Khanna Et Al (2013) - Triguna and Well-Being

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.

69]

Original Article

Relationship between Triguna theory and well-being indicators


Pulkit Khanna, Kamlesh Singh, Surbhi Singla, Vivek Verma
Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology Delhi, Hauz Khas, New Delhi, India

Address for Correspondence: Ms. Pulkit Khanna,


Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology – Delhi,
Hauz Khas, New Delhi - 110 016, India.
E-mail: pulkit1007@gmail.com

ABSTRACT

The Indian perspective of personality deals with the tri‑dimensional classification of Gunas (Sattva,
Rajas, and Tamas) entailing physical, mental, and spiritual elements of personality. The present study
aims to examine the relationship between Gunas and well‑being indicators such as psychological capital,
personality, life satisfaction, and subjective happiness. The study was conducted on two samples.
Vedic Personality Inventory[1] and Mental Health Continuum‑Short Form[2] were administered to both
samples. The first sample consisted of 80 Indian professionals (males = 51 and females = 29) with mean
age = 28.8 years (SD = 7.19) who were administered Psychological Capital Questionnaire[3] and Big‑Five
Personality Inventory[4] and the second sample consisted of 110 students (males = 82 and females = 28)
with mean age = 21 years (SD = 2.72) who were administered Satisfaction with Life Scale[5] and Subjective
Happiness Scale.[6] Across both studies, Sattva was found to be positively correlated with well‑being.
Rajas and Tamas were negatively correlated with well‑being. Higher levels of Sattva and well‑being were
reported in the older age‑group. Males scored higher on Rajas while no gender differences were found
in well‑being.
Key words: Big‑Five, Gunas, PsyCap, well‑being

INTRODUCTION speculative, practical and ritualistic, and of course,


systematic and scientific understanding of human
Psychologists over the years have defined human nature.”[8]
personality using many models and theories. The
Guna theory of tri‑dimensional classification of human
The Indian perspective of personality refers to both the
personality emanates from Indian psychology (IP). The
biological and psychological system. The psychological
manifestation of the Gunas and their influence on the
system dealing with the Trigunas was the focus of the
human mind and behavior has interested psychologists
present study. This system incorporates the physical,
and researchers across the world.[7]
mental, and spiritual aspects of personality.[9] The inherent
richness of the triadic language of the Gunas offers a wider
Indian psychology and perspective of personality
scope to understand human nature as contrasted with the
While IP is indigenous in origin, its appeal is universal. bipolar descriptions of Western psychology.[9] With its focus
It has been said that IP is a “complex subject variously on the innate qualities of the three Gunas whose varying
viewed as esoteric and spiritual, philosophical and balance embodies personality, the Indian perspective
offers a deeper, holistic, and spiritual understanding of
Access this article online human nature.[9]
Quick Response Code
Website:
www.ijoyppp.in Understanding the three Gunas
The Guna theory originates from the Sankhya school
DOI: of Indian philosophy which states that the entire
10.4103/2347-5633.157888 physical universe or “prakriti” is made up of three
constituents – Sattva, Rajas, and Tamas.[10] These Gunas

International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013 69


[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.69]

Khanna, et al.: Gunas and well-being

interact with the environment and are expressed as socialization and the Guna theory. Kohlberg’s work
personality traits. In the absence of accurate English on the stages of moral development beginning from
translations for the Gunas, Chakraborty equated the word “Preconventional Morality” up to “Postconventional
“illumination” for Sattva, “movement” for Rajas, and Morality” deals with the concepts of values, morality,
“obstruction” for Tamas.[11] Gunas encompass all existence justice, etc., which also find a mention in the Guna
and actions, and there is a dynamic transformation of theory.[19] Maslow’s reference to the presence of a certain
energy among them. degree of “sainthood” across all people is strongly
reminiscent of the omnipresence of the three Gunas
An individual’s mood at any given time is a manifestation across all individuals.[20]
of the Gunas; each of which embodies a different way of
internal feeling and outwardly relation with the world.[9] Mental health and well‑being
Each Guna can dominate other Gunas at all times. However,
each moment is characterized by the dominance of one Well‑being has been understood as the process of pain
Guna over the others. It is also possible for a combination avoidance and pleasure attainment (hedonism) and also
of Gunas to pervade a person. in terms of focus on meaning and pursuit of fulfillment of
the true individual nature (eudaimonia).[21,22]
Sattva Guna
Sattva is characterized by balance, peace, equanimity, and Although the clinical tradition views well‑being through
qualities such as cleanliness, truthfulness, dutifulness, the lens of disorders, the psychological tradition focuses
detachment, discipline, contentment, and staunch on the area of “subjective well‑being” ‑ a multi‑dimensional
determination.[12] Sattvic quality has been described as evaluation of a person’s life and includes cognitive
being “free from attachment and vanity and absolutely satisfaction and affective evaluations of moods and
unruffled in success and failure.”[13] emotions.[23]

Rajas Guna The present study deals with well‑being with reference
Prabhupada has described Rajas as an intermediary to Keyes model encompassing three aspects of well‑being
between Tamas and Sattva.[14] Rajas is intense, dynamic, under the same umbrella.[24,25]
passionate and is marked by agitation, anxiety, nervousness.
Attributes of Rajas include intense activity, desire for sense Hedonic/emotional well‑being (EWB) primarily deals
gratification, little interest in spiritual elevation, envy of with pleasure attainment and pain avoidance, taking into
others, and materialistic mentality.[12] account one’s emotional experiences in a typical month.
The concept of social well‑being (SWB) broadly includes
Tamas Guna the aspects of social integration, social contribution, social
It is manifested in dullness, lethargy, fatigue, and even coherence, social actualization, and social acceptance
depression. Qualities associated with Tamas include which indicate an individual’s well‑being about his social
mental imbalance, anger, ignorance, arrogance, and setting. Another integral component - Ryff’s[26] model of
helplessness.[12] Dasgupta explains “the quality of Tamas psychological well‑being (PWB) that includes autonomy,
overcomes the illumination of knowledge and leads to environmental mastery, personal growth, positive
many errors. Tamas, being a product of ignorance, blinds relations with others, purpose in life and self‑acceptance
all living beings and binds them down with carelessness, is considered in the present study. This multi‑component
idleness and sleep.”[13] model of well‑being was assessed by the Mental Health
Continuum‑Short Form (MHC‑SF) [2] and used in the
“Triguna” and its correlates present study.

In the Indian psychological literature, Guna theory is Rationale of this study


empirically tested and accepted.[15] The Guna constructs
are backed by strong psychometric evidence such as While substantial work has been done to explore the
reliability and validity which renders them compatible Western modules of personality; there exists a need
with empirical studies influenced by the Western to explore the indigenous Vedic theory of Triguna
style.[1,12,16,17] personality in relation with popular concepts such
as Big‑Five, well‑being, PsyCap, life satisfaction, and
Further, the Guna theory is echoed in the Western subjective happiness. Different cultures have reported
categorization of values. Brown and Chatterjee[18] have that characteristics associated with the three Gunas
focused on the similarity between Kohlberg’s three correlate with well‑being (though using different models)
level sequential model of cognitive development in in ways similar to those that are supported by this study.

70 International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013


[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.69]

Khanna, et al.: Gunas and well-being

Greater the presence of Sattva, greater is the experience of 56 items including 15 Sattva items, 19 Rajas items,
Ananda (bliss).[27] Fruit and vegetable rich diets (a Sattvic and 22 Tamas items. Wolf reported that VPI has good
quality) have been found to correlate with well‑being.[28] internal consistency ranging from α =0.70 to 0.92 for
The present study aimed to bridge this gap by focusing on the three Gunas.[1] Reliability values of α =0.74–0.79
the human temperament from a more holistic perspective. were reported in the present study.

Further, this study attempted to address the shortcoming Psychological capital questionnaire
of relying only on Western psychological constructs for Psychological Capital Questionnaire‑24[3] has 24 items that
understanding the psychology of indigenous peoples.[27] include six items for each of hope, efficacy, resilience,
Instead of creating a forced equivalence with constructs and optimism. Reliability ranging from α =0.88 to 0.89
borrowed from Western thought, studying human nature has been originally obtained for this instrument.[29] The
in terms of Guna classification may offer the scope to deal present study reported the alpha reliability for total
with some nuances inherent to nonWestern cultures. PsyCap at α =0.84; ranging from α =0.63 to 0.84 for the
constituent factors with the exception of “optimism” for
Based on the review of the literature, it was hypothesized which α =0.42.
that Sattva would be positively correlated with well‑being
indicators, while Rajas and Tamas would be negatively Big‑five inventory
correlated with well‑being indicators. The 44‑item self‑rating version of the Big‑Five
Inventory (BFI)[4] was used. It contained statements for
MATERIALS AND METHODS the five personality factors (openness, conscientiousness,
extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) to assess
Participants the respondent’s perceptions about his/her self. Typical
internal consistency coefficients for the BFI trait scores
The present study was conducted in two parts involving
have been reported to range from α = 0.75 to 0.90.[30] In the
different samples as explained below:
present study, internal consistency ranged from α = 0.88
• Sample 1: The first sample consisted of 80 Indian
to α =0.95 for different personality factors.
professionals from different organizations, including
51 males and 29 females (mean age = 28.8 years and
SD = 7.19). Of them, 28 were married, 49 single, 1 separated, In addition to the MHC‑SF and VPI explained above, the
and 2 did not share their marital status following tests were administered to sample 2.
• Sample 2: The second sample included 110 Indian
students ‑ 82 males and 28 females (mean age = 21 years Satisfaction with life scale
and SD = 2.72). These participants were enrolled in Satisfaction with Life Scale[5] is a 5‑item self‑report scale
undergraduate, postgraduate or doctoral courses. that measures global life satisfaction. While the scale was
originally reported to have a coefficient alpha of α =0.87,
Tests used the present study found the alpha reliability of the test to
be α =0.75.
Separate booklets containing tests, demographic
information sheet and consent letter were prepared Subjective happiness scale
for sample 1 and sample 2. The following tests were Subjective Happiness Scale [6] is a self‑report scale
administered to sample 1. comprising four items that measure global subjective
happiness. Alpha reliability ranging from α = 0.79 to 0.94
Mental health continuum‑short form has been previously reported for this instrument.[6] The
The MHC‑SF[2] contains 14 items, and it assesses three present study found this instrument to have an alpha
dimensions namely: EWB, SWB, and PWB on a 6‑point reliability of α = 0.79.
Likert type scale. The internal consistency for this
instrument was originally estimated to be > 0.80.[25] The
RESULTS
present study found an overall test consistency of α =0.88,
ranging from α =0.75 to 0.84 for the three aspects of Correlation analysis between Guna and well‑being
well‑being. scores across both samples was employed. Correlation
wa s also studie d a t the le ve l of both s a m pl e s
The Vedic personality inventory individually in terms of Guna scores and the other
The Vedic Personality Inventory (VPI) is the most tests administered. Differences in mean scores on Guna
extensively researched and validated psychological and well‑being across the variables of age and gender
assessment tool based on the three Gunas.[1] It contains were also studied.

International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013 71


[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.69]

Khanna, et al.: Gunas and well-being

Table 1 shows the correlation among the Gunas and Sattva than younger group [Table 2]. This finding indicated
different aspects of well‑being based on the results a rise in Sattvic element of personality with age.
of VPI and MHC‑SF across both samples. Significant
positive intra‑test correlation was seen among all The older group scored significantly higher than younger
aspects of well‑being. As regards VPI, Sattva was found group on EWB, PWB as well as total MHC.
to be negatively correlated with both Rajas (r = −0.19,
p < 0.01) and Tamas (r = −0.41, p < 0.01) while Rajas
The positive correlation between Sattva and well‑being
and Tamas were positively correlated (r = 0.63, p < 0.01).
[Table 1] was reinforced by the finding that both were
Further, Sattva was positively correlated with each of
EWB (r = 0.40, p < 0.01), SWB (r = 0.29, p < 0.01), PWB found to be significantly higher in the older group.
(r = 0.51, p < 0.01) as well as total well‑being (r = 0.48,
p < 0.01). On the other hand, both Rajas and Tamas were Table 3 shows no significant difference in Sattva and Tamas
negatively correlated with all aspects of well‑being. between males and females. However, Rajas was found
to be significantly higher in case of males as compared to
Relation between Gunas and other well‑being indicators in females. There was no significant gender difference in
scores of mental health (well‑being) and its constituent
In sample 1, Sattva was positively correlated with factors.
PsyCap as a whole (r = 0.36, p < 0.01) as well as with
efficacy (r = 0.27, p < 0.05) and hope (r = 0.49, p < 0.01).
DISCUSSION
Rajas and Tamas were not significantly correlated with
any aspect of PsyCap. As regards the relation between the Our findings about the correlation between Gunas and
Indian and Western model of personality, the correlations well‑being across both studies supported literature
between Gunas and Big‑Five reflected an interesting reporting that EWB was positively correlated with PWB
picture. Sattva was positively correlated with extraversion
and SWB.[31] In the case of VPI, Tamas was correlated
(r = 0.27, p < 0.05), agreeableness (r = 0.28, p < 0.05),
negatively with Sattva and positively with Rajas across
conscientiousness (r = 0.40, p < 0.01), openness (r = 0.33,
both samples. These findings were aligned with Vedic
p  < 0.01) and negatively correlated with neuroticism
(r = −0.30, p < 0.01). Rajas was negatively correlated with
agreeableness (r = −0.41, p < 0.01), conscientiousness Table 1: Mean, SD, alpha reliability, and correlation
(r = −0.38, p < 0.01), openness (r = −0.26, p < 0.05) and between VPI and MHC‑SF across both samples
positively correlated with neuroticism (r = 0.41, p < 0.01). 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
EWB 0.76#
Tamas too showed a similar pattern of correlation, being
SWB 0.50** 0.75#
negatively correlated with agreeableness (r = −0.27, PWB 0.62** 0.53** 0.84#
p  < 0.05), conscientiousness (r = −0.41, p < 0.01), Total 0.77** 0.84** 0.88** 0.88#
Openness (r = −0.30, p < 0.01) and positively correlated MHC
with neuroticism (r = 0.36, p < 0.01). Both Rajas and Sattva 0.40** 0.29** 0.51** 0.48** 0.74#
Rajas −0.29** −0.15* −0.26** −0.26** −0.19** 0.76#
Tamas were not significantly correlated with extraversion.
Tamas −0.31** −0.24** −0.34** −0.35** −0.41** 0.63** 0.79#
Mean 10.64 13.43 21.36 45.43 76.37 69.46 68.05
In sample 2, Sattva showed positive correlation with SD 2.77 5.21 5.49 11.35 10.14 13.89 16.59
both life satisfaction (r = 0.24, p < 0.01) and subjective n=190. **p<0.01, *p<0.05, #Alpha reliability coefficients. SD=Standard
deviation, MHC‑SF=Mental Health Continuum‑Short Form, VPI=Vedic
happiness (r = 0.24, p < 0.05). Subjective happiness was Personality Inventory, EWB=Emotional well‑being, SWB=Social well‑being,
negatively correlated with Rajas (r = −0.38, p < 0.01) and PWB=Psychological well‑being, MHC=Mental Health Continuum
with Tamas (r = −0.42, p < 0.01). However, life satisfaction
was not found to be significantly correlated with Rajas Table 2: Difference between Gunas and well‑being
and Tamas. based on age
Dimension ≤25  years (mean  (SD)) t value
Table 2 represents the results of the t‑test to study if there Group  1 (younger Group  2 (older
were any significant differences in Gunas and well‑being group) n=130 group) n=57
Sattva 71.65 (9.69) 74.72 (8.49) −2.07*
scores based on age of the participants. For ease of analysis,
Rajas 53.02  (10.19) 51.21 (11.29) 1.08
the participants from both studies were divided into two Tamas 43.63 (9.79) 44.65  (12.30) −0.60
categories, up to 25 years (younger group) and over 25 years EWB 10.22  (2.77) 11.44 (2.58) −2.82**
of age (older group). Since the information about age was SWB 12.88 (5.13) 14.33  (5.04) −1.80
not available for three participants from the total sample of PWB 20.34  (5.41) 23.58 (4.94) −3.87**
Total MHC 43.44  (10.99) 49.35  (10.8) −3.40**
190, “N” in this analysis was 187. No significant difference
n=187, **p<0.01, *p<0.05. SD=Standard deviation, EWB=Emotional
was reported between the two groups on Rajas and Tamas. well‑being, SWB=Social well‑being, PWB=Psychological well‑being,
However, the older group scored significantly higher on MHC=Mental Health Continuum

72 International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013


[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.69]

Khanna, et al.: Gunas and well-being

Table 3: Difference in Gunas and well‑being between PWB.[37,38] Older adults tend to find their activities more
males and females enjoyable and less stressful than younger people.[39] It was
Dimension Mean  (SD) t found that males scored higher on Rajas as compared to
Males  (n=133) Females  (n=57) females. Bem’s sex role inventory classified characteristics
Sattva 72.92 (9.2) 72.21  (10.24) 0.47 like “aggressive,” “ambitious” and “competitive” (all of
Rajas 54.46  (10.01) 48.00  (10.51) 4.02**
Tamas 44.6  (10.89) 41.93  (9.70) 1.60
which correspond to Rajas) as being masculine as opposed
EWB 10.48  (2.74) 11.02  (2.85) −1.22 to feminine.[40] Gender differences on account of adopting
SWB 13.28 (5.23) 13.77 (5.19) −0.60 socially appropriate gender roles could potentially explain
PWB 21.29 (5.44) 21.51 (5.65) −0.25 why males scored significantly higher than females on
Total MHC 45.05  (11.01) 46.30  (12.16) −0.69
Rajas. The conventional Indian gender role stereotype
n=190, **p<0.01. SD=Standard deviation, EWB=Emotional well‑being,
SWB=Social well‑being, PWB=Psychological well‑being, MHC=Mental Health of the male assuming the characteristics of aggression,
Continuum ambition and pursuing material goals (dimensions of
Rajas) and the consequent self‑fulfilling prophecies in
knowledge that Sattva is related positively to well‑being, social behavior could explain higher Rajas in males as
while Tamas is largely antagonistic to well‑being. The compared to females in the present study.[41] Reinforcement
inverse relationship between Sattva and Tamas found in for this explanation came from research that the country or
our study has also been reported in other research on the environmental context of the study influences the gender
Gunas.[16,12,31] Sattva has been reported to be positively differences in competitiveness and risk‑taking ‑ both
correlated with EWB, PWB and SWB while Rajas and of which are attributes associated with the Rajasic
Tamas were negatively correlated with all well‑being personality.[42‑45] Thus, there is a need to be careful so as to
indicators.[31] not generalize the findings from Western culture to other
different cultural contexts and vice versa.
The finding of the present study wherein the older group
scored significantly higher on Sattva when compared to
CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTION
the younger group can be understood better in the light
of existing literature that supports universal maturational The present study was a step ahead towards integrating
changes in adult personality. A decline in aggression and the holistic Indian approach of personality with widely
an increase in control has been reported on the basis of a used Western concepts. However, this study had certain
longitudinal study of twins over 10 years period.[32] There limitations. The predominance of males in both samples
appears to be a decline in neuroticism and enhancement hindered a fair gender‑based comparison of test scores.
of positive personality traits from early adulthood to Moreover, all participants were limited to urban, educated
midlife.[33] Older adults have been found to differ from population.
younger adults in terms of better impulse control and
moral responsibility. [34] While these findings do not
Future research involving more diverse and representative
emanate from IP per se, the personality characteristics
samples is required to attain conclusive evidence about the
like positivity, impulse control and morality in which they
report an increase from younger to older adults correspond relation between Gunas and well‑being indicators and its
with Sattvic personality characteristics. In this sense, the trends across different segments of the population. Existing
present study provided a culturally sensitive reinforcement literature pertaining to Guna profiles of participants
to existing evidence in the area. Further support for showed that mean Guna scores were different across
this finding came from the use of VPI in the Western different cultures. The American sample [35] showed
context which showed that the scores on VPI goodness significantly higher Sattva score and lower scores on
subscale (goodness was substituted for Sattva to make it Rajas and Tamas in comparison to Indian and Czech
more relevant for Western sample) correlated positively samples.[31] Since Sattva and well‑being were found to be
with increased age.[35] Herein, test–retest reliability ranging highly correlated, they plausibly shared some percentage
from 0.73 to 0.89 was obtained for the subscales of VPI.[35] of variance. Further, America has been ranked higher on
This reinforces the stability of assessment of these traits. happiness (which is related to well‑being) than Czech
Republic and India.[46] The moderating and mediating
As regards the older group being higher on well‑being variables accounting for such trends remain to be studied
as compared to the younger group, these findings were by future research. The Indian module may potentially
aligned with research[36] that suggests that the cognitive emerge as another model to understand human nature and
aspect of subjective well‑being is positively correlated with personality. Future work in the area can look at a holistic
age. Some studies have reported that with increasing age, picture integrating different indigenous components,
adults feel greater happiness and satisfaction with their including Ayurveda, Yoga, folk medicines, etc., and their
lives and also report higher levels of some dimensions of impact on body‑mind processes.

International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013 73


[Downloaded free from http://www.ijoyppp.org on Wednesday, December 25, 2019, IP: 210.160.217.69]

Khanna, et al.: Gunas and well-being

REFERENCES 27. Gergen KJ, Gulerce A, Lock A, Misra G. Psychological science in cultural


context. Am Psychol 1996;51:496‑503.
1. Wolf D. The Vedic Personality Inventory: A study of the Gunas. J Indian 28. Martin A, Cherubini A, Andres‑Lacueva C, Paniagua M, Joseph J.
Psychol 1998;16:26‑43. Effects of fruits and vegetables on levels of vitamins E and C in the brain
2. Keyes CL. Brief description of the mental health continuum‑short and their association with cognitive performance. J Nutr Health Aging
form (MHC‑SF), 2009. Available from: http://www.sociology.emory.edu/ 2002;6:392‑404.
ckeyes. [Last cited on 2012 Jun 22]. 29. Luthans F, Avolio BJ, Avey JB, Norman SM. Positive psychological capital:
3. Luthans F, Youssef CM, Avolio BJ. In: Psychological Capital. New York: Measurement and relationship with performance and satisfaction. Pers Psychol
Oxford University Press; 2007. p. 237‑8. 2007;60:541‑72.
4. Benet‑Martínez V, John OP. Los Cinco Grandes across cultures and ethnic 30. John OP, Srivastava S. The big‑five trait taxonomy: History, measurement,
groups: Multitrait multimethod analyses of the Big Five in Spanish and and theoretical perspectives. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors.
English. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;75:729‑50. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford
5. Diener  E, Emmons  RA, Larsen  RJ, Griffin  S. The Satisfaction With Life Press; 1999. p. 102‑39.
Scale. J Pers Assess 1985;49:71‑5. 31. Singh K, Slezackova A. Relationship between Gunas and mental health,
6. Lyubomirsky S, Lepper H. A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary flourishing, positive and negative experience: An Indian and Western
reliability and construct validation. Soc Indic Res 1999;46:137‑55. perspective. Presented at 2nd International Conference on Positive Psychology
in the Czech Republic, Brno, Czech Republic, May 22‑24, 2013.
7. Shilpa S, Murthy CG. Development and standardization of Mysore Triguna
scale. Sage Open 2012;2:1‑10. 32. McGue M, Bacon S, Lykken DT. Personality stability and change in early
adulthood: A behavioural genetic analysis. Dev Psychol 1993;29:96‑109.
8. Rao KR, Paranjpe AC, Dalal AK. In: Handbook of Indian Psychology.
New Delhi: Cambridge University Press; 2008. 33. Aldwin CM, Levenson MR. Aging and personality assessment. Annu Rev
Gerontol Geriatr 1994;14:182‑209.
9. Wilberg P. Gunas: The triadic key to yogic psychology. In: Selected Writings
on the New Yoga. Vol. 1. 2007. p. 5-17. Available from: http://www. 34. McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr, Pedroso de Lima M, Simões A, Ostendorf F,
Peterwilberg.org. [Last retrieved on 2013 Mar 5]. Angleitner A, et al. Age differences in personality across the adult life span:
Parallels in five cultures. Dev Psychol 1999;35:466‑77.
10. Chakraborty SK. In: Managerial Effectiveness and Quality of Work Life:
Indian Insights. New Delhi: Tata McGraw‑Hill; 1987. 35. Stempel HS, Cheston SE, Greer JM, Gillespie CK. Further exploration of the
Vedic Personality Inventory: Validity, reliability and generalizability. Psychol
11. Chakraborty SK. In: Human Response in Organizations: Towards the Indian
Rep 2006;98:261‑73.
Ethos. Calcutta: Vivekananda Nidhi; 1985.
36. Shmotkin D. Subjective well‑being as a function of age and gender:
12. Wolf DB. A psychometric analysis of the three gunas. Psychol Rep
A multivariate look for differentiated trends. Soc Indic Res 1990;23:201‑30.
1999;84:1379‑90.
37. Heidrich SM, Ryff CD. The self in later years of life. In: Sperry L, Prosen H,
13. Dasgupta S. In: A History of Indian Philosophy. Great Britain: Cambridge
editors. Aging in the Twenty‑First Century: A Developmental Perspective.
University Press; 1961.
New York: Garland; 1996. p. 73‑102.
14. Prabhupada AC. In: Srimad Bhagavatam. Hong Kong: Bhaktivedanta Book
38. Ryff CD, Keyes CL. The structure of psychological well‑being revisited.
Trust; 1976.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1995;69:719‑27.
15. Murthy PK, Kumar SK. Concept triguna: A critical analysis and synthesis.
39. Lavery J, Horley J. Lifestyle, activity, and prevention. Paper Presented at the
Psychol Stud (Mysore) 2007;52:103‑13.
11th Annual Meeting of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, Chicago, April,
16. Pathak NS, Bhatt ID, Sharma R. Manual for classifying personality on
1990.
tridimensions of Gunas‑An Indian approach. Indian J Behav 1992;16:1‑14.
40. Bem SL. The measurement of psychological androgyny. J Consult Clin
17. Rao PV, Harigopal K. The three Gunas and ESP: An exploratory investigation.
Psychol 1974;42:155‑62.
J Indian Psychol 1979;2:63‑8.
41. Major B, Carnevale PJ, Deaux K. A different perspective on androgyny:
18. Brown MI, Chatterjee S. The relevance of the Guna theory in the congruence of
Evaluations of masculine and feminine personality characteristics. J Pers
eastern values and western management practice. J Hum Values 1999;5:93‑102.
Soc Psychol 1981;41:988‑1001.
19. Kohlberg L. In: Stages in the Development of Moral Thought and Action.
42. Booth AL, Nolen PJ. Choosing to compete: How different are girls and boys?
New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1969.
IZA Discussion Paper No. 4027, 2009.
20. Pandey A, Gupta RK. Spirituality in management: A review of contemporary
43. Booth AL, Nolen PJ. Gender differences in risk behaviour: Does nurture
and traditional thoughts and agenda for research. Glob Bus Rev 2008;9:65.
matter? IZA Discussion Paper No. 4026, 2009.
21. Kahneman D. Objective happiness. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N,
44. Gneezy U, Rustichini A. Gender and competition at a young age. Am Econ
editors. Well‑being: Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. New York: Russell
Rev 2004;94:377‑81.
Sage Foundation Press; 1999. p. 3‑25.
45. Sutter M, Rützler D. Gender differences in competition emerge early in life.
22. Waterman AS. Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal
IZA Discussion Paper No. 5015, 2010.
expressiveness (Eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J Pers Soc Psychol
46. Helliwell JF, Layard R, Sachs J, editors. World Happiness Report 2013.
1993;64:678‑91.
New York: UN Sustainable Development Solutions Network; 2013.
23. Younes MS. Positive mental health, subjective vitality and satisfaction with
life for French physical education students. World J Sports Sci 2011;4:90‑7.
24. Keyes CL. Social well‑being. Soc Psychol Q 1998;61:121‑40.
How to cite this article: Khanna P, Singh K, Singla S, Verma V.
25. Keyes CL. Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the Relationship between Triguna theory and well-being indicators. Int J
complete state model of health. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005;73:539‑48. Yoga - Philosop Psychol Parapsychol 2013;1:69-74.
26. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of
Source of Support: Nil, Conflict of Interest: None declared
psychological wellbeing. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989;57:1069‑81.

74 International Journal of Yoga - Philosophy, Psychology and Parapsychology  Vol. 1  Jul-Dec-2013

You might also like