Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Psychopathology of Childhood Social Phobia

DEBORAH C. BEIDEL, PH.D., SAMUEL M. TURNER, PH.D ., AN D TRACY L. MORRIS, PH.D.

ABSTRACT

Objective: To describe the clinical syndrome of social phobia in preadolescent children. Method: Fifty children with DSM-
IV social phobia were assessed with semistructured diagnostic interv iews, self-report instruments, parental and teacher
ratings, a behavioral assessment, and daily diary recordings . In addition, the behaviors of these children were compared
with those of a sample of normal peers. Results: Children with social phobia had a high level of general emotional over-
responsiveness, social fear and inhibition, dysphoria, loneliness, and general fearfulness . Sixty percent suffered from a
second , concurrent disorder. Socially distressing events occurred quite frequently and were accompanied by maladaptive
coping behaviors . In addition, children with social phobia had significantly poorer social skills. There were few differences
based on gender or race. Conclusions: Children with social phobia suffer pervasive and serious functional impairment. In
addition, the clinical presentation suggests specific avenues for psychosocial interventions. J. Am . Acad. Child Ado/esc.
Psychiatry, 1999, 38(6):643-650. Key Words: social phobia , gender, race, comorbidity, functional impairment.

Although shyness and social isolation have been studied imately 1%. However, the rate may rise dramatically be-
by developmental and social psychologists, onl y during cause of the diagnostic revisions in DSM-IV(APA, 1994).
the past 2 decades have clinical researchers addressed the Kendall and Warman (1997) reported that 18% of their
nature and treatment of maladaptive social fears. Social clinic sample met DSM-I/l-R criteria for social phobia,
phobia was first included in the DSM-JII (American whereas 40% of the same sample met DSM-IV criteria.
Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980) and its revision , Similarly, if rates for DSM-IJI-R social phobia and
DSM-/lI-R (APA, 1987). Neither publ ication restricted avoidant disorder were combined (e.g., McGee et al.,
this diagnosis to adults, but social phobia rarel y was 1990) , the population prevalence rate for social phobia
diagnosed in children. One probable reason was that the would be 3% to 4%. Therefore, a significant number of
child and adolescent section of the diagnostic manuals chiJdren are affected by this disorder,
listed 2 very similar conditions. Avoidant disorder of Studies describing the clinical features of childhood
childhood described children with social-evaluative fears social phobia and comparisons with peers without psy-
and sometimes, social avoidance. Overanxious disorder chiatric disorders are virtu ally nonexistent. Beidel (1991)
also contained criteria that were social-evaluative in reported that children with DSM-JII-R social phobia
nature. Because of the substantial symptom overlap, could be differentiated from normal peers on the basis of
children with social fears received various diagnostic higher trait anxiery, lower perceptions of cognitive com-
labels (e.g., Beidel, 1991; Francis et al., 1992). petence, higher distress ratings during a behavioral task,
Figures based on DSM-/lI-R criteria placed the popu- higher ratings of daily social distress, and negative coping
lation prevalence of social phobia in children at approx- strategies. Although Beidel (1991) highlighted some impor-
tant considerations, to date there are no studies addressing
DSM-IV childhood social phobia.
Accepted November 4, 1998.
This study examined the clinical syndrome of DSM-
Drs. Beidel and Turner are with the M aryland Ce nter fOr Anxiety Disorders,
Department of Psychology. University ofMaryland, College Park. Dr. Morris is IV social phobia in preadolescent children and com-
with the Department of Psychology. ~st Virginia University. Morgantown. pared aspects of their functioning to that of children
This research was supported in part by NIMH grant MH53 703 . without psychiatric disorders. This study also examined
Reprint rrqllests to Dr. Beidel, Maryland Center fOr An xiety Disorders,
racial or gender differences in clinical presentation. In
Department of Psychology. University of Maryland, College Park. M D 2 0742.
0890-85 67/99/3806-0643 ©199 9 hy the American Academ y of C hild addition to traditional di agnostic interviews and self-
and Adolescent Psychiatry. report inventories, a direct assessment of social behavior

J . AM. A CA D , C H I L D A D O LE SC. P SYC H IAT RY. '>8 : (, . J U N E 1999 643


HEID EL ET AI..

was undertaken. No study (Q date has assessed directly dentl y rated by a seco nd clin ician . The result ant I( coefficient was
0.85 for the diagn osis of social phob ia.
social interaction skills, even though social skills deficits
Self-Report Inventories. C hild ren completed the followin g instru-
have been reported in adults with generalized social pho- ments. The C hildren's Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1985 ) assesses the
bia (Turner et al., 1994) and in socially isolated children presen ce and severity of depressed mood . The Junior Eysenck Per-
(e.g., Rubin et al., 1990) . sonality Inventory (EPI) (Eyscnck and Eysenck, 1968) measures neurot-
icism , extro version, and psych ot icism. This study used th e first 2
subscales. Neuroti cism is defined as general overresponsiveness to events
METHOD and situations, and Extroversion assesses outgoing. uninhibited, impul-
sive, and sociable tendencies. T he Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory
Subjects for C hildren (SPAI-C) (Beidel et al., 1995) assesses potent ially fearful
social encounters, includi ng ph ysiological, cognitive, and behavioral
The patient sample con sisted of 50 child ren referred for treatment
components of social phob ia. The Loneliness Scale (Asher and Wh eeler.
of social phobia to the Med ical Un iversity of South C arolin a's
1985) assesses feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction.
Anx iety Prevention and Treatment Research Center. Children were
Parental and Teacher Rating Scales. Parents of children with social
referred by parents, school cou nselor s, or other clinicians, or th eir
phobia completed the C hild Behavior C hecklist (CBCL) (Achenbach,
parents responded to announccrnenr s regarding free treatment fo r
1991) . In addition, the child's teacher completed the Teacher's Report
"shy" child ren. Mean age was 10.1 years (range 7-13 years) and chil-
Form (TRF) (Achenbach, 199 I) . In most cases, teachers were un a-
dr en were enrolled in the 2nd throu gh 8th grade. There were 33
ware of the specific treatment progr am, but in some cases th ey knew
white child ren (66%). 14 African -American children (28 % ), I bira-
th at the ch ild was part of a resear ch study. Although no t ever y
cial child (white and African- Amer ican; 2%) , and 2 Hi span ic chil-
teacher partic ipated, th ose who d id (11 = 28) were reimbursed $5 .00.
dren (4%) . Seventy percent were from the middle class accordi ng to
Behauioral Assessment. T he behavio ral assessment evaluated skill
the Hollingshead Inde x of Social Position . T here were 22 boys (44 %)
and anxiety in 2 tasks: social interaction s (role-play task) and read ing
and 28 girls (56%). All were enro lled in regular classroom sett ings
aloud before a small audie nce (performa nce task). O rder of task pre-
and were of normal intelligence. Mean IQ score was 103 as estimated
sentatio n was random ized. For th e read-aloud task. th e child read
by the Block Design and Vocabul ary subsectio ns of th e WIS C-Ill
aloud the story of Jack and the Bean sralk for 10 minutes (Beide l and
(Wech sler, 1991) .
Turn er, 1988). The audience included a same-age peer and 2 young
T he normal sample consisted of 22 child ren recruited as role-play
ad ults. The social int eracti on task con sisted of 5 role-play sce nes
confederates and "peer helpers" in a social phobia behavioral treatment
requiring int eraction with a same-age peer. including ( I) carrying on
program . Fifteen of these child ren had served as normal controls in a
a co nve rsatio n, (2) giving a co mpliment , (3) graciously receiving
previou s study assessing risk facto rs for anxiety (Beidel and Turn er,
anot her's offer of help . (4) receiving a compliment. and (5) request-
1997 ). C hi ld ren were interviewed with th e Sched ule for Affect ive
ing ano ther to change negative beh avior. The role-pla y partner was a
Disorders and Schiwphren ia for Schoo l-Age C hild ren modified fo r
peer of the same age and gende r selected from the normal co ntrol
anxiety d isord ers (Last, 198 6 ). Non e met d iagnostic criteria for any
pool describ ed abo ve. Peers received training, an d the ir respo nses
d isorder. The remain ing 7 children d id not part icipate in a semistruc-
were standard ized using "cue cards."
tur ed interv iew but were interv iewed clinically by the first aut hor. All
T he assessment was videot aped and rated for skill o n as-poi nt
parents den ied the prescncc of any psychiatric d isorder in the children,
Likert scale (1 = not at all effective; , =very effective) by raters unaware
and no child had ever been in treatm ent . All children were enrolled
of the child 's classificat ion . Perceived anxiety was rated o n a 4-po int
in regular classroom settings and none had academic difficulti es. The
scale (I = not at all an xiou s and 4 = severely anxious) . Twen ty-five
average age was 11.7 years (range 9- 14 years). There were 7 boys
percent of the videotapes were rated by a second rater, also un aware
(32% ) and 15 girls (68%); 16 (73% ) were white, 4 (18%) were African-
of the child's diagnosis. Int errater reliabiliries (Pearson correlation
Ameri can, and 2 (9%) were Hi span ic; the majority (75%) were from
coefficients) ranged from r = 0.80 to r = 0.94. In addition . speech
the middle class. There were no significant group differences on any
lat ency (i.c ., time length to resp ond to the peer's prompt ) was
dem ographic variable. Furtherm ore , there were no differences
betwe en normal controls who received a diagnostic interv iew and assessed. Finally, children rated thei r d istress using a '-point Likert
scale, where I = extremel y anxious and 5 = completely relaxed.
th ose who did not on any demograph ic o r dependent variables.
Daily Diary Ratings. For 2 weeks. each ch ild completed a dail y
d iary whi ch listed pot entially fearful social situatio ns and possible
Assessment respon ses (e.g., see Beidel er al., 1991 ). Because more than on e event
Semistructured Interview. C hild ren with social phobia and their could occur per day. child ren could com plete more th an on e d iary
parent s were interviewed by I of th e first 2 aut ho rs using the Anxiety entry per day. Events listed included performing in front of a group.
Di sorders Interv iew Sched ule for C hild ren (AD IS-C ) (Silverman interacting with a teacher or peer, eati ng in front of oth ers, and using
and Albano , 1996) to document th e presen ce of social ph obi a and a publi c rest room . Respon ses included various types of avoidance .
add itional diagnoses, including depression , anxiety disorder s, atten- presence of ph ysical symptoms. co mpliance with the task. and posi-
tion-deficit/hyperactivi ty disorder, co nd uct disorder, psychoti c d is- tive coping skills. The diary included an "other" categ or y allowing
o rde rs, and selec ti ve muti sm . T he ADI S-C uses DSM -I V (APA, additional listings. In add ition . child ren rated their distress using a 5-
1994) criteria and lists potentially socially fearful situa tions that arc point Likert scale. Ch ildren were rewarded with fast-food gift certifi-
rated o n a 5-poi nt Likert scale. Kendall (19 94) and Kend all and cates for diary completio n. Because not all child ren recorded for the
Sout ha m-G erow ( 1996) have reported interrarer reliability (I() coeffi- ent ire 14 days (mean = 12.1 days) . the percentage of days du ring
cients for the ADI S-C anxiety d isorde rs categories ranging from 0.85 whi ch a d istre ssing event occ urred was calculated o n th e basis of
to 1.0. In the current study, d iagnosis was determ ined by the clini- number of days recorded (number of events/number of days recorded).
cian on the basis of infor mation provided by both ch ild and parent . C hi ld ren with di agn osed socia l phob ia completed th e ent ire
Twenty-five percent of the interviews were videotaped and inde pen - assessment. Those with out a d iagnosis (no rmal peers) com pleted th e

644 J . AM. ACAD. CHILD AD O LESC . PSYCHIATRY. cIH:6. J UN E 19 9 9


C H I L D H O O D SO CIAL PHOBIA

TABLE 1 the number of situations endorsed, only 11 % of the


Types of Social Situations Feared by C hild ren children were classified as having the specific subtype of
With Social Phobia (n = 50)
social phobia, consistent with other data indicating that
0/0 Endorsing at
most children exhibited a pervasive pattern of social dis-
Situatio n Least Moderate Distress
tress (Beidel and Turner, 1988).
Reading aloud in front of the class 71 Table 1 lists the percentage of children reporting at
Musical or athletic performances 61
Joining in on a conversation 59 least moderate distress (rating of 2 or higher on the 0-4
Speak ing to ad ults 59 scale) for each ADIS-C situation. Performing in front of
Starring a conversation 58 others (reading aloud in front of the class, and music or
Writing on the blackboard 51
athletic performances) was most frequently identified,
Ordering food in a restaurant 50
Attending dances or activity nights 50 followed by general conversational interactions (speak-
Taking tests 48 ing to adults, initiating or joining in on peer conversa-
Parties 47 tions). Less frequently endorsed situations included having
Answering a question in class 46
Working or playing with other children 45 one's picture taken, eating in front of others, answering
Asking the teacher for help 44 or talking on the telephone, and walking in the hallways/
Physical edu cation class 37 hanging out by the locker. These data illustrate a situ-
Group or team meetings 36
Having picture taken 32 ational continuum that creates social distress. Although
Using school or public bathrooms 24 just the presence of others may elicit distress for some
Inviting a friend to get together 24 children, the general requirement to interact or perform
Eating in the school cafeteria 23
in public creates substantial distress for the majority of
Walking in the hallway!
hanging out at the lockers 16 children with this disorder.
Answering or talking on the telephone 13 Consistent with the ADIS-C situations, daily diary data
Eating in front of others 10
also identify a broad pattern of social distress (Table 2).
Dating NA
The "o th er" category was most commonly endorsed
Note: NA = not applicable.
TABLE 2
Frequency of Distressful Events and Children's Responses (n = 50)
EPI and the SPAI-C. In addition . 19 normal chi ld ren co mpleted
the behavioral assessment . These children completed the assessment 0/0 of Events!
before they were trained as role-play partners. Responses
Endorsed

RESULTS Event
I had to perform in front of others
Comparisons of social-phobic children and the nor- (sing , dance, play an instrument) 17
mal peers were conducted using t tests. When normal The teacher called on me to answer a question 15
comparison data were not available, statistical analyses A popular kid spoke to me 13
I had to talk to so meo ne on the telephone II
were conducted using l-sarnple t tests (SPSS, 1996). I had to eat in a public place 9
I had to use a public restroom 6
Social Anxiety and Distress I had to work with a popular kid in class 4
Other 27
Children with social phobia reported substantial dis- Response
tress across many social situations including (1) public Did what I was supposed to do 37
performances (reading or reciting in front of others, per- Pret ended I was sick. so I would not have to go 14
Cried 11
forming in a play, or playing soccer) and (2) ordinary social Waited to go to the bathroom until I got home 9
interactions (starting conversations, joining in on con- Told myself not be nervous, it would be OK 6
versations, talking to adults, or answering the telephone). Got a stomach ache or headache 6
Pretended I didn't hear the pcrson talking to me 4
DSM-IVallows for the assignment of subtypes of social Refused to do what I was asked 3
phobia. The generalized subtype is assigned when chil- Did not go to the place (baseball game)
dren experience distress across a broad range of social so I would not have to do it 3
Hid my eyes so I was not called on I
encounters. The specific subtype is used when fears are
Other 9
limited to only a few social situations. On the basis of

J. AM. ACA D . CHIl.D ADOLES C. PSYCHIATRY. .\8:6 , J UN E 1999 645


BEIDEL ET AL.

(27%), and it included unstructured social interactions candy higher than that reported by a normal sample (2.2;
(asking the teacher a question or introducing oneself to t49 = 3.39,p < .05, I-sarnple z test) (Beidel et al., 1991).
an unknown peer). Other situations included perform-
Social and Performance Skills of Children With Social Phobia
ing in front of a group (17%), answering questions in
class (15%), and speaking to a popular child (13%). Children with social phobia often are described by
These data illustrate an important point. Although per- their parents as "loners." Parents and teachers report that
formance situations appear to be the most universally they often do not play with others at recess, ostensibly
distressful, general social interactions are the situations preferring to sit alone and engage in solitary activities.
most frequently encountered. During a clinical interview, one boy stated, "all I want is
With respect to frequency, daily diary data reveal that one friend." These clinical reports were confirmed by
socially distressful events occur often (Table 2). Overall, significant group differences on the EPI Extroversion
79% of the diary entries recorded a distressing event. subscale (social phobia mean = 12.1 [SO = 4.9] versus
Thus, approximately 5 distressful events occurred every normal mean = 21.4 [SO = 2.7]; t64 = 9.66,p < .0005).
7 days, significantly higher than that recorded by a nor- In addition, expressions of loneliness were supported by
mal sample (mean = 2.2, as reported by Beidel et al., significantly higher scores on the Loneliness Scale when
1991; t49 = 3.16,p < .05, I-sample r test). compared to the mean score of "average" children (as
Finally, children with social phobia are aware that their reported by the scale's authors; mean = 40.9 [SO = 11.1]
social distress is outside of what is considered "normal" for versus mean = 29.9 [SO = 8.0]' respectively; t49 = 6.76,p <
the situation. For example, despite a strong desire, one .0005, I-sample r test),
boy was only able to participate in a school play when all Socially withdrawn children have poor social skills
the characters wore masks "because then they could not (Rubin et al., 1990). Compared with normal peers, chil-
see how badly I blush." Such clinical descriptions of high dren with social phobia were rated as less skilled (t61 =
social distress are confirmed by significant group differ- 5.91,p< .0005) and more anxious (t61 = 1O.81,p< .0005)
ences on the SPAI-C total score (social phobia mean = when reading aloud (Table 3). During the social skills
25.8 [SO = 10.4] versus normal mean = 4.2 [SO = 4.1]; task, they were also less interpersonally skilled (t62 = 7.70,
t69 = 12.55, P < .0005). In addition, children with social P < .0005) and more anxious (t62 = 9.83, P < .0005).
phobia rated themselves as more anxious during both the Finally, they had significantly longer speech latencies
read-aloud task (t60 = 4.22, P < .005) and the social skills (t62 = 4.57, P < .0005).
task (t61 = 4.75, P < .0005) (Table 3). Finally, daily diary
Functional Limitations as a Result of Social Phobia
data also affirm higher distress. The mean anxiety rating
for distressful events was 3.4 (on a scale of 1-5), signifi- The loneliness data suggest that social phobia affects
friendship patterns and impairs other aspects of func-
tioning. On the AOIS-C interview, 75% reported no or
TABLE 3 few friends, 50% were not involved in any extracurricu-
Performance on the Behavioral Assessment Task
lar or peer activities, 50% reported that they did not like
Social Phobia Normal Controls
(n = 19)
school, and 10% refused to attend school regularly.
(n = 50)
Daily diary data also confirm the impact on daily func-
Rating Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p
tioning. Approximately 35% of the distressing events
Read-aloud task resulted in use of an avoidance strategy (pretended not
Effectiveness" 2.4 (0.9) 4.0 (1.0) .0005
to hear the person talking to them, hid their eyes so they
Anxiety" 2.9 (0.9) 1.2 (0.4) .0005
Self-rating' 3.1 (1.4) 4.3 (0.8) .0005 would not be called on, pretended to be sick, refused to
Role-play scenes do as they were asked, did not go to the distressing place,
Effectiveness 1.9 (0.8) 3.8 (0.8) .0005
waited to go to the bathroom until at home).
Anxiety 3.0 (0.8) 1.4 (0.5) .0005
Speech latency 4.6 (3.1) 1.4 (1.2) .0005
Presence of Concurrent Diagnoses
Self-rating 3.1 (1.4) 4.5 (0.9) .0005
Sixty percent of children with social phobia had a
" Higher ratings indicate greater effectiveness.
b Higher ratings indicate greater anxiety. secondary Axis I diagnosis (Table 4). The majority
C Higher ratings indicate less anxiety. (36%) were anxiety disorders. Generalized anxiety dis-

646 J. AM. ACAD. CHIl.D ADOl.ESC. PSYCHIATRY..~8:6. JUNE 1999


CHILDHOOD SO C IAL PHOBIA

TABLE 4 icantly different from that of the normative group (t28 =


Concurrent Diagnoses in a Sample of Social-Phobic Children 6.31, P < .000 5, I-sample t test). In both cases, the
Secondary Diagnosis % scores represent elevated, but not marked, anxiety and
None 40 depression.
Generalized anxiety disorder 10 As noted, daily diary data indicate that children some-
Arrenrion-deficir/hyperactiviry disorder 10 times avoid or exhibit oppositional behavior when feel-
Simple phobia 10
ing anxious (Table 2). However, in general children with
Selective mutism 8
Separation anxiety disorder 6 social phobia are not often perceived by parents or teachers
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 6 as having behavior problems. On the CBCL Behavior
Depre ssion 6 Problems subscale, the mean T score was 65.4 (SO =
Panic disorder 2
Adjustment disorder with anxious and depressed mood 2 9.3), significantly different from the 50th percentile
(t49 = 6.54, P < .0005, I-sample t test). On the TRF
Behavior Problems subscale, the mean T score was 57.6
order (10%), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (SO = 9.8), again significantly different from the 50th
00%), and specific phobia 00%) were most common. percentile (t28 = 4.30, P < .0005; l-sarnple t test).
Selective mutism was diagnosed in 8% of the sample. Few children met criteria for externalizing disorders
Even when diagnostic criteria are not met, children 00%), consistent with parental and teacher reports. On
with social phobia have symptoms of general anxiety the CBCL Externalizing subscale, the mean T score was
and many specific fears. Overall, 87% of children with 58.5 (SO = 9.9), significantly different from the norma-
social phobia endorsed at least one nonsocial situation as tive group (t49 = 5.98, P < .0005, l-sample t test). Simi-
moderately distressful (Table 5). Fears of injections (51%) larly, on the TRF Externalizing subscale, the mean score
and blood tests (35%) were most common, followed by for the social-phobic group was 54.6 (SO = 8.7) , again
fears of high places (30%), blood (28%) , and the dark significantly different from the 50th percentile (t29 =
(23%). In many instances, the fear did not impair daily 3.01, P < .01, l-sample t test). Even though there were
functioning, thus not warranting a specific phobia diag- statistically significant differences, the lack of externaliz-
nosis. However, the high prevalence indicates that these ing diagnoses suggests that despite the occasional pres-
children also suffer distress in nonsocial situations. ence of some oppositional behaviors, these characteristics
Furthermore, their significantly higher score on the EPI
Neuroticism scale (mean = 10.5 [SO = 5.0] versus mean =
TABLE 5
6.8 [SO = 4.2] ; t(yj = 2.77, P < .05) suggests a general tend- Specific Fears in Children With Social Phobia (n = 50)
ency toward overresponsiveness to various events and % Endorsing
situations. This tendency is consistent with general ex- Fear at Least Moderate Fear
pressions of fear and may contribute to general anxious
Gett ing shots 51
overarousal as well. Hav ing blood tests 35
Few children with social phobia had concurrent affec- High places 30
tive disorders (6%). However, dysphoric mood sometimes Seeing blood from a cur or scrape 28
existed. Children with social phobia had significantly Darkness 23
Bees/insects 21
higher Children's Depression Inventory scores when com- Thunderstorms/lightning 21
pared with the normative sample (mean = 11.2 [SO = 7.5] Doctors/dent ists 21
versus mean = 9.0 [SO not available from manual] ; t 49 = Loud noises 19
Watt:r (swimming pool or ocean ) 16
2.1, P < .05, l -sample t test). Choking 16
Parents reported anxious and depressed moods in Catching a disease/germs 16
social-phobic children as indicated by the mean CBCL Planes 14
Vomiting 12
Internalizing scale Tscore of 67.8 (SO = 8.2). By I-sample Elevators 9
t test, this score was significantly different from the Dogs 5
normative sample T score of 50 (t49 = 15.1, P < .0005). Costumed characters 2
On the TRF, children with social phobia had an Interna- Note: Listing of fears is from the Anxiety Disorders Interview
lizing T subscale score of 62.9 (SO = 11 .2), again signif- Schedule for Children (Silverman and Albano , 1996).

J. AM . ACAD . C H I LD AD O L ESC. PSY CHIATRY. 38: 6. JUN E 1')99 647


BEIDEL ET AL.

were not severe enough to be perceived as a significant Thus, like their adult counterparts, children with social
problem. phobia often suffer in silence.
Children with social phobia scored higher on neurot-
Effects of Gender
icism and lower on extroversion, suggestinga temperament
Gender differences in clinical presentation, examined characterized by general emotional overresponsiveness
with Hotelling's T 2 , did not show an overall significant (high neuroticism) and the antipathy of outgoing, unin-
group difference. However, because no previous studies hibited, impulsive, and sociable inclinations (low extro-
examined gender effects, differences on specific variables version). The emotional overresponsiveness also is
also were examined. There were no significant differ- supported by the presence of a substantial number of
ences in the percentage of boys and girls who had sec- moderately distressing, specific fears. In addition, these
ondary diagnoses, or the presence of any particular children may be depressed and lonely. Although the
diagnosis, teacher ratings, behavioral assessment ratings, study design does not allow one to determine whether
daily diary ratings, and most parental ratings and self- these behaviors preceded or resulted from restricted social
report measures. However, on the CBCL Internalizing interactions, Perrin and Last (1993) indicated that in
subscale, girls were rated significantly higher than boys most cases, social phobia precedes the onset of depression.
(mean = 70.1 versus mean = 64.6, respectively; t47 = This would suggest that dysphoria most often is second-
2.44, P < .025). Girls also scored higher than boys on the ary to social phobia, but this is in need of further study.
EPI Neuroticism scale (mean = 11.8 versus mean = 8.9, Bernstein et al. (1996) and Francis et al. (1992)
respectively; t47 = 2.02, P < .05). Finally, there was a reported that males and females were equally likely to
trend for girls to score higher than boys on the SPAI-C develop social phobia, as was the case in this investigation.
(mean = 28.2 versus mean = 22.9, respectively; p < .08). With respect to cornorbidiry, 60% of this sample had a
concurrent disorder, most commonly another anxiety
Effects of Race disorder (36%), and specifically generalized anxiety dis-
Using the same strategy as for gender, we examined order, specific phobia, or separation anxiety disorder.
potential differences in the clinical presentation of African- However, the rate of comorbidity for children with
American and white children with social phobia. There DSM-IV social phobia was lower than rates reported
were no group differences in the percentage of children using DSM-III-R criteria (approximately 66%-80%)
who met criteria for an additional diagnosis, a specific (Last et al., 1992; Strauss and Last, 1993). The most par-
additional diagnosis, or any other dependent variable. simonious explanation is that the restructured diagnos-
However, as with gender, there was a trend suggesting dif- tic criteria eliminated co-occurrence of social phobia
ferences on the SPAI-C. Specifically, African-American and avoidant disorder of childhood and vastly decreased
children tended to have lower scores than white children the co-occurrence of social phobia and overanxious dis-
(mean = 21.2 versus mean = 27.0;p < .08). order (now included under generalized anxiety dis-
order). The pattern of concurrent disorders found in this
study is strikingly similar to those found in studies of
DISCUSSION
adult social phobia (e.g., Turner et aI., 1991). Although
These data illuminate the clinical picture of child- rates of affective and externalizing disorders were low in
hood social phobia. These children suffer substantial this sample, these results should be interpreted cau-
emotional distress and impairment in their daily social, tiously as they may, in part, reflect referral practices to our
academic and family functioning. They have few if any specialty anxiety clinic. Furthermore, the higher rate of
friends, are extremely lonely, and avoid extracurricular affective disorders reported by Strauss and Last (1993)
activities. They are generally anxious and experience also may reflect the fact that their sample was older (mean
somatic symptoms such as headaches and stomach aches. age = 14.9 years) than the current sample. More accurate
In extreme cases, school refusal or selective mutism is estimates of concurrent disorders can be gleaned from
present. Although not exhibiting behaviors that typically community samples.
lead teachers to complain or parents to seek help, they This study also examined the clinical presentation of
have seriously impaired interpersonal functioning which social phobia by race and gender. Some subsamples were
prevents engagement in "typical" childhood activities. smaller than what might be considered optimal, but the

648 J. AM. ACAD. CHIl.D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, -'8;(" JUNE 1999


C H I L D H O O D SOCIAL PHOBIA

results indicate that overall the clinical syndrome did not 1990), because most social behavior is learned byengage-
differ by race or gender. With respect to gender, only on ment in peer interactions. Without the knowledge or
the EPI Neuroticism scale did girls score higher than boys. ability to initiate and maintain social relationships, these
Furthermore, parents rated their daughters as exhibiting children run the risk of remaining socially isolated. Fur-
significantly more anxious and depressive symptoms than thermore, interventions that do not address skill deficits
their sons, consistent with an extensive clinical literature run the risk of an attenuated treatment outcome. Two
suggesting that females report greater anxiety and fear and psychological interventions developed specifically for
suffer more anxiety disorders. It is interesting that there children and adolescents with social phobia (Cognitive-
were no differences in actual observed behaviors. This Behavioral Group Treatment for Adolescents, Albano
raises the question of whether the apparent differences et al., 1995; Social Effectiveness Training for Children,
were "real" or merely reflecting a tendency for females to Beidel et al., 1994) include social skills training compo-
more freely reveal their anxieties and fears. This issue nents. These results suggest a need for interventions to
needs to be addressed further in community samples. incorporate strategies to address social skills deficiencies.
There were no differences between African-American
Limitations
and white children, other than a trend for African-
American children to have lower SPAI-C scores . Al- This study is not without some limitations. First, some
though the African-American sample contained only 14 normal peers did not participate in a structured diagnostic
children, mean scores were very similar for both groups. interview, but were clinically interviewed by a very expe-
Thus, this is not a situation in which the 2 groups had rienced clinician. Second, peers did not complete the
very different mean scores but a lack of statistical power entire assessment battery, but they participated in the
precluded the finding of statistical significance, except most important part of the assessment (i.e., the behavioral
perhaps for the SPAI-C. Overall, our results are consis- assessment of social skills). Third, the sample of African-
tent with those reported by Neal et al. (1995). That study American children with social phobia was small. A larger
did not find race-based differences in children's top 10 sample is desirable. Fourth, this sample was drawn from
fears. Thus, the findings here suggest that the clinical those seeking treatment at an anxiety disorders clinic and
presentation of social phobia is consistent across race thus may not represent the entire population of children
and gender but do not addres s whether parameters asso- with social phobia. Finally, the syndrome documented by
ciated with development and manifestation differ by this study pertains to a clinic sample and could differ
gender or race. These questions will need to be addressed somewhat from a community sample .
in future studies.
Clinical Implications
Consistent with studies of adults (e.g., Turner et al.,
1986), a very important finding was that children with Despite these limitations, however, this is the first
social phobia had significantly poorer social skills than study of the psychopathology of DSM-IV social phobia
children without psychiatric disorders. This is the first in preadolescent children and the first study to use a
study to directly assessand compare social skills. In addi- broad-based assessment strategy, particularly a behav-
tion, social-phobic children had longer speech latencies. ioral assessment using raters unaware of the child's diag-
Longer latencies are characteristic of behavioral inhibi- nostic status. Thus, despite its limitations, it provides a
tion, a temperamental style detected at very early ages (e.g., first examination of what apparently is a highly prev-
Kagan et al., 1987). The current study does not allow us alent childhood disorder. Finally, based on this clinical
to determine whether long speech latencies preceded the presentation, interventions combining skill -building
development of social phobia, but the data do indicate and anxiety reduction appear necessary for maximum
that long latencies are characteristic of both behavioral successful treatment outcome. The results of such an
inhibition and childhood social phobia, suggesting the intervention currently are being analyzed and will be
possibility of some relationship between these constructs. reported in a forthcoming article.
In addition to documenting the clinical syndrome,
findings regarding poor social skills have important the- REFERENCES
oretical and clinical implications. Developmental psy-
Achenbach TM ( 199 1), Manual fOr th« Cross-Info rmant Program [or th«
chologists long have suggested that early social isolation CBCL/4 - / 8. YSR and TRf." Burlington: University of Vermont Depart-
prevents the acquisition of social skills (Rubin er al., ment of Psychiatr y

J . AM . ACAll . CHILll A ll O L ESC . PS YC H IAT RY, .\ H: (" J UNE l'i'i'i 649


REID El. ET A I..

Albano AM . Marren PA. H olt CS . Heimberg RC . Barlo w DH ( 199 ~ ), Ke ndall PC, Sou tham -Gerow MA (19% ). Lo ng-term foll ow-up o f a
Cog nitive- behavioral group treatment tor soc ial pho hia in adole scent s. m gnitive-hehavio ralthera py for an xiety-d isordered youth. J Consult Clin
J Ner» Mrm Dis Ill,t (,49- 6)(' Psye/Jol64: 72 4- 730
American Psychi at ric Associarion (19110). Diagnostic 'l1Id St atistica] Ma nua] of Kend ;lll PC, \'qan nan MJ (1997). Anxiety d isord ers in yout h: d iagnost ic co n-
Mmr,rI Disorders, .lrd edition (DS/If-IIIi. Washingto n. DC: American sistency across DSM -II/ ·R and DSM- IV. J Anxiety Disord IO:45.\-4(d
Psychiat ric Assoc iatio n Kovacs M ( I 9H5). T he Ch ildren 's De pression Inventory (CD I). Psychopbarm-
Ameri can Psychiat ric Association ( 19H7) . DiaglloJtic alld Statistica! MiI1l11al of acol Bull 21 :9'i5-998
Me nta ! D isorders, .lrd cdi tian-rruised (DSM- / II-Ri. Wa'hington , DC: Li st CG ( 1') 86). Sched" lrfOrAJjel'lil'r Disorders and Schizophrenia f or School-
Ame rican Psychiat ric Associatio n Agr Childre» (Prrsent Episode-Rel'isrd). Pittsbur gh : University of Pittsbur gh
American Psych iatr ic Associati on ( ]')<)4 ). Di,rgIlOJri,' lU/{I Stat istica] Manual of LaSI CG . Perri n S. Her sen M . Kazdin AE (1992). DSM·II/-R anxic rv dis-
Menta l Disorders, -uh edition (DSM -IVi. Washingto n. DC: Americ an order s in ch ild ren: socio dcmog raphic and clinic al characteristics, 'J A m
Psychiatric Associat ion Am d Child Adolesc Psychiatry J I: 1070-1076
Asher SR. Wheele r VA ( 19H~) . Ch ild re n's lon eli ness: a compa riso n of McGee R. Feeha n M . W 'i1liam; S er al. (1')')0). DSM- II/ diso rders in a b rge
rejected and neglected peer statLIS. J Consult Clin P~ychol ~.\ :'iOO-~O~ sam ple of ado lescent s. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr y 29:6 11-6 19
Bcidcl DC (199 1). Social phob ia and overanx ious di sorde r in schoo l-age Neal AM. Ward Brown BJ (1995 ). Fears an d anxiety d isorde rs in African
childr en . J Alii Acad Child Adolesc PsydJi,ttry .\O :~4'i-~52 Am erican chi ldre n. In: A nxirtv D isorders ill A(Timll Americans. Fried man
Reidel DC, Nea l AM . Lederer AS (1,)9'1). T h'e feasibility an d validit y o f a S. cd . New Yor k: Spri nger. PP 65- 75 .
da ily diary fiJr the assessm ent of an xiety in children. {lr/Jill' Ther 22:~O~ -~ 17 Perr in S. Last CG (1993 ). Tw o yea r foll ow-u p of ch ildren with a nxiety
Beid cl DC . Tu rne r SM ( l'lHll). Corno rbidi ry o f !l'st anxiet y a nd o ther disorders. Prcscm cd at [he Anx iety D isorders Associati on of America
anx iety diso rders in ch ild ren , J Ahuorm ClJildPsvchol 1 6 :2 7 ~ -2 H 7 Annual Co nventio n. C ha rlesto n. SC
Bcide l DC, Turner SM (19')7). At risk lor anxiet y. I: psychopathology in the Ru hi n KH . LeM'ltl' l] , Lollis S (1990 ). Socia l wit hdrawal in chi ld hood:
oflsp ring of anxious paren t' . J Alii A"ul Child Adoles« Psychiatry .\6:9 111-926 develop menta l path ways to peet rejection. In: Pa r Rrjretioll ill Child·
Bcidel DC, Tu rner SM. Mo rris 1'1. (1994). Social El.!ectil'Cllrss !Z" r,tpy fiJI' h ood, As her SR . C o ie JD . eds . C a mbr id ge, En gl and: Cam b ridge
Ch ildren: A Treatment Ma nual, Charleston : Med ical Univers ity of South University Press. PI' 217-249
Carolina Silver man W. Alba no AM ( 19%) . !ZJr Anxietv D isorders Intrn -ieto Schrd"lr
Bcid cl DC, Tu rner SM. Mo rris 1'1. (11)<) ~ ). A new ins tru me nt to ,m ess f or Children (DSM -/ V). Alba ny, NY: (;re~i nd Publ ication s
ch ild h ood social anxiety an d phob ia: the Soc ial Ph ob ia and Anxiety SPSS ( 19% ). Statistica! /~lckagr for the Socia! Scirncrs. Chicago: SPSS
Inventory lor Child ren. Psycho! Assrss 10:7.\- 79 Strauss CC . Last CG (199.\). Social and simple phobias in ch ild ren . J Anxiety
Bern stein CA . Borch ardt CM .l'erw ein AR (19%). An xiety d isorders in ch il- Disord7:141 -152
d ren and ado lescents: a review of the p'lSt 10 years: J Alii Ac,/{I eN id Turner SM . Beid cl DC, Bord en JW. Sta nley MA . jacob RG (1991 ). Soc ial
Adolrsr PJychi,ttry Vi : 1110-111 9 ph ob ia: Axis I and Axis II correlates. J Ab norm Psycho! 100:102-10 6
Eysenck H] , Eyscnck SBC ( I%ll) . /If,tltl",I J''' thejunior Eysmck Pm ollalil)' Tumcr SM . Beidc] D C, C oole)' MR. Woody S. Messer SC ( 1994). A mult i-
lnumtorv. San Diego : Education al and ln d ustri ul Tcsring Service co mpone nt behavior al treat m en t I[ }{ soc ial phobia: social effectiven ess
Fran cis G . l.asr C G. Stra uss CC ( 1992). Avoidant di so rder and social phobia thera py. 8e/1<1I' Res !Zw .U :3111-.Nll
in c h ild re n and ad o lesce n ts. J Alii I!c'ld Child Adolesc PJychi,ttry Turner SM . Heidel DC, Dancu CV. Keys D ( 1'i1l6). Psych op arholog y of
.~ I : IOH(,-10 94 social phob ia an d its relatio nsh ip to avo idant pe rso nalit y d iso rder. J
Kagan j. Rem ick jS. Snidman N (19H7). T Ill' ph)"iology and psychology of Ab uorm Psychol'i5 :38'i-3')4
be havio ral inhibition in ch ild ren. Child Dr» 'i H: 1 4 ~ ')-1 4 7.' Wechsler D ( 199 I ). Tbr Wre/"Irr lntrlligrncr Sm lr for e bildrm· lI 1. San
Kend all PC ( 1994). Treating a nxiety disorders in ch ild ren: result s of a Antonio , TX : Psychological Corporatio n
random ized cli nical trial. 1 Cansult Gill Psycho! (,2 : 100-1 10

M at ernal Feeding Practices and C h ild h oo d Obesity : A Focus Group Study of Low-Income Mothers. A my E. Baugh cu m .
Kathleen A. Burklow. Ph D . C indy M . Decks. MEd, R D . Scott W. Powers. Ph D . Rob ert C W hi taker. M D , MPH
Objectii«: 'J[ ) iden tify m at ernal beliefs an d practices ab out ch ild fe eding that are associated with the de velopment of ch ild h oo d o be-
siry, /) (Jigll: Fo ur tocus grou ps . O n e grou p of dieti tians from the Su p p leme n ta l N u tr it ion Progr a m I[ ){ Wom e n, In fants. a nd
C hil d re n (W IC) in th e No rthern Kentucky Health Di stri ct an d .~ grou ps of mothe rs wi th chi ld ren enrolled in WI C Smillg: T he
W IC p rogram in the Nort hern Ken tu cky Healt h D istrict . "'mi.-ip,1II1S: Fifteen W IC die titians a nd 14 m other s ( 14 to 34 yea rs of
age) wi th yo u ng chi ld ren ( 12 to 36 months of age) enrolled in W IC Results: T he mother s in thi s st udy (1) beli eved th at it wa s berrer
to ha ve a hea vy in fanr becau se infant weight was th e bes t marker of child healrh an d su ccessful parenting. (2) fe ared th at their in fants
were nor gett ing eno ugh to ea t. which led th em 10 introd uce rice ce rea l a nd ot he r soli d foo d 10 the di et s befo re th e recomme nded
ages . a nd 0) used foo d 10 shape th eir chi ldre n's beh aviors (eg. to rew ard go od be hav io r or to cal m fussiness ). T he m other s acknowl-
edged th at so me of thei r ch ild -feed ing pr act ices went against the advice of the ir W IC n ur ritio nists a nd ph ysician s. Instead. th e par-
ricip an ts relied o n t heir m others as th eir m ain so u rce of in fo rmatio n abo u t ch ild feed ing, Conclusions: Phy sicians and allied h ealth
p rofessionals dis cu ssing chi ld h oo d growth with m others sho u ld avo id im p lying that in fant weight is nec essarily a me asure o f ch ild
health or parental co m pe tence. Parents wh o use foo d 10 sa tisfy th e ir ch ild ren's emo tional needs or 10 p rom ot e good b eh avior in t hei r
chil d ren ma y p romote obesity hy interle ring w ith thei r ch ild ren's ahi lit), to regu lat e th eir ow n foo d int ake. Int ervent ion s 10 alte r
chi ld- feeding pr actices sh ou ld include ed ucati on of gra ndmothers. Ar ch Ped iatr Adolesc Med 1998; 15 2: 10 10-10 14

A bst fllcrs Jel,,·ted by Mic!Jile/ J- M ,r/olley. M .D .. ASJiJltlllt Editor.

650 J. AM . ACA ll . C I I I Ll ) A ll OLESC . P S YCHI AT R Y. .IH :( • • J UNE 1')') 9

You might also like