311.

Oppliger, Patrice A., and Dolf Zillman. “Dis- Thacker, Kathy. “Brassy vs. Classy Creative: Waller, David S. “Attitudes Towards Offen-
gust in Humor: Its Appeal to Adolescents.” Defining Taste in Advertising.” Adweek, March sive Advertising: An Australian Study.” Journal
Humor 10, 4 (1997): 421–37. 22, 1993. of Consumer Marketing 16, 3 (1999): 288–94.

280 JOURNAL OF ADVERTISING RESEARCH September 2003

View publication stats

You might also like