Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/315151019

Intervention for Peer Mediation and Mother-Child Interaction: The Effects on


Children’s Mediated Learning Strategies and Cognitive Modifiability

Article  in  Contemporary Educational Psychology · March 2017


DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.005

CITATIONS READS

9 390

1 author:

David Tzuriel
Bar Ilan University
112 PUBLICATIONS   2,115 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Mediated Learning Experience View project

Cognitive Education Programs View project

All content following this page was uploaded by David Tzuriel on 16 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Contemporary Educational Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cedpsych

Intervention for peer mediation and mother-child interaction: The


effects on children’s mediated learning strategies and cognitive
modifiability
David Tzuriel a,⇑, Rinat Caspi b
a
Bar Ilan University, Israel
b
Givat Washington Academic College of Education, Israel

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The effects of intervention for peer mediation and mother-child mediated learning experience (MLE)
Available online 16 March 2017 strategies on children’s MLE strategies and cognitive modifiability (CM) was investigated on a sample of
100 mother–child dyads. CM was examined in domains of executive functions and analogical reasoning.
Keywords: The MLE interactions were analyzed by Observation for Mediated Interaction (OMI) scale. Children of high-
Mediated learning strategies and low-mediating mothers were assigned to experimental (n = 49) and control (n = 51) conditions. The
Peer-mediation experimental group received a peer-mediation program and the control groups received an alternative
Cognitive modifiability
creativity program. The children (in Grade 3) were assigned as mediators of learners (in Grade 1) and
Dynamic assessment
Planning
taught them analogical reasoning problems. The peer interaction was videotaped and analyzed by the
Self-regulation OMI. All children were given dynamic assessment after the interaction. Children in the experimental
group showed higher MLE strategies and CM than did children in the control condition. Mediators of
low-MLE mothers in the experimental group showed greater CM than did mediators in the control group.
Structural equation modeling analysis showed that mediators’ cognitive modifiability was explained by
proximal factors of treatment, mothers’ quality of mediation (QM) and mediators’ QM; learners’ cognitive
modifiability was explained by mediators’ QM. The findings were discussed in relation to the MLE theory
and earlier findings.
Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction ties and to change one’s own cognitive structures (Feuerstein et al.,
2002; Tzuriel, 2001, 2013). Our research is guided mainly by Feuer-
An important question asked frequently by researchers and stein’s theory of MLE (Feuerstein et al., 2002; Tzuriel, 1999, 2001,
clinician is whether participation in a cognitive education program 2013) and Vygotsky’s socio-cultural theory of mental development
aimed at developing mediated learning experience strategies (MLE, (e.g., Rogoff, 1990, 1994, 1999; Tudge, 1996; Valsiner, 1988;
Feuerstein, Feuerstein, Falik, & Rand, 2002) compensates for low Vygotsky, 1962, 1978, 1981; Wertsch, 1985). In the following sec-
quality of mother-child interaction as regard the effects on devel- tions we introduce briefly (a) Feuerstein’s MLE theory, (b) the Peer
opment of children’s MLE strategies, and cognitive modifiability Mediation with Young Children (PMYC) Program and specifically on
(CM) in domains of executive functions and cognitive skills. In its effects on children’s MLE strategies and CM, and (c) the effects
the current study we try to deal with this question by applying a of parent-child MLE interaction on children’s CM.
relatively novel cognitive education program: The Peer Mediation
with Young Children (PMYC, e.g., Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel
& Shamir, 2010). The main outcome variables were children’s 1.1. Feuerstein’s mediated learning experience (MLE) theory
MLE strategies, as observed in peer interactions and their cognitive
modifiability. Cognitive modifiability is defined as the individual’s According to the MLE theory parents are conceived of as active-
propensity to learn from new experiences and learning opportuni- modifying agents in directing and shaping their children’s develop-
ment. MLE interactions are defined as a process in which parents or
substitute adults interpose themselves between a set of stimuli
⇑ Corresponding author at: School of Education, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan and the human organism and modify the stimuli for developing
5290002, Israel. children. In mediating the world of stimuli, parents may use differ-
E-mail address: David.Tzuriel@biu.ac.il (D. Tzuriel). ent strategies such as focusing, alerting attention, changing the

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.005
0361-476X/Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 303

stimulus frequency, order, and intensity, relating new information (e.g., facial expression, tone of voice, rituals, repetitious
to familiar contexts, and regulating the order and timing of new actions) or verbally (e.g., illuminating a current event, activ-
information. Good mediators relate also to children’s motivational ity, or learned context, relating it to past or current events,
aspects by arousing their attention, curiosity, and vigilance, focus- and explaining its value). Mediation for meaning may be
ing on relevant aspects of the situation, and providing meaning to expressed by enthusiasm in the presence of an event and/
neutral stimuli. From a cognitive perspective, adequate mediation or explaining the personal significance and unique impor-
facilitates development of cognitive functions required for tempo- tance of a stimulus that to the child might look neutral.
ral, spatial, and cause-effect relations. Children gradually internal- (c) Mediation for Transcendence (expanding) is characterized by
ize the MLE processes, which become integrated mechanisms of going beyond the concrete context or the immediate needs
change in the future. As children develop internalized self- of the child. The mediator usually tries to reach for general
mediation strategies, their parents gradually withdraw from the principles and/or goals that are not bound to the ‘‘here and
situation and allow the children more autonomy in implementing now” or the specific and concrete aspects of the situation.
the acquired mediated strategies. Adequate MLE interactions facil- Mothers, for example, might go beyond the children’s con-
itate the development of various cognitive functions, learning sets, crete experience and teach strategies, rules, and principles
mental operations, strategies, reflective thinking, and need sys- in order to generalize to other situations. Mediation of tran-
tems. The acquired and internalized MLE processes allow develop- scendence occurs either in spontaneous family contexts
ing children later to use them independently, to benefit from novel (e.g., eating, bathing) or structured contexts. In both situa-
learning experiences in varied contexts, and to modify their own tions, parents use the interaction to mediate important gen-
cognitive systems. Mediational processes are complex, circular, eralized principles, rules, values, and concepts. It should be
and depend not only on parental characteristics but also on chil- noted that although mediation for transcendence depends
dren’s cognitive strengths and deficits, motivational orientation, on the first two criteria, the combination of all criteria
emotional needs, behavioral tendencies, stimulus characteristics, becomes a powerful vehicle for the development of cogni-
and situational conditions (Tzuriel, 2013). tive modifiability and the widening of the individual’s need
Lack of or inadequate MLE may be derived from two broad cat- system.
egories: (a) lack of environmental opportunities for mediation, and (d) In Mediation of Feelings of Competence, mediators initially
(b) inability of the children to benefit from mediational interac- arrange the environment to ensure the children’s success
tions that are potentially available. In the first case, lack of or inad- and interpret the environment in a manner that conveys to
equate mediation derives from parents’ low educational level, the children the awareness of the capability of functioning
traumatic life events, lack of parents’ awareness of the importance independently and successfully. This is done in various
of mediation, and lack of knowledge and/or sophistication in ways, such as reorganizing the children’s environment so
applying MLE strategies. In the second case, children might suffer as to ensure opportunities for success, explaining the rea-
from physical and/or mental disabilities that act as barriers to reg- sons for successes and failures, and by rewarding the chil-
ister mediation that may be offered to them. Feuerstein et al. dren for attempts to master the situation and cope
(2002) suggested 12 strategies of MLE, but only the first five have effectively with current problems. Mediators provide feed-
been operationalized in studies of infants and young children back not only for successful solutions but also for partially
(Klein, 1988; Klein, Weider, & Greenspan, 1987; Lidz, 1991; successful performances and for attempts at mastery.
Tzuriel, 1996, 1999, 2001, 2013; Tzuriel & Eran, 1990; Tzuriel & (e) In Mediation for Self-Regulation of Behavior the mediators reg-
Ernst, 1990; Tzuriel & Weiss, 1998) and in studies on peer media- ulate children’s responses, depending on the task demands,
tion (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, 2010) and as well as on the children’s behavioral styles. Regulation of
siblings (Klein, Zarur, & Feldman, 2003; Tzuriel & Hanuka-Levy, behavior is carried out by either inhibiting impulsive ten-
2014). dencies or by accelerating inefficient slow behavior. This
According to Feuerstein and colleagues, an adult-child interac- mediation is of critical importance in helping children regis-
tion can be defined as mediated interaction if it contains three ter information accurately, delay immediate gratification,
basic ‘‘ingredients”: Intentionality and reciprocity, meaning, and and pace the inner rhythm of response as a function of task
transcendence. These criteria are considered to be necessary for demands. Mediation of regulation of behavior affects the
an interaction to be classified as MLE. Other criteria can contribute whole process of mental activity in input, elaboration, and
to cognitive development but are not regarded as necessary and/or output phases of the mental act. It can be carried out by ana-
sufficient in a mediated interaction. lyzing the task components, inhibiting acting-out behavior,
delaying immediate gratification, focusing on task character-
(a) Intentionality and Reciprocity is an interaction characterized istics, and eliciting metacognitive strategies.
by efforts to create in the child a state of vigilance, and to
According to Feuerstein, the MLE strategies used in parent-child
facilitate an efficient registration of the information (input
interactions help children internalize learning mechanisms, facili-
phase), an adequate processing (elaboration phase), and
tate learning processes and self-mediation, give indications about
accurate responding (output phase). The reciprocity compo-
future changes of cognitive structures, develop deficient cognitive
nent is of crucial importance to the quality and continuation
functions, and provide for the ability to benefit in the future from
of the mediation process; when mediated children respond
mediation in other contexts. For example, a child who receives
to mediators’ behavior, it enables mediators to adjust their
adequate mediation for Transcendence (i.e., expanding information
mediation and continue the process efficiently. A parent
to teach rules and strategies) internalizes this specific type of
can draw a child’s attention to a specific aspect of a drawing,
mediation and will tend to apply it efficiently in other contexts.
highlighting its specific features, and sensitively waiting and
The efficient use is not limited only to provision of mediation by
even encouraging the child’s response.
others but also to generation of transcendence when confronted
(b) Mediation of Meaning refers to interactions in which the
with new situations. Children will transfer the rules and strategies
mediator tries, when presenting an object, to emphasize its
learned previously to other problems that vary in terms of content
significance and worth. This is done by expressing affect
domain, levels of complexity, novelty, and abstraction. In the same
and interest and by pointing to its importance and value.
way, children who experience adequate mediation of meaning
The significance of a stimulus can be conveyed nonverbally
304 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

internalize this interaction and tend to use it later in various con-


texts. They will be not only more open to mediation of meaning but
also will initiate attachment of meaning to new information rather
Mothers’ QMI
than passively waiting for meaning to appear.

1.2. Level I and level II of proximal factors


Mediators’
Mediator’s QMI Cognitive
In Feuerstein’s terms the effects of the cognitive mediational Modifiability
program and the mothers’ quality of mediation are considered as
proximal factors that explain directly the children’s cognitive mod-
ifiability. Feuerstein, however, did not specifically refer to the Treatment
effects of children’s own experience of mediation to others on their
own cognitive modifiability. In the current study we proposed a
synthesis of a constructivist approach with the MLE theory as
regards a distinction between two levels (I and II) of proximal fac-
tors. The causal model is presented in Fig. 1. Based on the construc-
tivists’ theory (e.g., Brownstein, 2001; Rhodes & Bellamy, 1999) we
assume that an actual activity in a mediation process such as peer-
mediation is considered to be a more powerful factor for internal-
izing mediation strategies than is being exposed to mediation from
a mediating agent (e.g., parent, teacher). That of course depends on Mothers’ QMI
the duration and the intensity of each type of interaction. Based on
earlier studies on peer-mediation we posed a question whether a
relatively short-term intervention for peer-mediation serves as a ’
potent proximal factor (level II) in enhancing children’s cognitive Mediator’s QMI Cognitive
Modifiability
modifiability beyond the effect of mother-child MLE (level I).

1.3. The peer mediation with young children (PMYC) program


Treatment

The PMYC (e.g., Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004) is a relatively novel


approach based on the sociocultural theory of Vygotsky (1962,
1978, 1981) and the mediated learning experience (MLE) theory of
Feuerstein (e.g., Feuerstein et al., 2002). In some ways the PMYC Fig. 1. Schematic model of the effects of proximal factors on children’s cognitive
is similar to the Peer Assisted Learning (PAL) approach (Brown, modifiability (mediators and learners).
Metz, & Campione, 1996; Crook, 1998; Griffin & Griffin, 1997;
Rogoff, 1990, 1994, 2003; Topping, Dekhinet, Blanch, Corcelles, & variety of problem-solving tasks. (c) The peer-mediation principles
Duran, 2013; Tudge, 1996; Webb, 2009); both are focused on are structured, but allow considerable room for flexibility and cre-
strategies for enhancing children’s learning processes, for taking ativity. (d) The status of the learner and that of the mediator are
personal responsibility and control for their own acquisition of distinguished; that is, the mediator is perceived as more compe-
knowledge and skills and for constructing meaningful solutions tent than the learner on the basis of both age and experience.
to problems. The effectiveness of PAL has been demonstrated with The objectives of the PMYC program are to enhance (a) a medi-
regard to children’s academic achievement (e.g., Fuchs & Fuchs, ational teaching style, (b) learning skills and cognitive modifiability,
2005; Griffin & Griffin, 1997; O’Donnell & King, 1999; Rohrbeck, (c) learning skills of learners mediated by their peers, and (d)
Ginsburg-Block, Fantuzzo, & Miller, 2003; Topping, 2001), attitudes learning-to-learn and school performance among mediators. The
toward learning (e.g., King, 1997), and social and behavioral skills PMYC includes learning of the basic ideas and MLE strategies,
(e.g., Kamps, Barbetta, Leonard, & Delquadri, 1994; McAuliffe & adapted to the children’s age level (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004). The
Dembo, 1994). Piaget (1967) has already argued that cognitive mediators learn that these principles will assist them in helping
development is viewed in part as an outcome of peer interaction their peers as well as themselves in solving problems and indepen-
that enhances different cognitive perspectives, and leads, conse- dent learning in different contexts. The program includes basic
quently, to socio-cognitive conflict. Such conflict motivates each conditions for effective peer interaction, such as respecting the
participant in the interaction to elaborate on his or her thinking learner, showing empathy, and providing ‘‘personal space” for
processes, to justify a position, and to present a convincing activity.
response. These processes require construction of new schemas The PMYC program is characterized by three operational com-
and novel thought patterns. Vygotsky added, significantly, that ponents: (a) teaching of the basic MLE principles adapted for young
the interaction with a more cognitively competent partner, children in terms of level of terminology and strategies, (b) observ-
whether child or adult, is more effective than interaction with a ing and discussing a didactic film introducing the MLE principles,
same-level partner. Vygotsky’s followers (e.g., Rogoff, 1990; and (c) practicing the MLE principles with peers by using multime-
Tudge, 1996) have argued that cognitive development takes place dia programs and conventional materials. A didactic videotape is
when learners, displaying different levels of cognitive attributes, aimed at enhancing internalization of each mediation principle.
cooperate to complete a given task. A peer-cooperation process, The program also includes learning aids such as computer pro-
moreover, helps the learners construct shared understanding. grams, games, and stickers with visual symbols of the principles,
The PMYC is unique with regard to four aspects: (a) It is based verbal slogans, and work sheets. Each mediation principle is
on a comprehensive theoretical model integrating cognitive and assigned a visual symbol, a name, and a verbal slogan. For example,
emotional components and reflects MLE principles that promote mediation for feelings of competence is represented by the visual
‘‘learning how to learn” among young children. (b) It is beyond symbol of a ‘‘smiley” face, the symbol’s name is ‘‘Smiley” and the
content domains or contexts of learning and can be applied to a verbal slogan is ‘‘Tell me ‘good for you’ in everything I do cor-
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 305

rectly.” Mediation for regulation of behavior is represented by a et al., 2002; Klein, 1988, 1996; NICHD Early Child Care Research
visual symbol of a ‘‘stop sign,” the symbol’s name is ‘‘Stop,” and Network, 2006; Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005; Tzuriel,
the verbal slogan is ‘‘Stop and think, before and after you drill.” 1999, 2001, 2013; Vandell et al., 2010; Vygotsky, 1978). Grappling
The intervention program is carried out in seven lessons of with the specific mediational strategies that enhance children’s
90 min each over a period of three weeks. The lessons include pre- thinking skills, researchers have suggested a variety of parents’
sentation of the mediation principles, explanation of their signifi- behavior such as distancing (e.g., Siegel, 1982), scaffolding (e.g.,
cance in general and in peer-mediation situations in particular, Mattanah, Pratt, Cowan, & Cowan, 2005; Stright, Herr, & Neitzel,
and practice and application of the principles in a range of learning 2009; Wood, 1989), responsiveness (e.g., Bornstein & Tamis-
situations. For the purpose of enhancing the mediators ‘‘learning LeMonda, 1990), and MLE (Feuerstein et al., 2002; Klein et al.,
how to learn” skills each stage includes reflection on the mediators’ 1987; Tzuriel, 1999, 2001, 2011a, 2011b, 2013). Some researchers
own process of learning. have focused on the concept of attachment in early years as a pre-
dictor of cognitive development in childhood (e.g., Mikulincer,
1.3.1. The effect of PMYC on cognitive modifiability Shaver, & Pereg, 2003; Moss & St-Laurent, 2001; Sroufe, 1988;
The efficacy of the PMYC program has been demonstrated in West, Mathews, & Kerns, 2013). Research findings show clearly
several studies with children in grades 1–3 (e.g., Shamir & that specific activities of parents relate to their children’s cognitive
Tzuriel, 2004) and in secondary high school students (Trabelsi, development and that both the child and the parents influence the
Tzuriel, & Kashy-Rosenbaum, 2015). The program was found to child’s intellectual development (i.e., Berk & Spuhl, 1995;
be effective in improving MLE strategies of children (Shamir, Bornstein, 1985, 1989a, 1989b; Bornstein, Azuma, Tamis-
Tzuriel, & Guy, 2007; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010), including children LeMonda, & Ogino, 1990; Cristofaro & Tamis-LeMonda, 2012;
with learning disabilities (Shamir & Lazerovitz, 2007; Trabelsi Klein, 1988, 1991, 1996; Klein & Alony, 1993; Korat, Ron, & Klein,
et al., 2015), and in facilitating their cognitive modifiability as mea- 2008; Rodriguez et al., 2009; Tzuriel, 1999, 2001, 2013).
sured by dynamic assessment (DA) measures (Trabelsi et al., 2015; Several studies were designed primarily to investigate the rela-
Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, 2010). The PMYC was also found to be tive effects of distal factors (e.g., socio-economic status, parental
effective in enhancing math performance (Shamir & Tzuriel, emotional rejection) and proximal factors (MLE) in the determina-
2004; Shamir et al., 2007; Trabelsi et al., 2015). The learners’ cog- tion of the child’s cognitive modifiability as well as studying con-
nitive modifiability was also found to be enhanced in a computer- text and subjects’ characteristics in determining the adequacy of
ized environment more than in a non-computerized learning MLE strategies in the family and among peers (e.g., Trabelsi et al.,
environment (Shamir et al., 2007). 2015; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010) and teachers (e.g., Tzuriel, Kaniel,
In all previous studies children who participated in the PMYC Zeliger, Friedman, & Haywood, 1998). The general hypothesis in
program were compared with control children who were given all these studies was that MLE interactions as representing learn-
an alternative program. All children had been taught in a short ing processes within the family are more accurate in predicting
training session to solve specific tasks that they later taught their the DA post-teaching performance than outcome static measures
younger peers. In all studies the mediators’ interactions with their or DA pre-teaching measures. The rationale behind this hypothesis
younger peers were videotaped and analyzed using the Observation is that adequate MLE offers children increased ‘‘psychological
of Mediation Interaction scale (OMI, Klein, 1988; Klein et al., 1987). tools” that serve to expand and differentiate their zone of proximal
The OMI was adapted for peer mediation using age-appropriate development (ZPD). This is why the upper level of ZPD measures
tasks (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004). Mediators were instructed to teach would be more accurate as a predicted outcome of MLE interac-
their peers a problem-solving task such as analogies or subject tions than static test performance (Tzuriel, 2001).
matter such as math (Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010). A major finding repeated in almost all studies was that chil-
The findings of previous studies showed repeatedly that medi- dren’s post-teaching scores (or gain scores) on DA measures were
ators receiving intervention for peer mediation demonstrated better predicted by MLE mother-child interactions than were static
more frequently usage of mediation strategies (Shamir & Tzuriel, test scores (or pre-teaching DA scores). For example, in a study on
2004; Shamir, Tzuriel, & Rozen, 2006) and a higher level of cogni- kindergarten kibbutz children (n = 47) they were administrated
tive modifiability in analogical reasoning tasks (Shamir et al., 2007; Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM, Raven, 1956) and a
Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, 2010) as well as in math (Shamir et al., DA measure for young children: The Children’s Inferential Thinking
2006) than did mediators who received an alternative program. Modifiability test (CITM, Tzuriel, 1992). Three scores are derived
The efficacy of the program was remarkable when the program’s from the CITM: pre-teaching, post-teaching, and gain. In a series
effects were studied with learners, who themselves did not receive of three stepwise regression analyses, the CPM and MLE-Total
any training program, but were taught by their experimental peers. scores were assigned as predictors of the pre-teaching, post-
The learners showed a higher level of mediation strategies (Shamir teaching, and gain scores, respectively. The pre-teaching (static
& Tzuriel, 2004) and a higher pre- to post-teaching improvement measure) was predicted only by the CPM (R = 0.40, p < 0.004), the
on seriation tasks (Shamir et al., 2007; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007) post-teaching was predicted by both MLE–Total and CPM
and in math (Shamir et al., 2007) than did their counterparts (R = 0.69, p < 0.002), and the gain was predicted only by MLE–Total
who were taught by mediators receiving an alternative program. score (R = 0.43, p < 0.001). This intriguing predictability pattern
These findings suggest that the mediators who receive an interven- found across the three regression analyses indicates that the more
tion how to mediate internalize the mediation strategies, which the criterion score was saturated with teaching effects, within the test-
help them to be more effective in teaching their peers, as well as ing DA procedure, the greater was the variance contributed by MLE
to demonstrate higher modifiability on the cognitive tasks. mother–child processes.
A structural equation model (SEM) analysis was applied in a
1.4. Parent-child MLE interactions and children’s cognitive series of six studies in which distal factors (e.g., socioeconomic
modifiability level, severity of ADHD), and proximal factors (i.e., MLE strategies)
of cognitive modifiability were compared (e.g., Tzuriel & Ernst,
The effects of parent-child interaction on cognitive develop- 1990; Tzuriel & Weiss, 1998). In all of these studies mother-child
ment captured the interest of researchers for several decades interactions were observed during free-play and/or structured
(e.g., Belsky et al., 2007; Berk & Spuhl, 1995; Bornstein & Tamis- (teaching) situations using the Observation of Mediation Interaction
LeMonda, 1990; Burchinal, Vandel, & Belsky, 2014; Feuerstein (Klein et al., 1987). One of the major findings repeated in almost all
306 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

studies was that children’s post-teaching scores on DA measures 2010), we expected the PMYC program to be effective with
were better predicted by MLE mother-child interactions than by all children but especially with children whose mothers
static test scores such as pre-teaching DA scores or Raven’s Stan- demonstrate a low level of use of MLE strategies.
dard Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1956). Examination of the differ- (b) To what degree does the PMYC program facilitate process-
ential prediction pattern of cognitive modifiability by specific MLE oriented cognitive behaviors as compared with outcome-
strategies showed that (a) MLE criteria were predicted by the distal oriented scores? Because the PMYC program is focused on
factors of mothers’ SES, acceptance/rejection and children’s per- mediation processes, one would expect to see the direct
sonality orientation, (b) the distal factors did not explain directly effects on process oriented measures of cognition to be more
the children’s cognitive modifiability, (c) the most striking finding powerful than on outcome-oriented measures. On the basis
emerging from all six studies is that the MLE strategy that has of previous findings, we assumed that the practice of peer
appeared as most powerfully associated with cognitive modifiabil- mediation following the PMYC training would support the
ity is mediation for transcendence (i.e., expanding principles beyond mediators’ ‘‘learning-to-learn” skills as observed in process
the immediate context, beyond the ‘‘here and now”). oriented measures of planning and self-regulation and out-
come measure of analogical reasoning. Subsequent changes
1.5. Questions, objectives and expectations of the study in the mediators’ cognitive outcome would reveal how well
the mediators had improved their own self-regulated learn-
1.5.1. Major questions of the study ing in response to the mediation experience.

(a) To what degree the PMYC program can compensate for lack In the present study we used two tests before and after the
of or deficient MLE strategies in families and consequently PMYC program. Before the PMYC (Time A) the tests were adminis-
enhances development of children’s MLE strategies and cog- tered in a standardized way, whereas after the PMYC the tests were
nitive modifiability. On the basis of previous peer-mediation administered in a DA format, which included pre-teaching (Time
studies (e.g., Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, B), and post-teaching (Time C) phases (see Fig. 2). In-between

Videotaping of Mother-Child Interaction (a)

Grade 3 Children Grade 3 Children Grade 1 Children


Mothers with Low MLE Mothers with High MLE (n=100)
(n = 49) (n = 51)

Pre-Intervention Phase (b) Pre-Intervention


Vocabulary & Similarities (WISC-R) Phase (b)
Seria-Think Instrument: Pre-Teaching CITM:
CMB Analogies: Pre-Teaching Pre-Teaching

PMYC Program (c) Substitute Program (c)


Children to Mothers with Children to Mothers with
Low MLE (n = 26) Low MLE (n = 23)

Children to Mothers with Children to Mothers with


High MLE (n = 23) High MLE (n = 28)

Videotaping of Peer-Mediation Interaction (d)


Grade 3 Mediators taught Grade 1 Learners the CITM Teaching
Phase

Grade 3: Post- Grade 1: Post-


Intervention (e) Intervention (e)
Dynamic Assessment
Seria-Think Instrument and Children’s Inferential Thinking
CMB Analogies: Pre-Teaching, Modifiability (CITM):
Teaching, and Post-Teaching Post-Teaching

Fig. 2. Design and procedure of the study.


D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 307

the pre- and post-teaching phase each child was taught individu- and on the outcome-oriented measure as tapped in the CMB
ally for about 45 min the principles of solving problems. The pre- Analogies subtest (Tzuriel, 1995), than will mediators in the
to post-teaching improvement was taken as indication for cogni- control group.
tive modifiability. It should be noted that the mothers of the medi- 5. Children of mothers with low use of MLE strategies in the
ators were also observed interacting with their children and that experimental group will show higher cognitive modifiability
the learners were tested before and after being taught by their peer than will comparable children in the control group. We
mediators. expected the PMYC program, given to the experimental group,
The process-oriented measure was the Seria-Think Instrument to be a compensating factor enhancing cognitive modifiability.
(Tzuriel, 2000a), which assesses executive functions of planning 6. Mediators’ cognitive modifiability will be explained by all prox-
behavior and self-regulation (control of impulsivity). The imal factors of treatment, mothers’ quality of mediation (QM)
outcome-oriented measure was the Analogies subtest from the and mediators’ QM. The causal paths of influence for mediators
Cognitive Modifiability Battery (e.g., Tzuriel, 1995, 2000b; Tzuriel are presented in Fig. 1. On the basis of the distinction between
& Shamir, 2010). A group of children in Grade 3 were assigned to levels I and II of the proximal factors we expected that the level
the experimental group who received the PMYC program. They II of mediators’ QM will be more robust in explaining the medi-
were compared with children in a control group who received an ators’ cognitive modifiability than the level I factors of mothers’
alternative program. Children in both groups were asked to teach QM and treatment (see Fig. 1- mediators).
problems from the Children’s Inferential Thinking Modifiability 7. Learners’ cognitive modifiability will be explained directly by
(CITM, Tzuriel, 1989) test to younger peers in Grade 1. The interac- mediators’ QM; whereas the mediators’ QM will be explained
tion was videotaped and analyzed later by the OMI scale. At the by the proximal factors of treatment and mothers’ QM (see
end of the intervention children in both groups were given the Fig. 1-learners).
process-oriented and outcome-oriented measures in a DA format
(see design in Fig. 2). 1.5.4. Specific questions of the study
The questions are of an exploratory nature since no previous
1.5.2. Objectives of the study theory or research has been found to support a clear expectation.

(a) To study to what degree the PMYC program enhances MLE 1. Will mothers show greater use of MLE strategies than their chil-
strategies of children when interacting with their peers. dren do in the children’s role as mediators in the peer’s interac-
(b) To study the interaction effect of mothers-child MLE strate- tions? In general, we assume that mothers show higher level of
gies and PMYC program on children’s MLE strategies. MLE strategies than their children in their role as mediators in the
(c) To study the interaction effects of mother-child MLE strate- peer interaction; however, the differences between mothers and
gies and PMYC program on children’s cognitive their children will be greater in the control condition than in
modifiability. the experimental condition (participation in the PMYC program).
(d) To study the relative effects of level I (mothers’ quality of 2. Which factor will predict better the children’s quality of medi-
mediation) and level II (mediators’ quality of mediation) as ation, mothers’ quality of mediation or participation in the
proximal factors on cognitive modifiability of mediators. PMYC program? The answer to this question would carry
applicative meanings for development of intervention programs
1.5.3. Expectations that may compensate for lack of mediation within the family.

1. Mediators who participate in the PMYC program (experimental 2. Method


group) will display a higher level of MLE strategies when
observed teaching their peers than will mediators in the control 2.1. Participants
group who did not participate in the program. It should be
noted that although the effects of the PMYC on MLE strategies The sample was composed of 100 mother-child pairs and 100
has been demonstrated and validated in earlier studies (e.g., pairs of mediator-learner children; mediators came from the third
Shamir & Tzuriel, 2007; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010), we intended grade and learners from the first grade. Each child in the mother-
to replicate this finding. child pair was assigned later as mediator, in a peer-mediation con-
2. Learners in the experimental group will show higher respon- dition to a young learner.
siveness to mediation than will learners in the control group.
Because the PMYC program was expected to affect the media- 2.1.1. Children’s sample
tion level of mediators we expected that consequently the The sample (100 pairs of mediators/learners) was drawn from
responsiveness of the learners will be also higher in the exper- schools located in a central region in Israel. The mediators were
imental than in the control group. In other words, the learners’ randomly drawn from 14 classes in the 3rd grade, and the learners
responsiveness indicates another facet of the effectiveness of were randomly drawn from 12 classes in the 1st grade. The
peer mediation. mediator-learner dyads came from the same school. The mediators
3. Higher correlations between mothers’ and their children’s MLE were assigned to one of four subgroups (using a 2  2 design)
strategies will be found in the experimental than in the control based on mother-child quality of mediation (low versus high)
group. Equal correlations in the experimental and control and treatment (experimental versus control). In order to assign
groups would indicate that MLE strategies are transmitted from the children to high- versus low-level of mother-child MLE, the
parents to children spontaneously without any need for further interaction of each mother-child pair was videotaped and analyzed
intervention with the children. On the other hand, higher corre- for frequency of MLE strategies using the OMI (see Measures and
lations in the experimental than in the control group would Procedure). Based on the median score (Med. = 62) of the overall
indicate that the intervention enhances the trans-generational mother’s Quality of Mediation Index (see Results), children were
transmission of mediation patterns. assigned first to two groups: children of mothers with a high level
4. Mediators in the experimental group will show more pre- to of mediation (n = 51) and children of mothers with a low level of
post-intervention improvement on the process-oriented mea- mediation (n = 49). Half of the mediators were assigned to an
sures as tapped in the Seria-Think Instrument (Tzuriel, 2000a) experimental group who participated in the PMYC program and
308 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

the other half received an alternative program. Since gender effects in children trained how to mediate than in children trained in
were found to affect the quality of interaction (e.g., King, Staffieri, & alternative program (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Shamir et al., 2006).
Adelgais, 1998; Rekrut, 1994; Topping, 2001) the gender of medi- Activation refers to interactions in which the mediator is perform-
ators and learners was counterbalanced in each of the experimen- ing the task for the learner (nonverbal) or tells the learner the right
tal and control groups. The gender composition in the mediators’ answer (verbal).
group (51 boys and 49 girls) was similar to that of the learners’ In earlier studies, mother-child or peer dyads were videotaped
group (45 boys and 55 girls). In order to avoid transmission of during free-play and/or structured teaching situations and ana-
information among children in the same class regarding the nature lyzed later with the OMI. In each interaction, mediators and learn-
of intervention children from each class were assigned to either an ers alike were rated on the five MLE criteria by two trained
experimental or a control group. All children were assigned into observers. Both observers were trained for 30 h in rating mediation
experimental (49 pairs) and control (51 pairs) groups. The mean strategies; training included theoretical understanding of the MLE
age of the mediators in the experimental and control groups was criteria and mostly by observing and analyzing videotapes depict-
105.45 (SD = 3.97) and 104.27 (SD = 4.08) months, respectively. ing mediation processes. The OMI was adapted for the observation
The mean age of learners in the experimental and control groups of kindergarten children interacting with their parents (Bettan &
was 81.70 (SD = 3.90) and 80.34 (SD = 4.73) months, respectively. Tzuriel, 2007; Isman & Tzuriel, 2008; Tzuriel, 1999; Tzuriel &
In order to control for subjects’ verbal level, we administered Eran, 1990; Tzuriel & Ernst, 1990; Tzuriel & Hatzir, 1999; Tzuriel
two verbal subscales from the WISC-R95 (vocabulary and similar- & Weiss, 1998) in peer-mediation (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004;
ities). A one-way MANOVA of Treatment with the two verbal sub- Shamir et al., 2006, 2007; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, 2010) and sib-
scales as dependent variables revealed that the experimental group lings’ mediation studies (Klein et al., 2003; Tzuriel & Hanuka-
scored at about the same level as did the control group, Levy, 2014).
F(2, 97) = 2.07, p > 0.05. In the present study mother-child and peer interactions were
carried out in structured teaching situations. Each interaction
2.1.2. Parent’s sample was videotaped for 20 min and later analyzed by the OMI. In
Parental years of formal education was similar for experimental mother-child interactions each mother was asked to teach her
and control groups; mothers – v2(2, 100) = 0.78, p > 0.05, fathers – child two types of tasks (see Procedure) requiring abstract level
v2(2, 98) = 0.51, p > 0.05. Parental level of occupation was classified of thinking. In peer mediation each older child was asked to teach
on an ordinal 5-point scale starting from 1 (e.g., unemployed) to 5 his/her younger peer complex problems requiring inferential
(e.g., academic profession, manager). The parents’ level of occupation thinking (see Procedure). Each behavioral category of mediation
was similar in both experimental and control groups; mothers – was coded and given a score of 1.
v2(2, 100) = 0.13, p > 0.05, fathers – v2(2, 100) = 2.43, p > 0.05. The reliability coefficients of four studies on mother-child inter-
In order to control for the socioeconomic status (SES) of sub- actions with kindergarten and school age children were adequate
jects, a SES index was constructed using principal component fac- ranging from 0.54 to 0.95 (Tzuriel, 1999). Similar reliability coeffi-
tor analysis. SES is considered to be an important determinant of cients between 0.85 and 0.95 for the different MLE strategies were
children’s cognitive level (e.g., Espy, Molfese, & Dilalla, 2001; found in the context of peer mediation of school age children
Seifer, 2001; Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D’Onofrio, & (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010) and for infants
Gottesman, 2003; White, 1982) as well as on the parents’ MLE (Klein, 1988). The reliability of the MLE strategies was tested in
strategies (Korat et al., 2008; Tzuriel & Ernst, 1990). The analysis the current study by two experienced raters who were blind to
was based on demographic variables of the parents’ occupation, the study hypotheses. Cronbach alpha coefficients of mother-
education, and living condition (e.g., a ratio of number of rooms child interaction on a sample of 10 pairs for the five MLE strategies
at home by number of family members). The factor analysis were as follows: Intentionality and reciprocity, 0.79; meaning,
revealed one factor with an eigenvalue higher than 1.00 that 0.87; transcendence, 0.86; feelings of competence, 0.86; self-
explained 54% of the variance. Following the factor analysis find- regulation, 0.90.
ings a SES index was calculated for each subject based on a For some of the analyses there was a need to develop a compos-
weighted score of factor loadings  demographic variable. Com- ite measure of mother’s quality of mediation based on all MLE cri-
parison of the experimental and control groups revealed no signif- teria. Since each criterion was found to have differential
icant differences on the SES index, t(95) = 0.17, p > 0.05. significance in predicting cognitive modifiability (e.g., Tzuriel,
1999) a simple average of the criteria would cause a bias. In order
2.2. Measures to construct a modified Quality of Mediation Index (QMI) we asked
five clinicians with expertise in use of the MLE approach to rate
2.2.1. Observation of mediation interaction (OMI) each of the MLE strategies for their contribution to children’s cog-
The OMI (Klein, 1988, 1996; Klein & Alony, 1993; Klein et al., nitive modifiability. Each of the clinicians had at least 15 years of
1987) is a rating scale based on the first five MLE criteria. These cri- research experience. Interclass correlation showed an inter-rater
teria were empirically defined and developed into an observation reliability coefficient of 0.83. The average weighted score for the
scale of parent-child interactions and adapted later for peer inter- different MLE strategies were as followed: Intentionality and
actions (e.g., Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007, 2010), reciprocity (I), 1.60; meaning (M), 2.60; feelings of competence
sibling’s interaction (Klein, Zarur, & Feldman, 2002; Klein et al., (C), 3.06; regulation of behavior (B), 3.20; transcendence (T), 3.60.
2003; Tzuriel & Hanuka-Levy, 2014) and teacher-students’ interac- The QMI was finally computed by multiplying the MLE score of
tions (Tzuriel, 2011b; Tzuriel et al., 1998). The OMI provides a pro- each criterion by the weighted score and dividing it by the total
file of mediation that is based on the five MLE strategies as follows: MLE score. The computation is carried out by the following
Intentionality and Reciprocity (Focusing), Mediation for Meaning, equation:
Mediation for Transcendence, Mediation of Feelings of Competence
P
and Mediation for Regulation of Behavior. ðI  1:60 þ M  2:60 þ C  3:06 þ B  3:20 þ T  3:60Þ
In addition to the five MLE strategies we included a non- QMI ¼ P
ðI þ M þ C þ B þ TÞ
mediation behavior, activation, found to be used significantly less
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 309

The division of the multiplied weighted score by the total sum most holes there is no way of knowing their depth without using
of MLE scores enables the construction of an index that expresses the measuring cylinder.
the quality of mediation relative to its contribution to cognitive The Seria-Think Instrument is administered in a DA mode, using
modifiability while controlling for the amount of mediation in each a preliminary baseline phase followed by pre-teaching, teaching,
MLE strategy. and post-teaching phases. The preliminary baseline phase is aimed
at familiarizing the children with the test materials and the specific
2.2.2. The Seria-Think Instrument rules required to solve the problems. This phase also includes two
The Seria-Think Instrument (Tzuriel, 2000a) is a DA instrument example problems. In the current study the pre-teaching, teaching,
aimed at assessment of process-oriented cognitive behavior in the and post-teaching phases each contained 6 problems. In the pre-
mathematics domain. The test is based on a variety of arithmetic teaching phase the problems required placing the cylinders in an
skills, especially seriating and math operations. The problems of increasing order and in the post-teaching phase in a decreasing
the instrument require cognitive functions such as planning, self- order. In order to solve the problems, children have to apply a
regulation, systematic exploratory behavior, simultaneous consid- four-step strategy (P-M-C-S): (1) predict the height of the cylinder
eration of several sources of information, and need for precision. above the surface level of the block, (2) measure correctly the
The Seria-Think Instrument, 5  5 version (see Fig. 3), is composed depth of the hole, (3) compute the required length of cylinder
of a wooden block, 12 cm  12 cm  10 cm with five rows of holes, (i.e., by adding depth to the predicted height), and (4) select the
five holes in each row, a set of cylinders with various heights, and a correct cylinder (some are longer than the measuring rod). The task
measuring rod divided equally into 11 one-cm units. requires comparing the results not only within each hole but also
In the first row of the block all holes have the same depth between holes. In teaching how to solve the problems, the media-
(1 cm); in the second row the depth of the holes increases progres- tor emphasizes the PMCS four-step strategy. The children are
sively and in the third to fifth rows the depths of the holes is in a mediated for strategies of planning behavior (e.g., preparing the
mixed order. Each task in the Seria-Think involves insertion of the solution outside the holes before inserting the cylinders), restraint
cylinders into the holes so as to get lines of cylinders with either of impulsivity in data gathering, need for precision (e.g., measuring
equal height, regularly increasing height, or regularly decreasing the depth of the hole and the length of cylinder), simultaneously
height above the surface level of the block. Children are asked to considering multiple sources of information (e.g., depth of the hole,
perform the task with as few insertions as possible; however, they required height, length of cylinder), comparative behavior (e.g.,
are allowed to use the measurement rod as much as they wish. comparing the depth of the hole to the required height), and exact
The children are instructed to be as careful as possible to insert computation (e.g., subtracting the depth of the hole from the cylin-
a cylinder in a hole only once. In order to avoid trial-and-error der’s length to get the correct height).
behavior they are encouraged to use the measuring rod as many Responses are recorded according to three criteria: Outcome
times as they need to do. In order to solve the problems, the chil- (accuracy of solution), number of insertions, and number of measure-
dren must first predict the requested height, measure the depth of ments required for solving the problem; the last two are process
holes, calculate the requested length of the cylinder, and choose measures. These scores may reveal qualitative aspects about the
the correct cylinder from a pile of cylinders. The task requires children’s patterns of dealing with the problems. For example, a
not only comparing the depth of holes to the cylinders’ length for child who measures only rarely and makes many unnecessary
each hole but also comparing the result of one hole to the result insertions might be perceived as impulsive compared with a child
of the next hole (and sometimes to results of all other holes) based who makes frequent measurements but fewer insertions, the latter
on the task demands (increasing, decreasing, or equal heights). For considered to be more reflective.
Cronbach alpha reliabilities based on a sample of grade 1 chil-
dren (n = 48) for pre- and post-teaching phases (using a 3  5
holes’ version), respectively, were: Number of measurements
0.37 and 0.66, number of insertions 0.78 and 0.85 (Tzuriel,
2000a). Cronbach alpha reliability for accuracy could not be com-
puted because of a ceiling effect; most subjects reached the correct
solution after the process of inserting and measuring. Cronbach
alpha reliabilities were calculated based on the sample of the cur-
rent study and the 5  5 version of the test. The children received
the test three times--once before the PMYC in a static way (pre-
teaching) and twice after the PMYC as part of the DA procedure
(pre-teaching and post-teaching). Cronbach alpha reliabilities of
accuracy scores for the three administrations were 0.76, 0.86,
and 0.90, respectively. Cronbach alpha reliabilities for the three
administrations of number of insertions were 0.71, 0.73, and
0.82, respectively and for number of measurements, 0.82, 0.88,
and 0.92, respectively. The Seria-Think Instrument was validated
in several studies in Israel (Tzuriel, 2000a) and the Netherlands
(Resing, Tunteler, De Jong, & Bosma, 2009) and is used as a clinical
tool for assessment and intervention.

2.2.3. Analogies subtest from the cognitive modifiability battery (CMB)


The CMB (Tzuriel, 1995, 2000c) is a DA instrument designed for
children from kindergarten to fourth grade. All analogies are based
on four dimensions: Color, height, number, and position. After a
preliminary-baseline phase in which the examinees are familiar-
ized with the main dimensions of the test and the basic rules of
Fig. 3. The Seria-Think Instrument. solving classical type analogies (e.g., A:B :: C:D), the pre-teaching,
310 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

teaching, and post-teaching phases are administered. Scoring of the Cognitive Assessment System (Das & Naglieri, 1997). It was
the Analogies subtest is carried out by giving each correctly solved decided therefore to use these tasks for the mother-child interac-
problem a score of 1; the maximal score for each phase (pre- and tions. Detailed analysis of the children’s sample showed that the
post-teaching) is 10. Cronbach-alpha reliabilities reported by difficulty level of the Children’s Inferential Thinking Modifiability Test
Tzuriel (2000c) were 0.80 and 0.85 for pre- and post-teaching (CITM, Tzuriel, 1989) was most appropriate to tap individual differ-
phases, respectively. Cronbach alpha reliability coefficients based ences. It was decided therefore to use the CITM test as a DA mea-
on the current sample were 0.68, and 0.70 for the pre-teaching sure to be administered to the learners in grade 1.
phase administered before and after the PMYC and post-teaching
phase after the PMYC, respectively. The CMB has been validated 2.4. Procedure and design
in several studies in the UK (e.g., Lauchlan & Elliott, 2001) and in
Israel (Tzuriel, 2000c; Tzuriel & George, 2009; Tzuriel & Shamir, Data gathering was composed of the following steps: (a) video-
2007, 2010) and is used as a clinical tool for assessment and taping of mother-child interactions (n = 100 pairs), with the grade
intervention. 3 children as mediators; (b) pre-intervention tests to mediators (c)
application of the PMYC program to mediators in the experimental
2.2.4. Vocabulary and similarities subtests (WISC-R95) group and an alternative creativity program for mediators in the
In order to control for subjects’ verbal level, two subscales from control group; (d) videotaping of peer mediation interactions
the WISC-R95 (Hebrew version) were administered: Vocabulary (100 pairs); (e) post-intervention DA of the mediators in grade 3.
and similarities. The WISC-R95 (Wechsler, 1995) is a conventional The design of the study portraying also the procedure is presented
intelligence test for children at ages of 6–16 years. We chose only in Fig. 2.
these two subtests as they proved to represent the verbal ability of
children more than other subtests from the verbal domain of the (a) Videotaping of mother-child interactions was carried out at
WISC-R95 (Jensen & Reynolds, 1982; Meesters, van Gastel, Ghys, the children’s homes. Mothers were asked to teach their
& Merckelbach, 1998). children two structured cognitive tasks, analogical problems
from the Children’s Conceptual and Perceptual Analogical Mod-
2.2.5. Children’s inferential thinking modifiability (CITM) test ifiability (CCPAM) - Construction Analogies test (Tzuriel, 2002,
The CITM (Tzuriel, 1989, 2001) is aimed at assessing young chil- 2007) and Non-Verbal Matrices from the Cognitive Assessment
dren’s ability to solve problems that require inferential thinking as Scale (Das & Naglieri, 1997), both tasks requiring higher-
well as their ability to modify their performance following a teach- order abstract thinking. Although the CCPAM Construction
ing phase. The CITM was used in the current study only as a tool for Analogies test is composed of two sets of problems, concep-
teaching the inferential tasks to the learners. The task requires sys- tual and perceptual, the mothers were asked to teach their
tematic exploratory behavior, control of impulsivity, spontaneous children only the nine conceptual problems. The Non-
comparative behavior, planning, inferential–hypothetical (‘‘iffy”) Verbal Matrices are composed of 1 training problem and 11
thinking, and simultaneous consideration of multiple sources of test problems. For each problem there were 6 alternatives
information. Cronbach-alpha reliability coefficients of the pre- to choose from at the bottom of the page.
and post-teaching phases are 0.82 and 0.82, respectively (Tzuriel,
1989). The CITM validity were established in several developmen- Each mother received instructions (both written and oral) about
tal and educational studies (Samuels & Tzuriel, 1998; Tzuriel, the task and two training items for each task before starting the
1989; Tzuriel & Eran, 1990; Tzuriel & Kaufman, 1999; Tzuriel & interaction. The instructions included the problem-solving rules
Weiss, 1998). and the solution but not how to teach her child. During instructions
the child was in another room and was not exposed to them. The
2.3. Pilot study interactions lasted between 15 and 30 min; roughly equal time
was given for each task. In order to analyze equal time sections
A pilot study was carried out with two goals in mind: (a) to for each dyad, only 15 min from each interaction were observed
examine adequate types of tasks for mother-child interactions in (7.5 min of each task). At the end of the interaction session each
both free-play and structured situations, (b) to examine the type mother answered a demographic information questionnaire. Fol-
of dynamic assessment measure that is appropriate for the teach- lowing the mother-child videotape session each interaction was
ing phase of the peer mediation. Since the teaching phase is given rated by the OMI rating scale. Each mother-child interaction was
by 3rd grade children to their peers in 1st grade, it was important then observed and rated for the frequency of using each MLE strat-
to find out the adequacy of the teaching process as related to level egy. On the basis of these ratings mothers were then assigned to
of complexity and abstraction of the test, smoothness and fluency low- versus high-mediating groups using the Quality of Mediation
of teaching. There were two samples in the pilot study. The first Index (QMI). The QMI was constructed in this study to reflect the
sample was composed of mother-child dyads (n = 10), the age of relative weight of each MLE strategy in terms of its importance
the children being 8–9 years. Each dyad was videotaped during to cognitive modifiability of children (see Measures). Mothers
15 min of a free-play situation (e.g., memory game, puzzle, letters in each of the low- and high-mediation groups were further
game, magic games) and 15 min of a structured situation (e.g., cog- divided randomly into experimental (n = 49) and control (n = 51)
nitive analogy tasks using figural and pictorial elements). The sec- groups.
ond sample was composed of 6 grade 1 children (n = 6) children
ages 6–7 years. The children were given two dynamic assessment (b) All mediators (grade 3) were given the Vocabulary and Sim-
measures. ilarities subscales of the WISC-R and the pre-teaching tests
Detailed analysis of the mother-child interactions revealed that of the Seria-Think and CMB Analogies (Time A). All learners
the structured situation triggered more verbal interactions than were given the CITM pre-teaching test (Time A). Testing was
did the free-play situation and allowed better observation of the carried out individually in two 45-min sessions. All tests
MLE strategies used. In addition, we found that the best tasks elic- were administered by a team of graduate students who were
iting interactions were the Construction Analogies of the Children’s trained for 15 h in administering the tests. The data gathered
Conceptual and Perceptual Analogical Modifiability Test-Construction in this phase were compared later with post-intervention
Analogies (Tzuriel, 2002) and the Non-Verbal Matrices Test from data.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 311

(c) The PMYC program was applied with the experimental tion of a full DA at the end of the intervention allowed eval-
mediators (see PMYC intervention program) during three uation of the effect of intervention not only on static
weeks whereas the control group received an alternative performance but also on the cognitive modifiability or
program aimed at training of creativity; no mediation prin- ‘‘learning to learn” capacity that could have been changed
ciples were taught in this group. The creativity program by exposure to the PMYC program. Since one of the declared
was applied in small groups of 8–10 children. This alterna- goals of The PMYC program is to change thinking processes,
tive program was chosen because it did not focus on media- a novel DA approach should be adapted focusing on assess-
tion principles though it contributes to children’s cognitive ment of the modifiability of the cognitive process per se
development. The PMYC program was administered by rather than merely outcome (Tzuriel, 2011a, 2011c).
expert trainer during 7 45-min sessions; the alternative cre-
ativity program was also administered by the same expert The pre-teaching test was administered to the mediators indi-
trainer for the same amount of time as the experimental vidually during one session of 1 h. The teaching and post-
group. The creativity program was based on the Young Thin- teaching phases were administered during two sessions of
ker Program (Ganihar-Raz & Cahana, 1997) included topics 45 min each; the sessions were consecutive with a short break
such as brain storming, flexible search of solutions, critical between them. The teaching and post-teaching phases were
thinking, and creative solution of conflicts. At the end of each administered between 4 and 8 weeks after the pre-teaching phase.
program all mediators were instructed, for 1 h, on the CITM The tests were administered by trained examiners with previous
tasks; the CITM tasks were then used for teaching the learn- experience.
ers in the next step of the peer-mediation session. It should
be emphasized that instruction included understanding of
3. Results
the rules for solution but not the specific mediation strate-
gies for teaching them. The purpose was to observe how
3.1. Effects of the peer mediation for young children (PMYC) program
the mediators in the experimental and control groups apply
on mediators’ MLE strategies and activation behavior and on learners’
mediation strategies in a new context of teaching their peers
responsiveness to peers’ mediation
the CITM problems.
(d) For each mediator in grade 3 a learner from grade 1 was
The purpose of presenting the effects of the PMYC program on
assigned. The mediators in both the experimental and con-
MLE strategies in a peer mediation interaction is to replicate find-
trol groups were asked to teach the learners nine items from
ings of earlier studies (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Shamir et al., 2006,
the teaching phase of the CITM. The teaching sessions were
2007; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2010). Although these effects have been
videotaped for 20–30 min between 2 and 15 days from the
shown previously we wanted first to establish the effects of the
end of the intervention program. Before starting the video-
PMYC before comparing it to the effects of mother-child MLE
taping session each mediator received an instruction page
interactions.
with a summary of the rules for solving the CITM problems,
and recall of rules was verified. Only the first 20-min period
of the video was taken later for analyzing the peer- 3.1.1. Mediators’ MLE strategies
mediation. The OMI, adapted for peer-mediation by Shamir The effects of PMYC program on MLE strategies of mediators
and Tzuriel (2004), was used to obtain ratings of peer- was examined by one-way MANOVA of treatment with the five
mediated interactions. The children’s mediation strategies MLE strategies as dependent variables. The findings revealed a sig-
were later compared to the mother-child strategies, using nificant main effect of treatment, F(5, 92) = 7.50, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.29,
the same observation parameters and technique. indicating higher mediation scores in the experimental group than
in the control group, thus supporting partially expectation 1. Uni-
Given that the time interval between the end of the program variate analysis showed that the mediation level of the experimen-
and the peer-interaction session varied across individual dyads, tal group was significantly derived from mediation for
we calculated Pearson correlation coefficients between MLE strate- transcendence and mediation of feelings of competence. The
gies and number of days from end of program to the peer interac- means, standard deviations and the univariate statistics for each
tion session. The correlations ranged between 0.00 and 0.09 (ns) for MLE strategy are presented in Table 1.
the different mediation categories, allowing us to ignore the inter- The standard deviations were high relative to the means, a find-
val variable. ing that might cause bias in the analyses; therefore, we also used
Mann-Whitney U nonparametric analyses. The findings showed,
(e) All mediators at this stage were given two DA measures: The similar to the parametric analysis, that mediators in the experi-
CMB Analogies and the Seria-Think Instrument. The DA mea- mental group scored higher than did mediators in the control
sures included all three phases of the test: Pre-teaching group on both mediation for transcendence, Z = 3.35, p < 0.001,
(Time B), teaching, and post-teaching (Time C). Administra- and mediation for feelings of competence, Z = 4.83, p < 0.001.

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and univariate analyses of mediators’ MLE strategies in the experimental and control groups.

MLE strategies Experimental Control F(1, 96) g2


M (SD) M (SD)
Intentionality & Reciprocity 12.60 (11.76) 9.68 (8.33) 2.03
Meaning 9.25 (7.92) 8.04 (9.16) 0.49
Transcendence 7.69 (6.57) 3.62 (3.78) 14.24*** 0.13
Feelings of Competence 23.25 (9.46) 13.42 (8.39) 29.68*** 0.24
Regulation of Behavior 6.42 (4.98) 5.50 (5.13) 0.81
***
p < 0.001.
312 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

Similar findings were found after controlling for the mediators’ assume that mothers will show higher levels of MLE strategies
socio-economic level, verbal ability (e.g., WISC-R Vocabulary and than their children. On one hand it is plausible to assume that
Similarities), and learners’ cognitive level (as determined by the mothers would show higher level of MLE strategies when interact-
pre-teaching CITM score), F(5, 82) = 7.14, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.30. Uni- ing with their children than those revealed by their children when
variate analysis showed that treatment differences derived mainly interacting with their peers. It is due probably to their rich experi-
from mediation for transcendence, F(1, 86) = 15.63, p < 0.001, ence, and deep involvement and sense of responsibility toward
g2 = 0.15 and mediation for feelings of competence, their children. On the other hand, children participating in a struc-
F(1, 86) = 23.12, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.21. tured program aimed at enhancement of mediation strategies such
as the PMYC might develop a qualitative style of teaching and
3.1.2. Mediators’ activation behavior in the experimental and control reveal higher MLE strategies than their mothers, in spite of their
groups young age.
In order to determine whether activation behavior was different This question was examined by repeated-measures MANOVA of
in the two research groups we carried out independent t-tests with treatment (experimental versus control) by mediation agent
Treatment as independent variable. The control group (M = 1.94, (mother versus child) and the five MLE strategies the dependent
SD = 3.57) showed significantly higher activation score than did variables. We included in the analysis a category of Activation
the experimental group (M = 0.63, SD = 1.52), t(96) = 2.39, p < 0.05. (i.e., showing the solution rather than mediating). The activation
behavior was found in earlier studies as contradicting mediation
3.1.3. Learners’ responsiveness to peers’ mediation strategies and negatively related to cognitive modifiability (e.g.,
According to expectation 3 the learners’ responsiveness to Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Tzuriel & Hanuka-Levy, 2014).
mediation of mediators in the experimental group should be The analysis revealed a significant interaction of treat-
higher than responsiveness to mediation in the control group. ment  mediation agent, F(6, 90) = 4.31, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.22. The
The learners’ responsiveness was measured by the five MLE strate- means, standard deviations, and the repeated measures results
gies used in the interaction. For example, when a mediator asked for each MLE strategy are presented in Table 3. Three significant
for the color of an object and the learner responded, it was coded interactions of treatments by mediation agent were found for three
as mediation for meaning from the learner’s perspective. In the MLE strategies: Intentionality and Reciprocity, Transcendence, and
interest of clarity, the learners are labeled as experimental or con- Feelings of Competence. These interactions are portrayed in Figs. 4–
trol only because they were matched to their peers in the experi- 6, respectively. The findings show that in the experimental group
mental or control groups. In spite of the fact that learners did not children showed consistently higher mediation than mothers
receive any intervention we expected that learners in the experi- whereas in the control group mothers showed higher mediation
mental group would demonstrate greater responsiveness than than children.
would their control counterparts. The type of responsiveness cate- Simple main effect analyses on the interactions showed that in
gories matched the MLE strategies: Reciprocity, meaning, and tran- the experimental group children received significantly higher
scendence. Because of lack of responsiveness to mediation of mediation scores on Intentionality and Reciprocity, F(1, 47) = 6.87,
feelings of competence and regulation of behavior (i.e., learners p < 0.05, g2 = 0.13 (Fig. 4) and on Feelings of Competence,
usually do not try to give their mediators feelings of competence F(1, 47) = 22.47, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.32 (Fig. 5) than mothers. However,
or regulate the mediators’ behavior) these strategies were not mothers and children showed about equal scores on mediation for
included in the analysis. A one-way MANOVA of treatment was Transcendence, F(1, 47) = 0.88, ns (Fig. 6). Similar comparisons
carried out with the three categories as dependent variables. There between mothers and children in the control group reveal no sig-
was a significant main effect of treatment, F(1, 94) = 7.69, p < 0.001, nificant differences for Intentionality and Reciprocity,
g2 = 0.20. The means, standard deviations and the univariate statis- F(1, 49) = 2.35, p < 0.05 and for Feelings of Competence,
tics for each MLE strategy are presented in Table 2. As can be seen F(1, 49) = 1.62, p > 0.05, whereas in mediation for Transcendence
in Table 2, the results support expectation 2. Learners in the exper- mothers show higher scores than children, F(1, 49) = 15.06,
imental group showed greater use of mediation strategies than did p < 0.001, g2 = 0.24. Mann-Whitney U nonparametric analyses
learners in the control group. The differences were significant in were carried out separately for the experimental and control
mediation for meaning and mediation for transcendence. Because groups in two MLE strategies (due to the relatively high SD): Inten-
of the large standard deviations relative to the means we con- tionality/Reciprocity and Transcendence. The findings showed that,
ducted non-parametric analyses. Mann-Whitney U tests showed similar to the parametric analysis, in the experimental group chil-
similar significant differences for mediation for meaning, Z = 2.41, dren scored higher than mothers on Intentionality and Reciprocity,
p < 05, and mediation for transcendence, Z = 3.02, p < 0.05. Z = 1.97, p < 0.05. No significant results were found for mediation
for Transcendence, Z = 1.00, p > 0.05. In the control group, however,
mothers showed about the same scores as their children on Inten-
3.2. The interaction effect of mother-child MLE strategies and PMYC
tionality and Reciprocity, Z = 1.89, p > 0.05, but higher mediation
program on children’s MLE strategies
for Transcendence than their children, Z = 3.39, p < 0.001.
In summary, children participating in the PMYC program show
An interesting question of the current study was whether moth-
higher mediation strategies than their mothers whereas children
ers will demonstrate higher MLE strategies than their children. We

Table 2
Means, standard deviations, and univariate analyses of learners’ responsiveness for mediation in the experimental and control groups.

MLE criteria Experimental Control F(1, 96) g2


M (SD) M (SD)
Reciprocity 12.55 (10.15) 10.77 (7.46) 0.99
Meaning 13.64 (9.87) 8.55 (9.06) 7.05** 0.07
Transcendence 0.93 (1.92) 0.28 (.64) 5.10* 0.05
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 313

Table 3
Means, standard deviations, and statistics of repeated measures MANOVA of MLE strategies by treatment by mediation agent.

MLE strategies Experimental Control Treatment  Mediation


Agent
Mother Child Mother Child
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(1, 96) g2
**
Intentionality & Reciprocity 7.60 (4.09) 12.60 (11.76) 11.90 (6.47) 9.68 (8.33) 9.18 0.09
Meaning 9.73 (4.73) 9.25 (7.92) 9.48 (4.60) 8.04 (9.16) 0.25
Transcendence 6.60 (5.37) 7.69 (6.57) 6.62 (3.84) 3.62 (3.78) 8.73** 0.08
Feelings of Competence 16.54 (6.26) 23.25 (9.46) 15.36 (6.24) 13.43 (8.39) 17.20*** 0.15
Regulation of Behavior 5.94 (3.52) 6.42 (4.98) 5.14 (3.22) 5.50 (5.13) 0.01
Activation 0.19 (.57) 0.63 (1.52) 0.44 (1.21) 1.94 (3.57) 4.18* 0.04
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.

14.5 9
Mother Child Mother Child
Intentionality & Reciprocity

8
12.5

Transcendence
7

10.5
6

5
8.5

4
6.5
Experimental Control 3
Group Experimental Control
Group
Fig. 4. Mediation for intentionality and reciprocity in mother-child interaction in
the experimental and control groups. Fig. 6. Mediation for transcendence of mothers in mother-child interaction and in
siblings’ interaction in the experimental and control groups.

24 Mother Child
2.4 Mother Child
Feelings of Competence

22
2
Activation

20 1.6

1.2
18

0.8
16
0.4

14 0
Experimental Control
12 Group
Experimental Control
Fig. 7. Activation behavior in mother-child interaction and in siblings’ interaction
Group in the experimental and control groups

Fig. 5. Mediation for feelings of competence in mother-child interaction in the


experimental and control groups. Z = 3.01, p < 0.01. Among mediators the frequency of activation as
expected was higher in the control than in the experimental group,
who had not learned how to mediate show either equal or lower F(1, 96) = 5.54, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.06. No significant differences were
mediation strategies than their mothers. found between mothers in the experimental and control groups,
Analysis of Activation scores reveal a significant interaction of F(1, 96) = 1.71, p = p > 0.05.
treatment  mediation agent, F(1, 96) = 4.18, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.04.
The interaction is depicted in Fig. 7. Simple main effect analyses 3.3. To what extent children’s quality of mediation is determined by
on the interaction of activation showed that children received sig- the mothers’ quality of mediation as compared with the effects of the
nificantly higher scores than their mothers in both the experimen- PMYC program?
tal, F(1, 47) = 5.15, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.10 and the control, F(1, 49) = 10.01,
p < 0.01, g2 = 0.17 groups. Nonparametric analyses showed similar One of the intriguing questions of this study is to what degree
findings in the experimental, Z = 2.62, p < 0.01 and control groups, there is a similarity between mothers’ and children’s MLE strate-
314 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

gies and to what extent participating in an intervention program by the scores on Time C after controlling for measures on Times A
aimed at enhancing the MLE strategies would change the correla- and B, in a MANCOVA or ANCOVA analysis.
tion pattern. We studied these questions using two types of statis-
tical analyses: Pearson correlation and hierarchical regression
3.4.1. Process- oriented measures
analysis.
The Seria-Think Instrument yields two process-oriented mea-
sures, number of insertions and number of measurements, and one
3.3.1. Pearson correlations outcome-oriented score. The effect of the PMYC program on cogni-
Pearson correlations carried out between mothers’ and chil- tive modifiability was analyzed by a one-way MANCOVA in which
dren’s MLE strategies showed that the Quality of Mediation Index the dependent variables were the scores in Time C (post-
(QMI, see Method) of mothers was significantly correlated with intervention, post-teaching) and the covariates were measure-
children’s QMI in the experimental group, r = 0.37, p < 0.01, but ments in Time A (pre-intervention, pre-teaching) and Time B
not in the control group, r = 0.04, ns (Fisher Z = 2.06, p < 0.05), (post-intervention, pre-teaching). The findings indicate that, in
thus supporting expectation 3. Similarly, mediation for feelings of general, the children in the experimental group improved their
competence was significantly correlated in the experimental functioning significantly as compared with children in the control
group, r = 0.31, p < 0.05, but not in the control group, r = 0.07, group, F(3, 90) = 3.29, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.10. The means and standard
ns (Fisher Z = 1.87, p < 0.05). deviations and univariate analyses of the Seria-Think measures
are shown in Table 5. As can be seen in Table 5, the significant
improvement was attributable mainly to the number of insertions
3.3.2. Hierarchical regression analysis (see Fig. 8) and the number of measurements (see Fig. 9), thus sup-
In order to answer the above question, we applied a four-step porting expectation 4. As expected, no significant differences were
hierarchical regression analysis in which the criterion (predicted) found in the performance score of the Seria-Think Instrument. It
variable was the children’s QMI score (see Table 4). In step I we should be noted that most children reach almost maximal perfor-
entered the verbal ability score to control for its possible effects mance at the end of the process.
on mediation. Verbal ability was computed as the sum of scaled As can be seen in Fig. 8 the number of insertions decreased
scores of the Vocabulary and Similarities subtests. In step II and slightly from Time A to Time B; the experimental group was not
III the variables of training in mediation (PMYC program) treat- significantly different from the control group in Time A,
ment and the mothers’ QMI were entered, respectively. In step IV t(98) = 0.10, p > 0.05 or in Time B, t(98) = 0.97, p > 0.05; however,
the interactions between the predicting variables were entered from Time B to Time C, the children in the experimental group
using a stepwise method. Mothers’ QMI added only 3% to the chil- showed a steeper decrease in number of insertions than did chil-
dren’s QMI as compared to 20% contributed by training for media- dren in the control group, t(98) = 2.98, p < 0.01.
tion. The meaning of this finding is that participating in the PMYC Fig. 9 shows that the number of measurements increased
program had a much stronger effect on the children’s quality of medi- slightly from Time A to Time B. The experimental group was not
ation than did the mothers’ quality of mediation. The significant significantly different from the control group in Time A,
interaction of treatment  mothers’ QMI indicates that a positive t(98) = 0.09, p > 0.05 or in Time B, t(98) = 0.07, p > 0.05; however,
and significant relation between mothers’ QMI and children’s from Time B to Time C, children in the experimental group showed
QMI exists only in the experimental group (r = 0.37, p < 0.01) but a greater increase in number of measurements than did children in
not in the control group (r = 0.04, p > 0.05). In other words, the the control group, t (98) = 1.71, p < 0.05.
PMYC intervention program enhanced the correspondence In summary, the experimental group showed after the interven-
between mothers’ mediation and their children’s mediation. tion program an improvement from pre-teaching (Time B) to post-
teaching (Time C) that was greater than that shown by the control
group in both process-oriented measures. The improvement was
3.4. The effect of the PMYC program on children’s cognitive
revealed after a short teaching phase given within the DA proce-
modifiability in process-oriented and outcome-oriented measures
dure and is considered as indicative of cognitive modifiability.
Both measures (Analogies and Seria-Think) were administered
three times: Before the intervention (Time A) and after the inter- 3.4.2. Outcome-oriented measure (analogies)
vention (Time B and Time C). Times B and C were parts of the DA The Analogies scores were analyzed by a one-way ANCOVA in
Pre- and post-teaching, respectively. This design allows not only which the independent variable was treatment (experimental ver-
assessment of pre- to post-intervention changes but also improve- sus control), the dependent variable was the Analogies score at
ment on DA at the end of the intervention. The DA findings are con- Time C (e.g., post-teaching, post-intervention), and the covariates
sidered to be an accurate measure of cognitive modifiability were the Analogies scores in Time A and Time B. The findings
following the intervention program (Feuerstein et al., 2002; revealed a significant main effect of treatment, F(1, 96) = 4.37,
Tzuriel, 2001, 2011a, 2011c). Cognitive modifiability was assessed p < 0.05, g2 = 0.04, indicating that children in the experimental

Table 4
Hierarchical regression analysis of children’s QMI by treatment and mothers’ QMI.

Predictors Step I (b) Step II (b) Step III (b) Step IV (b)
Verbal Ability 0.18* 0.14 0.14 0.14
Treatment 0.45*** 0.45*** 0.44***
Mothers’ QMI 0.16* 0.14
Treatment  Mothers’ QMI 0.19*
R2 0.03* 0.23*** 0.26*** 0.29***
DR2 0.03* 0.20*** 0.03* 0.03*
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 315

Table 5
Means and standard deviations of the number of insertions, number of measurements, and performance scores on the Seria-Think Instrument in the experimental and control
groups before and after the PMYC program.

Experimental Control F(1, 92) g2


M (SD) M (SD)
Number of Insertions
Time A 70.92 (33.19) 71.53 (26.34) 8.18** 0.08
Time B 59.92 (22.22) 64.37 (23.84)
Time C 39.92 (14.21) 49.57 (17.86)
Number of Measurements
Time A 26.04 (21.76) 25.67 (22.05) 5.39* 0.06
Time B 27.41 (17.34) 27.16 (18.31)
Time C 45.88 (23.40) 38.76 (17.93)
Performance
Time A 29.16 (2.38) 29.45 (1.77) 0.49
Time B 29.51 (1.80) 29.55 (1.59)
Time C 29.57 (1.93) 29.43 (2.44)

Note: A = Pre-teaching/Pre-intervention, B = Pre-teaching/Post-intervention, C = Post-teaching/Post-Intervention.


*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

75 BEFORE PMYC AFTER PMYC BEFORE PMYC AFTER PMYC

Experimental 5.5
Number of Insertions

65 Control
Analogies Score

55
4.5

45
Experimental

Control

35 3.5
A B C A B C

Time Time

Fig. 10. CMB analogies scores in the experimental and control groups before and
Fig. 8. Number of insertions in the Seria-Think Instrument in the experimental and
after the PMYC program.
control groups.

BEFOR PMYC AFTER PMYC C than did the control group, thus indicating greater cognitive
modifiability and supporting expectation 4. It should be empha-
44
Number of Measurements

sized that the teaching phase of the DA procedure was similar


for both the experimental and the control groups.

3.5. Can the PMYC program compensate for low mother-child MLE
strategies?
34
The major objective of the current study was to examine how
the cognitive modifiability of children who had not benefitted from
Experimental
adequate mother-child MLE strategies is enhanced as a result of
intervention for peer mediation. We anticipated a significant inter-
Control
action between mothers’ level of mediation and treatment effects
24
A B C (experimental versus control) on children’s cognitive modifiability.
Time More specifically, the differences between the experimental and
control groups should be larger among children of mothers with
Fig. 9. Number of measurements in the Seria-Think Instrument in the experimental low use of MLE strategies than among children of mothers with
and the control groups. high MLE strategies. This expectation was examined separately in
outcome-oriented (CMB Analogies) and process-oriented (Seria-
group achieved higher Analogies scores after the intervention than Think) measures.
did children in the control group (see Fig. 10). The means and stan-
dard deviations of the Analogies scores are presented in Table 6. 3.5.1. CMB analogies
Fig. 10 shows that experimental and control groups improved In order to test this expectation, we carried out ANCOVA of
their performance to about the same extent from Time A to Time treatment by mothers’ level of mediation (2  2), where the depen-
B, but the experimental group improved more from Time B to Time dent variable was the CMB Analogies score at time C (post-teach
316 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

Table 6
Means and standard deviations of the CMB analogies scores by mothers’ quality of mediation, treatment and time.

Mothers’ Quality of Mediation Experimental Control


Low High Total Low High Total
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Time A 4.00 (1.77) 3.26 (1.81) 3.65 (1.81) 3.61 (1.62) 4.21 (2.32) 3.94 (2.03)
Time B 4.42 (1.90) 4.04 (1.77) 4.24 (1.83) 3.96 (1.66) 4.21 (2.32) 4.41 (1.69)
Time C 6.12 (2.63) 5.00 (2.30) 5.59 (2.52) 4.04 (2.16) 5.64 (1.70) 4.92 (2.07)

Note: A = Pre-teaching/Pre-intervention, B = Pre-teaching/Post-intervention, C = Post-teaching/Post-Intervention.

ing/post-intervention) and the covariates were the CMB Analogies insertions and number of measurements at Time C (post-teach
scores at times A (pre-teaching/pre-intervention) and B (pre-teach ing/post-intervention) and the covariates were the same variables
ing/post-intervention). The analysis revealed a significant interac- at Time A (pre-teaching/pre-intervention) and Time B (pre-teach
tion, F(1, 94) = 5.69, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.06. In Table 6 we present the ing/post-intervention). The findings showed no significant main
means and standard deviations of the CMB Analogies in Times A, effect for mothers’ MLE level, F(3, 88) = 0.63, p > 0.05, nor any inter-
B, and C. The interaction is portrayed in Fig. 11. Simple main effects action of treatment by mothers’ MLE level was found, F(3, 88) = 0.35,
analysis for each group of mothers’ level of mediation shows the p > 0.05. These results confirm expectation 5 for the outcome-
following. In the group of low-mediating mothers, cognitive mod- oriented measure (CMB Analogies) but not for the process-
ifiability of children in the experimental group was higher as com- oriented measure (Seria-Think Instrument).
pared with children from the control group, F(1, 45) = 8.79, p < 0.01,
g2 = 0.16. In the group of high-mediating mothers no significant 3.6. Structural equation modeling: the effects of proximal factors on
differences were found between the experimental and control mediators’ and learners’ cognitive modifiability
groups, F(1, 47) = 0.23, p > 0.05.
Simple main effects analyses were carried out separately for the We examined the effects of treatment and mothers’ QMI on
experimental and control groups. The findings revealed that in the mediators’ QMI, and the effect of mothers’ QMI on children’s cog-
low-mediating mothers group children in the experimental group nitive modifiability. More specifically we were interested to find
showed significantly higher cognitive modifiability than did chil- out the relative effects of all variables on children’s cognitive mod-
dren in the control group, F(1, 45) = 8.79, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.16. In the ifiability and whether mediators’ QMI serves as a mediating vari-
high-mediating mothers group no significant differences were able between treatment and mothers’ QMI and the outcome
found between the experimental and control groups, measure of children’s cognitive modifiability. The schematic mod-
F(1, 47) = 0.23, p > 0.05. The picture is different in the control group. els of the SEM analysis are presented above in Fig. 1.
As can be seen in Fig. 11 children of high-mediating mothers in the The SEM analysis is estimated by the measurement error and
control group showed higher cognitive modifiability as compared path coefficients as measured by the maximum likelihood (ML)
with children of low- mediating mothers, F(1, 47) = 5.40, p < 0.05, method. Testing the fitness of the theoretical model to the data
g2 = 0.10. was carried out by a procedure suggested by Steiger (1990) and
Kline (2011). Testing the fitness of a model to empirical findings
3.5.2. Seria-Think Instrument is usually carried out by several indices of ML measures: Goodness
The interaction effects of treatment and mothers’ mediation of Fit Index (GFI), Comparative Fitness Index (CFI, Bentler, 1990),
level on children’s cognitive modifiability was examined also on Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the statistic
the two process-oriented measurements as dependent variables. of v2, and the ratio of v2/df. The model chi-square is the most basic
A MANCOVA of treatment by mother’s Level of mediation (2  2) fit statistic, and it is reported in virtually all reports of SEM analy-
was carried out where the dependent variables were number of ses. The model is considered as fit when the v2/df < 3 and is not

Fig. 11. CMB analogies score by mothers’ mediation level and treatment before and after the PMYC program.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 317

significant. A model is considered also as fit to the data when the bility on number of insertions and number of measurements of
GFI and CFI are higher than 0.95, and the RMSEA is lower than the Seria-Think Instrument, and (c) learners’ cognitive modifiabil-
0.05 (Bollen & Curran, 2006). ity on the CITM test. The findings are presented in Figs. 12–14,
Children’s cognitive modifiability scores were computed for respectively.
each of the DA measures (i.e., mediators’ CMB Analogies and Model 1. The SEM analysis revealed that Model 1 is adequate
Seria-Think number of insertions and number of measurements, thus confirming expectation 6, v2 = 2.69, df = 3, v2/df = 0.90,
and learners’ (CITM) by means of a regression analysis. A cogni- p = 0.44; NFI = 0.916; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.00. As can be seen in
tive modifiability index (CMI) was computed for each score. The Fig. 12 Mothers’ QMI and treatment explain significantly the medi-
CMI was computed by means of regression analysis in which the ators’ QMI (b = 0.16 and b = 0.46, respectively). The mediators’
residual score on post-teaching (Time C) was extracted after con- QMI, in turn, explained significantly the mediators’ cognitive mod-
trolling for the pre-teaching (Time B) score of the dynamic test ifiability on CMB (b = 0.16). The findings support our expectation
given after the intervention. In other words, the residual of the that the L-I proximal factors explain significantly the L-II proximal
post-teaching score was computed after controlling for the factor of mediators’ QMI and that the mediators’ QMI explains sig-
explained variance of the pre-teaching score; the residual values nificantly the mediators’ cognitive modifiability.
represent the unexplained variance left after subtracting the vari- Model 2. The SEM analysis revealed that Model 2 is adequate
ance contributed by the pre-teaching score. This procedure is thus supporting expectation 6, v2 = 4.42, df = 5, v2/df = 0.88,
considered to be a better measure reflecting change than a sim- p = 0.848; NFI = 0.491; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.000. Fig. 13 of Model
ple gain score that has the disadvantage of possible ceiling effect 2, shows that mothers’ QMI and treatment explain significantly
and regression to the mean effect (Embretson, 1992). Thus, we the mediators’ QMI (b = 0.17 and b = 0.46, respectively). The
computed three CMI, one for the CMB Analogies, two for the mediators’ QMI, in turn, explained significantly the mediators’ cog-
Seria-Think measures (i.e., number of insertions and number of nitive modifiability on number of measurements (b = 0.23) and
measurements) and one for the learners’ CITM. Accumulating evi- number of insertions (b = 0.24). As expected the two process-
dence from educational research provides indications that a score oriented outcome measures were significantly related
reflecting individual differences in ‘‘modifiability” added substan- (b = 0.40). The findings confirm expectation 6 according to which
tially to the predictive power of learning (Embretson, 1992) and the L-I proximal factors explain significantly the L-II proximal fac-
future academic success (Haywood & Lidz, 2007; Sternberg & tor of mediators’ QMI.
Grigorenko, 2002; Tzuriel, 2000a, 2000b; Tzuriel, Kaniel, Kanner, Model 3. The third model refers to the effects of the mediation
& Haywood, 1999). strategies used by the mediators on learners’ cognitive modifiabil-
Since mediators’ cognitive modifiability was measured by two ity. The SEM analysis revealed that Model 3 is adequate thus sup-
instruments, performance oriented and process oriented, we car- porting expectation 7, v2 = 0.36, df = 3, v2/df = 1.20, p = 0.948;
ried out separate analyses for each. In order to test our expecta- NFI = 0.99; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.000.
tions, we created three models to be analyzed by SEM analysis. The findings, presented in Fig. 14, show clearly that the proxi-
The factors of mothers’ QMI, Treatment and mediators’ QMI were mal factors of mothers’ QMI and treatment explain significantly
identical in all three models, whereas the outcome variable was the mediators’ QMI (b = 0.17 and b = 0.45, respectively). The
different in each model. The outcome variables in the first, second mediators’ QMI, in turn, explains significantly the learners’ cogni-
and third models were respectively: (a) mediators’ cognitive mod- tive modifiability (b = 0.18). The findings support our expectation
ifiability on the CMB Analogies, (b) mediators’ cognitive modifia- that the L-I proximal factors explain significantly the L-II proximal

Mothers’ QMI

Mediator’s
Cognitive
Mediator’s QMI .16*
Modifiability in
CMB Analogies

Treatment

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

Fig. 12. Structural Equation Model (SEM) analysis: the effects of proximal factors on mediators’ cognitive modifiability in the CMB analogies.
318 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

Mother’s QMI

Mediator’s
Cognitive
Modifiability in
Number of
Measurements

Mediator’s QMI -.40***

Mediator’s
Cognitive
Modifiability in
Number of
Insertions

Treatment

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001


Fig. 13. Structural Equation Model (SEM) analysis: the effects of proximal factors on mediators’ cognitive modifiability in number of insertions and number of measurements
of the Seria-Think Instrument.

factor of mediators’ QMI and that the mediators’ QMI explain sig- Caregivers program (MISC, Klein, 1992, 2003; Lifshitz & Klein,
nificantly the learners’ cognitive modifiability. 2007). The MISC program is based on the first five criteria of Feuer-
In summary, all three models support our expectations regard- stein’s MLE theory and is aimed at improving the quality of medi-
ing the effects of proximal factors on children’s cognitive modifia- ation of early age caregivers. The researchers reported that
bility. In general, mother’s mediation strategies and the treatment mothers in the experimental group showed a significant improve-
(PMYC program) are the main factors affecting directly the chil- ment in mediation for Transcendence three years after the end of
dren’s (mediators) MLE strategies whereas children’s cognitive the program whereas mothers in the control group showed a
modifiability (of either mediators or learners) is directly affected decrease. Tzuriel and Shamir (2010) studied the effectiveness of
by the quality of mediators’ MLE strategies. the PMYC with a sample of 78 dyads of Grade 3 mediators who
taught kindergarten learners analogies and math problems. Medi-
ators in the experimental group showed significantly higher medi-
4. Discussion ation for Transcendence than did mediators in the control group
when teaching math. Mediators in the experimental group showed
4.1. The effect of the PMYC program on children’s MLE strategies also higher mediation of Feelings of Competence than did media-
tors in the control group in teaching both analogies and math. It
The findings reveal that children participating in the PMYC pro- seems that experimental mediators internalized the mediational
gram used higher levels of mediation than the control children, in teaching style and applied it later when are asked to mediate to
each of the five MLE strategies. However, significant differences others. It seems that in spite of the fact that the MLE criteria are
were found in two MLE strategies: Mediation for Transcendence abstract in nature, it is possible to translate them into manageable
and Mediation for Feelings of Competence (Table 1). These findings behavioral referents that can be usefully applied even with young
coincide with the emphasis given in the PMYC program on gener- children.
alized rules and principles of MLE and on rewarding the learner for These findings confirm partially expectation 1 and coincide
attempts to solve the problems given. Similar findings were with previous findings demonstrating the effectiveness of the
reported by Klein and Alony (1993) on infants and by Tzuriel and PMYC in eliciting mediation strategies in young children (Shamir
Shamir (2010) on school age children. & Tzuriel, 2004; Shamir et al., 2006, 2007; Tzuriel & Shamir,
Klein and Alony (1993) studied a sample of mothers of 1-year- 2010). It is intriguing to find out that the two MLE strategies found
old infants who received the Mediational Intervention for Sensitizing as most discriminating between the two groups were Transcen-
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 319

Mothers’ QMI

Learner’s
Cognitive
Mediator’s QMI .18*
Modifiability in
CITM

Treatment

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

Fig. 14. Structural Equation Model (SEM) analysis: the effects of proximal factors on learners’ cognitive modifiability in the CITM test.

dence and Feelings of Competence. The ratio between the control whereas the learners’ exposure to mediation was carried out indi-
and the experimental groups for these two strategies in the current rectly, only during the peer-mediation phase. It should be noted
study were, respectively: 1:2.12 and 1:1.73. Similarly, in three ear- also that the analysis was carried out only on three MLE strategies
lier studies (Shamir & Tzuriel, 2004; Shamir et al., 2006; Tzuriel & of the learners: Reciprocity, Meaning, and Transcendence. Natu-
Shamir, 2010) it was reported that mediation for Transcendence rally, the learners did not tend to self-regulate the behaviors of
and Mediation for Feelings of Competence were found with the their mediators or provide them with feelings of competence.
highest ratio between the control and the experimental groups. Responsiveness in these two categories was almost zero and there-
These findings shed light on the impact of the PMYC program in fore excluded from analysis. Similar findings were reported in an
eliciting mediation strategies of young children. Young children earlier study where learners in the experimental group showed
might show spontaneously, in their interaction with their peers, higher level of responsiveness only in strategies of Meaning and
some indications for adequate mediation, as evidenced in the con- Transcendence than the children in the control group (Shamir &
trol group. However, an intervention aimed at teaching MLE prin- Tzuriel, 2004).
ciples in a structured way and practicing them during short and
supervised intervention (seven sessions) reveals a dramatic change 4.2. The interaction effect of mother-child MLE strategies and PMYC
in their mediational teaching style. The children used more princi- program on children’s MLE strategies
ples and generalizations, offered more mediations relating to rules
and insights (Transcendence) and used more rewarding and feel- The question whether there are differences between mothers’
ings of success (competence) with their peers. Consequently, these and children’s MLE strategies in relation to the effects of the PMYC
children acted less for their partners as compared with their con- program are of importance because the answer to this question
trol counterparts. would provide an indication for cross generational transmission
The results indicate also that children in the control group, of MLE strategies. We expected that mothers will naturally show
devoid of the mediational repertoire acquired during the interven- a higher level of mediation than their children. Young children
tion, used significantly higher level of activation than the experi- do not have the opportunities, the experience, the language skills,
mental group (i.e., the mediator shows or tells the answer to the and the awareness to the importance of mediation as compared
learner rather than elicits a response leading to problem solving with their mothers. However, an intervention for mediation with
behavior). Children in the control group used the time given for children might change the pattern of differences by tapering the
teaching their peers to activate them rather than scaffold their mother-child difference. The findings indicate that in the control
behavior in a mediational way. group, as expected, mothers showed significantly higher level of
The learners’ responsiveness to mediation was evident in two mediation than their children only in mediation for Transcendence
MLE strategies: Meaning and Transcendence. In both strategies whereas in the experimental group children showed superiority
learners in the experimental group showed higher responsiveness over mothers though not in a significant way (Table 3 and Fig. 6).
than learners in the control group (Table 2), thus confirming expec- Mediation for Transcendence requires a relatively higher level of
tation 2. This finding is of special importance as it indicates that abstraction and verbal ability than other MLE strategies. The find-
learners in the experimental group benefit from an interaction ings in the control group are explained by the fact that mothers
with qualified peers (mediators) by showing higher level of who have more experience and higher abstraction skills than their
responsiveness. One should bear in mind that the intervention in children would use this strategy more frequently than their chil-
this study was applied to the mediators in the experimental group dren. However, it is fascinating to see that in the experimental
320 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

group children showed the same level of mediation for Transcen- 4.4. The effect of the PMYC program on children’s cognitive
dence as their mothers (with slight superiority), in spite of the fact modifiability in process-oriented and performance-oriented measures
that this strategy is considered as more difficult than others and
usually is rare (Tzuriel, 2013). Moreover, mediators in the experi- As expected, the PMYC program was found to be effective in
mental group showed higher level of MLE than their mothers in changing children’s cognitive modifiability on both process-
strategies of Intentionality and Reciprocity (Fig. 4) and Feelings oriented and outcome-oriented measures. One should bear in mind
of Competence (Fig. 5). These findings indicate that the interven- that the PMYC program is not based on teaching a specific content.
tion for peer mediation with children raised the children’s level It is rather a process-oriented program designed to teach children
of mediation so that in some strategies they were even higher than how to mediate to peers effectively, irrespective of the specific con-
their mothers’ mediation. The theoretical significance of these find- tent. The contents of the tests used were definitely different from
ings is in conceptualizing young children as significant and trusted the PMYC activities; hence the superiority of the experimental over
mediators in the learning process of their peers. The practical the control group in cognitive modifiability parameters is indica-
implications are related to construction of programs aimed at peer tive of the powerful transfer effects of the peer-mediation training.
teaching which will be effective at least as programs depending on As can be seen in Figs. 8–10 mediators in the experimental group
adults’ teaching. improved their number of insertions (indicating control of impul-
The significant interaction of treat  mentmediation agent on siveness, Fig. 8) and number of measurements (planning behavior,
activation behavior (Fig. 7) indicates that mediators in the control Fig. 9) as well as their analogical reasoning (outcome variable)
group used more activation than mediators in the experimental from pre-to post-teaching phases of the DA (Time B to Time C)
group whereas mothers showed about the same level in both much more than did mediators in the control group. These
groups. These findings clearly indicate that the PMYC program improvements, supporting expectation 4, were in spite of the fact
not only enhanced the MLE strategies of mediators’ in the exper- that both groups received an equal-time teaching phase as part
imental group but also depressed their activation behavior. It of the DA procedure. The theoretical conceptualization behind
seems that when children do not obtain MLE strategies to be these differences is that learning qualitative mediation strategies
used in peer interaction (i.e., control group) they tend to use combined with their application in peer interaction helps media-
a high ‘‘dose” of an activation behavior when teaching their tors to be better learners in other contexts and become more cog-
peers. nitively modifiable. These DA findings clearly indicate that
mediators in the experimental group internalized the mediation
principles and knew how to benefit from mediation given to them
4.3. To what extent children’s quality of mediation is determined by in a different context and consequently improved their perfor-
the mothers’ quality of mediation as compared with the effects of the mance more than children in the control group. Thus, children
PMYC program? who learn how to mediate become not only better mediators but
also better learners, as reflected in their cognitive modifiability
The relation between parents’ and children’s mediation teach- scores. In Vygotsky’s (1978) terms, the peer-mediation experience
ing style is transmitted by internalization processes during the for- enabled the mediators to advance from a lower zone of proximal
mative years of development. Previous research has already development (pre-intervention) to a higher zone of proximal devel-
indicated a correspondence between mothers’ and grandmothers’ opment (post-intervention). Later studies by Tzuriel and Shamir
mediation strategies (Isman & Tzuriel, 2007) which was explained (2007, 2010) have consistently supported the earlier studies show-
by internalization mechanisms. Our findings (Table 4) are surpris- ing positive effects of participation in the PMYC on cognitive
ing as they indicate that the treatment effect is stronger in predict- modifiability.
ing the children’s QMI (b = 0.44) than the mothers’ QMI (b = 0.14). These findings support the claim that since the declared goals of
The contribution of mothers’ QMI is expressed only in the signifi- a cognitive intervention program is to change thinking processes
cant interaction with treatment showing a significant relation and teaching learning how to learn, a novel DA approach should
between the mothers’ QMI and children’s QMI only in the experi- be adapted focusing on assessment of the modifiability of the cog-
mental group (r = 0.37, p < 0.01) but not in the control groups nitive process itself (Tzuriel, 2011c). Previous studies showed also
(r = 0.04, ns). The lack of relation in the control group might be that comparing pre- to post-intervention static cognitive scores
explained by the fact that internalization of mother’s mediation does not necessarily demonstrate the effectiveness of a cognitive
strategies at young age (i.e., 8–9 years old) is not sufficient by itself program and that a DA procedure was found to be successful in
to be applied in a context of peer mediation. It seems that in order revealing the effects of the program (Tzuriel, 2011c; Tzuriel &
for an internalized mediation strategy to be applied children need Alfassi, 1994; Tzuriel & George, 2009; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2007,
a deliberate intervention aimed at actualizing the mediation 2010; Tzuriel et al., 1999).
strategies acquired from parents. In other words, children need a
‘‘maturing” process (i.e., intervention for mediation) to make their 4.5. Can the PMYC program compensate for low mother-child MLE
raw internalized mediation capacities ripe enough to be used later strategies?
with others.
An interesting finding was that the relation between mothers’ The analysis of mediators’ cognitive modifiability by mothers’
and children’s MLE strategies has emerged only for mediation of QMI and treatment reveals that the PMYC training was more effec-
feelings of competence (experimental group, r = 0.31, p < 0.05; tive among mothers with low QMI than among mothers with high
control group, r = 0.07, ns). It might be that, relative to other QMI (Table 6 and Fig. 11), thus supporting expectation 5 with
MLE strategies, mediation of feelings of competence is an easier respect to the analogies test. The significant improvement in ana-
strategy to adopt and use later with others. However, this finding logical reasoning in the DA procedure given at the post-
might also be contributed to the range of scores which was much intervention phase (Time B to C) shown by children of low-
higher for this specific strategy than for other strategies (see mediating mothers in the experimental group indicates that the
Table 3). Further research is required to establish the transmis- program compensates for the low mediation in this group. It
sion of MLE strategies from parents to children as well as the should be emphasized that these children performed at the same
effects of other mediation agents such as teachers and significant level as children of high-mediating mothers. Thus, the program
others. brought about homogeneity of cognitive modifiability of children
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 321

in the experimental group without distinction between children of The theoretical model was also confirmed as regard the learn-
low- and high- mediating mothers. Among children of mothers ers’ cognitive modifiability. As can be seen in Fig. 14 the learners’
with high QMI, as expected, no significant difference was found cognitive modifiability was significantly explained by the media-
between children trained to mediate and non-trained children in tor’s QMI, which in turn was explained by treatment and mothers’
each of the pre- and post-intervention phases (Times A, B and C). QMI (of mediators). It should be emphasized that in spite of the
It seems that children from families in which mothers provide a fact that the teaching phase where learners were exposed to medi-
high level of mediation do not need as much intervention as do ation by their peers was relatively short (i.e., 60 min) it has an
children whose mothers provide a low level of mediation. These impact on performance of learners. Qualitative peer mediation is
findings support the idea that Level II of proximal factor (peer effective mainly because of the MLE strategies used. Mediators
mediation training) is more powerful than Level I of the proximal used MLE strategies in teaching their young counterparts how to
factor (mother’s mediation) in determining children’s cognitive make comparisons, systematically explore the problem, use speci-
modifiability as reflected in a DA procedure. This finding supports fic task related strategies, inhibit impulsivity, and use verbal tools
the constructivist’s approach (e.g., Brownstein, 2001; Rhodes & to solve the problems correctly.
Bellamy, 1999) according to which an actual experience is a pow- These findings raise some practical implications in terms of
erful factor in a learning process. In the current study’s specific cost-effectiveness consideration. For example, one can ask whether
context, a short-term intervention for peer- mediation has been it is better to invest directly in intervention with children, prepar-
revealed as more powerful than mothers’ mediation in determin- ing them to be better mediators and consequently develop better
ing their cognitive modifiability. This finding however requires fur- learning skills or investing in developing parental mediation skills.
ther studies with different samples of children, including children In the current study we did not use an intervention for mediation
with learning difficulties. with mothers. However, considering the impact of intervention
with children against spontaneous natural mediation of mothers
4.6. Structural equation modeling: the effect of proximal factors on raise the question of what is the most effective way to impact chil-
mediator’s and learner’s cognitive modifiability dren’s cognitive modifiability, a question that is waiting for further
research.
Feuerstein, Rand, and Hoffman (1979) made a distinction As expected the number of measurements during the problem
between distal and proximal determinants of cognitive develop- solving process was negatively related to the number of insertions.
ment. The distal factors (i.e., poverty, socioeconomic status, organic That relation indicates that the more measurements were taken
elements, and emotional states), in spite of their importance, do the less insertions were made. It is interesting to note that our
not explain directly individual differences in learning, cognitive SEM model was empirically corroborated in both type of measures;
change, and cognitive development. The distal factors might corre- performance (analogies) and process. The mediators’ QMI (Level 2)
late with learning ability, but they affect learning ability only was explained by both the mothers’ QMI and treatment (Level 1)
through the ‘‘prism” of the proximal factor – MLE processes. MLE whereas the cognitive modifiability of the mediators was
processes on the other hand, are considered as proximal factors explained only by the mediators’ QMI (Level 2). These findings con-
of cognitive development because they explain directly and invari- firm our theoretical distinction between Level 1 and Level 2 of
ably the development of cognitive development. In the current proximal factors. It is possible though that intervention with moth-
study we suggest to conceive the proximal factor as composed of ers to enhance their mediation skills would make their mediation
two levels of influence. Level 1 is related to mediation given by a skill a more potent factor in determining their children’s cognitive
mediation agent (e.g., parent, teacher, peers) whereas Level 2 is modifiability.
related to the experience of an active mediation to others. Based
on a constructivists’ approach we hypothesized that Level 2 is
associated with children’s cognitive modifiability more than Level 4.7. Conclusions
1. We tested this hypothesis in a set of three SEM analyses (see
schematic model in Fig. 1). The findings on both measures of cog- In conclusion, this study provided evidence for the potential of
nitive modifiability of mediators (Figs. 12 and 13) confirm our peer mediation approach program to foster children’s cognitive
hypothesis regarding the two levels of causal explanation of cogni- modifiability and compensate for lack or deficient qualitative
tive modifiability. These findings support the theory that active mother-child mediation strategies. In further studies it is sug-
involvement in mediation to others is a powerful determinant of gested to explore the effects of other mediation agents such as
one’s own cognitive modifiability. It verifies what is known by lay- fathers, siblings and grandmothers, especially in families of low
man psychology that the best way to develop learning skills is to socioeconomic status and children with special needs. Some evi-
actually teach these skills. It is interesting to note that both dence for the effectiveness of peer mediation was reported with
process-oriented measures (i.e., number of insertions and number children with learning disability mediating to their peers who were
of measurements) have emerged as significant predictors of cogni- also with a learning disability (i.e., Trabelsi et al., 2015) but more
tive modifiability in spite of the fact that both are interrelated mea- studies are required to establish these findings.
sures of children’s planning and self-regulating skills. The first
represents a tendency to control impulsivity and the second a ten-
Acknowledgement
dency for cautious inspection and repeated measurement of
objects before selecting them as correct answers.
The authors thank H. Carl Haywood and Lou Falik for their help
Another intriguing result was that treatment has emerged as a
in proof reading of the manuscript.
more powerful factor in explaining mediators’ QMI (a = 0.46)
than the mothers’ QMI (a = 0.16–0.17). The Treatment’s predictive
strength was found in spite of the fact that Treatment is a binary References
variable (experimental = 1, control = 2) whereas Mothers’ QMI con-
tains multiple levels. This finding verifies further, from a different Belsky, J., Vandell, D., Burchinal, M., Clarke-Stewart, K. A., McCartney, K., & Owen, M.
statistical angel, that the effects of intervention for peer mediation The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2007). Are there long-term
effects of early child care? Child Development, 78, 681–701.
are more powerful than mothers’ QMI in determining children’s Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological
cognitive modifiability. Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246.
322 D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323

Berk, L. E., & Spuhl, S. T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech, and task Klein, P. S. (1992). Assessing cognitive modifiability of infants and toddlers:
performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, Observations based on mediated learning experience. In H. C. Haywood & D.
145–169. Tzuriel (Eds.), Interactive assessment (pp. 223–250). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bettan, F., & Tzuriel, D. (July, 2007). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of mother- Klein, P. S. (1996). Early intervention, cross-cultural experiences with a mediational
child mediated learning experience strategies among ADHD children: Links approach. New York and London: Garland.
with parental, contextual, and child characteristics. In Paper presented at the Klein, P. S. (2003). Early intervention: Mediational intervention for sensitizing
11th international conference of the international association for cognitive caregivers. In A. S. H. Seng, L. K. H. Poo, & O. S. Tan (Eds.), Mediated learning
education and psychology (IACEP), University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA. experience with children: Applications across countries (pp. 68–84). Singapore:
Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2006). Latent curve models: A structural equation McGraw-Hill Asia.
approach. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons. Klein, P. S., & Alony, S. (1993). Immediate and sustained effects of maternal
Bornstein, M. H. (1985). How infant and mother jointly contribute to developing mediation behaviors in infancy. Journal of Early Intervention, 17, 177–193.
cognitive competence in the child. Proceedings of the National Academy of Klein, P. S., Weider, S., & Greenspan, S. I. (1987). A theoretical overview and
Science, 82, 7070–7073. empirical study of mediated learning experience: Prediction of preschool
Bornstein, M. H. (1989a). Between caretakers and their young: Two modes of performance from mother-infant interaction patterns. Infant Mental Health
interaction and their consequences for cognitive growth. In M. H. Bornstein & J. Journal, 8, 110–129.
S. Bruner (Eds.), Interaction in human development (pp. 197–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Klein, P. S., Zarur, S., & Feldman, R. (2002). Mediation in a sibling context: The
Erlbaum. relations of older siblings mediating behavior and younger siblings task
Bornstein, M. H. (1989b). Cross-cultural developmental comparisons. performance. Infant and Child Development, 11, 321–333.
Developmental Review, 9, 171–204. Klein, P. S., Zarur, S., & Feldman, R. (2003). Mediational behaviors of preschoolers
Bornstein, M. H., Azuma, H., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Ogino, M. (1990). Mother and teaching their younger siblings. Infant and Child Development, 12, 233–242.
infant activity and interaction in Japan and in the United States: I. A Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:
comparative macroanalysis of naturalistic exchanges. International Journal of The Guilford Press.
Behavioral Development, 13, 267–288. Korat, O., Ron, R., & Klein, P. (2008). Cognitive mediation and emotional support of
Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (1990). Activities and interactions of mothers fathers and mothers to their children during shared book-reading in two
and their firstborn infants in the first six months of life. Child Development, 61, different SES groups. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 7, 223–247.
1206–1217. Lauchlan, F., & Elliott, J. (2001). The psychological assessment of learning potential.
Brown, A. L., Metz, K. E., & Campione, J. C. (1996). Social interaction and individual British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 647–665.
understanding in a community of learners: The influence of Piaget and Lidz, C. S. (1991). Practitioner’s guide to dynamic assessment. New-York: Guilford.
Vygotsky. In A. Tryphon & J. Vonech (Eds.), Piaget-Vygotsky, the social genesis Lifshitz, H., & Klein, P. S. (2007). Comparison of mediation between
of thought (pp. 148–157). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press. paraprofessionals and individuals with intellectual disability: Vocational
Brownstein, B. (2001). Collaboration: The foundation of learning in the future. rehabilitation centers versus special education schools. European Journal of
Education, 122, 240–247. Special Needs Education, 22, 459–470.
Burchinal, M. R., Vandel, D. L., & Belsky, J. (2014). Is the prediction of adolescent Mattanah, J. F., Pratt, M. W., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2005). Authoritative
outcomes from early child care moderated by later maternal sensitivity? parenting, parental scaffolding of long-division mathematics, and children’s
Results from the NICHD study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. academic competence in fourth grade. Applied Developmental Psychology, 26,
Developmental Psychology, 50, 542–553. 85–106.
Cristofaro, T. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2012). Mother-child conversations at 36 McAuliffe, T. J., & Dembo, M. H. (1994). Status rules of behavior in scenarios of peer-
months and at pre-kindergarten: Relations to children’s school readiness. learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 163–172.
Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 12, 68–97. Meesters, C., van Gastel, N., Ghys, A., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Factor analyses of
Crook, C. (1998). Children as computer users: The case of collaborative learning. WISC-R and K-ABC in a Dutch sample of children referred for learning
Computers and Education, 30, 237–247. disabilities. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, 1053–1061.
Das, J. P., & Naglieri, J. A. (1997). Cognitive assessment system. Chicago: Riverside. Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect
Embretson, S. E. (1992). Measuring and validating cognitive modifiability as ability: regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of
A study in the spatial domain. Journal of Educational Measurement, 29, 25–50. attachment related strategies. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 77–102.
Espy, K. A., Molfese, V. J., & Dilalla, L. F. (2001). Effects of environmental measures on Moss, E., & St-Laurent, D. (2001). Attachment at school age and academic
intelligence in young children: Growth curve modeling of longitudinal data. performance. Developmental Psychology, 37, 863–874.
Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 47(1), 42–73. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2006). Child-care effect sizes for the
Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, R. S., Falik, L. H., & Rand, Y. (2002). The dynamic assessment NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. American Psychologist,
of cognitive modifiability: The learning propensity assessment device: Theory, 61, 99–116.
instruments, and techniques. Jerusalem: ICELP Press. O’Donnell, A. M., & King, A. (Eds.). (1999). Cognitive perspectives on peer learning.
Feuerstein, R., Rand, Y., & Hoffman, M. B. (1979). The dynamic assessment of retarded Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
performers. Baltimore: University Park Press. Piaget, J. (1967). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge and Kagan
Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2005). Peer-assisted learning strategies: Promoting word Paul.
recognition, fluency, and reading comprehension in young children. Journal of Raven, J. C. (1956). Guide to using the coloured progressive matrices, sets A, ab, and B.
Special Education, 39, 34–44. London: H. K. Lewis.
Ganihar-Raz, D., & Cahana, S. (1997). The young thinker: Manual of thinking activities Rekrut, M. D. (1994). Peer and cross-age tutoring: The lessons of research. Journal of
for children. Jerusalem: Branco-Weiss Institute (in Hebrew). Reading, 17, 356–362.
Griffin, B. W., & Griffin, M. M. (1997). The effect of reciprocal peer tutoring on Resing, W. C. M., Tunteler, E., De Jong, F. M., & Bosma, T. (2009). Dynamic testing in
graduate students’ achievement, test anxiety and academic self-efficacy. Journal indigenous and ethnic minority children. Learning and Individual Differences, 19,
of Experimental Education, 65, 197–209. 445–450.
Haywood, H. C., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assessment in practice: Clinical and Rhodes, L. K., & Bellamy, T. (1999). Choices and consequences in the reform of
educational applications. New York: Cambridge University Press. teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 50, 17–26.
Isman, E., & Tzuriel, D. (2007). The mediated learning experience (MLE) in a three Rodriguez, E. T., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Spellmann, M. E., Pan, B. A., Raikes, H., Lugo-
generational perspective. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, Gil, J., & Luze, G. (2009). The formative role of home literacy experiences across
545–560. the first three years of life in children from low-income families. Journal of
Isman, E., & Tzuriel, D. (2008). Relationship between mother-child mediated Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 677–694.
learning experience (MLE) strategies and mothers’ attachment style and mental Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context.
health. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 7, 388–410. New York: Oxford University Press.
Jensen, A. R., & Reynolds, C. R. (1982). Race, social class, and ability patterns on the Rogoff, B. (1994). Developmental transitions in children’s participation in sociocultural
WISC-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 423–438. activities. Santa Cruz, CA: University of California.
Kamps, D. M., Barbetta, P. M., Leonard, B. R., & Delquadri, J. (1994). Classwide peer Rogoff, B. (1999). Cognitive development through social interaction: Vygotsky and
tutoring: An integration strategy to improve reading skills and promote peer Piaget. In P. Murphy (Ed.), Learners, learning and assessment (pp. 69–82).
interactions among students with autism and general education peers. Journal London: Paul Chapman.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 49–61. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford
King, A. (1997). Ask to think tell why: A model of transactive peer tutoring for University Press.
scaffolding higher-level complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 32, Rohrbeck, C. A., Ginsburg-Block, M. D., Fantuzzo, J. W., & Miller, T. R. (2003). Peer-
221–235. assisted learning interventions with elementary school students: A meta-
King, A., Staffieri, A., & Adelgais, A. (1998). Mutual peer tutoring: Effects of analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 240–257.
structuring tutorial interaction to scaffold peer learning. Journal of Educational Samuels, M. T., & Tzuriel, D. (1998). The Children’s Inferential Thinking Modifiability
Psychology, 90, 134–152. test. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 16, 275–279 (Ramat-Gan, Israel:
Klein, P. S. (1988). Stability and change in interaction of Israeli mothers and infants. Bar-Ilan University).
Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 55–70. Seifer, S. (2001). Socioeconomic status, multiple risks, and development of
Klein, P. S. (1991). Improving the quality of parental interaction with very low birth intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Environmental effects on
weight children: A longitudinal study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12, 321–337. cognitive abilities (pp. 59–81). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
D. Tzuriel, R. Caspi / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 302–323 323

Shamir, A., & Lazerovitz, T. (2007). A peer mediation intervention for scaffolding Tzuriel, D. (2011b). Mediators of learning. In N. M. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of sciences
self-regulated learning among children with learning disabilities. European of learning (pp. 2157–2161). New York: Springer.
Journal of Special Needs Education, 22, 255–273. Tzuriel, D. (2011c). Revealing the effects of cognitive education programs by
Shamir, A., & Tzuriel, D. (2004). Children’s mediational teaching style as a function dynamic assessment. Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy and Practice, 18,
of intervention for cross-age peer mediation. School Psychology International, 25, 113–131.
59–78. Tzuriel, D. (2013). Mediated learning experience strategies and cognitive
Shamir, A., Tzuriel, D., & Guy, R. (2007). Computer-supported collaborative learning: modifiability. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13, 59–80.
Cognitive effects of a peer mediation intervention. Journal of Cognitive Education Tzuriel, D., & Alfassi, M. (1994). Cognitive and motivational modifiability as a
and Psychology, 6, 373–394. function of Instrumental Enrichment (IE) program. Special Services in the Schools,
Shamir, A., Tzuriel, D., & Rozen, M. (2006). Peer mediation: The effects of program 8, 91–128.
intervention, math level, and verbal ability on mediation style and Tzuriel, D., & Eran, Z. (1990). Inferential cognitive modifiability of kibbutz young
improvement in math problem solving. School Psychology International, 27, children as a function of mother–child mediated learning experience (MLE)
209–231. interactions. International Journal of Cognitive Education and Mediated Learning,
Siegel, I. (1982). The relationship between parental distancing strategies and the 1, 103–117.
child’s cognitive behavior. In L. Laosa & I. Sigel (Eds.), Families as learning Tzuriel, D., & Ernst, D. (1990). Cognitive modifiability of young children and mother-
environments for children (pp. 47–86). New York: Plenum. child mediated learning experience (MLE) interaction in low-, medium-, and
Sroufe, A. L. (1988). The role of infant–caregiver attachment in development. In J. high-SES. International Journal of Cognitive Education and Mediated Learning, 1,
Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 18–38). 119–135.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tzuriel, D., & George, T. (2009). Improvement of analogical reasoning and academic
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the achievements by the Analogical Reasoning Program (ARP). Educational and Child
person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New Psychology, 29, 71–93.
York: Guilford. Tzuriel, D., & Hanuka-Levy, D. (2014). Siblings’ mediated learning strategies in
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: Interval families with and without children with intellectual disabilities. American
estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173–180. Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119, 565–588.
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2002). Dynamic testing: The nature and Tzuriel, D., & Hatzir, A. (1999). The effects of mediational strategies of mothers and
measurement of learning potential. New York: Cambridge University Press. fathers and amount of time they spent with their young children on children’s
Stright, A. D., Herr, M. Y., & Neitzel, C. (2009). Maternal scaffolding of children’s cognitive modifiability. Unpublished manuscript, School of Education, Bar-Ilan
problem solving and children’s adjustment in kindergarten: Hmong families in University, Ramat Gan, Israel.
the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 207–218. Tzuriel, D., Kaniel, S., Kanner, E., & Haywood, H. C. (1999). The effectiveness of Bright
Topping, K. J. (2001). Peer assisted learning: A practical guide for teachers. Cambridge, Start program in kindergarten on transfer abilities and academic achievements.
MA: Brookline Books. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 114, 111–141.
Topping, K. J., Dekhinet, R., Blanch, S., Corcelles, M., & Duran, D. (2013). Paradoxical Tzuriel, D., Kaniel, S., Zeliger, M., Friedman, A., & Haywood, H. C. (1998). Effects of
effects of feedback in international online reciprocal peer tutoring. Computers & the Bright Start program in kindergarten on use of mediated learning strategies
Education, 61, 225–231. and children’s cognitive modifiability. Child Development and Care, 143, 1–20.
Trabelsi, G., Tzuriel, D., & Kashy-Rosenbaum, G. (July 2015). The effects peer Tzuriel, D., & Kaufman, R. (1999). Mediated learning and cognitive modifiability:
mediation with children in math program (PMC-M) on mediated learning Dynamic assessment of young Ethiopian immigrants in Israel. Journal of Cross-
strategies, mathematical discourse, executive functions and math performance Cultural Psychology, 30, 359–380.
among adolescents with learning disabilities. In Paper presented at the 13th Tzuriel, D., & Shamir, A. (2007). The effects of peer mediation with young children
conference of the international association for cognitive education and psychology, (PMYC) on children’s cognitive modifiability. British Journal of Educational
Athens, Greece. Psychology, 77, 143–165.
Tudge, R. H. (1996). The cognitive consequences of collaborative problem solving Tzuriel, D., & Shamir, A. (2010). Mediation strategies and cognitive modifiability in
with and without feedback analysis. Child Development, 67, 2892–2904. young children as a function of peer mediation with young children (PMYC)
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). program and training in analogies versus math tasks. Journal of Cognitive
Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Education and Psychology, 9, 48–72.
Science, 14, 623–628. Tzuriel, D., & Weiss, S. (1998). The effects of mothers’ acceptance/rejection
Tzuriel, D. (1989). Inferential cognitive modifiability in young socially attitudes, children’s personality and mothers’ mediated learning strategies on
disadvantaged and advantaged children. International Journal of Dynamic cognitive modifiability. Early Development and Parenting, 7, 79–99.
Assessment and Instruction, 1, 65–80. Valsiner, J. (1988). Developmental psychology in the Soviet Union. Brighton, UK:
Tzuriel, D. (1992). The children’s inferential thinking modifiability (CITM) test manual. Harvester.
Ramat Gan, Israel: Bar-Ilan University. Vandell, D. L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L., & Vandergrift, N. NICHD Early
Tzuriel, D. (1995). The Cognitive Modifiability Battery (CMB): Assessment and Child Care Research Network. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age
Intervention – Manual. Ramat Gan, Israel: Bar-Ilan University. 15 years? Child Development, 81, 737–756.
Tzuriel, D. (1996). Mother-child mediated learning strategies in free-play and Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
structured situations among low-, medium-, and high-SES levels. Child Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
Development and Care, 126, 57–82. process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tzuriel, D. (1999). Parent-child mediated learning transactions as determinants of Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.),
cognitive modifiability: Recent research and future directions. Genetic, Social, The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 144–188). Armonk, New York:
and General Psychology Monographs, 125, 109–156. Sharpe.
Tzuriel, D. (2000a). The Seria-Think instrument: Development of a dynamic test for Webb, N. M. (2009). The teacher’s role in promoting collaborative dialogue in the
young children. School Psychology International, 21, 177–194. classroom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 1–28.
Tzuriel, D. (2000b). Dynamic assessment of young children: Educational and Wechsler, D. (1995). Manual for the Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.).
intervention perspectives. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 385–435. Marrickville, Australia: The Psychological Corporation.
Tzuriel, D. (2000c). The cognitive modifiability battery: Assessment and Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives.
intervention. In C. S. Lidz & J. Elliott (Eds.), Dynamic assessment: Prevailing New York: Cambridge University Press.
models and applications (pp. 375–406). New York: Elsevier Science. West, K. K., Mathews, B. L., & Kerns, K. A. (2013). Mother-child attachment and
Tzuriel, D. (2001). Dynamic assessment of young children. New York: Kluwer cognitive performance in middle childhood: An examination of mediating
Academic/Plenum Press. mechanisms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 259–270.
Tzuriel, D. (2002). Dynamic assessment of learning potential. In J. W. Guthrie (Ed.), White, K. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic
Encyclopedia of education (2nd ed., pp. 127–131). New York: McMillan Press. achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–481.
Tzuriel, D. (2007). Transfer effects of teaching conceptual versus perceptual Wood, D. J. (1989). Social interaction as tutoring. In M. H. Bornstein & J. S. Bruner
analogies. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 6, 194–217. (Eds.), Interaction in human development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tzuriel, D. (2011a). Mediated learning and cognitive modifiability. In N. M. Seel
(Ed.), Encyclopedia of sciences of learning (pp. 2154–2157). New York: Springer.

View publication stats

You might also like