Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

In B. Gawronski, & B. K. Payne (Eds.). (2010).

Handbook of implicit social


cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press.

CHAPTER 1

A History of Implicit Social Cognition


Where Is It Coming From? Where Is It Now?
Where Is It Going?

B. Keith Payne and Bertram Gawronski

ithin the space of two decades, virtually In every topic of study, implicit social cognition
W every intellectual question in social psy~
chology, and many outside of it, has been shaped
is concerned with automatic/implicit/unconscious
processes underlying judgments and social behav;
by the theories and methods of implicit social cogni- ior. An indispensable part of this endeavor is the
tion. Many of those questions are pondered in this use of a new class of indirect measurement proce~
volume, involving the role of automatic/implicit/ dures such as the Implicit Association Test (!AT;
unconscious processes in attitudes (Petty & Briiiol, Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) and
Chapter 18), social judgment and decision making different kinds of sequential priming tasks (e.g.,
(Bodenhausen & Todd, Chapter 15), goal pursuit Fazio, jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; Payne,
(Ferguson & Porter, Chapter 17), prejudice and Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005; Wittenbrink,
stereotyping (Amodio & Mendoza, Chapter 19; Judd, & Park, 1997,"), which play a crucial role in
Trawalter & Shapiro, Chapter 20), self-concepts the chapters of this book. Thumbing through its
and self-esteem (Schnabel & Asendorpf, Chapter pages makes it clear how implicit social cognition
22; Zeigler-Hill & jordan, Chapter 21), social cog- has brought new insights, and also new centro~
nitive development (Olson & Dunham, Chapter versies, wherever it has led. In fact, implicit social
13), romantic relationships (Baldwin, Lydon, Mc- cognition has grown at such an incredible rate
Clure, & Etchison, Chapter 23), and social justice over the past years that it seems almost impossible
(Payne & Cameron, Chapter 24). Expanding be- to keep track of the rapidly progressing develop-
yond the traditional boundaries of social psycholo- ments in this area. The purpose of this handbook
gy, the basic ideas of implicit social cognition have is to provide a comprehensive overview of the cur~
also had an impact in many applied areas, includ~ rent state of the field, including key findings, cur-
ing health psychology (Wiers et al., Chapter 25), rent directions, and emerging themes in the area
clinical psychology (Teachman, Cody, & Clerkin, of implicit social cognition.
Chapter 26), forensic psychology (Snowden & In this introduction, we aim to provide a context
Gray, Chapter 27), consumer psychology (Perkins for the chapters that follow by highlighting some
& Forehand, Chapter 28), and political psychol- of the themes that keep surfacing in the field, trac-
ogy (Nosek, Graham, & Hawkins, Chapter 29). ing them to their historical roots, and identifying
2 I. History of Implicit Social Cognition

emerging themes that may guide future research. Roots ofAutomatic and Controlled
We hope that our historical synopsis will not only Processing in Attention Research
provide an orientation for the in-depth discussions
Some of the seminal articles that can be subsumed
of particular topics in the following chapters but
under the umbrella term implicit social cognition
also circumscribe the characteristics that define
emphasize the differences between automatic and
implicit social cognition as a particular way of
controlled cognition, with little mention of the
studying, understanding, and explaining human
unconscious. These reports took as their point of
behavior. In addition, we hope that our introduc-
departure the cognitive psychology work of Shif-
tion helps illuminate the historical roots of previ-
frin and Schneider (1977) and Posner and Snyder
ous and ongoing debates, which seems valuable for
(1975), among others. This work developed from
critical appraisals of theoretical interpretations in
a tradition of research on selective attention and
implicit social cognition.
short-term memory (e.g., Broadbent, 1971; Treis-
man, 1969). The key idea in these articles was that
WHERE IS IT COMING FROM? information processing could be divided into con~
trolled and automatic modes. Controlled process~
A history of implicit social cognition could easily ing was defined as demanding attention, limited
enough start with Freud and the psychoanalytic in capacity, and voluntarily initiated and altered.
unconscious; or with Augustine and Aquinas, Automatic processing was defined as needing little
who had a lot to say about the limits of introspec- attention, unlimited in capacity, and difficult to
tion in knowing thyself; or even with Plato and suppress voluntarily. These criteria will, of course,
Aristotle, who commented extensively on con~ sound very familiar to social cognition researchers
sciousness and intentional behavior. Our aim in because they are the forerunners, but not identi~
this introduction is more modest. It is to take a cal to, Bargh's (1994) "four horsemen" of automa-
selective look at roughly the last two decades in ticity (awareness, efficiency, intention, control).
which implicit social cognition has comprised a Although in Bargh's scheme conscious awareness
recognizable enterprise in its own right, estab~ is the first feature, the earlier cognitive theories
lishing itself as one of the most influential ap~ hardly mentioned consciousness.
proaches in social psychology. This process began Fazio and colleagues' seminal work showing
as researchers adapted ideas and methods from that attitudes can be automatically activated built
cognitive psychology to answer social psychologi~ on these cognitive theories of automatic and con~
cal questions. As we shall see, many of the lively trolled processing (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell,
debates in implicit social cognition can be traced & Kardes, 1986; see also Dovidio, Evans, & Tyler,
to which particular tradition of cognitive psychol~ 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983). Here too
ogy was appropriated. there was little concern with consciousness. The
term conscious appears only once in Fazio and col~
leagues' (1986) article and the term aware appears
Two Roots of Implicit Social Cognition twice. For Fazio and colleagues, "The key feature
Reports on implicit social cognition include a lot of such automatic activation, then, is inescapabil~
of hyphens and slashes in their key terms. Phrases ity" (p. 229). 1n the work ofShiffrin and Schneider
like automatic/implicit/unconscious processing and (1977) and Posner and Snyder (1975), the degree of
controlled/explicit/conscious processing abound. learning was critically important for automaticity.
Aside from being less than precise, such phrases Well~ learned items were detected or retrieved from
reveal a tension at the heart of the discipline. One memory automatically, whereas poorly learned
theme that recurs in the field, and in this book, is items required cognitive effort to search for them.
an uneasy relationship between notions of auto~ Based on these assumptions, Fazio and colleagues
maticity and unconsciousness. When these terms distinguished between well-learned (i.e., strong)
are thrown together, they sometimes leave readers attitudes that should be activated automatically
with only a vague idea of what is being studied. and poorly learned (i.e., weak) attitudes that
However, a sharper picture emerges when we take should not.
a step back and distinguish two intellectual tradi~ The connection between the strength of atti-
tions that gave rise to separate terminologies. One tudes and their ability to be automatically activat~
tradition is found in research on selective atten~ ed set the stage for the use of sequential priming
tion. The other is found in research on implicit techniques to measure attitudes without asking
memory. subjects to report them (Fazio et al., 1995; see also
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 3

Wentura & Degner, Chapter 6, this volume). This inner attitudes and beliefs, or are they instead the
novel approach to indirect measurement would products of the cultural environment? This de~
soon become important across many areas of psy~ bate is complex and multifaceted, and it recurred
chology and ultimately a central characteristic of in different variants. Examples include the early
implicit social cognition. Initially, the usefulness controversy between Devine (1989) and Fazio
of this approach was most obvious for studying ra~ and colleagues (1995), debates about the general-
cial attitudes (see Amodio & Mendoza, Chapter ity versus variability of automatic attitude activa~
19, and Trawalter & Shapiro, Chapter 20, this vol- tion (Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, & Fratto, 1992;
ume). Racial attitudes had always presented chal- Chaiken & Bargh, 1993; Fazio, 1993), and recent
lenges for researchers because self~presentation controversies about the role of personal versus ex~
motives meant many subjects would not honestly trapersonal associations in the IAT (Gawronski,
report their attitudes. Techniques such as the Peters, & LeBel, 2008; Nosek & Hansen, 2008a,
bogus pipeline showed that subjects typically hold 2008b; Olson & Fazio, 2004; Olson, Fazio, & Han,
more prejudiced attitudes than they are willing 2009). We consider three different aspects of the
to report under ordinary circumstances (Jones & debate in turn. First, the debate has sometimes
Sigal!, 1971). However, such deceptive procedures been understood as questioning whether auto~
are ethically controversial and practically cumber~ matic attitudes are "real." Both Fazio and Devine
some. Priming methods seemed to offer the paten~ showed that automatically activated stereotypes or
rial for a bona fide pipeline to reach respondents' attitudes were associated with biased impressions
true attitudes (Fazio et al., 1995). and behaviors. Insofar as the reality of an attitude
The challenge of racial attitudes was taken up is a question about whether it has consequences
by both Devine (1989) and Fazio and colleagues for thought and behavior, all sides of the debate
(1995). Both studies were informed by cognitive agree that the automatic responses are real.
theories of automatic and controlled processing A second aspect of this debate is whether there
(Posner & Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, are meaningful individual differences in auto~
1977) and priming techniques (Neely, 1977); matic responses. If automatic responses were like
both had little to say about conscious awareness language and virtually every healthy member of a
of attitudes; and both emphasized the idea that culture possessed them, variability in sequential
well~learned associations should be activated au~ priming scores would simply reflect measurement
tomatically, but weakly learned associations re~ error. A great deal of subsequent research showed
quire cognitive effort to be retrieved. For Fazio, that individual differences in priming and other
the implication was that individuals with weak or cognitive tasks are indeed meaningful and predic~
neutral racial attitudes simply would not show any tive of behavior (Perugini, Richetin, & Zogmais~
priming effects, whereas those with strongly nega~ ter, Chapter 14, this volume).
rive attitudes or strongly positive attitudes would A thornier aspect of this debate is whether peo~
show corresponding priming effects. However, for pie who show negative automatic associations with
Devine the important distinction was between ethnic groups should be considered prejudiced
the knowledge of a social stereotype versus the (Arkes & Tetlock, 2004; Banaji, Nosek, & Green-
personal endorsement of the stereotype. Because wald, 2004). Thisiis a philosophical and moral
everyone in a culture learns stereotypes in the issue more than an empirical one. Not surpris~
same way they learn about other categories, such ingly, it remains a topic of hot debate (see Payne &
as birds or foods, knowledge of stereotypes should Cameron, Chapter 24, this volume). The roots of
be well learned for virtually everyone. Stereotypes the debate can be traced as far as concepts of vir~
should, therefore, be automatically activated for rue in ancient Greek philosophy. Plato argued that
everyone. Personally endorsed beliefs, on the other virtue is about developing the right inclinations.
hand, should vary with one's values and motiva~ The virtuous person has good inclinations, where~
tions. Thus, in Devine's approach, inescapable ste~ as a wicked person has evil inclinations. However,
reotype activation was universal; it was endorsed Aristotle argued that both good and bad people
beliefs that distinguished prejudiced from unpreju- sometimes have wicked impulses, and that virtue
diced individuals. consists in regulating and channeling them well.
Here we see a second theme that reappears in Contemporary philosophers have begun integrat~
debates throughout implicit social cognition re~ ing insights from implicit social cognition with
search: Are the results of cognitive measurements, theories of ethics, creating an exciting exchange of
such as the ones revealed by sequential priming ideas across disciplines (e.g., Appiah, 2008; Doris,
tasks, best considered a reflection of the person's 2002; Kennett & Fine, 2009).
4 t. History of Implicit Social Cognition

Roots of Unconscious and Conscious ies, the "event" is often the presentation of a par~
Processing in Implicit Memory Research ticular word or a picture in a learning task. The
experimenter has perfect control over whether a
The studies we have discussed so far focused on
particular stimulus was presented and, therefore,
the distinction between automatic and controlled knows with certainty whether the subject experi~
processing, emphasizing notions of inescapability enced a particular learning episode. Moreover, the
rather than unconsciousness. However, in the fol- definition of implicit memory focuses on perfor~
lowing years, the automatic/controlled distinction mance, which is an operational definition. From
often gave way to the dichotomy between explicit this perspective, implicit memory is based on the
and implicit processes, interpreted as synonyms for effects of past experiences, controlled by the ex~
the terms conscious and unconscious. This shift was perimenter, on later performance, which is directly
ignited in part by Greenwald and BanaJi's (1995) observable. For implicit attitudes, in contrast, the
influential review, which built on a different re- definition refers to traces of past experience, which
search tradition of cognitive psychology. Rather mediate later responses. This definition is decid~
than building on theories of attention and short- edly more mentalistic than for implicit memory.
term memory, they drew on research on implicit In fact, implicit social cognition researchers usu~
memory (Banaji, 2001). Although measured in a ally do not control conscious awareness of past ex~
variety of ways, implicit memory has been defined periences giving rise to attitudes, stereotypes, and
fairly consistently as influences of past experience other such traces. Unlike implicit memory tests,
on later performance, in the absence of conscious the measures used in implicit social cognition
memory for the earlier experience (Jacoby & Dal- research do not test for awareness of the forma~
las, 1981; Schacter, 1987). Amnesic pattents, for rive experiences. Moreover, although Greenwald
example, tend to perform at chance when asked and Banaji (1995) clearly referreJ to unawareness
to recognize which words they have previously of the experiences giving rise to attitudes, subse~
studied. When asked, however, to guess how to quent writers have sometimes contlated aware~
complete word fragments, they perform more ac- ness of the source with awareness of the attitude
curately for studied than for unstudied words itself. This conflation has led to the fallacy that 1f
(Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1968). The effect of one assesses a construct with a measure that does
prior study on later performance, despite the in- not presuppose conscious introspection, then the
ability to consciously remember it, is taken to in- assessed construct must be introspectively inac~
dicate implicit memory. cessible. The available evidence clearly speaks
This conceptualization strongly shaped Green- against this assumption (Gawronski, Hofmann,
wald and Banaji's (1995) definition of implicit at- & Wilbur, 2006), but claims of unconsciousness
titudes as "introspectively unidentified (or inac- remain common. The degree to which the con~
curately identified) traces of past experience that structs measured with cognitive rests may be un~
mediate favorable or unfavorable feeling, thought, conscious and how to empirically confirm such
or action toward social objects" (p. 8). Just as im~ claims remains a topic of debate. This debate, in
plicit memory was defined as consequences of past turn, has inspired novel ways of thinking about
experience in the absence of conscious awareness consciousness and introspection (Hofmann &
for the experience, implicit attitudes were defined Wilson, Chapter 11).
as traces of past experience in the absence of con~
scious awareness for the experience. Implicitness
was identified with unconsciousness. In contrast
What Is "Implicit"
to earlier reports emphasizing the notion of au~
about Implicit Social Cognition?
tomaticiry in the sense of inescapability (Fazio Since Greenwald and Banaji (1995) coined the
et al., 1986), variants of the words consciou.~ and term implicit socia[ cognition, the two roots of im~
awareness are mentioned nearly a hundred times plicit social cognition have contributed to lively
in Greenwald and Banaji's paper. debates about the proper use of terminology, in
Although the parallel between concepts of im- particular with regard to the term implicit. Some
plicit memory and implicit attitudes is close, it is researchers have used the term to describe a par~
not complete. There was a subtle shift between the ticular characteristic of measurement procedures,
two concepts that has important consequences for namely measures that provide indicators of psy~
implicit social cognition. In the case of implicit chological attributes (e.g., attitudes) without hav~
memory, it is the experience of some past event ing to ask participants to verbally report the de~
that is not consciously available. In memory stud~ sired information (e.g., Fazio & Olson, 2003). Yet
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 5

other researchers have used implicit to describe the predicted that "when such measures do become
constructs assessed by a particular class of mea- available, there should follow the rapid develop-
surement procedures, namely constructs assessed ment of a new industry of research on implicit cog-
by tasks that do not require conscious introspec- nitive aspects of personality and social behavior"
tion and, therefore, might reflect psychological at- (p. 20). Their prediction has not been disappoint-
tributes that are introspectively inaccessible (e.g., ed. With the development of the !AT (Greenwald
Banaji, 2001). et al., 1998), implicit social cognition research
Because such terminological issues are a mat- seemed to hit a tipping point. Although sequential
ter of semantics and linguistic conventions, they priming tasks had been available for several years
cannot be resolved empirically. At the same time, (e.g., Dovidio et al., 1986; Fazio et al., 1986; Gaert-
different use of the same terms often produces ner & McLaughlin, 1983), researchers now had a
conceptual confusion, which can have disruptive task that made implicit social cognition research
effects on cumulative research progress (Machado much easier. Facilitation scores from priming pro-
& Silva, 2007). To overcome these problems, re- cedures often suffered from low reliability and
searchers have developed detailed taxonomies of relatively small effect sizes. However, the !AT had
the various properties of measurement procedures reasonably good reliability and very large effects.
(De Houwer & Moors, Chapter 10, this volume) In fact, even though the term implicit social cogni-
and mental representations (Carlston, Chapter 3, tion was originally interpreted in a much broader
this volume). The most comprehensive conceptual sense (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995), it has become
analysis has been provided by De Houwer and col- almost synonymous with research using indirect
leagues, who suggested using the terms direct and measurement procedures, such as the IAT and its
indirect to describe features of measurement proce- derivates (Teige-Mocigemba, Klauer, & Sherman,
dures and the terms implicit and explicit to describe Chapter 7, this volume), sequential priming tasks
features of the psychological attributes that are as- (Wentura & Degner, Chapter 6, this volume), and
sessed by different measurement procedures (e.g., sophisticated indirect paper-and-pencil methods
De Houwer, Teige-Mocigemba, Spruyt, & Moors, (Sekaquaptewa, Vargas, & von Hippe!, Chapter 8,
2009). With regard to the term implicit, De Hou- this volume). When the number of studies using
wer further suggested using this description in the indirect measurement procedures started to sky-
meaning of the term automatic, such that it may rocket around the turn of the millennium, the dif-
describe the unconscious, efficient, unintentional, ferent roots of implicit social cognition left their
or uncontrollable nature of the assessed constructs own traces by inspiring two parallel, largely inde-
(Moors, Spruyt, & De Houwer, Chapter 2, this pendent streams of research.
volume).
Even though terminological systems like the
one proposed by De Houwer are a matter of con-
Two Parallel Streams of Research
vention, the normative request implied by these The first stream had its origin in the idea of ines-
taxonomies is the call for researchers to be precise capability, derived from the notion of automatic-
about what they mean when they use the term im- ity in attention research. This stream was mainly
plicit. These norms encourage researchers to speci- theory driven, in tbtat empirical predictions were
fy in which particular sense a process is automatic derived from established theories of attitude-
(De Houwer eta!., 2009) and what exactly remains behavior relations, most notably Fazio's motivation
outside of conscious awareness (e.g., past experi- and opportunity as determinants (MODE) model
ences that give rise to attitudes vs. attitudes per (Fazio, 1990; Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999). The
se; see Gawronski et al., 2006). Detailed taxono- central assumption in the MODE model is that
mies and clarity in "defining our terms" will not direct and indirect measures assess the behavioral
themselves settle empirical questions, but they can effects of the same underlying mental representa-
strongly facilitate research progress by preventing tion, the only difference being the degree of con-
conceptual misunderstandings. 1 trol that participants have over their responses.
Whereas responses on self-report measures can
be easily altered, indirect measures such as the
A NEW INDUSTRY OF RESEARCH IAT and sequential priming tasks -constrain par-
ticipants' opportunity to control their responses.
Greenwald and Banaji (1995) ended their review This distinction captures the theoretical core of
with a call for the refinement of individual differ- the MODE model, which argues that automati-
ence measures of implicit social cognition. They cally activated attitudes will guide judgments and
6 1. History of Implicit Social Cognition

behavior if either the motivation or the opportu- Unexpected Malleability


nity to engage in deliberate processing is low. If,
The explosion of research using indirect measures
however, both motivation and opportunity are
also led to discoveries that forced a reexamination
high, the impact of automatic attitudes on self-
of some of the field's core assumptions. One such
reports may be diluted or inhibited by deliberate
discovery was that the scores obtained with indi~
processes. These assumptions imply that the pre-
rect measures often increased, decreased, or even
dictive validity of a given measure should depend
reversed as a function of the context (Gawronski
on the overlap between the processing conditions
& Sritharan, Chapter 12). Spending 5 minutes
during the completion of the measure and the
imagining a "strong woman" led to weaker implicit
processing conditions of the relevant behavior.
gender stereotyping (Blair, Ma, & Lenton, 2001),
For instance, indirect measures should be better
and viewing photos of admired African Ameri~
predictors of spontaneous behavior, whereas direct
cans such as Martin Luther King, ]r., along with
self-report measures should be better predictors of
disliked white Americans such as serial killer jef-
deliberate behavior (Perugini et al., Chapter 14,
this volume). frey Dahmer reduced implicit race bias (Dasgupta
A second, parallel stream of re.search had its & Greenwald, 2001). Racial bias on indirect mea-
origin in the notion of unconsciousness, as de- sures was even reversed by the background con~
rived from cognitive research on implicit memory. text. In one study, pictures of African Americans
In contrast to the single~representation assump~ in the role of prisoners evoked negative implicit
tion implied by the MODE model, this stream was evaluations, but presenting the same individuals in
characterized by an operational equation of mea~ the role of lawyers elicited positive implicit evalua-
surement outcomes with distinct psychological tions (Barden, Maddux, Petty, & Brewer, 2004; see
constructs, which favored a dualism of two inde~ also judd, Simpson, & Carver, 2001; Wittenbrink,
pendent representations in memory: a conscious, judd, & Park, 2001).
explicit representation and an unconscious, im~ The ease of shifting measurement scores chal~
plicit representation (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). lenged a dominant assumption at the time that the
Because there was no theoretical framework avail~ associations assessed by indirect measures had be~
able that could specify how the two kinds of repre- come automatized by a long history of learning and
sentations may differentially influence judgments must, therefore, be difficult to change (e.g., Wil-
and behavior, empirical research in this stream son, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000; see also Olson &
was largely effect focused and method driven. The Dunham, Chapter 13, this volume). This assump-
main topics of interest were whether indirect mea~ tion was based on earlier research using perceptual
sures predict behaviors that direct measures do learning tasks, and Shiffrin and Schneider (1977)
not predict or whether indirect measures explain had indeed shown that information was retrieved
variance in behavior over and above variance that automatically only after hundreds or thousands of
can be explained by direct measures. As reviewed trials of learning. Thus, findings that the scores of
by Perugini and colleagues (Chapter 14, this vol- indirect measures could be swayed one way or the
ume), research has documented several distinct other by immediate changes in context presented
patterns by which indirect and direct measures a puzzle. The tWo answers that have been offered
may differentially predict behavior. For example, to resolve this puzzle introduced a third theme to
in some cases one measure or the other alone implicit social cognition, namely the debate about
predicted behavior; in other cases they predicted whether responses on indirect measures reflect sta~
behavior additively; and in other cases they syn~ ble representations in long-term memory or fluid
ergistically combined to predict behavior. There constructions that are generated on the spot. In
is replicable evidence for each of these patterns, the eyes of construction theorists, the high malle~
but this seems to be a case in which theory has ability of indirect measurement scores confirmed
not yet caught up to the data. Many models such their assumption that contexts intluence what
as the MODE model and others reviewed in this information is used to construct an attitude from
chapter can accommodate these findings, but it one moment to the next, and that these principles
is not clear that any current models make unam~ apply equally to direct and indirect measurement
biguous a priori predictions for when each pattern procedures (e.g., Schwarz, 2007). In fact, the very
should be expected. Developing such theories is idea that indirect measurement procedures would
an important next step we hope researchers will assess rigid "things" in memory independent of the
pursue. context was seen as ill founded.
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 7

In contrast to this view, representation theo- between directly and indirectly assessed evalua-
rists argued that malleability in measurement tions was much higher for those who were will-
es
scores does not reflect context-dependent changes ing to openly express prejudice compared with
m
in mental representations but, rather, responses to those who were motivated to control prejudiced
:h fundamentally different objects (e.g., Fazio, 2007). reactions {e.g., Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Fazio et al.,
li- 1995; Gawronski, Geschke, & Banse, 2003; Payne,
According to this view, contextual information
simply influences how a given object is catego- 2001). Expanding on these findings, research has
rized. To the degree that different categories are shown that the spontaneity of self-reports plays
es a significant role in this regard, such that direct
associated with different evaluations in long-term
:it and indirect measurement scores show higher cor-
memory, contextual cues can lead to different
), respondence when self-reports are based on quick,
responses to the same object, even though the
ri- respective category associations that underlie intuitive gut reactions rather than deliberate anal-
:h these responses are highly stable. As Schwarz and yses (e.g., Gawronski & LeBel, 2008; Hofmann,
,[-
Bohner (2001) pointed out, this debate cannot be Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005;
ta resolved on empirical grounds because any result jordan, Whitfield, & Zeigler-Hill, 2007). A second
a- predicted by one theoretical framework can be re- critical factor was measurement error. Cunning-
n.-
interpreted by the other one. Yet, inspired by this ham, Preacher, and Banaji (2001) demonstrated
"j t debate, researchers have started to study the learn- that when latent-variable analyses were used to
ing mechanisms that lead to context-dependent or correct for measurement error, the correlation
in context-independent responses on indirect mea- between directly and indirectly assessed racial at~
a- sures (e.g., Rydell & Gawronski, 2009). By shift- titudes was quite substantial (see also Hofmann,
'e ing the focus from mere demonstrations of con- Gawronski, et al., 2005). A third factor concerns
k, text effects to experimentally controlled learning whether the attitude objects evaluated in direct
experiences and their directly observable effects and indirect measures are similar or not. Con-
tl- on performance, this line of research could pos- ceptual correspondence (Hofmann, Gawronski,
1e
sibly lay the groundwork for a new way of study- et al., 2005) refers to whether different measures
e- ing context effects on indirect measures that goes assess conceptually comparable constructs; struc-
,d beyond the debate between representational and tural fit (Payne, Burkley, & Stokes, 2008) refers
Li- constructionist models. to whether direct and indirect measures involve
S.. similar stimuli and responses. Overall, measure-
p- ment scores of direct and indirect tests show much
al Unex[>ected Correspondence higher correspondence when they are matched in
7) to Self-Report Measures
their concepts and test structures than when they
.d If the context dependence of indirect measure- do not align. Finally, the correspondence of mea-
of ment scores made researchers rapidly reexamine surement scores depends on several aspects of the
of their assumptions, another cause for reexamina- underlying representations. For instance, direct
tion accumulated more slowly over several years. and indirect measurement scores are more strongly
Early studies often found little or no correspon- related when the assessed attitudes are strong and
dence between direct and indirect measurement perceived as distinctive (Nosek, 2005).
:o scores (Rydell & McConnell, Chapter 16, this The discovery of these moderators shifted the
Jt volume). This divergence reinforced the idea that question from whether responses on direct and
a- indirect measures reflected unconscious represen- indirect measures are related to identifying the
id tations that are inaccessible to verbal self-report. conditions under which they are related {Hof-
n If implicit representations were unconscious, then mann, Gschwendner, et al., 2005). This research
self-reports would certainly not be expected to suggested that direct and indirect measures, in
:d track them. However, as the field's data base grew, fact, reveal highly similar results when the tasks
more and more studies showed surprisingly high are reliable, the stimulus materials are presented
m correspondence under certain conditions (Hof- in similar ways, and participants base their self-
mann, Gschwendner, Nosek, & Schmitt, 2005). reports on quick, intuitive gut reactions rather
"lt One of the critical factors, identified in early re- than deliberate analyses. Taken together, these
·y search under the framework of the MODE model findings posed a challenge to earlier claims that
ld (Fazio et al., 1995), was the motivation to control indirect measures assess unconscious representa-
•e the expression of one's attitudes. For instance, tions (Gawronski, LeBel, & Peters, 2007). In fact,
in the context of racial attitudes the correlation unconsciousness of a mental representation, like
8 1. History of Implicit Social Cognition

rhe nonexistence of black swans or the absence of target, as in the Smgle-Category !AT (Karpinski
light in a closed refrigerator, is notoriously difficult & Steinman, 2006) and the Single-Target !AT
to prove. Even when there is no evidence for their (Wigboldus, Holland, & van Knippenberg, 2004 ).
presence, it is always possible you have not looked Because there is only one target ohject, these
in the right place or at the right time (Popper, measures avoid the problem of relative compari-
1934). But together, these findings suggested that, sons. However, these tasks still involve compari-
as long as researchers look in the right way, people sons across separate hlocks of compatible versus
seem to have much greater introspective access to incompatible trials, exposing them to another set
their mental representations than was commonly of methodological critiques (Teige-Mocigemba et
assumed. al., Chapter 7, this volume). [n a nutshell, these
tasks assume that participants' attitudes, stereo-
types, or other knowledge structures provide the
SECOND-GENERATION source of compatibility effects. That is, candy bars
MEASURES and pleasant words are compatible because both
are positively evaluated. However, the valence of
Most of the early research on implicit social cogni~ the items may be only one source of compatihil-
tion used either the !AT (Greenwald et al., 1998) ity effects. As noted by Rnthermund and Wentura
or available variants of sequential priming (e.g., (2001), participants might pair the items along
Fazio et al., \995; Wittenhrink et al., \997). How- any dimension that is salient at the time. Hence,
ever, both types of indirect measures had their rarticipants might simplify the sorting task by
problems. Whereas sequential priming tasks often subjectively recoding the task using any salient
suffered from low measurement reliability, the IAT heuristic. Such "salience asymmetries" could ere~
involved a number of structural problems that ate compatibility effects on the tasks for reasons
produced method-related confounds. These issues that are unrelated to the associations of interest
inspired researchers to develop new measurement to the researcher. This issue has been addressed
procedures that tried to overcome the problems by several new methods that present congruent
of existing tasks. The result was a second genera- and incongruent trials randomly in a single hlock
tion of measurement procedures that expanded on rather than blocked. The first !AT-derived task
early work on the IAT and priming tasks. that avoided its original block structures is De
Houwer's (2003) Extrinsic Affective Simon Task.
More recent examples incluJe the Single-Block
IAT-Inspired Methods !AT (Teige-Mocigemba, Klauer, & Rothermund,
An early recognized limitation of the IAT was the 2008) and the Recoding-Free !AT (Rothermund,
fact that it provides relative assessments involving Teige-Mocigemba, Gast, & Wentura, 2009).
two target objects (Teige-Mocigemba et al., Chap-
ter 7, this volume). For instance, if a person shows
a preference for candy bars over apples, we can-
Priming-Inspired Methods
not tell whether he or she has a particular yen for These methods all aimed at remedying some spe-
Snickers, an aversion to Granny Smiths, or some cific limitations of the IAT, perhaps because the
combination of both (Blanton, Jaccard, Gonzales, IAT's popularity made it an exciting target for
& Christie, 2006; Fiedler, Messner, & Bluemke, refining measurement techniques. However, dur~
2006). Nosek and Banaji (2001) were the first to ing the same period, research was progressing to
address this issue by developing the Go/No-Go refine priming methods as well (Wentura & De-
Association Task (GNAT). The GNAT includes gner, Chapter 6, this volume). Whereas Fazio and
one target object, such as apples, and attributes, colleagues (1986) had used sequential primmg to
such as good and bad words. In one block of tri- measure attitudes, Banaji and Hardin (1996) ap-
als, subjects press a key when they see apples or plied sequential priming to measure semantic re-
good words, and in the other block they press a key lations (e.g., stereotypes), and Wittenbrink and
when they see apples or had words. [f subjects are colleagues (1997) extended primmg paradigms
faster and more accurate when apples are paired to measure semantic and affective dimensions of
with good rather than bad words, it is inferred meaning independently.
that they have a positive attitude toward apples. Priming paradigms have the considerable ben~
Related approaches to measuring attitudes toward efit of simplicity. Subjects typically respond to the
single-target objects have been developed by sim- target items with a simple judgment, making these
plifying the I AT's dual-target structure to a single paradigms simpler for subjects to complete and for
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 9

ki researchers to interpret compared with IAT~related a large extent, these theories were domain specific
,T tasks. However, the response latency scores yielded in that they were concerned with particular phe~
). by priming tasks often involve large proportions of nomena in social psychology, including persuasion
se measurement error. One way around this problem (e.g., Chaiken, 1980; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986),
~i~ is to require participants to respond quickly, thus impression formation (e.g., Brewer, 1988; Fiske
~i~ shifting influences of the primes from response & Neuberg, 1990), and dispositional attribution
us latencies to accuracy rates (Draine & Greenwald, (Gilbert, 1989; Trope, 1986). In 2000, Smith and
et 1998). Payne (2001) used this strategy to measure DeCoster published an influential review article in
et the relationship between racial stereotypes and which they reconceptualized the domain~specific
se perceptions of weapons. Target items were more processes proposed by earlier theories in terms of
J· likely to be misidentified as guns when they were a general set of processes underlying a variety of
1e preceded by black face primes than white face phenomena. Their distinction between associative
rs primes. Priming effects in accuracy were found to and rule;based processes set the foundation for a
h be more reliable than priming effects in response theoretical reinterpretation of direct and indirect
of latencies (Payne, 2001, 2005). measures as reflecting the outcomes of two qualita;
1- Another approach to this problem is to dispense tively distinct processes (e.g., Gawronski & Boden·
ra with latency and accuracy altogether and instead hausen, 2006; Rydell & McConnell, 2006; Strack
tg use judgments of ambiguous stimuli. Murphy and & Deutsch, 2004; see also Deutsch & Strack,
e, Zajonc (1993) showed that participants rated Chi- Chapter 4, this volume). The important difference
'Y nese ideographs more favorably when they were from earlier theorizing is that these models empha·
lt primed with emotionally pleasant pictures and less size the principles by which processes operate (e.g.,
favorably when they were primed with unpleas· associative vs. rule based) rather than the condi;
lS ant pictures. Payne, Cheng, and colleagues (2005) tions under which they operate (e.g., unconscious,
st adapted this paradigm to measure individual dif- efficient, unintentional, uncontrollable). These
d ferences in attitudes toward the prime stimuli. operating conditions then become empirical ques~
\t Their affect misattribution procedure proved to tions to be tested.
k have large effect sizes and high reliability, helping The most influential model in this regard is
k to address some of the key difficulties with sequen· Strack and Deutsch's (2004) reflective-impulsive
'e rial priming methods. model, which distinguishes between associative
(. and propositional processes. Associative processes
k involve the activation of associations in memory,
l, THEORETICAL DEVELOPMENTS which is guided by the principles of similarity and
l, spatiotemporal contiguity. Propositional processes,
Parallel to the development of second~generation in contrast, are conceptualized as the validation
measures, the field of implicit social cognition of the information implied by activated associa;
has been reshaped by two major theoretical ad~ tions, which depends on syllogistic principles of
vances. One involves the emergence of general~ logical consistency (Gawronski & Strack, 2004).
ized, domain~independent dual~process models; The central difference between the two kinds of
e the other involves the development of formal processes lies in th~ role of subjective truth or ac;
•r process models that describe and quantify the curacy. Whereas the activation of associations is
contributions of distinct processes to performance assumed to occur independently of whether these
0 on cognitive tasks. Together, these theoretical de~ associations are regarded as accurate or inaccurate,
velopments have had a significant impact on how propositional processes are inherently concerned
:l researchers interpret the scores obtained with in~ with the validation of activated information. This
J direct measures. distinction has been related to the outcomes of
direct and indirect measures. Direct self;report
measures assess the subjective validity of proposi~
Generalized Dual-Process Models
:1 tional statements about some state of affairs (e.g.,
s At the dawn of the new millennium, social psy~ "How much do you agree with the statement ... "),
,f chology witnessed a remarkable shift in the gen~ whereas indirect measures assess the activation-
erality of theorizing. Research in the 1980s and that is, the momentary accessibility-of associa;
1990s was characterized by a strong influence of tions independent of whether they are regarded as
dual-process theories, which divided the realm of accurate or inaccurate.
social cognitive processes into effortless, automatic Despite their impact on the field, general-
r processes versus effortful, controlled processes. To ized dual;process models have also inspired some
10 l. History of Implicit Social Cognition

controversy, \vith ~ome authors argumg that the elusion that people had stronger stereotypical as-
distinctions they draw are roo narrow and or hers sociations when they respondel{ quickly. Instead,
arguing that they are too hroad. For example, Krug- the most likely interpretation is that stereotypical
lanski, Erh, Prerro, 1\hnnettt, and Chun (2006) ar- rcpresentdtions were rhe same for p~uucipants re~
gued that it is unnecessary to distinguish between sponding fast and those responding slow. Other
processes on the has is of operating principles such processes critteal to responding accurately mu:.t
as assuClative versus propositional processmg be- have het'n int1uenced by specJed responding.
cause virtually all judgment can be interpreted as Findings like these suggest that indirect mea~
the result of a smgle inference process so long as sures retlcct not only automatic processes but ,JtsL)
researchers srectfy parameters, such as relevance controlled processes. The distinction hetwcen au-
of infnrmation, processing difficulty, and cognitive tomatic and controlled processes allow:. research~
resources needed to reach a judgment. ln comwsr, ers to predict, for example, that when people are
Sherman (2006) argued rhar more than fl}/l) pro~ tired, distracted, or rushed, they are more likely to
cesses are often needed to describe the interrby respond hased on automatic impulses than when
between automatic :::md controlled processes. Snll, they are energetic, tixused, and unhurried (Fa:io
generali:ed dual~process models have advanced & Towles-Schwen, l999; Strack & Deutsch, 2004 ).
the held, at a minimum, hy prnv1ding heunsticatly The fact that m;-my Jual~process tht.'Ories predict
useful frameworks for interpreting distinctions these result:-; is hurh a strength and a weakness for
between indirect and direct measures (Hofmann, the theories. On the one hand, predicti\'e rmver
Gawronski, et aL, 2005). In addition, the models is vital for a theory's value. On the other hand,
h:we provided theoretical links to recent develop~ dual-process models do not usually specify exactly
ments m social neuroscience (Ito, Chapter 5, this how automatic and contro\\ed processes relate tu
volume), and they inspired novel ways of think~ each other. However, in understanding conflicts
ing about retlective versus impulsive determinants between automatic and controlled intluences, it i:-.
of socral behavror (Strack & Deutsch, 2004) anJ often essential to know how they are related. Fur~
the malleability of associative representations mal process models have been develored tn answer
(Gawronski & BoJenhausen, 2006). these mnre specific questions.
One such model is the process J.is~ocbtion (PO)
model, oripnally developed by Jacoby (l99l) to
Formal Process Models
serarate impltcir 01nd explicit memory processes.
of Indirect Measures
Payne (2001) arplted the model tn sepCJ.rating au-
Whereas generali:ed dual-process models seek to tommie and controlled int1uences in a priming
explain a wide variety of findings on the basis of task, illustrating its usefulness for studying un~
a broad central dtstinction, another new direction plicit social cognition. One variant of thi~ model
shaping implicit social cognition aims to get vt'ry assumes that rtutomatic processes drive behavior
specific about rhe processes underlying individual only when control over behavior fails (Jacoby,
effects, tasks, or behaviors. Formal process models 1991). A second variant of the mude\ assumes that
describe and quantify the contnbutions of mul- autnmatic processes are instead dominant, and
tiple processes to performance on particular tasks that controlled 'processes only drive behavior in
and have recently been applied to indirect mea- the absence of an automatic influence (LIIl.cls;:~y &
sures (Sherman, Khuer, & Allen, Chapter 9, this Jacoby, l994). Testing how well drfferent models
volume). Indirect measures operate on the prem~ describe expenmemal data can be used hoth to
ise that their scores are driven by underlying atti~ answer theoretical questions about how unobserv-
tudes, stereotypes, or whatever representation is of able processes are related to each other and aLso to
interest to the researcher. However, a great deal of create L}uantitative estimates of those underlying
research suggests that the reality is more compli- processes (Payne, Jacoby, & Lambert, 2001).
cated than that. Many variables have heen shmvn A second related model is the Quad mode\
to aftect task perfnrmance bes1des the mental con, (Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, Hugenberg, &
tents they were meant to measure. For examrle, Groom, 2005; Sherman et al., 2008). Whereas
Payne (2001) observed greater stereotype hias in the PO variants assume that either controlled or
weapon misidentifications when participants re, automatic processing is dominant whenever they
spunded under time pressure than when they re- conflict, the Quad model assumes that either kinJ
sponded at their own pace. If one assumes that the of prncess can be dominant. The Quad model adds
only process at work was automatic stereotyping, a third parameter that retlects whether automatic
then such findings would lead to the strange con- or controlled processes "win" when they are in
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 11

conflict and a fourth parameter for guessing biases. reviewing the chapters of this Handbook, we iden~
Bishara and Payne (2009) have recently described tified three themes that may guide future research
a unified framework for understanding PO and the and theorizing in implicit social cognition.
Quad model, as well as other related models such A first theme~one that has been recurring
as Stahl and Degner's (2007) ABC model, within in different variants~is the notorious tension
a single family of models (Payne & Bishara, 2009). between person~based and situation~based ap~
These models are all multinomial models, which proaches. This tension is reflected in early debates
describe a variety of unobserved processes as prob~ about whether automatically activated associa~
abilities. Multinomial models are agnostic about tions reflect mere knowledge of cultural stereo~
temporal order, and so they cannot differentiate types (Devine, 1989) or personal attitudes (Fazio
between processes that occur relatively early or et al., 1995). It is reflected in the current contra~
late. However, Klauer, Voss, Schmitz, and Teige~ versy about the role of personal versus extraper~
Mocigemba (2007) proposed a diffusion model sonal associations in the IAT (Gawronski et al.,
that incorporates both accuracy (as with multino~ 2008; Nosek & Hansen, Z008a, 2008b; Olson &
mial models) and also reaction times. Fazio, 2004; Olson et al., 2009). And it is reflected
Multinomial and diffusion models are exciting in the debate about whether responses on indirect
new directions because they offer means to test measures reveal stable representations in memory
theories about the processes underlying indirect (Fazio, 2007) or context-dependent evaluations
measures and simultaneously quantifying those that are constructed on the spot (Schwarz, 2007).
processes. These models also offer potential for Even though the details of these debates differ in
integrating insights across many different kinds various ways, they are all concerned with the ques~
of indirect measures because the same models can tion of whether responses on indirect measures tell
be applied across many different tasks to identify us something about the individual or something
their common and distinct components. One of about the individual's environment. Framed in
the earliest insights provided by the application of this way, the conflicting positions might bring
formal process models is that experimentally in~ up painful memories of the person-situation de~
duced variations in indirect measurement scores bate in social and personality psychology. How~
can be due to different underlying mechanisms ever, we believe that looking back at this debate
(Gawronski & Sritharan, Chapter 12, this vol- can be helpful in identifying novel, integrative
ume). Whereas some effects have been found to ways of thinking about implicit social cognition.
reflect genuine changes in underlying associations A particularly useful direction in this regard may
(e.g., Sherman et al., 2008; Stewart & Payne, be social cognitive approaches to study person X
2008), others stemmed from experimentally in- situation interactions (e.g., Cervone, Shadel, &
duced impairments in executive control (e.g., Gov~ jencius, 2001; Mischel & Shoda, 1995). One way
orun & Payne, 2006; Sherman et al., 2008). Given to do this is by investigating the role of individual
that changes in indirect measurement scores are experiences in shaping an individual's responses
typically interpreted as indicating changes in the in various situational contexts (e.g., Rydell &
underlying mental representations, formal process Gawronski, 2009). A complementary approach is
models provide an important means to avoid mis~ to study the ways tlpt dispositions lead individuals
interpretations of empirical data and distortions in to select different situations. Even though such an
general theorizing. approach cannot answer every question of the just~
mentioned debates, it has the potential to provide
a more comprehensive picture by including (1) a
WHERE IS IT GOING? person's individual experiences, (2) the context in
which these experiences occurred, and (3) the role
Implicit social cognition is arguably one of the of contextual cues in activating individual experi~
liveliest and most active research areas in social ences associated with a given context. Given that
psychology. The steady increase in the number of research on implicit social cognition heavily relies
's publications is just one indicator in this regard. on both individual difference (e.g., prediction of
r Given the exponential growth of implicit social behavior; Perugini et al., Chapter 14, this volume)
y cognition over the past decade, we want to consid~ and experimental designs (e.g., attitude formation
j er some of the emerging themes that may shape the and change; Gawronski & Sritharan, Chapter 12,
future of implicit social cognition. As the quantum this volume), future research adopting a person X
c physicist Niels Bohr put it, prediction is very dif situation interaction perspective could be helpful
1 ficult, especially about the future. Nevertheless, in to shed new light on some of the recurring disputes
12 1. History ot Implicit Social Cognition

about \Vhcther indirect measures tell us something not simply provide a mentali::.tic redescription of
ahout the person or about the person's environ~ the available heh,wi(1raluhservations.
mcnt. An important insight that can help to prevent
A second trend that seems to he emerging from circularity 1s the treatment of behavioral observa~
implicit social cognition is enhanced cross~ralkand tions (e.g., pcrfl mnance on indirect measures) as
continuously shrinking boundaries het\veen suh~ behaviors that need to be explamed rather than
disciplines. The adoption of indirect me1sures and as mental cnnstructs that explain behavior. From
theoretical models from social psychology in other this perspective, mental con~tructs can still serve
fields is only one example in this regard. In fact, an im~"~ortant function in explaining behavior,
it seems as· though many St Kial psychologi:::.ts in~ hur unly to the degree that the theoretical as-
terested in hasic questions have discovered applied sumptions about these constructs imply novel
research as an exciting context tn demonstrate the prediction,.;; about behavior that can be empiri-
power of their methods and theuries. Great exam, cally scrutini::ed. Recent adv::mces in implicit so~
pies in this regard are rrovided hy the chapters in cial cognition have already made significant steps
the la::-t section of this book, which review insights in this directit)n, and the emerging concern with
gained from imrlicir social cognition in the areas these issues mdicates that they might become even
of health psychology (Wiers et al., Chapter 25), more important in the future. Thus, even though
clinical psychology (Teachmao, Cody, & Clerkin, implicit social cognition started as an inherently
Chapter 26), forensic psychology (Snowden & mentalistic aprroach, it clearly has the potential
Gray, Chapter 27), consumer psychology (Perkins to make a unique contribution to the closing "de~
& Forehand, Chapter 28), and political psychol- cade of behavior," and we look forward to the in~
ogy (Nosek, Grafwm, & Hawkms, Chapter 29). sights it will convey in the years to cnme.
After decades of incre1s1ng compartmentalization
in psychology, we see this development as an excit~
ing trend thar may ultimc1tely lead to a more com~ ACKNOWLEDGMENT
prehensive picture of the human mind.
A final theme that we identified in several chap~ We Me grJtcful fur the thl\Ughtfut cmnments of G~len
BuJenh<HL~Cn an,J Ru-;s Fa:Jtl un <m earlier Lirafr of this
ters in this volume is a reanalysis of the mle of men-
chd~"~ter.
tal concepts (~1s l)pposed to behavioral responses)
in psychological theorizing. This trend 1s most ex-
plicit in requests not to interpret the responses on
NOTE
psychological measures as direct reflections of men~
tal concepts. After all, responses on any kind of di~ l. Fm the sake of conceptual clarity, we follL1W De
rect or indirect measures are behavioral responses Houwer and colleagues' (2009) recommenda-
(Amodio & Mendoza, Chapter 19; De Houwer tion to use the terms direct and indirect to de~
& Moors, Chapter 10; Gawronski & Sritharan, scrihe procedural characteristics of measurement
Chapter 12; Perugmi et al., Chapter 14; Sherman pmcedures and the terms implicit and explicit to
et al., Chapter 9). Tu he sure, it makes sense to descrihe psychological features of the constructs
as:::.ume that these behavioral responses have their asse::.scd hy diff~rent kinds d. measurement proce-
roots in reople's minds. However, the undispu- dures (see also [)e Houwer & Moors, Chapter 10,
table fact that behavioral responses are mediated this volume).
by mental processes does not allow researchers tn
equate these responses with the mental processes
that presum<thly underlie them. A useful example
REFERENCES
to illustrme this idea is the basic nouon of formal Avpi<lh, K. A. (2008). Experiments m ethics. Cambridge,
process models of indirect measures, which treat MA: Harv~rd University Press.
pertl)fmance on indirect measmes as behavioral Arkes, H. R., & Tetlnck, P. E. (2004). Attributions of
outcomes thm arc joindy determined by multirle implicit prejudice, m "would jesse Jackson 'fail' the
distinct processes. From the perspective of these Implicit Association Test.'" Ps':'Jchological Inquiry, 15,
models, the challenge fl)r researchers is to develop 257-278.
Bnnaji, M. R. (2001 ). Implicit attitudes can he measured.
thetJrics of mental functioning that integrate the
In H. L. Ruediger, J. S. N<lirne, \.Neath, & A. Sur-
available set of behavioral observations in a parsi~ prenant (Eds.), The narure of remembering: E.'iSa\'s in
rnonious manner. Ideally, these models also imply remembering Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117-1 SO). Wash-
novel prediCtions about behavior, so that they do ingtnn, DC: American Psychological Assuciatinn.

L
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 13

Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic ste- De Houwer, J. (2003). The Extrinsic Affective Simon
reotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136-141. Task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 77-85.
Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (Z004). De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., &
No place for nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A norma-
& Tetlock. Psychological Inquiry, 15, Z79-Z89. tive analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135,
Barden,)., Maddux, W. W., Petty, R. E., & Brewer, M. 347-368.
B. (2004). Contextual moderation of racial bias: The Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their
impact uf social roles on controlled and automatical- automatic and controlled components. Journal of Per-
ly activated ani tudes. Journal of Personality and Social sonality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-18.
Psychology, 87, 5-ZZ. Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: moral behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univer-
Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in so- sity Press.
cial cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. SruH (Eds.), Dovidio,). F., Evans, N., & Tyler, R. B. (1986). Racial
Handbook of social cognition (pp. 1-40). Hillsdale, NJo stereotypes: The contents of their cognitive repre-
Erlbaum. sentations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. 22, ZZ-37.
(1992). The generality of the automatic attitude acti- Draine, S. C., & Greenwald, A G. (1998). Replicable
vation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimen-
ogy, 62, 893-9IZ. tal Psychologyo General, 127, Z86-303.
Bishara, A. J., & Payne, B. K. (2009). Multinomial pro- Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual dif-
cess tree models of comrol and automaticity in weap- ference measure of motivation to control prejudiced
on misidentificmion. Journal of Experimental Social reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Psychology, 45, 5Z4-534. 23, 316-3Z6.
Blair, I. V., Ma, J., & Lemon, A. (2001). Imagining ste- Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which atti-
reotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereo- tudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an inte-
types through mental imagery. Journal of Personality grative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
and Social Psychology, 81, 8Z8-84L experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75-109).
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzales, P. M., & Christie, C. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(2006). Decoding the Implicit Association Test: Im- Fazio, R. H. (1993). Variability in the likelihood of au-
plications for criterion prediction. journal of Experi- tomatic attitude activation: Data reanalysis and com-
mental Social Psychology, 42, 192-Z!Z. mentary on Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, and Pratto
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impres- (1992). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
sion formation. In T K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), 64, 753-758.
Advances in social cognition (Vol. l, pp. 1-36). Hills- Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation as-
dale, NJ' Erlbaum. sociations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25,
Broadbent, D. (1971). Decision and stress. Oxford, UK: 603-637
Academic Press. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams,
Cervone, D., Shadel, W. D., & )encius, S. (ZOO!). Social- C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an
cognitive theory of personality assessment. Personal- unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide
ity and Social Psychology Review, 5, 33-51. pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic process- 69, 1013-1027.
ing and the use of source versus message cues in per- Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M.A. (2003). Implicit measures
suasion. journal of Personality and Social Psychology, in social cognition research: Their meaning and use.
39, 7SZ-766. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, Z97-327.
Chaiken, S., & Bargh, J. A. (1993). Occurrence versus Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., &
moderation of the automatic attitude activation ef- Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of
fect: Reply to Fazio. Journal of Personality and Social attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 64, 759-765. 50, ZZ9-Z38.
Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugen- Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T (1999). The MODE
berg, K., & Groom, C. (2005). Separating multiple model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken
processes in implicit social cognition: The quad- & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psy-
model of implicit task performance. Journal of Per- chology (pp. 97-116). New Yorko Guilford Press.
sonality and Social Psychology, 89, 469-487. Fiedler, K., Messner, C., & Bluemke, M. (2006). Unre-
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. solved problems with the "1," the "A:' and the "T":
(2001). Implicit attitude measurement: Consistency, Logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit As-
stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Sci- sociation Test OAT). European Review of Social Psy-
ence, 12, 163-170. chology, 17, 74-147.
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A G. (ZOO!). On the mal- Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of
leability of automatic attitudes: Combating auto- impression formation, from category-based to indi·
matic prejudice with images of admired and disliked viduating processes: Influences of information and
individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P.
ogy, 81, 800-814. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
14 1. History of Implicit Social Cognition

ogy (Vol. 23, pp. 1-74). San Diego, CA Academic ]one,, E. E., & Sigal!, H. (1971). The bogus p1pelin"
Press. A new paradigm for measuring affect and attitude.
Gaertner, S. L, & McLaughlin,]. P. (1983). Racial ste· Psychological Bulletin, 76, 349-364.
reotypes: Associations and ascriptions of positive and Jordan, C. H., Whitfield, M., & Zeigler-Hill, V (2007).
negative characteristics. Social Psychology Quarterly, Intuition and the correspondence between implicit
46, 23-30. and explicit self-esteem. Journal of Personality and So~
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associa~ cial Psychology, 93, 1067-1079.
rive and propositional processes in evaluation: An judd, C. M., Simpson, J. A, & Carver, C. S. (Eds.).
integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude (2001). Attitudes and social cognition [Special issue].
change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692-73L Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (5).
Gawronski, B., Geschke, D., & Banse, R. (2003). Im~ Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single
plicit bias in impression formation: Associations in- category Implicit Association Test as a measure of
fluence the construal of individuating information. implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and
European]ournal of Social Psychology, 33, 573-589. Social Psychology, 91, 16-32.
Gawronski, B., Hofmann, W., & Wilbur, C. (2006). Are Kennett, J., & Fine, C. (2009). Will the real moral judg-
"implicit" attitudes unconscious? Consciousness and ment please stand up? Ethical Theory and Moral Prac-
Cognition, 15, 485-499. tice, 12, 77-96.
Gawronski, B., & LeBel, E. P. (2008). Understanding Klauer, K. C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., & Teige-Mocigemba,
patterns of attitude change: When implicit measures S. (2007). Process~components of the Implicit Asso~
show change, bur explicit measures do not. Journal of dation Test: A diffusion model analysis. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1355-1361. Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 353-368.
Gawronski, B., LeBel, E. P., & Peters, K. R. (2007). Kruglanski, A., Erb, H., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., &
What do implicit measures tell us?: Scrutinizing rhe Chun, W. (2006). On parametric continuities in the
validity of three common assumptions. Perspectives world of binary either ors. Psychological Inquiry, 17,
on Psychological Science, 2, 181-193. 153-165.
Gawronski, B., Peters, K. R., & LeBel, E. P. (2008). Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L L. (1994). Stroop process·
What makes mental associations personal or extra~ dissociations: The relationship between facilita-
personal?: Conceptual issues in the methodological tion and interference. journal of Experimental Psy~
debate about implicit attitude measures. Social and chology: Human Perception and Performance, 20,
Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1002-1023. 219-234.
Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2004 ). On the proposi- Machado, A., & Silva, F. j. (2007). Toward a richer view
tional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance of the scientific method: The role of conceptual anal~
changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of ysis. American Psychologist, 62, 671-681.
Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 535-542. Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995 ). A cognitive-affective
Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking hghtly about other" system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing
Automatic components of the social inference pro- situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance
cess. ln J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh {Eds.), Unintend~ in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102,
ed thought (pp. 189-211). New York' Guilford Press. 246-268.
Govorun, 0., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego~depletion and Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition,
prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled com~ and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and
ponents. Social Cognition, 24, 111-136. suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. ( 1995). Implicit and Social Psychology, 64, 723-739.
social cognition: Attitudes, self~esteem, and stereo- Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from
types. Psychological Review, 102, 4--27. lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading
Greenwald, A G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. activation and ..limited-capacity attention. Journal of
(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit Experimental Psychology' General, 106, 226-254.
cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565-584.
& Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the cor- Nosek, B. A., & BanaJi, M. R. (2001). The Go/No-Go
relation between the Implicit Association Test and Association Task. SociaL Cognition, 19, 625-666.
explicit self-report measure. Personality and Social Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. (Z008a). The associations
Psychology Bulletm, 31, 1369-1385. in our head belong to us: Searching for attitudes
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A, & and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and
Schmitt, M. (2005). What moderates implicit~explicit Emotion, 22, 553-594.
consistency? European Review of Social Psychology, Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. (2008b). Personalizing the
16, 335-390. Implicit Association Test increases explicit evalua~
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: tion of target concepts. European journal of Psycho~
Separating automatic from intentional uses of memo- logical Assessment, 24, 226-236.
ry. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513-541. Olson, M. A, & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the in-
Jacoby, L L, & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship fluence of extra~personal associations on the Implicit
between autobiographical memory and perceptual Association Test: Personalizing the lAT. Journal of
learning. Journal of Experimemal Psychology: General, Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653-667.
110, 306-340. Olson, M. A., Fazio, R. H., & Han, H. A. (2009). Con-
1. History of Implicit Social Cognition 15

cep(Ualizing personal and extrapersonal associa- Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation
tions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, in context. Social Cognition, 25, 638-656.
152-170. Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (2001). The construction of
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role attitudes. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.}, Blackwell
of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiv- handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes
ing a weapon. Journal of Penonality and Social Psy- (pp. 436-457). Malden, MA. Blackwell.
chology, 81, 181-192. Sherman, J. (2006). On building a better process model:
Payne, B. K. (2005). Concep(Ualizing control in social It's not only how many, but which ones and by which
cognition: How executive control modulates the ex- means/ Psychological Inquiry, 17, 173-184.
pression of automatic stereotyping. Journal of Person- Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Gonsalkorale, K., Hugen-
ality and Social Psychology, 89, 488-503. berg, K., Allen, T. )., & Groom, C. j. (ZOOS). The self-
Payne, B. K., & Bishara, A. j. (2009). An integrative regulation of automatic associations and behavioral
review of process dissociation and related models in impulses. Psychological Review, 115, 314-335.
social cognition. European Review of Social Psychol- Shiffrin, R., & Schne~der, W. (1977). Controlled and
ogy, 20, 272-314. automatic human information processing: II. Per-
Payne, B. K., Burkley, M., & Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why ceptual learning, automatic attending and a general
do implicit and explicit attitude tests diverge?: The theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127-190.
role of structural fi.t. journal of Personality and Social Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, j. (2000). Dual-process mod-
Psychology, 94, 16-31. els in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual
Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, 0., & Stewart, B. integration and links to underlying memory systems.
(2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribu- Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108-131.
tion as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality Stahl, C., & Degner,]. (2007). Assessing automatic ac-
and Social Psychology, 89, 277-293. tivation of valence: A multinomial model of EAST
Payne, B. K., Jacoby, L. L., & Lambert, A. j. (2005). performance. Experimental Psychology, 54, 99-112.
Attitudes as accessibility bias: Dissociating auto- Stewart, B. 0., & Payne, B. K. (2008). Bringing auto-
matic and controlled components. In R. R. Hassin, matic stereotyping under control: Implementation
j. S. Uleman, & j. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new uncon- intentions as efficient means of thought controL
scious (pp. 393-420). Oxford, UK• Oxford Umversity Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1332-
Press. 1345
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, j. T. (1986). The elaboration Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impul-
likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), sive determinants of social behavior. Personality and
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, Social Psychology Review, 8, 220-247.
pp. 123-205). New York: Academic Press. Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Rotherffiund, K.
Popper, K. R. (1934). Logic of scientific discovery. New (2008). Minimizing method-specific variance in the
York: Basic Books. lAT. The single block lAT. European journal of Psy-
Posner, M. 1., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and chological Assessment, 24, 237-245.
cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective
processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hills- attention. Psychological Review, 76, 282-299.
dale, N)> Erlbaum. Trope, Y. (1986).ldentificarion and inferential processes
Rothermund, K., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Gast, A., & in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93,
Wentura, D. (2009). Minimizing the influence of re- 239-257
ceding in the IAT: The Recoding-Free Implicit As- Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1968). A new
sociation Test (IAT-RF). Quarterly Journal of Experi- method of testing long-term retention with special
mental Psychology, 62, 84-98. reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972-974.
Rothermund, K., & Wenrura, D. (2001). Figure-ground Wigboldus, D. H. )., Holland, R. W., & van Knippen-
asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). berg, A. (2004 ). Si~gle target implicit associations. Un-
Zeitschrift fUr Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 94-106. published manuscript.
Rydell, R. )., & Gawronski, B. (2009). I like you, I like Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A
you not: Understanding the formation of context- model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107,
dependent automatic attitudes. Cognition and Emo- 101-126.
tion, 23, 1118-1152. Wittenbrink, B., judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evi-
Rydell, R. )., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understand- dence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its
ing implicit and explicit attitude change: A systems relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of
of reasoning analysis. journal of Personality and Social Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274.
Psychology, 91, 995-1008. Wittenbrink, B., judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Sponta-
Schacter, D. (1987). Implicit memory: History and cur- neous prejudice in context: Variability in automati-
rent status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- cally activated attitudes. journal of Personality and
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501-518. Social Psychology, 81, 815-827.

You might also like