Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1997, Vol. 89, No. 2, 289-297 0022-0653/97/S3.00

Effects of Accelerated Reading Rate on Memory for Text Among


Dyslexic Readers
Zvia Breznitz
University of Haifa

The author tested the hypothesis that gains in reading performance occurring among dyslexic
children during individually paced accelerated reading are partially attributable to changes in
short-term memory (STM) processing. Dyslexic children's performance on a series of
standard STM-sensitive tasks administered during both self- and fast-paced reading condi-
tions confirmed this hypothesis. Trie findings provided strong support for a causal role of
STM functioning in text processing but indicated that reading acceleration enhanced STM
only when dyslexic children were able to rely on contextual cues.

Individually paced reading rate acceleration has been case of partial decoding (i.e., where an unfamiliar item is
found to increase reading effectiveness in various groups of incompletely decoded or when multiple pronunciations are
readers and across two languages, English and Hebrew. possible; Share, 1995); and (c) integration of information
This effect is particularly pronounced when reading skills within and between sentences using syntactic organization
are less proficient (see Breznitz, 1993, for a review). Ex- processes. According to this view, STM may be used to
perimental data indicate that in normal novice readers, the temporarily retain words and phrases being decoded so that
benefits of fast-paced reading are at least partially attribut- they can be arranged and encoded into meaningful sen-
able to lowered vulnerability to visual distraction (Breznitz, tences or propositions (Carpenter & Just, 1989; Just &
1988, 1997), enhanced word recognition due to increased Carpenter, 1987; Perfetti & Lesgold, 1977).
similarity between vocal output of oral reading and stored In current models of reading, a differentiation is made
pronunciations of words (Breznitz, 1990), and improved between STM, which is held to be related to short-term,
short-term memory (STM) functioning (Breznitz & Share, passive storage functions, and working memory (WM),
1992). On the basis of findings that reading performance which is held to be responsible for active, temporary pro-
improves substantially among dyslexic children during cessing functions. Hence, investigators generally associate
reading acceleration (Breznitz, 1993, 1997), I sought to the first of the above-mentioned roles with STM, and the
explore the extent to which these effects can be ascribed to latter two with WM (Swanson, 1994). As in the Breznitz
changes in STM processes in this population. To achieve and Share (1992) investigation, I sought to assess short-term
this aim, I replicated the experimental paradigm used in the capacity rather than active processing. Hence, the term STM
Breznitz and Share study on a reading-level matched sample is used here.
of dyslexic children.
Readers use a variety of strategies to minimize the func-
Considerable evidence has demonstrated a robust, posi- tional limitations of STM. Among these are rehearsal, or-
tive relationship between reading ability and performance ganization of informational units into chunks, and parsing
on STM tasks (Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). STM refers to a (Cohen & Heath, 1990; Shankweiler & Crain, 1986). These
temporary storage system that holds information during WM strategies operate by maintaining incoming material in
cognitive processing. This system is characterized by lim- short-term storage or by restructuring it so that the amount
ited capacity and rapid, spontaneous decay. These limita- of information retained is increased.
tions pose constraints on the execution of information pro- STM constraints may also be overcome through external
cessing tasks such as reading (Baddeley, 1986). manipulations. One method that has been shown to be
Verbal or phonological STM is thought to play a signif- effective in this respect is increasing stimulus presentation
icant part in the acquisition of decoding skills, the mastery rate (Cohen, 1982; Dempster, 1981; Torgesen & Houck,
of which is critical to learning to read. Among the roles 1980). This finding has served as the basis for the earlier
attributed to verbal STM in reading are (a) provision of study examining the effects of reading-rate acceleration on
buffer storage during the process in which unfamiliar words STM functioning among normal novice readers (Breznitz &
are decoded through the application of grapheme-to- Share, 1992). That study used an experimental paradigm
phoneme conversion rules and sound blending (Baddeley, that has been used extensively to investigate the effects of
1986; Jorm, 1983); (b) integration of postlexical contextual reading rate acceleration on reading performance in indi-
information with word identification processes, as in the viduals at a wide range of reading levels (Breznitz, 1987a,
1987b, 1988, 1990, 1991, 1993; Breznitz, DeMarco, &
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hakerem, 1993; Breznitz, DeMarco, Shammi, & Hakerem,
Zvia Breznitz, School of Education, University of Haifa, Mt. 1994). In this paradigm, individuals are prompted to accel-
Carmel, Haifa 31095, Israel. Electronic mail may be sent via erate their reading rates in a computer-controlled reading
Internet to redc401@uvm.haifa.ac.il. situation. The individual acceleration rate is determined on

289
290 BREZNITZ

the basis of the fastest rate displayed by the reader over a correlation between STM and gain in reading comprehen-
series of items read at his or her normal self-paced reading sion during reading acceleration.
rate. Hence, the rate of acceleration remains within the One explanation proposed to account for these findings is
reader's own range of demonstrated ability. that acceleration may enable readers to partially overcome
Data obtained through this research paradigm consistently STM limitations of rapid decay and limited capacity, which
indicate that reading rate can function as an independent impede successful information processing. Fast-pacing may
variable that influences the quality of reading performance. operate on STM functioning by increasing the units (either
Decoding accuracy and comprehension scores increase sig- in number or size) concurrently available in storage, thereby
nificantly when readers are prompted to read faster than enlarging the context in which the reading process takes
their normal rate (Breznitz, 1987a, 1988,1991). Supporting place. Yet although it has been empirically demonstrated
the idea that reading rate can act independently are studies that the gains in reading performance during this manipu-
showing that visual word identification speed is both a lation are at least partially attributable to improved STM
powerful discriminator of reading ability (Perfetti, 1985) functioning among normal beginning readers, it remains to
and a strong predictor of later reading proficiency (Lesgold be established whether these results can be extended to
& Resnick, 1982; and review by Compton & Carlisle, dyslexic readers.
1994). The above findings suggest that reading rate may be To clarify this issue, I sought to assess the impact of the
essential to the establishment of early reading skills and fast-paced reading phenomenon on STM processing among
might serve as a reliable diagnostic measure. dyslexic children. As in the Breznitz and Share (1992)
An alternative position views reading rate as a dependent experiments, a number of tasks were administered that have
variable, which is an outcome of the effectiveness of text been shown to be particularly sensitive to STM functioning
decoding and comprehension (Carver, 1990; Gough & Tun- (Baddeley, 1986; Jorm, 1983; Perfetti & Lesgold, 1977;
mer, 1986). In most developmental models of reading ac- Simon, 1974). These tasks assessed recall and recognition
quisition, more efficient decoding is presumed to lead to of exact content and word order, recency versus primacy
effects, and detection of wording changes as opposed to
improved comprehension, and greater integration of these
semantic changes. I hypothesized that, like normal novice
two skills is held to result in more rapid, automatic word
readers, dyslexic readers engaged in fast-paced reading rel-
identification (Coltheart, 1987; Ehri & Wilce, 1983; Frith,
ative to self-paced reading would show significant perfor-
1986; Morrison, 1991; Perfetti, 1985). mance gains on tasks sensitive to STM function.
Supporting this position are direct investigations of read-
ing acceleration (e.g., Carver, 1990). In Carver's systematic
studies on reading acceleration among adult readers, nega- General Method
tive correlations between rate and comprehension were ob-
tained. In these studies, reading was accelerated at set rates Participants
across participants. It was found that the average optimal
rate for college students consists of about 300 words per Twenty-three dyslexic children (18 boys and 5 girls) partici-
minute, with a range of 13%, depending on the difficulty of pated in all four experiments reported in this study. The definition
the reading material. of developmental dyslexia accepted in Israel is based on the
revised Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
It must be clarified here that the findings obtained through
(3rd ed.; DSM-III-R American Psychiatric Association, 1987)
Breznitz's paradigm are not incompatible with these results. diagnostic criteria. Accordingly, the dyslexic participants were
Breznitz's method of inducing individuals to read at the characterized by selective impairment of reading despite normal
fastest rate demonstrated during normal self-paced reading intelligence, visual acuity, motivation, lack of educational oppor-
invariably uses the individual's own rate, hence the manip- tunities, and instruction. Furthermore, they were free of "hard"
ulation that was found effective operated within the range of neurological signs and did not suffer from primary or severe
13% reported by Carver (1990, p. 146). In fact, the average behavioral or psychiatric disturbances. All of the 23 dyslexic
speed increase of college students turned out to be about children were retarded in reading by at least 1 year and 8 months,
10% (Breznitz et al., 1993; Breznitz et al, 1994). as stipulated by Israeli criteria for dyslexia (Nitzan Center for
Learning Disabilities, 1988).
In sum, reading rate may operate both as an independent Dyslexic participants were recruited from two learning disabil-
and a dependent variable. In the present study, as in earlier ity clinics of the Department of Education in Haifa. The dyslexic
research using the Breznitz acceleration paradigm, reading readers were referred to the clinics by teachers or school psychol-
rate acts independently. ogists. At the time of their referral, all children were diagnosed by
The data obtained to date using this procedure clearly an intake battery, which included an evaluative test of their reading
indicate that the reading performance of poor novice decoding and comprehension skills (Reading Skills Test; Nitzan
(Breznitz, 1987a) and dyslexic readers (Breznitz, 1993, Center for Learning Disabilities, 1988). The average number of
1997) improves substantially during reading acceleration in years of reading retardation of the children was 1 year and 11
months {SD - 0.5) and ranged from 1 year and 8 months to 2 years
terms of increased decoding accuracy and comprehension.
and 2 months. The mean age of the sample was 9 years 3 months
The results of the Breznitz and Share (1992) study revealed (SD — 0.3). All children were of middle-class background and
large gains in STM task performance at a fast-paced as attended public elementary schools in an urban area of northern
compared with a self-paced reading rate among normal Israel; Hebrew was their primary language. All children were
second-grade readers. Results further revealed a negative right-handed. On the basis of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
EFFECTS OF READING ACCELERATION AMONG DYSLEXICS 291

Children—Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1977), mean IQ in the test contained six items of increasing difficulty. Items consisted of
sample was 110 (SD = 0.5; see data in Table 1). two to four declarative sentences (in Hebrew). Immediately after
the reading of each item, the passage was removed from the screen
and the child was asked to recount what he or she had just read.
Instrumen ta tion
The responses to this open-ended question, designed to assess
The research tasks were identical to those used in the Breznitz recall of factual content, were assessed by propositional analysis
and Share (1992) study. Because Israeli norms are available only (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). Following this, the child was given a
for the WISC-R, raw scores are reported for the remaining tests. short multiple-choice inferential question. Finally, recall and rec-
As in Breznitz and Share, all tasks in all the experiments were ognition of content were tested using three questions each. Two
administered to the same children to facilitate between-task com- sample items are the following:
parisons. The stability of the reading rates across tasks indicated 1. Easy item. "A child came home in the afternoon and asked
that reactive effects associated with repeated testing were not a his mother for food." This statement was followed by a
problem. multiple-choice inferential question: "What did the child
A number of verbal and nonverbal STM tests were adminis- ask for? (a) salt, (b) a plate, (c) a cup of water, (d) a piece
tered. Verbal memory was assessed using the Hebrew version of of bread." Then three recall questions were asked: "(a)
the Digit Span Test (both backward and forward) and Sentences Who came? (b) What did he do? (c) What did he ask for?"
Memory from the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale Memory Three recognition questions concluded the focus on the
subtests (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986). Additional tests of statement: "(a) Who came home? A boy or a mother? (b)
verbal memory function (both backward and forward) were ad- When did he come home? In the morning or afternoon? (c)
ministered according to Simon's (1974) procedures. These in- Whom did he ask? Father or mother?"
cluded tests of letter span and nonsense syllable span, with strings 2. Difficult item. "All Jacob's friends have watches. Only
of letters and syllables ranging in length from two to nine items. Jacob has no watch, Jacob went out to play with his friends.
Nonverbal memory was assessed using the Stanford-Binet Intel- His mother told him to come home at seven o'clock."
ligence Scale Object Memory and Bead Memory subscales. Means Multiple-choice inferential question: "How did Jacob
and standard deviations for memory measures are presented in know when to come home? (a) he asked his mother, (b) he
Table 1. asked his friends, (c) he listened to the news on the radio,
(d) he looked at his watch." Recall questions: "(a) Who did
not have a watch? (b) With whom did Jacob go out to play?
Experiment 1
(c) At what time did Jacob have to be home?" Recognition
Experiment 1 was designed to assess recognition and recall for question: "(a) What was the name of the boy, Jacob or
semantic (propositional) content and for exact wording of text read Gad? (b) What did Jacob's friends have? A football or a
in both self-paced and fast-paced conditions. This first experiment watch? (c) Who told Jacob to come home at seven o'clock,
was designed to replicate the basic fast-paced effect with standard his friends or his mother?"
measures of reading performance. A second objective was to In each manipulation (fast paced or self-paced) scores were
assess the influence of the fast-paced manipulation on measures computed as follows: For each item, the factual content recall was
more closely associated with STM functions. I predicted that the analyzed by propositional analysis and the number of correct
accelerated reading rate would result in significant gains among responses was computed. The maximum possible scores were 6 for
the dyslexic readers in both semantic memory and in memory for the multiple-choice inferential questions, and 18 (6 X 3) each for
exact wording. factual content recognition and recall questions. Test forms were
randomly distributed among conditions in a fully counterbalanced
design. The order of conditions was fixed: self-paced, fast paced,
Method and again self-paced.
Three parallel forms of the Comprehension Test for Grades 1 The children read three forms of the test. In the first condition,
and 2 (Ortar & Segev, 1980) were administered. Each form of the the children read items at their own natural reading pace (self-
paced). In the second condition (fast paced), the children were
prompted to read at a faster rate. To control for a possible warm-up
Table 1 effect, I administered a third condition of self-paced reading.
Means and Standard Deviations of General Ability, All printed materials were presented on an IBM personal com-
Reading Achievement, and Short-Term Memory (TV = 23) puter (IBM-PC). To monitor reading errors, I required the children
Measure M SD to read all items aloud as accurately as possible. Reading protocols
were recorded on audio cassette.
WISC-R Blocks (scaled score, In the self-paced reading condition, children were requested to
max. = 19) 11.9 2.82 read all items at their own natural pace. Each item was presented
WISC-R Vocabulary (scaled score, individually on the screen, and the children were asked to com-
max. — 19) 9.5 4.21
39.2 9.92 mence reading immediately. Several training trials were given
Reading ability (%, max. = 100)
S-B Digit Span (max. = 9) 4.2 0.21 prior to each set of six items. To eliminate the possible confound-
S-B Sentence Memory (max. = 22) 14.1 3.82 ing of regressive eye movements, I erased each item immediately
Letter span (max. = 9) 4.1 0.36 after reading was completed. Times for each item were recorded
Syllable span (max. = 5) 0.7 0.20 by a computer that was activated when the child commenced and
S-B Object Memory (max. = 9) 4.6 0.52 deactivated when pronunciation of the last item was completed.
S-B Bead Memory (max. = 20) 11.1 1.21 Per letter reading rates were calculated for each item, yielding six
Note. WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children- separate per letter reading rates.
Revised; max. = maximum score; S-B = Stanford-Binet Intelli- In the fast-paced reading condition, the presentation rate was
gence Scale. calculated for each child according to the highest per letter reading
292 BREZNITZ

rate achieved on the six items in the self-paced condition. Thus, a fast-paced condition compared with a self-paced condi-
material was presented at the highest rate of demonstrated capa- tion. Primacy and recency effects were also examined.
bility for each child. To minimize disruption of natural eye move-
ments, I adopted the following procedure: The entire item ap-
peared on the screen and the child was requested to begin reading Experiment 2
aloud immediately. As soon as reading commenced, material was
erased letter by letter at the maximum per letter rate as above. Method
Oral reading errors, reading rate, comprehension, and recall and
recognition of content were recorded in all three conditions. Each child read aloud three parallel sets of material using the
pacing order of Experiment 1. Each set of items consisted of six
meaningful declarative sentences ordered in increasing length
Results from 5 to 10 words (total of 45 words). In both conditions, the
children were asked to repeat each sentence in backward order and
As in previous work (Breznitz & Share, 1992), there was then in forward order according to the principle of "last in, first
no suggestion of a warm-up effect. No significant differ- out." Factual recall was then tested with an open-ended question,
ences were found witfiin the dyslexic children between the as described in Experiment 1. Examples of 5-word and 10-word
first and second self-paced conditions, F < 1.00 (see Table items are the following: "Gad went home in the rain" (5 Hebrew
2). Therefore, in all the subsequent analyses the results of words), and "Ruth recovered from a serious illness and returned to
the fast-paced reading condition were compared with the school. The doctor forbade her to exercise" (10 Hebrew words).
results of the first self-paced condition. Results for Exper- Oral reading errors and reading rates for each item were re-
iment 1 appear in Table 2. corded. Three measures of recall were taken: recall of exact
A within-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) was wording (the correct order backward and forward), correct recall of
conducted to determine differences between self-paced and words regardless of whether these were in the correct order (item
recall), and recall of content as assessed by a propositional
fast-paced conditions on reading rate, accuracy, and com-
analysis.
prehension. In the fast-paced condition, reading rates sig-
nificantly increased by 19%, and decoding accuracy signif-
icantly increased by 28%. The four measures of Results
comprehension revealed that during fast-paced reading, in-
ferential comprehension increased significantly by 26% and The results for propositional recall and backward and
factual content recall by 6%. No significant gain was found forward item and order recall of exact wording are shown in
in recall and recognition for exact wordings. Table 3. A similar pattern of results emerged for the first
This first experiment reveals that in contrast to normal and second self-paced conditions as compared with fast-
readers (Breznitz & Share, 1992), dyslexic children did not paced reading, hence data from the first self-paced condition
improve in memory for exact wording in the fast-paced was used for comparison.
condition. The data were not clear as to whether the positive As in Experiment 1, scores for speed, accuracy, and
effects of the fast-paced manipulation occurred through comprehension measured by propositional analysis of con-
STM. Hence, in the second experiment more explicit evi- tent recall revealed significant gains in the fast-paced con-
dence was sought. This experiment was designed to exam- dition. Measures of item and order recall for the exact
ine memory for wording and word order in sentences read in wording of material read in the fast-paced conditions

Table 2
Experiment 1; Means and Standard Deviations for Reading Rate, Decoding Accuracy,
and Comprehension in Self-Paced and Fast-Paced Conditions
Self-paced 1 Fast-paced Self-paced 2
Measure M SD M SD F{\, 21) M SD
Per letter reading
rate (s) 0.32 o.so 0.27 0.10 13.10** 0.33 0.70
Accuracy (No. of
errors) 29.00 15.01 22.70 5.36 6.31** 27.60 14.80
Inferential
comprehension
(range = 0-6) 3.11 0.92 3.93 0.86 7 22** 3.12 0.85
Propositional
recall (%) 27.70 10.20 33.30 9.70 11.20** 28.20 10.50
Exact wording
recall (%) 14.10 2.70 15.30 3.20 ns 13.90 2.90
Wording
recognition (%) 19.70 4.20 20.10 3.90 ns 19.20 4.50
Note. F tests analyze the differences between participants in the self-paced 1 and the fast-paced
conditions.
**p< .001.
EFFECTS OF READING ACCELERATION AMONG DYSLEXICS 293

Table 3
Experiment 2: Means and Standard Deviations for Propositional Recall and Backward
and Forward Item and Order Recall in Self-Paced and Fast-Paced Reading Conditions
Self-paced 1 Fast-paced Self-paced 2
Measure M SD M SD F{\, 21) M SD
Propositional recall (No.) 26.9 11.1 32.2 10.6 3.96** 26.5 9.7
Backward item recall (%) 11.2 5.6 10.9 3.5 1.01 10.8 3.9
Backward order recall (%) 6.3 1.7 6.6 2.1 1.44 5.9 1.9
Forward item recall (%) 33.1 12.2 44.2 13.5 9.57** 32.6 10.1
Forward order recall (%) 29.2 10.6 37.1 9.7 7.37** 30.1 11.6
Accuracy (No. of errors) 24.1 11.8 17.2 8.3 5.36** 23.9 9.7
Per letter reading rate (s) 0.3 0.7 0.2 0.6 3.22** 0.2 0.9
Note. F tests analyze the differences between participants in the Self-paced 1 and the fast-paced
conditions.
**p < .001.

showed significant gains only in forward order recall. No creased more in the fast-paced condition than recency recall
gains were found in the backward order recall. In general, did, F(l,21) = 8.33, p < .001.
these results support the view that among dyslexic children, To the extent that recall of the most recent item is rela-
improvements in comprehension obtained with fast-paced tively more dependent on STM processes than recall of the
reading can result from an increase in the number of ele- primacy item, the data of Experiment 2 failed to support the
ments being held in STM. However, the finding that unlike view that the fast-paced manipulation for the dyslexic op-
normal readers, dyslexic children increased performance erates directly only through STM processing. The data in
only for forward order recall, may indicate a reliance on Experiments 1 and 2 indicated that there was a tendency for
contextual information by dyslexic readers. the effects of the fast-paced manipulation to be stronger for
An examination of primacy and recency effects in self- memory for context (semantics) than for exact wording in
paced and fast-paced conditions was also conducted. I hy- the items being rated. However, a direct comparison of the
pothesized that if the fast-paced manipulation operates pri- magnitude of these two effects was not possible because
marily through STM, then recall of the last (most recent) wording and semantic processing in both studies were as-
parts of the items would show a greater fast-paced advan- sessed with different measures.
tage than recall of the earlier parts of the items, as recency Experiment 3 was designed to directly compare memory
effects are more dependent on STM (Brooks, 1975). Data for wording with memory for semantic information. It was
indicated that as opposed to normal readers (Breznitz & expected that if the fast-paced manipulation was operating
Share, 1992), dyslexic children recalled more primacy parts through STM among the dyslexic children, an interaction
of the items (i.e., the beginnings of sentences) than recent would occur between memory condition (semantic vs.
parts (ends of sentences) during both self- and fast-paced wording) and pacing condition, with fast-paced reading
trials. producing relatively more accurate detection (i.e., fewer
In a 2 X 2 repeated measures ANOVA, there was a false alarms) of wording than semantic changes. However,
significant main effect for item position (primacy vs. re- if the fast-paced manipulation was not operating through
cency), with recall of the primacy higher than recall of STM, an interaction would be expected between memory
recency, F(l, 21) = 5.03, p < .002. A significant main condition (semantic vs. wording) and pacing condition, with
effect was also obtained for pacing condition (first self- fast-paced reading producing relatively more accurate de-
paced vs. fast paced), with the fast-paced reading rate recall tection of semantic than of wording changes.
being higher, F(l, 21) = 7.21, p < .001. (Descriptive
statistics are presented in Table 4.) A significant Pace X
Condition interaction indicated that primacy recall in- Experiment 3
Method
Table 4 Experiment 3 was a 3 X 2 within-subjects design consisting of
Experiment 2: Primacy and Recency Recall Effects in the three reading-rate manipulations used in Experiments 1 and 2,
Self-Paced and Fast-Paced Reading Conditions and two memory conditions (wording vs. semantic). Each memory
condition contained three parallel reading forms, with each of four
Self-paced Self-paced passages ranging in length from two to four sentences. The pas-
1 Fast-paced 2 sages were characterized by increasing length and difficulty, con-
Item M SD M SD M SD taining 20, 25, 30, and 35 words. Following the presentation of the
target passage, two test passages appeared simultaneously on the
Last (No.) 1.9 0.5 2.1 0.7 1.7 0.8 computer screen: The original unaltered version and a second
First (No.) 3.2 0.8 5.6 0.4 3.3 0.5 version in which either a semantic or wording change had been
294 BREZNTTZ

made to a single word. The children were requested to identify the version of the passage was superior in the wording condi-
correct version. In the semantic condition, a nonsynonymous con- tions than in the semantic wording conditions; F(l, 21) =
tent word was substituted, as indicated by brackets in this example: 4.05,p < .05 in the self-paced manipulation, and F(l, 21) =
"On Saturday, there were many people at the beach. A boat was 7.45, p < .05 in the fast-paced. That is, the dyslexic children
sailing on the sea. Suddenly, a strong wind began to blow. The were more often fooled by meaning-altering changes than
wind pushed the boat towards the sea [beach]." In the wording
condition, a synonym was substituted, as indicated by brackets in by words that preserved meaning. A significant Pace X
the following example: "During her vacation, Ruth went to a Condition interaction was found, F(l, 21) = 12.11, p <
summer camp in the Jerusalem hills. She spent three days there. .001. These results are the reverse of those found among
On Tuesday afternoon, she came back [returned] home." The normal novice readers.
location of the altered item was either 4,5, or 7 words from the end Experiment 4 further investigated the effects of reading
of the passage. (The location of the change in the latter example acceleration on STM functioning. In this experiment, the
has been altered in the translation from Hebrew to English.) Both children were given a target item from the text they had just
semantic and wording forms were read first in self-paced condi- read aloud and asked to recall the item immediately preced-
tions, then in fast-paced conditions, as in Experiments 1 and 2.
Semantic and wording forms were counterbalanced. The order of
ing the probe and the item immediately following the probe.
the two test versions of the passage (altered and unaltered) was It was predicted that the fast-paced manipulation would
also counterbalanced across test items. I also assessed free recall produce significant improvements in recall relative to self-
for each item with a proposition^ analysis, as in Experiment 1. All paced reading.
materials were read aloud, and oral reading errors were recorded.

Experiment 4
Results
Method
Scores for correct detection, together with basic reading
performance measures, are presented in Table 5. As in Bach child read two parallel sets of material, each consisting of
Experiments 1 and 2, both self-paced conditions produced six items. Each item was a declarative sentence ordered in increas-
an identical pattern of results as compared with the fast- ing length from 5 to 10 words. Sample items included the follow-
paced condition. As in the previous experiments, it is clear ing: "Gad went home in the rain" (5 Hebrew words) and "The
that differences between fast-paced and self-paced condi- water in the kettle boiled, and mother poured tea into a cup for her
tions are not attributable to task order. 6-year-old son, Yossie" (10 Hebrew words). Immediately after the
child completed reading an item, the experimenter repeated a
The data in Table 5 indicate gains on basic measures of single word (the probe). The child was asked to recall the word that
reading performance. Reading time, accuracy, and compre- had appeared immediately before the probe and the word that had
hension significantly increased in the fast-paced as com- appeared immediately after the probe (in that order). The probe
pared with the self-paced condition. In both self- and fast- word was located in consecutively different positions from Posi-
paced conditions, correct identification of the unaltered tion 2 (in Sentence 1) through Position 7 (in Sentence 6). For each

Table 5
Experiment 3: Means and Standard Deviations for Detection of Wording and Semantic
Changes in Self-Paced and Fast-Paced Reading Conditions
Self-paced 1 Fast-paced Self-paced 2
Measure M SD M SD F(l, 21) M SD
Wording charLges
Detection 8 (No.) 2.0 0.6 2.1 0.8 ns 2.1 0.9
Recall (%) 30.2 9.7 38.1 11.9 7.6** 29.2 10.1
Accuracy (No. of
errors) 24.2 6.7 16.3 7.2 8.2** 25.3 9.1
Per letter reading
rate (s) 0.3 0.7 0.2 0.1 12.1** 0.3 0.9

Semantic changes
Detection 3 (No.) 2.4 0.8 3.2 0.7 11.2** 2.2 0.8
Recall (%) 35.3 7.7 47,5 9.1 8.2** 33.4 10.7
Accuracy (No. of
errors) 22.3 11.7 15.4 7.2 9.3** 23.7 11.6
Per letter reading
rate (s) 0.3 0.6 0.2 0.8 12.8** 0.3 0.7
Note. F tests analyze the differences between participants in first self-placed 1 and fast-paced
conditions.
a
Maximum score = 4.
**p < .001.
EFFECTS OF READING ACCELERATION AMONG DYSLEXICS 295
manipulation, a child could score a maximum of 6 on prior items account for the fast-paced reading phenomenon among this
and 6 on subsequent items, As in the other experiments, items were population. To achieve this end, I replicated the task format
read in both self-paced and fast-paced manipulations, with parallel of an earlier study on novice readers (Breznitz & Share,
sets of sentences counterbalanced. Because all prior experiments
demonstrated that task order is irrelevant, the second self-paced
1992) on a sample of dyslexic children.
condition was not used in the Experiment 4. Free recall was also Consistent with the findings of Breznitz (1993), I found
tested following probed recall. that reading acceleration significantly enhances reading per-
formance in specifically disabled readers. Dyslexic children
can read faster than they normally do, and by doing so,
Results increase decoding accuracy and comprehension. However,
The results for Experiment 4 are presented in Table 6. the extent to which this gain may be ascribed to improved
Consistent with previous results, reading rates and accuracy STM operation is debatable.
were significantly higher in the fast-paced reading condition Fast-paced reading produced a markedly different pattern
as compared with the self-paced condition. The dyslexic of effects on STM among this population as compared with
children recalled more prior words than subsequent words normal beginning readers. Because the dyslexic children in
during self- and fast-paced reading. This pattern of results is this study were reading-level matched to the normal novice
similar to the results of the primacy and recency effects readers in the Breznitz and Share (1992) study, the differ-
observed in Experiment 2, which indicated that the recall of ences between the two groups in performance on the STM
items among dyslexic readers is related to context. tasks used in both studies were statistically assessed. A
repeated measure multivariate analysis of variance revealed
a significant Group X Condition interaction in forward
General Discussion versus backward item recall, F(2, 42) = 73.0, p < .001,
recency versus primacy item recall, F(2, 42) = 46.7, p <
Earlier research has revealed that individually paced read-
.001, and wording versus semantic detection, F(2, 42) =
ing acceleration significantly improves reading performance
11.3, p < .001. During reading acceleration, the dyslexic
among readers at all levels of ability. Investigations of the
children improved on forward item recall (M = 11.2) but
etiology of this phenomenon among normal novice and
more advanced readers have shown that the gains in reading not backward item recall (M — 0.3), increased their recall of
performance during fast-paced reading are attributed, at primacy items (M = 2.4) but not recency items (M — 0.2),
least in part, to lower distracribility (Breznitz, 1988), in- and improved their performance on semantic detection
creased similarity between the vocal output and stored pro- (M = 0.8) but not wording detection (M = 0.1). In contrast,
nunciations (Breznitz, 1990), increased phonological pro- normal beginning readers improved backward item recall
cessing (Breznitz, 1997), and improved STM functioning more significantly than forward item recall (M — 27.1 and
(Breznitz & Share, 1992). M = 26.1, respectively), increased recency item recall more
significantly than primacy item recall (M — 2.2 and M —
The effects of acceleration are pronounced among dys-
0.3, respectively), and improved their performance on
lexic readers (Breznitz, 1993, 1997). Gains in decoding
wording detection but not semantic detection (M = 0.8 and
accuracy and comprehension are substantial during this
manipulation. Many researchers have demonstrated that M - 0.1, respectively).
deficits in STM are characteristic of the reading disabled This pattern of results indicates that unlike normal begin-
(Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). In light of the importance of ning readers, dyslexic children were more often fooled by
STM to decoding and comprehension processes, I sought to synonomous word substitutions than by substitutions in
assess the extent to which changes in STM functioning which words' meanings were changed. In addition, they
recalled more prior items (i.e., earlier words) in sentences
than subsequent (later) words during fast-paced (as well as
self-paced) reading.
Table 6
Experiment 4: Reading Rate, Accuracy, and Probed These findings suggest that STM functioning among dys-
Recall Scores in Self-Paced and Fast-Paced lexic children was facilitated only when reliance on context
Reading Conditions was feasible. Conversely, the normal novice readers in the
Breznitz and Share (1992) study showed sizable gains on
Self-paced 1 Fast-paced STM tasks that were free from context effects. These read-
Measure l, 21) ers appeared to operate according to the principle of "last in,
last out," whereas the opposite was true of dyslexic
Prior words
recalled (No.) 3.1 1.5 4.2 1.3 5.73* children.
Subsequent Further supporting the idea that dyslexic readers may
words recalled depend on context during processing in STM is the finding
(No.) 1.9 0.7 2.1 1.0 ns that among dyslexic children, memory for factual content
Accuracy (No. of
errors) 14.1 2.3 9.2 3.1 7.21 (as measured by prepositional analysis) correlated signifi-
Per letter reading cantly with primacy recall (r = .52) but not with recency
rate (s) 0.3 0.6 0.2 0.5 9.23 recall (r = .14). Experiment 3 revealed that in both self- and
.05. fast-paced reading conditions, content recall correlated sig-
296 BREZNITZ

nificantly with semantic detection (r = .66) but not with (Seidenberg, 1990), an additional possibility is that WM
wording detection (r - .17). may act as a coordinating mechanism for information arriv-
These results suggest that STM resources may be used in ing from each of the three subsystems. Fast-paced reading
a different manner among dyslexic readers than among may improve this coordination by forcing information to
novice readers. Dyslexic readers may depend more on sen- arrive in WM at a faster rate, such that it is more likely that
tential content to deduce meaning. This interpretation is corresponding information from the three subsystems will
consistent with StanovicrTs (1980) interactive- present simultaneously.
compensatory model of reading, which posits that context Whether the above speculations are indeed the case is a
usage compensates for poor decoding ability. Experimental question that remains to be further explored. Additional
data is consistent with this view. Beginning and less skilled research using independent measures of both STM and WM
readers use a context-bound strategy more extensively for would contribute to our understanding of the effects of
word identification (Goldsmith-Phillips, 1989; Leu, De- fast-pacing on these factors in dyslexic and normal reader
Groff, & Simons 1986; Stanovich, 1984; Stanovich, West, populations.
& Feeman, 1981). This holds true even at the college level It is important to note that alongside the clear evidence of
(Ben-Dror, Pollatsek, & Scarpati, 1991). Although this improved STM or WM functioning, or both, during fast-
strategy is not cognitively economic in terms of engagement paced reading among dyslexic children, this appears not to
of limited resources, it may offer a means of lexical access be the sole explanatory context for the fast-paced phenom-
that is inefficiently achieved through phonological or ortho- enon in this group of readers. Research has revealed that
graphic codes. reading acceleration may promote more efficient operation
Researchers have demonstrated, with abundant empirical of other cognitive abilities: Fast-pacing appears to focus
evidence, that the main difficulty dyslexic readers encounter attention (Breznitz, 1988; Ronen & Breznitz, 1997) and to
lies in accessing the lexicon, particularly when this must overcome some of the phonological processing limitations
occur through use of the phonological code (Olson, Wise, characteristic of the specifically reading disabled (Breznitz,
Conners, & Rack, 1990; Snowling, 1980). The results ob- 1990, 1997). These results suggest that fast pacing may
tained in the self-paced condition in the present study sup- compel dyslexic children to engage other available cogni-
port this position. Decoding accuracy was clearly impaired tive resources more extensively to meet the demands of
among dyslexic children. Compared with the second-grade time-limited reading.
readers in the earlier (Breznitz & Share, 1992) study, the The current experiments point to the possibility that read-
fourth-grade dyslexic children in the current investigation ing acceleration might be of value as both a diagnostic and
had significantly lower word recognition scores. a remedial tool that can be used in combination with estab-
Reading acceleration may serve to increase available con- lished phonology-based techniques.
textual information in STM. As dyslexic readers may be
strongly context-dependent, it is conceivable that for them,
acceleration induces postlexical text integration processes, References
whereas for normal novice readers fast-pacing may enhance
prelexical word identification processes. The former effect American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.f rev.). Washington,
may be related to improvement in what is currently defined DC: Author.
as the WM system, although the latter may be associated Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Clar-
with increased efficiency in the STM system. In current endon Press.
models, WM is held to be responsible for short-term pro- Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identifi-
cessing functions of the system (such as rehearsal), whereas cation in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students.
STM is thought to be responsible for the passive storage Brain and Language, 40, 471-490.
functions (Swanson, 1994). Hence, for dyslexic children the Breznitz, Z. (1987a). Increasing first graders' reading accuracy and
effects of reading acceleration may enhance processing op- comprehension by accelerating their reading rates. Journal of
erations, whereas for normal readers acceleration may in- Educational Psychology, 79, 236-242.
crease capacity. Breznitz, Z. (1987b). Reducing the gap in reading performance
between Israeli lower- and middle-class first grade pupils. Jour-
Swanson (1994) has suggested that among other roles, nal of Psychology, 121, 491-501.
WM may act as a coordinator for different verbal memory Breznitz, Z. (1988). Reading performance of first graders: The
processes. In the present study, the dyslexic children re- effect of pictorial distractors. Journal of Research in Education,
duced decoding errors and increased comprehension and 83, 47-53.
context-based memory during reading acceleration. One Breznitz, Z. (1989). Teacher evaluation of pupils' reading perfor-
interpretation of this result is that for these children, fast mance: Criteria for assessment. Reading Performance, 26,
pacing may improve reading effectiveness by facilitating 174-181.
the interaction between short- and long-term memory stor- Breznitz, Z. (1990). Vocalization and pauses in fast-paced reading.
age functions through increased WM activity. As a result, Journal of General Psychology, 117, 153-161.
Breznitz, Z. (1991). The beneficial effect of accelerating reading
better integration between top-down and bottom-up pro- rate on dyslexic readers' reading comprehension. In M. Snow-
cesses may occur. ling & M, Thomson (Eds.), Dyslexia: Integrating theory and
On the basis of connectionist models of reading that practice (pp. 236-245). London: Whurr.
portray the reading process as involving parallel activation Breznitz, Z. (1993). Short-circuiting phonological limitations in
of phonological, orthographic, and semantic subsystems dyslexia: The beneficial effects of accelerated reading pace. In
EFFECTS OF READING ACCELERATION AMONG DYSLEXICS 297
P. Tallal, A. M. Galaburda, R. R. Linas, & C. Von Euler (Eds.), Morrison, F. J. (1991). Learning (and not learning) to read: A
Temporal information processing in the nervous system: Vol. developmental framework. In L. Reiben & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.),
682. Special reference to dyslexia and dyspkasia: Annals of the Learning to read (pp. 163-174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
New YorkAcadamy of Sciences (pp. 321-322). New York: New Nitzan Center for Learning Disabilities. (1988). Reading Skills
York Academy of Sciences. Test. Tel Aviv, Israel: Nitzan.
Breznitz, Z. (1997). Enhancing the reading of dyslexic children by Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F. A., & Rack, R. (1990). Organi-
reading acceleration and auditory masking. Journal of Educa- zation, heritability, and remediation of component word recog-
tional Psychology, 89, 103-113. nition and language skills in disabled readers. In T. H. Carr &
Breznitz, Z., DeMarco, A., & Hakerem, G. (1993). Topographic B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component
measures of cerebral activity during reading of text at fast- and skills approaches (pp. 261-322). San Diego, CA: Academic
slow-paced rates. Brain Topography, 6, 117-121. Press.
Breznitz, Z., DeMarco, A., Shammi, P., & Hakerem, G. (1994). Ortar, G., & Segev, N. (1980). Reading comprehension test for
Self-paced versus fast-paced reading rates and their effect upon grades one through five. Jerusalem, Israel: Ministry of Educa-
comprehension and event-related potentials. Journal of Genetic
tion and Culture.
Psychology, 155, 397-408.
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford Univer-
Breznitz, Z., & Share, D. L. (1992). The effect of accelerated
sity Press.
reading rate on memory of text. Journal of Educational Psy-
chology, 84, 193-199. Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1977). Discourse comprehen-
Brooks, D. N. (1975). Long- and short-term memory in head sion and sources of individual differences. In M. A. Just & P. A.
injured patients. Cortex, 11, 329-340. Carpenter (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension (pp.
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1989). The role of working 144-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
memory in language comprehension. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky Ronen, R., & Breznitz, Z. (1997). The effects of reading rate
(Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert acceleration on attention in dyslexic readers. Manuscript in
A. Simon (pp. 31-68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. preparation,
Carver, R. P. (1990). Reading rate: A review of research and Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). Dyslexia in a computational model of
theory. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. word recognition in reading. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R.
Cohen, R. L. (1982). Individual differences in short-term memory. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 243-273). Hillsdale,
International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 11, NJ: Erlbaum.
43-77. Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and
Cohen, R. L., & Heath, M. (1990). The development of serial reading disorder: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139-168.
short-term memory and the articulatory loop hypothesis. Intel- Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine
ligence, 14, 151-171. que non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
Coltheart, M. (1987). The psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Simon, H. A. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science, 183, 482-488.
Erlbaum. Snowling, M. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme
Compton, D. L., & Carlisle, J. F. (1994). Speed of word recogni- correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Ex-
tion as a distinguishing characteristic of reading disabilities. perimental Child Psychology, 29, 294-305.
Educational Psychological Review, 6, 115-140. Stanovich, K. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model
Dempster, F. N, (1981). Memory span sources of individual and of individual differences in the development of reading fluency.
developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63-100. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32-71.
Ehri, L. C , & Wilce, L. S. (1983). Development of word identi- Stanovich, K. (1984). The interactive-compensatory model of
fication speed in skilled and less skilled beginning readers. reading: A confluence of developmental, experimental and ed-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 3-18. ucational psychology, RASE, 5(3), 11-19.
Frith, U. (1986). Psychological aspects of orthographic skills:
Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Feeman, D. J. (1981). A longitu-
Development and disorder. In G. Augst (Ed.), New trends in
dinal study of sentence context effects in second grade children:
graphemics and orthography (pp. 218-233). Berlin, Germany:
Tests of an interactive-compensatory model. Journal of Exper-
Walter de Gruyter.
imental Child Psychology, 32, 185-199.
Goldsmith-Phillips, J. (1989). Word and context in reading devel-
opment: A test of the interactive-compensatory hypothesis. Swanson, H. L. (1994). Short-term memory and working memory:
Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 299-305. Do both contribute to our understanding of academic achieve-
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and ment in children and adults with learning disabilities? Journal of
reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6-10. Learning Disabilities, 27, 34-50.
Jorm, A. F. (1983). Specific reading retardation and working mem- Thomdike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford-
ory: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 311-342. Binet Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1987). The psychology of reading Torgesen, J. K., & Houck, D. G. (1980). Processing deficiencies in
and language comprehension. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. learning-disabled children who perform poorly on the Digit
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text Span test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 141-160.
comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phono-
363-394. logical processing and its causal role in the acquisition of read-
Lesgold, A. M., & Resnick, L. B. (1982). How reading disabilities ing skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
develop: Perspectives from a longitudinal study. In J. T. Das, R. Wechsler, D. (1977). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—
Mulcahy, & A. E. Wall (Eds.), Theory and research in learning Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
disabilities (pp. 155-187). New York: Plenum.
Leu, D. J., DeGroff, L., & Simons, H. (1986). Predictable texts and
interactive-compensatory hypotheses: Evaluating individual dif- Received February 29, 1996
ferences in reading ability, context use, and comprehension. Revision received July 15, 1996
Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 347-352. Accepted August 21, 1996 •

You might also like