Jurnal Fi

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Article

Deactivation of the Parahippocampal Gyrus Preceding


Auditory Hallucinations in Schizophrenia

Kelly M.J. Diederen, M.Sc. Objective: Activation in a network of Results: Groupwise analysis during audi-
language-related regions has been re- tory verbal hallucinations revealed brain
ported during auditory verbal hallucina- activation in bilateral (right more than
Sebastiaan F.W. Neggers, Ph.D.
tions. It remains unclear, however, how left) language-related regions and bilat-
this activation is triggered. Identifying eral motor regions. Prominent deactiva-
Kirstin Daalman, M.Sc. brain regions that show significant signal tion preceding these hallucinations was
changes preceding auditory hallucina- observed in the left parahippocampal
Jan Dirk Blom, M.D., Ph.D. tions might reveal the origin of these hal- gyrus. In addition, significant deactivation
lucinations. preceding hallucinations was found in the
Rutger Goekoop, M.D., Ph.D. Method: Twenty-four patients with a left superior temporal, right inferior fron-
tal, and left middle frontal gyri as well as
psychotic disorder indicated the pres-
René S. Kahn, M.D., Ph.D. ence of auditory verbal hallucinations in the right insula and left cerebellum. No
significant signal changes were revealed
during 3-Tesla functional magnetic reso-
prior to the matched balloon squeezing
Iris E.C. Sommer, M.D., Ph.D. nance imaging by squeezing a handheld
among the comparison subjects.
balloon. A one-sample t test was per-
formed to reveal groupwise activation Conclusions: Auditory verbal hallucina-
during hallucinations. To enable analysis tions in patients with a psychotic disorder
of brain activation 6 to 0 seconds pre- are consistently preceded by deactivation
ceding hallucinations, a tailored “selec- of the parahippocampal gyrus. The para-
tive averaging” method, without any a hippocampus has been hypothesized
priori assumptions concerning the he- to play a central role in memory recol-
modynamic response profile, was per- lection, sending information from the
formed. To control for motor-related ac- hippocampus to the association areas.
tivation, 1 5 healthy comparison subjects D ysfunction of this region could trigger
squeezed a balloon at matched time in- inadequate activation of right language
tervals. areas during auditory hallucinations.
(Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:427–435)

A uditory verbal hallucinations, or “hearing voices,”


constitute a cardinal feature of psychosis. The patho-
voices, it cannot explain how and where these experienc-
es originate in the brain. Because auditory hallucinations
physiology of auditory hallucinations remains unknown. arise without an external source (i.e., the experience of an
Previous research has shown that these hallucinations in actual voice), they must be triggered internally. Studying
schizophrenia are responsive to antipsychotic medica- brain activation in the time period preceding the halluci-
tion in approximately 70% of patients (1) and that the an- nations might reveal this trigger. Therefore, we not only
tipsychotic effect of these agents is most likely mediated investigated brain activation during auditory hallucina-
by antagonism at the dopamine type 2 (D2) receptors (2). tions but also identified brain activation prior to these
Therefore, a dopaminergic component is presumed to be hallucinations.
involved in the origin of auditory hallucinations.
A second line of evidence is derived from neuroimaging Method
studies demonstrating consistent activation of bilateral
language-related areas during auditory hallucinations (3– Participants
9), with the most prominent activation in the right homo- Forty-two patients with a psychotic disorder were recruited from
logue of Broca’s area (9). However, since these language- the Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht,
related areas barely have any D2 innervations (10, 11), this the Netherlands, and the Parnassia Bavo Group, the Hague, the
Netherlands. Patients were selected for further participation if they
cannot easily be linked to the suspected dopaminergic
met the following inclusion criteria: 1) auditory hallucinations with
component. Moreover, although functional imaging of a perceptual quality; 2) hallucination content that did not parallel
activity during hallucinations is helpful in understanding thoughts (consisting of commands or derogatory critique); 3) right-
which regions are involved in the experience of hearing handedness; 4) both hallucination and nonhallucination periods

This article is discussed in an editorial by Sohee Park and Katharine Thakkar (p. 366).

Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 427


DEACTIVATION OF PARAHIPPOCAMPAL GYRUS

TABLE 1. Characteristics of Patients With Psychotic Disorder and Healthy Comparison Subjects
Patients With Psychotic Disorder Receiv­ Patients With Psychotic Disorder Receiv­
ing Analysis of Brain Activation During ing Analysis of Brain Activation Prior to Healthy Comparison
Characteristic Auditory Verbal Hallucinations (N=24)a Auditory Verbal Hallucinations (N=15)b Subjects (N=15)b
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Age (years) 39 10 39 10 26 4
Age (years) at onset of auditory 23 15 22 16
verbal hallucinations
N % N % N %
Gender
Male 15 62.5 9 60 9 60
Female 9 37.5 6 40 6 40

Diagnosis
Schizophrenia 16 66.7 10 66.7
Schizoaffective disorder 2 8.3 2 13.3
Schizophreniform disorder 1 4.2 1 6.7
Psychosis not otherwise specified 5 20.9 2 13.3
No psychiatric diagnosis 15 100
Antipsychotic medication
Clozapine (mean dose: 375 mg) 7 29.2 3 20
Flupentixol (mean dose: 52.5 mg 4 16.7 4 26.7
per 3 weeks)
Risperidone (65 mg per 2 weeks) 2 8.3 2 13.3
Olanzapine (30 mg) 1 4.2 1 6.7
Pimozide (2 mg) 1 4.2 1 6.7
Penfluridol (30 mg per week) 1 4.2 1 6.7
Risperidone (3 mg) plus quetia­ 1 4.2 1 6.7
pine (100 mg)
Olanzapine (20 mg) plus fluanxol 1 4.2 1 6.7
(4 mg)
Zuclopenthixol (10 mg) 1 4.2
Haloperidol (40 mg) 1 4.2
Aripiprazole (unknown dose) 1 4.2
Fluanxol (6 mg) 1 4.2
Chlorprothixene (45–150 mg) 1 4.2
Medication-free 1 4.2 1 6.7
a 
Data included in the first analysis (analysis 1).
b 
Data included in the second analysis (analysis 2).

present during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); 5) view (12) according to DSM-IV criteria. On the day of the fMRI
a complete fMRI scan; and 6) hallucinations indicated correctly scan, the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS [13]) was
(i.e., onset of hallucinations followed by clear offset of hallucina- used for the assessment of symptoms. Detailed characteristics of
tions). To qualify for the analysis of brain activation preceding hal- hallucinations were assessed using the auditory hallucinations
lucinations, the following eighth inclusion criterion was added: a subscale of the Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales (14). As a com-
minimum of 6 seconds had to be present between successive hal- parison group, 15 healthy subjects were recruited. A demographi-
lucinations. A 6-second interval was chosen because fMRI blood- cal description of the patients and comparison subjects is sum-
oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response lasts approximately 4 to marized in Table 1. The study was approved by the Human Ethics
6 seconds (after neuronal activation) before reaching its peak. Also, Committee of the University Medical Center, Utrecht, the Neth-
since the time frame in which hallucinations are prepared is un- erlands. After complete description of the study to the subjects,
known, an extended time window was preferred. Several patients written informed consent was obtained.
were found unsuitable for inclusion because three did not fulfill
criterion 2, two did not meet criterion 4, four did not meet criterion Experimental Design and Data Acquisition
5, seven did not fulfill criterion 6, and two did not fulfill criterion Patients signaled the presence of auditory verbal hallucina-
7. An additional nine patients were excluded from the analysis of tions by squeezing a handheld balloon while functional scans
brain activation preceding hallucinations because they did not were obtained continuously (12). To correct for brain activation
meet criterion 8. This resulted in the inclusion of 24 patients in the related to balloon squeezes, the 15 healthy comparison subjects
analysis of brain activation during hallucinations (analysis 1) and were instructed to squeeze the balloon approximately 10 times at
15 patients in the analysis of brain activation prior to hallucina- random time intervals. This number was approximately matched
tions (analysis 2). Eighteen of the 24 patients in the first analysis to the mean number of hallucinations during the scanning peri-
and 11 of the 15 patients in the second analysis also participated in ods in the first analysis.
a previous study conducted by our research group (9). Activation maps were obtained using a Philips Achieva 3 Tesla
Patients were diagnosed by an independent psychiatrist using clinical MRI scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, the Neth-
the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History inter- erlands). Eight-hundred BOLD fMRI scans were acquired with

428 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010


DIEDEREN, NEGGERS, DAALMAN, ET AL.

TABLE 2. Characteristics of Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Patients With Psychotic Disorder


Psychotic Disorder Patient Group
Brain Activation Analyzed During Brain Activation Analyzed During Period
Hallucinations (N=24) Prior to Hallucinations (N=15)
Item on Psychotic Symptom Rating Subscale
Scales Ratings Mean SD Mean SD
Frequency 1–6 5 1 5 1
Duration 1–4 2 1 2 1
Location 1–4 2 1 2 1
Loudness 1–4 2 1 2 1
Beliefs about source of hallucinations 1–4 3 1 2 1
Negative content 1–5 3 1 3 1
Severity of negativity 1–5 3 1 3 1
Distress 1–5 3 1 3 1
Intensity of distress 1–5 2 1 2 1
Control over hallucinations 1–5 3 1 3 1
Number of voices 24 37 28 41

the following parameter settings: slices (coronal)=40, repetition neuronal activation (17). However, a prerequisite of using the
time=21.75 msec, echo time=32.4 msec, flip angle=10°, field of hemodynamic response function for the purpose of modeling
view=224×256×160, matrix=64×64×40, voxel size=4 mm isotro- fMRI data is a reasonably clear hypothesis about the onset
pic. This scan sequence achieves full brain coverage within 609 time of the event of interest. Since it is unknown when regions
msec by combining a three-dimensional PRESTO (principle of implicated in the cascade of events that lead to the conscious
echo shifting with a train of observations) pulse sequence with experience of hallucinations will approximately activate, the
parallel imaging (SENSE [sensitivity encoding]) in two directions, hemodynamic response function does not represent a suitable
using a commercial eight-channel SENSE head coil (15). After the basis function. Therefore, a set of finite impulse response
functional scans were performed, a high-resolution anatomical functions was selected to detect brain activation preceding
scan was conducted with the following parameters: repetition hallucinations. Finite impulse response function is the most
time=9.86 msec, echo time=4.6 msec, voxels=0.875×0.875×1, flip general of the basis functions used to model fMRI responses and
angle=8°. These parameters were acquired to improve localiza- makes no assumptions about the shape and temporal resolution
tion of the functional data (15). of the hemodynamic response. Using finite impulse response
functions, a number of successive time bins (“miniboxcars”)
Data Analysis were created, typically following the onset of an event (18),
Preprocessing. Functional MRI data were analyzed using with each independently modeling a single time point before
Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2) (Wellcome Department the hallucination. To enable analysis of brain activation up to 6
of Cognitive Neurology, London). Preprocessing included seconds prior to the hallucination, 10 finite-impulse-response-
realignment of fMRI time series to correct for head motion, function time bins were created, each coding for a period of
coregistration of fMRI data with the T1-weighted anatomical 0.609 second, corresponding to the interscan interval. If the
image, and spatial normalization to a standard Montreal first hallucination occurred sooner than 6 seconds after the
Neurological Institute template based on the T1-weighted scan start of the paradigm, no finite-impulse-response-function
with high anatomical contrast. Finally, images were smoothed time bins were modeled prior to the first hallucination. In the
using an 8-mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian kernel. context of a general linear model consisting of finite-impulse-
response basis functions fitted to fMRI time series on a voxel-
Statistical Analysis of fMRI Responses by-voxel basis, we effectively corrected for possible overlap of
subsequent BOLD responses (18). Therefore, this method is
Analysis 1: Brain activation during auditory verbal sometimes referred to as “selective averaging.” Seven additional
hallucinations. To compare activation during hallucination postonset hallucination/balloon squeeze finite-impulse-
periods with activation during nonhallucination periods, a model response-function time bins, designed to capture the prolonged
was created using balloon squeezes as the onset of a hallucination BOLD responses of neuronal events preceding hallucinations,
and the time between squeezes and releases as the duration of the were entered as covariates in this analysis. In addition, since the
hallucination. This model was convolved with the standardized hallucination/balloon squeeze itself was expected to explain a
hemodynamic response function from SPM2 to introduce typical considerable amount of variance in the data (9), it was entered
delays of fMRI responses and fitted to the data using general as a covariate, similar to the first analysis. To introduce typical
linear model estimation (16). A one-sample t test was performed delays of fMRI responses, this covariate was convolved with the
to enable groupwise analysis with a threshold of p<0.05, whole- hemodynamic response function from SPM2.
brain corrected by the false discovery rate combined with an To enable groupwise analysis, data were examined through
extent threshold of five voxels. repeated-measures univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA), with
Analysis 2: Brain activation preceding auditory verbal the regression coefficients of the finite-impulse-response-func-
hallucinations and balloon squeezes. To detect brain tion time bins as repeated measures. A nonsphericity correction
activation preceding hallucinations and random balloon was included in this ANOVA. Subsequently, F tests were used to
squeezes, the finite impulse response was chosen as the basis calculate statistical maps, revealing voxels that showed signifi-
function. Most commonly, the canonical hemodynamic cant signal changes at some point in the interval from 6 to 0 sec-
response function is used as a model to detect fMRI signal onds prior to the hallucination.
changes of interest. The hemodynamic response function The threshold was set at p<0.05, whole-brain corrected by the
reflects the average shape of the slow increase and subsequent false discovery rate combined with an extent threshold of five
decrease in BOLD signal after a very brief moment of voxels.

Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 429


DEACTIVATION OF PARAHIPPOCAMPAL GYRUS

FIGURE 1. Areas of Signifi cant Activation During Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Patients With Psychotic Disordera

3 5 3 5 3 5

SPM2 t statistics for the groupwise analysis depict significant activity in the precentral and postcentral gyri (left), insula (center), and right
a 

inferior frontal and right middle temporal gyri (right).

Since F tests are two-tailed, revealing both activation and deac- (SD=18). Their average scores on the positive, negative,
tivation, inspection of time courses was necessary to determine and general psychopathology subscales were 20 (SD=5),
the direction of the signal change. For this purpose, mean post-
stimulus time histograms displaying the average BOLD signal 19 (SD=6), and 35 (SD=10), respectively. Details about the
were constructed from the finite-impulse-response-function re- hallucinations as rated using the Psychotic Symptom Rat-
gression coefficients. Poststimulus time histograms were calcu- ing Scales interview are listed in Table 2, in which a rating
lated from 6 to 0 seconds preceding hallucinations (averaged over of “1” signifies absent hallucinations or a very mild form of
all subjects). hallucinations during the last 3 months.
Furthermore, since finite impulse response function makes
no assumptions about the shape and temporal resolution of the Hallucinations During fMRI Scans
hemodynamic response, it is important to verify whether effects
are indeed of a hemodynamic nature rather than being caused The average number of hallucinations during the fMRI
by movement artifacts, for instance, during the preparation of a scans was 14 (SD=0.85) for patients in the first analysis.
balloon squeeze. For this reason, the realignment parameters de- The average duration of a hallucination was 17 seconds
scribing head motion using three rotation and three translation (SD=0.28), adding up to a mean total duration of halluci-
parameters, derived from realignment during the preprocessing nations of 151 seconds (SD=121).
in SPM2, were acquired per subject for the 10 scans preceding the
hallucination and tested using repeated-measures multivariate For the second analysis, the average number of hallu-
analyses of variance (MANOVAs). In addition, if movement led to cinations, average duration of a hallucination, and mean
any significant signal changes, effects would most likely be present total duration of hallucinations were 11 (SD=5), 16 sec-
throughout the scanned volume, including gray and white matter onds (SD=15), and 129 seconds (SD=116), respectively.
and out-of-brain voxels. To screen for such artifacts, average sig- The average time between successive hallucinations was
nal changes in gray and white matter and out-of-brain voxels were
calculated and tested for the 10 scans preceding the hallucinations 38 seconds (SD=29).
using repeated-measures ANOVAs. The analysis for the signal from In the comparison group, the average number of bal-
out-of-brain voxels was identical to the other analyses except for loon squeezes was 12 (SD=4). The average duration of a
the correction for temporal autocorrelation, which was not em- balloon squeeze was 10 seconds (SD=6), adding up to a
ployed because voxels outside the head probably do not exhibit mean total duration of balloon squeezes of 122 seconds
typical temporal dependencies known for BOLD signals acquired
in the brain. (SD=75). The average duration between successive bal-
loon squeezes was 33 seconds (SD=20).
No statistical differences were found between the pa-
Results tient group in the second analysis and the comparison
group for the mean number, mean duration, and total du-
Clinical Evaluation ration of the hallucinations/balloon squeezes and for the
Patients were chronically psychotic, with a mean total time between successive squeezes and releases.
PANSS score of 73 (SD=15) for the 24 patients in the analy-
fMRI
sis of brain activation during hallucinations (analysis 1).
These patients had an average score of 19 (SD=5) on the Analysis 1: Brain activation during auditory verbal
positive subscale, an average score of 19 (SD=0.5) on the hallucinations. As seen in Figure 1, the group analysis for
negative subscale, and an average score of 35 (SD=9) on hallucinations revealed activation of multiple confluent
the scale assessing general psychopathology. The 15 pa- brain regions. These included language-related regions,
tients in the analysis of brain activation prior to halluci- such as the bilateral insula and inferior frontal gyrus
nations (analysis 2) had a mean total PANSS score of 73 (including Broca’s homologue) as well as the middle

430 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010


DIEDEREN, NEGGERS, DAALMAN, ET AL.

TABLE 3 . Signifi cantly Activated Vox els and Locations of Local Max ima During Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Patients
With Psychotic Disordera
Lobe Area Montreal Neurological Institute Coordinate (x, y, z) t Cluster Size
Left parietal Postcentral gyrus –52, –20, 44 4.98 235
Left parietal Postcentral gyrus –56, –20, 16 3.21 25
Right parietal Postcentral gyrus 36, –36, 52 3.59 227
Left parietal Inferior parietal lobule –52, –20, 44 4.96 112
Right parietal Inferior parietal lobule 36, –36, 52 3.59 43
Left frontal Precentral gyrus –36, –16, 64 4.77 111
Left frontal Precentral gyrus –48, 0, 16 3.00 12
Right frontal Precentral gyrus 56, 8, 32 3.53 113
Right sublobar Insula 36, 0, 12 4.47 205
Left sublobar Insula –48, 0, 4 3.26 129
Left sublobar Insula –24, 20, –4 2.78 8
Right cerebellum Cerebellum 20, –48, –16 4.27 571
Right cerebellum Cerebellum 8, –56, –6 2.90 5
Left cerebellum Cerebellum –20, –64, –16 2.75 11
Right frontal Inferior frontal gyrus 44, 16, 12 4.31 243
Left frontal Inferior frontal gyrus –48, 4, 8 3.72 50
Right frontal Superior frontal gyrus 28, 48, 20 4.07 43
Left frontal Superior frontal gyrus –28, –12, 68 3.32 7
Right parietal Supramarginal gyrus 48, –28, 44 3.50 136
Left parietal Supramarginal gyrus –56, –20, 16 3.21 27
Right temporal Superior temporal gyrus 44, –4, –8 3.47 20
Right temporal Superior temporal gyrus 64, –44, 16 2.82 9
Left temporal Superior temporal gyrus –48, 0, 0 3.46 40
Left temporal Superior temporal gyrus –56, 24, 16 3.08 18
Left temporal Middle temporal gyrus –48, –64, 12 3.55 58
Right temporal Middle temporal gyrus 60, –48, 0 2.73 15
Right frontal Middle frontal gyrus 40, –8, 60 3.37 51
Right frontal Middle frontal gyrus 44, 20, 36 3.10 11
a 
Threshold set at p<0.05, whole-brain false-discovery-rate-corrected.

temporal, superior temporal, and supramarginal gyri. the following two interconnected local maxima: one
Other significantly activated regions consisted of the located in the left parahippocampal gyrus and one in
bilateral inferior parietal lobule, precentral gyrus, the left cerebellum. Since these areas are anatomically
postcentral gyrus, cerebellum, and superior and middle unconnected, a functional connection of these maxima
frontal gyri. probably resulted from spatial smoothing. Inspection of
For more clarity with respect to the different functional the cluster coordinates revealed that 11 voxels were located
regions implicated in the group hallucination analysis, in the left parahippocampus, five were located in the left
masks, created using the Automatic Anatomical Labeling cerebellum, and one voxel was placed on the boundary
Atlas (19), were overlaid on the group results. Anatomic re- between these regions. Table 4 shows the coordinates of
gions were chosen based on the locations of significantly all significant local maxima in the group analysis. SPM2 F
activated local maxima in the groupwise hallucination statistics are shown in Figure 2.
analysis. Masks consisted of the bilateral inferior frontal, Figure 3 shows poststimulus time histogram plots from
middle temporal, superior temporal, supramarginal, pre- 6 to 0 seconds prior to hallucinations, averaged over all
central, postcentral, middle frontal, and superior frontal subjects, and a fitted sixth-order polynomial of all regions
gyri as well as the inferior parietal lobule, insula, and cer- showing significant signal changes in the group analysis.
ebellum. The amount of significantly activated voxels and From these poststimulus time histogram plots it can be
the coordinates of the local maximum and its t values are concluded that the reported regions were significantly de-
reported for every masked region in Table 3. activated.
Analysis 2: Brain activation preceding auditory verbal No significant effect of motion before the onset of hal-
hallucinations and random balloon squeezes. The lucinations was revealed by the realignment parameters
most prominent signal change preceding hallucinations (for the three translation parameters as well as for the
was observed in the left parahippocampal gyrus. In three rotation parameters), as identified by two separate
addition, significant signal changes were found in the left repeated-measures MANOVAs, with the 10 scans preced-
superior temporal, right inferior frontal, and left middle ing hallucinations as repeated measures.
frontal gyri as well as the right insula and left cerebellum. Three independent repeated-measures ANOVAs, with
The largest significantly activated cluster consisted of gray and white matter and out-of-brain signal changes

Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 431


DEACTIVATION OF PARAHIPPOCAMPAL GYRUS

TABLE 4 . Signifi cantly Activated Vox els and Locations of Local Max ima During 6 Seconds Preceding Auditory Verbal Hal-
lucinations in Patients With Psychotic Disordera
Lobe Area Montreal Neurological Institute Coordinate (x, y, z) F Cluster Size
Left limbic Parahippocampal gyrus –36, –24, –16 7.52 9
Left temporal Superior temporal gyrus –44, 16, –24 6.38 6
Left frontal Middle frontal gyrus –36, 16, 28 5.47 11
Left limbicb Parahippocampal gyrus –24, –28, –12 4.66 11
Left cerebellumb Anterior lobe, culmen –28, –36, –28 5.17 5
Right frontal Inferior frontal gyrus 52, 36, 8 5.07 5
Right frontal Inferior frontal gyrus 36, 32, 4 5.03 5
Right sublobar Insula 40, 8, 4 4.89 5
Left cerebellum Anterior lobe, culmen –24, –44, –16 4.07 5
a 
Threshold set at p<0.05, whole-brain false-discovery-rate-corrected.
b 
A functional connection of maxima in the left parahippocampal gyrus and the left cerebellum comprised 17 voxels, with 11 in the left para­
hippocampal gyrus, five in the left cerebellum, and one in the boundary between these regions.

as dependent variables and the 10 scans preceding hal- (21–24). Indeed, activation of association cortices origi-
lucinations as repeated measures, revealed no significant nally involved in the encoded fragment has consistently
signal changes, indicating that motion artifacts cannot been shown during memory retrieval (25–27).
explain the effects detected with the finite-impulse-re- In the case of auditory verbal hallucinations, increased
sponse analysis. dopaminergic stimulation may enhance the redistribu-
tion function of the parahippocampal gyrus, leading to
Brain Activation Preceding Balloon Squeezes
erroneous activation of an association cortex and hence
No significant signal changes were observed prior to the to incorrect recognition. Disinhibition of the parahippo-
random balloon squeezes. campal gyrus, demonstrated as deactivation preceding
hallucinations, then triggers the bilateral language-related
Discussion areas originally involved in the perception of speech frag-
This study investigated brain activation during audi- ments, as shown in the first analysis of the preset study.
tory verbal hallucinations in 24 patients with psychosis. According to this hypothesis, hallucinations result from
Brain activation during these hallucinations was primar- the spontaneous re-experience of memories, as already
ily present in bilateral language areas. Brain activation hypothesized in the nineteenth century (28). Support for
preceding hallucinations could be investigated in 15 sub- this hypothesis was provided in a study in which patients
jects. Groupwise analysis of signal changes up to 6 sec- with hallucinations showed difficulties identifying the
onds preceding hallucinations showed pronounced de- source of memories (29). Furthermore, increases in hallu-
activation in the left parahippocampal gyrus. Additional cinations have been associated with an increasing inabil-
deactivation was observed in the left superior temporal, ity to inhibit irrelevant memories (30).
left middle frontal, and right inferior frontal gyri as well Deactivation instead of activation of the parahippocam-
as the right insula and left cerebellum. These findings pus may seem at odds with its postulated role in memory
could not be attributed to motor activation, since group- recollection. However, deactivations have been reported
wise analysis of activity in the 6 seconds prior to random to be realistic phenomena, probably caused by short de-
balloon squeezes revealed no significant signal changes. creases in neuronal activity (31, 32). Previous studies on
Bilateral activation of language regions during hallucina- memory recollection have also reported deactivation of
tions is consistent with a previous study conducted by our the parahippocampus (33–37).
research group (9). Apart from parahippocampal deactivation, we observed
Most prominent deactivation preceding hallucinations significant deactivation preceding hallucinations in the
was observed in the parahippocampal gyrus. Because the left superior temporal and right inferior frontal gyri as well
parahippocampus is D2 innervated (20), this finding might as right insula. These areas correspond with regions sig-
represent an important link between dopaminergic over- nificantly activated during hallucinations and may result
activity and inadequate activation of bilateral language- from the information redistributed to them by the para-
related areas. The parahippocampus has been hypothe- hippocampus, preparing them for activation in the course
sized to play a central role in memory recollection, since it of hallucinations.
receives perceptual information from association cortices, Our results are partially consistent with those of previ-
such as the language areas, and forwards this informa- ous studies. Hoffman and colleagues (38) reported deacti-
tion to the hippocampus in order to be “recognized.” This vation of the parahippocampal and anterior cingulate gyri
perceptual information is then passed back to the para- preceding hallucinations in six patients as well as activa-
hippocampal gyrus from where it is redistributed to the tion of the left anterior insula and right middle temporal
association cortices involved in the original perception gyrus (38). In addition, Lennox and colleagues (39) found

432 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010


DIEDEREN, NEGGERS, DAALMAN, ET AL.

FIGURE 2. Areas of Signifi cant Activation Preceding Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Patients With Psychotic Disordera

4 7 4 7 4 7

4 7 4 7 4 7

SPM2 F statistics for the interval 6 to 0 seconds preceding auditory verbal hallucinations depict significant signal changes in the left parahip­
a 

pocampal gyrus, left superior temporal gyrus, left cerebellum, left middle frontal gyrus, right insula, and right inferior frontal gyrus.

activity in the right middle temporal gyrus in a single sub- duced the observed deactivations because such artifacts
ject, while Shergill and colleagues (40) found activity in should have been present in these regions also. Further-
the left inferior frontal and right middle temporal gyri pre- more, the movement parameters describing head motion
ceding hallucinations in two subjects. Deactivations were in the fMRI images did not differ significantly from zero
not discussed in these studies. before the hallucinations.
Since little a priori knowledge was available to indicate
Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research which neural and cognitive processes precede hallucina-
A limitation of this study is that most patients were tions, we selected a control condition that only controlled
treated with antipsychotic medication. Since the effect of for motor preparation, enabling us to identify brain acti-
antipsychotic medication is probably mediated by antag- vation related to the actual hallucinations. A limitation of
onism at the D2 receptors (2) and the parahippocampus is the control condition is that motor control was compared
D2 innervated (20), parahippocampal function is expected between subjects instead of within subjects. However,
to be influenced by D2 blockade. The patients included in a within-subjects analysis was impractical as a result of
this study suffered from medication-resistant hallucina- the interference of hallucinations during random balloon
tions. Therefore, deactivation of the parahippocampal gy- squeezing among the patients.
rus is expected to be present in medication-free patients In addition, a potential flaw is that significant deacti-
with psychosis prior to hallucinations, yet negative signal vation preceding hallucinations may have resulted from
changes may be more pronounced in these patients. To subjects’ delayed response to the hallucinations. In this
further explore this effect, future studies should focus on case, peak deactivation would be expected to occur in the
brain activation preceding hallucinations in medication- last seconds preceding hallucinations. However, in this
free patients with psychosis. study peak deactivation occurred between 4.87 and 2.44
Furthermore, a limitation of the finite impulse response seconds prior to hallucinations (Figure 3), which renders
can be that observed effects not necessarily reflect hemo- this explanation unlikely.
dynamic changes but instead any BOLD signal deviation Finally, it can be argued that deactivation preceding hal-
from baseline. Nonetheless, in our study, analyses of sig- lucinations results from the “poststimulus undershoot.”
nal changes in gray and white matter and out-of-brain When applying BOLD fMRI, peak activation is followed
voxels showed that movement artifacts could not have in- by a short period of deactivation, which is the poststimu-

Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 433


DEACTIVATION OF PARAHIPPOCAMPAL GYRUS

FIGURE 3 . Time Plots of Global Signal Changes in Regions lus undershoot. However, the poststimulus undershoot
of Interest During the Six Seconds Prior to Auditory Verbal is typically present between 10 and 30 seconds after an
Hallucinations in Patients With Psychotic Disorder
event. Since the average time between successive halluci-
A Parahippocampus Polynomial (parahippocampus)
nations was 38 seconds in our study, this explanation also
Inferior Frontal Polynomial (Inferior Frontal
appears unlikely.
Gyrus Gyrus)
An important strength of this study is the large sample
size and groupwise analyses. Furthermore, other explana-
1.0
tions for the present findings were ruled out by includ-
Global % Signal Change

ing a comparison group and investigating the BOLD time


0.5
course signals for motion artifacts. Finally, a particular
strength is that an assumption-free finite-impulse-re-
0
sponse model was used.
To test the model proposed in this study, future stud-
–0.5
ies should focus on comparing hallucinations with word
–1.0
recall. An elegant design would be one in which patients
recall their previous hallucinations
–1.5
6.09 5.48 4.87 4.26 3.65 3.05 2.44 1.83 1.22 0.61 0.00
Time Preceding Auditory
Conclusions
Verbal Hallucinations (seconds)
The present study showed that auditory verbal halluci-
B
nations are preceded by deactivation of the parahippocam-
Superior Temporal Polynomial (Superior
Gyrus Temporal Gyrus) pus. Since the parahippocampus is implicated in memory
Insula Polynomial (Insula) recollection, dysfunction of this region could trigger in-
adequate activation of language-related regions, leading
to auditory hallucinations. Indeed, activation of language
0.5 regions was detected during these hallucinations.
Global % Signal Change

0
Received April 1, 2009; revision received Sept. 9, 2009; accepted
Oct. 5, 2009 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040456). From the De-
partment of Psychiatry, Neuroscience Division, University Medical
–0.5 Center Utrecht and Rudolf Magnus Institute for Neuroscience, Hei-
delberglaan, the Netherlands; Parnassia Bavo Group, the Hague,
the Netherlands; Department of Psychiatry, University of Gronin-
–1.0 gen, Groningen, the Netherlands. Address correspondence and
reprint requests to Ms. Diederen, Neuroscience Division, Univer-
sity Medical Center Utrecht and Rudolf Magnus Institute for Neu-
–1.5 roscience, B01.206, Heidelberglaan 100, 3485CX, the Netherlands;
6.09 5.48 4.87 4.26 3.65 3.05 2.44 1.83 1.22 0.61 0.00 K.M.J.Diederen@umcutrecht.nl (e-mail).
Time Preceding Auditory T he authors report no financial relationships w ith commercial in-
Verbal Hallucinations (seconds) terests.
Supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scien-
C Middle Frontal Polynomial (Middle Frontal tific Research, Innovation Impulse, number 916.56.172.
Gyrus Gyrus)
Cerebellum Polynomial (Cerebellum)
References
1.5
1. Shergill SS, M urray RM , M cGuire PK: Auditory hallucinations:
a review of psychological treatments. Schizophr Res 1998;
Global % Signal Change

1.0
32:137–150
0.5 2. Seeman P, Lee T, Chau-Wong M , Wong K: Antipsychotic drug
doses and neuroleptic/dopamine receptors. Nature 1976;
0 261:717–719
3. Hoffman RE, Hampson M , Wu K, Anderson AW, Gore JC , Bu-
–0.5 chanan RJ, Constable RT, Hawkins KA, Sahay N, Krystal JH:
Probing the pathophysiology of auditory/verbal hallucina-
–1
tions by combining functional magnetic resonance imaging
–1.5 and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex 2007;
7:2733–2743
–2.0 4. M cGuire PK, Shah GM , M urray RM : Increased blood flow in
6.09 5.48 4.87 4.26 3.65 3.05 2.44 1.83 1.22 0.61 0.00 Broca’s area during auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia.
Time Preceding Auditory
Lancet 1993; 342:703–706
Verbal Hallucinations (seconds)

434 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010


DIEDEREN, NEGGERS, DAALMAN, ET AL.

5. Shergill SS, Brammer M J, W illiams SCR, M urray RM , M cGuire 22. Eichenbaum H, Schoenbaum G , Young B, Bunsey M : Function-
PK: M apping auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia using al organization of the hippocampal memory system. Proc Natl
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry Acad Sci U S A 1996; 24:13500–13507
2000; 57:1033–1038 23. Eichenbaum H: A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative
6. D ierks T, Linden D JE, Jandl M , Formisano E, Goebel R, Lanfer- memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 2000; 1:41–50
mann H, Singer W : Activation of Heschl’s gyrus during auditory 24. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C: The medial tem-
hallucinations. Neuron 1999; 22:615–621 poral lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007;
7. Shergill SS, Cameron LA, Brammer M J, W illiams SC , M urray 30:123–152
RM , M cGuire PK: M odality specific neural correlates of audi- 25. W heeler M E, Petersen SE, Buckner RL: M emory’s echo: vivid re-
tory and somatic hallucinations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry membering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proc Natl Acad
2001; 71:688–690 Sci U S A 2000; 97:11125–11129
8. Sommer IE, D e Weijer AD, D aalman K, Neggers SF, Somers M , 26. Kahn I, D avachi L, W agner AD : Functional-neuroanatomic cor-
Kahn RS, Slotema CW, Blom JD, Hoek HW, Aleman A: Can fM RI- relates of recollection: implications for models of recognition
guidance improve the efficacy of rTM S treatment for auditory memory. J Neurosci 2004; 28:4172–4180
verbal hallucinations? Schizophr Res 2007; 93:406–408 27. Johnson JD, Rugg M D : Recollection and the reinstatement of
9. Sommer IE, D iederen KM , Blom JD, W illems A, Kushan L, Slote- encoding-related cortical activity. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17:2507–
ma K, Boks M PM , D aalman K, Hoek HW, Neggers SFW, Kahn 2515
RS: Auditory verbal hallucinations predominantly activate the 28. Kahlbaum K: D ie Sinnesdelirien. Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psy-
right inferior frontal area. Brain 2008; 131:3169–3177 chiatrie und psychisch-gerichtliche M edizin 1866; 23:56–78
10. Levey AI, Hersch SM , Rye D B, Sunahara RK, Niznik HB, Kitt CA, 29. Brébion G , Amador X, D avid A, M alaspina D, Sharif Z, Gorman
Price D L, M aggio R, Brann M R, Ciliax BJ: Localization of D 1 and JM : Positive symptomatology and source-monitoring failure in
D 2 dopamine receptors in brain with subtype-specific anti­ schizophrenia: an analysis of symptom specific effects. Psychi-
bodies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993; 90:8861–8865 atry Res 2000; 95:119–131
11. Hurd YL, Suzuki M , Sedvall GC: D 1 and D 2 dopamine receptor 30. W aters FA, Badcock JC , M aybery M T, M ichie PT: Inhibition
mRNA expression in whole hemisphere sections of the human in schizophrenia: association with auditory hallucinations.
brain. J Chem Neuroanat 2001; 22:127–137 Schizophr Res 2003; 62:275–280
12. Andreasen NC , Flaum M , Arndt S: The Comprehensive Assess- 31. Shmuel A, Yacoub E, Pfeuffer J, Van de M oortele PF, Adriany
ment of Symptoms and History (CASH): an instrument for as- G , Hu X, Ugurbil K: Sustained negative BO LD, blood flow and
sessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry oxygen consumption response and its coupling to the positive
1992; 49:615–623 response in the human brain. Neuron 2002; 36:1195–1210
13. Kay S, Fiszbein A, Opler L: The Positive and Negative Syndrome 32. Smith AT, W illiams AL, Singh KD : Negative BO LD in the visu-
Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261–276 al cortex: evidence against blood stealing. Hum Brain M app
14. Haddock G, M cCarron J, Tarrier N, Faragher EB: Scales to measure 2004; 21:213–220
dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the Psychotic Symp- 33. Burgess N, M aguire EA, Spiers HJ, O ’Keefe J: A temporoparietal
tom Rating Scales (PSYRATS). Psychol M ed 1999; 29:879–889 and prefrontal network for retrieving the spatial context of life-
15. Neggers SF, Hermans EJ, Ramsey NF: Enhanced sensitivity with like events. Neuroimage 2001; 14:439–453
fast three-dimensional blood-oxygen-level-dependent func- 34. Ross RS, Slotnick SD : The hippocampus is preferentially associ-
tional M RI: comparison of SENSE-PRESTO and 2D -EPI at 3 T. ated with memory for spatial context. J Cogn Neurosci 2008;
NM R Biomed 2008; 21:663–676 20:432–446
16. Worsley KJ, Friston KJ: Analysis of fM RI time-series revisited— 35. Woodruff CC , Johnson JD, Uncapher M R, Rugg M D : Content-
again. Neuroimage 1995; 2:173–181 specificity of the neural correlates of recollection. Neuropsy-
17. Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M , Trinath T, O eltermann A: chologia 2005; 43:1022–1032
Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fM RI sig- 36. Henson RNA, Cansino S, Herron JE, Robb W GK, Rugg M D : A
nal. Nature 2001; 412:150–157 familiarity signal in human anterior medial temporal cortex?
18. Henson R, Rugg M , Friston KJ: The choice of basis functions in Hippocampus 2003; 13:301–304
event-related fM RI. Neuroimage 2001; 13(suppl 1):149 37. Weis S, Klaver P, Reul J, Elger CE, Fernandez G: Neural correlates
19. Tzourio-M azoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, of successful declarative memory formation and retrieval: the
Etard O, D elcroix N, M azoyer B, Joliot M : Automated anatomi- anatomical overlap. Cortex 2004; 40:200–202
cal labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatom- 38. Hoffman RE, Anderson AW, Varanko M , Gore JC , Hampson
ical parcellation of the M NI M RI single-subject brain. Neuroim- M : Time course of regional brain activation associated with
age 2002; 15:273–289 onset of auditory/verbal hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry 2008;
20. Joyce JN, Janowsky A, Neve KA: Characterization and distribu- 193:424–425
tion of [ 125 I]epidepride binding to dopamine D 2 receptors in 39. Lennox BR, Park SB, Jones PB, M orris PG: Spatial and temporal
basal ganglia and cortex of human brain. J Pharmacol Exp mapping of neural activity associated with auditory hallucina-
Ther 1991; 257:253–1263 tions. Lancet 1999; 20:644
21. Van Hoesen GW : The parahippocampal gyrus: new observa- 40. Shergill SS, Brammer M J, Amaro E, W illiams SC , M urray RM :
tions regarding its cortical connections in the monkey. Trends Temporal course of auditory hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry
Neurosci 1982; 5:345–350 2004; 185:516–517

Am J Psychiatry 167:4, April 2010 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 435

You might also like