Social Networks: Rozemarijn Van Der Ploeg, Christian Steglich, René Veenstra

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Social Networks
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/socnet

The way bullying works: How new ties facilitate the mutual reinforcement
of status and bullying in elementary schools
Rozemarijn van der Ploega,b, , Christian Steglicha, René Veenstraa

a
Department of Sociology, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
b
Department of Pedagogy and Educational Science, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: This study addresses the puzzle how high-status bullies in elementary school are able to maintain high status
Bullying among their classmates despite bullying (some of) them. The dynamic interplay between bullying and status was
Peer status studied, focusing on how relational bullying affects the creation, dissolution, and maintenance of status attri-
Creation and maintenance of ties butions, and vice versa. Longitudinal round-robin peer nomination data were obtained from 82 school classes in
15 Dutch elementary schools (2055 students; 50% boys) followed over three yearly measurements, starting out
in grades 2–5 when students were aged 8-11. An age-dependent effect of bullying on the creation of new status
attributions was found. Whereas the youngest group punished bullying by a refusal to attribute status to the
bully, this turned into a reward of bullying in the oldest groups. Unexpectedly, high-status bullies seemed to
avoid continual bullying of the same victims, pointing to explanations of why their status can persist.

Introduction A key ingredient of such an understanding is the social standing of


the involved individuals. High-status students are more visible and
Bullying is commonly defined as repetitive and intentionally nega- more accepted in the peer group, and what these students do also gains
tive behavior against a victim who finds it difficult to defend him- or visibility and acceptance. This way, they are in a position to set beha-
herself (Olweus, 1993). It can be formalized as a directed, negative vioral norms in the school class (Dijkstra and Gest, 2015). Students with
social relation in which one social actor (the bully) behaves negatively high social status among their classmates were shown to be influential
against another (the victim). The effects of bullying are generally not in their peer group and often serve as role models (Dijkstra et al., 2013).
limited to the bully-victim dyad. When bullying takes place in a social If they act as bullies, there is the danger that bullying becomes socially
group and under conditions of high visibility (e.g., among students in a accepted. A high-status bully can potentially lead a whole school class
school class or among colleagues at a workplace), third actors become into bullying. For this reason, many anti-bullying interventions focus on
witnesses and can react to the bullying, either pro-socially (by de- diminishing the social status of bullying by targeting group norms
fending the victim, or expressing disapproval to the bully), or anti-so- (Swearer et al., 2010), by encouraging others to disapprove of bullying.
cially (by siding with the bully, or even joining in the bullying). Also a Unfortunately, these interventions do not always work (e.g., Gaffney
non-reaction in the face of bullying conveys the social cue of tacit ap- et al., 2018; Nickerson, 2017). Especially bullies with high status tend
proval. Bullying is a group process that affects all the group members to continue bullying and keep their high status among peers, thus de-
(Salmivalli et al., 1996; Salmivalli, 2010) and is characterized by feating the intervention’s purpose (Salmivalli, 2014). In contrast to
complex interdependencies (Huitsing et al., 2014; Fujimoto et al., medium- or low-status bullies, these high-status bullies somehow suc-
2017). From an organizational viewpoint, bullying is a challenge to the ceed in convincing a sufficiently large number of their peers to attribute
social order in a task-related group. If bullying plays too prominent a status to them despite their bullying. Given the sensitivity of group
role in daily life, students cannot concentrate on school tasks and em- behavior to what high-status students do, it is essential to examine the
ployees cannot work well. It is in the interest of management (in the interplay between bullying and status attribution more thoroughly.
school class: the teacher) to contain bullying, and if necessary intervene While in the past, bullying was often considered an impulsive, un-
to reduce it. For such interventions to be successful the processes by controlled outburst of aggression (Olweus, 1978), scientists and prac-
which bullying spreads need to be understood in the first place. titioners today tend to agree that it predominantly involves proactive,


Corresponding author at: University of Groningen, Grote Rozenstraat 38, 9712 TJ, Groningen, the Netherlands.
E-mail address: rozemarijn.van.der.ploeg@rug.nl (R. van der Ploeg).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2018.12.006

0378-8733/ © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Van der Ploeg, R., Social Networks, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2018.12.006
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

strategic, and goal-directed behavior (Reijntjes et al., 2013a,b; Volk Theory


et al., 2017). Bullies are thought to bully in order to achieve dominance
and high social status in the peer group (Salmivalli et al., 1996). Bullies Bullying can take different forms, including physical bullying (hit-
were shown to be more strongly motivated by considerations of peer ting, kicking), relational bullying (ignoring, gossiping), verbal bullying
status than non-bullies (Cillessen and Mayeux, 2004; Sijtsema et al., (calling names, insulting), material bullying (stealing or damaging
2009). To achieve this high status, they strategically pick on easy vic- things), or cyber bullying (via social networking sites). All these are
tims, e.g., the physically weak or those who are rejected by other negative interactions and, if engaged in voluntarily, should result in
classmates (Sijtsema et al., 2009; Veenstra et al., 2007). This strategy dislike and mutual avoidance (cf. Homans, 1950, chapter 5). Compared
seems effective for them: Bullying was repeatedly found to be positively to positive networks, negative networks tend to generally have lower
associated with social status among peers in cross-sectional studies density and lower stability over time (for our own data, we can see this
(Caravita et al., 2009; De Bruyn et al., 2010). Longitudinal studies re- in Table 1). However, bullying deviates from Homans’ scenario of a
veal that the two concepts are entrained, referring to that they remain voluntary interaction in two important ways. First, there is a power
associated over time. High-status bullies tend to keep their high status, asymmetry between the bully and the victim. Bullying is an act of
and continue bullying (Cillessen and Borch, 2006; Reijntjes et al., dominance, voluntary only from the bully’s point of view, not the vic-
2013a,b; Sentse et al., 2015; see also for aggression instead of bullying: tim’s (Salmivalli et al., 1996). With education being compulsory in al-
Juvonen et al., 2013). On the conceptual level, it remains a puzzle how most all countries, opting out is not a realistic option for the victim. In
bullying and high status can reliably co-occur, and how some bullies short, being victimized is not a choice, but bullying is. Second, the bully
can successfully derive high status over longer time periods from the and the victim may evaluate the interaction differently. Whereas for the
very group they victimize. Although the existence of such a “con- victim, it is almost certainly experienced as negative, the case may lie
troversial” group of children has been documented at least since the different for the bully. Depending on whether this evaluation is positive
early 1980s (e.g., Coie et al., 1982), the literature remains inconclusive or negative, the bully may decide to repeat or discontinue the bullying.
about the underlying mechanisms. We intend to examine how status From these considerations, we conclude that to understand bullying, we
and bullying co-determine each other, this way contributing to a so- need to identify the conditions under which the bully decides to bully.
lution of the puzzle and facilitating the development of more effective Probably the most important aspect of this decision is social
anti-bullying interventions. standing in the peer group. As an act of dominance, the main effect of
We think that the lack of solid insights is rooted in inappropriate bullying is a reduction of the rank of the victim and an increase of the
methodology. Despite both concepts being defined on the dyadic level, rank of the bully in a group-internal pecking order or dominance
the literature on adolescent development has traditionally taken an hierarchy (Peets and Hodges, 2014). Additionally, there may be effects
actor level approach in studies of bullying (e.g., Olweus, 1993) as well relative to third parties. Because the bullying act attracts peers’ atten-
as peer status (e.g.,Cillessen et al., 2011). This choice of the level of tion, which is a limited resource, the bully as well as the victim gain
analysis makes it impossible to look at the finer-grained dyadic and visibility at the expense of third parties. Fig. 1 illustrates the combined
triadic patterns required for understanding the interplay between bul- effects. If we assume a triad of actors of equal social standing (the black
lying and peer status. Whereas such studies on social relationships and dot in the middle of the left panel), then the bullying act on the one
status have recently started emerging (Betancourt et al., 2018, for hand increases visibility of bully B and victim V while reducing it for
bullying and related behaviors see Appendix B), the specific puzzle of third parties T. On the other hand, in a dominance hierarchy, it in-
how bullying and status attribution affect one another has so far re- creases the rank of B and reduces the rank of V, while leaving the rank
mained unaddressed. To address this puzzle, we put forward what of T unaffected. If we operationalize social standing as a linear com-
might be termed a network understanding of the mechanisms that allow bination of visibility and rank in the dominance hierarchy, the resulting
high-status bullies to keep their high status while staying bullies, even preference orders depend on the direction of the preference gradient
when bullying is disapproved of. For this aim, we investigate the de- (see right panel of the figure). If actors strive for dominance (up) as well
terminants of status attribution over time, paying special attention to as visibility (right), they experience an incentive to bully.
the bullying of status recipients. And we examine the determinants of In Fig. 1, social standing is by way of simplification assumed to be a
bullying over time, paying special attention to the peer status of the direct result of bullying. It is true that the bullying act is a display of
bully. Because both processes depend on one another, we examine them dominance, and emphasizes a status difference. However, status among
as an interdependent system. peers is a network-mediated consequence of bullying and endogenous

Fig. 1. Dominance and visibility in a bully-victim-ter-


tius triad.
Note. Left panel: Differentiation in visibility and dom-
inance after bully B bullies victim V (the three actors
had initially identical social standing).
Right panel: Resulting rankings of the three actors
depending on the preference gradient after linear
combination of preferences for visibility and dom-
inance.

2
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

to the peer system. Peers decide to attribute or withhold status. Their understood, formalized, and ultimately analyzed from the involved
dyadic status attributions collectively determine whether the bullying actors’ perspective.
act is socially accepted and transformed into social standing among
peers (henceforth peer status). We conclude that if we are to understand The student’s decision to bully
the peer status of bullies, we need to identify the rules according to
which third parties decide to attribute status to their peers. By collec- Past research has shown that students with high status tend to be
tively rewarding or punishing bullying, peers set an injunctive group status-sensitive. They feel competition to maintain their high status and
norm, which in the past has been the receptacle for anti-bullying in- can resort to bullying to achieve their goal (Fujimoto et al., 2017;
terventions (Salmivalli, 2014; Wölfer and Scheithauer, 2014). However, Garandeau, Lee, et al., 2014; Pattiselanno et al., 2015; Volk et al.,
once a student has acquired a high status among peers, the very nature 2015). For instance, high-status students were shown to use verbal or
of status systems facilitates perpetuation of this high status. We know physical bullying to intimidate ‘fellow competitors’ who threaten their
since the seminal works by De Solla Price (1965) and Merton (1968) social standing (Cillessen and Rose, 2005), and to overtly demonstrate
that status and reputation systems have inherent dynamic tendencies to their superiority over competitors by exercising control over others
perpetuate and aggravate status differences. This implies that high- (Kolbert and Crothers, 2003). High-status students who challenge their
status individuals are much less vulnerable to incidental peer status high status peers are likely to increase their influence and power over
withdrawal, whatever it is they are doing. If they are able to keep their others (Peets and Hodges, 2014; Volk et al., 2015). This can foster re-
high status simply because they had high status in the past, other de- lational bullying as high-status students are in an ideal position to ex-
terminants of status become disposable. Whereas building up peer status clude peers and spread gossip (Faris, 2012; Faris and Felmlee, 2011;
may require specific behavior (e.g., acting pro-socially), from a certain Garandeau, Lee, et al., 2014; Reijntjes et al., 2013a,b). In addition,
status level on, this behavior is not required any more for maintaining high-status students may be less likely to abide by peer norms of pro-
high status. On the contrary, high-status individuals might even change sociality if they already entered a cycle of self-reinforcing status per-
the criteria of what it means to have high status (Dijkstra and Gest, petuation.
2015). In sum, we hypothesize that having higher peer status will facilitate
Thus, the center of our investigation needs to be the dynamic in- bullying (Hypothesis 1). More precisely, with regard to Fig. 2, we expect
terplay of the bully’s decision to bully a victim, and a third party’s that high status will facilitate the bullying of new victims over time
decision to attribute status to the bully. This is schematically depicted (creation bullying tie, Hypothesis 1a), and that existing bullying is
in Fig. 2. The central panel illustrates the problematic situation in continued (maintenance bullying tie, Hypothesis 1b).
which bullying is being rewarded by status attribution.
If we are to explain why this dysfunctional configuration persists The nature of peer status
over time, we need to assess how incoming status attributions inform
the bully’s decision to continue vs. stop bullying a given victim (com- Whether in a school class or in a workplace context, it is useful to
parison between middle and left panel of Fig. 2), and we need to assess carefully differentiate between social acceptance or being liked by one’s
how the bullying of a given victim informs a third party’s decision peers on the one hand, and the peers’ perceptions of how popular one is
whether or not to attribute status to the bully (comparison between in the whole group on the other hand (e.g., Dijkstra et al., 2015; Wolters
middle and right panel of Fig. 2). The arrows in the figure indicate what et al., 2014. The latter is revealed in the peers’ dyadic status attribu-
we might call “micro action”: one actor – the bully or the third party – tions as studied in this paper, whereas the former is revealed in the
deciding for or against a specific change. It is these micro actions that peers’ dyadic friendship nominations, which we do not examine here.
we need to understand and assess evidence for. Whereas the two Acknowledging that both concepts are treated under the same label
“loops” starting and ending in the dysfunctional triad help stabilize the “popularity” in different branches of the literature, we have added a
association between peer status and bullying, the two incoming arrows clarifying note on terminology in Appendix A. In particular, and in
build up this association, whereas the two outgoing arrows reduce it. As contrast to friendship, it is possible that an individual attributes status
indicated in the textual labels of these arrows, these tendencies can be to someone whose behavior he or she personally disapproves of, simply

Fig. 2. The dysfunctional bully-victim-tertius triad (middle) and the surrounding network mechanisms.

3
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

because the individual thinks there are sufficiently many others who do the same (Matthew effect in status attribution), (c) withdrawing status
appreciate the behavior in question. Status attributions reflect dom- from students who get bullied, and (d) bullying those who are low in
inance, prestige, and visibility in the peer group as a collective entity status.
(Bellmore and Cillessen, 2006). Conceptually, a status attribution does
not require any personal relationship to the nominated person. As such, Method
peer status is not bound by the same resource constraints as personal
relationships are. Whereas there is a natural limit in the amount of time The association between bullying and peer status is the result of a
and attention devoted to maintaining a personal relationship, the same dynamic interplay of the actions of bullies on the one hand and an
is not true for mere acknowledgements such as status attribution. Be- audience of status attributors on the other hand. Any empirical in-
cause of this, and again in contrast to friendship, status attributions can vestigation into these relational processes therefore requires a long-
show strong evidence for cumulative advantage processes (De Solla itudinal approach. We will apply stochastic actor-based modelling
Price, 1965; Merton, 1968). (Snijders et al., 2010) to a unique data set on bullying in elementary
schools. By linking antecedents to consequences in a longitudinal study
The student’s decision to attribute status and by incorporating feedback between the two variables of interest on
a fine-grained, dyadic level, we are able to differentially assess evidence
During late elementary and middle school years, discussions about for all network mechanisms.
who is popular or ‘cool’ are widespread (Lease et al., 2002; Shoulberg
et al., 2011), indicating that in this age range, status considerations play Data and sample
an important role in the social life of students. In particular in early
adolescence, antisocial and ‘tough’ behavior, such as physical and Data stem from the evaluation of the Dutch implementation of the
verbal bullying, tends to be perceived as ‘cool’ (Reijntjes et al., 2013a,b; KiVa anti-bullying program (Kaufman et al., 2018). An overview of
Rodkin et al., 2006; Salmivalli and Peets, 2009). Students sharing these published studies using these network data is given in Appendix B. Data
values are also inclined to socially reward fellow students for displaying collection covered 99 Dutch primary schools and took place in three
corresponding behaviors by attributing status to them. This way, they waves: May 2012 (pre-intervention), October 2012, and May 2013.
are setting a social incentive to bully (see Fig. 1). That in almost all Prior to the pre-assessment in grades 2–5 in May 2012 – and for new
bullying situations witnesses are present (e.g., Salmivalli, 2010) em- students prior to the other assessments – schools sent information on
phasizes that bullies depend on their classmates to gain peer status. the study and permission forms to parents. Passive rather than active
Why do they collectively set this incentive? As argued by Faris (2012), parental consent was sought in order to obtain the high response rates
peer status in any group is relative, and as such any loss of status for that are necessary for meaningful social network analysis (note that
victims is balanced by small status gains for everyone else. This is most observational research does not fall within the ambit of the Dutch Act
clear when considering status rankings. If a bullying act occurs in a on research on human subjects). Parents who did not want their child to
bully-victim-tertius triad among status-equals (Fig. 1), and if the small participate in the assessment were asked to return a form. Students
visibility gains for the victim are outweighed by the losses in dom- were informed at school about the research and gave oral consent. Both
inance, then tertius ends up on a status rank higher than the victim parents and students could withdraw from participation at any time.
without having done anything. Tertius therefore is doubly motivated to Participation rates were high: at most two students per classroom did
reward the bully. First as a personal gratification because the bullying not have consent.
act allowed tertius to increase in status (essentially, for picking some- When the pre-assessment was finished, schools were randomly as-
body else as victim), and second as a reification of the ranking itself, signed by the Netherlands Bureau for Economic Policy Analysis (CPB)
which one feels more comfortable with, the higher one’s own rank is. to the control condition (33 schools) or the intervention condition (66
In sum, we hypothesize that engagement in bullying behavior will schools). Control schools were asked to continue their “care as usual”
lead to higher peer status in the classroom (Hypothesis 2). More pre- anti-bullying approach until their participation in the KiVa program
cisely referring to the transitions depicted in Fig. 2, we expect that began in June 2014.
students who bully will attract more new peer status nominations than In the Netherlands, especially in large schools, it is not uncommon
students who do not bully (creation status attribution tie, Hypothesis2a), to change the classroom composition each year. However, our aim was
and they will also be able to maintain more of their existing incoming to longitudinally investigate developments in relatively stable peer
status attributions than students who do not bully (maintenance status groups. We therefore used data only from single-grade classrooms (N =
attribution tie, Hypothesis 2b). 83). Moreover, we needed classrooms networks present at all three
waves with less than 20% missing cases to perform social network
Interfering network mechanisms analyses (Ripley et al., 2018). As in one classroom 92% of the students
did not participate in wave 3, 82 classrooms from 15 schools (11 KiVa
Our hypotheses are roughly in line with theories that link the schools and 4 control schools) were suitable for the analyses. The total
sending or receiving of positive or negative ties to latent individual traits number of students was 2,055 (Mage = 9.71, range 6.76–13.06 in wave
and/or postulate a self-reinforcing, Matthew-effect type of mechanisms. 1; 50% boys). Missing data were handled the default way (Rigby et al.,
Addressing the former case of exogenous explanatory variables, in par- 2018, see section 4.3.2). All students were included, despite the pos-
ticular the motivational traits to strive for high social status in the sibility of having missing values for the variables at one of the waves,
classroom or to avoid losing status have been argued to constitute a la- for instance caused by absence during the assessment or non-consent
tent dimension explaining adolescents’ differences in terms of bullying (wave 1: 1%, wave 2: 1%, wave 3: 3%). These absent students could
and peer status (e.g., Gangel et al., 2017; Puckett et al., 2008; Cillessen nevertheless be nominated by others and were thus included in the
and Mayeux, 2004; Sijtsema et al., 2009). Unfortunately, our data set networks.
does not allow us to test this underlying explanation. Concerning the
latter case of endogenous explanatory variables, we will make sure that Procedure
self-reinforcing mechanisms are assessed and controlled for when
testing our hypotheses. Specifically, we will test to what degree there is Students completed online questionnaires on the schools’ computers
evidence for students (a) bullying a victim because other students al- during regular school hours, under supervision of their classroom tea-
ready are bullying the same victim (Matthew effect in bullying), (b) chers who were supplied with detailed instructions before the data
attributing status to a fellow student because other students already do collection started. Teachers were present to answer questions and, if

4
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

needed, help students in such a way that it would not affect their an- give a detailed description of the other effects used for modeling the
swers (e.g., by asking them questions such as “Which words are unclear dynamics of status attribution, and finish with a sketch of the corre-
to you?”). The order of questions, items, and scales used in this study sponding model for bullying dynamics.
were randomized to prevent any systematic order effects. Stochastic actor-based models of a single network distinguish be-
Difficult topics were explained in several instructional videos. In tween effects modeling the speed of the change process (rate effects) and
one video at the start of the questionnaire, students were told that their effects modeling the nature of the network changes (jointly con-
answers would remain confidential, but that their teacher might be tributing to the objective function). In our case of two co-evolving net-
given general feedback to improve the classroom climate. Before an- works, there are rate and objective function effects for the status at-
swering the questions about bullying and victimization, the term bul- tribution network on the one hand, and for the bullying network on the
lying was defined as formulated in Olweus’ Bully/Victim questionnaire other hand. We used an intercept model for the rate functions, referring
in another video (Olweus, 1996). Several examples covering different to that the rate function was not allowed to differ according to actor
forms of bullying were given, followed by an explanation emphasizing attributes or network position. To test our hypotheses about peer status
the intentional and repetitive nature of bullying and the power im- (referring to status attribution indegree) going together with bullying,
balance between bullies and victims. in Model 1 we estimated two effects. The status to bullying effect in-
dicates whether high indegree in status attribution (referring to being
Measures considered high in status by more others) implied high outdegree in the
bullying network (referring to bullying more others, Hypothesis 1).
Bullying Conversely, the bullying to status effect indicates whether a higher
At each time point, students were first asked to indicate whether outdegree in the bullying network implied a higher indegree in the
they were being victimized, using 11 bully/victims items covering the status attribution network (Hypothesis 2). In Model 2, these two effects
various types of bullying (Bully/Victims Questionnaire, Olweus, 1996). are further nuanced according to whether they explain the creation of
Those who indicated that they were victimized at least once on any of new ties (Hypotheses 1a and 2a) or the maintenance of existing ties
the items were asked to nominate the classmates who were victimizing (Hypotheses 1b and 2b). We tested, as a second type of network-crossing
them ("Who in your class always starts bullying you?"). A roster with effects, whether a high indegree in bullying (referring to being a victim)
the names of all the children in class was presented on the computer implied a lower indegree in status attribution (victimization to status
screen. Bully nominations were coded 1 and non-nominations 0. As our effect), or whether the converse was the case (status to victimization
study aimed to investigate active bullying behavior and not being no- effect).
minated as a bully, the resulting network matrix was transposed so that In addition to these cross-network effects, we included univariate,
the presence of a relation indicates a bully-victim relation instead of a structural effects of network change for both networks, which capture
victim-bully relation. It is likely that a different subgroup of bullies the tendencies of individuals to form and maintain relationships under
would have been identified if we had asked students to indicate whom specific network-structural conditions. These effects also serve to opti-
they bully. Dutch students seem more inclined to admit that they are a mize the goodness of fit of the model (Huitsing et al., 2014; Rambaran
victim of bullying than to confess that they bully as the prevalence of et al., 2015; Snijders et al., 2010).
self-proclaimed bullies was found to be lower compared to the pre- The following univariate, structural effects were added to explain
valence of self-proclaimed victims (Veenstra et al., 2007). the dynamics of status attribution. The outdegree effect expresses the
overall tendency of individuals i to attribute status to other individuals j
Peer status attribution in the network (notation: i→j). The reciprocity effect models the ten-
Students could nominate an unlimited number of classmates they dency to reciprocate a status nomination (i→j facilitates j→i). Two ef-
perceived as popular (“Who is popular in your class?”) at each time fects of triangular closure (referring to group formation) were included.
point. Similar to the bullying network, peer status nominations were The first is the transitive triplets effect, which reflects the tendency of
coded 1 and non-nominations 0, resulting in status attribution network individual i to attribute status to peers k who received status nomina-
matrices consisting of directed peer status nominations for each class- tions from peers j that i also attributes status to (transitive closure: i→j
room. Please consult Appendix A for understanding the use of the term and j→k together facilitate i→k). This group formation effect is in line
popularity in this context. with the assumption of a status hierarchy inside the group, which can
be seen from a simple tie count: k receives two ties, but sends none
Analytical strategy (high status), i sends two ties, but receives none (low status), and j sends
and receives one tie (middle rank; Snijders and Steglich, 2015). The
Our hypotheses were tested using longitudinal social network ana- second group formation effect is the three cycles effect, which in-
lysis with the RSiena software (Ripley et al., 2018). This software es- vestigates the tendency of individuals i, j, and k to form a non-hier-
timates the parameters of stochastic actor-based models of the (co-) archical group (cyclical closure: i→j and j→k together facilitate k→i).
evolution of (multiple) social networks over time (Snijders et al., 2010, In order to differentiate between individuals who received or sent
2013). Under this approach, the first observation of a networked social many ties, three degree-related effects were included. The degree-re-
system is conditioned upon, and subsequent observations are inter- lated effects were all measured with the square roots of the degrees
preted as the cumulative outcome of an unobserved series of small instead of the raw degrees (Huitsing et al., 2014; Snijders et al., 2010).
changes applied to the preceding observation. These unobserved small Indegree attractiveness reflects the tendency for those who receive many
changes are modelled as decisions made by individuals in the network status attributions to receive even more over time – known as the
about maintaining, dissolving, or creating the ties they have to others. Matthew effect on status reputation (Merton, 1968). This effect expresses
Parameter estimates are obtained through simulation-based inference, status differences that are (exclusively) captured in standardized peer
which is common for fitting models with intractable likelihood func- status measures (Cillessen and Rose, 2005), and accordingly we expect
tions to complex data sets (e.g., Gourieroux and Monfort, 1996). it to be strong in the data. Outdegree activity is about the tendency for
those who attribute status to many others to send even more attribu-
Model specification tions over time. Finally, indegree activity models the tendency to attri-
There are two main model parts, one for each dependent network bute status to others when being attributed status often oneself. One
variable (referring to bullying and status attribution). Because our hy- more effect we included was sex similarity, accounting for whether in-
potheses are about the effects of the two networks on each other, we dividuals were more likely to attribute status to others of the same sex
will first describe how these hypotheses are operationalized. We then than to the opposite sex.

5
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

The effects used to explain bullying dynamics are generally the


same as for status attribution dynamics. However, instead of the in-
degree activity effect, we included the outdegree attractiveness effect
which reflects the tendency to being victimized for those who bully
others (we expect a negative effect). Moreover, because of the low
density of the bullying network, the effects of reciprocity, transitive tri-
plets, and three cycles could not be identified in most classrooms. The
group formation effects therefore were dropped from the model speci-
fication, whereas the reciprocity effect was not estimated, but score
tested (we tested whether the model lacked fit, compared to an en-
riched model including the effect). Also the direct tie-level effects that
examine the main effects of dyad-level status attribution on bullying,
and vice versa, were included in the score tests.

Model building Fig. 3. Changes in bullying (referring to outdegree in the bullying network)
The co-evolution of the bullying and status attribution networks was over time by initial bullying and dyadic status attribution pattern over time.
analyzed in two steps. The first model included the main effects to test Note. Unit of analysis is the tertius-bully (T-B) dyad; each actor contributes to
hypotheses 1 and 2. We added endowment and creation parameters in multiple dyads.
Stable: T attributes status to B at two subsequent observations.
the second model so that the effects for the maintenance and formation
Lost: T attributes status to B at the first, but not at the second observation.
of ties could be distinguished (hypotheses 1a,b and 2a,b). The two
New: T attributes status to B at the second, but not at the first observation.
models were estimated separately for each classroom, using all three None: T does not attribute status to B neither at the first nor at the second
time points, and then combined in a meta-analysis (Snijders and observation.
Baerveldt, 2003).

Results reliable estimates of endogenous network effects (social ties explaining


other social ties). For the status attribution networks the Jaccard in-
Descriptive results dices were sufficiently high for RSiena analysis to proceed without
problems (referring to larger than .30; Ripley et al., 2018). For bullying,
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics of the bullying and status at- the indices were low (.18 and .19), but this is common across negative
tribution networks. The average degree shows that students bullied on tie studies and the indices were not critically low (Ripley et al., 2018).
average one to two classmates, and they on average attributed status to We therefore estimated model specifications with reduced complexity,
around three classmates. Status attributions tend to rise somewhat over for which the analyses gave no numerical problems .
time, whereas bullying nominations decrease. For both bullying and We investigated how changes in bullying activity from one ob-
status attribution, reciprocation tends to increase in the year under servation moment to the next were associated with changes in dyadic
study, and there was evidence for transitive closure. In both networks, status attributions while controlling for initial level of bullying. The
the majority of nominations occurred between students of the same sex results are depicted in Fig. 3. We see an overall pattern of regression to
(57–69%), but this level is far below the age-specific, almost complete the mean: below-average bullies (white bars) tend to increase their
sex segregation in friendship networks. bullying whereas above-average bullies (grey bars) tend to strongly
The Jaccard index indicates the proportion of stable relations decrease it. The differences between the four dyadic status attribution
among the total number of new, lost, and stable ties between observed change patterns are completely analogous for the two bully groups.
time-points. It needs to be sufficiently high if one wants to obtain Stable status attribution goes together with more bullying than lost

Table 1
Class-level descriptive statistics for bullying and status attribution networks per wave.
Bullying network Status attribution network

Wave 1 Wave 2 M (SD) Wave 3 Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 M (SD)


M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Average degree 1.94 (1.06) 1.38 1.17 (0.80) 3.11 (1.25) 3.16 (1.39) 3.37 (1.69)
(0.79)
SD indegree 3.06 (1.24) 2.48 (1.11) 2.23 (1.22) 3.51 (1.64) 3.73 (1.67) 4.00 (1.76)
SD outdegree 1.96 (0.78) 1.58 (0.71) 1.41 (0.75) 3.16 (1.23) 3.18 (1.19) 3.27 (1.51)
Reciprocity .16 (.10) .17 (.10) .19 (.17) .19 (.08) .19 (.09) .21 (.15)
Same sex nominations .57 (.14) .62 .68 (.19) .69 (.12) .68 (.10) .66 (.11)
(.17)
Transitivity .52 (.18) .47 .58 (.23) .53 (.14) .55 (.14) .57 (.14)
(.22)
Average class size 25.2 25.1 (4.4) 25.0 25.2 25.1 (4.4) 25.0
(4.2) (4.4) (4.2) (4.4)
Non-respondents 1% 1% 3% 1% 1% 3%
Wave 1→ 2 Wave 2→ 3 Wave 1→ 2 Wave 2→ 3
Hamming distancea 53.6 43.4 77.3 77.6
Jaccard indexb .19 (.10) .18 (.12) .32 (.12) .34 (.12)

Note.
a
Hamming distance is the number of tie changes.
b
Jaccard index is the fraction of stable ties among the new, lost, or stable ties N = 82 classrooms in 15 schools.

6
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Table 2
Meta-analyses of multiplex network analysis: bullying and status attribution.
Model 1 Model 2

Est. SE Est. SE

Bullying network
Structural network effects
Outdegree (density) −6.02 0.19***
maintenance −4.12 0.31***
creation −7.50 0.29***
Indegree attractiveness 1.27 0.05*** 1.19 0.04***
Outdegree attractiveness 0.16 0.14 −0.11 0.09
Outdegree activity 0.69 0.06*** 0.57 0.05***
Individual effects
Sex similarity 0.39 0.09*** 0.35 0.06***
Between networks effects
Fig. 4. Changes in peer status (referring to indegree in the status attribution
Status → victimization −0.09 0.06 −0.10 0.07
network) over time, by initial status and dyadic bullying pattern over time.
Status → bullying 0.24 0.04***
Note. Unit of analysis is the bully-victim (B–V) dyad; each actor contributes to maintenance bullying −0.41 0.15*
multiple dyads. creation bullying 0.82 0.19***
Stable: B bullies V at two subsequent observations.
Status attribution network
Lost: B bullies V at the first, but not at the second observation.
Structural network effects
New: B bullies V at the second, but not at the first observation. Outdegree (density) −5.34 0.16***
None: B does not bully V neither at the first nor at the second observation. maintenance −2.89 0.18***
creation −7.25 0.15***
Reciprocity 0.24 0.05*** 0.25 0.04***
status attribution. For the above-average bullies, this means they reduce Transitive triplets 0.08 0.02*** 0.07 0.01***
their bullying less under stable status attribution than they do under Three cycles −0.10 0.03*** −0.06 0.01***
lost status attribution. For the below-average bullies, it means that they Indegree attractiveness 0.90 0.03*** 0.88 0.02***
increase their bullying more under stable status attribution than they do Indegree activity 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.03
Outdegree activity 0.57 0.03*** 0.57 0.03***
under lost status attribution. When comparing dyads without an initial
Individual effects
status attribution in terms of whether there still is none at the next time Sex similarity 0.61 0.04*** 0.60 0.03***
point (pattern “none”) or whether there is one (pattern “new”), we see Between networks effects
the same differences: new status attribution goes together with more Victimization → status −0.02 0.02 −0.03 0.02
bullying than continued absence of status attribution. Taken together, Bullying → status 0.09 0.03**
maintenance status 0.13 0.07
these descriptive results are consistent with our hypotheses about creation status 0.25 0.07***
higher peer status being associated with more bullying (Hypotheses 1).
We also examined how changes in bullies’ peer status from one Note: Rate of change effects were omitted from the table. All effects, except for
observation moment to the next were associated with changes in their status attribution reciprocity, show significant variation over classrooms.
dyadic bullying behavior, while controlling for the initial level of peer N = 74 classrooms.
status (Fig. 4). We again see the overall pattern of regression to the * p < .05.
mean: students of below-average status (white bars) tend to increase ** p < .01.
*** p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
their peer status strongly, whereas students of above-average status
(grey bars) tend not to. There are some interesting modulations on
these general change patterns. Let us first compare initial bully-victim
dyads in terms of whether the bullying continues at the next time point students attributed status to those who provided status to them too).
(pattern “stable”) or stops (pattern “lost”). For the low-status bullies, Additionally, when students attributed status to one of their classmates
there are only negligible differences, but for the high-status bullies, the and this classmate attributed status to a third classmate, students were
difference is remarkable: they gain, on average, peer status under inclined to also attribute status to this third classmate over time
continued bullying of the same victims, whereas they lose peer status (transitive triplets, OR= 1.08, p < .001). The three cycles effect was ne-
when bullying is stopped. Second, we compare initial non-bullying gative (OR = 0.90, p < .001), which indicates that status attributions
dyads in terms of whether non-bullying continues (pattern “none”) or led to local hierarchies.
bullying is observed at the next time point (pattern “new”). For the low- The positive indegree attractiveness effects for both bullying (OR =
status as well as the high-status non-bullies, there is a clear status gain 3.56, p < .001) and status attribution (OR = 2.46, p < .001) showed
advantage in favor of starting a new bully-victim relation. Taken to- that often-bullied students attracted more being bullied nominations
gether, these descriptive results are in line with Hypotheses 2 about over time (Matthew effect in victimization), and that high-status students
engagement in bullying being associated with increased peer status. attracted more peer status nominations over time (Matthew effect in
status attribution). Moreover, students who bullied many others or who
Structural network effects attributed status to many others, tended to increase this tendency fur-
ther (outdegree activity, OR = 1.99 for bullying; OR = 1.77 for status
Table 2 presents mean estimates and their standard errors obtained attribution, p < .001).
from meta-analyses of RSiena-results for all school classes. To facilitate Lastly, the positive sex similarity effects in both networks indicated
interpretation of the results, we calculated the exponential function of that bullying relationships and status nominations were more likely to
the estimates (odds ratios, presented in text). Model 1 shows that stu- occur between students of the same sex (OR = 1.48 for bullying; OR =
dents tended to be selective in nominating classmates as a bully (out- 1.84 for status attribution, p < .001).
degree, OR = 0.002, p < .001) and in attributing status to classmates
(outdegree, OR = 0.005, p < .001). The positive reciprocity parameter The interplay between bullying and peer status
in the status attribution network (OR = 1.27, p < .001) indicated that
status nominations were likely to be reciprocated (referring to that The “between-network” effects in Model 1 revealed that a high

7
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

indegree in status attribution increased the likelihood of a high out- (Cillessen and Borch, 2006; Cillessen and Mayeux, 2004; Reijntjes et al.,
degree in bullying over time (status to bullying, OR = 1.27, p < .001). 2013a,b; Sentse et al., 2015), we found that bullying generally increases
This is in line with our hypothesis that having higher peer status is peer status among classmates. We also found that high status in-
associated with engagement in bullying over time (Hypothesis 1). dividuals tend to bully, but here the results are more nuanced than what
Moreover, it was shown that a high outdegree in bullying is associated actor-level analyses can reveal. When differentiating between the
with receiving more status attributions over time (bullying to status, OR creation and maintenance of specific ties in our two networks, we could
= 1.09, p = .004). In other words: being a bully makes you high in demonstrate that unlike low-status bullies, high-status bullies tended to
status. This outcome is consistent with our hypothesis that bullying was discontinue bullying the same victims, but replaced those with new
a way to gain peer status (Hypothesis 2). victims. Furthermore, when looking into the age-dependency of these
Model 2 unraveled the interplay between bullying and peer status effects, we could show that bullying is rewarded by peer status attri-
by distinguishing effects for the dissolution, maintenance, and forma- butions only for the higher grades of elementary school (grades 4 and
tion of ties. The outcomes concerning the development of bullying 5). For the youngest age group (grade 2) classmates even sanctioned
demonstrated that students with high status discontinue bullying their bullying by a withdrawal of status attributions. Taken together, our
former victims (maintenance bullying, OR = 0.66, p = .006) and start findings reiterate that bullying is a complex group phenomenon in
bullying classmates whom they did not bully before (creation bullying, which obtaining and maintaining high social standing plays a decisive
OR = 2.27, p < .001). Whereas the latter result is in line with our ex- role (e.g., Salmivalli, 2010; Volk et al., 2014), and that a network
pectation that high status is associated with the formation of new bully perspective deepens our understanding of the interplay between bul-
relations over time (Hypothesis 1a), the former, negative result is in lying and peer status.
contradiction to our expectation that high-status bullies would continue We think that our approach to analyze peer status not as actor-level
bullying their former victims (Hypothesis 1b); in fact they discontinued construct, but on the dyad level as a network of status attributions has
bullying them. broader potential in adolescence research. This is illustrated by our
For the status attribution network it turned out students who bullied unexpected finding of high-status bullies replacing their victims over
gained new status attributions over time (creation status, OR = 1.28, time, referring to discontinue existing bullying ties, but establish new
p < .001). Put differently, students who bullied received status from ones. Already previous social network analyses on bullying (Huitsing
classmates who did not provide them status before. This finding is et al., 2014) had indicated that, on the relational level, bullying is not
consistent with what we expected (Hypothesis 2a). The maintenance stable over time, whereas aggregated on the actor level (referring to the
status effect was positive, but not statistically significant (OR = 1.14, number of victims of a bully) it is. By design, an actor-level analysis
p = 0.065). Whereas the sign of the coefficient is in line with our ex- overlooks these dyad-level changes in bullying pattern. Thus, such
pectation (Hypothesis 2b), there was no significant evidence for existing dynamic patterns become only visible to the researcher by adoption of a
status attributions to be affected by bullying. Among classmates who longitudinal network perspective.
already considered you high in status, your bullying seems not to make This study raises new theoretical questions into how bullying and
a decisive difference in their decision whether or not to continue pro- peer status are intertwined. If high-status students replace their victims
viding you status. on a regular basis, it would be interesting to investigate whether they
The mean results of the score tests for the two omitted model start bullying particular fellow students who threaten their social
parameters in the bullying network were significant for bullying re- standing in the classroom (referring to other high-status classmates, see
ciprocity (M = 0.61, p < .001) and the direct tie-level effect of bullying also Peets and Hodges, 2014), or whether they seek new, low status
on peer status (M = -0.15, p = .013). Inclusion of these effects in the targets. In other words: is bullying behavior driven by the motivation to
model led to instability of the estimation algorithm, indicating that they keep high status, opportunity, or both? Empirically, this could be in-
refer to small fractions of the data in a few classrooms only. Because of vestigated by further describing the differences in the dynamics of
this, we consider it unlikely that their omission challenges our main bullying and status attribution. For instance, by distinguishing bullies
results. with reciprocated versus unreciprocated bullying relationships and test
how these networks evolve over time.
Supplementary analyses: grade differences Another important question is to what extent the classroom context
influences the interplay between bullying and peer status, as bullying
We additionally tested whether the results were homogeneous and peer status by definition both depend on the peer context
across grades (see Appendix C). For the higher grades patterns, results (Salmivalli, 2010; Salmivalli et al., 1996). Several anti-bullying inter-
were comparable to the findings in Table 2. Students who bullied tend ventions aim to change group norms such that bullies are less supported
to gain peer status among classmates and high-status bullies tend to by bystanders and that their antisocial behavior is less rewarded among
choose new victims. However, for students in the lower grades, bullying peers (Salmivalli, 2014; Wölfer and Scheithauer, 2014). Future research
led to less peer status (creation status, OR = 0.81, p = .001 in grade 2) should examine whether the interplay between bullying and peer status
or did not contribute to peer status (creation status, OR = 1.17, p = .079 is different in classrooms with strong anti-bullying norms and in-
in grade 3). vestigate among which students these differences occur.
Various ramifications of our analyses can be thought of. First, the
Discussion bullying and peer status networks in our study were examined in stable,
single-grade classrooms only. In many countries, classroom composi-
This study focused on bullying as strategic behavior linked to high tion is homogenous and remains the same during the students’ entire
social status in the classroom. The aim was to unravel the complex elementary school career. In the Netherlands, multi-grade elementary
interplay between bullying and one’s peer status on the dyadic level school classrooms are common and the composition of the classroom is
over time, which fills a serious gap in the existing literature on bullying. likely to change between school years (Veenman, 1995). Especially
We argued that bullying and peer status reinforce each other, and we status differences are very strong in these multi-grade classrooms. Now
used longitudinal multiplex network analysis to get more insight into that we have developed a framework to investigate the relational pro-
the relational patterns of bullying and peer status. Our study is the first cesses behind bullying and peer status, it would be interesting to ex-
that investigated how bullying ties affect the creation, dissolution, and amine whether these processes differ in age-heterogeneous, unstable
maintenance of status attribution ties, and vice versa, in a social net- classrooms. Second, seeing that a substantial share of the bullying and
work framework. victimization takes place outside the classroom (Huitsing et al., 2014;
In line with previous studies employing an actor-level framework Van der Ploeg et al., 2015), it might be fruitful to move beyond the own

8
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

classroom and analyze cohort-wide or even school-wide networks. (Slaughter et al., 2015). Next to interventions aimed at changing group
Third, the focus of our study was on bullying in general. Nevertheless, norms and enhancing defending behavior (Salmivalli, 2014), teaching
bullying behavior can occur in several forms (Salmivalli et al., 2011): bullies in higher grades prosocial ways to gain or maintain high status
physical, verbal, material, relational, and cyberbullying. Some types are therefore is probably essential to effectively intervene in school bul-
more visible or more rewarded than others, which makes it likely that lying (Ellis et al., 2015; Swearer et al., 2010).
the different types of bullying are also differently related to peer status. Our finding that bully-victim relations in the higher grades of ele-
mentary school seem to be unstable over time might be an important
Practical implications finding for the design of new anti-bullying interventions or the eva-
luation of existing ones. Indicated anti-bullying interventions that aim
Increasingly, researchers are collecting rich data on relations be- to solve existing bullying situations (Garandeau, Poskiparta et al., 2014;
tween children and adolescents through network questions (Veenstra Van der Ploeg et al., 2016) should acknowledge that bullies tend to
et al., 2013; even from 6-years on: Verlinden et al., 2014). Such net- switch victims and refine their strategies to effectively target the
work analyses provide insights in selection processes, referring to that complexity of bullying. Rather than solving specific bullying situations
bullies or victims flock together, and influence processes, referring to or helping particular victims, it is the norm that bullying is a way to
that friends’ bullying behavior or friends’ victimization is contagious achieve peer status that should be altered.
(Lodder et al., 2016; Sentse et al., 2013; Turanovic and Young, 2016).
Our results illustrate that peer status plays an essential role in in- Acknowledgements
volvement in bullying behavior. The use of longitudinal social network
data helps to better understand the relational patterns of bullying and The data used in this study are collected for the evaluation of the
peer status. The findings imply that bullying is an effective strategy to Dutch version of the KiVa anti-bullying program.We thank all the stu-
obtain and maintain high status in the classroom, particularly in higher dents, teachers, and school administrators who participated in the data
grades. However, our study also reveals that not all high-status students collection. This research was funded by the Dutch Ministry of Education
engage in bullying and that not all bullies gain peer status. This might (Onderwijs Bewijs nr: ODB10025) and NWO VICI453-14-016. The
reflect that other factors or behaviors explain status attributions. In funders had no role in the analyses, decision to publish, or preparation
previous studies, prosocial behavior has been linked to peer status of the manuscript.

Appendix A. A clarifying note on the notion of popularity

Because of diverging definitions in the two different disciplinary communities that constitute our intended readership, we decided to avoid using
the notion of popularity. These two disciplines are adolescent development (AD), and social network analysis (SNA). As one aim of our study is to provide
a bridge between these disciplines, a clarifying note is expedient. In SNA, the notion of popularity is essentially a synonym for indegree in any
directed network, referring to the number of ties received (Wasserman & Faust, 1994). It is commonly contrasted with expansiveness or activity,
defined as outdegree, referring to the number of ties sent – and it is in particular agnostic to the content of the network. You could, for example, be
SNA-popular in a bullying network, which simply means that you are bullied by many classmates.
In the sociological and psychological literature on AD, however, popularity is very narrowly defined by relationship content, namely as peer-
acknowledged social status (popularity among peers; Eder, 1985; Adler and Adler, 1998;Cillessen et al., 2011). It is typically assessed as indegree in a
binary network of peer nominations in response to the question, “Who is popular in your class?” and it is commonly contrasted with similar constructs
based on other relationship content, such as social acceptance or likeability, which are assessed as indegree in networks obtained with other name
generators (Whom do you hang out / play with?; Whom do you like?; Who is your friend?). The AD literature identified important differences
between these concepts (e.g., Parkhurst and Hopmeyer, 1998; LaFontana and Cillessen, 1999; De Bruyn et al., 2010; Dijkstra et al., 2010; Schwartz
et al., 2010), which is why researchers in this tradition strongly insist on not using the word popularity for the indegree in networks other than the
one assessing popularity in the narrow sense.
Whereas AD-popularity satisfies the formal definition of SNA-popularity, the opposite obviously is not the case. When you are bullied by many
classmates (referring to when you are popular as a victim in SNA terminology), it certainly does not mean you have high social status among them
(AD-popularity). However, if you are SNA-popular in a status attribution network, then the definition coincides with AD-popularity. As this little
excursion illustrates, using the word popularity is ambiguous and would obfuscate our message to the reader. Because of this, we chose to use the
words indegree (with an explicit reference to the network on which it is calculated) when we mean SNA-popularity, and (peer) status when we mean
AD-popularity. And we speak of status attribution when we refer to the directed network of AD-popularity nominations.

Appendix B. Studies on the same data as ours or taking a similar approach

So far, only a few studies investigated bullying and related behaviors, such as defending, as a directed dyadic relationship. One of the first studies
(Huitsing and Veenstra, 2012) used dyadic information on bullying and defending to obtain an impression of the social structure of a classroom. It
turned out that ingroup and outgroup effects were important in explaining the group process of bullying. In general, most bullies were boys, with
boys bullying both boys and girls (Huitsing et al., 2014; Huitsing and Veenstra, 2012). Defending occurred mostly among same-sex classmates
(Huitsing et al., 2014; Huitsing and Monks, 2018; Sainio et al., 2011).
From two studies focusing on defending behavior (Oldenburg et al., 2018; Sainio et al., 2011) we know that aspects of liking, peer status, and
friendship play a role. Victims liked their classmates who defended them and perceived them as popular (Sainio et al., 2011). In addition, victims
were likely to be defended by classmates who they perceive as friends or who perceive them as friends, whereas, it was unlikely that victims were
defended by classmates they disliked or by whom victims were disliked. Moreover, defending was more likely when the victim and (potential)
defender shared friends or disliked the same classmates (Oldenburg et al., 2018).
In a study on the coevolution of defending, bullying and victimization networks, it was found that over time victims with the same bullies
defended each other, defenders were at risk of becoming victimized by the bullies of the victims they stood up for, bullies with the same victims
defended each other, and defenders of bullies started to bully those victims too (Huitsing et al., 2014). These findings show that using a social
network approach can be fruitful for investigating complex relationships such as bullying and peer status.

9
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Appendix C. Meta-analyses of multiplex network analysis per grade: bullying and status attribution

Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade4 Grade 5

Est. SE Est. SE Est. SE Est. SE

Bullying network
Structural network effects
Outdegree (density)
maintenance −4.27 0.58*** −3.47 0.61*** −4.86 0.86*** −3.56 0.24***
creation −7.76 0.50*** −7.01 0.53*** −7.38 0.46*** −7.69 0.80***
Indegree attractiveness 1.19 0.12*** 0.98 0.04*** 1.19 0.07*** 1.38 0.07***
Outdegree attractiveness 0.08 0.15 −0.16 0.16 −0.11 0.18 −0.27 0.27
Outdegree activity 0.66 0.04*** 0.60 0.05*** 0.60 0.08*** 0.43 0.17**

Individual effects
Sex similarity 0.17 0.12 0.22 0.10* 0.50 0.11*** 0.57 0.15***

Between networks effects


Status → victimization −0.27 0.25 −0.24 0.14 0.04 0.07 0.01 0.10
Status → bullying
maintenance bullying −0.22 0.37 −0.13 0.22 −0.96 0.27*** −0.29 0.29
creation bullying 0.32 0.40 0.79 0.43 1.29 0.38** 0.88 0.24***
Status attribution network
Structural network effects
Outdegree (density)
maintenance −1.81 0.19*** −2.69 0.32*** −3.25 0.29*** −3.91 0.55***
creation −6.82 0.27*** −6.57 0.27*** −7.30 0.23*** −8.37 0.31***
Reciprocity 0.27 0.07*** 0.32 0.08*** 0.25 0.09*** 0.18 0.07*
Transitive triplets 0.11 0.02*** 0.06 0.03* 0.07 0.02*** 0.04 0.02*
Three cycles −0.11 0.05* −0.03 0.03 −0.07 0.03* −0.06 0.02*
Indegree attractiveness 0.78 0.05*** 0.83 0.04*** 0.90 0.04*** 0.99 0.05***
Indegree activity 0.05 0.09 −0.01 0.05 0.06 0.04 0.06 0.03*
Outdegree activity 0.44 0.03*** 0.54 0.04*** 0.62 0.05*** 0.70 0.08***

Individual effects
Sex similarity 0.60 0.07*** 0.58 0.07*** 0.63 0.07*** 0.59 0.05***

Between networks effects


Victimization → status −0.01 0.04 −0.01 0.03 −0.02 0.03 −0.07 0.05
Bullying → status
maintenance status 0.22 0.13 0.18 0.15 0.13 0.16 0.02 0.15
creation status −0.21 0.06** 0.16 0.09 0.37 0.10*** 0.53 0.15***

Note. Rate of change effects were omitted from the table.


Grade refers to wave 1, in waves 2 and 3 students were in grades 3–6.
All effects, except for status attribution reciprocity, show significant variation over classrooms.
*p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 (two-tailed tests).

References De Solla Price, D.J., 1965. Networks of scientific papers. Science 510–515.
Dijkstra, J.K., Gest, S.D., 2015. Peer norm salience for academic achievement, prosocial
behavior, and bullying: implications for adolescent school experiences. J. Early
Adler, P.A., Adler, P., 1998. Peer Power: Preadolescent Culture and Identity. Rutgers Adolesc. 35 (1), 79–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431614524303.
University Press, New Brunswick, NJ. Dijkstra, J.K., Cillessen, A.H.N., Lindenberg, S., Veenstra, R., 2010. Same-gender and
Bellmore, A.D., Cillessen, A.H.N., 2006. Reciprocal influences of victimization, perceived cross-gender likeability: associations with popularity and status enhancement: the
social preference, and self-concept in adolescence. Self Identity 5 (3), 209–229. TRAILS study. J. Early Adolesc. 30 (6), 773–802. https://doi.org/10.1177/
https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860600636647. 0272431609350926.
Betancourt, N., Kovács, B., Otner, S.M., 2018. The perception of status: how we infer the Dijkstra, J.K., Cillessen, A.H.N., Borch, C., 2013. Popularity and adolescent friendship
status of others from their social relationships. Netw. Sci. 6 (3), 319–347. https://doi. networks: selection and influence dynamics. Dev. Psychol. 49 (7), 1242–1252.
org/10.1017/nws.2018.13. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030098.
Caravita, S.C.S., Di Blasio, P., Salmivalli, C., 2009. Unique and interactive effects of Dijkstra, J.K., Kretschmer, T., Lindenberg, S., Veenstra, R., 2015. Hedonic, instrumental,
empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Soc. Dev. 18 (1), 140–163. and normative motives: differentiating patterns for popular, accepted, and rejected
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00465.x. adolescents. J. Early Adolesc. 35 (3), 308–328. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Cillessen, A.H.N., Borch, C., 2006. Developmental trajectories of adolescent popularity: a 0272431614535092.
growth curve modelling analysis. J. Adolesc. 29 (6), 935–959. https://doi.org/10. Eder, D., 1985. The cycle of popularity: interpersonal relations among female adolescents.
1016/j.adolescence.2006.05.005. Sociol. Educ. 58, 154–165.
Cillessen, A.H.N., Mayeux, L., 2004. From censure to reinforcement: developmental Ellis, B.J., Volk, A.A., Gonzalez, J.M., Embry, D.D., 2015. The meaningful roles inter-
changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Dev. 75 (1), vention: an evolutionary approach to reducing bullying and increasing prosocial
147–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00660.x. behavior. J. Res. Adolesc. 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12243.
Cillessen, A.H.N., Rose, A.J., 2005. Understanding popularity in the peer system. Curr. Faris, R., 2012. Aggression, exclusivity, and status attainment in interpersonal networks.
Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14 (2), 102–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005. Soc. Forces 90 (4), 1207–1235. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sos074.
00343.x. Faris, R., Felmlee, D., 2011. Status struggles. Am. Sociol. Rev. 76 (1), 48–73. https://doi.
Cillessen, A.H.N., Schwartz, D., Mayeux, L. (Eds.), 2011. Popularity in the Peer System. org/10.1177/0003122410396196.
Guilford Press. Fujimoto, K., Snijders, T.A.B., Valente, T.W., 2017. Popularity breeds contempt: the
Coie, J.D., Dodge, K.A., Coppotelli, H., 1982. Dimensions and types of social status: a evolution of reputational dislike relations and friendships in high school. Soc.
cross-age perspective. Dev. Psychol. 18 (4), 557. Networks 48, 100–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2016.07.006.
De Bruyn, E.H., Cillessen, A.H.N., Wissink, I.B., 2010. Associations of peer acceptance and Gaffney, H., Ttofi, M.M., Farrington, D.P., 2018. Evaluating the effectiveness of school-
perceived popularity with bullying and victimization in early adolescence. J. Early bullying prevention programs: an updated meta-analytical review (online first).
Adolesc. 30 (4), 543–566. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431609340517. Aggress. Violent Behav. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.001.

10
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Gangel, M.J., Keane, S.P., Calkins, S.D., Shanahan, L., O’Brien, M., 2017. The association Ripley, R., Snijders, T.A.B., Boda, Z., Vörös, A., Preciado, P., 2018. Manual for RSiena.
between relational aggression and perceived popularity in early adolescence: a test of University of Oxford, Nuffield College, Oxford, UK.
competing hypotheses. J. Early Adolesc. 37 (8), 1078–1092. https://doi.org/10. Rodkin, P.C., Farmer, T.W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., 2006. They’re cool: social status and
1177/0272431616642327. peer group suppports for aggressive boys and girls. Soc. Dev. 15 (2), 175–204.
Garandeau, C.F., Lee, I.A., Salmivalli, C., 2014a. Differential effects of the KiVa anti- https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-9507.2006.00336.x.
bullying program on popular and unpopular bullies. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 35 (1), Sainio, M., Veenstra, R., Huitsing, G., Salmivalli, C., 2011. Victims and their defenders: a
44–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2013.10.004. dyadic approach. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35 (2), 144–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Garandeau, C.F., Poskiparta, E., Salmivalli, C., 2014b. Tackling acute cases of school 0165025410378068.
bullying in the KiVa anti-bullying program: a comparison of two approaches. J. Salmivalli, C., 2010. Bullying and the peer group: a review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 15
Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42 (6), 981–991. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014- (2), 112–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007.
9861-1. Salmivalli, C., 2014. Participant roles in bullying: how can peer bystanders be utilized in
Gourieroux, C., Monfort, A., 1996. Simulation-based Econometric Methods. Oxford uni- interventions? Theory Pract. 53 (4), 286–292. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.
versity press. 2014.947222.
Homans, G.C., 1950. The Human Group. Harcourt, World, and Brace, Inc., New York, NY. Salmivalli, C., Peets, K., 2009. Bullies, victims, and bully-victim relationships in middle
Huitsing, G., Monks, C.P., 2018. Who victimizes whom and who defends whom? A childhood and early adolescence. In: Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W.M., Laursen, B. (Eds.),
multivariate social network analysis of victimization, aggression, and defending in Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford, New York, pp.
early childhood. Aggress. Behav. 44, 394–405. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21760. 322–340.
Huitsing, G., Veenstra, R., 2012. Bullying in classrooms: participant roles from a social Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., Kaukiainen, A., 1996. Bullying
network perspective. Aggress. Behav. 38 (6), 494–509. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab. as a group process: participant roles and their relations to social status within the
21438. group. Aggress. Behav. 22 (1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-
Huitsing, G., Snijders, T.A.B., Van Duijn, M.A.J., Veenstra, R., 2014. Victims, bullies, and 2337(1996)22:1<1::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-T.
their defenders: a longitudinal study of the coevolution of positive and negative Salmivalli, C., Kärnä, A., Poskiparta, E., 2011. Counteracting bullying in Finland: the KiVa
networks. Dev. Psychopathol. 26 (3), 645–659. https://doi.org/10.1017/ program and its effects on different forms of being bullied. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35 (5),
S0954579414000297. 405–411. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411407457.
Juvonen, J., Wang, Y., Espinoza, G., 2013. Physical aggression, spreading of rumors, and Schwartz, D., Tom, S.R., Chang, L., Xu, Y., Duong, M.T., Kelly, B.M., 2010. Popularity and
social prominence in early adolescence: reciprocal effects supporting gender simila- acceptance as distinct dimensions of social standing for Chinese children in Hong
rities? J. Youth Adolesc. 42 (12), 1801–1810. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012- Kong. Soc. Dev. 19 (4), 681–697. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.
9894-0. 00558.x.
Kaufman, T.M., Kretschmer, T., Huitsing, G., Veenstra, R., 2018. Why does a universal Sentse, M., Dijkstra, J.K., Salmivalli, C., Cillessen, A.H.N., 2013. The dynamics of
anti-bullying program not help all children? Explaining persistent victimization friendships and victimization in adolescence: a longitudinal social network perspec-
during an intervention. Prev. Sci. 19, 822–832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121- tive. Aggress. Behav. 39 (3), 229–238. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21469.
018-0906-5. Sentse, M., Kretschmer, T., Salmivalli, C., 2015. The longitudinal interplay between
Kolbert, J.B., Crothers, L.M., 2003. Bullying and evolutionary psychology. J. Sch. bullying, victimization, and social status: age-related and gender differences. Soc.
Violence 2 (3), 73–91. https://doi.org/10.1300/J202v02n03_05. Dev. 24 (3), 659–677. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12115.
LaFontana, K.M., Cillessen, A.H.N., 1999. Children’s interpersonal perceptions as a Shoulberg, E.K., Sijtsema, J.J., Murray-Close, D., 2011. The association between valuing
function of sociometric and peer-perceived popularity. J. Genet. Psychol. 160, popularity and relational aggression: the moderating effects of actual popularity and
225–242. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221329909595394. physiological reactivity to exclusion. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 110 (1), 20–37. https://
Lease, A.M., Kennedy, C.A., Axelrod, J.L., 2002. Children’s social constructions of po- doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.008.
pularity. Soc. Dev. 11 (1), 87–109. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00188. Sijtsema, J.J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Salmivalli, C., 2009. Empirical test of bullies’
Lodder, G.M.A., Scholte, R.H.J., Cillessen, A.H.N., Giletta, M., 2016. Bully Victimization: status goals: assessing direct goals, aggression, and prestige. Aggress. Behav. 35 (1),
selection and influence within adolescent friendship networks and cliques. J. Youth 57–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20282.
Adolesc. 45 (1), 132–144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0343-8. Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C.C., Henry, J.D., 2015. Meta-analysis of Theory of
Merton, R.K., 1968. The Matthew Effect in Science: the reward and communication Mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. 86 (4),
systems of science are considered. Science 159 (3810), 56–63. https://doi.org/10. 1159–1174. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12372.
1126/science.159.3810.56. Snijders, T.A.B., Baerveldt, C., 2003. A multilevel network study of the effects of delin-
Nickerson, A.B., 2017. Preventing and intervening with bullying in schools: a framework quent behavior on friendship evolution. J. Math. Sociol. 27 (2–3), 123–151. https://
for evidence-based practice. School Ment. Health 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.1080/00222500305892.
s12310-017-9221-8. Snijders, T.A.B., Steglich, C.E.G., 2015. Representing micro-macro linkages by actor-
Oldenburg, B., Van Duijn, M.A.J., Veenstra, R., 2018. Defending one’s friends, not one’s based dynamic network models. Sociol. Methods Res. 44 (2), 222–271. https://doi.
enemies: a social network analysis of children’s defending, friendship, and dislike org/10.1177/0049124113494573.
relationships using XPNet. PLoS One 13 (5), e0194323. Snijders, T.A.B., van de Bunt, G.G., Steglich, C., 2010. Introduction to stochastic actor-
Olweus, D., 1978. Aggression in the Schools: Bullies and Whipping Boys. Hemisphere based models for network dynamics. Soc. Netw. 32 (1), 44–60. https://doi.org/10.
(Wiley), Washington, DC. 1016/j.socnet.2009.02.004.
Olweus, D., 1993. Bullying at School: What We Know and What We Can Do. Blackwell Snijders, T.A.B., Lomi, A., Torló, V.J., 2013. A model for the multiplex dynamics of two-
Publishing, Malden, MA. mode and one-mode networks, with an application to employment preference,
Olweus, D., 1996. The Revised Olweus bully/victim Questionnaire. Research Center for friendship, and advice. Soc. Netw. 35 (2), 265–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Health Promotion (HEMIL Center), University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway. socnet.2012.05.005.
Parkhurst, J.T., Hopmeyer, A., 1998. Sociometric popularity and peer-perceived popu- Swearer, S.M., Espelage, D.L., Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., 2010. What can be done about
larity: two distinct dimensions of peer status. J. Early Adolesc. 18, 125–144. https:// school bullying? Linking research to educational practice. Educ. Res. 39 (1), 38–47.
doi.org/10.1177/0272431698018002001. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X09357622.
Pattiselanno, K., Dijkstra, J.K., Steglich, C., Vollebergh, W., Veenstra, R., 2015. Structure Turanovic, J.J., Young, J.T.N., 2016. Violent offending and victimization in adolescence:
Matters: the role of clique hierarchy in the relationship between adolescent social social network mechanisms and homophily. Criminology 54 (3), 487–519. https://
status and aggression and prosociality. J. Youth Adolesc. 44 (12), 2257–2274. doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12112.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0310-4. Van der Ploeg, R., Steglich, C., Salmivalli, C., Veenstra, R., 2015. The intensity of victi-
Peets, K., Hodges, E.V.E., 2014. Is popularity associated with aggression toward socially mization: associations with children’s psychosocial well-being and social standing in
preferred or marginalized targets? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 124, 112–123. https://doi. the classroom. PLoS One 10 (10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141490.
org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.002. Van der Ploeg, R., Steglich, C., Veenstra, R., 2016. The support group approach in the
Puckett, M.B., Aikins, J.W., Cillessen, A.H.N., 2008. Moderators of the association be- Dutch KiVa anti-bullying programme: effects on victimisation, defending and well-
tween relational aggression and perceived popularity. Aggress. Behav. 34 (6), being at school. Educ. Res. 58 (3), 221–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131881.
563–576. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20280. 2016.1184949.
Rambaran, J.A., Dijkstra, J.K., Munniksma, A., Cillessen, A.H.N., 2015. The development Veenman, S., 1995. Cognitive and noncognitive effects of multigrade and multi-age
of adolescents’ friendships and antipathies: a longitudinal multivariate network test classes: a best-evidence synthesis. Rev. Educ. Res. 65 (4), 319–381. https://doi.org/
of balance theory. Soc. Netw. 43, 162–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2015. 10.3102/00346543065004319.
05.003. Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Zijlstra, B.J.H., De Winter, A.F., Verhulst, F.C., Ormel, J.,
Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M.M., Goossens, F.A., Olthof, T., Van de Schoot, R., Aleva, E.A., 2007. The dyadic nature of bullying and victimization: testing a dual-perspective
Van der Meulen, M., 2013a. Developmental trajectories of bullying and social dom- theory. Child Dev. 78 (6), 1843–1854. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.
inance in youth. Child Abuse Negl. 37 (4), 224–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 01102.x.
chiabu.2012.12.004. Veenstra, R., Dijkstra, J.K., Steglich, C., Van Zalk, M.H.W., 2013. Network-behavior dy-
Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M.M., Olthof, T., Goossens, F.A., Van de Schoot, R., Aleva, E.A., namics. J. Res. Adolesc. 23 (3), 399–412. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12070.
Van der Meulen, M., 2013b. Costs and benefits of bullying in the context of the peer Verlinden, M., Veenstra, R., Ringoot, A.P., Jansen, P.W., Raat, H., Hofman, A., et al.,
group: a three wave longitudinal analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41 (8), 2014. Detecting bullying in early elementary school with a computerized peer-no-
1217–1229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9759-3. mination instrument. Psychol. Assess. 26 (2), 628–641. https://doi.org/10.1037/

11
R. van der Ploeg et al. Social Networks xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

a0035571. bullying research. Aggress. Violent Behav. 36 (July), 34–43. https://doi.org/10.


Volk, A.A., Dane, A.V., Marini, Z.A., 2014. What is bullying? A theoretical redefinition. 1016/j.avb.2017.07.003.
Dev. Rev. 34 (4), 327–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.09.001. Wölfer, R., Scheithauer, H., 2014. Social influence and bullying behavior: intervention-
Volk, A.A., Della Cioppa, V., Earle, M., Farrell, A.H., 2015. Social competition and bul- based network dynamics of the fairplayer.mAnual bullying prevention program.
lying: an adaptive socioecological perspective. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Welling, L., Aggress. Behav. 40 (4), 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21524.
Shackelford, T. (Eds.), Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology. Springer Wolters, N., Knoors, H., Cillessen, A.H., Verhoeven, L., 2014. Behavioral, personality, and
Internation Publishing Switzerland. communicative predictors of acceptance and popularity in early adolescence. J. Early
Volk, A.A., Veenstra, R., Espelage, D.L., 2017. So you want to study bullying? Adolesc. 34 (5), 585–605. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431613510403.
Recommendations to enhance the validity, transparency, and compatibility of

12

You might also like