Hammill Institute On Disabilities: Sage Publications, Inc

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Hammill Institute on Disabilities

Strategy Training and Teacher- vs. Student-Controllled Study Conditions: Effects on LD


Students' Spelling Performance
Author(s): Steve Graham and Sally Freeman
Source: Learning Disability Quarterly, Vol. 8, No. 4 (Autumn, 1985), pp. 267-274
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc.
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1510590 .
Accessed: 14/06/2014 14:06

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc. and Hammill Institute on Disabilities are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Learning Disability Quarterly.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
STRATEGYTRAINING AND TEACHER-VS.
STUDENT-CONTROLLLEDSTUDY
CONDITIONS: EFFECTS ON LD STUDENTS'
SPELLING PERFORMANCE
Steve Graham and Sally Freeman

Abstract. This study examined learning disabled students' spelling performance


in response to strategy training and variations in study conditions. After training
in the use of a five-step study strategy, subjects studied spelling words under one
of three conditions: (a) directed study; (b) student-controlled; and (c) teacher-
monitored. Students assigned to a control group studied words in any manner they
chose. Results indicated that students who were taught the five-step study strategy
recalled the correct spelling of more words than controls who devised their own
study method. However, the spelling performance of students who received strategy
training was not differentially affected by variations in study conditions. Results were
interpreted to suggest that learning disabled students' spelling difficulties are
associated with problems in self-regulation of organized, strategic behavior.

Deliberate learning for the purpose of im- which learningfor the purpose of immediate recall
mediate recall is important to academic success is critical.The deliberate memorization of a basic
since many school tasks require purposive spellingvacabularyis an integralpartof most spell-
memorization of ideas, words, symbols, and so ing programs (Graham, 1982, 1983a). Although
forth. Compared to normal students, children spelling problems are particularlypronounced in
identified as learning disabled (LD) exhibit per- the LD population (Carpenter & Miller, 1982;
vasive performance deficitson tasks requiringim- Moran, 1981; Poplin, Gray, Larsen, Banikowski,
mediate recall (Bauer, 1979; Connor, 1983; & Mehring,1980), spellinginstructionfor disabled
Torgesen, 1980). LD students' poor performance learnershas received littleattentionin the research
on memory tasks has traditionallybeen attributed literature. It is generally agreed, however, that
to specific structuralor ability deficits. However, allowing students to devise their own methods for
recent research indicates that these students' per- studying spelling words is not advisable (Graham
formance problems are frequentlyassociated with & Miller, 1979).
strategy-productiondeficiencies, that is, difficulties According to Tobias (1976), students with
in self-regulation of organized, strategic behavior severe academic difficultiesrequire a great deal
(Harris,in press; Torgesen & Kail, 1980; Wong,
1980, 1982). Although LD students are often
capable of using appropriate learning strategies, STEVE GRAHAM, Ed.D., is Assistant Professor,
they typically fail to use them spontaneously or Department of Special Education, Universityof
flexibly in response to the requirements of a Maryland.
specific task. SALLYFREEMAN,Ph.D., is Assistant Professor,
Spelling is an example of an academic area in School of Education, University of Montana.

Volume 8, Fall 1985 267

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
of instructional support, especially when the for several reasons. As Johnston (1984) noted,
material to be learned is novel. Several ex- mastery of a study strategy allows students to
perimentershave examined whether instructional develop productivelearningbehaviorswhich may
support via modificationsin the externalprovision lead to increasedmotivationand on-task behavior.
of the lesson (i.e.,teacher-controlledinterventions) Student-controlled procedures may also insulate
results in an improvement in LD students' im- a given learning task, thereby reducing student
mediate recall of spelling words. For example, in distractibility.However, Doyle (1983) indicated
a study by Foster and Torgesen (1983), the spell- that without a great deal of training, many low-
ing task was structured so that all subjects were ability students will use learning strategies only
required to repetitively spell target words in the when instructionalprompts are available. Thus,
same manner. In two other studies (Bryant, even afterLD students have been taught a specific
Drabin, & Gettinger, 1981; Gettinger, Bryant, & study strategy, they may still require teacher
Fayne, 1982), effective teaching principles such assistance in order to apply it successfully.
as mastery learning, cumulative and distributed The purpose of the present study was to com-
practice, and corrective feedback were incor- pare LD students' immediate recall of spelling
porated into the spellingtask. Although these pro- words, after instruction in the use of a specific
cedures were found to be effective,developing and study strategy, under study conditions which
implementing instructionalstrategies add to the varied in terms of teacher versus student control.
alreadytaxing demands on a teacher'stime. Final- Fourth-gradeLD students were assigned to one
ly, in additionto often failingto obtaindurableand of four study conditions. In directed study,
generalizable effects (Harris, in press; Sabatino, students were firsttaught a five-step study strategy
Miller,& Schmidt, 1981), teacher-controlled in- and then given verbal guidance by an instructor
terventions may preclude students learning how in how to apply each strategy step during subse-
to learn. quent study sessions. Students in the student-
LD students' performance on tasks that require controlled group were taught the same five-step
immediate recall may also be improved by procedure and were told to use the strategyto in-
teaching them effective and efficient study dependently directtheir own study behavior. The
strategies (Graham, in press; Harris, 1982). To teacher-monitored condition, in turn, involved a
date, Bendell, Tollefson, and Fine (1980) have combinationof teacher and student controlin that,
conducted the only published study which ex- after being taught the aforementioned strategy,
amines whether prompting LD students to direct subjects were told not only to use the procedure
their own learning through the use of a specific independently but also that the instructorwould
study strategy(i.e.,student-controlledintervention) render assistance whenever necessary. Finally, a
results in improved immediate recall of spelling group of students in a free study condition were
words. In this investigation, LD adolescents with allowed to learn words in any manner they chose,
an external locus-of-control orientationincreased thus acting as controls. The performance of the
the number of correctly recalled spelling words subjects in the three strategy-traininggroups was
when they were told to independently use a study compared to determine optimal teaching pro-
procedure described by the examiner. In contrast, cedures for LD students. In addition, a mean per-
disabled learnerswith an internallocus-of-control formance by the controlgroup, significantlypoorer
orientation performed best when assigned to a than that of the three strategy-traininggroups,
study condition in which they were allowed to would provide evidence that LD students' spell-
learn words in any manner they chose. The find- ing difficultiesare associated with problemsin self-
ings from this study suggest that LD students' regulation of organized, strategic behaviors.
responses to different types of study conditions Since scores on a dictated spelling test may be,
may vary depending upon their locus-of-control in part, a function of prior spelling achievement
orientation. and/or locus of control (Bendell et al., 1980;
Although the resultsof the Bendell et al. (1980) Deno, Mirkin,Lowry, & Kuehnle, 1980), a spell-
investigation provide only partialsupport for the ing achievement test and a standardizedmeasure
use of student-controlledinterventionsin spelling, of locus-of-control orientation were administered
teaching children a strategy for regulating their to each subject priorto any of the study sessions.
own study behavior should prove advantageous In addition, the on-task behavior of subjectsin the

268 Learning DisabilityQuarterly

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
student-controlled and free-study conditions was (IAR)scale, developed to assess "children's beliefs
monitored. Data on the on-task behavior of sub- that they, ratherthan other people, are responsi-
jects in the other two study conditions were not ble for their intellectual-academic successes and
collected since these students were prompted to failures"(Crandall,Katkovsky,& Crandall, 1965,
stay on task. p. 91), consists of 34 orally administered items
which are designed to measure a student's locus-
METHOD of-control orientation (internal or external) in
Subjects academic situations. The developers of the scale
Subjects, all of whom were enrolled in public have reported a test-retestreliabilitycoefficient of
schools in Northern and Central Indiana, were .69 for third- through fifth-gradestudents. Addi-
selected on the basis of the following criteria:(a) tionally, MacDonald (1973) found that the IAR
fourth-grade placement; (b) parental consent to has "acceptablereliabilityand evidence of both
participate in the study; (c) IQ scores between divergent and convergent validity"(p. 195).
84-116 on either the Stanford-Binet Intelligence The Testof WrittenSpelling (TWS)is a dictated-
Test or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for word test consisting of two subtests - Predictable
Children-Revised; (d) scores of 7 or less on a Wordsand UnpredictableWords - that measures
pretestof the 30 wordstargetedfor instructiondur- a student's ability to spell phonetic and non-
ing the experimentalconditions;and (e) identifica- phonetic words (Larsen & Hammill, 1976). The
tion as learning disabled by their local school authors reportinternalreliabilitycoefficientsof .80
system. Since selection criteriafor identificationas or greater for grades two through seven. Also,
learningdisabled often differacross school districts validitycoefficients ranging from .63 to .92 have
(DeRuiter,Ferrell,& Kass, 1975; Shepard, Smith, been noted for the TWS and four other spelling
& Vofir, 1983), a two-step process was im- tests.
plemented (Graham, 1980) to provide a substan- Dictated-word tests. Two dictated-wordtests,
tial degree of certainty that subjects were indeed A and B, containing 15 items each, were designed
learning disabled. First, the LD teachers in each specificallyas part of this study. The followingtwo
of the participatingelementary schools completed criteriawere used to insurethat the test items were
a summary sheet on prospective subjectspertain- appropriate:(a) the meaning of each word should
ing to age, sex, grade, IQ, achievement test scores, be familiarto fourth-grade students; and (b) test
attemptedremedialproceduresand results,clinical items should consist of words that fourth-grade
impressions, and other pertinentinformationsuch students infrequently encounter in their reading
as medical background. Second, three specialists or writingactivities. On the basis of these criteria,
independently rated each summary on a scale 30 test items were selected. Accordingto informa-
from 1 to 100 as to the probabilitythat the sub- tion compiled by Dale and O'Rourke (1976), the
ject met the U.S.O.E. definition of learning meaning of each test item was familiarto 75% or
disabilities.Each specialist had at least two years more of all fourthgraders.Furthermore,examina-
of graduate training in learning disabilities and tion of the Thorndike and Lorge (1963)
three years' experience in diagnosing this syn- vocabulary list revealed that each item occurred
drome. Subjects were judged to be learning dis- less than 50 times per million writtenwords. The
abled if none of the independent ratings was 75 30 test items were randomly assigned to test A or
or less and if their mean ratingswere 80 or above. B, and theirposition on each test was randomized.
Based on these criteria, 40 subjects were Observation procedures. On-taskbehavior
selected from an initialpool of 84 fourth-gradeLD was assessed via a 30-second momentary time-
students. Thirty-two of the subjects were male, sampling system (Sulzer-Azaroff& Mayer, 1977).
eight were female. Subjects'mean IQ was 97, with Using this procedure, the examiner records
a mean chronological age of 10 years and 8 whether or not the subject is on task at the end
months. The subjects' average score on the of each 30-second interval.On-task behavior was
30-word pretest was 2.7; their mean grade defined as students (a) having eye contact with
equivalent on the Testof WrittenSpelling (Larsen the list of study words or the practice sheet used
& Hammill, 1976) was 2.53. for writingthe study words; (b)pronouncingwords
Instrumentation on the study list; (c) asking the examiner to pro-
The Intellectual Achievement Responsibility nounce a word; (d) verbalizingthe steps in a word-

Volume 8, Fall 1985 269

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
study technique; (e) spelling a word out loud; (f) Subjects in the free-study group received in-
asking the examiner how much time is left in the struction and practice in how to play the spelling
study session; (g) performing a step in a word- game, Individual Challenge (see Graham,
study technique; and/or (h) writingwords on the Freeman, & Miller, 1981). To play this game the
practice sheet. student selects a word and writes it verticallyon
In order to determine reliability,two examiners a piece of paper. The student then tries to write
independently scored four of the experimentalses- a word from each letter. The instructional pro-
sions. Interobserveragreement, computed as 100 cedures used to teach the spelling game were
x number of agreements divided by number of similarto those used in masteringthe word-study
agreements plus disagreements,ranged from 81% technique.
to 95% Study sessions. Afterthe trainingsession each
Procedures subjectwas presented with several sheets of paper
Prior to data collection, three instructors in- and a writtenlist of the 15 words from one of the
volved in the study were required to attend six two dictated-word tests. To control for possible
practicesessions. Duringthe firsttwo sessions they order effects, half the subjects in each study con-
were familiarizedwiththe experimentalprocedures dition were randomly assigned items from form
and the assessment instruments. During the last A; the other half received items from form B. The
four, participantspracticedadministeringboth the examinerpronounced each of the assigned words,
assessment instruments and the treatment pro- and subjectswere informedthat they were allowed
cedures to a normally achieving fourth-grade 30 minutesto studythe wordsbeforehavingto take
student. a spelling test.' Subjects were furthertold that the
Followingthe practicesessions, the IAR and the examinerwould pronounce, on request, any of the
TWS were individuallyadministeredto each sub- study words. Two days later, subjects participated
ject, whereupon the 40 LD students were ran- in a second study session, identicalto the firstwith
domly assigned to one of the four study condi- the exception of the word lists. In the second ses-
tions: directed study, student-controlled,teacher- sion subjectswho had studied words from form A
monitored, and free study. Each subject was also were requiredto study items fromform B, and vice
randomly assigned to one instructorwho directed versa.
the study sessions. All procedureswere individual- At the conclusion of each study session, the ap-
ly administered.' propriatedictated-wordtest was administered.The
Student training. Each subjectparticipatedin examiner pronounced each test item, used it in a
a 20-minute trainingsession. Subjects assigned to sentence, and pronounced it again. Subjectsin the
the directed-study,student-controlled,or teacher- student-controlled and free-study groups were
monitored groups received instructionand prac- asked to describe the method they had used to
tice in using a five-step word-study strategy to study the spelling words.
study unknown spelling words. This strategy, Study conditions. Subjectswho were assigned
which was based on Graham's (1983a) recornm- to the directed-studycondition were verballyguid-
mendations, required the students to: (a) say the ed by the instructorduringthe two study sessions.
word; (b) write and say the word; (c) check the For each word, the examiner read the study-
word; (d) trace and say the word; (e) write the strategy steps to the student. If the subject per-
word from memory and check; and (f) repeat the formed a step correctly,the examiner proceeded
first five steps. to the next step. If a step was performed incor-
Trainingin the use of the word-study strategy rectly, the examiner demonstrated the correct
involved (a) modeling: the subjectwas directedto response and asked the subjectto repeat the step.
watch the examiner model the procedure; (b) If all 15 assigned words were completed before
practice with assistance: under the directionof the 30 minutes had expired, the subject was allowed
examiner, the student practiced using the pro- to restudy self-selected words.
cedure with several different words; and (c) In contrast, subjects in the teacher-monitored
demonstration of proficiency: if the subject was group were explicitlyinstructedto independently
able to successfully apply the word-study strategy use the word-studystrategyand were told that the
to two consecutive words Withoutany assistance, instructor would provide assistance when
instruction was terminated. necessary. Assistance included (a) prompting the

270 Learning Disability Quarterly

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 1
Analysis of Covariance with Repeated Measures

Source df SS F

Between-Subjects
Study Conditions(A) 3 148.549 3.88*
1st Covariate(TWS) 1 846.060 66.28* *
2nd Covariate(IAR) 1 .220 .02
All Covariates 2 846.066 33.14* *
Errorbetween 34 433.983
Within-Subjects
Sessions (B) 1 .612 .22
Ax B 3 3.237 .38
Errorwithin 36 101.650

*p .05.
* p .001.

student to use the study strategy or a particular The four-levelbetween-subjectsvariablewas study


step; (b)providingcorrectivefeedback if a step was condition, whereas the within-subjects variable
performed incorrectly; (c) answering questions represented the two study sessions. The depen-
related to the spelling task; and (d) promptingthe dent variablewas subjects'scores on the dictated-
student to stay on task. If the subject completed word tests; the covariatesconsisted of the subjects'
all 15 words before 30 minutes had expired, scores on the IAR and TWS.
he/she was instructed to restudy self-selected
words. During each study session, the examiner RESULTS
recorded the amount and kind of assistance Separate univariate analyses of variance were
provided. performedto determine any significantdifferences
Subjects assigned to the student-controlled between the four study groups on important
group were explicitlydirectedto use independently subject-selectionvariables.Thus, no significantdif-
the word strategy, whereas subjects in the free- ferences between the groups were found on
study condition were simply told to study the word chronologicalage (F = 1.19, df =3/36, p < .01),
list. Students in both groups were informed that IQ (F = 196, df = 3/36, p < .01) or present
the only assistance the examiner would provide scores (F = 2.33, df = 3/36, p < .01).
was pronouncing, upon request, any of the study A summary of the analysis of covariance with
words. During each study session, examiners repeated measures is presented in Table 1; ob-
recorded on-task behavior via the observation tained and adjusted means are listed in Table 2.
instruments. Although subjects' scores on the TWS and the
Design combination of TWS and IAR scores were
An experimentalposttest-only design (Cambell significantly related to performance on the
& Stanley, 1963) was employed to examine the dictated-wordstests, IAR scores were not. In ad-
effectiveness of the four study conditions. Data dition, the F ratio for interactionand session was
were analyzed using a 4 x 2 analysis of covariance not statisticallysignificant. The F ratio for study
with repeated measures (Dixon & Brown, 1977). group, however, was significant. Consequently,

Volume 8, Fall 1985 271

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
multiple comparisons were made among the ad- directed to study in any manner they chose. This
justed means by using the Newman-Keuls test finding is consistent with those of several other
(Hicks, 1973). Resultsshowed that at the .01 level, studies (Bryantet al, 1981; Foster& Torgesen,1983;
the mean performance of the subjects in each of Gettinger et al., 1982). In addition, LD students
the three experimental groups was greater than can be taught how to successfully direct and
the mean score of subjects in the control group regulate their own study behavior. Results of this
(free-studycondition). However, no significantdif- study as well as those of previous research
ferences were noted between the mean scores of (Graham, 1983b; Harris & Graham, 1985;
the three experimental groups. Kosiewicz,Hallahan,Lloyd, & Graves, 1982) sug-
A one-way analysis of variance was performed gest that self-controlprocedures can be effective-
to determine any significant differences between ly used to improve LD students' writing skills.
the student-controlled and the free-study groups The present study also provides support for the
on on-task behavior. Results showed that the two contention that students with learning problems
groups did not differ significantly (F = 3.09, should not be allowed to devise theirown methods
df = 1/18, p < .05). for studying unknown spelling words. Subjects
who were told to study in any manner they chose
DISCUSSION were able to immediately recall the correct spell-
Results of the present study indicate that LD ing of less than 20% of the words they were
students' performance on an academic task such directedto learn duringa 30-minute study period.
as spelling can be improved via adult-imposed At least two explanations may be offered for these
processing plans or structure.Thus, LD students students' poor performance. One, the students
who were taught a task-specificstudy strategyand may not have been actively engaged in the pro-
were instructedto use the procedureindependent- cess of learning.This explanation,however, seems
ly and/or received direct instructionalassistance unlikely. Although students assigned to the self-
were able to immediately recall the correct spell- controlled condition correctly recalled the spell-
ing of more words than students who were ing of more words than students in the free-study

Table 2
Obtained and Adjusted Means for Dependent Measure

Standard
Study Condition Obtained Mean Deviation Adjusted Mean

DirectStudy
Session One 8.50 5.64 6.90
Session Two 8.50 4.52 6.90
Teacher-Monitored
Session One 5.40 3.24 6.90
Session Two 5.30 3.19 7.10
StrategyTraining
Session One 7.80 4.08 6.60
Session Two 7.50 5.04 6.30
Free Study
Session One 2.20 4.66 3.20
Session Two 3.00 3.59 4.00

272 Learning DisabilityQuarterly

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
group, the two groups' on-task behavior was REFERENCES
similar.A second and more plausible explanation R.
Bauer, (1979). Recallaftera shortdelayandacquisi-
is that free-study subjects did not spontaneously tion in learningdisabledand nondisabledchildren.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 596-607.
produce effective strategic behaviors. Informal
observationsby the examiners revealed that these Bendell,D., Tollefson,N., & Fine,M. (1980). Interac-
tion of locus-of-controlorientationand the perfor-
students often used ineffective study procedures
mance of learningdisabledadolescents.Journalof
such as repeatedly rereading a stimulus item or
Learning Disabilities, 13, 83-86.
writingthe correct spelling of the word only once. Bryant,D., Drabin,I., & Gettinger,M. (1981). Effects
Furthermore, it was possible to improve spelling of varyingunitsizeon spellingachievementin learn-
performance by either training and/or inducing ing disabledchildren.Journalof LearningDisabilities,
students to perform operations that have been 14, 200-203.
shown to result in retention of information. Cambell,P., & Stanley,J. (1963). Experimental
and
In terms of immediate recall, no significantdif- quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago:
ferences were noted between the direct-study, RandMcNallyPub. Co.
teacher-monitored, and strategy-traininggroups. Carpenter,D., & Miller,L. (1982). Spellingabilityof
This finding suggests that once instructionin an reading disabled LD students and able readers.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 65-70.
appropriate spelling strategy has been provided,
a student-controlled intervention can be as effec- Connor,F (1983).Improving forlear-
schoolinstruction
TheTeachersCollegeInstitute.
ningdisabledchildren:
tive as teacher-controlledinstruction.Consequent-
Exceptional Education Quarterly,4, 23-44.
ly, providinginstructionalsupportto students who Crandall,V.C.,Katkovsky, W.,& Crandall,V.J.(1965).
already possess an effective word-study method Children'sbeliefsin theirowncontrolof reinforcement
may have littleor no effect on their performance. in intellectual-academicachievementsituations.Child
Since self-control procedures may result in pro- Development, 91-109.
ductive learning behaviors and may minimizethe Dale, E., & O'Rourke,J. (1976). The living word
supervisory time required of teachers (Harris, vocabulary.Elgin,IL:Field EnterpriseEducational
1982; Johnston, 1984), the use of student- Corp.
controlled interventions to improve spelling per- Deno,S., Mirkin,P., Lowry,L., & Kuehnle,K. (1980).
formance warrantsfurther investigation. Relationshipamong simple measures of spelling and
Finally,no main effect was found for session or performanceon standardizedachievement tests. Min-
the interactionbetween session and study condi- neapolis,MN:University of Minnesota.(ERICDocu-
ment ReproductionServiceNo. ED 197 508)
tion, indicating that the four student groups
demonstrated a similarperformance pattern over DeRuiter,J., Ferrell,W., & Kass,C. (1975). Learning
classification
disability by Bayesianaggregationof test
a short period of time. Significantdifferencesmay
results. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 8, 32-39.
have emerged if the number of study sessions had
Dixon,W.,& Brown,N. (1977).Biomedicalcomputer
been increased.This possibilityshould be examined programsP-series.Berkeley:University
of California
in the future. In addition, it is recommended that Press.
subsequent investigationsfocus on delayed as well Doyle,W. (1983). Academicwork.Reviewof Educa-
as immediate recall of words targeted for tional Research, 53, 159-199.
instruction. Foster,K., & Torgesen,J. (1983). The effectsof direct
In conclusion, the present study provides studyon the spellingperformanceof two subgroups
evidence that some of the spelling difficultiesof of learningdisabledstudents. LearningDisability
LD children are associated with problems in self- Quarterly, 6, 252-257.
regulation of organized, strategicbehaviors rather Gettinger,M.,Bryant,D.,& Fayne,H. (1982). Design-
than specific ability or structural deficits. Even forlearning-disabled
ingspellinginstruction children:
An emphasison unitsize, distributedpractice,and
though LD students appear capable of using task-
trainingfor transfer.Journalof Special Education, 16,
specific strategies effectively, they often fail to 439-448.
spontaneously engage in strategicbehaviors that Graham,S. (1980). Wordrecognitionskillsof learning
adequately match the requirements of a specific disabledchildrenand average students.Reading
task. As demonstrated in this study, however, LD Psychology, 2, 23-33.
students'performanceon an academic task which Graham,S. (1982). Compositionresearchand prac-
requiresdeliberatememorizationcan be improved tice: A unified approach. Focus on Exceptional
by trainingthem to use an efficientstudy strategy. Children, 14, 1-16.

Volume 8, Fall 1985 273

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Graham, S. (1983a). Effective spelling instruction. written expression abilitiesin learning disabled and
Elementary School Journal, 83, 560-568. non-disabled students at three grade levels. Learn-
Graham, S. (1983b). The effect of self-instructionalpro- ing Disability Quarterly,3, 46-53.
cedures on LD students' handwritingperformance. Sabatino, D., Miller,P., & Schmidt, C. (1981). Can in-
Learning Disability Quarterly,6, 231-234. telligence be alteredthroughcognitive training?Jour-
Graham, S. (in press). Teaching basic academic skills nal of Special Education, 15, 125-144.
to learning disabled students: A model of the Sheppard, L., Smith, M., & Vofir,C. (1983). Character-
teaching-learning process. Journal of Learning istics of pupils identified as learning disabled.
Disabilities. American Educational Research Journal, 20,
Graham, S., Freeman, S., & MillerL. (1981). Spelling 309-331.
games and activities.W. Lafayette,IN:Purdue Univer- Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. (1977). Applying
sity. (ERICDocument ReproductionService No. ED behavioranalysisprocedures withchildrenand youth.
208 425) New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Graham, S., & MillerL. (1979). Spelling research and Tobias, S. (1976). Achievement treatment interactions.
practice: A unified approach. Focus on Exceptional Review of Educational Research, 46, 61-74.
Children, 12, 1-16. Torgesen, J. (1980). The use of efficienttask strategies
Harris, K. (in press). The effects of cognitive training by learningdisabledchildren:Conceptual and educa-
on privatespeech and task performanceduringprob- tional implications. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
lem solving among learning disabled and normally 13, 364-371.
achieving children. Journal of Abnormal Child Torgesen, J., & Kail, R. (1980). Memory processes in
Psychology. exceptional children. In B. Keogh (Ed.), Advances in
Harris,K. (1982). Cognitive-behaviormodification:Ap- special education (Vol. 1) (pp. 55-101). Greenwich,
plication with exceptional students. Focus on Excep- CT: JAI Press.
tional Children, 15, 1-16. Thorndike, E., & Lorge, I. (1963). The teacher's word
Harris, K., & Graham, S. (1985). Improving learning book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau of
disabled students' composition skills: A self-control Publications.
strategy training approach. Learning Disability Wong, B. (1980). Activatingthe inactive learner: Use
Quarterly,8(1), 27-36. of questions/prompts to enhance comprehension
Hicks, C. (1973). Fundamental concepts in the design and retention of implied information in learning
of experiments. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & disabled children. Learning Disability Quarterly,3,
Winston. 28-37.
Johnston, P. (1984). Instruction and student in- Wong, B. (Ed.). (1982). Metacognition and learning
dependence. Elementary School Journal, 84, disabilities[Specialissue]. Topicsin Learningand Lear-
338-344. ning Disabilties,2(1).
Kosiewicz, M., Hallahan, D., Lloyd, J., & Graves, A.
(1982). Effects of self-instructionand self-correction FOOTNOTES
procedures on handwriting performance. Learning 'A pilot study was conducted with four fourth-grade
Disability Quarterly,5, 71-78. LD students to determine the number of words to be
Larsen, S., & Hammill, D. (1976). Testof writtenspell- studied and the length of the study session. On the basis
ing. San Rafael, CA: Academic Therapy Pub. of pilot-studydata, it was decided that 15 words would
McDonald, A. (1973). Internal-externallocus of con- be presented during each session and that subjects
trol. In J. Robinson & P. Shaver (Eds.), Measures of would be allowed 30 minutes to study the words.
social-psychologicalattitudes (pp. 169-244). Ann Ar- This study was in part supported by a grantfrom the
bor: University of Michigan, Institute for Social United States Office of Education (G008102716). The
Research. ideas expressed herein are the authors' and do not
Moran,M. (1981). Performanceof learningdisabledand necessarily reflect USOE position or policy.
low-achieving secondary students on formalfeatures
of a paragraph-writing task. Learning Disability
Quarterly,4, 271-280. Requests for reprints should be addressed to: Steve
Poplin, M., Gray, R., Larsen, S., Banikowski, A., & Graham, Department of Special Education, University
Mehring,T. (1980). A comparison of components of of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742.

274 Learning DisabilityQuarterly

This content downloaded from 195.78.108.40 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 14:06:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like