Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

Psychological Review Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1997, Vol. 104, No. 3, 499-523 0033-295X/97/$3.00

The Emergence and Early Development of Autobiographical Memory

Mark L. Howe and Mary L. Courage


Memorial University of Newfoundland

The authors provide a new framework that integrates autobiographical memory with other early
achievements (e.g., gesturing, language, concept formation). In this theory, the emergence and early
development of autobiographical memory does not require the invocation of specialized neurological
or multiple memory mechanisms but rather arises as a natural consequence of developments in
related domains including in the "software" that drives general memory functioning. In particular,
autobiographical memory emerges contemporaneously with the cognitive self, a knowledge structure
whose features serve to organize memories of experiences that happened to "me." Because this
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cognitive self emerges in the 2nd year of life, the lower limit for early autobiographical memories
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

is set at about 2 years, with subsequent accumulation of memories linked to improvements in


children's ability to maintain information in storage.

One problem that has challenged scientists for more than a ware,'' namely, the emergence of the cognitive self. In effect, our
century is the phenomenon of infantile amnesia. Specifically, review provided the kernel for the claim that the development of
why is it that adults are unable to recall childhood events that the cognitive self late in the 2nd year of life (as indexed by
occurred before the age of 2-3 years, yet are able to recall visual self-recognition) provides a new framework around
events that occurred after this age? This question is even more which memories can be organized. With this cognitive advance
perplexing in the light of ample evidence that infants and tod- in the development of the self, we witness the emergence of
dlers are able to encode event information and to store it for autobiographical memory and the end of infantile amnesia.
considerable periods of time (for reviews, see Howe & Courage, In the present article, we develop a more formal theory of
1993; Rovee-Collier & Bhatt, 1993). This apparent discontinu- the emergence and early development of autobiographical mem-
ity is at the heart of the enigma of infantile or childhood amnesia. ory, one that links changes in a number of areas of cognitive,
Historically, most explanations have invoked either retrieval fail- linguistic, and socioemotional functioning, to the personaliza-
ure (e.g., repression, mismatches between initial encoding and tion of event memory. Although we recognize that these develop-
later retrieval contexts) or storage failure (e.g., perceptual or mental shifts do not occur independently, we regard the matura-
neurological immaturity, inadequate encoding) to explain this tion of the cognitive self as a cornerstone in the emergence of
discontinuity in the accessibility (or availability) of early auto- autobiographical memory. In giving this preeminence to the
biographical memories. Invariably, these have failed to meet the cognitive self, our theory stands in marked contrast to a number
rigors of empirical scrutiny and have been rejected, leaving of other contemporary views that give primacy to sociolinguistic
the enigma unresolved (see Howe & Courage, 1993, for a factors in the emergence of autobiographical memory and shift
discussion). its onset (and the corresponding offset of infantile amnesia)
Recently, we (Howe & Courage, 1993) have adopted a new late into the preschool years (Fivush & Hamond, 1990; K. Nel-
approach to this recalcitrant problem. In a review of the litera- son, 1993; Pillemer & White, 1989; Snow, 1990; Tessler &
ture in this area, we argued that there was no evidence for Nelson, 1994). Because we have addressed the fundamental
discontinuity in memory, that the fundamental components of differences between these approaches to this problem before
the information processing system are present and functioning (e.g., see Howe & Courage, 1993), we will not reiterate the
in the human neonate, and that the seeming discontinuity in the issues again here. Instead, the primary purpose of this article is
recall of personally experienced events originates not in the to develop the core of our approach into a more comprehensive
memory ' 'hardware'' per se, but in other areas of memory "soft- theory of the emergence and early development of autobiograph-
ical memory.
To do this, we confront three fundamental issues. First, we
Mark L. Howe and Mary L. Courage, Department of Psychology, revisit the popular proposition that there exists a basic disconti-
Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John's, Newfoundland, nuity in memory during the first 2 years of life and memory
Canada.
after the first 2 years of life. Reviewing recent advances in the
Preparation of this article was supported by Natural Science and
infant memory literature, we show how the same variables that
Engineering Research Council of Canada Grants OGP0003334 and
affect memory in childhood and adulthood also affect memory
OGP0093057.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mark (in the same way) during the first 2 postnatal years. With this
L. Howe, Department of Psychology, Memorial University of Newfound- evidence in hand, and with the evidence presented here as well
land, St. John's, Newfoundland, Canada A1B 3X9. Electronic mail may as earlier (e.g., Howe & Courage, 1993) that neurological and
be sent via Internet to mhowe@morgan.ucs.mun.ca. perceptual changes during this time frame do not uniquely im-

499
500 HOWE AND COURAGE

pact the onset of autobiographical memory, we can safely and continuity-discontinuity, unitary-multiple memory systems,
finally rule out the proposition that infantile amnesia is due structure-process) are unsuitable. Therefore, in order to be
to some qualitative shift in neural hardware during memory clear and to make contact with more traditional usage, we spec-
development. ify what we mean in our use of these terms.
Second, we link our theoretical framework with the literature Consider first the terms continuity-discontinuity and the issue
on other significant changes that occur during the 18-24-month of unitary versus multiple memory systems. Although there is
age range, including how our theory squares with the literature no consensus concerning whether memory development consists
on language development. Specifically, we argue that (a) mem- of a unitary or multiple memory system (or systems) that
ory is fundamentally amodal even in the earliest months of life; changes in a continuous or discontinuous fashion, our analysis
(b) language plays an ancillary, not deterministic, role in the more closely resembles that found in recent dynamic modeling
expression of those memories; (c) the onset of the cognitive efforts (e.g., see Howe, 1994; Howe, Rabinowitz, & Grant,
self is no different from the onset of other cognitive categories 1993; Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Thelen & Smith, 1994). Consis-
or knowledge structures that serve to organize memories early tent with the idea in dynamic models that growth involves both
in life and, that like these other concepts, derive from nonverbal continuities and discontinuities, our claim is that in some cir-
representations and experience that become verbalizable only cumstances, performance aspects of memory development may
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

after they are fully functioning cognitive entities; and (d) recent appear discontinuous when in fact they are being driven by
evidence from the study of receptive language, gesturing, and underlying processes that are continuous. For example, memory
gesture-language transitions confirm the primacy of the devel- development in the age ranges we are discussing is continuous
opment of the self in the acquisition of autobiographical in the sense that the hardware structures (e.g., the underlying
memory. neurological, sensory, and attentional mechanisms; see the sec-
Third, in addition to accounting for how the onset of autobio- tion Neuroscience and Memory Continuity) and software pro-
graphical memory is continuous with early (event) memory, cesses (e.g., strategy use, metamemory, acquired knowledge,
our theory also provides an explanation for the often observed language) that are necessary for the formation of (encoding),
increase in the number of memories adults are able to recall maintenance in (storage), manipulation of (e.g., receding), and
from their childhoods as a function of age in childhood. That subsequent access to (retrieval) information in long-term mem-
is, why should a 20-year-old's memories of early life be more ory (see the section Integrating the Self Into Models of Memory:
numerous from when they were 7 years old than when they were A Dynamic View of Memory and Retention) are functional very
5 years old than when they were 3 years old? Although the onset early in life and can be used in the establishment of all memo-
of the cognitive self sets a lower limit for early autobiographical ries, autobiographical and otherwise. Memory development ap-
memories at around 2 years, one that is consistent with the data pears discontinuous in the sense that autobiographical memory
(e.g., Usher & Neisser, 1993), we have yet to explain these requires the establishment of a functional cognitive structure
subsequent increases in childhood memories that occur follow- pertaining to the self, that is, one that can mediate memories of
ing the onset of the cognitive self. This growth in childhood events that happened to a ' 'me.'' As this cognitive or knowledge
memories has been known for some time (e.g., Waldvogel, structure does not come "on-line" until 18-24 months of age,
1948) but has rarely been accounted for in theories of infantile autobiographical memory appears to be discontinuous at the
amnesia. Our answer to this part of the puzzle is that as children level of memory performance, that is, the establishment of the
get older, the ability to maintain information in storage increases. cognitive self at this age is a necessary precondition for the
This increase in storage maintenance is not confined to autobio- formation and subsequent articulation of autobiographical mem-
graphical memory but typifies all of memory functioning (for ories. However, the establishment of this cognitive sense of self
a review, see Howe & O'Sullivan, in press). is probably continuous. As we argue later in this article, the self
Finally, we conclude by pointing out some of the predictions does not suddenly appear at 18-24 months. Rather, the self,
that fall out of our theory as well as some of the methodological like other knowledge structures, begins organizing from birth
limitations inherent in studying very early memory development. but does not become functional (i.e., affect memory behavior,
In particular, we consider between- and within-individual differ- in this case) until it reaches a certain viability. Thus, at the level
ences in the onset and growth of autobiographical memory as of memory performance, the emergence of autobiographical
well as problems concerning the definition of autobiographical memory appears to be discontinuous. However, in terms of basic
memory, distinguishing autobiographical memory from other cognitive processes, the growth of the self per se, and the growth
episodic or event-like memories, and questions concerning the and development of knowledge structures in general, its develop-
use of nonverbal as well as verbal indices of memory. As it ment is continuous. Ergo, its behavioral manifestation does not
turns out, just because a report is verbal in its expression does require the creation of new laws, neural structures, or mathemat-
not mean it is accurate. Thus, verbal measures may provide ical functions to account for its appearance (see later sections
no more faithful a window on autobiographical memories than in this article). Consistent with our perspective, the development
nonverbal measures. All of these issues provide fodder for future of autobiographical memory does not necessitate the construc-
research. tion of a new memory system. Rather, autobiographical memory
In what follows, we discuss each of these points in turn. can be integrated into an already evolving and dynamic memory
Before doing this, however, we need to clarify our use of certain system, one whose growth is contingent upon the interplay of
terms. Because our model of memory and development is funda- continuity and discontinuity that characterizes development at
mentally dynamic, many of the traditional dichotomies (e.g., all levels of structure and processing.
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 501

Concerning the structure-process distinction, structures can of ingenious tasks that have harnessed the enormous curiosity
exist at various levels, ranging from hardware (e.g., neurological of the human infant whose response repertoire is limited by
structures such as the hippocampus) to software (e.g., knowl- perceptual, cognitive, and motor immaturities. For example, re-
edge structures such as the cognitive self). The use of the term searchers exploiting infants' visual responsiveness and robust
structure to describe both hardware and software highlights the preference for novelty have used habituation, paired-compari-
fact that alterations in either can lead to catastrophic changes son, and other familiarization procedures to show that even
in functioning. For example, a lesion in the hippocampus can neonates can recognize patterns seen previously (for reviews,
lead to profound retrograde amnesia just as the elimination of see Fagan, 1990; Rovee-Collier & Bhatt, 1993; Slater & Mor-
a single link in a complex hierarchical knowledge structure can rison, 1991; J. Werner & Perlmutter, 1979). Baillargeon, DeVos,
lead to the inaccessibility of massive amounts of information and Graber (1989) employed visual preference to index immedi-
linked to that missing segment. Processes that operate on these ate retention and established that 8-month-olds looked longer at
structures do so in part because of the very existence of the an impossible event than a possible event involving the displace-
structures and can in fact lead to alterations in the very structures ment of a toy seen 70 s earlier. Moreover, recognition is not
they serve. This dynamic interchange can be seen, for example, limited to the visual modality. Catherwood (1993) showed that
in the synaptic and dendritic changes that result from experience, 8-month-olds could retain information about the shape of an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ones that alter the "wiring diagram" of the brain and have been object explored haptically over a 5-min retention interval, in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

specifically associated with learning. This alteration facilitates spite of the presence of interfering haptic stimuli. Swain, Zelazo,
new learning because it opens up new opportunities (pathways) and Clifton (1993) used habituation and recovery of a head-
for encoding new experiences (e.g., Greenough, 1993; turn response to demonstrate that neonates exposed postnatally
Greenough, Black, & Wallace, 1987; Quartz & Sejnowski, in to speech sounds retained a memory for those sounds over a
press). As well, processes that lead to the formation and organi- 24-hr interval. Finally, Cernoch and Porter (1985) found that
zation of categories may begin, at least when there is little neonates could recognize their mothers axillary odor 12 hr fol-
information concerning the category, by establishing a set of lowing a familiarization procedure. Although studies using ha-
linear relations. When additional information is infused into this bituation and novelty preference procedures attest to the readi-
structure through acquisition processes, a linear series may no ness of the infant to begin processing information from its earli-
longer be appropriate, and the structure may shift to one that is est days, these methods have certain limitations as procedures
hierarchical. Finally, some processes can operate on a number to study long-term retention (see Rovee-Collier & Bhatt, 1993;
of different structures. For example, the processes of encoding, but see also Bahrick & Pickens, 1995).
storage,' and retrieval as outlined in the section Integrating the Over the past decade a variety of alternative procedures more
Self Into Models of Memory: A Dynamic View of Memory and appropriate to the study of long-term retention processes in the
Retention operate on most, if not all, long-term memory traces. preverbal child have been implemented. In the most comprehen-
Having established, at least in outline form, some of the sive investigation of infant memory to date, Rovee-Collier and
dynamic properties of this theory, we now turn to the main her colleagues employed a mobile conjugate reinforcement para-
business of explicating the details of our proposal more thor- digm to identify the factors that affect 2- to 6-month-olds' im-
oughly. We begin with an overview of very young infant and mediate and long-term retention of an operant foot-kick re-
toddler memory development, one that continues from our ear- sponse. They found that 6-month-olds learned the basic contin-
lier article (Howe & Courage, 1993) and that we believe finally gency faster than 2- and 3-month-olds and that despite similar
puts to rest the notion that infantile amnesia is due to the devel- levels of performance at the end of acquisition, infant age and
opment of multiple memory systems. This is followed by argu- the length of the retention interval were positively correlated.
ments concerning the amodal nature of memory and the role of Further, they found that as research with older children and
language as an expressive device, the primacy of the cognitive adults has shown, retention was affected by factors such as
self in the offset of infantile amnesia, and the role of receptive amount and distribution of practice, the match between the prox-
language and gesture in communicating early memories. We then imal (mobile) and distal (context) cues present at acquisition
outline a general model of memory, describe the development of and those at long-term retention, and exposure to reinstating or
storage-maintenance skill in childhood memory in general, and interfering stimuli following simple forgetting and acquisition,
link it to the accrual of memories across the childhood years. respectively (see Rovee-Collier & Bhatt, 1993; Rovee-Collier &
Finally, we describe methodological issues concerning the study Shyi, 1992, for reviews). The operant conditioning paradigm
of early memory, including problems associated with differenti- has also been effectively used to explore neonates' ability to
ating and defining autobiographical memory. retain auditory information. DeCasper and his colleagues
showed that newborn infants can recognize the prosodic charac-
teristics of a prose passage heard in the last trimester of their
The Continuity of Memory Development
prenatal life and have identified a number of variables that affect
Early Memory and Memory Continuity
1
As an example of the dynamic interplay between structure and pro-
Extant literature on early memory development is replete with cess, the term storage is used to indicate both the structure that is
examples of the infants' proficiency in encoding, storing, and memory storage as well as the processes that lead to memory storage.
retrieving information. This evidence has been garnered with a Whether we are referring to the structure or process aspect of storage
stunning array of procedures and paradigms and an assortment should be clear from the context throughout this article.
502 HOWE AND COURAGE

their recognition of auditory stimuli (DeCasper & Prescott, 1 and 2 years later. However, a longitudinal study of infants'
1984; DeCasper & Spence, 1986, 1991; M. J. Spence & Free- memories of a toy-play event experienced at home when they
man, 1996). were 10 and 14 months old and in a laboratory setting when
Infants' readiness to imitate motor activities performed by an they were 32 and 60 months old, revealed progressively less
adult model has provided another window on the development recollection of the event over time (Myers, Perris, & Speaker,
of long-term retention. Meltzoff and Moore (1994) demon- 1994). Similarly Boyer, Barron, and Farrar (1994) failed to find
strated that 6-week-oIds reproduce certain facial expressions evidence of recollection of a nine-action event sequence learned
and head movements modeled by an adult and retain them over by 20-month-olds and tested after a 12- to 22-month delay.
a 24-hr retention interval. Further advances in infants' recall of Finally, in a recent investigation of children's memories for
action sequences occur during the second half of the first postna- injuries requiring emergency room treatment, Howe, Courage,
tal year, at which time 9-month-olds' show deferred imitation and Peterson (1994b) reported that children who were younger
of novel object-specific actions witnessed (but not performed) than 2 years old expressed recollection of their accidents nonver-
by them 24 hr earlier (Meltzoff, 1988b). Older, 14-month-olds bally after a 5-day retention interval but expressed very little
retained six novel actions in memory for 1 week following live recall of the events 6 months later.
modeling (Meltzoff, 1988c) and for 24 hr following symbolic What is abundantly clear from this literature is that from their
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

modeling using television (Meltzoff, 1988a). Recently, Meltzoff earliest days infants can encode, store, and retrieve a variety of
(1995) found that 14- and 16-month-olds retained modeled mul- information about the stimuli to which they are exposed and the
tiple acts across 2- and 4-month retention intervals. A particular events they experience. Moreover, they can retain this informa-
strength of Meltzoff's research is that the novel activities were tion over considerable periods of time. It is important to note,
modeled only briefly without verbal instruction and were not however, that although infants and very young children retain
performed by the infants themselves prior to the retention test. something of the events and experiences that have occurred in
As infants' limitation of the novel actions was based on the their past, exactly what it is that they remember is an empirical
stored representation of what they had seen previously, they are question. When children possess the narrative skills to talk about
assumed to index recall rather than simple recognition. events in relation to themselves at about 2-3 years of age, it is
Using a similar paradigm, Bauer and her colleagues employed clear that the events have been organized autobiographically in
elicited imitation to show that 11- to 24-month-olds represent memory. If, however, their recognition or recall of prior events
order information in their recall of two to five component event is expressed nonverbally or is limited to single words or simple
sequences and that recall is facilitated and prolonged (to several motor responses (as was the case in many of the studies cited
hours in the youngest infants and up to 6 weeks in the older above), we cannot be sure that their behavior reflects anything
children) if the components of the events contain enabling rela- more than conditioned responding or reenacted motor sequences
tions, are familiar, and are accompanied by verbal cues at the cued by the experimental situation. Although these behaviors
retention test (Bauer & Dow, 1994; Bauer & Hertsgaard, 1993; are an important testament to the enduring effects of early expe-
Bauer & Mandler, 1989, 1992; Bauer & Shore, 1987; Bauer & rience, such evidence that very young children remember certain
Thai, 1990; Bauer & Travis, 1993). Recently, Bauer, Hertsgaard, aspects of their infant experiences should not obscure the fact
and Dow (1994) reported that infants who were 13, 16, and 21 that what they actually remembered was sometimes fragmentary,
months old at the time of acquisition recalled some of these especially following protracted retention intervals.
event sequences after an 8-month retention interval. Recently, several researchers (see Bauer, 1995; Mandler,
Subsequent developments bring further sophistication to 1990b; Meltzoff, 1990, 1995) have attempted to provide some
young children's memory accomplishments, at which time im- closure on this intractable problem by establishing criteria that
provements in performance may be due not to an increase in distinguish between nonverbal methods that simply provide evi-
memory hardware per se, but to a growing refinement in related dence of retention over time (e.g., novelty preference, operant
cognitive and representational processes. For example, De- conditioning) and those that indicate recall of a specific event
Loache and her colleagues found that 24- to 30-month-olds provided in the absence of perceptual support for the event (e.g.,
were better than 18- to 22-month-olds at using landmark cues, deferred or elicited imitation). For example, visual recognition
strategies such as rehearsal and monitoring, and spatial categori- memory inferred from a novelty preference test or from the
zation to aid the retrieval of hidden toys (DeLoache & Brown, mobile conjugate reinforcement procedure does not unambigu-
1983, 1984; DeLoache, Cassidy, & Brown, 1985; DeLoache & ously index recall, as these procedures do not require infants to
Todd, 1988). Finally, studies of young children's memory for generate actions based on stored representations. In contrast,
both naturally occurring and contrived events that occurred dur- deferred and elicited imitation procedures require the infant to
ing their infant and toddler years are retained and under certain reproduce an action or a sequence of actions that they witnessed
conditions can persist for months or years, although with the briefly (but did not perform) on an earlier occasion. As the
passage of time recollection of these events becomes increas- imitated responses are generated solely on the basis of stored
ingly fragmentary. For example, McDonough and Mandler representations and cannot be attributed to recognition or proce-
(1994) found some evidence of recall of single object-specific dural learning, there is an emerging consensus that these tech-
actions in groups of 2-year-olds who had participated in an niques are analogous to cued verbal recall used to assess recall
experiment when they were 11 months old. Ferris, Myers, and in older children and adults (see Bauer, 1995, Mandler, I990b,
Clifton (1990) found that certain aspects of a laboratory proce- and Meltzoff, 1990, for discussions). We return to this point
dure experienced when infants were 6 months old were recalled later (see Some Methodological Issues).
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 503

In any event, although the nature of infants' representational Sandberg, 1994) the potential usefulness of these cues in facili-
processes is not well understood at present, there is evidence tating recall has not been explored systematically (but see De-
that many of the mechanisms that govern storage and retrieval Loache & Todd, 1988). Thus, information provided by temporal
processes in infancy are the same as those that regulate memory order, verbal labels, context, categorization, and spatial location
processes in older children and adults. That is, infant and toddler are encoded in infants' representations of events and can poten-
memory performance varies as a function of (a) the amount tially serve to organize memory and facilitate recall as they do
and distribution of practice (see Rovee-Collier & Bhatt, 1993), for older children and adults. (Later, we argue that the self is
(b) organization (see Bauer, 1995), (c) reinstatement (Howe, like these other organizers in its mnemonic function. For now,
Courage, & Bryant-Brown, 1993), (d) postevent information our point is simply that very early memory shares much in
(Boiler, Grabelle, & Rovee-Collier, 1995; Rovee-Collier, Borza, common with memory in older children and adults.)
Adler, & Boiler, 1993), (e) retroactive interference (see Rovee- The view that many of the basic memory processes are funda-
Collier & Boiler, 1995), ( f ) conditions at encoding (e.g., encod- mentally continuous across development is contrary to the con-
ing specificity; see Rovee-Collier & Shyi, 1992), and (g) the ventional view of infant memory as discontinuous and qualita-
effect of retrieval on subsequent recall (e.g., test effects; Fi- tively different from that of older children and adults. Those who
vush & Hamond, 1989; Myers et al., 1994), to name a few. hold this view look to research with animals, verbally proficient
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

All of these effects are well-known in the literatures on older humans, and clinical case studies of human amnesics to argue
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

children's and adults' memory (e.g., see Howe, 1988, 1991, that there are (at least) two structurally, functionally, and devel-
1995; Howe, Courage, & Bryant-Brown, 1993; Howe & O'Sul- opmentally different memory systems. The first and most primi-
livan, in press; Marche & Howe, 1995; Schneider & Pressley, tive system is said to dominate infant memory for the first 2
1989). Although there is marked continuity in the functioning years of life and has been variously described as an "early"
of the human information-processing system across age (for a (Schacter & Moscovitch, 1984), "procedural" (Nadel & Zola-
similar view, see Bauer, Hertsgaard, & Wewerka, 1995), it is Morgan, 1984; Squire, 1987), "implicit" (Schacter, 1987), or
equally clear that there are also substantial developmental ad- a "habit" (Bachevalier, 1991; Bachevalier & Mishkin, 1984)
vances in memory performance with age (e.g., faster encoding, system capable only of retaining the perceptual and motor skills
storage, and retrieval processes; increases in the ability to main- demonstrated by conditioning and short-term recognition mem-
tain information in storage for longer periods of time) that are
undoubtedly kindled by changes in other cognitive processes
2
(e.g., changes in knowledge, strategies, attentional processes, Throughout this article, we frequently ascribe changes in memory
metamemory).2 performance in childhood to underlying cognitive advances. This is be-
Perhaps the most important aspect of this continuity for our cause there is an enormous literature that provides direct evidence that
thesis about the role of the cognitive self in the onset of autobio- such advances in many aspects of memory performance are causally
linked to these underlying changes in cognitive structures and processes
graphical memory, is the evidence that infants' memories show
(e.g., Bjorklund, 1995; Flavell, Miller, & Miller, 1993; Schneider &
many of the same organizational properties that characterize
Pressley, 1989). This emphasis on changes in software does not, how-
memory processes in older children and adults. For example,
ever, preclude the contribution of alterations in hardware to memory
the work of Bauer and her colleagues indicates that from at least development. For example, mylenization and consequent changes in the
11 months, infants encode temporal order in their representation rate of neural transmission may well be related to changes in speed of
of event sequences and use it to guide recall and that the presen- information processing (e.g., see Case, 1995), including rates of encod-
tation of a verbal cue at retention also facilitates retrieval ing and retrieval. Similarly, neurological development of the frontal lobes
(Bauer & Dow, 1994; Bauer & Hertsgaard, 1993; Bauer & has been linked to advances in inhibitory processes that have been
Mandler, 1989, 1992; Bauer & Shore, 1987; Bauer & Thai, associated, in turn, with increasing proficiency in many areas of cogni-
1990). In addition, studies by Rovee-Collier show that infants tive development (see chapters in Dempster & Brainerd, 1995). Finally,
mature neural connectivity is achieved by both progressive dendritic
encode contextual information into their memories of the mobile
arborization and regressive pruning (e.g., Chugani, Phelps, & Mazziotti,
conjugate reinforcement procedure and that the presence of
1987; P. R. Huttenlocher & De Courten, 1987), events that have been
these cues at retention exert a powerful influence on memory
linked to advances in learning (e.g., Greenough, 1993). Although these
performance (see Rovee-Collier & Shyi, 1992). Finally, there advances in hardware are substantial, and their contribution to cognitive
is ample evidence that, at encoding, from about 3 months infants development undoubtedly significant, direct and causal links have yet
categorize stimuli on the basis of perceptual similarity and by to be established between specific changes at the level of hardware and
about 9 months on the basis of conceptual similarity (e.g., Ei- specific advances in cognitive performance. In any event, the existence
mas & Quinn, 1994; Hayne, Rovee-Collier, & Ferris, 1987; of causal links between neurological developments and memory perfor-
Mandler, Fivush, & Reznicfc, 1987; Mandler & McDonough, mance does not pose a problem for our dynamic model in which ad-
1993; Quinn, 1994; Roberts, 1988; Roberts & Cuff, 1989). vances in neural hardware as well as in cognitive software are both
related to changes at the level of performance. Our point is simply that
Although it has been documented that categories are highly
(a) at the current time, these links are better established empirically for
effective in organizing memory in older children and adults
software than hardware changes, (b) it is very unlikely that dramatic
(e.g., Howe, Brainerd, & Kingma, 1985; Schneider & Pressley,
shifts in hardware underlie the establishment of autobiographical mem-
1989), their role in organizing memory and facilitating long- ory late in the 2nd year of life, and (c) changes in hardware are not
term recall in infants has not been addressed directly. Similarly, likely to target autobiographical memory in isolation, but rather, as the
although infants are sensitive to spatial location (Baillargeon examples given here indicate, are likely to affect many aspects of our
et al., 1989; Diamond, 1985; J. Huttenlocher, Newcombe, & cognitive functioning.
504 HOWE AND COURAGE

ory procedures. In contrast, the memory system of older children based on a pre-explicit memory system to the onset of an explicit
and adults has been described as "later," "declarative," "ex- memory system that occurs between 6 and 12 months of age,
plicit,' ' or a ' 'memory'' system, respectively, capable of storing supposedly heralds (among other things) the onset of recall in
information about experiences and events that are available (or infancy. However, with this time frame in mind, it is difficult to
accessible) to conscious recollection and verbal report. Clearly, reconcile the necessity of such a qualitative shift with the recent
however, the weight of the evidence reviewed here on early findings of Meltzoff and Moore (1994), who reported that 6-
memory does not support a multiple memory approach (see week-old infants showed deferred imitation of facial expres-
Howe & Courage, 1993, for a discussion of these and other sions after a 24-hr delay. They argued that this demonstration
related issues). Consistent with other recent approaches (e.g., of recall memory establishes that infants can generate motor
Bauer et al., 1995), we believe the evidence squares best with actions on the basis of stored representations, an achievement
the idea that the underlying processes controlling memory devel- heretofore reserved for developmentally more advanced infants
opment are continuous with age, although changes in levels of (but seeAnisfeld, 1996).
performance may appear discontinuous. To the extent that the crux of C. A. Nelson's (1995.) argument
is that different kinds of memory performance are supported by
different neural substrates, both of which have parallel develop-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Neuroscience and Memory Continuity


mental histories, his case is cogent. However, not everyone
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Although we have argued that there is a fundamental continu- would agree that the evidence that he has presented justifies the
ity in basic memory processes (encoding, storage, retrieval) stronger claim that there are different and possibly independent
across development, this is not to imply that the diverse exam- memory systems underlying the varieties of memory perfor-
ples of mnemonic competence (variously assessed through rec- mance he discusses (e.g., Roediger, Rajaram, & Srinivas, 1990;
ognition, recall, or savings measures) that we have described are Rovee-Collier, 1990). In fact, as C. A. Nelson himself points
created equal in their cognitive prerequisites, their neurological out, the relationship between brain structure and memory func-
substrates, or their developmental course. In fact, in a recent tion is not strictly isomorphic and that more important, regard-
review and analysis of the literature on the relationship between less of age, memory behavior, or species, the brain works as an
tasks commonly used to assess infant memory and their hypothe- integrated system receiving inputs from and outputting to both
sized prerequisite neural correlates, C. A. Nelson (1995) has local and distal structures.
fashioned a persuasive argument to the contrary. Drawing on The nature of this integrated neurocognitive system is cur-
diverse evidence from the clinical literature on adults with brain rently the subject of active research, the results of which will
injuries (and memory impairment), experimental analogues to serve both to update our knowledge and provoke revisions to
brain injury conditions provided by lesioning nonhuman spe- extant hypotheses about memory processes and their neurologi-
cies, and the developmental literature documenting memory per- cal substrates. As an example, until relatively recently, the hippo-
formance in immature human (intact) and nonhuman (lesioned campus (and its related structures in the medial temporal re-
and intact) subjects, he argued for the existence of multiple gion) was believed to be a late-developing structure, the delayed
memory systems with different developmental time courses. maturation of which was considered the likely candidate for the
Specifically, he contends that certain neurological structures that late onset of an explicit or declarative memory system (Nadel &
develop early in postnatal life (e.g., the hippocampus, striatum, Zola-Morgan, 1984; Schacter & Moscovitch, 1984). However,
cerebellum, olivary-cerebellar complex) are sufficient to sustain there is now evidence that the hippocampus and its surrounding
a "pre-explicit" or procedural memory system that makes pos- structures (with the exception of the dentate area, where devel-
sible the types of recognition memory performance expressed opment is protracted over the first 3 or 4 postnatal years) in
in certain early novelty preferences, habituation, instrumental human and nonhuman primates matures relatively early in post-
and classical conditioning, and visual expectancy tasks. In con- natal life (Berger & Alvarez, 1994; Chugani. 1994; Chugani &
trast, performance on certain other "explicit" memory tasks Phelps, 1986; Kretschmann, Kammradt, Krauthausen, Sauer, &
(e.g., delayed nonmatch to sample, cross-modal recognition, Wingert, 1986; O'Neil, Friedman, Bachevalier, & Ungerleider,
deferred and elicited imitation) and working memory tasks 1986) and is sufficient to mediate the simple, reflexive novelty
(e.g., A-not-B, delayed response), depend, in addition, on later preferences demonstrated in the infancy period. However, al-
developing structures of the medial temporal lobe (e.g., the though components of the hippocampal formation become func-
amygdala), inferior temporal cortical regions, and regions of tional in early infancy, certain neocortical (e.g., inferotemporal)
the prefrontal cortex, which do not begin to come on-line in components and their reciprocal connections with the hippocam-
human infants until the latter half of the first postnatal year. Far pus develop more slowly (Bachevalier, Brickson, & Hagger,
from being complete, these structures continue to mature across 1993; Bachevalier & Mishkin, 1984). These later developing
infancy and childhood and, in tandem with other cognitive and structures operating in synergy with the hippocampus mediate
linguistic achievements, underlie the significant improvements performance on more complex memory tasks that evolve over
in memory performance evidenced during these early years. the period of infancy (e.g., cross-modal recognition, deferred
Critical to the issue at hand, C. A. Nelson argued that the last imitation, and delayed nonmatch to sample). Thus, although
half of the first postnatal year marks a qualitative shift in the the hippocampus plays a changing role in mediating memory
development of early memory that is coincident with (or perhaps performance, it may also contribute to the underlying continuity
enabled by) qualitative changes in neurological maturation in of memory processes.
regions of the developing brain. This shift from early functioning In addition, only a revision to the currently accepted assump-
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 505

tions about the memory abilities or systems that are lost (i.e., Essentially, then, information defining a concept is repre-
declarative or explicit) or spared (i.e., procedural or implicit) sented without reference to the modality through which it was
in amnesia can account for the recently reported demonstration experienced and without reference to its lexical entry. Although
of the acquisition of new declarative memories in patients with lexical entries, as well as other information including instruc-
amnesia (Hamann & Squire, 1995). These authors reported that tions on how to form images of the construct, may be linked to
under conditions of study-only as opposed to the more usual the representation that stands for the concept (or at least "lo-
study-test, patients with amnesia acquired new word knowl- cated" nearby that representation), these instruction sets do not
edge (albeit slowly) and that this knowledge generalized and define the concept. Rather, they simply represent ways in which
persisted over a 1-month delay period. What this implies is that the concept can be encoded and decoded. That is, information
in spite of their brain pathologies, which included damage to can be input and output in spatial (imaginal) or serial (verbal)
the integrity of the medial temporal lobe and the midline dience- formats (among others), but these formats should not be con-
phalic structures, an alteration in the conditions of learning fused with the way in which these concepts are represented in
ameliorated the normally pervasive deficits to declarative mem- memory.
ory apparent in patients with amnesia. Given that we accept this difference between symbols
It is evident that our knowledge of the structure-function (words) and the things they stand for in memory, the question
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

relationships of human memory is itself in a state of develop- remains, what is the role of language in memory in general, and
ment and that the question of whether or not there are multiple what is its role in autobiographical memory in particular? What
memory systems is unresolved. However, as we have maintained the literature seems to indicate is that language facilitates the
previously, in the absence of incontrovertible evidence to the organized expression of memory output (narrative), autobio-
contrary, it is simply more parsimonious to consider the develop- graphical and otherwise. However, we must again be careful not
ment of memory in infancy in terms of a unitary memory sys- to confuse the form of output with the original representation
tem, one that underlies the continuity of memory processes that of that information in memory. That is, although language can
we have documented (see Howe & Courage, 1993, for further convey the contents of memory, as do other modes of expression
discussion). In any event, we wish to emphasize that C. A. (e.g., gesturing, mathematical equations, art), the expression
Nelson's (1995) perspective on the development of infant mem- need not be isomorphic with the memory representation. Thus,
ory, with its critical qualitative shift to the onset of an explicit the often-noted correlation between improvements in autobio-
memory system in the last half of the 1 st postnatal year, in no graphical recall and children's narrative skill may simply reflect
way undermines our contention that the offset of infantile amne- increases in children's ability to express their memories, not
sia and the onset of autobiographical memory, which occurs necessarily improvements in the memories themselves! As we
considerably later at about 2 years of age, is not mediated by a (and others) have pointed out elsewhere (Bauer, 1993; Howe &
qualitative shift in neurological development. Courage, 1993; Mandler, 1990a), because verbal recall is typi-
cally the only measure of autobiographical memory that is used,
Amodal Nature of Memory and the Role of Language it is no wonder that improvements in narrative skill were seen
as indicative of improvements in autobiographical memory itself
As the infant-memory literature illustrates so clearly, language rather than improvements in children's expressive capacity.
is not necessary in order to represent information in memory. Of course, to the extent that language (or the lexical unit) is
In fact, evidence from learning in newborns (e.g., Kaye & associated with the representation of a concept in memory, there
Bower, 1994) as well as older infants (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1984; is the possibility that language, like other cues (e.g., temporal
Meltzoff & Borton, 1979; Meltzoff & Moore, 1994) indicates order [Bauer & Dow, 1994], context [Rovee-Collier & Shyi,
that early memory is amodal. Similar arguments have been for- 1992]), can serve to redintegrate or reinstate the trace at the
warded in the later-childhood and adult-memory literatures such time of recall. Such verbal reinstatement has been shown in
that information is not only thought to be stored amodally but young children (e.g., Howe, Courage, & Bryant-Brown, 1993).
also in a nonlinguistic format. Although it is by no means clear Although "narrative rehearsal" of events, for example with a
precisely what form memory representations take—neural nets parent, should have similar effects, it is not known at this time
(e.g., White, 1992), proposition lists (e.g., Anderson, 1978), whether they, in fact, do (e.g., see Fivush, 1994; Goodman,
feature vectors (e.g., Flexser & Tulving, 1978), connectionist Quas, Batterman-Faunce, Riddlesberger, & Kuhn, 1994). In
networks (e.g., Fodor & Pylyshyn, 1988; Hanson & Burr, 1990), fact, it is well known that reconstructions of events through
and other distributed memory systems (e.g., Murdock, 1982) — conversations with others often result in distortions of fact, ones
it has become obvious over the past few decades that this age- that are consistent with the recaller's and to some extent the
old problem cannot be resolved by using language-based repre- listener's current beliefs and expectations (e.g., D. P. Spence,
sentational units at any age (e.g., see Anderson, 1978, 1979; 1982; Wagenaar, 1988, 1990).
Palmer, 1978; Pylyshyn, 1979). In fact, as Mandler (1992) and
others (e.g., Karmiloff-Smith, 1992) have pointed out, concep- The Emergence of the Cognitive Self, Not Language,
tual representational systems are present and operating well Portends Autobiographical Memory
before language is available for the expression of those concepts.
Indeed, the acquisition of various linguistic terms is predicated
Emergence of the Cognitive Self
on the child having previously formed concepts to which lin- When children are about 18 to 24 months old, a new organizer,
guistic units refer (e.g., Mandler, 1992). the cognitive self, becomes available to facilitate autobiographi-
506 HOWE AND COURAGE

cal recall. We have argued that only with this achievement do and self-touching) when confronted with their images and. by
event memories become organized autobiographically. Although about 22 months of age, correctly label the image. Collectively,
conjecture about the nature and function of the self has a long these behaviors provide a consistent picture of an infant who
and hoary tradition, and remains an elusive concept with a multi- recognizes the mirror image as me.
plicity of definitions, its developmental course is still the focus It should be noted that visual self-recognition is only one
of active research (e.g., Cicchetti & Beeghly, 1990; Damon & facet of the self-concept, one that is relatively easy to operationa-
Hart, 1988; Kopp & Brownell, 1991; Neisser, 1993). Years of lize for research with infants. The self-concept (and self-aware-
reflection from theorists of diverse perspectives (Bowlby, 1969; ness) implies more than recognition of one's physical features
Darwin, 1877; Freud, 1959; James, 1961; Mahler, Pine, & Berg- and is a fundamental aspect of social cognitive development that
man, 1975; Mead, 1934; Piaget, 1954) has yielded a consensus has nascent roots in the early weeks of life and continued evolu-
on two important issues still germane to modern developmental tion throughout childhood and adolescence (see Damon & Hart,
theories of the self. First, most agree that at birth infants are 1988, for a review). However, many authors agree that the
probably unaware of their separateness from the environment achievement of mirror self-recognition marks an important de-
and acquire this awareness following a gradual process of indi- velopmental milestone in the 2nd year of life (Asendorpf &
viduation, which likely begins in the early weeks of life (Melt- Baudonniere, 1993; Butterworth, 1990; Kagan, 1981; Lewis,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

zoff, 1990; Neisser, 1993). Second, there exist at least two 1986; Meltzoff, 1990; Neisser, 1993) and that a critical step is
fundamental (though complexly interrelated) facets of the self, reached when children are able to represent themselves as an
the /, a subjective sense of the self as a thinker, knower, and object of knowledge and imagination. Thus mark-directed be-
causal agent, and the me, an objective sense of the self with havior reflects more than self-recognition per se and may, in
the unique and recognizable features and characteristics that fact, signal a more pervasive transition in cognitive development
constitute one's self-concept (but see Neisser, 1988, for an alter- (also see Karmiloff-Smith, 1992).
native perspective on the facets of the self that focuses on infor- In contrast to the substantial corpus of research on the self
mation analysis). The mechanisms and processes underlying the ax object of knowledge (the me), empirical work on the self as
growing awareness of the self in both (all) its aspects are the subject of experience (the /) has been slower to accrue. Al-
subject of investigation in both theoretical and empirical do- though it is widely recognized that this aspect of the self is
mains (Bullock & Lutkenhaus, 1990; Case, 1991; Cicchetti, fundamental to infants' growing self-awareness and sense of
1991; Damon & Hart, 1982, 1988; Emde, Biringen, Clyman, & agency (e.g., Kagan, 1981), and that its development must be
Oppenheim, 1991; Harter, 1983; Lewis, 1991; Lewis & Brooks- explicated for a full understanding of the self system, it has
Gunn, 1979; Meltzoff, 1990; Neisser, 1991; Pipp, Fischer, & been more difficult to operationalize. Only relatively recently,
Jennings, 1987). As a detailed consideration of this literature have researchers devised a variety of "self-involvement-in-ac-
is beyond the scope of this article and has been reviewed in tion" tasks (i.e., those in which the child recognizes the out-
greater detail elsewhere (Howe & Courage, 1993), we restrict come as a product of his or her own participation) that articulate
ourselves to a-summary of the critical features of this research. this aspect of the self as subject of experience more precisely
Until recently, empirical investigation of the self in infancy (Bullock & Lutkenhaus, 1990; Meltzoff, 1990; Pipp et al.,
has focused almost exclusively on the self ax object of experi- 1987). In spite of the paucity of empirical work, speculation
ence (i.e., the me). Particular attention has been paid to the has been rampant that the infant's sense of self as subject of
development of visual self-recognition assessed by observing experience commences long before the onset of self-recognition,
the infants' reactions to their images in mirrors, photos, and and has primitive origins in the processes of sensory perception,
movies (Amsterdam, 1972; Berenthal & Fischer, 1978; Bul- self-control (e.g., Butterworth, 1990; Neisser, 1993), and imita-
lock & Lutkenhaus, 1990; Darwin, 1877; Dixon, 1957; Johnson, tion (Meltzoff, 1990) in the early weeks and months of life.
1983; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Lewis, Brooks-Gunn, & Although a detailed review of this literature is beyond the scope
Jaskir, 1985; Lewis, Sullivan, Stanger, & Weiss, 1989; Pipp et of this article, we provide a brief overview of some recent
al., 1987; Preyer, 1893; Priel & De Schonen, 1986; Schulman & theoretical perspectives along with supporting empirical re-
Kaplowitz, 1977). Collectively, these studies reveal that from search on the development of the self as subject of experience.
about 3 months of age infants are both attentive and positive Current theories of the early development of the / aspect of
toward their mirror image and within several months can dis- the self vary in the emphasis placed on the primacy of percep-
criminate their facial features from that of another infant (Fadil, tual, social, and affective factors in the structuring of this "pre-
Moss, & Bahrick, 1993). By about 9 months they begin to symbolic" self in the 1st year of life (see Cicchetti & Beeghly,
show awareness of the contingency cues provided by the tandem 1990; Kopp & Brownell, 1991; Neisser, 1993, for reviews).
movement of the image with themselves and use these cues for One of the most comprehensive treatments of the subject has
play and imitation. They also know something of the reflective been provided by Neisser (1988, 1993) who proposed a cogni-
properties of mirrors and turn to locate spatially objects and tive analysis of the self based on the varieties of information to
people that they see reflected. Ml self-recognition of the mirror which infants (and older children and adults) have access, each
image as their own occurs at about 18 months of age when of which specifies a different type of self. Of particular relevance
infants first respond to a spot of rouge that has been covertly to the period of infancy are the ecological self (an awareness
applied to their noses by touching their own noses rather than the of where one is, what one is doing, and what one has done)
mirror image. Coincident with mark-directed behavior, infants and the interpersonal self (awareness of social affordances in
begin to show self-consciousness (shy smiling, gaze aversion, interaction with others). These have their origins early in in-
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 507

fancy and are fundamentally rooted in perception and perceptual change in one's sense of self occurs at about this time. Although
processes (see J. J. Gibson, 1966). For example, data relevant the exact nature of this change remains elusive, it is clear that
to the ecological self come from studies showing infants' dis- the self at this point achieves whatever critical mass is necessary
crimination of and responsiveness to looming objects (Carroll & to serve as an organizer and regulator of experience (also see
Gibson, 1981), parallel flow in the optic array specifying real Emde et al., 1991; Kagan, 1981). The fact that this achieve-
(Lee & Aronson, 1974) or illusory (Butterworth, 1990) move- ment in self-awareness (recognition) is followed shortly by the
ment, occlusion (Baillargeon, Spelke, & Wasserman, 1985), onset of autobiographical memory is, we believe, more than
and perceptual affordances (E. J. Gibson et al., 1987). The coincidental.
foundations of at least a rudimentary interpersonal self in the As we pointed out earlier, we view the establishment of the
early months of life can be witnessed in infants' imitation of cognitive self as no different than the establishment of other
facial expressions (Meltzoff & Moore, 1994), protoconvers- conceptual categories or knowledge structures that serve to or-
ations (Bateson, 1975), and affective exchanges with their care- ganize memories. That is, although the cognitive self permits
takers (Stern, 1985, 1993). A third aspect of the self (although event memories to become personally organized with respect
not directly perceived), is the conceptual self, which enables to me, this is no different from children's ability to organize
infants to begin to take themselves as objects of thought. The information about "dogginess" with memories of other ani-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ability to think explicitly about oneself probably begins to mals. And, like most other concepts and categories acquired
emerge with the realization that one is the object of another early in life, the self emerges prior to the use of productive
person's attention, an achievement unlikely to appear before the language about that concept. For example, 9-month-old infants
end of the 1 st year. At this time, shared attention to objects and can acquire the concept of animal prior to the use of language
events becomes a common mode of parent—child interaction for that category (e.g., Mandler & McDonough, 1993; Roberts,
(Tomasello, 1993; Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 1993), an activ- 1988). As Slobin (1985) points out, children must already have
ity that serves to foster the infant's nascent understanding of concepts of objects and events, as well as relational notions
what other people are attending to or talking about—the object about them, in order for language to be acquired (also see
of attention sometimes being the child himself or herself. Mandler, 1992). In a similar vein, Bates (1990) noted that the
Another contemporary viewpoint has been contributed by acquisition of any natural language requires a preexisting theory
Meltzoff (1990). He has argued that studies of (a) social mir- of self. Consistent with this view, and as we have argued else-
roring (in which 14-month-old infants preferentially attended where (Howe & Courage, 1993), the concept of the self pre-
to adults who imitated their actions), (b) social modeling (in cedes any elaboration children might achieve once mastery of
which infants from birth are able to imitate certain modeled the language enables them to think and talk about 7 and to
actions), and (c) self-practice (in which infants use imitation elaborate their categorical knowledge of me. These requisite
to represent information to the self through reenactment of past linguistic achievements begin with the emergence of the pro-
events) provide insights into the origins of the self that are nominal references to /, me, and you, which appear in a child's
rooted in the rudimentary imitative skills of the neonate. These productive vocabulary between 22-24 months (Fenson et al.,
provide a foundation from which subsequent development of 1994) and, as longitudinal studies have shown, about a month
the self proceeds. He has suggested that beyond this, imitative after the achievement of visual self-recognition (Berenthal &
interchanges between infants and others provide a unique vehicle Fischer, 1978; Bullock & Lutkenhaus, 1990; Lewis & Brooks-
for elaborating the similarity between self and other, as well as Gunn, 1979; Pipp et al., 1987).
for the understanding that others, like the self, have thoughts,
intentions, and feelings. Like Neisser's (1993) vision of the Autobiographical Memory Subsequent to the
conceptual self, these insights can be considered as primitive
Emergence of the Self
building blocks for the child's developing theory of the mind.
In sum, contemporary theories of the development of the self There is a substantial literature that shows that toddlers begin
are consistent in the position that important foundations of the to recount their memories of personally experienced (i.e., auto-
development of the self-as-subject are evident in early infancy, biographical) events as soon as they acquire the rudiments of
perhaps from birth. Although the two aspects of the self system productive language, even before they have reliable control over
develop in tandem over the first 2 years, by the time the infant the use of past-tense markers (e.g., "I see big fish," following
shows mirror self-recognition she or he already has a healthy a trip to the aquarium). In one of the earliest studies of toddlers'
self-awareness (see Butterworth, 1990). recollections, Sachs (1983) reported the case of Naomi, who
To date, the issue of precisely how the various facets of the began to talk about events in her immediate past when she was
self become integrated into a full self system developmentally 17 months old and about more distant events when she was 26
has not been completely explicated. However, what is critical months old. Similarly, K. Nelson (1989) reported that 21-
to our thesis that a sense of self is fundamental to the develop- month-old Emily's spontaneous narratives from the crib con-
ment of autobiographical memory is that by 18-24 months, tained numerous references first to routine and later (after she
infants have a concept of themselves that is sufficiently viable was 25 months old) to specific personal events that had hap-
to serve as a referent around which personally experienced pened. In a longitudinal study of 5 toddlers between 24 and 31
events can be organized in memory. In fact, the contributors to months of age. Miller and Sperry (1988) noted that all of the
a special issue of Developmental Review, ' 'Development of the children were able to recount events that had happened at least
Self" (Kopp & Brownell, 1991) concur that a significant 1 day previously. Further, over the 7-month interval, the toddlers'
508 HOWE AND COURAGE

rate of talk about personal experiences doubled, their production disagreement that what younger children recall is highly accu-
of temporally ordered sequences increased substantially, and rate and durable over considerable periods of time.
they provided more information spontaneously. Interestingly, In spite of the impressive evidence of very young children's
these early narratives were primarily about negative events and recall of personally experienced events, there are those who
often contained evaluative content (e.g., "Me big fall down"), argue that autobiographical memory does not emerge until the
although much of this evaluation was conveyed nonverbally late preschool years (K. Nelson, 1989, 1993; Perner & Huffman,
through gesture. More recently, Howe et al. (1994b) reported 1995; Pillemer & White, 1989). This proposition that autobio-
that 2-year-old children were able to provide coherent and de- graphical memory undergoes a protracted period of development
tailed recall of traumatic injuries and ensuing emergency room has its origin in two different theoretical perspectives. Critical
treatments that they had experienced several days previously. to these is the assumption that there is a fundamental distinction
The children's recall (at least for the central details of the inci- between event memory and autobiographical memory, one that
dents) was still robust when tested 6 and 12 months later (Howe rests on the child's acquisition of sophisticated representational
et al., 1994a, 1994b). Fivush, Gray, and Fromhoff (1987) inter- skills that permit him or her to ". . . use the verbal representa-
viewed 2.5-year-olds about a variety of novel events (e.g., a trip tion of another person to set up a representation in one's own
to the zoo) that they had experienced either in the recent (less mental representation system, thus recognizing the verbal ac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

than 3 months) or more distant (more than 3 months) past. count as a reinstatement of one's prior experience" (K. Nelson,
Although the children recalled information about only half of 1993, p. 12), or to possess autonoetic (i.e., self-knowing) con-
the events probed, what they did recall was highly accurate. sciousness such that "to remember something as experienced
Further, there was no difference in the amount recalled at the requires there to be a mental representation of the fact that the
two times. Finally, Hudson (1993) found that 24- to 30-month- event is known because it has been experienced" (Perner &
olds recalled specific information about events that had occurred Ruffman, 1995, p. 543). Thus, in the former (social interaction)
up to 10 months previously, including one 26-month-old child model, autobiographical memory evolves in the wake of conver-
who recalled an event that had occurred when she was just 1 sational interactions between the child and significant others:
year old. Although these studies indicate that toddlers have accu- in particular, elaborative mothers. As a consequence, the child
rate and durable recall of personally experienced events, their acquires narrative skills that, over and above providing an outlet
reports are often fragmentary and heavily dependent on ques- for the reporting of personal experiences, serve to structure
tions and prompts from the listener to elicit additional detail how these experiences are represented in memory. In the latter
available in memory but not spontaneously reported. (autonoetic) model, autobiographical memory becomes possi-
In contrast, older preschoolers and school-age children pro- ble only following achievements in metacognition, whereby chil-
vide better structured and more cohesive narrative recollections dren are able to have recollective experiences of remembering
(i.e., containing more referential detail, orienting information, (as opposed to simply knowing about) past events. According to
evaluative comment, use of temporal markers) about personally Perner and Ruffman, such recollective experiences are unlikely
experienced events than do younger children, and further, they before the age of 3-5 years because before that age children
require less prompting and support from the listener to do so do not understand the relationship between informational access
(see Fivush, 1993; Fivush, Haden, & Adam, 1995). However, (e.g., seeing) and knowledge. Although Perner and Ruffman do
as memorial abilities are confounded with linguistic proficiency not specify the mechanism or mechanisms that might underlie
and narrative skill in studies of autobiographical memory across this metacognitive advance, they suggest that the social interac-
the preschool years, it is not clear whether (or to what extent) tion model in general, and mothers' elaborated talk about past
older children's recall of personal events is superior.to that of episodes in particular, might well play a significant role in the
younger children or is simply an artifact of their more advanced evolution of autonoetic consciousness, autobiographical mem-
narrative skills. For example, Hamond and Fivush (1990) found ory, and in children's developing theory of the mind.
no effect of age (2.5 or 4.5 years) or retention interval (6 months Although both of these models may be correct in describing
or 18 months) on either the accuracy or amount of children's subsequent software developments that may lead to increases in
recall of a trip to Disney world. Similarly, Sheingold and Tenney storage maintenance of autobiographical memories, they do not
(1982) found no effect of age or retention interval on children's provide an explanation for the lower limit or onset of personal-
recall of the birth of a sibling. In contrast, other researchers have ized memories. Further, despite the growth of interest in the
found developmental differences in the amount, consistency, and experiential aspect of autobiographical remembering (the auto-
durability (but not the accuracy) of information recalled (Fi- noetic component), it is not clear that such an experience is
vush, 1993; Howe et al., 1994a, 1994b; Pillemer & White, 1989; critical to the establishment of autobiographical memory.
Todd & Perlmutler, 1980). However, future research may con- Clearly, although any complete theory of autobiographical mem-
firm that the discrepancy is more apparent than real, perhaps ory must incorporate this experience of remembering (e.g., see
attributable to procedural differences such that the amount of Conway, 1996), the existence of personalized memories is not
structure provided (e.g., free versus cued recall), the distinc- contingent on such experiencing.
tiveness of the event in question (e.g., unique versus routine),
the type of information considered (e.g., central or peripheral), Receptive Language and Gesturing
or interviewer variables (e.g., mother versus stranger). Interest-
ingly, in spite of the debate over whether younger children recall We have argued throughout this article that the recognition
less than older children or simply report less, there is little of the cognitive self is the critical event that launches autobio-
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 509

graphical memory and that subsequent achievements in produc- of self-referent pronouns before about 18 months (Fenson et
tive language provide an expressive outlet for these memories al., 1994). More important, it is also in this time frame that
that serve to preserve (through reinstatement) or could poten- words comprehended, but not spoken, can cue preverbal infants'
tially alter (through reconstructive processes) memory records recall of temporally ordered events (Bauer & Hertsgaard, 1993),
of personally experienced events. However, it would be naive to thus revealing a function for receptive language in the organiza-
assume that language development commences with its produc- tion of event retrieval at this time.
tion in speech. There is extensive documentation that infants are Coincident with the onset of language comprehension, infants
receptive to (and can remember) linguistic input from the last begin to use the gestural mode to communicate. The first ges-
trimester of their prenatal life (DeCasper & Spence, 1986). In tures used in intentional communication at about 9 months ex-
addition, they are remarkably proficient in the perception and press requests (proto-imperatives) and showing and giving
discrimination of speech contrasts and attentive to aspects of (proto-declaratives) followed by pointing at about 11 months
language that facilitate subsequent achievements in comprehen- (Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979,; Bates,
sion and production late in the 1st postnatal year (Aslin, Pi- Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975). More recent developmental stud-
soni, & Jusczyk, 1983; DeCasper & Spence, 1986; Kuhl, 1987). ies of symbolic gesturing (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Good-
Further, there is evidence that in many areas of language develop- wyn & Acredolo, 1993) revealed that infants' use of gesturing
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ment comprehension of word, grammatical form, and gesture to communication expands rapidly in the 2nd postnatal year, at
predates their production in speech and action (see Bates, which time they produce many gesture—referent pairs that are
O'Connell, & Shore, 1987, for a review). Given this early pre- used symbolically to denote objects, requests, attributes, replies,
disposition to process and remember linguistic forms, it be- and events. However, such gestures are used as transitional forms
hooves us to consider whether the self might be represented in and replaced by appropriate verbal labels as soon as they can
receptive language or gesture (e.g., self-pointing) prior to its be articulated, perhaps serving, as H. Werner and Kaplan (1963)
appearance in visual self-recognition when the infant is about suggest, to ease the infant from sensorimotor to representational
18 months and in productive language at about 22 months. functioning. Consistent with this progression from gesture to
To anticipate the outcome of the brief overview of the litera- word, it is our contention that infants first reveal the emergence
ture on receptive language and gesture that follows, we provide of their cognitive self by using the gestural mode (i.e., mark-
evidence that just as comprehension precedes production in lan- directed behavior to their mirror image) and that only later is
guage development in general, comprehension (or receptive un- this self-recognition expressed in spoken language.
derstanding) of the sense of self (as evidenced in mirror self- Infants' comprehension of self-referent pronouns and their
recognition and, coincidentally, in self-pointing and the compre- use of the gestural mode to communicate is accompanied by
hension of self-referent pronouns) precedes its production in the appearance of another marker of the developing sense of
spoken language about the self by several months. Further, we self: first-person pointing. It is noteworthy that although third-
contend that this discrepancy between nonverbal comprehension person pointing (you) is evident at 9-11 months, first-person
and verbal expression marks a transition in the development of pointing (/ or me) is not seen before about 18 months (Bates
the infant's concept of me, a period during which the cognitive et al., 1987; Pettito, 1993). Bates et al. (1987) suggest that this
and linguistic components of the self as object are being compre- discontinuity between first- and third-person pointing reflects
hended, organized, and consolidated. Finally, we argue that only an underlying uncertainty in the emerging understanding of the
when this consolidation has occurred will the self be sufficiently self. Specifically, if third-person pointing indicates that the infant
viable to serve as an organizer for autobiographical memory. has made a distinction between self and object (/) as H. Werner
More than coincidentally, we believe, it is at this time that young and Kaplan (1963) have argued, then the delay in first-person
children begin to tell us about the personal events of their lives pointing might reflect the fact that the cognitively more complex
that they recall. concept of self as object (me) is not in place. Therefore, it seems
Although infants are able to discriminate speech units with reasonable to conclude that the coincident advances witnessed in
increasing sophistication in the first 6 months of life, it is not the gestural achievements of self-pointing and self-recognition
until about 8 or 9 months that they show comprehension of the and in infants' beginning comprehension of self-referent pro-
meaning of words (Bates et al., 1987; Fenson et al., 1994; nouns, all of which emerge when infants are about 18 months
Goldin-Meadow, Seligman, & Gelman, 1976; Oviatt, 1985) old, signal that at some level the infants' sense of self is being
such that by 13 months they have a receptive vocabulary of consolidated in this time frame. In any event, the marked discon-
about 17-97 words (but see Tomasello & Mervis, 1994, for tinuity (at the level of performance) in the onset of first- and
some scepticism). In any event, by their first birthday infants third-person referential pointing may well reflect the infants'
can readily acquire (and express in visual recognition and in uncertainty about the nature of the self and the changing per-
pointing) new word meanings through instruction (Oviatt, 1985; spective needed to sort out / and you, which are only expressed
Reznick & Goldfield, 1992; Woodward, Markman, & Fitzsim- some months later in productive language.
mons, 1994), an achievement that anticipates a vocabulary spurt Goldin-Meadow and her colleagues (Goldin-Meadow, Ali-
in language comprehension that parallels the spurt in production bali, & Church, 1993; Morford & Goldin-Meadow, 1992) attach
(Reznick & Goldfield, 1992). However, although parental re- particular developmental significance to the discovery of dis-
ports indicate that infants recognize their own names at about crepancies between gesture and speech, regarding them as
8 months (which does not in and of itself signal mature recogni- "windows on the mind" of a child in transitional states of
tion of the self), there is little evidence of the comprehension knowledge acquisition. They have shown that in certain prob-
510 HOWE AND COURAGE

lem-solving and language acquisition domains, gesture-speech some of the key factors that contribute to memory development
mismatches signal that a child is struggling to consolidate fledg- (e.g., changes in strategies, knowledge) are known, it is not
ling concepts preparatory to advancing into a new knowledge clear from a basic-process perspective why such improvements
state. When a child is in such a transitional state, knowledge occur. That is, although a number of variables have been identi-
that can he expressed in the gestural mode is still unavailable fied as important in memory development (for a review, see
for (and thus mismatched with) expression in the speech mode. Schneider & Pressley, 1989), these variables, by necessity, exert
These observations concerning gesture-speech mismatches their effects al the level of basic processes (i.e., storage and
have potential implications for the development of the concept retrieval). Although we know that these factors, for example
of the self and its expression in self-language and in the verbal strategic rehearsal, can lead to better recall, it is not clear
recounting of self-experiences (i.e., autobiographical memo- whether these effects arise because the stability of traces in
ries). Specifically, it is only following the emergence of the storage increases, the ease of retrieving these traces increases,
cognitive self as expressed through gestures such as mirror self- or both. It is only when we discover what basic process (or
recognition and self-pointing that infants are able to consolidate processes) mediates these improvements in retention that we
their burgeoning knowledge about the self and to move on to a gain a clearer understanding of the nature of memory develop-
new level of competence that enables them to use self-language ment in general, and autobiographical memory in particular.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and narrative skills to report autobiographical memories in


speech. Integrating the Self Into Models of Memory: A
In sum, we propose that infants first reveal their awareness
Dynamic View of Memory and Retention
of the cognitive sense of self nonverbally. Beginning at about
18 months, they are able to use the gestural mode (i.e., mark- A number of researchers investigating the role of the self in
directed behavior to their mirror image; self-referent pointing) memory have concluded that the self, inasmuch as it is an orga-
to communicate an early awareness of the self that is the corner- nized knowledge structure, can be used to mediate memories
stone of autobiographical memory. Also in this time frame (and (for an overview, see Greenwald & Banaji, 1989). Like any
consistent with other developmental phenomena in which com- other knowledge structure (e.g., see Bjorklund, 1987), the self
prehension precedes production; see Bates et al., 1987; Goldin- can be used to interpret and organize incoming information.
Meadow et al., 1993), infants begin to understand self-referent Indeed, Klein and Kihlstrom (1986) proposed a single-factor
pronouns that only later, between about 22 and 24 months, can theory of organization and memory, one that accounted for both
be expressed in productive language about the self. This evi- self and semantic encoding effects. This view that the self be-
dence suggests strongly that during the latter half of the 2nd haves like any other organizational scheme in memory is consis-
postnatal year of life, infants are consolidating the cognitive, tent with our position that autobiographical memory is function-
linguistic, and (as noted earlier in The Emergence of the Cogni- ally no different than any other type of memory. If this is true,
tive Self, Not Language, Portends Autobiographical Memory) then surely autobiographical memory must be accommodated
affective components of their sense of self as object of experi- by current, mainstream models of memory.
ence (me). However, it is also clear that there is a marked In this section, we illustrate how this can be accomplished
discontinuity (in terms of performance) in the translation of using a recently developed model of how memories are initially
this awareness from gestural to verbal expressions, a discrep- acquired and how they are maintained over long-term retention
ancy that portends a readiness to move on to a more advanced intervals. Before articulating the details of this trace-integrity
level of self-awareness that includes the ability to describe the framework it is important to note that the model is dynamic
experiences of the self autobiographically. inasmuch as (a) mathematically, the state of the memory trace
at time MS a simple function of the current conditions plus the
The Accumulation of Autobiographical Memories state of the trace at time r — 1 (for an exegesis of the Markovian
assumptions associated with this model, see Brainerd, Howe, &
Across Childhood
Desrochers, 1982; Brainerd, Howe, & Kingma, 1982; Howe &
We now turn to our explanation for the often observed in- O'Sullivan, in press, and for a general description of dynamic
crease in childhood memories that occurs after the onset of the models, including Markovian assumptions, see Bogartz, 1994;
self. Specifically, although the onset of the cognitive self can Howe & Rabinowitz, 1994); (b) the individual trace evolves
explain the origin of autobiographical memory, setting the lower as a consequence of the interaction between the nominal envi-
limit for such memories at around 2 years of age, the question ronmental stimuli or events and information already stored in
remains, why is it that adult memories of childhood become memory (see Howe & Brainerd, 1989); (c) the slorage medium
more numerous across the early years in childhood? 1b antici- itself is volatile, with trace reorganization and alteration oc-
pate the outcome of this section, the evidence we review shows curring on a regular basis (also see Tulving, 1984; for similar
that throughout childhood, the ability to maintain information arguments in the area of autobiographical memory, see Rob-
in storage increases, not only in autobiographical memory, but inson, 1996); and (d) storage and retrieval processes, although
in all of memory (Howe & O'Sullivan, in press). clearly different, interact not just in the sense that encoding and
We have already reviewed considerable evidence illustrating retrieval contexts need to overlap for successful recollection,
that even young toddlers can retain information over protracted but so too do receding and retrieval contexts (see Estes, 1988;
periods of time and evidence that there exist clear age improve- Tulving, 1984). Also, the advantages of this approach include
ments in children's long-term retention performance. Although its consistency with the assumptions of any number of other
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 511

formal models of memory (for a review, see Howe & O'Sulli- of traces through die mechanism of redintegration (e.g., Horo-
van, in press). Moreover, the model applies to the acquisition witz & Prytulak, 1969; Howe & Brainerd, 1989; Howe, Cour-
and retention data generated from any number of long-term age, & Bryant-Brown, 1993). That is, traces can be (re)con-
memory paradigms (e.g., free and cued recall, paired-associates structed simply as a function of being tested, often because of
recall) across different type of materials (e.g., picture-word the cues (both internal and external) that are generated in the
lists, categorized lists, event sequences, stories) and ages (from testing situation. Because examples of retrieval related reminis-
infancy to late adulthood; see Howe & O'Sullivan, in press). cence are relatively well-known in the literature (e.g., see
Theoretically, the trace-integrity framework turns on a simple Payne's, 1987, review of hypermnesia), we do not reiterate
idea, namely, the disintegration-redintegration hypothesis. them here. However, because storage related reminiscence pro-
Here, as in any number of other theories, memory traces consist cesses are less well-known (but see Brainerd, Reyna, Howe, &
of primitive elements (e.g., features, nodes) that are bound to- Kingma, 1990; Chechile, 1987; Howe & O'Sullivan, in press),
gether (integrated) to form cohesive structures (also see review we provide a somewhat detailed illustration of them, at least as
in Estes, 1988). Trace formation at acquisition is driven by expressed in this and related models. We do this in part because
integration tendencies in which storage and retrieval simply rep- it illustrates the dynamic link between storage and retrieval
resent different aspects of the continuum of integration. That is, processes in this model. As well, our discussion illustrates how
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

early in the trace-acquisition process, the central problem is reconstruction involves changes at the level of storage, a phe-
one of encoding primitive elements and developing a stable nomenon that has taken on considerable importance in recent
representation of the to-be-remembered item or event in storage. models of autobiographical recall of true and false memories
Once a stable representation has formed, later acquisition pro- (e.g., Conway, Collins, Gathercole, & Anderson, 19%).
cesses are concerned with the establishment of a reliable re- Recall that simply because a trace has a recall probability of
trieval route, one that produces the item on demand. That storage zero at the beginning of a retention test does not mean that that
and retrieval are viewed as constituents of a single underlying item has vanished from memory. Rather, because it can also
(trace integrity) memory continuum is consistent with other mean that the trace is simply indistinguishable from other traces
contemporary theories (e.g., Chechile, 1987; Metcalfe, 1990), in the background, it is possible that there still exists some level,
particularly those of the encoding specificity variety (e.g., Ack- albeit low, of trace integrity. Redintegration of some of these
erman, 1987; Tulving, 1984). primitive elements and their bonds during test opportunities can
The critical factor determining memorability, then, is the de- lead to increased integration and recovery of information in
gree to which the "glue" that provides cohesion among the storage. Again, although the idea of altering information at the
trace elements is intact. During initial learning, this cohesion level of storage may seem novel, it has been around for some
increases to a point at which an item can be recalled with some time (e.g., see Howe & Brainerd, 1989, for a review, and Pia-
nominal probability greater than zero (an index that, at the very get & Inhelder's, 1973, discussion of reconstructive memory).
least, sufficient features have been encoded that a trace has been For example, historically, the idea that changes in memory, in
established in storage) followed by a point at which an item particular storage failures, were not equated with trace erasure
can be recalled withp = 1.00 (an index of trace retrievability but rather a progressive modification, can be traced back to
or item accessibility). During any retention interval, trace integ- Gestalt psychology. Here, memory architectures consisted of
rity can (a) remain at asymptote (no change in storage or re- fluid structures that were subject to change while resident in
trieval), (b) dissipate such that the recall probability remains storage. In particular, Kohler (1929, 1941) argued that with
above 0 but is less than 1 (a change associated primarily with time, traces would undergo spontaneous reorganization in accor-
failures of the accessibility or retrieval type), or (c) disintegrate dance with laws similar to those governing perception (particu-
to a level where the probability of recall is zero (a change larly the law of similarity, a notion more recently echoed by
associated primarily with failures of the availability or storage Tulving, 1984, among others). For example, traces become dis-
type). Changes of the disintegrative variety can arise from any torted over time because they interact with other similar traces,
number of processes that lead to the dissolution of bonds uniting the consequence of which is that traces metamorphose in the
primitive trace elements (e.g., trace decay, interference, cross- direction of "good gestalts" (simple, well-integrated represen-
talk among items). It is important to note that, unlike some tations) that are easier to preserve in memory but that now lack
theories in which storage failures are identified with trace era- the episodic information that was originally encoded.
sure or trace absence, such failures in the present framework, More recently, changes in the architecture of traces at the
although producing zero recall, do not carry this extreme impli- level of storage have figured prominently in long-term memory
cation. Rather, items that have crossed the zero recall threshold models including search of associative memory (SAM; Men-
have not necessarily vanished from memory, but rather, may sink & Raaijmakers, 1988), theory of distributed associative
simply be indistinguishable from the background noise of other memory (TODAM; Murdock, 1982), parallel distributed pro-
memory elements. This raises the interesting possibility that cessing (POP) models (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1985), and
such traces may become refurbished during retention tests, a neural models (e.g., Greenough & Bailey, 1988). Indeed, in-
possibility that is discussed shortly. creasing numbers of memory theorists have adopted the view
Regardless of the level of disintegration that occurred across that "storage and retrieval may be distinguished conceptually
the retention interval, changes in a trace's status can occur sim- but must be closely related in practice, for the way information
ply as a function of retention tests. These changes are referred is coded is a major determinant of its retrievability under possi-
to as reminiscence. Reminiscence is treated as the refurbishment ble test conditions" (Estes, 1988, p. 360). Not only is the way
512 HOWE AND COURAGE

information is stored important but so too is what happens to can be fleshed out again is not new either, having appeared in
it in storage across the retention interval. For example, Tulving other theories in such forms as redintegration (Horowitz &
(1984), like us and others (see Estes, 1988), defines the product Prytulak, 1969) or reactivation (e.g., Rovee-Collier & Shyi,
of encoding or engram (the memory trace) as consisting of 1992). In addition, there is evidence indicating that there can
bundles of features (the exact nature of which is not critical to be changes in stored traces simply as a function of test trials
any of the assumptions discussed here). More important, Tulv- (e.g., Brainerd & Reyna, 1996; McDaniel & Masson, 1985;
ing (1984) notes that Richardson, 1985). As we have pointed out previously (e.g.,
Howe & Brainerd, 1989), because our model acknowledges
One of the distinctive characteristics of engrams of events is their
that storage is a dynamic, not static, medium and that storage
mutability: Functional properties of engrams change over time. Re-
failures are not equated with trace absence, reconstruction of
cuding is the generic name of related operations and processes that
traces in storage is a real, albeit somewhat empirically infrequent
take place after the encoding of the original event and thereby bring
about changes in the engram . . . . The receding of an original possibility.
engram . . . is governed by the similarity of interpolated events to 1b summarize, in the trace-integrity framework, storage and
the original event, similarity of encoding operations performed on retrieval are regarded as processes constituting a single memory
interpolated events, or both. (p. 230) continuum in which traces consist of collections of primitive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

elements. Traces that are integrated during the learning phase


Similar ideas lie at the heart of any number of other theories of can experience disintegration across the long-term retention in-
memory, including recoding during retrieval (Allen, Mahler, & terval, a process that, like acquisition, has both a storage and a
Estes, 1969), trace dispersion (Shepard, 1961; Shepard & retrieval component. Similarly, during the long-term retention
Chang, 1963), trace incrementing (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, test itself, traces can be redintegrated simply as a consequence
1981), convolution (e.g., Murdock, 1982), memory blending of the testing situation, a process of trace recovery that can
(McClelland & Rumelhart, 1985; Metcalfe, 1990), as well as involve both storage and retrieval. This conceptualization of
neural models (e.g., Greenough & Bailey, 1988). Indeed, in storage and retrieval has gained broad acceptance in the memory
Chechile's (1987) model, feature bundles (memory traces) are development literature, is consistent with other conceptualiza-
held together with different strengths. Feature losses occur when tions of these processes in other models, and is an example
bonds are destroyed, with trace recallability being a function of of one of three classic positions of forgetting (e.g., Crosland,
the most weakly bound feature in the critical set of features 1921).
comprising the trace.
Our model is certainly consistent with these propositions
Yes, But What Does This Have to Do With
about trace features, bonds, mutability in storage, and the close
Autobiographical Memory ?
connectedness of storage and retrieval. It is also consistent with
most developmental speculations concerning memory, especially What the trace-integrity framework does is (a) sets a general
those of the encoding specificity persuasion (e.g., Ackerman, context, one that is common to long-term memory in general,
1987) as well as with findings on age changes in children's in which to situate autobiographical memory, and (b) explains
reconstructive memory (e.g., Brainerd & Reyna, 1990; Inhelder, the accumulation of autobiographical memories using a simple,
1969; Liben, 1977; Piaget, 1968; Piaget & Inhelder, 1973). For basic process mechanism, namely, storage maintenance. By us-
example, Piaget and Inhelder (1973) reported improved mem- ing the same mechanisms to explain autobiographical memory
ory over a 6- to 12-month interval for a seriated array. These as those used to explain other memories and their development,
improvements could not be accounted for by simple reminis- we simplify and enhance our understanding of memory develop-
cence effects. Rather, they were attributed to reconstruction of ment at both a very specific as well as a global level.
stored information based on better articulated cognitive schemes Situating autobiographical memory. In order to situate au-
that served to reorganize memories. Note that this account is tobiographical memory, consider how a system such as the one
similar to the gestalt argument that "autonomous 'forces' acting described in the preceding section might encode an event both
upon memory slowly transform their contents into a form more before and after the emergence of the cognitive self. When fea-
closely resembling a good gestalt—a simple, well-organized, tures are sampled from the current encoding array (e.g., word
and stable structure" (Bower & Hilgard, 1981, p. 316). It is list, ongoing event) or nominal stimulus and stored in long-term
also consistent with Paris and Lindauer's (1977) observation memory, additional interpretive or internal contextual features
that, may be added. It is well-known that at encoding, only a subset
of features that characterize an event are actually encoded, de-
An adequate model of memory must not assume that the input to
pending on a number of factors (e.g., a feature's salience, the
a memory store is retrieved intact. . . . Memory is not a repository
encoder's expectation, attentional factors), with the number of
for experiences that are encoded and recalled one time; rather, mean-
ingful material is subject to continuous reorganization in memory, features being encoded varying in a probabilistic fashion. Thus,
(p. 52) reminiscent of stimulus sampling theory (e.g., see Hilgard &
Bower, 1975; Neimark & Estes, 1967), a functional stimulus
Clearly, the idea that stored information is modifiable to a (the stored trace of encoded features) is extracted, one con-
point of partial dissolution, blending or becoming restructured sisting of a subset of features, from the nominal situation.
with other information, or being overwritten is not unique to Clearly, until the self becomes a viable cognitive entity, one
us. Equally obvious is that the idea that such altered information with recognizable features, the encoding of such features into
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 513

the functional trace is impossible. However, once the self be- (1996) alerts us to the importance of self transitions in landmark
comes viable, its features gain the potential to be sampled, memories in one's autobiography. That is, as the self goes
although as we have seen with other features in the nominal through various transitions, events associated with these transi-
situation, by no means does this guarantee that they are sampled. tion points are well remembered (e.g., Csikszentmihalkyi &
Whether self features are sampled is a probabilistic process, Beattie, 1979). Although such findings highlight the importance
one that would be contingent on the same factors as any other of changes in the self in autobiographical memories, it is equally
feature (e.g.. salience, attention, the extent or centrality of par- likely that such transitions represent unique occurrences in one's
ticipation by the self in the event). Such encoding variability life. As distinctive and unique experiences tend to be more mem-
(also see Flexser & Tulving, 1978) may explain fluctuations in orable in general (e.g., Hunt & McDaniel, 1993), it would
the numbers of early memories related to the self as there may not be surprising to obtain similar effects in autobiographical
be a rather restricted set of features from which to sample in memory. Indeed, Linton (1979) has obtained such findings in
the first place. As more features are added to the "urn," the her studies of autobiographical memory, and Brewer (1988)
greater the likelihood that at least some self features are sampled found uniqueness of an event to be the best overall predictor of
and encoded in the functional trace for an event. Thus, although autobiographical recall.
there is no chance of encoding self features prior to the emer- In any event, the point is quite simple. Autobiographical mem-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

gence of a viable cognitive self, this does not mean that events ories can be accounted for using the same principles as those
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cannot be remembered as events, as attested to by the ever- found in contemporary models in the mainstream literatures on
growing literature in this field (Bauer, 1995). Similarly, even memory and memory development. This is consistent with other
when a viable cognitive self emerges, events may still remain research on the self as a system of knowledge that organizes
events if features of the self are not sampled and encoded in memories like any other knowledge structure (e.g., Green-
the stored trace. The effects of this sampling variability might wald & Banaji, 1989; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986) as well as
be particularly noticeable early in the establishment of the self other models of autobiographical memory (e.g., Conway, 1996;
as the number of features defining the self may be relatively Robinson, 1996). Indeed, unless additional data become avail-
restricted. As the number of features associated with the self able that indicate the need for a distinct memory system, one
increases, the likelihood of at least some of these features being with different memory processes, it is considerably more parsi-
sampled increases, changing memory for an event into memory monious to view the establishment and subsequent development
for an event that happened to me: A memory that is now, by of autobiographical memory in the manner outlined here.
definition, autobiographical. Thus, as we have stated throughout, Accumulating autobiographical memories. Howe and
simply having a viable cognitive self sets the bottom limit as to O'Sullivan (in press), in a review of a number of studies in
when autobiographical memories can be established; however, which this model had been used, showed that storage processes
it does not mean that such memories need be established at that tended to dominate forgetting during childhood and that these
age. Indeed, if there are few features associated with the self, storage failure rates declined with age. That is, regardless of
then the likelihood is low that those features are incorporated whether recall was free or cued, or whether the materials were
into the memory trace for an event. As that feature set grows, pictures, words, categorized, or uncategorized, storage failures
so too does the probability that self features are incorporated swamped retrieval failures. Interestingly, not only were storage
into the memory trace, making that event memory autobiograph- failure rates greater than retrieval failure rates for all compari-
ical. This, in addition to the growth in storage capacity over sons, but average storage failure rates were almost three times
longer and longer intervals, can account for the accumulation of greater than average retrieval failure rates. Developmentally, the
autobiographical memories as we age in childhood, accounting data showed that storage failure rates tended to decline with
for the sorts of growth or increased frequencies of childhood age, whereas no changes were observed in retrieval failure rates.
memories with age (e.g., Wetzler & Sweeney, 1986). In fact, Thus, there were two prominent trends identified, namely, that
with the increase in features (knowledge) concerning the self, (a) regardless of age, storage failures were more prominent
memories related to the self may be more efficiently organized than retrieval failures and (b) what develops in children's long-
in storage, contributing to their increased longevity. term retention is the ability to maintain memory traces in storage
This proposal is consistent with suggestions by others that across the retention interval. That such consistency was obtained
growth in knowledge structures promotes more efficient organi- across a wide variety of studies provides relatively convincing
zation of information in storage, which in turn promotes long- and converging evidence that forgetting in childhood is due to
term retention of that information (e.g., Bjorklund, 1987). In- alterations of information at the level of storage not simply
deed, as already noted, this is a dynamic process and storage failures to retrieve information that remains intact in long-term
medium such that new autobiographical memories add features memory.
to the self, which in turn influence subsequent encoding of auto- What is the specific implication of these findings for the early
biographical events and can oftentimes promote the reorganiza- development of autobiographical memory? Clearly, even though
tion (or receding) of self information in storage. Such effects very young children can recall events that took place many
are anticipated in any number of theories of autobiographical months ago or even a year or more ago (for a review, see
memory (e.g., Conway, 1996; Robinson, 1996). For example, Howe & Courage, 1993; Hudson, 1993), the results of Howe
Robinson (1996) emphasizes the importance of transformations and O'Sullivan's (in press) analysis indicates that the informa-
of subsequent experiences on information already in memory, tion loss that does occur for these children is very likely to
particularly with reference to changes in the person. Conway result from storage, not retrieval, failures. Because younger chil-
514 HOWE AND COURAGE

dren are more susceptible to storage failure than older children, offset of infantile amnesia. Rather, like the development of other
it would seem that earlier childhood memories are less intact knowledge structures in memory, infants acquire a cognitive
than later childhood memories, thus providing a basic-process sense of self, one around which event memories can be personal-
explanation of why more memories are available to adults from ized and preserved as autobiographical. And, like other struc-
later than earlier childhood. Quite simply, from a basic-process tures, categories, and concepts in memory, the cognitive sense
perspective at least, what this literally means is that less informa- of self emerges and is represented nonverbally, only later being
tion is available in memory from our earlier experiences than expressed, not determined, using language. Thus, the offset of
from our later experiences. As our trace-maintenance skills de- infantile amnesia and the onset of autobiographical memory
velop, so too does the numerosity of our memories over more does not require the appearance of a separate memory system
and more protracted retention intervals. Clearly, then, failures nor must it await developments in language, autonoetic aware-
to recall early life experiences are not simply a matter of finding ness, or metacognition that occur late in the preschool years
the right retrieval cue, but rather, a failure in the availability of (e.g., K. Nelson, 1993; Perner & Ruffman, 1995). Rather, ac-
that information in storage. cording to our theory, it is the natural consequence of young
A question arises concerning the generalizability of these toddlers' more general tendency to develop nonverbal represen-
findings from the episodic memory tasks reviewed by Howe and tational structures that describe the world around them (e.g.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

O'Sullivan (in press) to autobiographical memories discussed Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Mandler, 1992).
in this article. Although this clearly is an empirical question, Similarly, the subsequent ability to retain more autobiographi-
there is considerable evidence that memory behaves much the cal information with age in childhood develops not as a function
same way in situ as in the laboratory (e.g., Brainerd & Ornstein. of advances in narrative skills, but rather as a natural conse-
1991). As an example, consider children's memory for trau- quence of global improvements in children's general memory
matic events. As we have noted elsewhere, studies of children's abilities, namely, the capacity to maintain information in storage
memories for traumatic events reveal that such memories are over longer and longer intervals. Although a number of skills
similar to those for nontraumatic events. Indeed, there is an may be involved in, or at least correlated with, this improve-
emerging consensus that traumatic memories differ only from ment, including developments in language, strategies, knowl-
nontraumatic ones inasmuch as the trauma itself makes that edge, and gist extraction, the one common denominator to
event unique and distinctive against the background of other changes in children's retention over time is the basic ability of
events in one's life (Howe et al., 1994b). Thus, although trauma keeping information intact in storage.
may serve to enhance event memory through uniqueness (but
see the literature on "flashbulb" memories; e.g., Winograd &
Some Methodological Issues
Neisser, 1992), traumatic memories appear to behave very much
like nontraumatic memories. As with any theory, our speculations raise as many questions
Our basic-process perspective that subsequent gains in mem- as they answer. For example, there remains the intractable prob-
ory in general, and autobiographical memory in particular, lem of determining exactly what infants and toddlers remember
comes from additional developments in children's ability to re- about events, an issue that is as much one of methodology as it
tain information in storage is consistent with other theories of is of discerning the nature of representation. At the heart of the
memory development as well as our more global perspective problem is the requirement that unambiguous evidence for the
that, given our current state of knowledge (see Footnote 1), (autobiographic) recall of events can only be provided through
changes in memory with age are primarily of the software, not verbal report: a requirement that, by definition, excludes the
hardware, variety. That is, better storage maintenance may derive period of infancy (see Pillemer & White, 1989). However, as
from advances (both quantitative and qualitative) in strategy we know that infants are well able to recall events months before
use (e.g., Bjorklund, 1990), knowledge base (e.g., Bjorklund, they have productive language (e.g., Bauer & Mandler, 1992;
1987), or extraction of gist (e.g., Brainerd & Reyna, 1993). Meltzoff, 1988c), it defies logic to conclude that because the
Regardless of which approach (or approaches) is preferred, the traditional output device (language) is unavailable, so too is the
data clearly indicate that developmental improvements in long- memory. Because we cannot ask preverbal infants if they are
term retention are confined primarily to changes at the level of accessing a specific past experience when we test for retention,
information storage. we must look to infants' behavior for evidence of recall.
This leaves the thorny issue of establishing a set of necessary
and sufficient conditions for determining whether a nonverbal
Conclusions, Predictions, and Caveats
behavior is indicative of recall of a specific past event. As noted
In this article, we have argued that the emergence and subse- earlier, Meltzoff (1990) has argued that evidence of deferred
quent development of autobiographical memory are controlled imitation is an appropriate index of the recall of action se-
by the discovery of the cognitive self and increases in the ability quences. Specifically, as the to-be-recalled information is ac-
to maintain information in memory storage, respectively. We quired following only brief exposure, without opportunity to
have provided what we believe is convincing evidence that the practice, and is recalled after a significant delay (thus ruling
underlying structures and processes that drive memory develop- out explanations based on procedural or sensorimotor learning),
ment operate in a continuous fashion from birth onward; that such deferred imitation reflects the operation of at worst a
is, there is no need to posit separately developing memory sys- "nonverbal episodic-like" and at best a declarative memory
tems or additional hardware advances to account for the sudden system. Using similar arguments, Bauer et al. (1994, 1995) as
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 515

well as Mandler (1990b) contend that the elicited imitation task cognitive self. For example, there is considerable variability
in which infants replicate temporally ordered action sequences across individuals in the age of their first autobiographical mem-
following a delay can be considered a nonverbal analogue of ories when, as adults, they are asked to recall their earliest
cued verbal recall. As the action sequences are not perceptually experiences (e.g., Usher & Neisser, 1993). One explanation, in
present at retention, their temporal order must have been en- addition to the obvious possibility that there exist individual
coded at initial learning and retrieved from a representation of differences in forgetting rates, is that these differences in recall-
the event. More recently, McDonough, Mandler, McKee, and ing early experiences are related to corresponding individual
Squire (1995) examined deferred imitation in patients with am- variation in the development of storage maintenance. Moreover,
nesia with damage to the hippocampal formation, and who they may be linked to variation in (a) the age of onset of
showed impairments to long-term (declarative) memory on the cognitive self (although age variation here is certainly less
standard tests. These patients performed poorly on tests of de- substantial than the age variation for adult recall of early memo-
ferred imitation, ones analogous to those used by Meltzoff ries) or (b) the propensity to encode self-relevant features into
(1995), compared to normal control adults as well as those memory traces for early events. Although we have discussed
with damage restricted to the frontal lobes. McDonough et al. the second variation earlier, it is interesting to note with respect
suggest that deferred imitation is dependent on brain structures to variation in age of onset that developmental trends in early
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

essential for declarative memory and that infants who show self-recognition during the 2nd year of life show substantial
deferred imitation have an early capacity for declarative mem- individual differences in the age of onset of mark-directed be-
ory. In light of this research, there is a growing consensus that havior (Berenthal & Fischer, 1978; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn,
deferred imitation tasks meet the criteria for what is referred to 1979; Lewis, Brooks-Gunn, & Jaskir, 1985; Schneider-Rosen &
as declarative memory except, of course, the ability to verbalize Cicchetti, 1984, 1991). These studies showed that whereas
what is being recalled. If such findings hold, deferred imitation about 25% of 15- to 18-month-old infants showed mark-directed
may constitute a nonverbal analogue of traditional declarative behavior to the red spot on their noses, others did not show
memory tasks. Given that the behavioral evidence provided in self-recognition until the end of the 2nd year, at which time
these imitation paradigms is indicative of the declarative recall about 75% showed mark-directed behavior.
of events, we are still left with the question of whether these Although these individual differences in the age of onset of
nonverbal representations are organized autobiographically. We visual self-recognition have not been fully explored, there is
contend that until the development of the cognitive self in the evidence that they originate primarily in maturational rather
2nd year, such organization is logically very unlikely. than social or experiential factors. Specifically, according to
It should also be pointed out that simply having verbal con- Lewis and Brooks-Gunn (1979), neither the child's sex nor a
firmation that someone is recalling a specific life episode does variety of general measures of social experience such as moth-
not make it so. For example, it is well known that narrative er's education, family socioeconomic status, birth order, or num-
truth (i.e., one's narrative construction of an autobiographical ber of siblings were related to onset of self-recognition. Further-
event) is not necessarily veridical, either in time or in fact, with more, maltreated infants, whose aberrant caretaking environ-
the historical truth (i.e., what actually happened; e.g., D. P. ments result in well-documented delays or deviations in their
Spence, 1982). Moreover, there is evidence that the verbal re- emotional development as it relates to the self (Cicchetti &
counting of autobiographical events is subject to other forms of Beeghly, 1987; Cicchetti & Carlson, 1989; Kaufman & Cic-
distortion, ones that are consistent with the recaller's current chetti, 1989; Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti, 1984), are not de-
beliefs and expectations as well as the expectations of the lis- layed in the onset of mirror self-recognition (Schneider-Rosen &
tener (Barclay, 1988;Wagenaar, 1988,1990). Even those memo- Cicchetti, 1991). In contrast, infants who have delayed matura-
ries that are thought to be stressful, like those contained in so- tion (e.g., Down syndrome, familial mental retardation, autism)
called flashbulb memories, are subject to distortions and inaccu- do show delays in visual self-recognition (Cicchetti, 1991;
racies (Weaver, 1993; Winograd & Neisser, 1992). Thus, even Hill & Tomlin, 1981; Loveland, 1987, 1993; Mans, Cicchetti, &
verbal indices of memory may not provide reliable measures of Sroufe, 1978; Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti, 1991; Spiker &
memories for one's life events. The interesting implication of Ricks, 1984). However, these infants can succeed at the self-
this observation is that if conversations about our life events are recognition task if and when they reach a mental age comparable
said to reactivate and maintain autobiographical information in to that of nondelayed infants who succeed at the task, an obser-
memory over time (e.g., K. Nelson, 1993), but such narrative vation that perhaps reflects an important cognitive component
recall is often tainted by distortions, then would not such conver- of this underlying maturation. Consistent with this, Brooks-
sations distort the original memory it was intended to reify? Gunn and Lewis (1984) found that recovery to a novel stimulus
following habituation (itself an index of cognitive performance)
was related to onset of self-recognition, with the infant's ability
Individual Differences and Some Predictions
to notice subtle stimulus change suggested as the causal link.
Clearly, a number of issues remain to be clarified, both defini- In any event, the near-universal appearance of mark-directed
tionally and empirically. Despite this, however, we believe sig- behavior among infants who have attained the maturational pre-
nificant progress has been made in linking major developments requisites suggests that visual self-recognition may be so "ca-
early in life to the personalization of event memory. Indeed, a nalized" that its emergence is not conspicuously influenced by
number of predictions fall out of our theorizing, ones that capi- variations in social or child-care experiences (but see Lewis,
talize on individual differences in the early emergence of the Brooks-Gunn, & Jaskir, 1985, for some evidence to the con-
516 HOWE AND COURAGE

trary). Indeed, Kagan (1981) has argued that the origins of the tion between a sample of Bedouin infants who had never seen
self-concept are seen in the maturation of the child and that the their facial image prior to testing with mirrors and a sample of
social environment plays only a minor role in this development. babies who had prior mirror experience. Clearly, the Bedouins
Consistent with this evidence, then, individual differences in the were able to construct a body image from perceptual and sensory
onset of early autobiographical memories should be related to cues that they could coordinate and map onto the visual stimulus
maturational, not social or experiential, factors associated with provided by the mirror. The performance of the Bedouin babies
the emergence of the cognitive self. This hypothesis stands in stands in marked contrast to that of a group of blind children
direct contrast to the social-constructivist position (e.g., Tes- who, though obviously unable to utilize mirror information,
sler & Nelson, 1994) in which the onset of autobiographical showed no recognition of self in language or imaginative play
memory is said to be directly related to parental conversational until they were 4-5 years old (Fraiberg, 1977). Thus, vision
styles (e.g., narrative and paradigmatic styles). may be uniquely suited to synthesize information about the
Moreover, differences in the onset of autobiographical mem- ecological and interpersonal selves provided by other modalities,
ory in atypical populations should be directly related to delays and its absence in the blind child significantly delays this
in the establishment of the cognitive self and not chronological achievement. Indeed, Neisser (1993) has argued that an aware-
age. Additional variations in the onset of autobiographical mem- ness of the face and its importance for personal relations under-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ory, of course, can be attributed to more global differences in lies the evaluative self-consciousness that is at the heart of ma-
memory functioning, particularly those controlling variability ture self-recognition. Consistent with this, Bigelow (1995) dis-
in the sampling of self features. It is important to note, however, cussed delays in both the interpersonal and ecological selves
that although the weight of evidence favors the idea that the experienced by blind infants. We, of course, would predict that
onset of self-recognition is rooted in maturational processes, it these delays in the onset of the ecological self, in particular,
is not the case that the mirror behavior of children with atypical would be accompanied by delays in the age of onset of autobio-
cognitive development or those with adverse social environments graphical memory. This remains a question for future research
is identical to that of normally developing children. In fact, as, to our knowledge, there are no studies in which delays in
examination of behaviors coincident with mark-directed behav- autobiographical memory in blind children have been examined.
ior reveal differences that may well reflect differences in social The development of mirror self-recognition heralds the begin-
interactional variables. For example, normally developing chil- ning of the cognitive sense of self as object (me) that serves,
dren show a combination of surprise followed by shyness or according to our theory, as a foundation for the organization of
embarrassment when they notice the red dot on their noses, but our earliest autobiographical memories. Indeed, the variation in
they are generally positive in their responses to their self-image the age of onset of mirror self-recognition may underlie the
(Lewis et al., 1989). Likewise, children with maturational or variation in the age of onset of earliest recoverable autobio-
cognitive delays (e.g., Down syndrome) are also positive toward graphical memory that occurs between 2 and 3 years, with a
their mirror images (Cicchetti, 1991). On the other hand, chil- few isolated instances of memories recovered before the age of
dren who have been maltreated and those with autism show 2 years (Usher & Neisser, 1993). It might be expected, then,
more neutral and negative behavior in response to their mirror that with individual variation in the pace of subsequent develop-
images (Cicchetti, Beeghly, Carlson, & Toth, 1990). Clearly, ments in the emergence of the cognitive self, comes correspond-
then, although social and experiential factors may not determine ing advances in the development of autobiographical memory,
the onset of early autobiographical memory, they may determine although this has not been demonstrated. Specifically, prior to
the contents of these early memories. That is, the aspects of the the first appearance of me, events that the infant experienced
self that are encoded into autobiographical memories depend could not have been encoded with reference to the self: They
on one's experience. were not personally experienced, and there was no autobio-
Overall, then, the self as object is launched at a point in graphical framework. The way that these ' 'preself'' experiences
development when the infant is maturationally ready. Although are represented is an empirical question—perhaps as condi-
the origin of this readiness is unknown, it undoubtedly rests in tioned responses, perhaps as fragments of knowledge—we sim-
part on prerequisite perceptual knowledge fundamental to what ply do not know. Clearly, however, they do exist as nonperson-
Neisser (1993) has described as the ecological self. Thus, chil- alized events as shown by the numerous studies documenting
dren who are autistic or who have been maltreated have no the existence of early event memory (Bauer, 1995).
difficulty perceiving their bodies in general (and their faces in As the self emerges and continues to evolve, it serves to
particular) as me. However, achieving Neisser's interpersonal organize personally experienced events in memory, just as the
self, which (though also rooted in perception) evolves through memories themselves may serve to adumbrate our selves. How-
reciprocal interaction with others, presents a difficulty for in- ever, with the continued evolution of the self come changes in
fants whose interpersonal relationships are aberrant. This may the way in which memories are encoded and maintained in
explain their discrepant affective responses to their mirror storage as well as changes in the way memories are retrieved
images. and restored. Just as maturational differences in the initial onset
Interestingly, the development of self-recognition has also of the cognitive self is one factor related to individual variability
been studied in atypical populations who, through the dictates in the laying down of the first autobiographical memory, so too
of culture or accident of nature, do not have access to their are individual and age differences in the subsequent elaboration
reflected mirror images. For example, Priel and De Schonen of the self-concept to the type of features incorporated into,
(1986) reported no age difference in the onset of self-recogni- and accumulated with, subsequent autobiographical memories.
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 517

Indeed, it is entirely possible that autobiographical memories, memory to other developments and well-known principles in
given they have not undergone substantial (re)constructive pro- cognition, we hope that we have moved a step closer toward
cessing, may contain features associated with earlier stages developing a more comprehensive and integrated picture of
(e.g., Neisser, 1993) of one's self. As already noted, Conway memory and its development over time.
(1996), among others, has alerted us to the importance of life's
transition points in stable autobiographical memories. These
References
results are consistent with the idea that within-individual age
differences in the types and contents of events encrypted into Ackerman, B. P. (1987). Descriptions: A model of nonstrategic memory
autobiographical memory are related to changes in one's sense development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development
of self. However, any link forged between these changes and and behavior (Vol. 20. pp. 143-183). New York: Academic Press.
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal
the contents of autobiographical memories that go beyond the
infants. Child Development, 59, 450-466.
purely correlational must await progress in dissevering influ-
Allen, G. A., Mahler, W. A., & Estes, W. K. (1%9). Effects of recall
ences other than the self from autobiographical memory reports
tests on long-term retention of paired associates. Journal of Verbal
(e.g., receding, reconstruction at retrieval, narrative skills). Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 463-470.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Summation Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297-305.


Anderson, J. R. (1978). Arguments concerning representations for men-
In this article, we have tried to provide an overarching theo- tal imagery. Psychological Review, 85, 249-277.
retical framework of early personal memory, one that serves to Anderson, J. R. (1979). Further arguments concerning representations
integrate it with other major achievements at this age. In particu- for mental imagery: A response to Hayes-Roth and Pylyshyn. Psycho-
logical Review, 86, 395-406.
lar, we have developed a theory in which the emergence and
Anisfeld, M. (1996). Only tongue protrusion modeling is matched by
early development of autobiographical memory does not require
neonates. Developmental Review, 16, 149-161.
the invocation of specialized neural mechanisms or separate
Asendorpf, J.B., & Baudonniere, P.-M. (1993). Self-awareness and
memory systems requiring new processes or fundamental laws other-awareness: Mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation
of operation. Rather, our theory is clear that autobiographical among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 88-95.
memory arises as a natural consequence of developments in Aslin, R. N., Pisoni, D. B., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1983). Auditory develop-
other, related domains as well as other developments, particu- ment and speech perception in infancy. In M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos
larly in the software, that drives memory functioning in general. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 11. Infancy and develop-
First, the onset of autobiographical memory is linked to the mental psychobiology (pp. 573-687). New 'Vbrk: Wiley.
development of a new functional knowledge structure, the cogni- Bachevalier, J. (1991). Cortical versus limbic immaturity: Relationship
to infantile amnesia. In M. Gunnar & C. Nelson (Eds.), Develop-
tive self. Like other categories that emerge in cognitive develop-
mental behavioral neuroscience: The Minnesota Symposium on Child
ment, the cognitive self can be used to organize memories, in
Psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 129-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
this case, memories of experiences that happened to me. The
Bachevalier, J., Brickson, M., & Hagger, C. (1993). Limbic-dependent
emergence of a functional cognitive self at around the end of
recognition memory in monkeys develops early in infancy. Neuro
the 2nd year of life sets the lower limit for early autobiographical Report, 4, 77-80.
memories at about 2 years of age, an estimate that is consistent Bachevalier, J., & Mishkin, M. (1984). An early and late developing
with most of the empirical data (see Howe & Courage, 1993). system for learning and retention in infant monkeys. Behavioral Neu-
Although personalized memories are feasible at this point, roscience, 98, 770-778.
whether such memories are laid down is contingent on sampling Bahrick, L., & Pickens, J. (1995). Infant memory for object motion
and encoding features of the self in functional memory traces. across a period of three months: Implications for a four-pbase attention
Whereas the establishment of this cognitive self appears to be function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 343-371.
Baillargeon, R., DeVos, J., & Graber, M. (1989). Location memory in 8-
controlled maturationally, the contents of autobiographical
month-old infants in a nonsearch AB task: Further evidence. Cognitive
traces are determined by a host of factors and are contingent
Development, 24, 345-367.
on feature sampling probabilities.
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object perma-
Second, the accumulation of personal memories across age nence in 5-month-olds. Cognition, 20, 191-208.
in childhood is a simple consequence of a more general phenom- Barclay, C. R. (1988). Truth and accuracy in autobiographical memory.
enon that occurs in children's memory development, namely, an In M. Gruenberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical as-
improvement in the maintenance of information in long-term pects of memory: Current research and issues (pp. 289-294). New
storage (also see Howe & O'Sullivan, in press). Thus, autobio- York: Wiley.
graphical memories, like other memories, accumulate because Bates. E. (1990). Language about me and you: Pronominal reference
the processes used to maintain information in storage (e.g., and the emerging concept of self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly
(Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 165-182).
strategies, verbal and nonverbal; knowledge; gist extraction) are
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
better developed and more sophisticated in older than younger
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
children. Depending on the amount of receding of memories
(1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication
while they are resident in storage, and the extent of (re)con- in infancy. New Tfork: Academic Press.
struction at the time of recall, autobiographical recollections Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of per-
may reflect critical features of the self that were encoded at the formatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205-226.
time the traces were laid down. By linking autobiographical Bates, E., O'Connell, B., & Shore, C. (1987). Language and communi-
518 HOWE AND COURAGE

cation in infancy. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant develop- event information on infants' memory for a central target. Journal of
ment (2nd ed, pp. 149-203). New York: Wiley. Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 372-396.
Bateson, M. C. (1975). Mother-child exchanges: The epigenesis of con- Bower, G. H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1981). Theories of learning (5th edi-
versational exchanges. In D. Aronson & R. W. Rieber (Eds.), Annals tion). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 263. Developmental psy- Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic
cholinguistics and communication disorders (pp. 101-112). New Books.
York: New York Academy of Sciences. Boyer, M. E., Barren, K. L., & Farrar, M. J. (1994). Three-year-olds
Bauer, P. J. ( 1993). Identifying subsystems of autobiographical memory: remember a novel event from 20 months: Evidence for long-term
Commentary on Nelson. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect memory in children? Memory, 2, 417-445.
in development: The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. Brainerd, C. J., Howe, M. L., & Desrochers, A. (1982). The general
26. pp. 25-37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. theory of two-stage learning: A mathematical review with illustrations
Bauer, P. J. (1995). Recalling past events: From infancy to early child- from memory development. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 634-665.
hood. Annals of Child Development, 11, 25-71. Brainerd, C. J., Howe, M. L., & Kingma, J. (1982). An identifiable
Bauer, P. J., & Dow, G. A. (1994). Episodic memory in 16 and 20- model of two-stage learning. Journal of Mathematical Psychology,
month-old children: Specifics are generalized but not forgotten. Devel- 26, 263-293.
opmental Psychology, 30, 403-417. Brainerd, C. J., & Ornstein, P. A. (1991). Children's memory for wit-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bauer, P. J., & Hertsgaard, L. A. (1993). Increasing steps in recall of nessed events: The developmental backdrop. In J. Doris (Ed.), The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

events: Factors facilitating immediate and long-term memory in 13.5 suggestibility of children's recollections: Implications for eyewitness
and 16.5-month-old children. Child Development, 64, 1204-1223. testimony (pp. 10-20). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Bauer, P. J., Hertsgaard, L. A., & Dow, G. A. (1994). After 8 months Association.
have passed: Long-term recall of events by 1- and 2-year-old children. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1990). Gist is the grist: Fuzzy-trace
Memory, 2, 353-382. theory and the new intuitionism. Developmental Review, 10, 3-47.
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1993). Memory independence and mem-
Bauer. P. J., Hertsgaard, L. A., & Wewerka, S. S. (1995). Effects of
ory interference in cognitive development. Psychological Review, 100,
experience and reminding on long-term recall in infancy: Remember-
42-67.
ing not to forget. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 260-
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1996). Mere memory testing creates
298.
false memories in children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 467-478.
Bauer, P. J., & Handler, J. M. (1989). One thing follows another: Effects
flrainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Kingma, J. (1990). The
of temporal structure of 1- to 2-year-olds' recall of events. Develop-
development of forgetting and reminiscence. Monographs of the Soci-
mental Psychology, 25, 197-206.
ety for Research in Child Development, 53(3-4, Serial No. 222).
Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1992). Putting the cart before the horse:
Brewer, W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical
The use of temporal order in recall of events by one-year-old children.
events. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsid-
Developmental Psychology, 28, 441-452.
ered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the stud\' of memory
Bauer, P. J., & Shore, C. M. (1987). Making a memorable event: Effects
(pp. 21-90). New York: Cambridge University Press.
of familiarity and organization of young children's recall of action
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1984). The development of early visual
sequences. Cognitive Development, 2, 327-338.
self-recognition. Developmental Review, 4, 215-239.
Bauer, P. J., & Thai, D. J. (1990). Scripts or scraps: Reconsidering the
Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1990). Who am I? Self-understanding
development of sequential understanding. Journal of Experimental
in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217-238.
Child Psychology, 50, 287-304.
Butterworth, G. (1990). Self-perception in infancy. In D. Cicchetti &
Bauer, P. J., & Travis, L. L. (1993). The fabric of an event: Different
M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp.
sources of temporal invariance differentially affect 24-month-olds'
119-137). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
recall. Cognitive Development, 8, 319-341.
Carroll, J., & Gibson, E. J. (1981, April). Differentiation of an aperture
Berenthal, B. 1., & Fischer, K. (1978). Development of self-recognition
from an obstacle under conditions of motion by 3-month-olds. Paper
in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 44-50.
presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Berger, B., & Alvarez, C. (1994). Neurochemical development of the
Development, Boston.
hippocampal region in the fetal monkey: II. Immunocytochemistry
Case, R. (1991). Stages in the development of the young child's first
of peptides, calcium-binding proteins, DARPP-32, and monoamine
sense of self. Developmental Review, 11, 210-230.
innervation in the entorhinal cortex by the end of gestation. Hippocam-
Case, R. (1995). Capacity based explanations of working memory
pus, 4, 84-114. growth: A brief history and a reevaluation. In F. M. Weinert & W.
Bigelow, A. E. (1995). The effects of blindness on the early development Schneider (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in
of the self. In P. Rochat(Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research growth and development (pp. 23-44). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 327-347). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Catherwood, D. (1993). The robustness of infant haptic memory: Testing
Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute its capacity to withstand delay and haptic interference. Child Develop-
to the development of organization in children's memory: An interpre- ment, 64, 702-710.
tive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93-130. Cernoch, J. M., & Porter, R. H. (1985). Recognition of maternal axillary
Bjorklund, D. F. (Ed.). (1990). Children's strategies: Contemporary odors by infants. Child Development, 56. 1593-1598.
views of cognitive development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Chechile, R. A. (1987). Trace susceptibility theory. Journal of Experi-
Bjorklund, D. F. (1995). Children's thinking: Developmental functions mental Psychology: General, 116, 203-222.
and individual differences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Chugani, H. T. (1994). Development of regional brain glucose metabo-
Bogartz, R. S. (1994). The future of dynamic systems models in devel- lism in relation to behavior and plasticity. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer
opmental psychology in light of the past. Journal of Experimental (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 153-175).
Child Psychology, 58, 289-319. New York: Guilford Press.
Boiler, K., Grabelle, M., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1995). Effects of post- Chugani, H. T., & Phelps, M. E. (1986). Maturational changes in cere-
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 519

bral function in infants determined by [18]FDG positron emission action as indicated by infants' performance on AB. Child Develop-
tomography. Science, 231, 840-843. ment, 56, 868-883.
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotti, J. C. (1987). Positron emis- Dixon, J. C. (1957). Development of self-recognition. Journal of Ge-
sion tomography study of human brain functional development. An- netic Psychology, 91, 251-256.
nals of Neurology, 22, 487-497. Eimas, P. D., & Quinn, P. C. (1994). Studies on the formation of percep-
Cicchetti, D. (1991). Fractures in the crystal: Developmental psychopa- tually based basic-level categories in young infants. Child Develop-
thology and the emergence of the self. Developmental Review, 11, ment, 65, 903-917.
271 -287. Emde, R. N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R. B., & Oppenheim, D. (1991).
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1987). Symbolic development in mal- The moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowledge.
treated youngsters: An organizational perspective. New Directions for Developmental Review, 11, 251 -270.
Child Development, 36, 5-29. Estes, W. K. (1988). Human learning and memory. In R. C. Atkinson,
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (Eds.). (1990). The self in transition: R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens'handbook
Infancy to childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. of experimental psychology: Vol. 2. Learning and cognition (2nd ed.,
Cicchetti, D., Beeghly, M., Carlson, V., & Toth, S. (1990). The emer- pp. 351-415). New York: Wiley.
gence of the self in atypical populations. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly Fadil. L., Moss, L., & Bahrick, L. (1993, March). Infants'visual recog-
(Eds.), The self in transition; Infancy to childhood (pp. 309-344). nition of their own faces. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press. the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Cicchetti, D., & Carlson, V. (1989). Child maltreatment: Theory and Pagan, J. F. (1990). The paired-comparison paradigm and infant intelli-
research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. gence. In A. Diamond (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy of
New "fork: Cambridge University Press. Sciences: Vol. 608. The development and neural bases of higher cog-
Conway, M. A. (1996). Autobiographical knowledge and autobiographi- nitive functions (pp. 337-364). New ^fork: New %rk Academy of
cal memories. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies Sciences.
in autobiographical memory (pp. 67-93). New "fork: Cambridge
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, I. S., Bates, E., Thai, D., & Pethick,
University Press.
S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Mono-
Conway, M. A., Collins, A. E, Gathercole, S. E., & Anderson, S. J. graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Serial
(1996). Recollections of true and false autobiographical memories. No. 242).
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 69-95.
Fivush, R. (1993). Developmental perspectives on autobiographical re-
Crosland, H. R. (1921). A qualitative analysis of the process of forget-
call. In G. S. Goodman & B. L. Bottoms (Eds.), Child victims, child
ting. Psychological Monographs, 29( 1, Whole No. 130).
witnesses: Understanding and improving testimony (pp. 1-24). New
Csikszentmihalkyi, M., & Beattie, O. V. (1979). Life themes: A theoreti- York: Guilford Press.
cal and empirical exploration of their origins and effects. Journal of
Fivush, R. (1994). Young children's event recall: Are memories con-
Humanistic Psychology, 19, 45-63.
structed through discourse? Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 356-
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding
373.
from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 53, 841-864,
Fivush, R., Gray, J. T, & Fromhoff, F. A. (1987). Two-year-olds talk
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and
about the past. Cognitive Development, 2, 393-409.
adolescence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Fivush, R., Haden, C., & Adam, S. (1995). Structure and coherence of
Darwin, C. R. (1877). An autobiographical sketch of an infant. Mind,
preschoolers' personal narratives over time: Implications for childhood
2, 186-294.
amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 32-56.
DeCasper, A. J., & Prescott, P. A. (1984). Human newboms' perception
Fivush, R., & Hamond, N. (1989). Time and time again: Effects of
of male voices: Preference, discrimination, and reinforcing value. De-
repetition and retention interval on 2-year-olds' event recall. Journal
velopmental Psychobiology, 17, 481—491.
of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 259-273.
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech in-
Fivush, R., & Hamond, N. (1990). Autobiographical memory across the
fluences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and
preschool years: Toward reconceptualizing childhood amnesia. In R.
Development, 9, 133-150.
Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1991). Auditory mediated behavior
children (pp. 223-248). New York: Cambridge University Press.
during the prenatal period: A cognitive view. In M. Weiss & P. Zelazo
Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (1993). Cognitive develop-
(Eds.), Newborn attention: Biological constraints and the influence
ment (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
of experience (pp. 142-176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Flexser, A. J., & Tulving, E. (1978). Retrieval independence on recogni-
DeLoache, J. S.,&Brown,A. L. (1983). Very young children's memory
tion and recall. Psychological Review, 85, 153-171.
for the location of objects in a large-scale environment. Child Devel-
opment, 54, 888-897. Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive
DeLoache, J. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Where do I go next? Intelligent architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3-71.
searching by very young children. Developmental Psychology, 20, Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the blind. New York: Basic Books.
37-44. Freud, S. (1959). Instincts and their vicissitudes. In E. Jones (Ed.),
DeLoache, J, S., Cassidy, D.J., & Brown, A. L. (1985). Precursors of Collected papers ofSigmund Freud (pp. 317-385). New York: Basic
mnemonic strategies in very young children's memory. Child Devel- Books.
opment, 56, 125-137. Gibson, E. J., Riccio, G., Schmuckler, M. A., Stoffgregen, T. A., Rosen-
DeLoache, J. S., & Tbdd, C. M. (1988). Young children's use of spatial berg, D., & Taormina, J. (1987). Detection of traversabilily on sur-
categorization as a mnemonic strategy. Journal of Experimental Child faces by crawling and walking infants. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Psychology, 46, 1-20. chology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 533-544.
Dempster, EN., & Brainerd, C. J. (Eds.). (1995). Interference and Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Bos-
inhibition in cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. ton: Houghton Mifflin.
Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide Goldin-Meadow. S., Alibali, M. W, & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions
520 HOWE AND COURAGE

in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. ( 1994a, November). Chil-
Review, 100, 279-297. dren 's memories of traumatic events. Paper presented at the annual
Goldin-Meadow, S., Seligman, M., & Gelman, R. (1976). Language in meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, MO.
the two-year-old. Cognition, 4, 189-202. Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (I994b). How can I
Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Riddlesberger, remember when "I" wasn't there: Long-term retention of traumatic
M. M., & Kuhn, J. (1994). Predictors of accurate and inaccurate experiences and emergence of the cognitive self. Consciousness and
memories of traumatic events experienced in childhood. Conscious- Cognition, 3, 327-355.
ness and Cognition, 3, 269-294. Howe, M. L., & O'Sullivan, J. T. (in press). What children's memories
Goodwyn, S., & Acredolo, L. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word: tell us about recalling our childhoods: A review of storage and retrieval
Is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Child Develop- processes in the development of long-term retention. Developmental
ment, 64, 688-701. Review.
Greenough, W. T. (1993). Brain adaptation to experience: An update. Howe, M. L., & Rabinowitz, F. M. (1994). Dynamic modeling, chaos,
In M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader and cognitive development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
(pp. 319-322). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. ogy, 58, 184-199.
Greenough, W. T., & Bailey, C. H. (1988). The anatomy of a memory: Howe, M. L., Rabinowitz, F. M., & Grant, M. J. ( 1993). On measuring
Convergence of results across a diversity of tests. Trends in Neurosci- (in) dependence of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ence, 11, 142-147. 737-747.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience Hudson, J. A. (1993). Reminiscing with mothers and others: Autobio-
and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539-559. graphical memory in young two-year-olds. Journal of Narrative and
Greenwald, A. G.. & Banaji, M. R. (1989). The self as a memory sys- Life History, 3, 1-32.
tem: Powerful, but ordinary. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. ( 1993). The enigma of organization
chology, 57, 41-54. and distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and language, 32, 421-445.
Hamann, S. B., & Squire, L. R. (1995). On the acquisition of new Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. ( 1994). The coding
declarative knowledge in amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-
1027-1044.
147.
Hamond, N. R., & Fivush, R. (1990). Memories of Mickey Mouse:
Huttenlocher, P. R., & De Courten, C. (1987). The development of syn-
Young children recount their trip to Disneyworld. Cognitive Develop-
apses in striate cortex of man. Human Neurobiology. 6, 1 -9.
ment, 6, 433-448.
Inhelder, B. ( 1969). Memory and intelligence in the child. In D. Elkind &
Hanson. S. J., & Burr, D. J. (1990). What connectionist models learn:
J. H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development (pp. 337—364).
Learning and representation in connectionist networks. Behavioral
New ferk: Oxford University Press.
and Brain Sciences, 13, 471-518.
James, W. (1961 ). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: Harper.
Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system. In
(Original work published 1892)
E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4.
Johnson, D. B. (1983). Self -recognition in infants. Infant Behavior and
Social and personality development (4th ed., pp. 275-385). New
Development, 6, 211-222.
York: Wiley.
Kagan, J. (1981). The second year: The emergence of self-awareness.
Hayne, H., Rovee-Collier, C., & Ferris, E. (1987). Categorization and
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
memory retrieval by three-month-olds. Child Development, 58, 750-
Karmiloff-Smith, A. ( 1992) . Beyond modularity: A developmental per-
767.
spective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hilgard, E. R., & Bower, G. H. (1975). Theories of learning (4th ed.).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kaufman, J., & Cicchetti, D. ( 1989). Effects of maltreatment on school-
age children's socioemolional development: Assessments in a day-
Hill, S., & Tomlin, C. (1981). Self recognition in retarded children.
camp setting. Developmental Psychology, 25, 516-524.
Child Development, 52, 145-150.
Horowitz, L. M., & Prytulak, L. S. (1969). Redintegrative memory. Psy- Kaye, K. L., & Bower, T. G. R. ( 1994). Learning and intermodal transfer
chological Review. 76, 519-531. of information in newborns. Psychological Science, 5, 286-288.
Howe, M. L. (1988). Measuring memory development in adulthood: A Klein, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. F. ( 1986). Elaboration, organization, and
model-based approach to disentangling storage-retrieval contribu- the self-reference effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
tions. In M. L. Howe & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Cognitive development ogy: General, 115, 26-38.
in adulthood (pp. 39-64). New York: Springer-Verlag. Kohler, W. (1929). Gesta/t psychology. New York: Liveright.
Howe, M. L. (1991). Misleading children's story recall: Forgetting and Kohler, W. (1941). On the nature of associations. Proceedings of the
reminiscence of the facts. Developmental Psychology, 27, 746-762. American Philosophical Society, 84, 489-502.
Howe, M. L. (1994). Dynamic modeling of cognitive development. Kopp, C.B., & Brownel], C. A. (Eds.). (1991). The development of
[Symposium]. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58(2). the self: The first three years [Special Issue] . Developmental Review,
Howe, M. L. ( 1995). Interference effects in young children's long-term
retention. Developmental Psychology, 31, 579-596. Kretschmann, J. J., Kammradt, G., Krauthausen, L, Sauer, B., & Wingert,
Howe, M. L., & Brainerd, C. J. (1989). Development of children's long- F. ( 1986). Growth of the hippocampal formation in man, Bibliotheca
term retention. Developmental Review, 9, 301-340. Anatomica, 28, 27-52.
Howe, M. L., Brainerd, C. J., & Kingma, J. (1985). Development of Kuhl, P. K. (1987). Perception of speech and sound in early infancy. In
organization in recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, P. Salapatek & L. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of infant perception, Vol.
230-251. 2: From perception to cognition (pp. 275-382). San Diego, CA:
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of Academic Press.
infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113. 305-326. Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1984). The intermodal representation
Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Bryant-Brown, L. (1993). Reinstating of speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 361-381.
preschoolers' memories. Developmental Psychology, 29, 854-869. Lee, D. N., & Aronson, E. (1974). Visual proprioceptive control of
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 521

standing in human infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 529- Meltzoff, A. N. (1988a). Imitation of televised models of infants. Child
532. Development, 59, 1221-1229.
Lewis, M. (1986). Origins of self-knowledge and individual differences Meltzoff, A. N. (1988b). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-
in early self-recognition. In A. Greenwald & J. Suls (Eds.), Psycho- term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psy-
logical perspectives on the self (Vol. 3, pp. 55-78). Hillsdale, NJ: chology, 24. 470-476.
Erlbaum. Meltzoff, A. N. (1988c). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-
Lewis, M. (1991). Ways of knowing: Objective self-awareness or con- olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217-
sciousness. Developmental Review, 11, 231-243. 255.
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisi- Mellzoff, A. N. (1990). Towards a developmental cognitive science: The
tion of self. New \brk: Plenum Press. implications of cross-modal matching and imitation for the develop-
Lewis, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Jaskir, J. (1985). Individual differences ment of representation and memory in infancy. In A. Diamond (Ed.),
in early visual self-recognition. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1181- Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 608. The develop-
1187. ment and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (pp. 1-37). New
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C, & Weiss, M. (1989). Self-develop- York: New Ifork Academy of Sciences.
ment and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146-156. Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). What infant memory tells us about infantile
Liben, L. S. (1977). Memory in the context of development: The Piage- amnesia: Long-term recall and deferred imitation. Journal of Experi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tian approach. In R. Kail & J. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the mental Child Psychology, 59, 497-515.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

development of memory and cognition (pp. 297-331). Hillsdale, NJ: Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by hu-
Erlbaum. man neonates. Nature, 282, 403-404.
Linton, M. (1979). Real-world memory after six years: An in vivo study Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the
of very long term memory. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83 -
Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 69-76). New tork: 99.
Academic Press. Mensink, G. J., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (1988). A model for interfer-
Loveland, K. (1987). Behavior of young children with Down syndrome ence and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434-455.
before the mirror: Finding things reflected. Child Development. 58,
Metcalfe, J. (1990). Composite holographic associative recall model
928-936.
(CHARM) and blended memories in eyewitness testimony. Journal
Loveland, K. (1993). Autism, affordances, and the self. In 0. Neisser
of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 145-160.
(Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 237-253). New York: Cambridge Uni-
Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. L. (1988). Early talk about the past: The
versity Press.
origins of conversational stories about personal experiences. Journal
Mahler, M., Pine, R, & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth
of Child Language, 15, 293-315.
of the infant. New M>rk: Basic Books.
Morford, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1992). Comprehension and pro-
Mandler, J. (1990a). Recall and its verbal expression. In R. Fivush &
duction of gesture in combination with speech in one-word speakers.
J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children. New
Journal of Child Language, 19, 559-580.
York: Cambridge University Press.
Murdock, B.B., Jr. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of
Mandler, J. (1990b). Recall of events by preverbai children. In A. Dia-
item and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609-
mond (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 608.
626.
The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (pp.
Myers, N. A., Ferris, E. E., & Speaker, C. J. (1994). Fifty months of
485-516). New York: New %rk Academy of Sciences.
memory: A longitudinal study in early childhood. Memory, 2, 383-
Mandler, J. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives.
415.
Psychological Review, 99, 587-604.
Nadel, L., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1984). Infantile amnesia: A neurobiolog-
Mandler, J., Fivush, R.. & Reznick, J. S. (1987). The development of
ical perspective. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Advances in the study of
contextual categories. Cognitive Development, 2, 339-354.
communication and affect: Vol. 9. Infant memory (pp. 145-172).
Mandler, J., & McDonough, L. (1993). Concept formation in infancy.
New York: Plenum.
Cognitive Development, 8, 291-318.
Neimark, E. D., & Estes, W. K. (1967). Stimulus sampling theory. San
Mans, L., Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Mirror reaction of
Francisco, CA: Holden-Day.
Down's syndrome infants and toddlers: Cognitive underpinnings of
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self knowledge. Philosophical Psy-
self-recognition. Child Development, 49, 1247-1250.
chology, 1, 35-59.
Marche, T, & Howe, M. L. (1995). Preschoolers report misinformation
despite accurate memory. Developmental Psychology, 31, 554-567. Neisser, U. (1991). Two perceptually given aspects of the self and their
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.). (1985). Parallel distrib- development. Developmental Review. 11, 197-209.
uted processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. Neisser, U. (Ed.). (1993). The perceived self. New York: Cambridge
II. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: Bradford. University Press.
McDaniel, M., & Masson, M. E. J. (1985). Altering memory representa- Nelson, C. A. (1995). The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive
tions through retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- neuroscience perspective. Developmental Psychology, 31, 723-738.
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 371-385. Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA:
McDonough, L., & Mandler, J. M. (1994). Very long-term recall in Harvard University Press.
infants: Infantile amnesia reconsidered. Memory, 2, 339-352. Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobio-
McDonough, L., Mandler, J., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. (1995). graphical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 1-14.
The deferred imitation task as a nonverbal measure of declarative O'Neil, J. B., Friedman, D. P., Bachevalier, J., & Ungerleider, L. G.
memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 92, (1986). Distribution of muscarinic receptors in the brain of the new-
7580-7584. born rhesus monkey. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 12, 809.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Oviatt, S. L. (1985). Tracing developmental changes in language com-
Chicago Press. prehension ability before twelve months of age. In Papers and reports
522 HOWE AND COURAGE

on child language and development (Vol. 24, pp. 87-94). Stanford, of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 608. The development
CA: Stanford University. and neural bases of higher cognitive functions, (pp. 572-595). New
Palmer, S. E. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. "Ybrk: New "fork Academy of Sciences.
In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). The "memory system" of prelinguistic in-
259-303). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. fants. In A. Diamond (Ed.), Annals of the New \ork Academy of
Paris, S. G., & Lindauer, B. K. (1977). Constructive aspects of chil- Sciences: Vol. 608. The development and neural bases of higher cog-
dren's comprehension and memory. In R. Kail & J. Hagen (Eds.), nitive functions, (pp. 517-542). New York: New York Academy of
Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 35- Sciences.
60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rovee-Collier, C., & Bhatt, R. S. (1993). Evidence of long-term memory
Payne, D. G. (1987). Hypermnesia and reminiscence in recall: A histori- in infancy. Annals of Child Development, 9, 1 -45.
cal and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 5-27. Rovee-Collier, C., & Boiler, K. (1995). Interference or facilitation in
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory and autonoetic infant memory? In F. Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference
consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood and inhibition in cognition (pp. 61-103). San Diego, CA: Academic
amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516-548. Press.
Ferris, E. E., Myers, N. A., & Clifton, R. K. (1990). Long-term memory Rovee-Collier, C., Borza, M. A., Adler, S. A., & Boiler, K. (1993). In-
for a single infancy experience. Child Development, 61, 1796-1807. fants' eyewitness testimony: Effects of postevent information on a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Pettito, A. (1993). On the ontogenetic requirements for early language prior memory representation. Memory & Cognition, 21, 267-279.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

acquisition. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. Rovee-Collier, C., & Shyi, G. (1992). A functional and cognitive analy-
McNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: sis of infant long-term retention. In M. L. Howe, C. J. Brainerd, &
Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 365-383). V. F. Reyna (Eds.), Development of long-term retention (pp. 3-55).
Ultrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. New %rk: Springer-Verlag.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of
Basic Books. displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In K. Nelson (Ed.),
Piaget, J. (1968). On the development of memory and identity. Worces- Children's language (Vol. 4, pp. 1-28). New York: Gardner Press.
ter, MA: Clark University Press.
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1973). Memory and intelligence. New York: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
Basic Books.
tion, 13, 501-518.
Pillemer, D. B., & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by
Schacter, D. L., & Moscovitch, M. (1984). Infants, amnesics and disso-
children and adults. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child develop-
ciable memory systems. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Advances in the
ment and behavior (Vol. 21, pp. 297-340). San Diego, CA: Academic
study of communication and affect: Vol. 9. Infant memory (pp. 173-
Press.
216). New York: Plenum.
Pipp, S., Fischer, K., & Jennings, S. (1987). Acquisition of self- and
Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1989). Memory development between 2
mother-knowledge in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 23, 86-96.
and 20. New Ybrk: Springer-Verlag.
Preyer, W. (1893). Mind of the child (Vol. 2). New York: Appleton.
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The relationship between
Priel, B., & De Schonen, S. (1986). Self-recognition: A study of a
affect and cognition in maltreated infants: Quality of attachment and
population without mirrors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
the development of visual self-recognition. Child Development, 55,
ogy, 41, 237-250.
648-658.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1979). Validating computational models: A critique
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Early self-knowledge and
of Anderson's indeterminacy of representation claim. Psychological
emotional development: Visual self-recognition and affective reactions
Review, 86, 383-394.
to mirror self-images in maltreated and non-maltreated infants. Devel-
Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (in press). The neural basis of cognitive
opmental Psychology, 27, 471-478.
development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain
Schulman, A., & Kaplowitz, C. (1977). Mirror image response during
Sciences,
the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiolagy, 10, 133-
Quinn, P. C. (1994). The categorization of above and below spatial
142.
relations by young infants. Child Development, 65, 58-69.
Sheingold, K., & Tenney, Y. J. (1982). Memory for a salient childhood
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative
event. InU. Neisser (Ed.), Memory observed: Remembering in natural
memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93-134.
contexts (pp. 201-212). New York: Freeman.
Reznick, J. S., & Goldfield, B. A. (1992). Rapid change in lexical devel-
opment in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, Shepard, R. N. (1961). Application of a trace model to the retention of

28, 406-413. information in a recognition task. Psychomelrika, 26, 185-203.

Richardson, J. T. E. (1985). The effects of retention tests upon human Shepard, R. N., & Chang, J.-J. (1963). Forced-choice tests of recogni-
learning and memory: An historical review and an experimental analy- tion memory under steady-state conditions. Journal of Verbal Learn-
sis. Educational Psychology, 5, 85-114. ing and Verbal Behavior, 2, 93-101.
Roberts, K. (1988). Retrieval of basic-level category in prelinguistic Slater, A., & Morrison, V. (1991). Visual attention and memory at
infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 21-27. birth. In M. Weiss & P. Zelazo (Eds.) Newborn attention: Biological
Roberts, K., & Cuff, M. D. (1989). Categorization studies of 9- to 15- constraints and the influence of experience (pp. 256-277). Norwood,
month-old infants: Evidence for superordinate categorization? Infant NJ: Ablex.
Behavior and Development, 12, 265-288. Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making
Robinson, J. A. (1996). Perspective, meaning, and remembering. In capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language
D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographi- acquisition (Vol. 2, pp. 1157-1256). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
cal memory (pp. 199-217). New York: Cambridge University Press. Snow, C. (1990). Building memories: The ontogeny of autobiography.
Roediger, H. L., Ill, Rajaram, S., & Srinivas, K. (1990). Specifying In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy
criteria for postulating memory systems. In A. Diamond (Ed.), Annals to childhood (pp. 213-242). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
EARLY AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY 523

Spence, D. P. (1982). Narrative truth and historical truth. New York: TUlving, E. (1984). Precis at Elements of Episodic Memory. Behavioral
Norton. and Brain Sciences, 7, 223-238.
Spence, M. J., & Freeman, M. S. (1996). Newborn infants prefer the Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and the begin-
maternal low-pass filtered voice but not the maternal whispered voice. nings of memory for four early life events. Journal of Experimental
Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 199-212. Psychology: General, 122, 155-165.
Spiker, D., & Ricks, M. (1984). Visual self-recognition in autistic chil- Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Calibration and the effects of knowledge and
dren: Developmental relationships. Child Development, 55, 214-225. reconstruction in retrieval from memory. Cognition, 28, 277-296.
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New \fork: Oxford University Wagenaar, W. A. (1990). My memory: A study of autobiographical
Press. memory over six years. Cognitive Psychology, IS, 225-252.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New M}rk: Waldvogel, S. (1948). The frequency and affective character of early
Basic Books. childhood memories. Psychological Monographs, 62(4, Whole No.
Stem, D. N. (1993). The role of feelings for an interpersonal self. In 291).
U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 205-215). New York: Cam- Weaver, C. A., III. (1993). Do you need a "flash" to form a flashbulb
bridge University Press. memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 39-
Swain, I., Zelazo, P., & Clifton, R. (1993). Newborn infants' memory 46.
for speech sounds retained over 24 hours. Developmental Psychology, Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

29, 312-323. Werner, J., & Perlmutter, M. (1979). Development of visual memory
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Tessler, M., & Nelson, K. (1994). Making memories: The influence of in infancy. In H. W. Reese & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child
joint encoding on later recall by young children. Consciousness and development and behavior (Vol. 14, pp. 1-56). New \brk: Academic
Cognition, 3, 307-326. Press.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (Eds.). (1994). A dynamic systems approach Wetzler, S. E., & Sweeney, J. A. (1986). Childhood amnesia: An empiri-
to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT cal demonstration. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp.
Press. 191-201). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Todd, C. M., & Perlmutter, M. (1980). Reality recalled by preschool White H. (1992). Artificial neural networks: Approximation and learn-
children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Children's memory: New directions ing theory. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
for child development (pp. 69-85). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Winograd, E., & Neisser, U. (Eds.). (1992). Affect and accuracy in
Tomasello, M. (1993). On the interpersonal origins of self-concept. recall: Studies of "flashbulb" memories. New York: Cambridge Uni-
In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 174-184). New York: versity Press.
Cambridge University Press. Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994).
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psy-
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495-552. chology, 30, 553-566.
Tomasello, M., & Mervis, C. (1994). The measurement is great, but
measuring comprehension is still problem. Monographs for the Soci- Received May 4, 1995
ety for Research in Child Development, 59, (5, Serial No. 242), 174- Revision received September 17, 1996
179. Accepted September 24, 1996 •

You might also like