Ges Ten 1976

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Consulting and Clinital Psychology

1976, Vol. 44, No. 5, 77S-786

A Health Resources Inventory: The Development of a Measure


of the Personal and Social Competence of
Primary-Grade Children
Ellis L, Gesten
University of Rochester
A series of interrelated studies describe the scale development and empirical
validation of the Health Resources Inventory (HRI), a teacher measure of
primary-grade children's competency-related behavior. Oblique factor analysis
of the HRI for matched samples (N = 298 and N — 294) yielded comparable
solutions, each with five internally consistent competence dimensions. Tcst-
retcst reliability was .87 for the full scale and ranged from .72 to .91 on in-
dividual factor scales. Parametric findings revealed that (a) girls had con-
sistently higher competence scores than boys, (b) county children had signifi-
cantly higher scores than city children, and (c) there were no consistent
grade level differences. Correlations between the HRI and a symptom scale
indicate that although competence and pathology arc strongly (inversely) re-
lated for overall, global judgments, competence and pathology arc more in-
dependent at the level of individual factors. The HRI discriminated between
normal and disturbed children, and in a more stringent validity test, sensitively
differentiated competence levels within a normative sample. Limitations of the
inventory are discussed along with implications for future work.

Psychology in the past decade has been Gardner, & Zax, 1967; White, 1973; Zax &
shifting its focus away from pathology, toward Cowen, 1972; Zax & Specter, 1974). The rea-
effective functioning. Those who urge such a sons for mental health's historical focus on
change cite the inadequacies of intervention symptoms have been extensively documented
strategies based on the illness, or medical, elsewhere (Albee, 1967; Bloom, 196S; Sara-
model of disorder (Cowen, 1973; Cowen, son & Ganzer, 1968). They are less relevant
to the present discussion than one important
consequence of that preoccupation. Simply
This research is based on a dissertation submitted put, we know woefully little about "normal"
in partial fulfillment for the PhD degree at the Uni- behavior, a subject that has been largely ig-
versity of Rochester. Appreciation is gratefully ac- nored and often denned by default as the
knowledged to Emory L. Cowen, who as research
advisor was an unfailing source of useful criticism,
absence of symptoms. Our understanding of
guidance, and support throughout this project. Other effective, optimal human functioning is even
committee members, all of whom gave generously of more primitive.
their time were Michael L. Davidson, Howard P. Many arguments have been developed sup-
Iker, and Gerald A. Gladstein. porting the need for a psychology of health or
I am also grateful to Robert A. Caldwell, Ray-
mond P. Lorion, and Alice B. Wilson for their competence to replace, or at least temper,
contributions to data collection and analyses. A spe- past emphases on pathology. Jahoda's seminal
cial note of thanks goes to the many school teachers volume (Jahoda, 1958) prepared for the
and administrators whose patience and cooperation Joint Commission advanced one of the first
made this project happen.
Parts of this article were presented at the 46th models for viewing positive mental health.
annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Associ- From the perspective of treatment, White
ation, New York, April 3, 1975. (1973) reasons persuasively that competencies
This research was supported by Grant MH 11820- in clients must be identified and reinforced
04 from the National Institute of Mental Health,
Experimental and Special Training Branch.
to maximize outcomes. Kelly (Note 1), from
Requests for reprints should be sent to Ellis Ges- an ecological-community psychology perspec-
ten, Department of Psychology, University of Ro- tive, suggested that concepts of health must
chester, Rochester, New York 14627. take into account both the nature of the
775
776 ELLIS L. GESTKN

person and the social settings in which the doing this will require appropriate assessment
person is involved. For Kelly, competence tools.
judgments rest on observation and analysis of Accordingly, this study seeks to develop a
transactions and relationships between persons measure, for teacher use, of the school-related
and settings over time. Notwithstanding these personal and social competencies of primary-
cogent arguments and community psychol- grade children. Teachers are good judges of
ogy's growing plea for a psychology of health, children's competencies both because they
there has been little research to shift the con- have significant contacts with them that form
cepts of positive mental health and compe- a rich observational base and because they
tence from rallying cry to scientific constructs. have been shown to be reasonably sensitive
Two key, related factors have made re- raters of wa/adaptation in children (Beilin,
search in this area difficult. First, mental 1959; Clarfiekl, 1974).
health professionals lack an adequate con-
ceptual system to permit them to communi- METHOD
cate systematically about positive, adaptive Health Resources Inventory (HRI) Scale
behavior. Hollister (1967), for example, noted Development
that although the English language contains
the word "trauma" to describe psychologically Hem selection and teacher instructions. Lacking a
definitive framework for defining positive mental
debilitating experiences, it lacks a word to health, scale items were drawn from several sources:
describe the opposite, that is, psychologically (a) prior health scales (Garmczy, 1973; Jones, 1972;
health-building experiences. He coined the Kohn & Rosman, 1973; Rolf, 1972; Ross, Lacey, &
word stren to denote such events. Although Parton, 196S; Rubcnstcin & Fisher, 1974; Sceman,
1963; White, 1972); (b) statements about healthy
labeling a concept does not assure its common functioning in the literature (Jahoda, 1958; Shoben,
understanding, at least it encourages com- 1957; Smith, 1968; White, 1959); and (c) health
munication and research about it. In this characteristics suggested by teachers, mental health
manner, Finkel's recent studies (Finkel, 1974, professionals, and parents in pilot work. An initial
1975) have helped to articulate the construct pool of 79 items was first reduced by having several
teachers judge each item for (a) case of rateabilitj'
underlying the label stren and, by so doing, and (b) estimated variability across children. Items
have advanced our understanding of healthy that teachers considered difficult to rate or unlikely
functioning. to discriminate were dropped. Several clinical psy-
Lack of instrumentation also limits research chologists screened the remaining items for redun-
dancy or ambiguous wording.
on psychological health and competence. But The final sample of 54 items ostensibly covered
if mental health's focus is to move from re- several competence-related dimensions including self-
pairing dysfunction toward building for health, concept, affective expression, classroom response, mo-
new measures are needed. An important cau- tivation, interpersonal skills, achievement, and so-
tion to keep in mind in developing such mea- cialization. Even though these labels were useful
descriptively in the early scale development stages,
sures is that competence may in part be situ- ultimate scale structures were established empirically
ation specific. Such a view is consistent with through factor analysis.
converging evidence from several sources (Mc- Teachers rated all 54 HRT items according to
Clelland, 1973; Wallace, 1966) that the how well they described children on a S-point scale
from not at all (1) to very well (5). A second small
search for global tests that can measure nearly section of the rating form included three 7-point
everything in nearly everyone is illusory. Spe- scales (a) "How well do you know the child?," (b)
cific tests are needed to measure specific "How likable is he (she) ?," and (c) "How serious
competence-related variables in specific set- are his (her) school adjustment problems?"
tings. Procedure. Sixty-five volunteer teacher raters for
this research came from 12 schools, 5 in the Ro-
The school environment is an extremely chester City School District and 7 in two adjacent
important one in which to assess competence county districts. All schools had agreed to partici-
and its development. Both because schools pate in a broad series of normative studies of chil-
offer ready access to many children and be- dren's school adjustment problems and resources.
Teachers were randomly assigned one of two tasks
cause they arc so important in child sociali- (a) to complete I I R l s for 15 randomly selected
zation, they offer special opportunities for children from their class roster or (b) to complete
assessing and promoting competence. But HRIs and a symptom scale, the Classroom Activity
A HEALTH RESOURCES INVENTORY 777

Rating Scale (CARS), for six children. This procedure the third factor, consists of items reflecting effective
assured both an adequate HRI normative sample as interpersonal functioning (e.g., "plays enthusiasti-
well as a subgroup for which both health and pa- cally"; "is affectionate toward others"; "well-liked
thology data were available and equalized time by classmates"; "shares things with others"). Factor
demands across teachers. HRIs were filled out around 4, Rules, reflects the child's ability to function within
the midyear, by which time teachers had had 3—5 the constraints of the school environment (e.g., "is
months of contact with children. polite and courteous"; "follows class rules"; "is well-
Subjects, There were 592 subjects in the norma- behaved in school"; "accepts legitimate imposed
tive sample with the following demographic break- limits"). The last factor, Frustration Tolerance,
down: 311 boys, 281 girls; 300 city children, 292 measures the child's ability to cope with failure and
county children; 237 first graders, 197 second grad- other social pressures (e.g., "copes well with failure";
ers, and 158 third graders. There arc significant "accepts criticism well"; "faces the pressures of
socioeconomic differences between city and county competition well"). Sum Factors is a composite index
samples, reflected by higher income and educational of competence computed by summing the five indi-
levels and a greater percentage of white collar work- vidual factor scores. Intcrcorrclalions among indi-
ers in county families. Chi-squarc tests for the vidual HRI factors are positive and significant,
variables of sex, grade, and residence were all non- ranging from a low of .28 to a high of .53 (Mdn r
significant. = .41).
Factor analyses. To permit independent replica- The usual procedure of constructing approximate
tion of an obtained factor solution, the initial total factor scales exclusively from highly loading items
sample (A) was divided into two subgroups (298 in was not followed. Instead, exact factor scoring,
Sample B and 294 in Sample C) stratified by sex, taking into account item intercorrelations largely
grade, and residence. This was done by halving each ignored by approximate scoring methods, was used
teacher's contribution to the total subject pool across (Harmon, 1967).
these three demographic variables. Chi-square tests Reliability, Reliability was tested by having 11 of
indicated that there were no age, sex, or residence the 65 teachers do a second HRI rating of six ran-
differences between the subsamples. domly selected students 4-6 weeks after the initial
For both subsamples, HRI ratings were analyzed rating. At that time six students were no longer in
as follows: Item-item intercorrelations (A' = 1,431) their classes, leaving a reliability sample of 60 first
were computed, and the resulting matrices were fac- to third graders. Tcst-rctest reliabilities (rs) were as
tored by the principal-components procedure. A .90 follows: Sum Factors = .87 ; Good Student =- .83,
factor eigenvalue cutoff was selected, yielding a five- Gutsy — .77, Peer Sociability — .72, Rules - .91, and
factor solution that was consistent with prior think- Frustration Tolerance —' .87.
ing about the nature of competence. In each analysis,
five factors accounting for 71% of the total variance Parametric Analyses
were rotated following both varimax and oblique Separate parametric analyses of sex, grade, and
procedures (oblimin criterion, A = 0 and —.5; Har- residence differences were done for all five individual
mon, 1967) yielding three solutions per sample. Com- factors and the summary index. Each of these analy-
parisons could thus be made between orthogonal ses used the full sample of 592 subjects.
and oblique solutions within, as well as across sam-
ples, for factor stability. Competence-Pathology Relations
Although the orthogonal and oblique solutions were
comparable, the relatively more orthogonal (A — Both HRI and symptom data were collected for a
—.5) oblique solution yielded the most inlerpretable sample of 183 children consisting of 98 boys, 85
structure. All subsequent analyses are based on this girls; 69 city and 114 county children; and 76 first,
solution. 61 second, and 46 third graders. All subjects were
The final oblique factor solutions for Subsamples rated by the same teachers on both scales. Chi-square
B and C yielded essentially comparable results, sug- tests among variables of sex, grade, and city/county
gesting that the derived factor structure was stable. status were all nonsignificant. Symptoms were mea-
Therefore, the total sample (A, JV = S92) was used sured using the CARS, a 41-item behaviorally ori-
in subsequent analyses to maximize sample size. ented measure developed by Clarfield (1974). The
Factor structure. Table 1 summarizes items that CARS yields three basic problem dimensions:
contributed most (i.e., > a .10 cutoff in the factor Learning (14 items); Acting-Out (10 items); and
score coefficient matrix) to the HRI's factor struc- Shy-Anxious (12 items) as well as a factor sum and
ture. a total maladjustment score, which sums all 41 items.
Factor 1, Good Student, includes items related to
effective learning (e.g., "is good in arithmetic"; is
Normal and Disturbed Child Comparisons
good in reading"; "does original work"; "is a self- Forty-two boys attending ungraded classes in day-
starter"). Factor 2, called Gutsy, includes items treatment schools at two Rochester community
reflecting adaptive assertiveness or ego strength (e.g., mental health centers comprised the clinically identi-
"is able to question rules that seem unfair or un- fied patient group for this study. These subjects
clear to him"; "defends his views under group pres- averaged 112 months in age (range, 79-131). Chil-
sure"; "expresses ideas willingly"). Peer Sociability, dren are admitted to these special cducational-thcra-
778 ELLIS L. GESTEN

TABLE 1
1IKI ITEMS LISTED BY FACTOR

Factor Factor
score score
coeffi- coeffi-
Item cient" Loading Item cient" Loading

Factor 1 : Good Student Factor 3 : Peer Sociability

1. Functions well even 36. Tries to help others .10 .37


with distractions .10 .53 39. Has many friends .16 .56
3. Applies learning to 43. Knows his or her
new situations .11 .58 strengths and weak-
5. Is interested in school nesses -.10 -.11
work .11 .55
11. Does original work .15 .64
24. Is a self-starter .11 .56 [•'actor 4 : Rul es
26. Completes his home--
work .14 .54 7. Is well-behaved in
30. Works well without school .15 .78
adult support .10 .54 18. Follows class rules .16 .80
35. Is good in arithmetic .17 .72 29. Is trustworthy .14 .73
40. Works up to potential .11 .57 32. Carries out requests
47. Is a good reader .14 .66 and directions
_ _ _- .. . _ _ _ responsibly .10 .56
Factor 2 : Gulsy 42. Accepts legitimate
imposed limits .14 .74
~ " 53. Rarely requires re-
14. Defends his views strictions or sanctions .13 .71
under group pressure .21 .80 54. Is polite and courte-
19. Participates in class ous .16 .81
discussions .18 .72
20. Is able to question
rules that seem unfair Factor 5 : Frustration Tolerance
or unclear to him .25 .86
31. Expresses ideas will- 2. Feels good about
ingly .18 .73 himself or herself .11 .41
45. Expresses needs and 9. Approaches new ex-
feelings appropriately .14 .53 periences confidently .10 .38
48. Is comfortable as a .11 .51 12. Can accept things
leader and follower not going his way .15 .52
50. Is spontaneous .11 .52 15. Mood is balanced and
14 48
Factor 3 : Peer Sociability 16. Resolves peer prob-
lems on his own .13 .44
17. Copes well with
4. Has a good sense of failure .32 .84
humor .14 .51 23. Is generally relaxed .10 .39
6. Shares things with 26. Completes his home-
others .14 .48 work --.14 -.18
10. Is a happy child .12 .49 27. Has a lively interest
22. Is affectionate toward in his environment -.10 -.09
others .22 .70 38. Faces the pressures
25. Plays enthusiastically .25 .60 of competition well .16 .52
27. Has a lively interest 44. Adjusts well to
in his environment .14 .44 changes in the class-
34. Well liked by class- room routine .11 .41
mates .10 .60 46. Accepts criticism well .19 .60
Note. HRI -- Health Resources Inventory.
A HEALTH RESOURCES INVENTORY 779

peutic placements because of severe emotional prob- had significantly higher scores than city chil-
lems that preclude attending regular public school dren. Girls had significantly higher HRI scores
classes. Most patient group children were function-
ing significantly below grade level academically. The than boys on five of the six comparisons, the
local area pattern is such that the sample included exception being Gutsy. Although there was
both city and county children and was socioeco- some tendency for older children to have
nomically heterogeneous. Classroom teachers sub- higher HRI scores, the only significant grade
mitted HRI and CARS data for the disturbed sam- difference was for Gutsy. Only 2 of 28 possi-
ple (patient group), which was matched for age
and sex with 58 children (controls) from the norma- ble interaction terms, both involving a Sex X
tive sample. Location interaction, were statistically signifi-
A second subset of matched normal and disturbed cant.
groups was selected, controlling for overall pathology
using the CARS, as well as sex and age. The clinic
group averaged 10S months in age (range, 79-119)
Competence-Pathology Relations
and included 34 subjects (28 boys and 6 girls). Be- A S X 6 correlational matrix (see Table 3)
cause of stringent matching requirements, the selected summarizes relations between (a) the five
pathology-matched normative control group (N =
24) included only 18 males and 6 females. The chi- individual HRI factors and the HRI factor
square between variables of sex and group status sum (i.e., 6 variables) and (b) the three indi-
(clinic or normative control) was nonsignificant. vidual CARS factors, CARS factor sum, and
total CARS scores (i.e., 5 variables).
Extreme Group Competence Contrasts These 30 correlations include three distinct
Ten first- to third-grade teacher pairs from six groups: (a) HRI versus CARS summary
Rochester suburban schools participated in this study. scores, (b) individual HRI versus CARS fac-
One member of each pair was asked to identify chil- tors, and (c) individual HRI and CARS fac-
dren with different competence levels from his/her
class—three each of the (a) most competent, (b) tors versus the summary scores for each scale.
least competent, and (c) middle competent. Without The negative correlations indicate the inverse
knowledge of the first teacher's judgments or infor- relation between competence and pathology.
mation about the selection criteria, the second teacher Twenty-nine of the 30 correlations ranging
independently completed HRIs for the nine selected
children. To insure reasonably equivalent familiarity
from —.22 to —.81 were significant.
with the children, teacher pairs were only used if The highest correlations, around —.80, are
both members spent a significant part of the day those between the summary scores of the two
with all children in the group from which selections measures. The 10 correlations between the
were made (Mdn group size — 21; range, 14-46). five individual HRI factors and the two CARS
Most often this happened because of a team teach-
ing arrangement. summary scores range from —.43 to —.71,
with a median of —.61.
RESULTS The 15 correlations between individual HRI
The results are presented in four major sec- and CARS factors ranged from —.14 to —.80,
tions pertaining, respectively, to (a) para- with a median of —.50. The three most highly
metric analyses, (b) competence-pathology related variable pairs are (a) Rules and Acting
relations, (c) normal-disturbed comparisons, Out (-.80), (b) Good Student and Learning
and (d) extreme group competence contrasts. ( — .79), and (c) Frustration Tolerance and
Shy Anxious ( — .70). The four lowest corre-
Parametric Analyses lations are between (a) Good Student and
A three-way unweighted-means analysis of Shy Anxious (-.32), (b) Rules and Shy
variance (Winer, 1971) was used to test for Anxious ( — . 2 6 ) , (c) Peer Sociability and
competence differences by grade level (first, Learning ( — .22), and (d) Gutsy and Acting
second, and third), sex, and residence (city or Out (-.14).
county). Six separate analyses of variance
were done for the five individual factors and Normal-Disturbed Child Comparisons
the factor sum. Table 2 summarizes the main Six t tests for independent samples, five for
effect group mean comparisons. individual HRI factors and one for the sum-
On five of the six comparisons—on all ex- mary score, compared mean scores of the nor-
cept Frustration Tolerance—county children mal and disturbed samples. In all six cases
780 ELLIS L. GESTEN

TABLE 2
HRI MAIN EFFECT MEANS AND RESULTS op ANALYSES OF VARIANCE

HRI score (location) City (N = 300) County (N = 292)

Good Student 2.47 2.76 12.65***


Gutsy 3.08 3.40 18.26***
Peer Sociability 4.46 4.71 7.92**
Rules 3.23 3.58 17.35***
Frustration Tolerance 2.76 2.80 .16
Sum Factors 16.01 17.26 16.62***

HRT score (sex) Boys (N = 311) Girls (N = 281)

Good Student 2.48 2.77 11.43***


Gutsy 3.19 3.29 1.04
Peer Sociability 4.47 4.71 8.24**
Rules 3.19 3.64 33.99***
Frustration Tolerance 2.66 2.93 10.23**
Sum Factors 15.99 17.34 19.12***

HRI score (grade level) 1 (N = 237) 2 (N = 197) 3 (N = 158)

Good Student 2.58 2.61 2.74 .34


Gutsy 3.13 3.29 3.34 3.00*
Peer Sociability 4.58 4.59 4.59 .09
Rules 3.38 3.38 3.47 .41
Frustration Tolerance 2.75 2.72 2.92 1.88
Sum Factors 16.42 16.60 16.99 1.18
Note-. IIRt = Health Resources Inventory.
* l> < .05.
** f < .01.
*** f < .0(11.

the normative sample had significantly higher three criterion groups on the HRI factor and
HRI scores. summary scores. All six Fs were highly sig-
An identical set of comparisons was made nificant; in each case the high-competent
between the mean HRT scores of the pathol- (Sample A) teacher-rated groups had the
ogy-matched clinic and normative samples. highest mean HRI scores followed, respec-
None of these differences were significant. tively, by the middle (Sample B) and least
competent (Sample C) groups. Comparison of
Extreme Group Competence Contrasts subgroup means using Duncan's multiple-range
Six separate one-way analyses of variance test (Winer, 1971) indicates that in all six
tested mean competence differences among cases the differences between most and least

TABLE 3
CORRELATIONS BETWEEN HRI AND CARS

HRT Acting Out Shy Anxious Learning Factor Sum Raw Total

Good Student -.38** -.32** -.79** -.65** -.65**


Gutsy -.14 -.50** -.51** -.48** -.52**
Peer Sociability -.31** -.55** -.22* -.43** -.45**
Rules -.80** -.26** -.46** -.61** -.57**
Frustration Tolerance -.56** -.70** -.51** -.71** -.72**
Sum [''actors -.62** -.65** -.69** -.80** -.81**

Nate, HRI — Health Resources Inventory; CARS = Classroom Activity Rating Scale; N ~ 18.1
* f < .01.
** f < .001.
A HEALTH RESOURCES INVENTORY 781

TABLE 4 scale variance. Each component subscale is


HEALTH RESOURCES TN NORMAL AND internally consistent and represents an im-
DISTURBED GROUPS portant aspect of a broadly defined concept of
competence in young school children. Test-
C (2V =58) E (2V =42)
retest reliability was satisfactorily high.
Factor M SD M SD / W = 98) Parametric results for the most part paral-
Good Student 2.28 .95 1.86 .80 2.29* lel prior findings with symptom scales used
Gutsy 3.14 .97 2.49 .86 3.52***
with normal and disturbed children. For ex-
Peer
Sociability 4.32 .97 3.47 .99 4.24*** ample, just as boys are consistently judged to
Rules 2.97 1.06 2.36 .76 3.18**
Frustration
Tolerance 2.52 1.05 1.03 .82 7.55***
be more maladjusted than girls on symptom
Factor Sum 15.24 3.60 11.21 2.35 6.28*** measures (Brownbridge & VanVleet, 1969),
Note. C = normative group; E — disturbed group.
girls in this study were rated significantly
* /> < .02.
**p < .002.
more competent than boys except on the
*** p < .001. Gutsy (adaptive assertiveness) factor. Given
the significantly higher scores for girls on all
competent (A:C) and those between middle other competence factors, the absence of sex
and least competent (B:C) were statistically differences on Gutsy identifies these behaviors
significant. Three of the differences between as an area of relative strength for boys. This
most and middle competent groups (A:B) is consistent with cultural stereotypes by
were statistically significant (i.e., Good Stu- which boys are seen (and socialized) as ac-
dent, Frustration Tolerance, and Sum Fac- ceptably more assertive and aggressive than
tors; see Table 4). girls.
Just as past studies with symptom scales
DISCUSSION reported that urban children have more prob-
This discussion consists of four major parts lems than suburbanites (Clarfield, 1972;
dealing, respectively, with (a) HRI scale Glidewell & Swallow, 1969), city children in
development and parametric studies; (b) re- this study were judged to have fewer compe-
lations between competencies and problem tencies than county children. These findings
behaviors; (c) validity studies; and (d) limi- are consistent with earlier reports of social
tations and suggestions for future work. class differences in school adjustment (Glide-
well & Swallow, 1969). The prior demonstra-
Scale Development and Parametric Studies
tion of more problem behaviors in city as
Initial tests of the HRI's psychometric compared to county children plus the present
properties revealed a stable five-factor struc- finding that city children also have fewer
ture accounting for roughly 70% of the total interpersonal, academic, and self-esteem-

TABLE5
EXTREME GROUP COMPETENCE COMPARISONS

Group Duncan's
multiple-range
High (A) Middle (B) Low (C) test (p)

HRI factor M SD M SD M SD /<'« A:C B:C A:B

Good Student 3.47 .74 3.06 .83 1.40 .73 60.34 .01 .01 .05
Gutsy 3.76 .67 3.40 .69 2.49 .86 23.32 .01 .01 ns
Peer Sociability 5.09 .74 4.72 .73 4.05 .84 13.92 .01 .01 ns
Rules 3.83 .67 3.41 .82 2.66 .98 15.30 .01 .01 ns
Frustration Tolerance 3.33 .90 2.80 .91 1.57 .98 28.29 .01 .01 .05
Factor Sum 19.50 3.07 17.41 2.95 12.17 2.63 51.12 .01 .01 .01
Nole. N — 30 in each group; HRI = Health Resources Inventory,
»All /'" values are significant at the ,001 level.
782 KLUS L. GESTEN

related competencies, highlights the challenges perhaps understandably so, were between (a)
facing urban school systems. HRI Rules and CARS Acting Out (-.80),
Although the current data indicate that (b) HRI Good Student and CARS Learning
there are competence differences between ur- (-.70), and (c) HRI Frustration Tolerance
ban and suburban children, they do not indi- and CARS Shy Anxious ( — .70). It is reason-
cate why this is so. Clarifying this complex able that these correlations were high, since
question can contribute to better school en- in each case the construct definitions of the
vironments and interventions. However, these paired variables are similar. Illustratively,
same findings can also be interpreted to re- Rules measures a child's ability to function
flect the inappropriateness of using identical well within the framework of the school en-
competence indicators for socioeconomically vironment, and Acting Out reflects the inabil-
diverse groups. Although some care was taken ity to do so. The two factors are unipolar,
to avoid such bias in developing the measure, and they reflect opposite ends of a logical
nonetheless, certain items (e.g., "is polite continuum.
and courteous") do, in part, reflect middle- Other individual factor, cross-measure cor-
class values. relations, however, did not follow the pattern
There were few competence differences of high-inverse health-pathology relations.
across age-grade groups. This finding is some- Indeed, the overall pattern of HR1-CARS
what inconsistent with earlier data suggesting factor scale correlations suggests that at the
that school adjustment problems increase with subscale level the measures account for a rea-
age (Cowen et al., 1973; Glidewell & Swallow, sonable amount of independent variance.
1969). But several theories about skill and The three most independent factor pairs
competence acquisition can accommodate the were (a) HRI Rules and CARS Shy Anxious,
present findings (Burner, 1970; White, 1959). (b) HRI Peer Sociability and CARS Learn-
According to these views competence is ac- ing, and (c) HRT Gutsy and CARS Acting
quired slowly as part of a broader develop- Out. A common element that binds these in-
mental process that involves small day-to-day stances is the absence in each case of a logical
increments throughout infancy and early or conceptual basis for a cross-scale relation.
childhood. Additionally, the growth of compe- Rules identifies a child who lives in accord-
tence is in part dependent on the development, ance with the established regularities of the
of functional systems (e.g., intellectual, per- environment, and Shy Anxious, the child who
ceptual, etc.) whose maturational markers do has problems of shyness and withdrawal. Al-
not correspond to primary age-group delinea- though the latter can be real and disconcert-
tions (see Table 5). ing, they do not typically involve challenges
to class order.
Competence-Pathology Relations The correlation of .14 between HRI Gutsy
The correlations between HR1 and CARS and CARS Acting Out, the only nonsignificant
dimensions support some prior assumptions cross-scale interfactor correlation, is among
about relations between health and pathology the study's more interesting findings. Gutsy
but challenge others. The highly significant is adaptive assertiveness; CARS Acting Out
overall interscale correlations (e.g., CARS taps aggressive acting-out behaviors. Although
sum factor vs. HRI sum factor = —.80; prior data (Beilin, 1959; Glidewell & Swal-
CARS total score vs. HRI sum factor -- low, 1969; Schrupp & Gjerde, 1953; Sparks,
— .81) support the traditional view that health 1952) indicating strong teacher concern about
and pathology are inversely related. Thus, a children's aggressive acting-out behavior
child's overall competence can be predicted might, on the surface, suggest a relationship
reasonably well from his/her total CARS between Acting Out and Gutsy, this was not
(symptom) score. found. Teachers discriminated well between
A strong inverse relation was also found on problem-related and assertively adaptive ag-
several cross-measure, individual factor corre- gressiveness. The low Gutsy-Acting Out
lations. The three highest such correlations, correlation identifies an important area of
A HEALTH RESOURCES INVENTORY 783

independence between the health and symp- crux of the conceptual argument on which the
tom scales used and helps to dispel the view present competence model rests is that compe-
that the presence of pathology necessarily tence is best seen as a sum, perhaps weighted
implies the absence of competencies. (Jones, 1972), of many "competencies" in dif-
ferent domains. Although the size of the first
Empirical Validity unrotated factor decreased significantly in the
rotated terminal solution along with increased
Recognizing that there is no single defini- weighting of other competence components,
tive validity criterion for a test, a step-by-step the initial first-factor dominance has implica-
incremental approach for establishing the tions for the view of competence being ad-
HRI's validity was adopted. Accordingly, two vanced. Several alternative explanations can
empirical studies confirmed the scale's ability be considered to explain the finding, (a) Sta-
to discriminate between clinically disturbed tistical integrity and meaningfulness of the
and normal children and to distinguish levels adopted solution notwithstanding, competence
of competence within the normative sample. may still be largely unidimensional. (b)
This second finding is especially significant in Teachers' ratings of children's competence
light of attenuated competence scores found may be influenced by such factors as halo
among suburban children. Even though the effects or global "good-bad" sets, which—
"least competent" normative group included perhaps strengthened by the almost exclusive
a high percentage of relatively healthy chil- use of positively worded scale items—once
dren, and few with severe competence deficits, formed, bias individual item ratings, (c) Com-
the measure discriminated not only between petence, like intelligence, may have a general
the most and least competent teacher-identi- factor. But, as with intelligence, there may
fied children but also between both extreme still be meaningful competence subcomponents
groups and midcompetent children. that, though related, have considerable inde-
That the HRI did not discriminate between pendence and diagnostic utility. In light of
the disturbed sample and a select (bottom) both correlational and validity data, this third
extreme symptom group from the normative explanation seems most plausible, especially
sample is not disconcerting for two reasons: when combined with the second, nonmutually
(a) The high negative HR1-CARS overall exclusive, possibility.
correlation favors such a finding, and (b) the Two other methodological issues relate to
requirement that the groups be symptom the use of teachers as raters. Like most other
(CARS) matched markedly restricted the raters, teachers are not neutral. They are in-
normative pool, both in size, and to those with vested in the system whose products they are
extreme symptom scores. Many children in being asked to judge, and their training and
this small, biased normative sample may have orientation to educational goals sensitize them
needed, or been in, treatment. more to some behaviors than to others. As a
counterweight for such bias, it is important
Overview that competence be studied from multiple
within-class perspectives, including judgments
Several limitations of this research qualify of both peers and independent observers.
the findings and point to directions for future Such an approach has recently been shown to
work. Two categories of concern that merit be useful in assessing class environments
specific mention are (a) methodological limi- (Kaye, Trickett, & Quinlan, in press). A sec-
tations of the studies themselves and (b) limi- ond limitation is that because only a few
tations of generalization. classes had a second observer who could judge
One important methodological concern child competencies, a single teacher had to
stems from the presence of a dominant first complete both the pathology and health scales
factor in the unrotated factor solution, similar for target children in the correlational study.
to Osgood's evaluative factor (Osgood, Suci, & Although teachers were told to do the two
Tannenbaum, 1957). In some contrast, the forms independently, this may not have been
784 ELLIS L. GESTEN

completely possible. Hence, health and pa- ing arena, establish independence, and choose
thology correlations may have been spuriously a career. Thus, there is a need to develop em-
inflated. pirically validated cross-age competence indi-
The generalizability of the present findings cators and/or new competence items and fac-
must also be qualified. The HRI was devel- tors for age groups other than those of the
oped as a competence measure for young present subjects. Since new competencies are
school children. Since there is little reason to acquired most rapidly in the early years, com-
assume that competence indicators are neces- petence indicators are likely to change most
sarily the same either across situations or age rapidly in early childhood.
levels, the HRI should not be seen as a gen- Related to the issue of developmental dif-
eralized competence measure. Further work ferences in competence indicators is the ques-
to provide richer, less situationally bound, tion of the meaning and stability of judged
cross-sectional understandings of competence competence. Just how meaningful is it that a
requires that competencies be sampled in 7-year-old school child is seen as competent
other than school settings. If, for example, by his/her teacher? To what other behaviors
the home or neighborhood has different values does this relate? And what does it say for the
or places different environmental demands on future? Only through the development of age-
the child, competence-defining marker items appropriate and situation-appropriate compe-
would change substantially. Moreover, such tence measures can such questions be studied.
shifts might differ for different subareas. Il- Findings from the Berkeley Guidance Study
lustratively, if a home-based competence mea- (Livson & Peskin, 1967; Macfarland, 1938;
sure were to be developed for the age group Peskin, 1972) bear structurally on the ques-
used in this research, the Good Student and tion of time stability, notwithstanding differ-
Rules factors might not generalize well be- ences in the competence indices used in that
cause they are closely tied to the classroom. study and the present one. The Berkeley
By contrast, Peer Sociability, Gutsy, and study examined the differential power of four
Frustration Tolerance, being more generic age periods, from S to 16 years, in predicting
competence indicators, might be less situa- psychological health at maturity (Livson &
lionally bound and hence less affected. Analy- Peskin, 1967; Peskin, 1972). Long-term fol-
sis of convergence and divergence of compe- low-up of a group of 31 normal males and
tence data from multiple observational frame- females revealed complex discontinuities em-
works can illuminate the nature of effective bedded in an overall pattern of continuity in
functioning. health status. Following such a strategy, long-
It is also important to question whether term longitudinal data and valid later compe-
competence indicators for primary graders' tence measures would be needed to test the
classroom functioning apply to other age levels HRI's predictive power.
(e.g., upper primary, junior high school, etc.). Twidence from the present study suggests
Sullivan (1953), Erikson (1963), Jones that the HRI is a potentially useful measure
(1972), and Havighurst (19S3) have all ar- of primary-grade children's competence. Cer-
gued persuasively that competence skills differ tainly there is a need to learn more about
across age levels or stages of development. competencies and adaptive skills, in their own
Erikson, for example, argues that mastery of right, and as a corrective to past emphases on
the school environment is one of the major dysfunction and pathology. Although the HRI
competence challenges for the 6- to 12-year- offers a promising start in this direction, much
old. He speaks of the crucial development of remains to be learned about the acquisition,
peer interaction and academic and self-con- correlates, and consequences of competency
cept skills during these childhood years. On behaviors.
the other hand, what defines competence for REFERENCE NOTE
a 7-year-old is different than the defining
1. Kelly, J. G. Toward a psychology oj healthiness.
characteristics for an 18-year-old who must Ichabod Spencer lecture, Union College, Schenec-
separate from the family, make it in the dat- Uicly, New York, May 2, 1974.
A HEALTH RESOURCES INVENTORY 785
REFERENCES ence frontiers in education. New York: Wiley,
1967.
Albcc, G. W. The relation of conceptual models to
Jahoda, M. Current concepts of positive mental
manpower needs. In E. L. Cowen, E. A. Gardner,
health. (Joint Commission on Mental Illness and
& M. Zax (Eds.), Emergent, approaches to menial
Health Monograph Series No. 1). New York:
health problems. New York: Appleton-Ccntury-
Basic Books, 1958.
Crofts, 1967.
Jones, F. H. A four-year follow-up of vulnerable
Beilin, H. Teachers' and clinicians' attitudes toward adolescents: The continuity and interrelationship
the behavior problems of children: A reappraisal. of measures of social competence and psycho-
Child Development, 1959, 30, 9-25. pathology from adolescence to early adulthood.
Bloom, D. B. The "medical model," miasma theory Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
and community mental health. Community Men- California, Los Angeles, 1972.
tal Health Journal, 1965, 1, 333-338. Kaye, S., Trickett, E. J., & Quinlan, D. M. Alterna-
Brownbridge, R., & Van Vleet, P. (Eds.). Invest- tive methods for environmental assessment: An
ments in prevention: The prevention of learning example. American Journal of Community Psy-
and behavior problems in young children. San chology, in press.
Francisco, Calif.: Pace I. D. Center, 1969. Kohn, M., & Rosman, B. A two-factor model of
Bruner, J. S. Discussion: Infant education as viewed emotional disturbance in the young child: Valid-
by a psychologist. In V. H. Dencnberg (Ed.), ity and screening efficiency. Journal of Child Psy-
Education of the infant and young child. New chology and Psychiatry, 1973, 14, 31-56.
York: Academic Press, 1970. Livson, N., & Pcskin, H. Prediction of adult psycho-
Clarfield, S. P. An analysis of referral problems and logical health in a longitudinal study. Journal of
their relation to intervention goals in a school- Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 509-518.
based preventive health program. Unpublished doc- Macfarland, J. W. Studies in child guidance: I.
toral dissertation, University of Rochester, 1972. Methodology of data collection and organization.
Clarfield, S. P. The development of a teacher re- Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
ferral form for identifying early school maladap- Development, 1938, 3(6, Whole No. 19).
tation. American Journal of Community Psychol- McClelland, D. C. Testing for competence rather
ogy, 1974, 2, 199-210. than for "intelligence." American Psychologist,
Cowcn, E. L. Social and community interventions. 1973, 28, 1-14.
Annual Review of Psychology, 1973, 24, 423-472. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tanncnbaum, P. H.
Cowen, E. L., Gardner, E. A., & Zax, M. (Eds.). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University
Emergent approaches to menial health problems. of Illinois Press, 1957.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967. Peskin, H. Multiple prediction of adult psychological
Cowen, E. L., et al. The AML: A quick-screening health from preadolescent and adolescent behavior.
device for early detection of school maladaptation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
American Journal of Community Psychology, 1973, 1972, 3S, 155-160.
1, 12-35. Rolf, J. E. The social and academic competence of
Erikson, E. H. Childhood and society. New York: children vulnerable to schizophrenia and other
Norton, 1963. behavior pathologies. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Finkel, N. J. Strcns and traumas: An attempt at chology, 1972, 80, 225-243.
categorization. American Journal of Community Ross, A. 0., Laccy, H. M., & Parton, D. A. The
Psychology, 1974, 2, 265-273. development of a behavior checklist for boys.
Finkel, N. J. Strens, traumas, and trauma resolution. Child Development, 1965, 36, 1013-1027.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 1975, Rubenstein, G., & Fisher, L. The development of a
3, 173-178. measure of teachers' observations of student be-
Garmezy, N. Competence and adaptation in adult havior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
schizophrenic patients and children at risk. In S. chology, 1974, 42, 310.
R. Dean (Ed.), Schizophrenia: The first ten Dean Sarason, I. G., & Ganzer, V. J. Concerning the medi-
award lectures. New York: MSS Information, cal model. American Psychologist, 1968, 23, 507-
1973. 510.
Glidewell, J. C., & Swallow, C. S. The prevalence Schrupp, M. H., & Gjcrde, C. M. Teacher growth in
of maladjustment in elementary schools: A report attitudes toward behavior problems in children.
prepared for the Joint Commission on the Mental Journal of Educational Psychology, 1953, 44, 203-
Health of Children, Chicago: University of Chi- 213.
cago Press, 1969. Seeman, J. Teacher judgments of high and low ad-
Harmon, H. H. Modern factor analysis, Chicago: justment. Journal of Educational Research, 1963,
University of Chicago Press, 1967. 57, 213-216.
Havighurst, R. J. Human development and educa- Shoben, E. J. Toward a concept of the normal per-
tion. New York: Longnans, Green, 1953. sonality. American Psychologist, 1957, 12, 183-189.
Hollister, W. G. The concept of strcns in education: Smith, M. B. Competence and "mental health":
A challenge to curriculum development. In E. M. Problems in conceptualizing human effectiveness.
Bower & W. G. Hollister (Eds.), behavioral sci- In S. B. Sells (Ed.), The definition and measure-
786 ELLIS L. GESTEN

menl of mental health (PI-IS Publication No. White, R. W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept
1873). U.S. Department of Health, Education, and of competence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66,
Welfare, Public Health Service, 1968. 297-333.
Sparks, J. N. Teacher attitudes toward the behavior White, R. W. The concept of healthy personality:
problems of children. Journal of Educational Psy- What do we really mean? The Counseling Psy-
chology, 1952, 43, 284-291. chologist, 1973, 4, 3-12.
Sullivan, H. S. The interpersonal theory of psychi- Winer, B. J. Statistical principles in experimental
atry. New York: Norton, 19S3. design (2nd ed.) New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971.
Wallace, J. An abilities conception of personality: Zax, M., & Cowcn, E. L. Abnormal psychology:
Some implications for personality measurement. Changing conceptions. New York: Holt, Rinehart
American Psychologist, 1966, 21, 132-138. & Winston, 1972.
White, B. L. Pre-school project: Child rearing prac- Zax, M., & Specter, G. A. An introduction to com-
tices and the development of competence: A final munity psychology. New York: Wiley, 1974.
report, Washington, D.C.: Office of Economic Op-
portunity, Head Start Division, 1972. (Received December 29, 197$)

You might also like