Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Gender Differences in Food Choice: The Contribution of Health Beliefs and Dieting

Jane Wardle, Ph.D., Anne M. Haase, Ph.D., and Andrew Steptoe, D.Phil.
University College London

Maream Nillapun, Ph.D. and Kiriboon Jonwutiwes, Ph.D.


Silpakorn University

France Bellisle, Ph.D.


INSERM U341
Paris, France

ABSTRACT other countries show similar results (4,5). Curiously, behavioral


Background: Gender differences in health behaviors have medicine investigators, along with many clinical researchers,
been reported in many studies but causal mechanisms have have paid little attention to this striking phenomenon. A recent
been neglected. Purpose and Methods: This study examines 4 report by the Institute of Medicine (6) noted that “being male
food choice behaviors in a large sample of young adults from or female is an important fundamental variable that should be
23 countries and tests 2 possible explanatory mechanisms for considered when analyzing basic and clinical research” (p. 7).
the gender differences—women’s greater likelihood of dieting The report also stated that “the understanding of sex differ-
and women’s greater beliefs in the importance of healthy diets. ences in health and illness merits serious scientific enquiry in
Results: Women were more likely than men to report avoiding all aspects of biomedical and health-related research” (p. 4).
high-fat foods, eating fruit and fiber, and limiting salt (to a This “call to arms” was focused particularly on biological dif-
lesser extent) in almost all of the 23 countries. They were also ferences, but sex differences in behavior and cognition were
more likely to be dieting and attached greater importance to also noted to play a part in differences in health. Observations
healthy eating. Dieting status explained around 22% of the that the female survival advantage is a relatively modern phe-
gender difference in fat choices, 23% of fiber choices, and 7% nomenon, that it has varied considerably over time, and that it
of fruit, but none of the gender difference in salt. Health be- differs between countries add to the case for the importance of
liefs explained around 40% of the differences in each of the di- environmental and behavioral factors in sex differences in
etary behaviors and together they explained almost 50%. Gen- health (7–10).
der differences in food choices therefore appear to be partly The fact that the major diseases of adult life have a strong
attributable to women’s greater weight control involvement behavioral element (11) means that studies of sex differences in
and partly to their stronger beliefs in healthy eating. Conclu- health behaviors could play an important role in understanding
sions: Further research is needed to understand the additional sex difference in health and illness. A study of cardiovascular
factors that could promote men’s participation in simple disease mortality in 24 countries found that gender differences
healthy eating practices. in five risk factors explained over 40% of the gender difference
in mortality (12). Many studies of health behaviors have noted
(Ann Behav Med 2004, 27(2):107–116) that men have higher rates of risky behaviors and lower rates of a
range of healthy and hygienic practices than women (13–17). A
INTRODUCTION recent review confirmed that across more than 30 health behav-
Men live an average of 7 years fewer than women (1). iors, ranging from wearing seatbelts and smoking to physician
This results from a combination of higher male mortality in in- visits and nutrition, men demonstrated fewer healthy choices
fancy, more accidental deaths in young adult men, and a than women (18). Such extensive behavioral differences could
higher risk of the major causes of death in middle-aged men make a significant contribution to gender differences in morbid-
than middle-aged women. In the United States, for example, ity and mortality. The ubiquity of observations of gender differ-
the age-adjusted death rate for heart disease is twice as high in ences in health behaviors might be expected to have generated
men as women (2), and men’s cancer death rate is 1.5 times an extensive literature on the causal mechanisms, but as ob-
higher (3). Comparisons between male and female deaths in served in a recent review, gender issues, along with other demo-
graphic factors, have been relatively neglected in behavioral
medicine research (19).
This research was supported by Cancer Research UK.
Courtenay (20) argued that socially constructed gender
Reprint Address: J. Wardle, Ph.D., Cancer Research UK, Health Be- roles, which identify health risk behaviors with maleness and
haviour Unit, Dept. of Epidemiology & Public Health, University Col- health protective behavior with femaleness, are part of the
lege London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. mechanism for gender differences in health behaviors. He
E-mail:j.wardle@ucl.ac.uk pointed out that men on television smoke more, drink more, and
© 2004 by The Society of Behavioral Medicine. are more overweight, yet they are much less likely to be diag-

107
108 Wardle et al. Annals of Behavioral Medicine

nosed with illnesses (21–24)—thus appearing to be citizens who iour Survey [EHBS]; 17). They had been chosen to represent as-
can take health risks without paying health costs (20,25). Men pects of food choices on which there was consensus about the
who subscribe to the socially constructed gender roles would health benefits, which could be simply phrased, and would be
therefore be particularly at risk of premature death from dis- meaningful across the young adult sample drawn from many
eases where health behaviors play an important role. different countries. On the basis of existing work, we expected
Food choices are an area in which consistent behavioral dif- to see gender differences in food choices in most if not all of the
ferences have been observed. Numerous studies have reported participating countries. The study also tested the hypotheses that
that boys and men eat fewer fruits and vegetables, choose fewer gender differences would be most marked in food choices rele-
high-fiber foods, eat fewer low-fat foods, and consume more vant to dieting or “light” food consumption (fat and fruit), that
soft drinks than do women (26–32). Differences in reported differences in intake would be partly mediated by dieting status,
food choices have not always been reflected in differences in the that beliefs about the importance of healthy food choices would
proportion of energy consumed as fats, or fiber intake (33–37), be lower in men, and that differences in food choices would be
but this is partly because of gender differences in alcohol con- partly mediated by gender differences in beliefs. Finally, we ex-
sumption, which may add a substantial sum of energy as “drink amined how much of the gender differences in food choices
calories.” Adjusted for energy intake, women’s diets do tend to were left unexplained after taking account of dieting status and
be higher in micronutrients than men’s diets (35,38). health beliefs.
Men rate many health behaviors, including food choice be-
haviors, as less important than women rate them (18,39,40). METHOD
Courtenay’s (20) analysis made special mention of nutrition: Study Sample and Design
“[The socially prescribed male] would not be interested in learn- The IHBS is a self-report, questionnaire-based study of a
ing about health and nutrition, or cooking, and he would be un- wide range of health behaviors and attitudes carried out in uni-
concerned about his weight, diet or hygiene” (p. 10). It has also versity students from 23 different countries between 1999 and
been shown that men give lower priority to health compared 2001. The international perspective permits both cross-cultural
with other considerations such as taste or convenience in mak- comparisons and examination of the predictors of health behav-
ing food choices (4,41–45). Differences in beliefs about the im- iors in diverse cultural contexts. Data collection was carried out
portance of eating in accordance with health recommendations at one university in each of 19 countries and at two universities
could therefore contribute to explaining the gender differences in the other 4 countries. Questionnaire items were based on the
in food choice. forerunner of the IHBS, the EHBS; a very similar survey was
Another factor that might contribute to gender differences carried out in Europe between 1989 and 1991 (17,63). Data col-
in diet is women’s greater concern about weight control and lection was carried out by a network of collaborators from one
their higher frequency of dieting. In almost every study of atti- or two universities in each country, who distributed the IHBS
tudes to body weight or dieting, women report more worries questionnaires to students on non-health-related courses, usu-
about weight and make more attempts to control weight than do ally during lectures. Although questionnaire completion was
men (41,46–48). This applies equally in pre-adolescence and in voluntary, participation exceeded 90% in most countries. A total
old age (49,50), to the poor as well as the rich (51), and in devel- of 19,298 university students (men, n = 8,482; women, n =
oping as well as developed countries (52–54). Selecting lower 10,816), aged 17 to 30, completed the questionnaire. The ques-
fat and lower sugar foods, as well as increasing intake of fruits tionnaire items for the IHBS were deliberately selected to be
and vegetables, are important aspects of many weight-control- simple and were translated and back-translated into 17 lan-
ling diets (31,55,56), and therefore differences in dieting guages (Bulgarian, Czech, Dutch, Flemish, French, German,
might partly explain gender differences in food choices, particu- Greek, Hungarian, Icelandic, Italian, Japanese, Korean, Polish,
larly food choices such as fat restriction, which are linked with Portuguese, Romanian, Spanish, and Thai). This study is con-
dieting. cerned specifically with aspects of food choice, and therefore
Closely related to the dieting issue is the sociological analy- only items related to food choice behaviors, related beliefs, diet-
sis of women’s eating practices, which suggests that femininity ing status, gender, age, and smoking are reported.
is associated with “lightness” and delicacy in appetite (57,58).
According to this view, the volume of foods eaten should appear Measures
to be less and the type of foods selected should appear to be less Four food choice behaviors were assessed using single
substantial and more “healthy” for women than men (59–62). items: “Do you make a conscious effort to avoid foods that con-
Gender differences in food choice would therefore be most ap- tain fat and cholesterol?” (yes, no), “Do you make a conscious
parent where lightness or “low energy” were important differen- effort to eat foods that are high in fiber?” (yes, no), “How often
tiating factors, even where dieting, per se, is not an issue. do you eat fruit?” (daily, 2–3 times a week, once a week, less
This study examines gender differences in four food choice than once a week, never), and “Do you add salt to your meals?”
behaviors that were measured in the International Health Behav- (usually, sometimes, very occasionally, never). Reliability of
iour Survey (IHBS): avoiding fat, eating fiber, eating fruit, and these items was reported earlier (64). The fruit and salt items are
limiting salt (63). The same food choice behaviors had also been very similar to fruit and salt items in other food frequency ques-
assessed in the predecessor study (the European Health Behav- tionnaires (65,66), but the other two items represent subjective
Volume 27, Number 2, 2004 Gender and Healthy Food Choices 109

perspectives on food choices, similar to those included in food Demographic data including gender and age were collected
choice measures (67,68) rather than nutritional evaluations. from all participants.
Comparisons were conducted between respondents who re-
ported avoiding and not avoiding high-fat foods, eating or not
Statistical Analyses
eating high-fiber foods, eating fruit daily or less than daily, and
usually or less often adding salt. Age-adjusted prevalence of food choice behaviors, mean
Health beliefs that corresponded approximately to the four health belief scores, and smoking and dieting frequencies, for
food choices were rated on a scale from 1 (of very low impor- both men and women, were computed for each country sepa-
tance) to 10 (of very great importance), each with the stem rately and also for all 23 countries combined, controlling for any
“How important is it …?” The selected items to use were not to clustering effect by country. Results are presented as percent-
eat too much animal fat, to eat enough fiber, to eat enough fruit. ages or means with 95% confidence intervals. Gender differ-
ences in the dependent variables (reported food choices) and the
and not to add too much salt. For some analyses, a median split
potential mediating factors (health beliefs and dieting status) as
of the belief scores was used to characterize high and low be-
well as control variables (smoking status) were analyzed using
liefs. The belief items were not an exact match to the behaviors
chi-square tests and analysis of variance with confidence inter-
because it was deemed important to keep all items the same in
vals adjusted for age and clustering by country. Associations be-
the IHBS as they had been in the EHBS.
tween food choice behaviors and the potential mediating vari-
Dieting status was assessed by asking participants to re-
ables were analyzed with univariate logistic regression. Where
spond to the single question “Are you dieting to lose weight?”
the conditions for mediation were met (i.e., the potential mediat-
with a yes–no response format.
ing variables were related both to the independent and the de-
Smoking was included as a control variable in the analyses
pendent variables) (69), successive logistic regression models
in the light of evidence that smokers tend to eat less healthy diets
were used to assess the extent to which adding the potential me-
than nonsmokers and that the men have higher levels of smoking
diators (dieting status and health beliefs) to the basic model
than women in the IHBS sample (44). Smoking was measured
modified the odds ratio associated with gender. All statistical
by asking participants to select one of eight statements: “I have
analyses were done with STATA 6 (STATA Corporation, Col-
never smoked a cigarette, not even a puff,” “I have only ever lege Station, TX).
tried one or two cigarettes”; “I used to smoke sometimes, but I
don’t now”; “I don’t smoke cigarettes, but smoke a pipe or ci-
gars”; “I smoke cigarettes, but not as many as one per day”; “I RESULTS
usually smoke between 1 and 10 cigarettes per day”; “I usually The frequencies of each food choice behavior in men and
smoke between 10 and 20 cigarettes per day”; and “I usually women, adjusted for age and within-country clustering effects,
smoke more than 20 cigarettes per day.” For these analyses, two are presented in Table 1. Across the whole sample, women were
categories were created, with the first three response items des- 50% more likely than men to report avoiding high-fat foods and
ignated nonsmokers and the remaining five responses desig- eating high-fiber foods and 25% more likely to eat fruit at least
nated smokers. daily. They were, on average, 6% less likely to add salt, which

TABLE 1
Prevalence of Healthy Food Choices, Mean Ratings of Associated Beliefs, and Dieting and Smoking in Men and Women (95% CI)

Variable Womena Menb Tests of Significance

Age (years) 20.63 (20.53–20.73) 20.93 (20.82–21.04)


Dietary behavior (% of sample)
Avoiding fat 46% (42–58) 26% (23–29) χ2 = 825, p < .001
Eating high-fiber foods 40% (33–47) 27% (22–32) χ2 = 349, p < .001
Eating fruit at least daily 47% (41–54) 37% (31–43) χ2 = 225, p < .001
Never adding salt to meals 71% (64–77) 68% (61–74) χ2 = 20.8, p < .001
Health belief (rating = 1–10)
(Mean belief rating and 95% CI)
Avoiding high-fat foods 6.87 (6.83–6.92) 5.73 (5.67–5.78) F = 1025, p < .001
Eating enough fiber 7.06 (7.02–7.10) 6.18 (6.13–6.23) F = 718, p < .001
Eating enough fruit 8.13 (8.10–8.17) 7.37 (7.32–7.42) F = 700, p < .001
Avoiding adding salt 6.61 (6.56–6.66) 5.79 (5.74–5.85) F = 482, p < .001
Dieting (% dieting) 19% (15–23) 5% (4–7) χ2 = 803, p < .001
Smoking (% current smokers) 27% (25–35) 34% (30–38) χ2 = 120, p < .001

Note. Prevalence levels and mean ratings are age-adjusted for the total population, as well as adjusting the CIs for any clustering effect of data by country.
For the health beliefs, scores range from 1 (very low importance) to 10 (very high importance). CI = confidence interval.
an = 10,816. bn = 8,482.
110 Wardle et al. Annals of Behavioral Medicine

though small was still a significant effect (see Table 1). Effect Having shown significant gender differences in each of
sizes (binomial effect size display) for the gender difference of food choice behaviors and in the potential mediators, the next
the four behaviors ranged from .21 for fat (conventionally a set of analyses used logistic regression to quantify the gender
small effect) through .13 for fiber, .11 for fruit, and .03 for salt. differences for each food choice behavior and to see if the two
Table 2 shows the gender differences in food choice behav- potential mediators (dieting and beliefs) were related to the food
iors in each country separately. Gender differences were ob- choice behaviors. The first column of Table 4 shows the odds ra-
served for all 23 countries for avoiding fat and eating fruit daily tios for each food choice behavior, in relation to gender, dieting
and in 21 out of 23 countries for eating fiber. This consistency status, and beliefs in the importance of each behavior (high vs.
was not maintained for limiting salt, where women reported lim- low beliefs), controlling for age and within-country clustering.
iting salt more than men in 13 countries, and men more than The odds of healthier behavior in women compared with men
women in 9 countries. However, the gender differences in salt ranged from 2.44 for avoiding fat, 1.80 for fiber, and 1.56 for
avoidance were significant only in 6 countries, and all of these fruit to 1.15 for salt, and in all cases the confidence intervals
showed a female advantage. were higher than 1. The effects of dieting status were significant
Mean ratings of the related dietary beliefs over all countries for fat (odds ratio [OR] = 3.31), fiber (2.29), and fruit (1.31) but
combined are shown in Table 1, and individual country scores were not significant for salt. OR for being in the highest versus
are shown in Table 3. Food choices were accorded moderately the lowest half of the population for the related belief showed
high levels of importance for health; that is, all belief ratings av- strong and significant effects for all four behaviors, ranging
eraged over the midpoint of 5 and most were over 6. Both men from 2.27 for salt up to 6.08 for fiber.
and women rated eating fruit as the most important, followed by Given that beliefs were associated with both gender and
eating fiber. Fat avoidance and limiting salt were ordered differ- each of the four food choices, they met Baron and Kenny’s (69)
ently for men and women. Strikingly, women reported signifi- conditions for being potential mediators of the gender/food
cantly stronger beliefs in the importance of each of the aspects choice association. Dieting met criteria for mediation for fat,
of diet than men in almost every country in the sample. fruit, and fiber. Subsequent analyses therefore examined the
Percentages and confidence intervals for dieting and smok- gender effect after controlling for dieting (Model 2), beliefs
ing are also shown in Table 1. As expected, almost four times as (Model 3), and both dieting and beliefs (Model 4). The same
many women as men reported dieting to lose weight. Smoking analyses are illustrated for all four food choices to maintain con-
was more prevalent in men (34%) than women (27%). sistency of reporting, even though the absence of an association

TABLE 2
Prevalence of Food Choice Behaviors (% Avoiding Fat, Eating Fiber, Eating Fruit Daily, and Limiting Salt Intake) in
Men and Women Across 23 Different Countries

Country Avoiding Fat Men/Women Eating Fiber Men/Women Eating Fruit Daily Men/Women Limiting Salt Men/Women

Belgium 20.6 / 44.8* 17.5 / 40.4* 32.8 / 44.0* 81.4 / 76.9


Bulgaria 22.9 / 53.7* 12.5 / 25.4* 33.8 / 34.7 70.5 / 70.5
Columbia 36.3 / 55.7* 40.5 / 50.3* 38.1 / 51.7* 49.7 / 52.2
England 29.8 / 49.5* 26.0 / 38.7* 35.8 / 44.1* 70.3 / 73.0
France 16.6 / 38.2* 10.9 / 28.7* 36.4 / 53.5* 60.8 / 67.7*
Germany 21.6 / 54.2* 25.6 / 43.1* 31.4 / 56.1* 90.1 / 89.1
Greece 27.0 / 46.5* 34.8 / 49.0* 39.2 / 47.3* 50.3 / 60.9*
Hungary 21.4 / 36.5* 12.7 / 31.3* 29.5 / 39.8* 74.4 / 71.2
Iceland 25.2 / 50.1* 25.9 / 39.7* 22.7 / 30.1* 89.1 / 93.6*
Ireland 25.7 / 47.9* 26.2 / 41.9* 43.5 / 46.8 65.1 / 74.7*
Italy 23.2 / 46.6* 18.3 / 34.4* 57.9 / 63.8* 60.2 / 69.0*
Japan 21.6 / 35.7* 27.3 / 38.0* 11.9 / 12.1 97.8 / 98.5
Korea 11.3 / 13.2 38.7 / 58.8* 16.0 / 33.3* 91.7 / 90.8
Netherlands 23.2 / 41.0* 32.0 / 46.6* 37.3 / 53.9* 53.8 / 55.9
Poland 23.1 / 46.1* 24.0 / 37.8* 42.4 / 59.6* 51.8 / 46.9
Portugal 21.7 / 42.4* 21.2 / 39.6* 54.7 / 66.4* 89.6 / 95.3*
Romania 36.5 / 47.9* 25.5 / 30.8 24.3 / 32.3* 52.8 / 49.9
Slovak Republic 19.1 / 44.0* 24.9 / 41.6* 44.5 / 63.7* 68.2 / 72.7
South Africa 37.3 / 50.6* 38.1 / 35.3 23.1 / 34.2* 46.3 / 46.5
Spain 31.5 / 52.3* 31.1 / 49.6* 57.1 / 64.0 52.5 / 54.9
Thailand 34.5 / 59.2* 64.6 / 78.6* 28.2 / 49.5* 58.9 / 58.4
USA total 33.1 / 50.4* 21.2 / 20.4 31.7 / 35.5 84.5 / 83.0
Venezuela 30.9 / 51.2* 31.8 / 46.3* 33.3 / 38.2 63.0 / 58.6

*p < .01, indicating that the prevalence rates are significantly different between men and women.
Volume 27, Number 2, 2004 Gender and Healthy Food Choices 111

TABLE 3
Age-Adjusted Mean Ratings of Food Choice Beliefs (Avoiding Fat, Eating Fiber, Eating Fruit Daily, and Limiting Salt Intake) in
Men and Women Across 23 Different Countries

Country Avoiding Fat Men/Women Eating Fiber Men/Women Eating Fruit Daily Men/Women Limiting Salt Men/Women

Belgium 4.29 / 6.18 5.14 / 6.52 6.35 / 7.60 5.35 / 6.26


Bulgaria 5.60 / 7.06 5.49 / 6.58 7.95 / 8.77 5.94 / 6.47
Columbia 5.83 / 7.21 5.86 / 6.54 7.08 / 8.09 5.90 / 6.82
England 5.72 / 6.73 6.30 / 7.13 7.40 / 8.23 5.87 / 6.61
France 5.07 / 6.28 4.79 / 5.79 6.27 / 7.39 4.88 / 5.30
Germany 5.06 / 6.72 5.79 / 6.97 7.27 / 8.40 5.23 / 5.96
Greece 6.18 / 7.26 6.84 / 7.87 7.94 / 8.59 5.00 / 5.77
Hungary 5.20 / 6.80 5.13 / 6.81 7.58 / 8.90 5.61 / 6.81
Iceland 5.46 / 7.07 6.57 / 7.91 6.72 / 8.41 5.72 / 7.19
Ireland 5.96 / 7.05 6.36 / 7.23 7.46 / 8.06 5.95 / 7.20
Italy 5.72 / 6.92 5.93 / 6.96 7.21 / 7.90 5.64 / 6.63
Japan 8.00 / 8.52 8.29 / 8.62 7.34 / 7.46 8.19 / 8.59
Korea 6.10 / 6.98 7.14 / 7.99 7.75 / 8.23 6.34 / 7.44
Netherlands 4.47 / 5.50 5.94 / 6.60 6.97 / 7.89 4.70 / 5.40
Poland 4.88 / 6.50 5.07 / 6.26 7.63 / 8.37 4.58 / 4.48
Portugal 6.11 / 7.33 6.35 / 7.67 7.45 / 8.40 6.32 / 7.58
Romania 6.53 / 7.87 7.48 / 8.63 8.73 / 9.34 6.66 / 7.31
Slovak Republic 5.98 / 7.48 6.68 / 7.77 8.17 / 8.86 5.34 / 6.18
South Africa 6.10 / 6.51 6.98 / 7.28 8.10 / 8.50 6.57 / 7.38
Spain 6.24 / 7.24 6.39 / 7.21 7.68 / 8.02 5.66 / 6.59
Thailand 5.59 / 6.73 6.62 / 7.49 6.64 / 7.58 6.70 / 7.39
USA total 5.45 / 6.28 5.46 / 5.90 6.73 / 7.43 5.66 / 6.21
Venezuela 6.12 / 7.00 6.23 / 6.66 7.08 / 7.72 6.20 / 6.86

Note. The mean ratings were significantly different between men and women in most cases.

between dieting and salt intake meant that dieting was not a po- could have an important influence on health over the longer
tential mediator of this effect. Controlling for dieting reduced term. Recent research has indicated that adoption and adherence
the odds associated with gender for avoiding fat (22% reduction to basic healthy eating guidelines is associated with signifi-
in OR), eating fiber (23% reduction), and eating fruit (7% reduc- cantly better health outcomes (70–74). Given that we also know
tion). As expected, there was no reduction in the OR for salt in- that dietary preferences tend to track over the lifespan (75), our
take. Controlling for health beliefs (Model 3) had stronger ef- results suggest that young men and women are on different di-
fects, eroding the odds favoring women’s healthier choices for etary trajectories, which may ultimately contribute to the 7-year
fat (31% reduction), fiber (38%), fruit (39%), and salt (87%). In differences in life expectancy.
the final model, both dieting and health beliefs were included. As predicted, the effects were strikingly consistent interna-
This further reduced the female advantage in healthy eating over tionally, with women being more likely than men to avoid fat
either one alone for fat (47% reduction in OR), fiber (53%), and and eat fiber in all 23 countries and to eat more fruit in 21 out of
fruit (43%), suggesting that dieting and health beliefs exert 23 countries. Differences in salt intake were less consistent but
largely independent effects. nevertheless showed a significant female advantage in six coun-
The results shown in Model 4 show that there are still sig- tries. The fact that similar patterns are seen across such disparate
nificant gender differences in three of the four food choices, cultures with varied cuisines indicates that gender differences in
even after health beliefs and dieting status have been taken into food choice behaviors cannot be attributed to any particular id-
account. This indicates that there must be other factors, not mea- iosyncrasy of a Euro-American research participant bias. This
sured in this study, which make men and women behave differ- study was not concerned with cross-cultural differences per se
ently in relation to food choices. but rather with whether gender differences were observed across
these different settings. However, it is worth noting that there
DISCUSSION were differences in both the average level of each reported food
The results of this study demonstrate the predicted gender choice and in the extent of the gender differences across coun-
differences in four food choice behaviors, confirming that in this tries, which might be an interesting focus for future research.
well-educated, young, healthy, and reasonably affluent group, The main objective of these analyses was to consider
men are less likely than women to report that they try to follow whether beliefs about the importance of each of the food choices
important healthy eating recommendations. Although the ef- or dieting status (a prominent aspect of gender differences in
fects are small, if they translate into nutritional differences, they eating behavior) “explained” the gender differences in food
TABLE 4
Age-Adjusted Odds (With 95% CI) of Reporting Healthy Food Choices in Relation to Gender, Dieting, and Health Beliefs
(Controlling for Age, Smoking Status, and Within-Country Clustering)

Multivariate Odds Ratios Multivariate Odds Ratios Multivariate Odds Ratios Including
Univariate Odds Ratios Including Gender and Dieting Including Gender and Beliefs Gender and Dieting and Beliefs

Avoiding high-fat foods


Gender (women vs. men) 2.44 (2.14–2.79) 2.12 (1.85–2.43) 2.00 (1.75–2.29) 1.77 (1.54–2.02)
Dieting (dieting vs. not dieting) 3.31 (2.43–4.51) 2.71 (1.99–3.69) 2.46 (1.81–3.33)
Beliefs (high vs. low beliefs) 4.52 (3.73–5.46) 4.10 (3.42–4.91) 3.99 (3.33–4.77)
Eating high-fiber foods
Gender (women vs. men) 1.80 (1.50–2.16) 1.62 (1.37–1.93) 1.50 (1.29–1.74) 1.38 (1.19–1.60)

112
Dieting (dieting vs. not dieting) 2.29 (1.82–2.88) 2.00 (1.63–2.45) 1.87 (1.50–2.34)
Beliefs (high vs. low beliefs) 6.08 (5.14–7.20) 5.81 (4.91–6.87) 5.73 (4.84–6.78)
Eating fruit at least daily
Gender (women vs. men) 1.56 (1.38–1.75) 1.52 (1.35–1.72) 1.34 (1.19–1.51) 1.32 (1.18–1.49)
Dieting (dieting vs. not dieting) 1.31 (1.12–1.53) 1.16 (0.99–1.34) 1.12 (0.96–1.30)
Beliefs (high vs. low beliefs) 3.11 (2.57–3.75) 2.98 (2.46–3.60) 2.96 (2.45–3.59)
Never adding salt to foods
Gender (women vs. men) 1.15 (1.04–1.28) 1.13 (1.02–1.26) 1.02 (0.91–1.14) 1.03 (0.92–1.15)
Dieting (dieting vs. not dieting) 1.06 (0.83–1.35) 1.02 (0.80–1.30) 0.96 (0.75–1.23)
Beliefs (high vs. low beliefs) 2.27 (1.82–2.82) 2.26 (1.80–2.84) 2.27 (1.81–2.85)

Note. CI = confidence interval.


Volume 27, Number 2, 2004 Gender and Healthy Food Choices 113

choice. The dieting hypothesis predicted that behavioral differ- Despite a reduction in the size of the gender difference after
ences would be greatest for fat and fruit—dieters often change controlling for dieting and health beliefs, women were still more
their intake of these foods—and that controlling for dieting likely than men to report healthier food choices, which means
would reduce this effect. The first part of this prediction was that other factors must play a part. One possibility is that women
partly supported in that the gender difference was greatest for are not only more concerned about health considerations (as in-
fat, with fruit and fiber close behind, and salt showing only a dicated by their higher endorsement of the belief items) but are
very small gender difference. The second part of the prediction also more likely to translate these attitudes to actions, perhaps
also got mixed support. We found that controlling for dieting re- being in a more advanced “stage of change.” Women’s rela-
duced the gender differences for fat and fiber intake but not the tionships with food is often represented as different from men’s
gender difference in fruit or salt, indicating that dieting partly (42,79). Even at young ages, girls are more likely to be responsi-
explains why women eat less fat and more fiber but not why they ble for family meal preparation or to buy foods than are boys.
eat more fruit or less salt. Young women may therefore use these skills to feed themselves
Beliefs in the importance of each aspect of food choice for more healthily.
health were comparatively high overall, and significantly higher Smoking was controlled for in these analyses because it of-
in women than men, as reported in other studies (42,76). They ten differs by gender and it has been related to poorer dietary
also showed striking international consistency, with significant habits (80,81). However, even taking smoking into account, the
gender differences for each belief for almost every country in relationships still held for all the food choice behaviors.
the sample. Gender difference in beliefs in the importance of There are limitations to the generalizability of these find-
healthy eating explained a substantial amount of the gender dif- ings. The study participants were all university students and the
ferences in food choices, eliminating the (modest) gender differ- patterning of gender differences may be different in older or less
ence in salt and considerably reducing women’s greater concern educated groups. Research on other populations will be needed
over fat and fiber. This effect was independent of the dieting mo- to address this point. The data were collected in a self-report sur-
tivation because the dieting and health belief effects together ex- vey, and therefore a social desirability bias may be present, as
plained more of the gender difference in food choices than either well as inflated consistency among factors measured at the same
one alone. These results are consistent with the view that part of time point. On the other hand, this should not bias the assess-
the reason that men report making less healthy food choices is ment of gender differences, which is the focus of this study. The
that health is a less important motivation to them in the food study was cross-sectional and this limits any causal interpreta-
domain. tions about interrelations between beliefs and behaviors. The
It has been suggested that men feel more ambivalent about range of food choice behaviors was very limited, and we had no
healthy dietary behaviors (77,78), but if that were so, there items on important behaviors such as limiting sugar or limiting
should have been a wider spread of belief scores in men than snacks. This is a drawback of using data from a wide-ranging
women, which was not the case in this sample. Courtenay (20) health behavior study, where the number of items relating to any
also argued that healthy eating might be stereotyped as a “femi- one behavior often has to be limited. We also lacked any specific
nine” practice, and therefore men who are concerned with con- evidence that participants’ reports of food choices were re-
firming their masculinity might not choose healthier foods even flected in real nutritional consequences. However, the aim of
if they knew that they should. Again this implies that not only this study was not to compare the diets of men and women as
should mean belief scores be lower in men, but variance should such but to examine their food choice behaviors. Trying to avoid
be greater. In our results, the confidence intervals (an index of fat or to eat higher fiber foods are the kinds of generalizations of
variability) for health beliefs were a similar width in men and motives for choosing foods, which are often included in instru-
women, which is not consistent with the argument that there are ments examining eating behaviors (67,68).
different subsets of men, some of whom subscribe to the stereo- Notwithstanding these limitations, the results contribute to,
type and some who don’t. Our results suggested that men as a and take forward, the issue of gender differences in health be-
group were simply less enthusiastic about the benefits of healthy haviors. Many other studies have noted gender differences in as-
eating. It is also worth noting that although men’s ratings of the pects of diet, and as expected, our results confirm these findings.
importance to health of dietary behaviors were lower than In addition, they add to the basic finding by replicating it in 23
women’s, they were not low in absolute terms. Contrary to the countries, many of which have rarely been featured in reports of
notion that men place little or no importance on healthy eating, health behaviors in the literature. If women report healthier
our findings suggest that men believe eating healthily is impor- practices (or at least attempt to make healthier choices) all over
tant to some degree, just not as strongly as women. To that ex- the world, then this suggests that any explanations for the differ-
tent, men in this sample did not appear to be signing up to ences are more likely to reflect underlying biobehavioral char-
Courtney’s (20) “socially prescribed male role,” in which men acteristics of men and women than local cultural effects. The
are supposed to have no interest at all in diet and nutrition. How- data on beliefs showed striking international consistency, indi-
ever, it is worth noting that this sample is composed mainly of cating that women across the world are more convinced than
well-educated and higher socioeconomic status (SES) respon- men that dietary choices are important—a phenomenon that it-
dents and it is possible that the gender differences might be more self demands further investigation. Are women predisposed to
striking in a sample with a wider range of SES. be more careful about what they eat, are they more risk averse in
114 Wardle et al. Annals of Behavioral Medicine

this field, or are they more likely to be the nutritional gatekeep- (15) Liang W, Shediac-Rizkallah MC, Celentano DD, et al.: A popu-
ers, taking on this role to an extent even before they take on re- lation-based study of age and gender differences in patterns of
sponsibility for feeding a family? health-related behaviors. American Journal of Preventive Med-
Given the important role of health behaviors in many of the icine. 1999, 17:8–17.
(16) MacGregor ID, Balding JW, Regis D: Flossing behaviour in
major causes of morbidity and mortality and the well-estab-
English adolescents. Journal of Clinical Periodontology. 1998,
lished gender differences in health, research into the causes of 25:291–296.
gender differences in health behaviors seems to have been ne- (17) Wardle J, Steptoe A: The European health and behaviour sur-
glected. In line with the Institute of Medicine’s report (6), we vey: Rationale, methods and results from the United Kingdom.
hope that these results will stimulate other researchers to go be- Social Science and Medicine. 1991, 33:925–936.
yond simply noting gender differences, to seeking to understand (18) Courtenay WH, McCreary DR, Merighi JR: Gender and ethnic
them, and to researching ways of encouraging men to take as differences in health beliefs and behaviors. Journal of Health
much advantage as women of strategies to promote long-term Psychology. 2002, 7:219–231.
health. (19) Whitfield KE, Weidner G, Clark R, et al.: Sociodemographic
diversity and behavioral medicine. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology. 2002, 70:463–481.
(20) Courtenay WH: Engendering health: A social constructionist
REFERENCES examination of men’s health beliefs and behaviors. Psychology
(1) World Health Organization. 1997–1999 World Health Statistics of Men and Masculinity. 2000, 1:4–15.
Annual, Table 3. Retrieved July 16, 2002 from (21) Hazan AR, Glantz SA: Current trends in tobacco use on
http://www3.who.int/whosis/whsa. prime-time fictional television. American Journal of Public
(2) U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS): Health. 1995, 85:116–117.
Deaths: Final data for 1998 (DHHS Publication No. [PHS] (22) Hazan AR, Lipton HL, Glanz SA: Popular films do not reflect
2000–1120). National Vital Statistics Reports. 2000, 48:11. current tobacco use. American Journal of Public Health. 1994,
Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. 84:998–1000.
(3) American Cancer Society: Cancer Facts and Figures—2001. (23) Signorielli N: Drinking, sex, and violence on television: The
Atlanta, GA: Author, 2001. Cultural Indicators perspective. Journal of Drug Education.
(4) Lawlor DA, Ebrahim S, Davey-Smith GD: Sex matters: Secular 1987, 17:245–260.
and geographical trends in sex differences in coronary heart dis- (24) Wallack L, Breed W, Cruz J: Alcohol on prime-time television.
ease mortality. British Medical Journal. 2001, 323:541–545. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1987, 48:33–38.
(5) Lowe LP, Greenland PH, Ruth KJ, et al.: Impact of major car- (25) Signorielli N: Health images on television. In Jackson LD,
diovascular disease risk factors, particularly in combination on Duffy BK (eds), Health Communication Research: A Guide to
22-year mortality in women and men. Archives of Internal Med- Developments and Directions. 1998, 163–179.
icine. 1998, 158:2007–2014. (26) Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Resnick MD, et al.: Lessons
(6) Institute of Medicine: Exploring the Biological Contributions learned about adolescent nutrition from the Minnesota Adoles-
to Human Health: Does Sex Matter? Washington, DC: National cent Health Survey. Journal of the American Dietetic Associa-
Academy Press, 2001. tion. 1998, 98:1449–1456.
(7) Kirkwood T: Time of Our Lives: The Science of Human Ageing. (27) Shimakawa T, Sorlie P, Carpenter MA, et al.: Dietary intake pat-
Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999. terns and sociodemographic factors in the atherosclerosis risk and
(8) Perls T: Genetic and environmental influences on exceptional communities study. Preventive Medicine. 1994, 23:769–780.
longevity and the AGE nomogram. Annals of the New York (28) Li R, Serdula M, Bland S, et al.: Trends in fruit and vegetable
Academy of Sciences. 2002, 959:1–13. consumption among adults in 16 US states: Behavioral Risk
(9) Waldron I: Contributions of changing gender differences in be- Factor Surveillance System, 1990–1996. American Journal of
havior and social roles to changing gender differences in mor- Public Health. 2000, 90:777–781.
tality. In Sabo D, Gordon D (eds), Men’s Health and Illness: (29) Beer-Borst S, Hercberg S, Morabia A, et al.: Dietary patterns in
Gender, Power and the Body. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, six European populations: Results from EURALIM, a collabo-
1995:163–179. rative European data harmonization and information campaign.
(10) Waldron I, Johnston S: Why do women live longer than men? European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2000, 54:253–262.
Part II. Journal of Human Stress. 1976, 3:19–29. (30) Patterson RE, Haines PS, Popkin BM: Health lifestyle patterns
(11) McGinnis JM, Foege WH: Actual causes of death in the United of U.S. Adults. Preventive Medicine. 1994, 23:453–460.
States. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1993, (31) Liebman M, Cameron BA, Carson DK, et al.: Dietary fat reduction
270:2207–2212. behaviours in college students: Relationship to dieting status, gen-
(12) Jackson R, Chambless L, Higgins M, et al.: Gender differences der and key psychosocial variables. Appetite. 2001, 36:51–56.
in ischemic heart disease and risk factors in 46 communities: (32) World Health Organization Health Policy for Children and Ad-
An ecologic analysis. Cardiovascular Risk Factors. 1998, olescents. Health Policy for Children and Adolescents, Series
7:43–54. No. 1. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization, 2000.
(13) Dean K: Self-care components of lifestyles: The importance of (33) Kennedy ET, Bowman SA, Powell R: Dietary-fat intake in the
gender, attitudes and the social situation. Social Science and US population. Journal of American College Nutrition. 1999,
Medicine. 1989, 29:137–152. 18:207–12.
(14) Kandrack MA, Grant KR, Segall A: Gender differences in (34) Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries and Food (UK): The dietary
health related behaviour: Some unanswered questions. Social and nutritional survey of British adults—Further analysis.
Science and Medicine. 1991, 32:579–590. London: HMSO, 1994.
Volume 27, Number 2, 2004 Gender and Healthy Food Choices 115

(35) Subar AF, Thompson FE, Kipnis V, et al.: Comparative valida- ders in Hong Kong. International Journal of Eating Disorders.
tion of the Block, Willett, and National Cancer Institute Food 1996, 20:77–84.
Frequency Questionnaires. American Journal of Epidemiology. (55) Kristal AK, Bowen DJ, Curry SJ, et al.: Nutrition knowledge,
2001, 154:1089–1099. attitudes and perceived norms as correlates of selecting low-fat
(36) Stephen AM, Sieber GM: Trends in individual fat consumption diets. Health Education Research. 1990, 5:467–477.
in the UK 1900–1985. British Journal of Nutrition. 1994, (56) Wardle J, Griffith J, Johnson F, et al.: Intentional weight control
71:775–788. and food choice habits in a national representative sample of
(37) Troiano RP, Briefel RR, Carroll MD, et al.: Energy and fat intakes adults in the UK. International Journal of Obesity. 2000,
of children and adolescents in the United States: Data from the 24:534–540.
National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys. American (57) Bourdieu P: Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of
Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2000, 72:1343S–1353S. Taste. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1984.
(38) Zive MM, Nicklas TA, Busch EC, et al.: Marginal vitamin and (58) Caplan P: Why do people eat what they do? Approaches to food
mineral intakes of young adults: The Bogalusa Heart Study. and diet from a social science perspective. Clinical Child Psy-
Journal of Adolescent Health. 1996, 19:39–47. chology and Psychiatry. 1996, 1:213–227.
(39) Furnham A, Kirkaldy B: Age and sex differences in health be- (59) Chaiken S, Pliner P: Women, but not men, are what they eat:
liefs and behaviours. Psychological Reports. 1997, 80:63–66. The effect of meal size and gender on perceived femininity and
(40) Wardle J, Steptoe A, Bellisle F, et al.: Healthy dietary prac- masculinity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1987,
tices among young Europeans. Health Psychology. 1997, 13:166–176.
16:443–450. (60) Pliner P, Chaiken S: Eating, social motive, and self-presentation
(41) Wardle J, Griffith, J: Socioeconomic status and weight control in women and men. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
practices in British adults. Journal of Epidemiology of Commu- ogy. 1990, 26:240–254.
nity Health. 2001, 55:185–190. (61) Rolls BJ, Fedoroff IC, Guthrie JF: Gender differences in eating
(42) Fagerli RA, Wandel M: Gender differences in opinions and prac- behavior and body weight regulation. Health Psychology. 1991,
tices with regard to a “Healthy Diet.” Appetite. 1999, 32:171–190. 10:133–142.
(43) Steptoe A, Pollard T, Wardle J: The development of a measure (62) Grogan SC, Bell R, Conner M: Eating sweet snacks: Gender dif-
of the motives underlying the selection of food: The Food ferences in attitudes and behaviour. Appetite. 1997, 28:19–31.
Choice Questionnaire. Appetite. 1995, 25:267–284. (63) Steptoe A, Wardle J: The European Health and Behaviour Sur-
(44) Steptoe A, Wardle J, Cui W, et al.: Trends in smoking, diet, vey: The development of an international study in health psy-
physical activity and attitudes to health in young adult Europe- chology. Psychology and Health. 1996, 11:49–73.
ans from 13 countries, 1990–2000. Preventive Medicine. 2002, (64) Steptoe A, Sanderman R, Wardle J: Stability and changes in
35:97–104. health behaviours in young adults over a one year period. Psy-
(45) Wardle J, Solomons W: Naughty but nice: A laboratory study of chology and Health. 1995, 10:155–69.
health information and food preferences. Health Psychology. (65) Schoenborn, CA: Health promotion and disease prevention:
1994, 13:180–183. United States, 1985. Vital Health Statistics, Series 10, 163.
(46) Bellisle F, Monneuse MO, Steptoe A, et al.: Weight concerns Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1988.
and eating patterns: A survey of university students in Europe. (66) Blaxter M: Health and Lifestyles. London: Tavistock, 1990.
International Journal of Obesity. 1995, 19:723–730. (67) Kristal AR, Shattuck AL, Henry HJ: Patterns of dietary behav-
(47) French SA, Jeffrey RW: Consequences of dieting to lose ior associated with selecting diets low in fat: Reliability and va-
weight: Effects on physical and mental health. Health Psychol- lidity of a behavioral approach to dietary assessment. Journal of
ogy. 1994, 13:195–212. the American Dietetic Association. 1990, 90:214–220.
(48) Serdula MK, Collins ME, Williamson DF, et al.: Weight control (68) Johnson F, Wardle J, Griffith J: The Adolescent Food Habits
practices of U.S. adolescents and adults. Annals of Internal Checklist: Reliability and validity of a measure of healthy eat-
Medicine. 1993, 119:6667–6671. ing behaviour in adolescents. European Journal of Clinical Nu-
(49) Gibbons KL, Wertheim EH, Paxton SJ, et al.: Nutrient intake of trition. 2002, 56:644–649.
adolescents and its relationship to desire for thinness, weight (69) Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator–mediator variable dis-
loss behaviours, and bulimic tendencies. Australian Journal of tinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic,
Nutrition and Diet. 1995, 52:69–74. and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social
(50) Jeffrey RW, Adlis SA, Forster JL: Prevalence of dieting among Psychology. 1986, 51:1173–1182.
working men and women: The healthy worker project. Health (70) Fung TT, Willett WC, Stampfer MJ, et al.: Dietary patterns and
Psychology. 1991, 10:274–281. the risk of coronary heart disease in women. Achives of Internal
(51) Roos E, Lahelma E, Virtanen M, et al.: Gender, socioeconomic Medicine. 2001, 161:1857–1862.
status and family status as determinants of food behaviour. So- (71) Hu FB, Rimm EB, Stampfer MJ, et al.: Prospective study of ma-
cial Science and Medicine. 1998, 46:1519–1529. jor dietary patterns and risk of coronary heart disease in men.
(52) Afifi-Soweid RA, Kteily MB, Shediac-Rizkallah MC: Preoccu- American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2000, 72:912–921.
pation with weight and disordered eating behaviors of entering (72) Liu S, Manson JE, Lee IM, et al.: Fruit and vegetable intake and
students at a university in Lebanon. International Journal of risk of cardiovascular disease: The Women’s Health Study.
Eating Disorders. 2002, 32:52–57. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2000, 72:922–928.
(53) Ford DA, Dolan BM, Evans C: Cultural factors in the eating (73) vanDam RM, Rimm EB, Willett WC, et al.: Dietary patters and
disorders: A study of body shape preferences of Arab students. risk for type 2 diabetes mellitus in U.S. men. Annals of Internal
Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1990, 34:501–507. Medicine. 2002, 136:201–209.
(54) Lee S, Leung T, Lee AM, et al.: Body dissatisfaction among (74) Walda IC, Tabak C, Smit HA, et al.: Diet and 20-year chronic
Chinese undergraduates and its implications for eating disor- obstructive pulmonary disease mortality in middle-aged men
116 Wardle et al. Annals of Behavioral Medicine

from three European countries. European Journal of Clinical (78) Sparks P, James R, Conner M, et al.: Ambivalence about
Nutrition. 2002, 56:638–643. health-related behaviors: An exploration in the domain of food
(75) Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI, et al.: Longitudinal tracking of choice. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2001, 6:53–68.
adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behav- (79) Rappoport L, Peters GR, Downey R, et al.: Gender and age dif-
iors. American Journal of Public Health. 1994, 84:1121–1126. ferences in food cognition. Appetite. 1993, 10:33–52.
(76) Zunft HJ, Friebe D, Seppelt B, et al.: Perceived benefits of (80) Burke V, Milligan RAK, Beilin LJ, et al.: Clustering of
healthy eating among a nationally-representative sample of health-related behaviors among 18-year-old Australians. Pre-
adults in the European Union. European Journal of Clinical ventive Medicine. 1997, 26:724–733.
Nutrition. 1997, 51:S41–S46. (81) Ma J, Betts N, Hampl JS: Clustering of lifestyle behaviors: The
(77) Povey R, Wellens B, Conner M: Attitudes towards following relationship between cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption,
meat, vegetarian and vegan diets: an examination of the role of and dietary intake. American Journal of Health Promotion.
ambivalence. Appetite. 2001, 37:15–26. 2000, 15:107–117.

You might also like