Self Effflow PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/226234360

Academic Self-Efficacy Beliefs and Quality of Experience in Learning

Article  in  Journal of Youth and Adolescence · March 2007


DOI: 10.1007/s10964-006-9069-y

CITATIONS READS
99 851

4 authors:

Marta Bassi Patrizia Steca


University of Milan Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca
63 PUBLICATIONS   1,049 CITATIONS    93 PUBLICATIONS   2,523 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Antonella Delle Fave Gian Vittorio Caprara


University of Milan University of Southern Mississippi
131 PUBLICATIONS   2,388 CITATIONS    187 PUBLICATIONS   11,221 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Flow studies View project

Eudaimonic and Hedonic Happiness Investigation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Patrizia Steca on 17 February 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


J Youth Adolescence
DOI 10.1007/s10964-006-9069-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

Academic Self-Efficacy Beliefs and Quality of Experience


in Learning
Marta Bassi · Patrizia Steca · Antonella Delle Fave ·
Gian Vittorio Caprara


C Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Abstract This study investigated learning activities and Keywords Optimal experience . Academic self-efficacy
associated quality of experience of students with different beliefs . Self-regulated learning
levels of perceived academic self-efficacy. Two groups were
formed out of 130 Italian adolescents (age 15–19), one with
Introduction
high and one with low academic self-efficacy beliefs (31 and
32 participants, respectively). Students provided valuation
Time spent learning accounts for a large portion of adoles-
of academic pursuits and aspirations, and were monitored
cents’ life (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984; Alsaker and
for one week with experience sampling method (ESM). At-
Flammer 1999), and success at school generally represents
tention was paid to the association of learning activities with
a desired attainment for both students and their families.
optimal experience, characterized by high perceived environ-
However, not all pupils have a good academic performance,
mental challenges matched by high personal skills, involve-
nor do all value or enjoy learning activities. Disaffection
ment, concentration and intrinsic reward. High self-efficacy
with school and drop-out are not exceptional, as adolescents’
students reported higher academic aspirations and pursuits
social environment is wide and rich in distractions, and par-
than low self-efficacy students. They also spent more time
ents’ monitoring becomes less restrictive than during child-
in homework, and primarily associated learning activities
hood (Furstenberg et al. 1998; Gasperoni 2001). Thus, fos-
with optimal experience. Results have educational impli-
tering adolescents’ motivation and enjoyment in learning has
cations in fostering motivation and enjoyment in learning.
called for great attention from school managers as well as
They also provide empirical support for the combination of
educational theorists (Ranson and Martin 1996; Gasperoni
self-efficacy beliefs and quality of experience in motivational
2001).
research.
Various perspectives and solutions have been proposed.
From a conservative educational position, more discipline,
M. Bassi () fixed curricula and higher external standards are consid-
Lecturer of Psychology at the Medical School of the University of
Milan, Italy. She received her Ph.D. in 1999 from the University
of Milan. She has conducted research in human development, A. Delle Fave
focusing on adolescents’ quality of experience and identity Professor of Psychology at the Medical School of the University
building, as well as on methodological aspects of experience of Milan, Italy. Her main research interests are the cross-cultural
sampling procedures. Dipartimento di Scienze Precliniche “LITA investigation of the quality of daily experience and its long-term
Vialba”, Università degli Studi di Milano, Via G.B. Grassi 74, developmental impact. She is supervisor in intervention projects
20157 Milano, Italy on migration, disability and social maladjustment
e-mail: marta.bassi@unimi.it
G. V. Caprara
P. Steca Professor of Personality Psychology at the University of Rome
Assistant Professor at the University of Milan “Bicocca”, Italy. “La Sapienza”, Rome, Italy. His major research interests include
She received her Ph.D. in 2004 from the University of Padua. Her personality development and personality assessment along the life
main interests focus on the study of subjective and psychological span. He is supervisor in longitudinal projects on psychosocial
well-being across the life span, as well as on methodological adjustment from childhood to young adulthood
aspects of longitudinal research

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

ered appropriate steps to foster students’ motivation and jective achievement (Lent et al. 1993; Pajares and Miller
attainment (Ryan et al. 1992). Although this perspective 1997).
still has some applications in early infancy, it proved to Academic self-efficacy beliefs shape students’ school and
be ineffective as children grow up and external rewards or career aspirations (Zimmerman et al. 1992; Zimmerman and
punishments lose their motivational power. In a recent lit- Bandura 1994; Bandura et al. 2001). They not only pro-
erature review, Eccles and Wigfield (2002) have shown that mote students’ ability in setting and pursuing academic goals
current motivational theories stress the active role of the (Zimmerman et al. 1992; Schunk 2003), but they also pro-
individual in regulating learning activities. Some theories vide profound gratification and satisfaction with their ac-
mainly focus on expectancy about success or failure, and complishment (Zimmerman 1995; Pajares 1997).
beliefs about one’s ability and performance. According to Among the approaches focused on task value, we took
these theories, crucial elements in motivating behavior are into consideration optimal experience which represents the
self-efficacy beliefs and personal control (Zimmerman 1995; central construct of the theory of psychological selection
Bandura 1997; Boekaerts et al. 2000). Other theories are pri- (Csikszentmihalyi and Massimini 1985; Massimini and
marily centered on task value, or the reasons individuals have Delle Fave 2000). This theory explores the long-term devel-
for engaging in different achievement tasks. Key elements opmental impact of the experience that individuals associate
are intrinsic rewards, interest, and quality of experience with daily activities. Within this framework, the perceived
(Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984; Csikszentmihalyi quality of experience is related to the subjective evaluation
and Schneider 2000; Ryan and Deci 2000; Hidi and of the environmental opportunities for action (challenges)
Harackiewicz 2001). and the personal capabilities (skills) in facing them. When
Within the first set of theories, we will focus on individuals perceive both high challenges and high skills
Bandura’s construct of perceived self-efficacy (1986 2001), in a given activity (Massimini et al. 1987), optimal expe-
while within the second set we will take into considera- rience or flow is likely to arise. Optimal experience is a
tion the construct of optimal experience (Csikszentmihalyi state of high concentration, involvement, control of the sit-
1975 1990; Massimini and Delle Fave 2000). Both have uation, clear goals and feedback, satisfaction and intrinsic
been applied to the developmental and educational domains, reward (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi 1988). Be-
providing relevant research findings. cause it is an extremely gratifying experience, associated ac-
Bandura’s social-cognitive theory outlines the central role tivities tend to be preferentially selected and replicated over
of people’s self-efficacy beliefs, namely their perceived ca- time. This process has been defined psychological selection
pabilities to exercise control over their level of functioning (Csikszentmihalyi and Massimini 1985): It plays a crucial
and environmental demands. These beliefs influence cogni- role in individual growth, as it directs a person’s life theme,
tive, motivational, affective, and decisional processes. They namely the activities, interests, and goals selectively culti-
further extend their influence over aspirations and strength vated in life (Csikszentmihalyi and Beattie 1979). The cog-
of goal commitment, level of motivation and perseverance in nitive, motivational and affective profile of optimal experi-
the face of difficulties, causal attribution for successes and ence has been widely confirmed across countries, and among
failures, perception of environmental challenges and imped- activities ranging from work (Delle Fave and Massimini
iments (Bandura 1997). 2003) to leisure (Jackson and Csikszentmihalyi 1999; Delle
In academic settings, students’ perceived self-efficacy af- Fave et al. 2003), and child rearing (Delle Fave and
fects their academic interest and motivation, management Massimini 2004).
of academic stressors, growth of cognitive competencies In the academic field, learning activities such as school
and accomplished achievement (Zimmerman 1995; Bandura work and homework are mainly characterized by high en-
et al. 1996; Bandura 1997; Pajares 1997). Various em- gagement and attention, but extremely low intrinsic motiva-
pirical findings showed that self-efficacy beliefs strongly tion and mood (Wong and Csikszentmihalyi 1991; Hektner
mediate the effect of individuals’ skills and other self- 1996; Delle Fave and Bassi 2000). Homework is more fre-
beliefs on subsequent performance and accomplishment, by quently associated with optimal experience, and school work
influencing effort and persistence in the face of failures with apathy, an experience characterized by below average
(Zimmerman and Bandura 1994; Zimmerman 1995; challenges and skills (Delle Fave 1996). This discrepancy
Bandura 1997). At any level of ability, students with high may be related to the different degree of autonomy and
perceived self-efficacy are more successful in school activ- self-regulation offered by the two activities (Hektner 2001;
ities and use more effective learning strategies (Bouffard- Bassi and Delle Fave 2004). The association of learning ac-
Bouchard et al. 1991; Zimmerman 1995; Schunk et al. 2000; tivities with optimal experience has both short-term conse-
Zhang and Zhang 2003). Furthermore, self-efficacy beliefs quences in terms of intrinsic reward, and far reaching impli-
in managing specific subject matters are more predictive cations in promoting longitudinal coherence in the amount of
of the interests and subsequent academic choices than ob- time devoted to study (Hektner 1996), in shaping individual

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

long-term goals (Asakawa and Csikszentmihalyi 1998; Delle self-efficacy students would spend more time in homework
Fave and Massimini 2005), and in predicting the level of aca- than low self-efficacy ones. We secondly analyzed the
demic career students are willing to pursue (Nakamura 1988; quality of experience of learning activities through the
Wong and Csikszentmihalyi 1991; Hektner 1996). experience fluctuation model (Massimini et al. 1987). Based
Reported literature on self-efficacy and optimal expe- on participants’ levels of perceived challenges and skills,
rience clearly shows similarities in approaching learning the model identifies specific experiential profiles, such as
behavior, assigning human agency a pivotal role in deal- optimal experience, relaxation, apathy, and anxiety. We
ing with environmental challenges. Joining perspectives, a expected the two groups of students to perceive different
number of motivational theories has combined cognitive levels of challenges in the learning activities and to report
and experiential constructs in a general model which can different experiential profiles. In general, we hypothe-
predict individual learning behavior (Eccles and Wigfield sized that high self-efficacy students would preferentially
2002). Besides specific characteristics, these theories suggest associate all learning tasks with optimal experience. In
that self-efficacy beliefs may influence behavior through the particular, we expected them to find optimal experiences in
mediating effect of associated interest and quality of expe- class work and homework, activities that could be perceived
rience (Eccles et al. 1998; Rheinberg et al. 2000). In spite as highly challenging and matching high personal skills.
of the substantial literature and the elaborate models on the Due to their high personal skills, high self-efficacy students
topic, to date no empirical study has investigated the in- should rarely perceive anxiety in academic activities, and
terplay between daily experience and self-efficacy beliefs in report relatively high shares of relaxation and apathy when
learning activities. The present study is thus an attempt in this confronted with low-challenging learning tasks. On the
direction, proposing a basically exploratory and descriptive opposite, we expected low self-efficacy students to perceive
analysis of the two constructs in the academic setting. very few opportunities for optimal experience in academic
tasks. Rather, they would have a prevailing experience of
The present study anxiety in all learning activities, particularly during class
work, and few occasions of relaxation.
For our purposes, two groups of Italian secondary-school Finally, we analyzed the affective, motivational and cog-
students were selected on the basis of their high and nitive components of the experience associated with learning
low perceived academic self-efficacy. We assessed their tasks, both (a) on average, and (b) when students perceived
past academic self-efficacy beliefs and achievement during high challenges and high skills in the activities, the condi-
junior high school, as well as their present valuation of aca- tion associated with optimal experience. Our first aim was to
demic pursuits and academic aspirations. These measure- identify similarities and differences between the two groups
ments were primarily performed in order to confirm the in the average experience associated with learning tasks and
identification of the two groups. In line with previous works in optimal experience. As presented above, learning activ-
(Bandura et al. 1996), we expected to find an early formation ities are generally characterized by high engagement and
of academic self-efficacy beliefs, and an early differentiation attention but low intrinsic motivation and mood (Wong and
among students with high and low self-efficacy beliefs, re- Csikszentmihalyi 1991; Hektner 1996; Delle Fave and Bassi
flecting differences in their academic achievement. We fur- 2000). We thus expected to replicate previous findings on
ther expected high self-efficacy students to set higher aca- school work and homework, for both groups. We did not
demic attainments and to aspire to higher educational levels formulate any specific expectation about class work as no
than low self-efficacy students. previous ESM study has been performed on this topic.
Through experience sampling method (ESM) Secondly, in line with the theory of psychological selec-
(Csikszentmihalyi et al. 1977), on-line repeated infor- tion, we expected that experience associated with high per-
mation was collected on the students’ daily activities, ceived challenges and high skills would be significantly more
associated quality of experience, and the levels of challenges positive than the average experience. The theory namely
and skills perceived in the situation. We expected to find attributes to this improvement a crucial role in promoting
differences between the groups characterized respectively students’ motivation and enjoyment in learning.
by high and low academic self-efficacy beliefs. First, we
analyzed students’ day-to-day time allocation, or time
budget, paying specific attention to learning activities, Method
namely school work (attending lessons), class work (tests
and exams), and homework (studying at home). As we Participants
expected that students would spend more time in activities
associated with positive experiences, we looked for differ- The participants in this study were 130 adolescents, 55 males
ences in self-regulated time out of school. In particular, high and 75 females, ranging in age from 15 to 19 years, with a

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

mean age of 17.25 years. They were all secondary school attainments, ranging from “not important” to “extremely im-
students. portant.” They reported the educational levels they expected
Data for the present study were collected in 2000 as part to complete on a 6-point scale, ranging from spending few
of an ongoing Italian longitudinal research that adopted a years in the high school they were enrolled in to gradu-
staggered, multiple cohorts design. Participants belonging ation from college. Students’ academic achievement was
to three cohorts were assessed on an annual basis during appraised by teachers who expressed their evaluations in the
elementary and junior high school, and on a two-year basis various subject matters using five levels of academic attain-
during adolescence. They varied widely in socioeconomic ments: insufficient, sufficient, good, very good, excellent.
background: They came from families of skilled workers, Daily time budget and associated quality of experience
farmers, professionals, and local merchants, as well as their were investigated through the experience sampling method
service staffs. The socioeconomic diversity of the sample (ESM) (Csikszentmihalyi et al. 1977). For one week, each
adds to the generalizability of the findings. The overall re- participant was given a digital diary and a booklet of expe-
search aim is to identify the psychosocial factors having a rience sampling forms (ESFs). Diaries were programmed to
major influence on the transition from childhood to adoles- send random acoustic signals 6–8 times a day from 8.00 am
cence and to adulthood. to 10.00 pm. When beeped, participants were asked to fill
The community adhered to a stringent consent procedure out a form, containing a standard set of open-ended ques-
for research. Proposals had to gain approval from a school tions and Likert-type 0–12 scales. The open-ended ques-
council, parents had to give their consent, and participants tions investigated thoughts, activities, locations, and social
were free to decline participation. The study was described to context, as well as short- and long-term goals of the activ-
school staff, parents and their children as a project designed ities: for example, when beeped “what were you doing?.”
to gain better understanding of adolescents’ development. Likert-type 0–12 scales measured, from ‘not at all’ to ‘to the
In 2000, adolescents were contacted via phone by a maximum,’ the affective, cognitive, and motivational com-
researcher who gave explanations on the research which ponents of experience, along with perceived challenges and
comprised the administration of the usual batteries of tests skills.
and ESM. They were asked to participate in the study, and to During scheduled sessions in a local school, participants
sign written consent. After testing, they received remunera- were asked to fill out a set of inventories including the aca-
tion of around 30 dollars for their participation. demic self-efficacy scale, and the valuation of academic
pursuits and aspirations. In this occasion, they were also
Instruments and procedure briefed about the use of ESM. After one week, the partic-
ipants handed in their ESM diaries and booklets, and were
Participants filled out the academic self-efficacy scale debriefed before getting remuneration.
(Bandura et al. 1996; Pastorelli et al. 2001) referring to
two broad domains of self-efficacy beliefs. The first domain Data analysis
concerned the perceived capability to successfully master
different curricular areas. The second domain referred to the In a preliminary phase of the study, we calculated a total
perceived capacity for self-regulating learning activities, in- score of participants’ academic self-efficacy beliefs, which
cluding the ability to structure environments conducive to corresponded to the mean value of the scores reported on
learning; to plan and organize studying times and activities; each of the 17 items of the scale. Academic self-efficacy
to use cognitive devices to enhance understanding and mem- beliefs total score ranged from 1.69 to 4.81 in the whole
ory of the material being taught; to seek out information and sample, with a mean value of 3.91 (SD = 0.61). Cronbach
get teachers and peers to help them with academic problems alpha of the scale was 0.87.
when needed; to motivate themselves to do their school work In order to identify participants with very high and very
and assignments within deadlines; and to pursue academic low levels of academic self-efficacy beliefs, we analyzed
activities when there are other interesting things to do. For the percentile distribution of self-efficacy beliefs total score.
example, the item “How well can you get teachers to help We selected participants falling below the 25th percentile of
you when you get stuck on schoolwork” measures students’ the distribution and participants falling above the 75th per-
perceived self-efficacy to enlist social resources. Participants centile. Thirty-two participants, 20 males and 12 females,
rated the strength of their beliefs on a 5-point response for- fell into the “low self-efficacy group” (LSE group), whereas
mat ranging from 1 (perceived incapability) to 5 (complete 31 participants, 10 males and 21 females, belonged to the
self-assurance in one’s capability). “high self-efficacy group” (HSE group). The mean value
Valuation of academic pursuits and academic aspirations of academic self-efficacy beliefs was 3.09 (SD = 0.51)
were also assessed (Bandura et al. 1996 2001). Adolescents in the first group, and 4.59 (SD = 0.14) in the second
rated on a 5-point scale the importance placed on academic group. The difference between the scores was significant,

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

(t(61) = −15.95 p < 0.001). As regards gender, the low activity, satisfaction, and goals perceived in the activity.
self-efficacy group was comprised more of males than fe- These variables belong to the validated ESM standard sheet
males, and the high self-efficacy group was comprised more (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1987), and their labels have
of females than males (χ 2 (2) = 5.77 p = 0.02). This com- been widely applied in ESM research (Csikszentmihalyi
position reflects previous studies on academic self-efficacy, and Larson 1984; Delle Fave and Bassi 2000). We adopted
with girls scoring higher than boys during both childhood a subject-level approach to the analysis of repeated ESM
and adolescence (Bandura et al. 2001; Caprara et al. 2002). scores, in which the individual is the unit of data organi-
Within groups, however, no significant differences in aca- zation (Larson and Delespaul 1992). This kind of analysis
demic self-efficacy level were detected between females and is conservative in that the assumption of independence is
males. For this reason and considering the general purposes not violated. Z-scores for each item were obtained for each
of our analysis, we did not take gender into account in data participant based on the item’s weekly mean. Aggregated
analysis. values (mean z-scores) for the low and high self-efficacy
In order to identify differences in the valuation of aca- groups were then calculated on the number of participants
demic pursuits and academic aspirations, we compared included in each group (Larson and Delespaul 1992).
groups using t tests. T tests were also used to identify T tests were performed in order to assess whether mean
between-group differences in past evaluations of academic z-scores significantly differed from zero within each group.
self-efficacy beliefs and school achievement. In particular, As we performed a large number of t tests on the same data
we examined data collected during the first, second and third set, we corrected significance levels by means of a Bonfer-
year of junior high school. Students’ past perceived aca- roni approach, which consisted in dividing the α level of
demic self-efficacy was assessed by the scale described above significance by the number of t tests performed (Delle Fave
(Bandura et al. 1996). Reliability scores were similar to the et al. 2003; Bassi and Delle Fave 2004). Therefore, adjusted
one obtained in 2000, with Cronbach alpha = 0.85 for the p values were obtained by multiplying observed p values by
first year of junior high school, Cronbach alpha = 0.88 for the number of t tests performed. T tests were also carried out
the second year, and Cronbach alpha = 0.87 for the third to compare mean z-scores between groups.
year. Finally, academic achievement was appraised aggre- Both the subject-level approach and the Bonferroni α cor-
gating teachers’ evaluations in the various subject matters rection represent rigorous methods in data handling and sta-
at the end of each school year. The number of participants tistical analysis. They protect against Type I errors, or the
in the comparisons slightly varies because of missing cases probability to wrongly reject the null hypothesis. In so do-
in the examined variables. ing, however, they also increase the risk of Type II errors,
As regards data collected by means of ESM, ESFs that or the probability to erroneously reject the alternative hy-
were completed more than 20 min after signal receipt were pothesis. Considering the gravity of Type I errors and the
discarded from analysis in order to avoid distortions due to exploratory purposes of our study, we opted for a cautious
retrospective recall (Larson and Delespaul 1992). For this approach to statistical errors and decided to primarily control
purpose, ESFs contain two questions about the time when for Type I errors.
the participants were beeped and the time when they filled out
the form. The final data set included 2,403 valid self-reports, The experience fluctuation model
1,195 for low self-efficacy students (average N reports =
37.3) and 1,208 for high self-efficacy students (average N The quality of students’ daily experience was further ana-
reports = 39). lyzed in relation to perceived challenges and skills through
In line with ESM literature (Csikszentmihalyi 1997a; the experience fluctuation model (EFM) (Massimini et al.
Delle Fave and Bassi 2000; Delle Fave et al. 2003), answers 1987). The Cartesian plane was partitioned into eight sectors
to open-ended questions were assigned a numeric code and of 45◦ , called channels. Partitioning was based on specific
grouped into larger functional categories. In this study, we ranges of challenges/skills ratios. Values of challenges and
focused on students’ daily activities, which were broadly skills were standardized for each participant, and their mean
aggregated into productive activities, leisure, and mainte- value—as well as the mean of all participants’ mean—was
nance (Csikszentmihalyi 1997a). In particular, specific at- equal to zero and corresponded to the center of the model,
tention was devoted to the investigation of learning tasks as called subjective mean (Fig. 1).
they were the core of our analysis. For each group, activ- A great number of studies has shown that each channel
ity frequencies were calculated as mean percentages of each identifies a specific experiential profile which is consistent
participant’s activity distribution. across age, gender, culture, and life domains (Haworth and
As regards scaled variables, we focused on the follow- Evans 1995; Csikszentmihalyi 1990 1997b; Delle Fave
ing cognitive, motivational and affective variables: concen- and Bassi 2000 2003; Delle Fave and Massimini 2004
tration, control, happiness, involvement, wish doing the 2005). In particular, when perceived challenges and skills

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Fig. 1 The experience


fluctuation model
(SM = subjective mean)

are balanced above subjective mean (channel 2), optimal Academic self-efficacy beliefs and achievement during
experience is reported. On the contrary, when they are junior high school
balanced below average (channel 6), apathy emerges. The
mismatch between high challenges and low skills (channel 8) As shown in Fig. 2, the two groups also differed in their
is associated with anxiety, and the mismatch between low past evaluations of academic self-efficacy beliefs, as well
challenges and high skills (channel 4) with relaxation. as in the objective achievement assessed by their teachers
The remaining challenges/skills ratios correspond to the during the first, second and third year of junior high school.
so-called transition channels (Delle Fave 1996) as they More specifically, high self-efficacy students reported higher
identify intermediate experiential states, namely arousal beliefs in their academic efficacy in the first (t(59) = −3.64,
(channel 1), control (channel 3), boredom (channel 5), and p < 0.01), second (t(61) = −4.06, p < 0.001) and third
worry (channel 7) (Csikszentmihalyi 1997b). (t(61) = −5.14, p < 0.001) year of junior high school.
In the present study, we focused on the distribution of Across the three grades, adolescents with high self-
learning activities in the major channels of the model: op- efficacy beliefs also obtained higher teachers’ evaluations
timal experience (channel 2), relaxation (channel 4), apathy of their academic achievement: t(50) = −3.84, p < 0.001,
(channel 6), and anxiety (channel 8). Finally, we investigated t(60) = −3.03, p < 0.01, and t(61) = −4.03, p < 0.001,
the quality of experience that the two groups associated with respectively for the first, second and third year of junior high
learning activities, on average and in channel 2 (optimal school.
experience). Between-group and within-group t-test com-
parisons were carried out in order to highlight significant Daily life activities
differences.
Table 1 illustrates the distribution of students’ activities.
Results Mean percentages were calculated on the basis of each
student’s activity distribution. Five activity categories were
Valuation of academic pursuits and academic aspirations identified: learning tasks, leisure, interactions, maintenance,
and ‘miscellaneous.’
T tests were performed to compare the mean scores of valua- For both groups, learning tasks comprised school work
tion of academic pursuits and academic aspirations reported such as attending lessons, listening to the teacher, and taking
by the two groups. High self-efficacy students placed sig- notes; class work, including oral and written tests; ‘other
nificantly more importance on academic attainments (M = school activities’ such as doing recess and talking with
4.39) and aspired to a higher educational level (M = 5.77) schoolmates during lessons; and homework, such as studying
than low self-efficacy students (M = 3.55 and M = 3.75, at home. Leisure included activities such as watching televi-
respectively). T-test values were t(61) = −3.44, p < 0.01 sion, hobbies and sports, reading magazines and books, lis-
for the first comparison, and t(60) = −6.99, p < 0.001 for tening to music, thinking of various topics and ‘other leisure
the second comparison. activities’ such as having a stroll in town and whistling.

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Fig. 2 Mean values of academic self-efficacy beliefs (ACSE) and achievement (ACH) for high and low self-efficacy students: evaluations during
the first (Y1), second (Y2) and third (Y3) year of junior high school

Maintenance included activities such as grooming, hygiene, Between-group t-test comparisons performed on activity
eating, and sleeping; interactions comprised talking, talking mean percentages showed that high self-efficacy students
on the phone, sending sms messages. Answers referring to spent more time in learning tasks (t(61) = 2.1, p < 0.041)
transport, chores, as well as activities such as waiting and than low self-efficacy students, while low self-efficacy stu-
observing the environment were aggregated in the category dents spent more time in maintenance (t(61) = 3.5, p <
‘miscellaneous.’ 0.001) than high self-efficacy students. In particular, as re-
Both groups spent most of their time in learning tasks, gards learning, the difference was attributed to the differen-
followed by leisure. For the low self-efficacy students, main- tial time investment in homework (t(48) = 2.77, p < 0.008),
tenance was third in frequency, followed by interactions. with high self-efficacy participants studying about twice as
The opposite ranking was reported by the high self-efficacy much as low self-efficacy participants.
students.
Distribution of learning tasks in the channels
Table 1 Mean percentage distribution of activities of the experience fluctuation model
Activities LSE Na Mean % HSE Na Mean %
Table 2 illustrates the distribution of learning tasks in the ma-
Learning tasks 32 30.6 (11.1) 31 37.1 (13.3) jor channels of the EFM. Some activities—especially class
School work 32 19.8 (9) 31 19.4 (8.7) work—were reported by few students, and thus mean per-
Class work 15 4.2 (2.4) 14 4.4 (2.3) centages could have been inflated. Therefore, for each activ-
Other school 20 5.6 (3.3) 22 6.7 (4.8))
ity in each group, percentages were calculated on the number
Home work 23 7.4 (5.3) 27 12.5 (7.5)
Leisure 32 29.6 (12.1) 31 26 (11.6) of self-reports and not on the number of participants.
Interactions 30 14.9 (9.2) 29 16.4 (8.8) High self-efficacy students mostly associated all learning
Maintenance 32 17.8 (7.3) 31 12.3 (5.1) tasks with optimal experience. Relatively high frequencies
Miscellaneous 29 8.9 (6.4) 27 10.6 (6.3) of relaxation and apathy were also reported in school work,
whereas anxiety was frequently reported during class work,
Note. LSE refers to low self-efficacy group; HSE refers to high
self-efficacy group. Mean percentages were calculated on participants’ and apathy with homework. Low self-efficacy students pri-
average distribution of activities. Standard deviations are reported marily associated class work with anxiety, and homework
within parentheses. with optimal experience and relaxation with the same per-
a
N participants. centage. They did not report any predominant experiential
profile during school work.

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Table 2 Percentage of youths’ time spent on learning activities across the four channels

School work Class work Home work


Channels LSE % (N = 230)a HSE % (N = 234)a LSE % (N = 23)a HSE % (N = 25)a LSE % (N = 66)a HSE % (N = 132)a

2—Optimal exp. 13.5 19.2 – 28 13.6 25


4—Relaxation 9.1 14.5 4.4 4 13.6 6.8
6—Apathy 14.4 14.1 13.0 8 9.1 15.9
8—Anxiety 13.5 9 26.1 16 10.6 9.1

Note. LSE refers to low self-efficacy group; HSE refers to high self-efficacy group. The dash indicates that LSE students never associated class
work with optimal experience.
a
N self-reports.

Quality of experience associated with learning tasks p < 0.007). A significant difference was detected between
the two groups for concentration (t(61) = 2.57, p < 0.013).
We finally investigated the quality of experience associated Class work was associated with below average satisfaction
with school work, class work and homework, both on av- (t(14) = 4.03, p < 0.042) by low self-efficacy students. A
erage and in channel 2 of the EFM, which corresponds to significant between-group difference was detected for this
optimal experience (challenges and skills values above sub- variable (t(27) = 3.08, p < 0.005). Finally, both groups as-
jective mean). Table 3 reports the values of the variables sociated homework with below average values of wish doing
concentration, control, happiness, involvement, wish doing the activity (LSE students: t(22) = 5.33, p < 0.004; HSE
the activity, satisfaction, and goals in the two groups of stu- students: t(26) = 4.88, p < 0.005). High self-efficacy stu-
dents. T tests were performed for each group in order to dents also reported significantly above-average concentra-
assess score differences from the mean (zero). Due to the tion (t(26) = 4.92, p < 0.005) and goals (t(26) = 6.45, p <
large number of tests performed on the same data set, signif- 0.005). T-test comparisons highlighted significant between-
icance values were corrected using the Bonferroni approach group differences for concentration (t(48) = 2.09, p < 0.05),
as described above. T tests were also performed to compare control (t(48) = 2.57, p < 0.02), and satisfaction (t(48) =
scores between groups. In this case, the Bonferroni correc- 3.40, p < 0.002).
tion was not required because of the small number of tests In channel 2 (optimal experience), high self-efficacy stu-
carried out on the same data set. dents reported significantly high satisfaction during school
In both groups, the average quality of experience asso- work (t(20) = 3.80, p < 0.047), and concentration and con-
ciated with school work was characterized by significantly trol during homework (t(16) = 5.51, p < 0.005, and t(16)
negative values of wish doing the activity (LSE students: = 4.00, p < 0.043, respectively). As regards homework, sig-
t(31) = 6.80, p < 0.004; HSE students: t(30) = 4.51, nificant between-group comparisons were detected for wish
p < 0.005), and by a significantly negative value of hap- doing the activity (t(20) = 2.56, p < 0.019), and satisfaction
piness for the low self-efficacy students (t(31) = 4.24, (t(20) = 2.49, p < 0.022).

Table 3 Quality of experience in learning activities on average and in channel 2

School work Class work Home work


LSE HSE LSE HSE LSE HSE
Average Ch. 2 Average Ch. 2 Average Ch. 2 Average Ch. 2 Average Ch. 2 Average Ch. 2
Variables (32)a (16)a (31)a (21)a (15)a (0)a (14)a (7)a (23)a (5)a (27)a (17)a

Concentration 0.17 0.45 − 0.17 0.45 0.28 0.70 0.96 0.17 0.36 0.57∗∗∗ 0.94∗∗∗
Control − 0.04 0.35 − 0.19 0.54 − 0.22 0.22 0.68 − 0.26 0.01 0.16 0.72∗
Happiness − 0.28∗∗ 0.09 − 0.18 0.47 − 0.34 − 0.17 0.03 − 0.44 0.22 − 0.23 − 0.06
Involvement − 0.05 0.42 − 0.08 0.42 0.46 0.44 0.54 − 0.13 0.15 0.07 0.38
Wish doing activity − 0.43∗∗∗ − 0.16 − 0.39∗∗∗ 0.01 − 0.57 − 0.11 − 0.05 − 0.85∗∗∗ − 1.13 − 0.59∗∗∗ − 0.00
Satisfaction − 0.10 0.11 − 0.03 0.59∗ − 0.90∗ 0.07 0.45 − 0.35 − 0.41 0.14 0.38
Goals 0.35 0.33 0.37 0.82 0.49 0.78 0.92 0.54 0.26 0.83∗∗∗ 0.75

Note. LSE refers to low self-efficacy group; HSE refers to high self-efficacy group.
a
N participants. On the basis of the Bonferroni approach, α levels must be divided by the number of t tests performed. Therefore to obtain adjusted
p values, observed p values were multiplied by the number of t tests performed. For the low self-efficacy group, N t tests = 35, thus ∗ p < 0.05
(where 0.05 = 0.0014 × 35); ∗∗ p < 0.01 (where 0.01 = 0.00028 × 35); ∗∗∗ p < 0.005 (where 0.005 = 0.00014 × 35). For the high self-efficacy
group, N t tests = 42, thus ∗ p < 0.05 (where 0.05 = 0.00119 × 42); ∗∗∗ p < 0.005 (where 0.005 = 0.000119 × 42).

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Compared with the average learning experience, both Activities perceived as opportunities for optimal experience
groups reported a general increase in the values of most are likely to be pursued and cultivated for their own sake.
variables in optimal experience (channel 2). Paired t-tests Through ESM on-line sampling, we explored students’
were performed within each group on the participants who time budget. As hypothesized, crucial differences in time al-
reported a given activity both on average and in chan- lotment were identified. Low self-efficacy participants spent
nel 2. Significant differences were detected only among significantly less time doing their homework. Most likely,
high self-efficacy students, in school work for the vari- when free to choose what to do with their self-regulated time
ables concentration (t(20) = 3.12, p < 0.006), control out of compulsory school hours, low self-efficacy students
(t(20) = 3.27, p < 0.004), happiness (t(20) = 4.38, eschewed studying and exam preparation to the advantage
p < 0.0003), involvement (t(20) = 3.15, p < 0.006), of other activities. In particular, they were significantly more
and satisfaction (t(20) = 4.22, p < 0.0004); and in often involved in relaxing and low-challenging maintenance
homework, for the variables concentration (t(16) = 2.74, activities–such as eating, resting and grooming–than high
p < 0.015), control (t(16) = 5.05, p < 0.0001), involve- self-efficacy students.
ment (t(16) = 2.17, p < 0.05), and wish doing the activity We next analyzed the distribution of learning activities in
(t(16) = 4.29, p < 0.0006). the major channels of the EFM. In line with our hypothesis,
high self-efficacy students mostly associated school work,
class work, and homework with optimal experience. In par-
Discussion ticular, they seemed to be well equipped to deal with ex-
tremely challenging situations, such as class work and ex-
The present study allowed us to shed light on the long-term aminations. They also successfully invested their skills in
and day-to-day meaning students attach to learning tasks dealing with the odds of externally-regulated activities, such
from the subjective agentic perspective shared by both as school work. School work, however, was associated with
perceived self-efficacy and optimal experience constructs. relaxation and apathy as well. This can be related to the
Past self-efficacy beliefs and academic achievement strongly fact that students are not offered personalized educational
confirmed the identification of two groups. Students with projects: As the levels of challenges are generally set as a
high self-efficacy beliefs reported feeling more efficacious global standard by teachers, high self-efficacy students may
than students with low self-efficacy beliefs in managing find them not challenging enough considering their personal
academic activities during the first, second and third year skills. During homework, these students retrieved a high per-
of junior high school. Furthermore, across the three grades, centage of optimal experiences, but also reported a relatively
high self-efficacy adolescents had received better teachers’ high percentage of apathy when confronted with perceived
evaluations and reported higher grades at the end of the low challenges. Contrary to high self-efficacy students, low
year. These findings confirm the strong link between self-efficacy students did not perceive a great amount of op-
students’ self-efficacy beliefs and their actual performance portunities for optimal experience in learning tasks. During
and attainments (Zimmerman 1995; Bandura 1997; Pajares schoolwork, no prevailing experiential profile was identi-
1997). Results do not allow us to make any inference about fied. This group primarily associated class work with anx-
times and processes of the building and consolidation of iety. Tests and exams very often exceeded personal skills,
children’s beliefs in their capacity to successfully manage and were never occasions for optimal experience. During
learning activities. However, they point to an early distinc- class work, these students also reported a relatively high
tion between children belonging to the lower extreme and frequency of apathy, showing cognitive, affective and mo-
those belonging to the upper extreme of the self-efficacy tivational disengagement from activities they perceived as
distribution. The identification of the two groups was unchallenging and imposed upon. As concerns homework,
further confirmed by the analysis of academic pursuits they reported equal percentages of optimal experience and
and aspirations: High self-efficacy students attributed more relaxation. In particular, when confronted with perceived low
importance to academic attainments and aspired to higher challenges, low self-efficacy students more frequently expe-
educational levels than low self-efficacy students. rienced relaxation rather than apathy. Data seem to hint that
In line with Bandura (1997), we hypothesized that low self-efficacy students preferentially associated learning
participants, according to their levels of perceived academic activities with globally positive experiences as long as they
self-efficacy, would preferentially devote their time to could stringently select them during their studying time. In
activities they felt competent in. We also added that, so doing, they greatly restricted homework time, and man-
according to the theory of psychological selection, the aged to avoid consistent shares of apathy compared with high
quality of experience associated with daily activities plays a self-efficacy students. However, they also got fewer chances
crucial, motivating role in performing them (Hektner 1996; to build on their skills and extend optimal experiences to
Massimini and Delle Fave 2000; Rheinberg et al. 2000): other more challenging learning domains, such as classwork.

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Finally, we investigated the quality of experience associ- even if, as typical of teenagers (Bassi and Delle Fave 2004),
ated with learning tasks, on average and in channel 2. The the variable wish to be doing the activity did not obtain
average experiential profiles associated with school work a significantly positive score. Thus, building on personal
and homework was similar to those found in previous works skills and calibrating challenges to individual capabilities
(Wong and Csikszentmihalyi 1991; Hektner 1996; Delle may not only prepare students for entering the job mar-
Fave and Bassi 2000). The experience of school work was ket in the future (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider 2000), but
globally negative and dissatisfying, with both groups signif- also provide opportunities for short- and long-term gratifying
icantly wishing to be doing a different activity. Low self- experiences.
efficacy students, however, perceived themselves as signifi-
cantly more concentrated than high self-efficacy students. Joining perspectives on learning
Since exams and tests are not scheduled on a daily ba-
sis in Italian schools, class work was reported by a small Results highlighted significant differences in the time budget
number of participants. Generally, it was characterized by and associated quality of experience between high and low
high concentration, involvement, and perceived relevance self-efficacy students, and also brought forward implications
for students’ goals. In these activities, low self-efficacy for practice. The two groups of students clearly diverged in
students reported significantly lower satisfaction than high their investment in learning activities not only in terms of
self-efficacy students. Finally, homework was associated on time and effort, but also in terms of enjoyment and gratifi-
average with significantly low values of wish doing the ac- cation. In particular, findings suggest that the more students
tivity by both groups. Homework usually takes place during feel competent in an activity and enjoy it, the more fre-
free time, when adolescents can engage in leisure and so- quently they perform it. On the one hand, these data stress
cializing activities instead. Nevertheless, high self-efficacy the crucial role of academic self-efficacy in motivating stu-
students reported significantly positive values of concentra- dents to pursue academic goals in the long and short terms.
tion and goals, thus perceiving the long-term importance of On the other hand, they underline the importance of the
studying not only to pass exams or the year, but also to quality of experience as a mediating element in the moti-
have access to university and/or a good job in the future. vational process (Rheinberg et al. 2000). Thus, considering
Moreover, these participants reported significantly higher the amount of apathy and relaxation reported by both high
levels of concentration during homework than low self- and low self-efficacy students at school, the assessment of
efficacy students, and also derived from it higher control and their academic self-efficacy as well as their quality of expe-
satisfaction. rience in learning activities can provide teachers with useful
In line with theoretical expectations and a number of information. Indeed, teachers play a crucial role in provid-
empirical findings (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi ing complex and at the same time enjoyable educational
1988; Massimini et al. 1987; Delle Fave 1996), differences challenges adequate to pupils’ skills (Bassi and Delle Fave
were identified between the experience reported on aver- 2004). To this regard, social-cognitive theory gives clear
age and in channel 2 (challenges and skills values above directions to intervention programs that aim to raise compe-
subjective mean). In channel 2, high self-efficacy students tence and confidence mainly through successful experiences
perceived significantly higher concentration, control, hap- of mastery in the various learning tasks (Bandura 1997). As
piness, involvement, and satisfaction during school work. academic self-efficacy beliefs seem to develop quite early
While studying at home, they also perceived significantly in students’ academic career, timely intervention is needed.
higher-than-average concentration, control, involvement and Consistently, the theory of psychological selection empha-
wish doing the activity, as well as significantly higher levels sizes the need for providing students with challenging op-
of satisfaction and wish doing the activity than low self- portunities for action and skill development in the school
efficacy students. In contrast, among low self-efficacy stu- environment. Optimal experiences can promote students’ en-
dents, no significant differences were detected between the joyment in performed activities, independently of the contin-
experience reported on average and in channel 2. This is gent marks (Delle Fave and Bassi 2000; Massimini and Delle
likely to be related to the small number of low self-efficacy Fave 2000). Finally, both theories identify self-regulation as
students retrieving optimal experience during learning ac- a powerful means for enhancing personal competence and
tivities. In particular, this is true of class work which they gratification.
never associated with optimal experience. However, results The present study was only an initial attempt to join the
from high self-efficacy students show that the association of two theoretical perspectives together. Our goal was to explore
learning activities with high challenges and high skills can the interplay between daily experience and self-efficacy be-
enhance related quality of experience. Under this condition, liefs in the learning domain. The use of on-line experience
students derive high cognitive investment, involvement, sat- sampling served this purpose, providing rich ecologically-
isfaction, and meaningful future goals from academic tasks valid data. However, it was also time consuming and rather

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

costly, thus bringing forward a number of limitations. The References


most serious one regards sample size. Given the conserva-
tive approach to data handling and statistical analysis we Alsaker F, Flammer A (eds) (1999) The adolescent experience.
adopted in this study, the relatively small sample size af- European and American adolescents in the 1990s. Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ
fected the statistical power in the analysis of the quality of
Asakawa K, Csikszentmihalyi M (1998) The quality of experience of
experience associated with learning activities. Future studies Asian American adolescents in activities related to future goals.
should rely on larger samples to augment statistical power. J Youth and Adolesc 27:141–163
However, when research hypotheses are extremely detailed, Bandura A (1986) Social foundations of thought and action: a social
cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewoods Cliffs, NJ
the subsamples suitable to test them become less numerous. Bandura A (1997) Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York,
In this kind of investigation, therefore, the sample size prob- Freeman
lem can only partly be overcome. In addition, the limited Bandura A (2001) Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu
sample size did not allow us to delve deeper into gender Rev Psychol 52:1–26
Bandura A, Barbaranelli C, Caprara GV, Pastorelli C (1996) Multi-
differences. Social-cognitive studies have reported that girls faceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs on academic functioning.
show higher perceived capabilities to manage learning tasks Child Dev. 67:1206–1222
and school activities, aspire to higher academic levels, and Bandura A, Barbaranelli C, Caprara GV, Pastorelli C (2001) Self-
set more ambitious achievement goals, than the males of the efficacy beliefs as shapers of children’s aspirations and career
trajectories. Child Dev 72:187–206
same age (Bandura et al. 2001; Caprara et al. 2002). These Bassi M, Delle Fave A (2004) Adolescence and the changing context
issues require further investigation. Stratification of high and of optimal experience in time: Italy 1986–2000. J Happiness Stud
low self-efficacy groups with equal gender numbers could be 5:155–179
an appropriate step. Boekaerts M, Pintrich PR, Zeidner MH (eds) (2000) Handbook of self-
regulation. Academic, San Diego, CA
In spite of these limitations, this study gathered detailed Bouffard-Bouchard T, Parent S, Larivée S (1991) Influence of self-
phenomenological information on the daily life and asso- efficacy on self-regulation and performance among junior and
ciated quality of experience of students with high and low senior high-school aged students. Int J Behav Dev 39:280–282
academic self-efficacy beliefs. Results opened up a series Caprara GV, Delle Fratte A, Steca P (2002) Determinanti personali
del benessere in adolescenza: Indicatori e predittori [Personal de-
of challenging questions and suggestions for more complex terminants of adolescents’ well-being: indicators and predictors].
data analyses. In line with the motivational theories dis- Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo 2:203–223
cussed in the introduction (Eccles et al. 1998; Rheinberg Csikszentmihalyi M (1975) Beyond boredom and anxiety. San
et al. 2000), we expected self-efficacy beliefs to direct be- Francisco, Jossey-Bass
Csikszentmihalyi M (1990) Flow. The psychology of optimal experi-
havior through the mediation of perceived quality of experi- ence. Harper and Row, New York
ence, and of optimal experience in particular. However, the Csikszentmihalyi M (1997a) Activity, experience and personal growth.
relation between the two constructs could be far more com- In: Curtis J, Russell S (eds) Physical activity in human experience:
plex. Optimal experience in learning activities could have interdisciplinary perspectives. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL,
pp 59–88
both synchronic and diachronic, cumulative consequences. Csikszentmihalyi M (1997b) Finding flow. The psychology of engage-
By providing intrinsic reward, optimal experience can sus- ment with everyday life. Basic Books, New York
tain long-term perseverance and effort in cultivating asso- Csikszentmihalyi M, Beattie O (1979) Life themes: a theoretical and
ciated activities. It could also represent a feedback to per- empirical exploration of their origins and effects. J Humanistic
Psychol 19:45–63
ceived self-efficacy: Direct experience of competence in high Csikszentmihalyi M, Csikszentmihalyi I (eds) (1988) Optimal
challenges/high skills situations could be cognitively elab- experience—psychological studies of flow in consciousness.
orated into rather stable self-efficacy beliefs. In their turn, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press
these beliefs could direct time and energy investment into Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R (1984) Being adolescent: conflict and
growth in the teenage years. Basic Books, New York
activities in which individuals perceive themselves as highly Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R (1987) Validity and reliability of the
competent vis à vis current challenges. This process would Experience Sampling Method. J Nerv Ment Dis 175:526–536
facilitate the retrieval of optimal experiences and the de- Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R, Prescott S (1977) The ecology of ado-
velopment of lasting high self-efficacy beliefs. In the long lescent activity and experience. J Youth and Adolesc 6:281–294
Csikszentmihalyi M, Massimini F (1985) On the psychological selec-
run, this process could go on in a virtuous circle, promot- tion of bio-cultural information. New Ideas in Psychol 3:115–
ing individual development, with respect to skill cultivation, 138
satisfaction, and goal setting. Further investigation is needed Csikszentmihalyi M, Schneider B (2000) Becoming adult: how
to test these hypothesized relations in order to shed light on teenagers prepare for the world of work. Basic Books, New York
Delle Fave A (1996) Esperienza ottimale e fluttuazioni dello stato di
adolescents’ motivational processes as well as offer guide- coscienza: risultati sperimentali [Optimal experience and fluctua-
lines for promoting enjoyment and engagement in the school tion in the state of consciousness]. In: Massimini F, Inghilleri P,
setting. Delle Fave A (eds) La Selezione Psicologica Umana—Teoria e
Metodo D’analisi. Cooperativa libraria I.U.L.M., Milano, pp 541–
568

Springer
J Youth Adolescence

Delle Fave A, Bassi M (2000) The quality of experience in adolescents’ Massimini F, Csikszentmihalyi M, Carli M (1987) The monitoring of
daily life: developmental perspectives. Genet Soc Gen Psychol optimal experience. A tool for psychiatric rehabilitation. J Nerv
Monogr 126:347–367 Ment Dis 175:545–549
Delle Fave A, Bassi M (2003) Italian adolescents and leisure: the role of Massimini F, Delle Fave A (2000) Individual development in a bio-
engagement and optimal experience. In: Verma S, Larson R (eds) cultural perspective. Am Psychol 55:24–33
Examining adolescent leisure time across cultures: developmental Nakamura J (1988) Optimal experience and the use of talent. In:
opportunities and risks. New directions in child and adolescent Csikszentmihalyi M, Csikszentmihalyi I (eds) Optimal
development. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 79–93 experience—psychological studies on flow in consciousness.
Delle Fave A, Bassi M, Massimini F (2003) Quality of experience Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 319–326
and risk perception in high-altitude rock climbing. J Appl Sport Pajares F (1997) Current directions in self efficacy research. In: Maeher
Psychol 15:82–99 ME, Pintrich PR (eds) Advances in motivation and achievement.
Delle Fave A, Massimini F (2003) Optimal experience in work and JAI Press, Greenwhich, CT, pp 1–49
leisure among teachers and physicians: Individual and bio-cultural Pajares F, Miller MD (1997) Mathematics self-efficacy and mathe-
implications. Leisure Stud 22:323–342 matical problem solving: implications of using different forms of
Delle Fave A, Massimini F (2004) Parenthood and the quality of ex- assessment. J Exp Educ 65:213–228
perience in daily life: a longitudinal study. Soc Indicators Res Pastorelli C, Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Rola J, Rozsa S, Bandura
67:75–106 A (2001) The structure of children’s perceived self efficacy: a
Delle Fave A, Massimini F (2005) The investigation of optimal expe- cross-national study. Eur J Psychol Assess 17:87–97
rience and apathy: developmental and psychosocial implications. Ranson S, Martin J (1996) Towards a theory of learning. Br J Educ
Eur Psychol 10:4 Stud 44:9–26
Eccles JS, Wigfield A (2002) Motivation beliefs, values, and goals. Rheinberg F, Vollmeyer R, Rollett W (2000) Motivation and action
Annu Rev Psychol 53:109–132 in self-regulated learning. In: Boekaerts M, Pintrich PR, Zeidner
Eccles JS, Wigfield A, Schiefele U (1998) Motivation to succeed. In: MH (eds) Handbook of self-regulation. Academic, San Diego, CA,
Damon W, Eisenberg N (eds) Handbook of child psychology, pp 503–529
Vol. 3. Wiley, New York, pp 1017–1095 Ryan R, Deci E (2000) Self-determination theory and the facilitation
Furstenberg FF, Cook TD, Eccles JS, Elder GH, Sameroff A (1998) of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am
Managing to make it: urban families and adolescent success. Uni- Psychol 55:68–78
versity of Chicago Press, Chicago Ryan R, Connell J, Grolnick W (1992) When achievement is not intrin-
Gasperoni G (2001) La scuola [The school]. In: Buzzi C, Cavalli A, De sically motivated: a theory of internalization and self-regulation
Lillo A (eds) Giovani Del Nuovo Secolo. Quinto Rapporto IARD in school. In: Boggiano A, Pittman T (eds) Achievement and mo-
Sulla Condizione Giovanile in Italia. Il Mulino, Bologna, Italy, tivation: a social-development perspective. Cambridge University
pp 20–24 Press, New York, pp 167–188
Haworth J, Evans S (1995) Challenge, skill and positive subjective Schunk DH (2003) Self-efficacy for reading and writing: influence of
states in the daily life of a sample of YTS students. J Occup Organ modeling, goal setting, and self-evaluation. Reading and Writing
Psychol 68:109–121 Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties 19:159–172
Hektner JM (1996) Exploring optimal personality development: a lon- Schunk DH, Ertmer PA, Boekaerts M (2000) Self-regulation and
gitudinal study of adolescents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, academic learning: self-efficacy enhancing interventions. In:
University of Chicago Boekaerts M, Pintrich PR, Zeidner M (eds) Handbook of self-
Hektner JM (2001) Family, school, and community predictors of ado- regulation. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 631–649
lescent growth conducive experiences: global and specific ap- Wong MM, Csikszentmihalyi M (1991) Motivation and academic
proaches. Appl Dev Sci 5:172–183 achievement: the effects of personality traits and the quality of
Jackson SA, Csikszentmihalyi M (1999) Flow in sports: the keys to experience. J Pers 59:539–574
optimal experiences and performances. Human Kinetics, Cham- Zhang L, Zhang X (2003) A study of the relationships among learning
paign, IL strategy use, self-efficacy, persistence and academic achievement
Hidi S, Harackiewicz JM (2001) Motivating the academically unmoti- in middle school students. Psychol Sci 26:603–607
vated: a critical issue for the 21st century. Rev Educ Res 70:151– Zimmerman BJ (1995) Attaining reciprocality between learning
180 and development through self-regulation. Human Dev 38:367–
Larson R, Delespaul PAEG (1992) Analysing experience sampling 372
data: a guidebook for the perplexed. In: deVries M (ed) The ex- Zimmerman BJ, Bandura A (1994) Impact of self-regulatory influ-
perience of psychopathology—investigating mental disorders in ences on writing course attainment. Am Educ Res J 31:845–
their natural settings. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 862
58–78 Zimmerman BJ, Bandura A, Martinez-Pons M (1992) Self-
Lent RW, Lopez FG, Bieschke KJ (1993) Predicting mathematics- motivation for academic attainment: the role of self-efficacy
related choice and success behaviors: test of an expanded social beliefs and personal goal setting. Am Educ Res J 29:663–
cognitive model. J Vocat Behav 42:223–236 676

Springer

View publication stats

You might also like