Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / l i n d i f

Daydreaming and its correlates in an educational environment☆


Sophie I. Lindquist ⁎, John P. McLean 1
School of Psychology, McElwain Building, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The experience of daydreaming is familiar to all, yet daydreaming and its correlates in an educational context
Received 12 May 2010 have yet to be adequately explored. This study investigated academic and other potential correlates of task-
Received in revised form 4 December 2010 unrelated images and thoughts (TUITs) during lectures. 463 undergraduate psychology students participated
Accepted 23 December 2010
across three lecture sessions. During lectures, an auditory probe was sounded five times; alerting participants
to record whether they were experiencing a TUIT at that moment. Results revealed significant negative
Keywords:
Daydreaming
correlations between TUIT frequency and age, detail of notes taken and course interest. Also, those seated in
Mind wandering the front third of the lecture experienced significantly fewer TUITs. Furthermore, as TUIT frequency increased,
Seating position there was a trend towards poorer performance in course examinations, which measure the learning of
Note taking lecture-based content.
Academic performance © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Thoughts unrelated to sensory input or the task at hand have been across laboratory and everyday life experiences (McVay, Kane, &
defined in a number of ways, including daydreaming (Singer, 1966), Kwapil, 2009). Furthermore, errors on the sustained attention to
task-unrelated images and thoughts (Giambra, 1995), task-unrelated response task (SART; see Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, &
thoughts (e.g. Filler & Giambra, 1973; Shaw & Giambra, 1993) and Yiend, 1997) are considered to be behavioural markers of day-
mind wandering (e.g. Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). Daydreaming is dreaming (e.g. Christoff, Gordon, Smallwood, Smith, & Schooler, 2009;
regarded as one of the most fascinating phenomena in the vast range Smallwood, Beach, Schooler, & Handy, 2008; Smallwood et al., 2004;
of human behaviour (Singer, 1975, 1976). Singer (1966) argues that Smallwood et al., 2009). Recently, Smallwood, Beach et al. (2008)
daydreaming represents a shift of attention away from some primary demonstrated a reduction in the P300 event-related potential for non-
physical or mental task or from a perceptual response to external targets prior to both behavioural and subjective reports of mind
stimulation towards a response to some internal stimulus. Consider- wandering relative to periods of being ‘on-task’. Furthermore,
able data has documented the apparent universality of daydreaming subjective reports of daydreaming are associated with activity in a
and the acknowledgement by large numbers of people that day- default network of cortical regions that are active when the brain is ‘at
dreaming is a general phenomenon in their daily lives (Klinger, 1987; rest’ (Mason et al., 2007) and more recently, activity in the default and
Singer, 1968, 2003) and generally reflect an individual's ‘current executive networks, two systems that have been assumed to work in
concerns’ (Klinger, 1987). opposition, suggesting that daydreaming may evoke a unique mental
The validity of subjective self-reports of daydreaming has been state allowing these systems to work in cooperation (Christoff et al.,
demonstrated in experimental studies. For example, daydreaming has 2009).
been found to vary as a function of stimulus presentation rate (e.g. Research has revealed a number of important demographic correlates
Antrobus, 1968; Antrobus, Singer, & Greenberg, 1966; Filler & related to daydreaming frequency. For instance, questionnaire and
Giambra, 1973; Giambra, 1995; Grodsky & Giambra, 1990–1991) laboratory studies of daydreaming have revealed that daydreaming
memory load (Smallwood et al., 2004; Teasdale et al., 1995) and mood frequency fluctuates as a function of age, such that as age increases,
induction (Seibert & Ellis, 1991; Smallwood, Fitzgerald, Miles, & frequency of daydreaming decreases (Giambra, 1974, 1977–1978, 1979–
Phillips, 2009). Individual differences in daydreaming also persist 1980, 1989, 1993, 1999–2000; Grodsky & Giambra, 1990–1991; Parks,
Klinger, & Perlmutter, 1988–1989). Several studies have found no gender
differences in daydreaming frequency (Giambra, 1989, 1995; Goldstein &
☆ This work has not been published elsewhere and has not been submitted Baskin, 1988; Singer, 1968; Singer & McCraven, 1961). However, in a
simultaneously for publication elsewhere. study by Giambra (1979–1980) involving 1200 participants, females at
⁎ Corresponding author. 8 Bendora Crescent, Palmerston, ACT 2913, Australia. every age group experienced significantly more daydreams and signifi-
Tel.: + 61 2 6240 2726.
E-mail addresses: s.lindquist@psy.uq.edu.au (S.I. Lindquist), john@psy.uq.edu.au
cantly more absorption in daydreaming than males.
(J.P. McLean). In terms of academic correlates, Wagman (1968) found that scores
1
Tel.: + 61 7 3365 6394. on the School and College Ability Test (SCAT) did not correlate with self-

1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.006
S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167 159

reported daydreaming frequency, however a strong negative correlation on responses where participants indicated zoning out revealed poorer
was observed for Grade Point Average (GPA; an averaged indicator of comprehension levels than the baseline performance of those
academic achievement over a range of courses within a fixed time participants who were randomly given text recognition probes,
period), but only for women. However, in a study by Gold, Andrews, and indicating that zoning out was associated with low levels of attention
Minor (1985–86), students were asked to record all school-related to the text (Schooler et al., 2004). Furthermore, on a final
daydreams experienced over a 14 day period. No differences were found comprehension test and intermittent recognition tests, negative
on the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) between students who experi- correlations between reading comprehension and both self-caught
enced five or more daydreams or less than five over the recording period, and probe-caught zone outs were observed. (Schooler et al., 2004).
however students recording five or more daydreams over the two week More recently, Smallwood, McSpadden, and Schooler (2008)
period had better GPAs at the end of the semester than students examined whether inattention during reading impairs the ability to
reporting less than five daydreams (Gold et al., 1985–86). These findings construct a mental model of ongoing events in a narrative, referred to as
indicate that daydreaming frequency may have important and poten- the “situation model of the narrative” (p 1144). Participants read a
tially complex implications in the educational domain. Sherlock Holmes novella, and provided reports of their mental state at
One reason why daydreaming may have implications within the both random and inference critical points (when a clue to the identity of
educational domain can be gleaned from experimental evidence the villain was revealed). Results revealed that the frequency of off-task
indicating that when daydreaming occurs, performance on a primary episodes was associated with poorer text comprehension (Smallwood,
task tends to suffer (McVay & Kane, 2009; McVay et al., 2009; Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2008). When awareness was taken into account,
Baracaia, Lowe, & Obonsawin, 2003; Smallwood, Obonsawin, & Heim, reports of off-task thoughts without awareness (zoning out) were a
2003; Smallwood et al., 2004). According to Smallwood and Schooler reliable predictor of text comprehension while reports with awareness
(2006) when the mind wanders, attention becomes divided between (tuning out) were not, indicating that the consequences of inattention
internal and external information, known as a decoupling of attention. during reading may depend on lacking awareness of being off-task
According to Smallwood and Schooler (2006) “mind wandering can be (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2008). It was also found that zoning out
viewed as a state of decoupled attention, because instead of monitoring prevented the retrieval of information from a specific interval in the text
online sensory information, attention shifts inward and focuses on one's and prohibited the linking of events to establish the identity of the
thoughts and feelings” (p 951). Smallwood, Fishman, and Schooler villain, with lapses occurring early in the narrative having the greatest
(2007) argue that “in principle, because mind wandering is a state of influence (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2008). Smallwood, McSpad-
decoupled attention, it represents a fundamental breakdown in the den et al. (2008) contend that these results confirm that zoning out
individual's ability to attend, and therefore integrate, information from during reading is an indication that the construction of a situation model
the external environment” (p 230). If attention is decoupled from the of the narrative has not been adequately formulated. These findings
task at hand during episodes of mind wandering, representations of the provide evidence that understanding ongoing events in everyday life
task may be less detailed than during periods of time when attention is depends on the ability to pay attention at critical points in time
focused on the task (Smallwood, O'Connor, Sudbery, & Obonsawin, 2007; (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2008).
Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). These studies provide evidence that daydreaming during reading
A recent review by Smallwood, Fishman et al. (2007) introduces interferes with text comprehension, suggesting that attention
the argument that mind wandering is a phenomenon of interest in the becomes decoupled and leads to superficial representations of the
educational domain. Smallwood, Fishman et al. (2007) argue that mind task. This is an important concept to investigate in a widely used
wandering is relevant to education because it represents a breakdown in teaching methodology — the lecture.
the normal coupling between the internal and external environments. An earlier study by Grodsky and Giambra (1990–1991) also
Smallwood, Fishman et al. (2007) contend that “mind wandering investigated daydreaming during reading. Participants were required to
prevents the successful encoding of information from the environment read passages of pre-determined high or low difficulty and high or low
and this relative absence of facts puts the individual at a disadvantage personal interest. The factor that determined TUIT frequency during
when forming the more general models needed for reading” (p 231). If reading was not passage difficulty, but interest: TUITs were more frequent
mind wandering interferes with the encoding of information, the when the text was uninteresting to participants (Smallwood & Schooler,
absence of factual information could lead to a disadvantage when 2006). This indicates that in the context of semantically meaningful tasks
formulating both the propositional and situational models required for a such as reading, rather than resource-driven factors such as task difficulty,
deep understanding of text (see Smallwood, Fishman et al., 2007). The interest appears to play an integral role in determining the frequency of
consequences of impaired encoding could “cascade downward through daydreaming (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006).
the cognitive system, so that simple deficits in superficial processing This study will address a gap in the literature by investigating the
could lead to more obvious deficits at a deeper level of analysis” academic and demographic correlates of daydreaming within a real
(Smallwood, Fishman et al., 2007, p 233). Evidence of this encoding world educational environment — the lecture. Previous research has
deficit can be found in an unpublished study by Schooler, Smallwood, investigated student attentiveness in classroom environments. Early
McSpadden and Reichle (2007; as cited in Smallwood, Fishman et al., work by Cameron and Giuntoli (1972) utilised consciousness
2007) where participants often failed to immediately notice when text sampling in college classes to investigate attentiveness in undergrad-
had turned to gibberish approximately 30% of the time, and continued uate classes. A bell was sounded at random intervals during the
reading for a significant number of words before coming to this classes, signaling students to respond to questionnaire items assessing
realisation (Smallwood, Fishman et al., 2007). Furthermore, probe- whether they were paying attention at that moment, as well as items
caught mind wandering was predictive of the likelihood of failing to requesting information about the nature of their thoughts. It was
detect when the text had become gibberish (Smallwood, Fishman et al., found that approximately 46% of students were paying attention at
2007). any given time during class, suggesting that the college classroom
Earlier work by Schooler, Reichle, and Halpern (2004) provides environment focuses less than half of the students' attention on the
insights into the impact of daydreaming on reading comprehension. content of the class (Cameron & Giuntoli, 1972). A more recent study
Participants read text and responded whenever they caught them- by Geerligs (1995) investigated the content of students' thoughts who
selves zoning out and were also randomly probed for zone outs. were engaged in problem-based small group discussions. Students
Zoning out was defined as a situation in which readers realised they were asked to report their thoughts at regular interruptions during
had no idea what they had just read and were thinking about the group discussion, signaled by an auditory probe. Thoughts were
something else altogether (see Schooler et al., 2004). Text recognition later classified into categories reflecting on-task and off-task thoughts.
160 S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167

Results revealed that 26% of students' thoughts were off-task during increases attention and results in greater concentration on the
the small group discussions, the majority of these regarding current material to be learned. Bligh (2000) further argues that note taking
concerns about daily matters (Geerligs, 1995). is an activity that maintains a slightly higher level of arousal because it
In a study of relevance to this investigation, Klinger (1984) is self-stimulating. Bligh (2000) supports this proposition by arguing
explored the inter-relationships between test anxiety, preparation, that enthusiastic, stimulating lectures often lead to increased note
thought content and academic performance during a college exam. taking. However, half of students surveyed by Hartley and Davies
During a course exam, students were signaled by auditory probe to (1978) reported that note taking sometimes interfered with their
stop work and record the content of their thoughts and respond to an understanding of the lecture content. Furthermore, studies have
item regarding their state anxiety. Thought content was classified by found that fewer notes are recorded as lectures proceed (Lloyd, 1968;
two independent judges into categories, including thoughts unrelated Scerbo, Warm, Dember, & Grasha, 1992).
to the exam (see Klinger, 1984). A non-significant negative associa- Benefits of handouts have also been reported. Northcraft and
tion was found between examination performance and the frequency Jernstedt (1975) found that the provision of lecture outlines resulted
of “other unhelpful to performance thoughts” (Klinger, 1984, p 1382). in better performance on subsequent objective examinations com-
While this study possesses the advantage of ecological validity and pared to full lecture transcripts. Furthermore, students who received
if taken at face value suggests that daydreaming frequency may have no handouts experienced the lowest levels of performance in the
no influence on academic performance, there are a number of examinations (Northcraft & Jernstedt, 1975). Furthermore, studies
methodological considerations that need to be taken into account. have found that the provision of skeletal handouts to students results
Firstly, this study took place during an exam, meaning the effects of in better performance on subsequent recall tests when compared to
daydreaming during the learning phase and the subsequent impact of standard handouts (Hartley, 1976) and full lecture transcripts
this on academic performance were not addressed. Importantly, due (Morgan, Lilley, & Boreham, 1988). A recent study also found that
to low frequencies of thoughts unrelated to the examination, this the provision of guided handouts resulted in significantly higher
category was added to a second category containing unhelpful outcomes on daily content quizzes when compared to no handouts
thoughts related to the exam, forming the other unhelpful to (Austin, Lee, Thibeault, Carr, & Bailey, 2002). It is possible that
performance thoughts category (Klinger, 1984). As such, this is not handouts, particularly incomplete or guided handouts, capture
a pure measure of thoughts unrelated to the exam, and accordingly student attention as vigilance is necessary in order to record
conclusions regarding daydreaming frequency and examination information omitted from these handouts.
performance cannot be drawn from this study. With reference to the literature reviewed, a number of potential
A potential shortcoming of these consciousness sampling studies is academic and demographic correlates of TUITs will be investigated.
the requirement of participants to report their thoughts in detail, which This study will investigate whether TUIT frequency differs as a
may have led to censorship as well as experimental demand function of age and gender, and whether Overall Position (OP) score, a
characteristics (Ericsson & Simon, 1980; Geerligs, 1995). In the present measure of high school level academic ability used to assess a
study, daydreaming will be operationalised as task-unrelated images student's suitability to study university courses in Queensland, is
and thoughts (TUITs), and will be measured using the probe-caught associated with TUIT frequency in first year university lectures. The
self-classification technique, in which individuals are interrupted during relationship between TUIT frequency and future academic achieve-
task performance and asked to report whether they were experiencing a ment will be investigated, indexed by examination marks, assignment
TUIT at the moment that the probe occurred, recorded by a yes/no marks and overall course marks. This study will also investigate
response (see Giambra, 1995; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). An whether student interest, as indexed by self-reported interest in the
advantage of this method is that individuals are not required to disclose course content and degree program, leads to a lower TUIT frequency.
the personal content of their thoughts, reducing the potential influence Finally, this study will explore the effect of seating position on TUIT
of demand characteristics (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). frequency, whether note taking is associated with a decrease in TUIT
While not directly examined in the daydreaming literature, factors frequency, and the effects of having lecture notes during the lecture
related to student learning have been found to influence achievement on TUIT frequency.
and are of interest to this investigation. Several studies have found that
academic achievement is influenced by seating position, with students
sitting towards the front of the room exhibiting higher levels of 1. Method
academic performance (e.g. Brooks & Rebeta, 1991; Stires, 1980).
However, other studies have found no effects (e.g. Millard & Stimpson, 1.1. Participants
1980; Wulf, 1977). Studies investigating the effects of seating position
on behaviours such as eye gaze, note taking and participation have Participants were 463 (138 males and 325 females) undergraduate
found that these behaviours are greatest among those sitting in the front psychology students attending an Introduction to Psychology lecture
or center of the room (Breed & Colaiuta, 1974; Levine, O'Neal, Garwood, on consciousness at the University of Queensland. The average age of
& McDonald, 1980; Sommer, 1969). Breed and Colaiuta (1974) argue the participants was 19.27 years with an age range of 17–53 years.
that all of these behaviours can be thought of as indicative of Eight participants were excluded from all further analyses due to not
attentiveness. The environmental hypothesis, which suggests that recording responses to all five TUIT probes. This study was conducted
students seated near the front of the room fall under the direct as part of an in-class demonstration of the empirical recording of
surveillance of the instructor and are obliged to pay attention out of daydreaming frequency, with data collection being incorporated into
courtesy, while students towards the back are more able to daydream or the lecture in a non-invasive manner. This study was designed to
talk, supports these findings (Stires, 1980). Potentially then, it is possible collect data from an in-class demonstration developed by John
that seating position may also influence student attention. McLean and run during his lecture on consciousness. Participants
Reviews of the note taking literature have revealed the beneficial gave informed voluntary consent to taking part in the study during
effects of note taking during lessons on recall (e.g. Bligh, 2000; Hartley the lecture. The data collection technique was designed to demon-
& Davies, 1978; Kiewra, 1985). The process function of note taking strate a method of collecting data on daydreaming, and does so by
suggests that the activity of note taking is beneficial independent of indexing the frequency of self-reported daydreaming. Due to the large
review, because it increases attention during the lecture and number of students enrolled in this course, the one lecture on
facilitates encoding of lecture ideas into long term memory (Kiewra, consciousness was repeated three times, enabling data to be collected
1985). For instance, Peper and Mayer (1978) claim that note taking from three individual groups of students.
S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167 161

1.2. Materials handouts completely blank, or if they specifically did not want their data
included in the academic performance analyses to not fill in their
1.2.1. TUIT frequency recording sheet student number on the demographic sheet. Access to students' course
This measure consisted of a table with five sections for each probe on results was only granted by the inclusion of a student number on the
which participants were able to report whether they had experienced a demographic sheet.
TUIT (see Appendix). On this sheet, participants were able to record Following this discussion, the lecturer began the lecture. During
whether or not they had been experiencing a TUIT at the moment that the fifty minute lecture, at the specific intervals of 8, 15, 25, 34 and
the probe occurred by circling either YES or NO. Instructions of how to 40 min, the auditory probe was sounded. When the probe sounded,
recognise a TUIT were provided on this measure: participants recorded by dichotomous yes/no response whether at the
moment that the probe occurred they had been experiencing a TUIT or
During the course of this lecture, you may find yourself thinking
attending to the material being presented. Following each probe,
about things other than the material being presented by your
participants were given 15 s to record their responses. The lecturer
lecturer. These thoughts may simply pop into your head, or you may
would then resume the lecture. At the completion of the lecture,
choose to think about something other than the lecture content. In
participants returned both measures to the experimenters. At the
this demonstration, we are interested in both of these instances of
completion of the academic semester, participants who provided their
daydreaming. For the purposes of this demonstration, daydreaming is
student number, thereby giving permission for access to their course
defined as any thoughts or images that are experienced throughout
results, had their data included in the analysis of the relationship
the lecture that are not related to the course material being presented
between academic achievement and TUIT frequency. In order to
during the lecture. Examples of daydreams include ‘What will I have
directly assess the effects of TUIT frequency on lecture content, scores
for dinner?’, ‘I wonder what my friends are doing now?’ and ‘I hope
from mid-semester and final exams, based solely on lecture content,
the bus home isn't too crowded’.
were combined and weighted, with the same procedure applied to
assignment marks for the purposes of comparison. Total course mark
and overall course grade were also included in the academic analyses.
1.2.2. Demographic sheet
Written response items requested included age, gender, OP score 2. Results
(typically ranging from 1 to 15), and degree program being studied
(see Appendix). OP scores are Queensland University entry ranking 2.1. Demographic analyses
scores and run in reverse numerical order, such that 1 represents the
highest possible academic ranking, 2 represents the second best Descriptive statistics for the demographic analyses are displayed in
ranking and so forth. Likert scale response items addressed interest in Table 1.
the lecture series and degree of note taking. Examples of these items The association between TUIT frequency and the independent
include ‘How interesting have you found John McLean's lectures and variables of age, course interest, note taking and OP score were
cognitive psychology so far?’ with responses ranging from (1) not examined via a non-parametric Kendall's Tau statistic. Significant
interesting to (5) extremely interesting and ‘How many notes on average negative associations were observed between TUIT frequency and age
do you record in addition to those provided on your lecture notes, or τ = −.110, p = .004, course interest τ = −.114, p = .005, and note
overall if you do not have lecture notes?’ with responses ranging from taking, τ = −.172, p b .001. These findings indicate that TUIT frequen-
(1) no notes to (5) very detailed notes. Option-based response items cy decreases as age, course interest and note taking increase.
requested information regarding seating position in the lecture Furthermore, a positive association was found between TUIT
theatre and whether notes had been printed out prior to the lecture. frequency and OP scores, τ = .094, p = .017, suggesting that as OP
Examples of these items include ‘Have you downloaded and printed the scores increase (indicating lower levels of academic ability), TUIT
lecture notes in advance for today's lecture (Yes/No)?’ and ‘Approxi- frequency also tends to increase. Course interest was also found to
mately where are you sitting in the lecture theatre (front third, middle have a positive association with both age, τ = .080, p = .045, and note
third, back third)?’ This sheet also included an option for students to taking, τ = .144, p = .001. These findings indicate that as age increases,
provide their student number if they wished for their data to be used interest in the course content also tends to increase and as course
in the academic performance analyses. interest increases, more notes are taken during lectures.
A t-test for independent groups was conducted in order to test the
1.3. Procedure possibility that TUIT frequency may differ as a function of gender.
Analyses revealed that on average, males experienced TUITs more
The undergraduate Introduction to Psychology: Physiological and frequently (M = 1.73, SE = .107) than females (M = 1.61, SE = .067).
Cognitive Psychology lecture on the topic of consciousness took place However, this difference was not significant t (461) = 1.012, p = .312
during the seventh week of semester 1, 2008 at the University of (equal variances assumed) and represents a very small effect r = .047.
Queensland, Australia. This was the seventh lecture in the series of To determine whether student interest in course content as indexed
twelve lectures for this course. The participants had recently completed by degree program leads to differences in TUIT frequency, a t-test for
their mid-semester examination for this course. Participants entered the
lecture theatre and were given a demographic sheet and a TUIT Table 1
frequency recording sheet. Once seated, participants were asked to Descriptive statistics for the demographic analyses.
complete the demographic sheet. Following this, participants were Mean Standard Standard TUITs
oriented to the topic of the lecture and then debriefed regarding the deviation error of
Percent
nature of the demonstration and informed of how to recognise when a mean
TUITing
TUIT occurs by the lecturer. Participants were informed that an alarm
TUIT frequency 1.64 1.222 .057 Overall 32.9
would sound around five times during the lecture, and when they heard
OP score 5.61 3.412 .174
this alarm, to report whether they were experiencing a TUIT at the Course interest 3.49 .710 .033 Probe 1 25.1
moment that the alarm occurred. Following this a demonstration of the Note taking 3.11 .808 .038 Probe 2 35.9
alarm was provided. Participants were not informed of when the Probe 3 27.9
auditory probes would be sounded. Participants were also informed that Probe 4 31.7
Probe 5 43.8
if they did not wish to take part in the demonstration, to leave all
162 S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167

independent groups was conducted. On average, there was very little Table 2
difference in TUIT frequency between psychology (M = 1.64, SE= .176) Descriptive statistics for the academic performance analyses.

and non-psychology majors (M = 1.65, SE= .059), t (85.333) = −.036, Mean Standard Standard TUITs
p = .971 (equal variances not assumed) with virtually no effect r = .004. deviation error of
Percent
A t-test for independent groups was conducted to discern whether mean
TUITing
having lecture notes during the lecture led to differences in TUIT
TUIT frequency 1.61 1.200 .066 Overall 32.2
frequency. On average, those with lecture notes (M = 1.58, SE= .069) OP score 5.41 3.266 .193
reported fewer TUITs than those without lecture notes (M = 1.80, Course interest 3.51 .693 .038 Probe 1 25.1
SE= .101).2 This difference did not reach significance, t (456) = 1.829, Note taking 3.08 .808 .044 Probe 2 36.5
p = .068 (equal variances assumed), and represents a very small effect Weighted exam mark 50.242 7.001 .383 Probe 3 24.9
Weighted assignment mark 27.604 4.202 .230 Probe 4 29.3
r = .085. Final exam mark 52.988 8.770 .480 Probe 5 44.9
A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted in order to Weighted total mark 77.846 9.630 .527
determine whether seating position within the lecture theatre during a Course grade 5.35 .927 .051
lecture has an effect on TUIT frequency. A significant effect of seating
position on TUIT frequency was found, F (2, 458)= 10.539, p b .001,
r = .210. Post hoc comparisons using the Gabriel method for slightly 1974, 1977–1978, 1989, 1993, 1999–2000; Grodsky & Giambra, 1990–
unequal sample sizes revealed that those seated in the front third of the 1991; Parks et al., 1988–1989), particularly the findings of Giambra
lecture theatre (M = 1.30) experienced significantly fewer TUITs than (1979–1980), which indicated that age groups separated by only a few
those seated in the middle third (M = 1.69, p = .010) and in the back years exhibit this relationship. While the association between TUIT
third (M = 1.95, p b .001). However those seated in the middle third did frequency and age was just outside of significance in the academic
not differ significantly from those seated in the back third. performance analyses, this can be attributed to the 28% reduction in
sample size and corresponding reduced variation in student age and does
2.2. Academic performance analyses not invalidate the significant negative association observed in the
demographic analyses. Giambra (1989) suggested that the existence of
There were 334 participants (99 males and 235 females; average this relationship may be due to TUIT likelihood being proportional to the
age = 19.19, age range 17–53) who gave permission for their data to amount of non-conscious processing in the cerebral cortex or in the brain
be included in the academic performance analyses. A further 15 as a whole (Giambra, 1989). With this in mind, the finding of this
participants were excluded from this analysis due to not having relationship within a lecture environment indicates that non-conscious
completed all assessment items for the course. One more participant processing may begin to decline as early as in the mid 20s. However, this
was excluded from the following analyses due to incorrect data entry. theory was tested using a wide range of ages, from late teens to the
Descriptive statistics for the academic performance analyses are elderly (e.g. Giambra, 1989; Grodsky & Giambra, 1990–1991). In the
displayed in Table 2. lecture environment however, the majority of students are in their late
The association between TUIT frequency and academic perfor- teens, with the majority of mature age students not representing ages
mance was investigated via a non-parametric Kendall's Tau statistic proportionate to that of the main student cohort, or of these previous
and is displayed in Table 3. studies. An alternative explanation of these findings is that older students
Significant negative associations were observed between TUIT may have more invested in their degree, or have more dedication to their
frequency and weighted exam mark, final exam mark, weighted total studies than younger students who have just left school, thus tend to
mark, and overall course grade. These results indicate that as TUIT focus more on the lecture content. This explanation is somewhat
frequency during lectures increases, there is a trend towards poorer supported by the positive association between age and course interest
performance in terms of the final exam, and mid-semester and final observed in this study. However, further research into this assumption is
exams combined, all of which measure the learning of lecture-based important in further understanding the mechanisms operating behind
content. Furthermore, a greater TUIT frequency during lectures also the inverse relationship between TUIT frequency and age.
results in a trend towards a lower total course mark and overall course The positive association between TUIT frequency and OP score in this
grade. Interestingly, TUIT frequency had a non-significant negative study contradicts previous studies demonstrating no relationship
association with weighted assignment mark, indicating that the effects between TUIT frequency and measures of academic ability (Gold et al.,
of TUIT frequency on academic performance were exclusive to 1985–86; Wagman, 1968). However, Gold et al. (1985–86) relied on the
assessments based on lecture content. For weighted exam mark, the recording of daydreams over a two week period, while the study by
only other significant correlation worth noting other than TUIT Wagman (1968) involved a questionnaire requesting ratings of how
frequency is the negative association observed with OP. Neither course frequent daydreams related to a number of motivational and interper-
interest, nor note taking had significant associations with weighted sonal aspects of behaviour were experienced by participants. These
exam mark. In contrast to the demographic analyses, the correlations approaches can be inaccurate due to factors such as forgetting and
between TUIT frequency and age and TUIT frequency and OP score were reconstruction errors and censorship (Geerligs, 1995). By using the
just outside of significance (p = .058; p = .059, respectively). extensively validated probe method that does not require the reporting
of the contents of consciousness, the present study may have captured an
3. Discussion effect that these prior studies were not able to tap in to. As a higher OP
score indicates poorer academic ability, this result suggests that increased
The purpose of this study was to investigate the demographic and TUIT frequency is related to lower levels of academic ability. This
academic correlates of daydreaming within an ecologically valid lecture relationship was just outside of significance in the academic performance
environment. The finding of an inverse association between TUIT analyses, however as previously mentioned this can be attributed to the
frequency and age is in agreement with previous research (e.g. Giambra, reduced sample size in comparison to the demographic analyses.
It is important to note however, that OP score is not only ranked on
the individual student's ability, but also on the ability of all students
2
The anonymous reviewers raised the possibility that having/not having lecture within their school and district. Therefore, OP score is not a holistic
notes may reflect differences in student interest, and thus represent a confounding
variable in this analysis. A t-test for independent groups confirmed that there was no
reflection of an individual's academic ability. Furthermore, not all
effect of having/not having lecture notes on student interest, t (453) = .091, p = .928, participants reported OPs, which is likely to be due to different
r = .004. Australian states using different university entry score calculation
S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167 163

Table 3
Correlation matrix of TUIT frequency and academic variables.

TUIT Weighted exam Weighted assignment Weighted total Final exam Grade Age OP Course Note
frequency mark mark mark mark interest taking

τ
TUIT frequency 1.000 −.136⁎⁎ −.056 −.119⁎⁎ −.119⁎⁎ −.134⁎⁎ −.084 .087 −.143⁎⁎ −.164⁎⁎
N 334 334 334 334 334 334 333 286 331 332
Weighted exam mark 1.000 .316⁎⁎ .755⁎⁎ .880⁎⁎ .734⁎⁎ .040 −.405⁎⁎ .038 .017
Weighted assignment mark 1.000 .568⁎⁎ .312⁎⁎ .607⁎⁎ .004 −.307⁎⁎ −.127⁎⁎ −.067
Weighted total mark 1.000 .730⁎⁎ .838⁎⁎ .038 −.426⁎⁎ −.027 −.012
Final exam mark 1.000 .714⁎⁎ .057 −.398⁎⁎ .018 .007
Grade 1.000 .038 −.449⁎⁎ −.036 −.033
Age 1.000 .066 .127⁎⁎ −.060
OP 1.000 .090 .038
Course interest 1.000 .139⁎⁎
Note taking 1.000
⁎⁎ Significant at the .01 level (2-tailed).

systems, compounded by international students enrolled in first year The effects of seating position on student attention as postulated by
psychology. While administering a standardised test of academic ability Breed and Colaiuta (1974) have been confirmed in the present study.
to participants directly prior to a similar study could be of value, the Students seated in the front third of the lecture theatre experienced
potentially onerous and impractical nature of such a test could make it significantly fewer TUITs than those seated elsewhere. This adds support
difficult to undertake within a lecture setting without interfering with to the environmental hypothesis, where students sitting in the front
the ecological validity of the study. Therefore, the influence of academic portion of the lecture theatre are under the direct observation of the
ability on TUIT frequency is in need of further investigation. lecturer, and thus are more obliged to be attentive (e.g. Stires, 1980).
The lack of a significant gender difference is in conflict with the This explanation is in agreement with a number of studies indicating
findings of Giambra (1979–1980), where females at every age group that seating location influences participation (e.g. Levine et al., 1980;
experienced significantly more daydreams and significantly more Montello, 1988), but up until now, no study has found evidence that
absorption in daydreaming than males. In the present study the inverse seating location has an impact on student attention during lectures.
trend was present: males daydreamed slightly more frequently than The significant negative association between TUIT frequency and
females. However, the study by Giambra (1979–1980) was retrospective note taking is in agreement with a number of authors who have
in nature; therefore it is possible that the greater levels of absorption in postulated this mechanism, but have not provided direct evidence in
daydreaming for females may lead to better recall of daydreaming support of this function (e.g. Bligh, 2000; Kiewra, 1987; Peper &
experiences in retrospective questionnaires. Furthermore, the lack of a Mayer, 1978). This finding also provides further support to the
gender difference is in agreement with a number of retrospective studies process function of note taking, whereby the activity of note taking is
(e.g. Goldstein & Baskin, 1988; Singer, 1968; Singer & McCraven, 1961) beneficial independent of review, because it increases attention
and laboratory studies (Giambra, 1989, 1995). However, the gender split during the lecture and facilitates encoding into long term memory
in this study was very uneven, with more than two-thirds of participants (Kiewra, 1985). The decrease in TUIT frequency observed with more
being female. It is possible that with a larger number of males, a detailed note taking suggests that the activity of note taking requires
significant difference may emerge. Indeed, Giambra's (1979–1980) study students to focus their attention on the lecture in order to record the
involved 1200 participants. However if a sample as large as that of key points. Furthermore, Piolat, Olive, and Kellogg (2005) argue that
Giambra's (1979–80) is required to detect a significant difference, then the time urgency of selecting key points and recording them while
the value of any observed gender effect in a practical sense is trivial and also comprehending new information places significant demands on
inconsequential. In light of this possibility and findings of previous the central executive and other components of working memory. This
research, it does not seem likely that gender has any reliable effect on suggests that note taking as a complex activity may not increase
TUIT frequency. attention to the lecture content per se, but rather redirect resources to
In the present study student interest was indexed by degree program the manipulation of information in working memory. Therefore, it
and self-reported interest in the course content. The observed negative would appear that one method of curtailing student inattention
association between course interest and TUIT frequency is suggestive of during lectures is through the active encouragement of students to
an effect similar to that observed by Grodsky and Giambra (1990–1991), participate in note taking during the lesson.
however the strength of this effect is marginal, and must be interpreted Although this effect did not reach significance, the finding of reduced
with caution. It is possible that the five item measure used was not TUIT frequency in students who had lecture notes is intriguing. The
sensitive enough to capture the influence of course interest on TUIT handouts provided in this study consisted of the lecturers' PowerPoint
frequency. No difference between those enrolled in a psychology degree slides, and as such a lot of information was delivered in the lecture that
program with those not enrolled emerged, possibly suggesting that in a was not included in these handouts. While several studies have found
lecture environment student interest has no effect on TUIT frequency. performance benefits associated with skeletal and guided handouts (e.g.
However, degree program may not be an accurate reflection of students Austin et al., 2002; Hartley, 1976; Morgan et al., 1988; Northcraft &
who are engaged in psychological studies. For instance, the Bachelor of Jernstedt, 1975), this is the first study to provide some evidence to
Arts and Bachelor of Science degree programs offer majors in suggest that the provision of lecture handouts may help students
psychology, and students engaged in these degree programs would maintain attention to the lesson, possibly by requiring vigilance in order
not have been captured by the measure in the present study. to record important information omitted from such handouts.
Furthermore, the sample sizes of the two groups were vastly uneven, This study provides evidence that TUIT frequency during lectures is
with only seventy participants being enrolled in a psychology degree reduced when: i) the course content is interesting to the student, ii)
program. If any differences do exist, the sheer number of students who students are seated at the front of the lecture theatre, iii) students are
were not enrolled in psychology degree programs may have suppressed actively engaged in note taking and iv) students have a copy of the
any potential effect. Therefore, it is possible that interest has an influence lecture notes. In a study of task-unrelated thought while encoding
on TUIT frequency, but at this stage the results remain equivocal. information, Smallwood, Baracaia et al. (2003) found that when
164 S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167

participants were instructed to encode categorically organized lists of While the association between TUIT frequency and weighted exam
words for future retrieval, a lower frequency of task-unrelated thought mark is of a relatively small magnitude, it needs to be placed in the
was observed relative to a random list condition and a categorical correct context. Lecture content in this course was examined by
condition in which participants were instructed to merely shadow the recognition-based multiple choice exams. Students also had the
words. This led Smallwood, Baracaia et al. (2003) to postulate that the opportunity to revise prior to these exams. With these factors in
effects of stimulus organization on the frequency of task-unrelated mind, the fact that a significant negative association still exists suggests
thought be considered as an interaction between the presentation of a that in the lecture environment daydreaming may indeed lead to a
stimulus set providing an “affordance” (p 456) for information to be decoupling of attention, leading to poorer representations of the lecture
readily encoded and whether active engagement with the task is material and resulting in poorer performance on lecture-based assess-
required. More recently, Smallwood, McSpadden, and Schooler (2007) ments. Would this effect be more pronounced if there was no
investigated the influence of mind wandering on encoding during a task opportunity for revision and if assessment was recall-based? An
where participants were required to encode words for future retrieval investigation is currently underway to examine this proposition
and detect targets with either high or low probability. Of particular (Lindquist & McLean, in preparation).
interest, it was found that high probability targets increased the A number of limitations within the present study are worthy of
automatic aspects of memory (retrieval via familiarity), indicating that scrutiny. To begin with, in order to conduct an ecologically valid
the frequent interruptions provided by the targets provided exogenous investigation of TUIT frequency within a real lecture environment,
support to attention by enabling participants to sustain their attention elements of experimental control were forfeited. However, despite
on the task (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2007). This can be this trade-off the findings of the present study provide important
conceptualised as an affordance, as it provided a task environment in insights into the academic and educational correlates of TUIT
which the features of the external environment mitigated the negative frequency, and set the agenda for further investigation. Furthermore,
consequences of mind wandering (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2007). imposing experimental conditions upon a lecture environment is not
Interest, seating position, note taking and lecture notes can potentially be only impractical, but would also severely limit the ecological validity
viewed as affordances in the lecture environment. Interest in the content of the investigation. While in principle this investigation was
of the lecture focuses student attention, suppressing TUITs and ecologically valid, it is possible that the demonstration itself may
potentially creating an affordance so that the lecture content can be have influenced the number of TUITs experienced, perhaps by making
readily encoded and understood. Furthermore, sitting towards the front participants more attuned to these episodes or even having
of the lecture theatre places the student directly in view of the lecturer, participants spend time thinking about the concept of TUITs.
increasing attentiveness (Breed & Colaiuta, 1974; Stires, 1980), while However, this demonstration has been conducted over a number of
lecture handouts afford students with a guide to the organization of the years during the consciousness lecture, and there has been no
lecture. In addition, the activity of note taking requires students to be indication or feedback from students that this has inflated the
actively engaged during the lecture, again suppressing TUITs by focusing frequency of TUITs, and some suggestion that the self-monitoring
attention on the task at hand and potentially facilitating the encoding of stemming from TUIT measurement may reduce TUITs (J. McLean,
information (Kiewra, 1985). These findings provide strategies to personal communication, 15 October, 2010). It is possible that the
educators that have the ability to reduce the frequency of TUITs within timing of the consciousness lecture during the semester may have had
the classroom, focusing student attention on the content of the class and an effect on exam performance; however this is not likely given that
also having an influence on academic outcomes. Further research into students were able to revise the course content prior to the final exam.
the effects of affordance and active engagement within the classroom The present study provides valuable insights into the educational
and whether these are contingent upon one another in both ecologically correlates of TUIT frequency, and an obvious direction for future
valid and simulated classroom settings would be of benefit. research is the investigation of other factors that may influence
The significant negative associations observed between TUIT student attention within the lecture environment, particularly
frequency and weighted exam mark, final exam mark, total course alertness (e.g. Versace, Cavallero, De Min Tona, Mozzato, & Stegagno,
mark and overall course grade suggest that increased TUIT frequency 2006) and caffeine consumption (e.g. Smith, Sturgess, & Gallagher,
during lectures has a negative impact on academic performance. Of 1999) in order to rule out these potential influences on the relation-
particular importance is the negative association observed between ships observed in this study.
TUIT frequency and weighted exam mark, an academic performance
measure based solely on the lecture content. What makes this
association even more fascinating is the lack of association between 3.1. Summary and future directions
TUIT frequency and weighted assignment mark, which suggests that
TUIT frequency during lectures has a selective effect on material The purpose of the present study was to investigate TUIT frequency
learned during lectures only. This finding adds support to the within an ecologically valid educational setting. A number of important
contention that when TUITs occur attention becomes decoupled associations were discovered, all of which have important implications
between the internal and external environments leading to superficial for current educational practices. Further studies should attempt to
representations of the task at hand (Smallwood, Fishman et al., 2007; conduct similar studies within other educational environments; such as
Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), and is also in agreement with the in tutorials, discussion groups, and even within schools, in order to fully
contention of Smallwood, Fishman et al. (2007) that TUITs have examine the impact of attentional lapses across all educational realms.
important implications in education due to the deficits in encoding Further studies should also attempt to disentangle some of the more
associated with TUITs and the absence of factual information that pertinent findings, such as the effect of note taking on TUIT frequency
results from this. These deficits in encoding may be due to a failure in and the impact of TUIT frequency on the learning of lecture-based
the ability to create and maintain a situation model of the lecture content, within a controlled laboratory environment in order to provide
narrative (Smallwood, McSpadden et al., 2008), leading to poorer a more solid basis upon which to justify the design of educational
academic outcomes. The findings of this study also extend on the environments to counteract the effects of attentional lapses on
reading comprehension work of Schooler et al. (2004), and Small- classroom learning. While some of the individual findings reported
wood, McSpadden et al. (2008) by demonstrating that in real world here may seem intuitive, the value of undertaking an empirically sound,
educational environments, increases in TUIT frequency have a quantitative examination of attention within the educational domain
negative impact on the learning and comprehension of information provides the potential to enhance the quality of teaching and learning
from lectures. environments.
S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167 165

Acknowledgements occur. When you hear the alarm, please indicate on this sheet whether at
the exact moment that the alarm occurred you were experiencing a
The authors would like to thank the three anonymous reviewers for daydream by circling YES. If when you heard the alarm you were not
their comments and ideas on earlier drafts of the manuscript. The authors experiencing any thoughts or images unrelated to the material being
would also like to thank the students of the University of Queensland presented and were paying attention to the lecture, please indicate by
enrolled in PSYC1020, semester 1 2008, for being good sports and circling NO. Information regarding the specific content of your thoughts
participating in this study. is not required. If you have questions please raise your hand.

Appendix A. Consciousness: In-class demonstration of daydream


frequency recording Probe Daydream experienced

Probe 1 YES/NO
During the course of this lecture, you may find yourself thinking about Probe 2 YES/NO
things other than the material being presented by your lecturer. These Probe 3 YES/NO
Probe 4 YES/NO
thoughts may simply pop into your head, or you may choose to think
Probe 5 YES/NO
about something other than the lecture content. In this demonstration,
we are interested in both of these instances of daydreaming. For the
purposes of this demonstration, daydreaming is defined as any thoughts
or images that are experienced throughout the lecture that are not Participation in this demonstration is voluntary. If you do not wish
related to the course material being presented during the lecture. to take part, simply leave your handouts blank. In addition, with your
Examples of daydreams include ‘What will I have for dinner?’, ‘I wonder permission the results will be used in the PhD research of Sophie
what my friends are doing now?’ and ‘I hope the bus home isn't too crowded’. Lindquist. For research purposes, it would be valuable to know some
Today's demonstration involves the daydream self-classification of the correlates of daydreaming, so if you could please fill out the
probe method, a method that has the ability to index the frequency of demographic sheet in addition to this recording sheet we will be able
self-reported daydreaming. This method involves the instruction of what to investigate these potential correlates. If you specifically do not want
constitutes a daydream, the sounding of auditory probes (e.g. a beep or to be included in analyses involving examination scores and course
alarm) during the lecture and the recording of whether a daydream was grades, please leave the student number field on your demographic
being experienced at the point in time when the probe sounded. On this sheet blank. If you take part in the demonstration but at the end of the
sheet you are able to record whether you are daydreaming or not lecture decide that you no longer wish to participate, do not return
whenever you hear the alarm during the course of this lecture. A total of your handouts at the end of the lecture. Please be advised that the
around five alarms will sound during this fifty minute lecture. However, coordinators of PSYC1020 will not be able to identify you from the
you will have no prior knowledge of the times at which these alarms will data recorded in this demonstration.

Consciousness: In-class Demonstration of Daydream Frequency Recording


Demographic Information Sheet

1. Age:.....................................

2. Gender:................................

3. Degree Program (e.g. B.A., B.Sc., B.Psy.Sc.):........................................

4. Overall Position (OP) Score (or alternative entry score):.......................................

Please circle your response for the following questions

5. How interesting have you found John McLean’s lectures and cognitive psychology so far?

1 2 3 4 5

(not interesting) (slightly interesting) (moderately interesting) (very interesting) (extremely interesting)

6. Have you downloaded and printed the lecture notes in advance for today’s lecture? Yes / No.

7. How many notes on average do you record in addition to those provided on your lecture notes, or overall if you do
not have lecture notes:

1 2 3 4 5

(no notes) (minimal notes) (moderate notes) (detailed notes) (very detailed notes)

8. Approximately where are you sitting in the lecture theatre?

Front third Middle third Back third

9. Student Number (optional: see below):.........................................................

(8 digit number as shown on student card)


166 S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167

In addition to the above potential correlates, it will be interesting Levine, D. W., O'Neal, E. C., Garwood, S. G., & McDonald, P. J. (1980). Classroom ecology:
The effects of seating position on grades and participation. Personality and Social
to see if academic performance correlates with daydreaming Psychology Bulletin, 6, 409−412.
frequency. If you wish to contribute to this analysis, please include Lindquist, S.I. & McLean, J.P. (n.d). Mind wandering during lessons: A study of the
your student number in the field above. All analyses will be conducted consequences of inattention during classroom learning on recognition and recall
performance, and the role of note taking. Manuscript in preparation.
after data is de-identified, that is, student numbers will be replaced Lloyd, D. H. (1968). A concept of improvement of learning response in the taught
with anonymous numerical labels. Thus, your identity will be lesson. Visual Education, 5, 23−25.
completely anonymous. Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N.
(2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent
(Please be aware that by providing your student number [please thought. Science, 315, 393−395.
provide student number as shown on your student card, i.e. the number McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory
should be 8 digits long and should not have an ‘s’ at the beginning], you capacity, goal neglect and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196−204.
are allowing permission to access your examination and overall course
McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2009). Tracking the train of thought from the
grade. Be advised that the PSYC1020 course coordinators will not have laboratory into everyday life: An experience-sampling study of mind wandering across
the ability to identify you by the information you provide on this controlled and ecological contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 857−863.
demographic sheet). Millard, R. J., & Stimpson, D. V. (1980). Enjoyment and productivity as a function of
classroom seating location. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 439−444.
Montello, D. R. (1988). Classroom seating location and its effect on course achievement,
participation and attitudes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 8, 149−157.
References Morgan, C. H., Lilley, J. D., & Boreham, N. C. (1988). Learning from lectures: The effect of
varying the detail in lecture handouts on note-taking and recall. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 2, 115−122.
Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus-independent thought. British Northcraft, G. B., & Jernstedt, G. C. (1975). Comparison of four teaching methodologies
Journal of Psychology, 59, 423−430. for large lecture classes. Psychological Reports, 36, 559−606.
Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., & Greenberg, S. (1966). Studies in the stream of Parks, C. W., Klinger, E., & Perlmutter, M. (1988–1989). Dimensions of thoughts as a function
consciousness: Experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous of age, gender, and task difficulty. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 8, 49−62.
cognitive processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 399−417. Peper, R. J., & Mayer, R. E. (1978). Note taking as a generative activity. Journal of
Austin, J. L., Lee, M. G., Thibeault, M. D., Carr, J. E., & Bailey, J. S. (2002). Effects of guided Educational Psychology, 70, 514−522.
notes on university students' responding and recall of information. Journal of Piolat, A., Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2005). Cognitive effort during note taking. Applied
Behavioral Education, 11, 243−254. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 291−312.
Bligh, D. A. (2000). What's the use of lectures? New York: John Wiley & Sons. Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). "Oops!":
Breed, G., & Colaiuta, V. (1974). Looking, blinking, and sitting: Nonverbal dynamics in Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured
the classroom. Journal of Communication, 24, 75−81. and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747−758.
Brooks, C. L., & Rebeta, J. L. (1991). College classroom ecology: The relation of sex of Scerbo, M. W., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., & Grasha, A. F. (1992). The role of time and
student to classroom performance and seating preference. Environment and cuing in a college lecture. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17, 312−328.
Behavior, 23, 305−313. Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2004). Zoning out while reading:
Cameron, P., & Giuntoli, D. (1972). Consciousness sampling in the college classroom or Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. In D. T.
is anybody listening? Intellect, 101, 63−64. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience (pp. 204−226). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.
sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and
to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, cognitive performance. Memory and Cognition, 5, 507−513.
8719−8724. Shaw, G. A., & Giambra, L. (1993). Task-unrelated thoughts of college students
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, diagnosed as hyperactive in childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 9, 17−30.
215−251. Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner
Filler, M. S., & Giambra, L. M. (1973). Daydreaming as a function of cueing and task experience. New York: Random House.
difficulty. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 37, 503−509. Singer, J. L. (1968). The importance of daydreaming. Psychology Today, 1, 18−26.
Geerligs, T. (1995). Students' thoughts during problem-based small-group discussions. Singer, J. L. (1975). The inner world of daydreaming. New York: Harper & Row Publishers.
Instructional Science, 22, 269−278. Singer, J. L. (1976). Daydreaming and fantasy. London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd.
Giambra, L. M. (1974). Daydreaming across the lifespan: Late adolescent to senior Singer, J. L. (2003). Daydreaming, consciousness, and self-representations: Empirical
citizen. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 5, 115−140. approaches to theories of William James and Sigmund Freud. Journal of Applied
Giambra, L. M. (1977–1978). Adult male daydreaming across the life span: A Psychoanalytic Studies, 5, 461−483.
replication, further analyses, and tentative norms based on retrospective reports. Singer, J. L., & McCraven, V. (1961). Some characteristics of adult daydreaming. Journal
International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 8, 197−228. of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 51, 151−164.
Giambra, L. M. (1979–1980). Sex differences in daydreaming and related mental Smallwood, J. M., Baracaia, S. F., Lowe, M., & Obonsawin, M. (2003). Task-unrelated
activity from the late teens to the early nineties. International Journal of Aging and thought whilst encoding information. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 452−484.
Human Development, 10, 1−34. Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain:
Giambra, L. M. (1989). Task-unrelated-thought frequency as a function of age: A Mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive
laboratory study. Psychology and Aging, 4, 136−143. Neuroscience, 20, 458−469.
Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O'Connor, R., et al. (2004).
stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Experimental Gerontology, 28, 485−492. Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement
Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657−690.
attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, Smallwood, J., Fishman, D. J., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). Counting the cost of an absent mind:
1−21. Mind wandering as an underrecognized influence on educational performance.
Giambra, L. M. (1999–2000). Frequency and intensity of daydreaming: Age changes and Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 230−236.
age differences from late adolescent to the old–old. Imagination, Cognition and Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, K. L., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods,
Personality, 19, 229−267. wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9,
Gold, S. R., Andrews, J. C., & Minor, S. W. (1985–86). Daydreaming, self-concept and 271−276.
academic performance. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 5, 239−247. Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one's
Goldstein, J., & Baskin, D. (1988). Sex differences in daydreaming behavior. Journal of home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders.
Mental Imagery, 12, 83−90. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 527−533.
Grodsky, A., & Giambra, L. M. (1990–1991). The consistency across vigilance and Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The
reading tasks of individual differences in the occurrence of task-unrelated and task- curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1144−1150.
related images and thoughts. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 10, 39−52. Smallwood, J., O'Connor, R. C., Sudbery, M. V., & Obonsawin, M. (2007). Mind-
Hartley, J. (1976). Lecture handouts and student note-taking. Programmed Learning and wandering and dysphoria. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 816−842.
Educational Technology, 13, 58−64. Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M., & Heim, D. (2003). Task-unrelated-thought: The role of
Hartley, J., & Davies, I. K. (1978). Note-taking: A critical review. Programmed Learning distributed processing. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 169−189.
and Educational Technology, 15, 207−224. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132,
Kiewra, K. A. (1985). Investigating notetaking and review: A depth of processing 946−958.
alternative. Educational Psychologist, 20, 23−32. Smith, A., Sturgess, W., & Gallagher, J. (1999). Effects of a low dose of caffeine given in
Kiewra, K. A. (1987). Notetaking and review: The research and its implications. different drinks on mood and performance. Human Psychopharmacology Clinical
Instructional Science, 16, 233−249. and Experimental, 14, 473−482.
Klinger, E. (1984). A consciousness-sampling analysis of test anxiety and performance. Sommer, R. (1969). Personal space. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1376−1390. Stires, L. (1980). Classroom seating location, student grades, and attitudes: Environment or
Klinger, E. (1987). The power of daydreams. Psychology Today, 21, 37−44. self-selection? Environment and Behaviour, 12, 241−254.
S.I. Lindquist, J.P. McLean / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 158–167 167

Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., et al. Wagman, M. (1968). University achievement and daydreaming behavior. Journal of
(1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Counseling Psychology, 15, 196−198.
Memory & Cognition, 23, 551−559. Wulf, K. M. (1977). Relationship of assigned classroom seating area to achievement
Versace, F., Cavallero, C., De Min Tona, G., Mozzato, M., & Stegagno, L. (2006). Effects of variables. Educational Research Quarterly, 2, 56−62.
sleep reduction on spatial attention. Biological Psychology, 71, 248−255.

You might also like