Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-019-01306-8

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

The interplay between self‑regulation and affectivity in binge eating


among adolescents
Eva Van Malderen1   · Lien Goossens1 · Sandra Verbeken1 · Elisa Boelens1 · Eva Kemps2

Received: 20 December 2018 / Accepted: 1 March 2019


© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Binge eating among adolescents is associated with negative developmental outcomes. From a cognitive perspective, the role
of impaired self-regulation is increasingly emphasized as an underlying factor in binge eating, whereas the affect regulation
model proposes that affectivity is a key factor in explaining binge eating. Studies combining both perspectives are scarce,
but necessary to add to the understanding of this pathological eating behavior. Therefore, the aim of the current study was
to investigate unique and joint contributions of both factors in understanding binge eating among adolescents. Participants
were 301 adolescents (10–17 years; 67.2% girls; Mage = 13.46 years; SD = 1.99) from the general community. Adolescents
self-reported on different types of binge eating episodes (loss of control over eating in general, objective and subjective binge
eating in particular), self-regulation (general self-regulation and inhibitory control) and affectivity (positive and negative).
The parents were questioned about their children’s self-regulatory capacities. Results revealed main effects of self-regulatory
capacities (adolescent report) and negative affectivity in predicting objective binge eating. In addition, negative affectivity
interacted with self-regulation (parent report) to predict objective binge eating, whereas positive affectivity interacted with
self-regulation (adolescent report) to predict subjective binge eating. No significant effects were found for loss of control
over eating specifically. Both self-regulation and affectivity each make unique as well as joint contributions to binge eating
among adolescents, with results differing across types of binge eating episodes and informants. Theoretical and practical
implications are discussed.

Keywords  Adolescents · Binge eating · Self-regulation · Affectivity

Introduction objective and subjective binge eating episodes. While both


types are characterized by loss of control over eating (i.e.,
Binge eating LOC), during an objective binge eating episode (i.e., OBE),
an objectively large amount of food is consumed, whereas
Recent prevalence rates suggest that worldwide approxi- during a subjective binge eating episode (i.e., SBE) [9], the
mately one third of adolescents report binge eating episodes amount of food consumed is considered to be large accord-
[1–4]. Over the past decade, researchers have found that the ing to the individual but not to others. Importantly, these
subjective feeling of loss of control while eating [5] can different types of binge eating episodes have been shown to
be considered the key feature of binge eating [6–8]. How- be clinically different constructs in adolescents and are dif-
ever, based on the amount of food that is eaten during a ferentially associated with clinical features such as depres-
binge eating episode, a distinction has been made between sive symptoms. For example, Fitzsimmons‐Craft et al. [10]
and Goldschmidt et al. [11] found that OBE may be indica-
tive for eating-related distress, whereas SBE may reflect
* Eva Van Malderen
eva.vanmalderen@ugent.be more generalized distress related to both eating and mood
psychopathology such as depressive symptoms. In addition,
1
Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Goossens, Soenens, and Braet [12] found that the combined
Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, presence of OBE and SBE was associated with more mood-
9000 Ghent, Belgium
related pathology. Despite these findings, previous stud-
2
School of Psychology, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, ies have often focused on either LOC and/or OBE as the
Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

primary outcome variable, whereas SBE has largely been In addition, Van Malderen et al. [2] found more overall self-
ignored. In addition, while these results seem to indicate that regulatory impairments among adolescents (10–17 years
the binge eating episodes differ with regard to mood-related old) who reported binge eating compared to those who did
pathology, it tells little about whether temperament-based not, but they did not find any group differences for inhibitory
affectivity dimensions (i.e., traits) may add to the explana- control specifically. Despite this evidence for the underlying
tion of different types of binge eating. role of self-regulation in binge eating, it remains difficult to
Overall, binge eating episodes are associated with a draw firm conclusions with respect to inhibitory control.
wide range of physical (e.g., weight/fat gain, obesity) and It is important to note that research regarding the role of
psychosocial (e.g., depressive symptoms, low self-esteem) self-regulation in binge eating has been conducted rather
problems [13, 14]. In addition, high comorbidity rates have independently from other possible explanatory factors. How-
been reported between binge eating and other types of eat- ever, it is assumed that problematic eating behavior such as
ing pathologies (e.g., restraint eating, food-related craving; binge eating may not be explained by one single factor (e.g.,
[15]). Moreover, longitudinal research has shown that binge self-regulation), but rather is the result of a combination of
eating may be a precursor to clinical eating disorders (e.g., multiple factors [30]. Therefore, an investigation of multiple
Boulimia Nervosa) or other types of psychopathology (e.g., explanatory factors will be crucial to furthering our under-
addiction) [16]. Given the high prevalence rates of binge eat- standing of binge eating in youth.
ing among adolescents and the many aversive and long-term
consequences, research into the factors that add to the under- Binge eating and affectivity
standing of this pathological eating behavior is necessary in
the context of prevention and early intervention. Over recent Another valuable research line in the binge eating domain
years, efforts have been made to examine these underlying focuses on the possible role of affectivity in binge eating
factors in youth, but they remain to be fully understood (e.g., among adolescents. Affectivity can be conceptualized as
[2, 17–20]). the way people react to and interact with the environment,
therefore often being referred to as the reactive dimension of
Binge eating and self‑regulation temperament [31, 33]. This reactive dimension of tempera-
ment consists of two factors: (1) negative affectivity, which
Self-regulation can be defined as the regulation of and by can be described as the susceptibility to experience negative
oneself to obtain long-term goals [21], and consists of dif- feelings and distress and (2) positive affectivity, which can
ferent cognitive capacities such as working memory, cog- be defined as the susceptibility to experience positive feel-
nitive flexibility, inhibitory control, problem solving and ings and joy [33]. Like self-regulation, negative affectivity
planning. It has been well established that difficulties with has been related to different kinds of psychopathology (e.g.,
self-regulation are a transdiagnostic risk factor for develop- [34, 35]). The affect regulation model proposes that high
ing psychopathology [22]. As maintaining healthy eating levels of negative affectivity are a key factor in the develop-
behavior requires a number of everyday self-regulatory skills ment and maintenance of binge eating [36–38]. In support,
(e.g., deciding what to do when confronted with palatable negative affectivity has been found to be a robust risk factor
foods) and given the compulsive nature of binge eating [23], for binge eating in adults (e.g., [39–41]). In children and
the role of self-regulation and specifically inhibitory control adolescents, negative affectivity has been associated with
is increasingly emphasized in binge eating research. Empiri- binge eating cross-sectionally, longitudinally and experi-
cal studies examining the relationship between inhibitory mentally (e.g., [42, 43]).
control and binge eating in adults show mixed results. Some As negative and positive affectivity are two distinct tem-
studies have found evidence for impaired inhibitory con- peramental traits in which people can vary independently,
trol in adults who experience binge eating (e.g., [24–26]), the literature on positive affectivity in adolescent psycho-
whereas others have not, but instead found impairments in pathology has grown in recent years [44]. In the area of
other aspects of self-regulation such as problem solving, depression in particular, low levels of positive affectivity
cognitive flexibility and working memory (e.g., [27, 28]). are considered to be a robust risk factor [45]; however, the
Although self-regulation is still developing during adoles- role of positive affectivity has also been acknowledged in
cence [4, 29], only limited studies have focused on inves- other types of psychopathology [46]. Although in the area
tigating its role in binge eating among younger samples. of eating problems, research on positive affectivity is still in
As in adult studies, findings have been inconclusive. For its infancy; some studies have demonstrated a role for this
example, Goldschmidt et al. [17] showed that obese children affectivity dimension [38]. For example, Smyth et al. [47]
(9–12 years old) who experience binge eating encountered observed a decrease in positive affectivity in women with
more problems with working memory compared to obese eating disorders. Furthermore, a recent lab study showed
children without binge eating and normal weight controls. that a decrease in positive affectivity may cause body

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

dissatisfaction in early adolescence [48]. However, the role binge eating in adult women. To date, only one study has
of positive affectivity in binge eating among adolescents is explored the interaction between self-regulation and affectiv-
currently unknown and its examination may increase our ity in an adolescent sample experiencing binge eating [59].
understanding of the contribution of affectivity to this patho- Results revealed that impaired inhibitory control in combi-
logic eating behavior. nation with high levels of negative affectivity were related
to greater overall severity of loss of control over eating [5].
Binge eating, self‑regulation and affectivity To our knowledge, no study has yet examined both self-
regulation and affectivity (i.e., both negative and positive
As self-regulatory processes and affectivity can influence affectivity) as explanatory factors in an adolescent sample
each other bi-directionally [49–51], researchers have shown experiencing different types of binge eating (i.e., LOC, OBE
increasing interest in combining these factors in eating and SBE). However, knowledge gained from such a study
pathology research. Within this line of research, negative will add considerably to our understanding of the underly-
urgency (i.e., the tendency to act impulsively when dis- ing mechanisms of these types of binge eating episodes in
tressed) has been found to play an important role in binge adolescence, because it can distinguish the unique and joint
eating (e.g., [38, 52]). However, because the construct of contributions of both vulnerability factors. Accordingly,
negative urgency constitutes a combination of both self- outcomes may determine which (combination of) aspects
regulatory and affective processes, it is not possible to dis- should be targeted in prevention and early intervention
tinguish the unique and combined influences of these pro- programs.
cesses. Thus, our understanding of the nature of the complex
interplay between these two factors in the context of binge Current study
eating remains to be fully understood.
Therefore, over recent years, efforts have been made to Thus, the aim of the current study was to investigate the
increase our understanding of both the unique and combined unique and joint contributions of self-regulation and affec-
influences of self-regulatory and affective processes on binge tivity in predicting binge eating among adolescents. Impor-
eating; however, studies have mainly been conducted in adult tantly, we included different types of binge eating (i.e., OBE,
samples. Moreover, in these studies, only LOC or OBE epi- SBE and LOC), as previous research has already shown that
sodes have been included as outcome variables, without differences between these types exist in terms of their associ-
considering SBE. For example, both Racine et al. [53] and ations with, for example, depressive symptoms [10–12]. To
Mason et al. [54] showed that adult women from the general this end, the study addressed two main research questions.1
community who experience LOC or OBE could be charac- First, it was examined whether self-regulatory capacities in
terized by a combination of high impulsive behavior (i.e., general, and inhibitory control in particular, and positive and
low self-regulation) and high levels of negative affectivity. negative affectivity (i.e., unique effects) significantly con-
In addition, in clinical populations, Manasse et al. [55] found tribute to different types of binge eating. In line with previ-
that adult women with binge eating disorder (i.e., at least ous research, it was hypothesized that greater self-regulatory
12 OBE episodes over the last 3 months according DSM-5) difficulties, as well as lower levels of positive affectivity and
[57] who experienced more inhibitory control difficulties higher levels of negative affectivity would independently
(i.e., low self-regulation), as well as higher levels of negative increase the likelihood of experiencing binge eating (e.g.,
affectivity, showed fewer benefits from treatment and any [18, 43, 48]). Second, the current study investigated whether,
treatment effects were slower to emerge. Furthermore, Leehr and how, both self-regulation and affectivity as vulnerability
et al. [57] found evidence for an interaction between high factors interact to predict binge eating among adolescents
impulsivity (i.e., low self-regulation) and impaired emotion (i.e., joint effects). Based on recent research in adult samples
regulation capacities (reflected in high levels of negative [53, 55], the combination of specifically poor self-regulatory
affectivity) in binge eating disorder among adults with the skills and high negative affectivity was predicted to produce
use of neuroimaging techniques. It is important to empha- the most vulnerable profile for experiencing binge eating.
size that all these studies focused exclusively on negative Because limited studies investigated the interaction between
affectivity without investigating the possible role of positive self-regulation and positive affectivity in the explanation of
affectivity. Smith et al. [58] did examine both affectivity binge eating in adolescents, no a priori prediction was made
dimensions (negative and positive) in interaction with self-
regulation in predicting the probability of binge eating in an
1
adult community sample. They found significant interactions   Importantly, hypotheses for both research questions were based
between high levels of impulsive decision making (i.e., low on the available evidence in the current literature and consequently
included only for LOC or OBE as outcome variables. Including SBE
self-regulation) and high levels of both negative and posi- as a third outcome variable was exploratory in nature and therefore
tive affectivity in predicting the probability of experiencing no a priori predictions were made for this type of binge eating.

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

about this relationship. Similarly, since previous research is episode during the last month [20]. Previous research has
inconclusive regarding the specific contribution of inhibitory shown that the ChEDE-Q has good reliability and validity
control in adolescent binge eating, no a priori predictions for assessing eating pathology among adolescents [62, 63].
were made about this aspect of self-regulation.
Self‑regulation

Method All participants and their parents filled out the ‘Behavior
Rating Inventory of Executive Function’ (BRIEF; [64, 65]).
Participants and procedure The adolescent version of the BRIEF is suitable for children
and adolescents between the ages of 5 and 18 years and
Participants were 301 adolescents (10–17  years; 67.2% consists of 68 items; the parent variant of the BRIEF has 75
girls; Mage = 13.46 years; SD = 1.99) and their parents from items. Response options for items range from 1 (never) to
the general community. Participants were recruited by 3 (often) in both versions of the questionnaire, with higher
3rd year psychology students in the context of a practical scores reflecting greater self-regulatory difficulties. Both
course. Each student was instructed to recruit two partici- versions of the questionnaire comprise two broad indices: a
pants between 10 and 17 years old. All youngsters signed ‘Behavioral Regulation’ index and a ‘Metacognition’ index.
an informed consent which provided information about the Each index is further subdivided into different underlying
study. As all participants were underage, their parents also subscales. In the current study, the ‘Behavioral Regulation’
provided active consent for their own participation in the index (BEH; e.g., ‘I/my child have/has trouble going from
study as well as that of their child. Data collection took place one activity to another’) and the ‘Inhibition’ subscale [INH;
in the participant’s home. Participants completed several e.g., ‘I/my child interrupt(s) others’] were used to assess
questionnaires on paper under supervision of the psychology self-regulatory skills. The BRIEF has been shown to have
student. All students were carefully instructed and trained in good reliability and validity [66]. Internal consistency in the
the data collection protocol, and contact details of the main current study was very good for both the adolescent version
researcher were provided in case there were any questions (BEH-A: α = .86 and INH-A: α = .72) and the parent version
during the home visit. No incentives were provided for par- (BEH-P: α = .92 and INH-P: α = .80).
ticipation. The procedure was approved by Ghent Univer-
sity’s Faculty Ethics Committee. The study is part of a larger Affectivity
PhD project on binge eating among adolescents.
The trait version of the ‘Positive and Negative Affectivity
Schedule for Children’ (PANAS-C; [67]) was used to cap-
Materials ture levels of positive and negative affectivity. This self-
report questionnaire consists of 30 items/emotions on a
Binge eating 5-point scale (ranging from 1 = not at all to 5 = a lot). Young-
sters indicate to which extent they usually experience each
To assess binge eating, participants self-reported on this eat- emotion. The PANAS-C has two subscales, one for assess-
ing behavior using the ‘Children’s Eating Disorder Examina- ing positive affectivity (PA; e.g., ‘happy’) and the other for
tion Questionnaire’ (ChEDE-Q; [60]). This questionnaire is assessing negative affectivity (NA; e.g., ‘sad’). Each sub-
a Dutch translation and adaptation for children and adoles- scale consists of 15 items. The PANAS-C has good validity
cents of the ‘Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire’ [67]. Internal consistency in the current study was very good
(EDE-Q; [61]). It can be used in youngsters between 8 and for both subscales (α = .87 for NA and α = .83 for PA).
18 years of age and differentiates different forms of binge
eating. The questionnaire provides a measurement of objec- Control variables
tive binge eating episodes (OBE) as well as subjective binge
eating episodes [9]. Youngsters were categorized into one Age and gender were self-reported. In addition, each partici-
or other group if they had at least one such episode over the pant was measured and weighed at home using calibrated
past month. Thus, a particular participant could be classified instruments, from which an adjusted body mass index (km/
into both the OBE and the SBE group. Moreover, as the feel- m2) was computed [(actual body mass index (kg/m2)/percen-
ing of loss of control over eating [5] is inherent in both forms tile 50 of body mass index for age and gender) × 100] [68].
of binge eating, this additional outcome measure can be Percentile 50 of body mass index was calculated using Flem-
derived from the responses to the OBE and SBE questions. ish normative data which includes age and gender of the par-
More specifically, adolescents were categorized as experi- ticipants [69]. As previous research has shown associations
encing LOC when they reported at least one OBE or SBE between binge eating and age, gender and adjusted body

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

mass index [4, 13, 20], these demographics were included Results
as control variables in all analyses.
Descriptive results
Statistical analysis
Adjusted body mass index of all participants ranged from
To investigate the research questions, a series of binary 58.62 to 189.36 (M = 104.76, SD = 21.47). Of the sam-
logistic regression analyses were conducted using SPSS ple, 9.8% of participants were classified as underweight
(version 24.0). This type of analysis can predict the prob- (adjusted body mass index ≤ 85), 74.7% as normal weight
ability of a categorical outcome variable (i.e., LOC, OBE (85 < adjusted body mass index < 120), 7.3% as overweight
or SBE) by different independent variables. Binge eating (120 < adjusted body mass index < 140) and 8.2% as obese
was considered a categorical variable (0 = no episode, 1 = at (adjusted body mass index ≥ 140) [73]. According to the
least one episode over the last month). Before running the ChEDE-Q [60], 31.5% of all adolescents reported having
analyses, all continuous variables were centered around had at least one episode of LOC over the past month (range
their means [70]. All analyses were performed with the 0–20). For OBE and SBE, the percentages were 18.2%
same hierarchical structure of inputting variables. First, a (range 0–16) and 19.1% (range 0–15), respectively. All
specific type of binge eating was entered as the categorical descriptive statistics (sample characteristics and correlations
dependent variable (either LOC, OBE or SBE). Second, age, between all variables of interest) can be found in Table 1.
gender and adjusted body mass index were entered as control
variables in block 1. Third, self-regulation (either BEH-A, The role of self‑regulation and affectivity in LOC
INH-A, BEH-P or INH-P) and affectivity (NA and PA) were
entered as independent variables in block 2 (main effects). Results of all logistic regressions with self-regulation, affec-
Lastly, the interactions between self-regulation and affectiv- tivity and their interaction in predicting the probability of
ity (NA × self-regulation and PA × self-regulation) were LOC revealed no significant main or interaction effects.
included in block 3. Given the different outcome measures More specifically, no significant effects were found with NA
for self-regulation (four BRIEF outcomes), this procedure as the moderator in the relationship between self-regulation
resulted in four logistic regressions for each type of binge and LOC, nor with PA as the moderator in this relationship.2
eating. For the sake of brevity, only the full logistic regres-
sion model (including independent variables of all blocks) The role of self‑regulation and affectivity in OBE
was displayed in tables in the result section. Furthermore,
only significant main and interaction effects are interpreted Results of the logistic regressions with self-regulation,
further, and when there was a significant interaction effect, affectivity and their interaction in predicting the probabil-
the remaining main effects were not interpreted further. ity of OBE revealed significant main and interaction effects
Significant interaction effects were interpreted using (Table 2).
PROCESS (model 1), an add-on utility in SPSS [71] to test Model 1, including BEH-A, was significant with signifi-
the conditional effect of the independent variable on the cant main effects of BEH-A (p = .050, OR = 1.549) and NA
dependent variable. To interpret the interaction effects, the (p = .020, OR = 1.660). Specifically, high BEH-A and high
Johnson–Neyman technique was used [72]. This technique NA significantly increased the probability of OBE.
reports the region of significance for the conditional inter- Model 2, including INH-A, was significant with a signifi-
action in terms of standard deviations from the mean of the cant main effect of NA (p = .005, OR = 1.724) such that high
moderator, as well as the percentile of the distribution cor- NA significantly increased the probability of OBE.
responding to that point. The significant interaction effects Model 3, including BEH-P, was significant with a sig-
were represented graphically showing the simple slopes for nificant main effect of NA (p = .005, OR = 1.743) as
the independent variable (self-regulation) at low (10th per- well as a significant BEH-P x NA interaction (p = .035,
centile) and high (90th percentile) levels of the moderator OR = .690). The interaction effect was significantly positive
(affectivity). For all analyses, p values ≤ .050 were consid- for NA < −.200 SDs (44.58th percentile). Figure 1 shows
ered statistically significant. Odds ratios (OR) were used as the interaction by way of simple slopes representing the
the effect size measure in all analyses. association between BEH-P and OBE at low (10th percen-
tile) and high (90th percentile) levels of NA. As shown, at

2
  For the sake of brevity, we have simply noted non-significant
results. Tables with an overview of the full results can be obtained
from the first author upon request.

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

Table 1  Descriptive statistics M or % (SD) Min–max BEH-A INH-A BEH-P INH-P NA PA

Gender 67.2% female


Age 13.46 (1.99) 10–17 .180 .158** −.062** −.041 .120* −.223**
AdjBMI 104.76 (21.47) 58.62–189.36 .034 .092 .035 .113 .036 .098
LOC 31.5% LOC 0–20
OBE 18.2% OBE 0–16
SBE 19.12% SBE 0–15
BEH-A 48.15 (9.29) 31–81 1 .816** .315** .305** .484** −.297**
INH-A 19.89 (4.26) 12–43 1 .214** .371** .236** −.070
BEH-P 39.89 (8.97) 28–75 1 .791** .306** −.194**
INH-P 13.44 (3.22) 10–27 1 .134** −.092
NA 31.56 (8.99) 15–58 1 −.301**
PA 42.41 (7.33) 13–59 1

AdjBMI adjusted body mass index, LOC loss of control over eating, OBE objective binge eating, SBE sub-
jective binge eating, BEH-A BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (adolescent report), INH-A BRIEF Inhi-
bition subscale (adolescent report), BEH-P BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (parent report), INH-P
BRIEF Inhibition subscale (parent report), NA negative affectivity, PA positive affectivity
*p ≤ .050
**p ≤ .010
***p ≤ .001

low levels of NA (solid line), high scores on BEH-P signifi- PA is only statistically significant at very low and very high
cantly increased the probability of OBE (b = .781, SE = .334, levels of PA. More specifically, at very low levels of PA
p = .019). However, at high levels of NA (dashed line), (−4.01 < PA < −1.53), low BEH-A significantly increased
BEH-P did not significantly change the probability of OBE the probability of SBE (p < .05), whereas at very high lev-
(b = −.152, SE = .231, p = .512). els of PA (1.97 < PA < 2.26), high BEH-A significantly
Model 4, including INH-P, was significant with a signifi- increased the probability of SBE (p < .05).
cant main effect of NA (p = .004, OR = 1.737) whereby high
NA again significantly increased the probability of OBE.
Discussion
The role of self‑regulation and affectivity in SBE
The aim of the current study was to further elucidate the
Results of the logistic regressions with self-regulation, affec- underlying mechanisms of binge eating among adolescents.
tivity and their interaction in predicting the probability of To this end, both the unique and the joint contributions of
SBE revealed one significant interaction effect in model 1, self-regulation (general self-regulation and inhibitory con-
including BEH-A (Table 3). trol specifically) and affectivity (positive and negative) were
Although the overall model itself was not significant, add- investigated in predicting the likelihood of experiencing dif-
ing the interaction terms to the model (block 3) significantly ferent types of binge eating (i.e., LOC, OBE and SBE).
improved the previous model which contained only the main
effects [χ2(2) = 6.108, p = .047]. BEH-A and PA significantly Unique and joint contributions of self‑regulation
interacted in predicting the probability of SBE (p = .030, and affectivity in binge eating
OR = 1.590). The interaction effect was significantly positive
for PA < −1.534 SDs (6.67th percentile) and significantly To address the first research question, the main effects of
negative for PA > 1.974 SDs (98.75th percentile). Figure 2 self-regulation and affectivity were examined as explana-
shows the interaction by way of simple slopes representing tory factors for LOC, OBE and SBE. Results revealed
the association between BEH-A and SBE at low (10th per- significant main effects of self-regulatory capacities and
centile) and high (90th percentile) levels of PA. As shown, negative affectivity, but only in predicting the probabil-
both at low levels of PA (solid line) and at high levels of PA ity of OBE. Specifically, greater self-regulatory problems
(dashed line), BEH-A did not significantly change the prob- (self-reported by the adolescent) and high negative affec-
ability of SBE (b = −.629, SE = .335, p = .061 and b = .408, tivity independently increased the likelihood of report-
SE = .267, p = .127, respectively). However, when inspecting ing OBE in youngsters. The independent contributions
the regions of significance, it is clear that the influence of of self-regulatory problems and high negative affectivity

13
Table 2  Logistic regression analysis: self-regulation × affectivity in predicting the probability of OBE
Model ­1a Model ­2b Model ­3c Model ­4d
Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

Covariates
Gender .677 −.376 (.448) .410 .985 −.442 (.446) .321 .734 −.386 (.450) .392 .722 −.380 (.448) .396
Age 1.312 .220 (.192) .252 2.024 .274 (.193) .155 2.098 .283 (.195) .147 1.886 .264 (.192) .170
AdjBMI 2.499 .303 (.192) .114 3.121 .333 (.188) .077 2.298 .290 (.191) .130 2.755 .317 (.191) .097
NA 5.420 .507 (.218) .020* 8.008 .545 (.193) .005** 7.844 .556 (.198) .005** 8.388 .552 (.191) .004**
PA .411 −.138 (.215) .521 .378 −.119 (.193) .539 .464 −.137 (.201) .496 .554 −.145 (.195) .457
Self-regulation 3.801 .437 (.224) .050* .001 .004 (.185) .981 2.568 .309 (.193) .109 .057 .045 (.188) .812
NA × self- 2.274 −.215 (.190) .259 .187 −.081 (.188) .666 4.444 −.372 (.176) .035* 2.766 −.317 (.191) .096
regulation
PA × self-regu- .189 .093 (.214) .664 .015 .024 (.195) .902 .053 −.038 (.165) .818 .205 −.079 (.175) .650
lation
Constant 41.003 −1.563 (.244) .000*** 45.516 −1.545 (.229) .000*** 44.056 −1.538 (.232) .000*** 47.449 −1.573 (.228) .000***
Model test χ2 (8) = 26.198, p = .001** χ2 (8) = 21.413, p = .006** χ2 (8) = 26.312, p = .001** χ2 (8) = 23.250, p = .003**
−2LL (Nagel- 198.669 (.171) 203.454 (.141) 199.349 (.170) 202.412 (.151)
kerke ­R2)

OBE objective binge eating, NA negative affectivity, PA positive affectivity, AdjBMI adjusted body mass index
*p ≤ .050
**p ≤ .010
***p ≤ .001
Each model includes a different aspect of self-regulation:
a
 BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (adolescent report)
b
 BRIEF Inhibition subscale (adolescent report)
c
 BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (parent report)
d
 BRIEF Inhibition subscale (parent report)

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

Fig. 1  Interaction between 0.5


BEH-P and NA in predicting 0.45 Low NA
the probability of OBE
0.4

Probability of OBE
0.35 High NA
b = -.152, SE = .231, p = .512
0.3

0.25

0.2

0.15

0.1
b = .781, SE = .334, p = .019
0.05

0
Low BEH-P High BEH-P

Note. BEH-P = BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (parent report); NA = Negative Affectivity; OBE = Objective Binge Eating
*
p ≤ .050, ** p ≤ .010, *** p ≤ .001

were in line with expectations based on similar findings in The second research question focused on whether and
adolescent samples [2, 17, 18, 42, 43]. The results provide how self-regulation and affectivity interact to predict LOC,
further evidence for the important roles of both cognitive OBE and SBE. Results revealed a significant interaction
(i.e., role of self-regulation; [22]) and affective (i.e., role between self-regulation (reported by the parent) and nega-
of negative affectivity; [38]) perspectives on vulnerability tive affectivity in predicting OBE. Closer examination of the
factors of binge eating in adolescents. nature of this interaction showed that, when negative affec-
Contrary to expectations, these main effects were tivity was low (i.e., low susceptibility to experience negative
observed only for one type of binge eating, namely OBE. feelings and distress), youngsters with poor self-regulatory
However, as OBE is recognized by the DSM-5 [57] as the capacities are significantly more at risk of OBE. By con-
main symptom of eating disorders such as bulimia nervosa trast, youngsters with high negative affectivity (i.e., high
and binge eating disorder, this specific type of binge eating susceptibility to experience negative feelings and distress)
episode may be the most clinically relevant and thus the only appear to be at greater risk of experiencing OBE independ-
one in which the main effects of general self-regulation and ent of their self-regulatory capacities. This finding high-
negative affectivity are fully expressed. lights the importance of high levels of negative affectivity
It is important to note that our results did not provide evi- for predicting OBE, which is in line with the central focus of
dence for unique effects of inhibitory control, nor for posi- the affect regulation model of binge eating [38]. Moreover,
tive affectivity in predicting binge eating. The lack of a main this interaction effect between self-regulation and negative
effect of inhibitory control is at odds with some previous affectivity seems to suggest that the role of self-regulation
studies showing impaired inhibitory control in binge eating in predicting OBE is subordinate to the role of negative
(e.g., [25, 26]), but in line with others that similarly showed affectivity, as its detrimental association with binge eating
no impairments in this aspect of self-regulation (e.g., [2, is expressed only in youngsters with low negative affectiv-
17]). It is important to note that all these studies differed ity. Importantly, this interaction between self-regulation and
in the assessment methods they used to capture inhibitory negative affectivity was observed only when self-regulation
control (e.g., questionnaires, fMRI, neuropsychological was reported by the parent. Adolescence can be described
tasks). Different assessment methods may capture differ- as a turbulent period with numerous challenges, for exam-
ent constructs, which may contribute to the mixed results ple, attaining increasing independence from parents. This
(e.g., [74, 75]). This highlights the need for future research may result in more frequent parent–child conflicts, which
to operationalize the construct of inhibitory control not only are often accompanied by high levels of negative affectiv-
using ‘trait-like’ measures of self-regulation in daily life ity [76, 77]. Thus, it is possible that adolescents’ negative
(e.g., with questionnaires such as the BRIEF) but also with affectivity becomes more noticeable or striking to parents in
‘state-like’ measures of self-regulation in a specific situation this age group. Particularly for youngsters with high levels of
(e.g., go/no-go task). The lack of a main effect of positive negative affectivity, this emotionality may dampen the role
affectivity, will be discussed below as this variable did not of poor self-regulatory skills in predicting OBE. To explore
independently predict binge eating, but did so in interaction this possibility, it is important that self-regulation as well
with self-regulation. as affectivity are reported by both the adolescent and the

13
Table 3  Logistic regression analysis: self-regulation × affectivity in predicting the probability of SBE
Model ­1a Model ­2b Model ­3c Model ­4d
Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p Wald χ2 B (SE) p
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

Covariates
 Gender .004 .023 (.385) .951 .007 −.032 (.386) .934 .003 −.020 (.381) .958 .003 −.023 (.382) .953
 Age 1.036 .187 (.183) .309 .916 .176 (.184) .339 1.160 .197 (.183) .281 1.451 .219 (.182) .228
 AdjBMI .017 −.023 (.178) .896 .002 −.009 (.175) .961 .050 −.040 (.179) .823 .044 −.038 (.179) .833
NA .000 −.002 (.195) .922 .006 −.014 (.184) .939 .094 −.057 (.185) .759 .071 −.047 (.178) .790
PA .484 −.136 (.195) .487 .015 −.023 (.188) .903 .214 −.090 (.183) .623 .081 −.052 (.182) .776
Self-regulation .077 −.056 (.203) .781 .085 −.050 (.173) .771 .114 −.066 (.195) .735 .475 .120 (.175) .491
NA × self- .047 .039 (.181) .828 1.672 −.238 (.184) .196 2.690 −.361 (.220) .101 2.177 −.292 (.198) .140
regulation
PA × self-regu- 4.706 .464 (.214) .030* .015 .023 (.186) .904 .805 −.164 (.182) .370 .746 −.149 (.172) .388
lation
Constant 41.200 −1.443 (.225) .000*** 47.871 −1.460 (.211) .000*** 47.692 −1.446 (.209) .000*** 50.414 −1.471 (.207) .000***
Model test χ2 (8) = 7.522, p = .482 χ2 (8) = 3.860, p = .870 χ2 (8) = 4.912, p = .767 χ2 (8) = 4.492, p = .810
−2LL (Nagel- 221.156 (.050) 224.817 (.026) 227.552 (.033) 227.972 (.030)
kerke ­R2)

SBE subjective binge eating, NA negative affectivity, PA positive affectivity, AdjBMI adjusted body mass index
*p ≤ .050
**p ≤ .010
***p ≤ .001
Each model includes a different aspect of self-regulation:
a
 BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (adolescent report)
b
 BRIEF Inhibition subscale (adolescent report)
c
 BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (parent report)
d
 BRIEF Inhibition subscale (parent report)

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

Fig. 2  Interaction between 0.5


BEH-A and PA in predicting the 0.45 Low PA
probability of SBE
0.4

0.35

Probability of SBE
High PA

0.3
b = .408, SE = .267, p = .127
0.25

0.2

0.15

0.1 b = -.629, SE = .335, p = .061

0.05

0
Low BEH-A High BEH-A

Note. BEH-A = BRIEF Behavioral Regulation index (adolescent report); PA = Positive Affectivity; SBE = Subjective Binge Eating
*
p ≤ .050, ** p ≤ .010, *** p ≤ .001

parent. As noted above, when self-regulation was reported good self-regulatory skills in combination with low positive
by the adolescents themselves, self-regulation and negative affectivity corresponds to the typical profile of unsuccessful
affectivity each individually predicted OBE, but not in inter- restrained eaters. Restrained eaters often impose strict die-
action. In line with the reasoning above, it may be that both tary rules but may be perfectly able to regulate themselves
factors are equally relevant from the adolescents’ perspective throughout the day. However, in certain circumstances,
(i.e., main effects), whereas negative affectivity may be more they may fail in their attempts for weight control which
important than self-regulation from the parents’ perspective may result in experiences of binge eating episodes [79–81].
(i.e., interaction effect). Results of the present study may add to the understanding
We further found that self-regulation (reported by the of the circumstances that may cause this breakdown. More
adolescent) interacted with positive affectivity to predict specifically, especially youngsters who are characterized by
SBE. Contrary to expectations (e.g., based on [53, 55]), it low levels of positive affectivity may experience SBE as
was not the combination of poor self-regulatory skills and a strategy to increase positive affectivity when confronted
low positive affectivity that was associated with the greatest with attractive food cues in the environment. Thus, SBE may
likelihood of experiencing SBE. Rather, both high and low function as an emotion regulation strategy to ‘up-regulate’
levels of positive affectivity were associated with a greater positive affectivity. This extends the affect regulation model
risk of SBE. In particular, good self-regulatory skills were of binge eating by showing that the positive reinforcement of
associated with a greater risk of SBE when positive affec- binge eating may not only be driven by decreased levels of
tivity was very low (i.e., very low susceptibility to experi- negative affectivity but also by increased levels of positive
ence positive feelings and joy), whereas poor self-regulatory affectivity [38].
skills was associated with a greater risk of SBE when posi- In the second profile, youngsters with poor self-regula-
tive affectivity was very high (i.e., very high susceptibility tory skills have a higher probability of experiencing SBE
to experience positive feelings and joy). when they have very high levels of positive affectivity.
Interestingly, the interaction between self-regulation and Similar to self-regulation, positive affectivity has also been
positive affectivity produced two ‘vulnerable profiles’ for described as a protective factor in adolescents’ development
experiencing SBE. Because the affect regulation model does (e.g., [46]); however, the present results reveal detrimental
not make specific predictions about the role of positive affec- effects of very high levels of positive affectivity in combi-
tivity in binge eating [38], the current results have impor- nation with poor self-regulatory skills. Previous findings in
tant added value in showing two possible affect regulatory the temperament literature may help to explain this result.
pathways for explaining SBE. In the first profile, youngsters More specifically, Rothbart et al. [33] stated that high levels
with good self-regulatory skills have a higher probability of of positive affectivity are a reflection of high positive emo-
experiencing SBE when they have very low levels of positive tional reactivity to the environment, which places additional
affectivity. Although good self-regulatory skills are gener- demands on self-regulatory capacities. In the current study,
ally considered to be a protective factor in the context of eat- adolescents may find it difficult to lower their high positive
ing pathology (e.g., [78]), the current results show that these emotional reactivity due to their poor self-regulatory skills,
can have an adverse impact in youngsters with very low whereby SBE functions as an emotion regulation strategy
levels of positive affectivity. More specifically, it may be that to ‘down-regulate’ positive affectivity. The potential risk

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

of high levels of positive affectivity has similarly been present design did not allow us to test whether these risk
acknowledged in the eating disorders domain (e.g., [82, factors also serve an important role in predicting the sever-
83]). Especially, Haynos et al. [84] examined the effect of ity of binge eating. Longitudinal and experimental designs
positive affectivity across different types of eating disorders. in samples with more individuals experiencing a broader
They found that the role of high positive affectivity was par- range of binge eating episodes (e.g., subclinical or clinical
ticularly relevant for individuals with anorexia nervosa of samples) are therefore needed. Furthermore, we exclusively
the binge–purge type, in which SBE may be a core symp- used questionnaires to operationalize all variables. To over-
tom [57, 85]. Although the focus of previous research in the come the possible mono-method bias in results [86], it is
binge eating domain has been predominantly on LOC and/or important that the current findings are replicated with dif-
OBE, the present findings strengthen the relevance of poor ferent assessment methods (e.g., behavioral tasks for self-
self-regulatory skills in combination with very high levels of regulation, experimental mood induction).
positive affectivity specifically in predicting SBE. Moreover,
as this interaction between self-regulation and positive affec- Implications
tivity was found only when adolescents themselves reported
on their self-regulatory skills, it emphasizes the subjective The current study has some important theoretical and practi-
experience of SBE. cal implications. First, the results provide further evidence
To summarize, self-regulation and affectivity both play for the role of self-regulatory problems as a risk factor for
unique and combined roles in predicting OBE and SBE. binge eating [2, 18]. Because this points to a general contri-
Although LOC is inherent in both types of binge eating, no bution of self-regulation, and because the role of inhibitory
significant effects were found for LOC. As demonstrated by control is still unclear, future research will need to disentan-
the current findings, there appears to be a substantial differ- gle the underlying factors of this umbrella term. In doing so,
ence in the underlying vulnerability patterns for OBE and it is important that not only inhibitory control is examined
SBE, which may have obviated any effects for LOC. but also other cognitive aspects of self-regulation (e.g., cog-
nitive flexibility, working memory) and that a combination
Strengths and limitations of various assessment methods (e.g., questionnaires and
behavioral tasks) will be used to measure these constructs
The current study has several strengths. First, it sought to [87]. Second, the present results provide further insight into
gain further insight into the complex reality of binge eat- the affect regulation model of binge eating by examining
ing by considering different types of risk factors (i.e., self- both positive and negative affectivity in binge eating. More
regulation and affectivity), both individually and in interac- specifically, results seem to indicate that binge eating may
tion with each other. As research has previously documented serve as an emotion regulation strategy to regulate levels
important differences between different types of binge eat- of negative or positive affectivity. While previous research
ing episodes [12], three types of episodes (i.e., OBE, SBE found that OBE was most associated with eating-related
and LOC) were included as outcome variables. Second, distress, whereas SBE was indicative for mood-related psy-
we recruited a large sample of 301 adolescents from the chopathology [10–12], the present study showed preliminary
general community (10–17 years old). This age group is of evidence for the role of trait affectivity in both types of binge
particular significance, as adolescence has been found to be eating episodes. Specifically, the results seem to suggest that
a risk period for developing eating problems such as binge negative affectivity may be relevant in explaining OBE,
eating [4]. Third, the self-regulatory skills of adolescents while positive affectivity seems to be more important in
were assessed by the adolescents themselves as well as their the context of SBE. Importantly, the current study assessed
parents (i.e., multi-informant). Finally, the current study these concepts as traits through the use of questionnaires.
extends on previous research based on the affect regulation Thus, future research endeavoring to replicate the current
model by including not only negative affectivity but also findings with, for example, an experimental mood induction
positive affectivity [38]. In doing so, the role of affectivity in design to measure state affectivity will be necessary.
the context of binge eating in adolescents could be examined Knowledge derived from research that focuses on clarify-
more comprehensively. ing the roles of self-regulation and affectivity can be used
The current study also has some limitations that need to to refine theoretical models about the development and
be acknowledged. While the chosen data analytic technique maintenance of eating problems in youth. Such models can
could test the probability of binge eating given a specific provide a starting point for setting up screening programs
set of independent variables, the cross-sectional design of to identify high-risk populations. The results of the present
the study means that we cannot draw inferences about the study demonstrate that there may be several important points
predictive role of self-regulation and affectivity in binge eat- to consider in such screening. In particular, the differen-
ing, nor can we conclude about causality. In addition, the tiation of types of binge eating seems important, because

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

different types are associated with (a combination of) dif- 6. Goossens L et al (2009) Loss of control over eating in overweight
ferent explanatory factors. Both self-regulation and affectiv- youngsters: the role of anxiety, depression and emotional eating.
Eur Eat Disord Rev 17(1):68–78
ity seem to be important adolescent characteristics to take 7. Fairburn CG, Wilson GT, Schleimer K (1993) Binge eating:
into account while screening for binge eating, and their role nature, assessment, and treatment. Guilford Press, New York
seems to depend on the type of binge eating (e.g., negative 8. Marcus MD, Kalarchian MA (2003) Binge eating in children and
affectivity for OBE, positive affectivity for SBE) and the adolescents. Int J Eat Disord 34(S1):S47–S57
9. Telman L et al (2015) Denken, doen, durven. Kind Adolescent
informant. Praktijk 14(3):39–41
10. Fitzsimmons-Craft EE et al (2014) Subjective and objective binge
eating in relation to eating disorder symptomatology, depressive
Conclusion symptoms, and self-esteem among treatment-seeking adolescents
with bulimia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev 22(4):230–236
11. Goldschmidt AB et al (2016) Association between objective and
In conclusion, the current study provides evidence for the subjective binge eating and psychopathology during a psychologi-
importance of cognitive as well as affective perspectives cal treatment trial for bulimic symptoms. Appetite 107:471–477
on binge eating. Specifically, self-regulation and affectivity 12. Goossens L, Soenens B, Braet C (2009) Prevalence and character-
istics of binge eating in an adolescent community sample. J Clin
both play unique and combined roles in predicting binge Child Adolesc Psychol 38(3):342–353
eating. However, results largely depended on the type of 13. Tanofsky-Kraff M et al (2011) A prospective study of pediatric
binge eating and the informant. For OBE, main effects of loss of control eating and psychological outcomes. J Abnorm Psy-
self-regulation (adolescent report) and negative affectivity chol 120(1):108
14. Sehm M, Warschburger P (2018) Prospective associations between
were found, as well as an interaction between self-regulation binge eating and psychological risk factors in adolescence. J Clin
(parent report) and negative affectivity. For SBE, however, Child Adolesc Psychol 47(5):770–784
only an interaction effect between self-regulation (adolescent 15. Tanofsky-Kraff M et al (2009) A prospective study of loss of con-
report) and positive affectivity was observed. Replication trol eating for body weight gain in children at high risk for adult
obesity. Int J Eat Disord 42(1):26–30
of these findings with different assessment methods and 16. Herpertz-Dahlmann B et al (2015) Eating disorder symptoms
designs is warranted. do not just disappear: the implications of adolescent eating-dis-
ordered behaviour for body weight and mental health in young
Author contributions  LG, EK and EVM designed the study and wrote adulthood. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24(6):675–684
the protocol. EVM and EB were responsible for the data collection, 17. Goldschmidt AB et al. (2017) Executive functioning in a racially
under supervision of LG. EVM conducted the statistical analyses and diverse sample of children who are overweight and at risk for
wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All other authors edited subse- eating disorders. Appetite 124:43–49
quent drafts of the manuscript, and have approved the final manuscript. 18. Kittel R, Schmidt R, Hilbert A (2017) Executive functions in ado-
lescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. Int J Eat Disord
Funding  The research was supported by the Special Research Fund of 50(8):933–941
Ghent University. The funding body had no role in the study design, 19. Neumark-Sztainer D et al (2011) Dieting and disordered eating
collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing of the manu- behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood: findings from a
script, or decision to submit the manuscript for publication. 10-year longitudinal study. J Am Diet Assoc 111(7):1004–1011
20. Tanofsky-Kraff M et al (2007) A multisite investigation of binge
eating behaviors in children and adolescents. J Consult Clin Psy-
Compliance with ethical standards  chol 75(6):901
21. Nigg JT (2017) Annual research review: on the relations among
Conflict of interest  On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful con-
states that there is no conflict of interest. trol, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition
for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
58(4):361–383
22. Rothbart MK et al (2011) Developing mechanisms of self-regu-
References lation in early life. Emot Rev 3(2):207–213
23. Gearhardt A, White M, Potenza M (2011) Binge eating disorder
1. Kelly NR et al (2016) Emotion dysregulation and loss-of-control and food addiction. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 4(3):201–207
eating in children and adolescents. Health Psychol 35(10):1110 24. Svaldi J et al (2014) General and food-specific inhibitory deficits
2. Van Malderen E et al (2018) Unravelling the association between in binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord 47(5):534–542
inhibitory control and loss of control over eating among adoles- 25. Manasse SM et al (2016) Slowing down and taking a second look:
cents. Appetite 125:401–409 inhibitory deficits associated with binge eating are not food-spe-
3. He J, Cai Z, Fan X (2016) Prevalence of binge and loss of control cific. Appetite 96:555–559
eating among children and adolescents with overweight and obe- 26. Balodis IM et al (2013) Divergent neural substrates of inhibitory
sity: an exploratory meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord 50(2):91–103 control in binge eating disorder relative to other manifestations of
4. Kessler RC et al (2013) The prevalence and correlates of binge obesity. Obesity 21(2):367–377
eating disorder in the World Health Organization World Mental 27. Monica D et  al (2010) Assessment of executive functions in
Health Surveys. Biol Psychiatry 73(9):904–914 obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Revista brasileira
5. Ellis ML, Weiss B, Lochman JE (2009) Executive functions in de psiquiatria 32(4):381–388
children: associations with aggressive behavior and appraisal pro- 28. Manasse SM et  al (2015) Do executive functioning deficits
cessing. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37(7):945–956 underpin binge eating disorder? A comparison of overweight

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

women with and without binge eating pathology. Int J Eat Disord 49. Heatherton TF, Wagner DD (2011) Cognitive neuroscience of
48(6):677–683 self-regulation failure. Trends Cogn Sci 15(3):132–139
29. Crone EA (2009) Executive functions in adolescence: inferences 50. Joormann J, Stanton CH (2016) Examining emotion regulation
from brain and behavior. Dev Sci 12(6):825–830 in depression: a review and future directions. Behav Res Ther
30. Stice E, Desjardins CD (2018) Interactions between risk factors in 86:35–49
the prediction of onset of eating disorders: exploratory hypothesis 51. Okon-Singer H et al (2015) The neurobiology of emotion–cog-
generating analyses. Behav Res Ther 105:52–62 nition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for
31. Zentner M, Bates JE (2008) Child temperament: an integrative future research. Front Hum Neurosci 9:58
review of concepts, research programs, and measures. Int J Dev 52. Fischer S, Smith GT, Cyders MA (2008) Another look at impul-
Sci 2(1–2):7–37 sivity: a meta-analytic review comparing specific dispositions
32. Rothbart MK, Posner MI, Kieras J (2006) Temperament, attention, to rash action in their relationship to bulimic symptoms. Clin
and the development of self-regulation. In: McCartney K, Phil- Psychol Rev 28(8):1413–1425
lips D (eds) Blackwell handbook of early childhood development. 53. Racine SE et al (2015) Examining associations between negative
Blackwell Publishing, Malden, pp 338–357 urgency and key components of objective binge episodes. Int J
33. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000) Temperament and Eat Disord 48(5):527–531
personality: origins and outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol 78(1):122 54. Mason TB et al (2018) Independent and interactive associations
34. Gulley LD, Hankin BL, Young JF (2016) Risk for depression of negative affect, restraint, and impulsivity in relation to binge
and anxiety in youth: the interaction between negative affec- eating among women. Appetite 121:147–153
tivity, effortful control, and stressors. J Abnorm Child Psychol 55. Manasse SM et al (2016) Does impulsivity predict outcome in
44(2):207–218 treatment for binge eating disorder? A multimodal investigation.
35. Hussong AM et al (2017) A systematic review of the unique pro- Appetite 105:172–179
spective association of negative affect symptoms and adolescent 56. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and sta-
substance use controlling for externalizing symptoms. Psychol tistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psy-
Addict Behav 31(2):137 chiatric Pub, Washington, DC
36. Masheb RM, Grilo CM (2006) Emotional overeating and its 57. Leehr EJ et al (2018) Food specific inhibitory control under
associations with eating disorder psychopathology among over- negative mood in binge-eating disorder: evidence from a mul-
weight patients with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord timethod approach. Int J Eat Disord 51(2):112–123
39(2):141–146 58. Smith KE et al (2019) A multimodal, naturalistic investiga-
37. Ivanova IV et al (2015) Negative affect mediates the relation- tion of relationships between behavioral impulsivity, affect, and
ship between interpersonal problems and binge-eating disorder binge eating. Appetite 136:50–57
symptoms and psychopathology in a clinical sample: a test of the 59. Goldschmidt AB et al (2019) Trait-level facets of impulsiv-
interpersonal model. Eur Eat Disord Rev 23(2):133–138 ity and momentary, naturalistic eating behavior in children and
38. Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK (2011) Revisiting the affect regulation adolescents with overweight/obesity. J Psychiatr Res 110:24–30
model of binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological 60. Decaluwé V, Braet C (1999) Child eating disorder examination-
momentary assessment. Psychol Bull 137(4):660 questionnaire. Dutch translation and adaptation of the eating
39. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B (2007) Maintenance of binge eating disorder examination-questionnaire, authored by CG Fairburn
through negative mood: a naturalistic comparison of binge eating & SJ Beglin (Ghent University, Unpublished manuscript)
disorder and bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 40(6):521–530 61. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ (1994) Assessment of eating disor-
40. Berg KC et al (2015) Negative affect prior to and following over- ders: interview or self-report questionnaire? Int J Eat Disord
eating-only, loss of control eating-only, and binge eating episodes 16(4):363–370
in obese adults. Int J Eat Disord 48(6):641–653 62. Van Durme K et al (2015) The detection of eating disorder
41. Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK (2015) Affect regulation and purging: symptoms in adolescence: a comparison between the children’s
an ecological momentary assessment study in purging disorder. J eating disorder examination and the children’s eating disorder
Abnorm Psychol 124(2):399 examination questionnaire. Behav Chang 32(3):190–201
42. Goldschmidt AB et al (2008) The clinical significance of loss of 63. Decaluwé V, Braet C, Fairburn C (2003) Binge eating in obese
control over eating in overweight adolescents. Int J Eat Disord children and adolescents. Int J Eat Disord 33(1):78–84
41(2):153–158 64. Gioia GA et al (2000) Behavior rating inventory of executive
43. Shomaker LB et al (2010) Salience of loss of control for pedi- function: BRIEF. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa
atric binge episodes: does size really matter? Int J Eat Disord 65. Huizinga M, Smidts DP (2010) Age-related changes in execu-
43(8):707–716 tive function: a normative study with the Dutch version of the
44. Gilbert KE (2012) The neglected role of positive emotion in ado- behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF). Child
lescent psychopathology. Clin Psychol Rev 32(6):467–481 Neuropsychol 17(1):51–66
45. Van Beveren ML et al (2018) Don’t worry, be happy: the role of 66. Gioia GA et al (2002) Confirmatory factor analysis of the behav-
positive emotionality and adaptive emotion regulation strategies ior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in a clinical
for youth depressive symptoms. Br J Clin Psychol 57(1):18–41 sample. Child Neuropsychol 8(4):249–257
46. Davis M, Suveg C (2014) Focusing on the positive: a review of 67. Laurent J et al (1999) A measure of positive and negative affect
the role of child positive affect in developmental psychopathology. for children: scale development and preliminary validation. Psy-
Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 17(2):97–124 chol Assess 11(3):326
47. Smyth JM et al (2007) Daily and momentary mood and stress 68. Rolland-Cachera MF, Akrout M, Péneau S (2015) History and
are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia ner- meaning of the body mass index. Interest of other anthropomet-
vosa patients in the natural environment. J Consult Clin Psychol ric measurements. In: The ECOG’s eBook on Child and Ado-
75(4):629 lescent Obesity. European Childhood Obesity Group (ECOG):
48. Goossens L et al (2017) Do changes in affect moderate the asso- Belgium
ciation between attachment anxiety and body dissatisfaction in 69. Roelants M, Hauspie R (2004) Groeicurven 2-20 jaar, Vlaan-
children? An experimental study by means of the Trier Social deren 2004 (Growth charts 2–20 years, Flanders 2004). Retrieved
Stress Test. Eat Behav 26:83–88 December, 2004. 3:2007 (Vrije Universiteit Brussel)

13
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

70. Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR (1991) Multiple regression: test- 80. Kitsantas A, Gilligan TD, Kamata A (2003) College women with
ing and interpreting interactions. Sage, Thousand Oaks, Newbury eating disorders: self-regulation, life satisfaction, and positive/
Park, CA negative affect. J Psychol 137(4):381–395
71. Hayes AF (2013) Model templates for PROCESS for SPSS and 81. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R (2003) Cognitive behaviour
SAS. Retrieved December, 2013. 12:2013 therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treat-
72. Hayes AF, Matthes J (2009) Computational procedures for prob- ment. Behav Res Ther 41(5):509–528
ing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS 82. Berner LA et al (2017) Temporal associations between affective
implementations. Behav Res Methods 41(3):924–936 instability and dysregulated eating behavior in bulimia nervosa. J
73. Braet C, Van Winckel M (2001) Behandelingsstrategieën bij kin- Psychiatr Res 92:183–190
deren met overgewicht. Bohn Stafleu Van Loghum (Care & Cure), 83. Selby EA et al (2015) A perfect storm: examining the synergistic
Houten, pp 11–26 effects of negative and positive emotional instability on promoting
74. Matton A et al (2017) Effortful control as a moderator in the weight loss activities in anorexia nervosa. Front Psychol 6:1260
association between punishment and reward sensitivity and eat- 84. Haynos AF et al (2017) Trajectories of higher-and lower-order
ing styles in adolescent boys and girls. Appetite 111:177–186 dimensions of negative and positive affect relative to restrictive
75. Claes L et al (2006) Impulsiveness and lack of inhibitory control eating in anorexia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol 126(5):495
in eating disorders. Eat Behav 7(3):196–203 85. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z (2011) Eating disorders, DSM–5 and
76. Simmons RG (2017) Moving into adolescence: the impact of clinical reality. Br J Psychiatry 198(1):8–10
pubertal change and school context. Routledge, New York 86. Spector PE (2006) Method variance in organizational research:
77. Cracco E, Goossens L, Braet C (2017) Emotion regulation across truth or urban legend? Organ Res Methods 9(2):221–232
childhood and adolescence: evidence for a maladaptive shift in 87. Dougherty DM et al (2005) Laboratory behavioral measures of
adolescence. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26(8):909–921 impulsivity. Behav Res Methods 37(1):82–90
78. Dohle S, Diel K, Hofmann W (2018) Executive functions and the
self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite 124:4–9
79. Papies EK, Stroebe W, Aarts H (2008) Healthy cognition: pro-
cesses of self-regulatory success in restrained eating. Pers Soc
Psychol Bull 34(9):1290–1300

13

You might also like