Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 135

WHY DO MOMMY AND DADDY LOVE YOU MORE?

AN INVESTIGATION OF
PARENTAL FAVORITISM FROM AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE

Anthony Michael Lauricella, Ph.D.

A Dissertation

Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green


State University in partial fulfillment of
the requirements for the degree of

SELECT ONE:

December 2009

Committee:

Gordon, Anne K., Advisor

Jorge M. Chavez
Graduate Faculty Representative

Eric Dubow

Richard Anderson
ii

ABSTRACT

Anne K. Gordon, Advisor

The current study examined the roles of parental certainty and offspring’s ability to

convert parental investment into fitness benefits for the parent (via social competitiveness) in

predicting parental favoritism. Participants were college students who had at least one fully-

biological sibling. Participants completed a series of on-line questionnaires that assessed their

personal experiences regarding parental favoritism (or non-favoritism) with each biological

parent. Additionally, they completed questionnaires that assessed their levels of health,

intelligence, ambition, physical attractiveness, and parental resemblance relative to their sibling.

As expected, results indicated that paternal resemblance predicted paternal favoritism, whereas

maternal resemblance did not predict maternal favoritism. Additionally, in partial support of a

hypothesis, fathers were shown to demonstrate favoritism in more specific areas than were

mothers. Contrary to predictions, health, offspring intelligence, ambition, and physical

attractiveness did not consistently predict parental favoritism. Discussion centers on the

implications of the findings, offers possible explanations regarding why certain expected results

were not obtained, and makes recommendations for future research.


iii

In the time of chimpanzees, I was a monkey.

-Beck
iv

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

First, I would like to express my gratitude to my advisor Anne K. Gordon, Ph.D. whose

patience, encouragement, and thoughtful comments provided me with the necessary support

throughout this arduous process. I would like to thank my committee members Jorge Chavez,

Ph.D., Eric Dubow, Ph.D., and Richard Anderson, Ph.D. whose challenging questions and

comments fostered a honing of my research skills. Additionally, I thank fellow graduate students

and other faculty members who helped shape my graduate experience. Lastly, I would like to

thank my supportive friends and family. Yes, mom and dad, I am all done now.
v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................................. 1

AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE OF PARENTAL CARE....................................... 4

Genetic Relatedness to Offspring .............................................................................. 6

Paternal certainty ........................................................................................... 7

OFFSPRING ABILITY TO CONVERT PARENTAL CARE INTO FITNESS.................. 13

Health ............................................................................................................ 13

Social competitiveness............................................................................................... 17

Characteristics associated with male reproductive success ....................................... 18

Characteristics associated with female reproductive success .................................... 22

THE PRESENT STUDY ....................................................................................................... 25

HYPOTHESES ............................................................................................................ 27

METHOD ............................................................................................................ 31

Participants ............................................................................................................ 31

Materials and Measures ............................................................................................. 32

Online questionnaires..................................................................................... 32

Informed consent ........................................................................................... 32

Instructions..................................................................................................... 32

Differential parental treatment sub-scale of the Sibling Inventory of Differential

Experience (SIDE) ......................................................................................... 33

Forced-choice and obviousness parental favoritism items ............................ 34

Relative physical resemblance to parents ...................................................... 35


vi

Relative health ............................................................................................... 36

Relative social competitiveness ..................................................................... 37

Relative intelligence....................................................................................... 38

Relative ambition and industriousness........................................................... 38

Relative physical attractiveness ..................................................................... 39

Demographics ................................................................................................ 40

Post-study questionnaire ................................................................................ 40

Debriefing ...................................................................................................... 40

Questionnaire order........................................................................................ 40

Procedure ............................................................................................................ 41

Debriefing ...................................................................................................... 42

RESULTS ............................................................................................................ 43

Overview ............................................................................................................ 43

Counterbalancing ........................................................................................... 43

Descriptive Statistics.................................................................................................. 43

Parental favoritism ......................................................................................... 43

Possible Correlates of Parental Favoritism ................................................................ 45

Sex differences and dyad composition effects............................................... 45

Number of fully biological siblings ............................................................... 47

Sibling hierarchy............................................................................................ 47

Age gap between siblings .............................................................................. 48

Summary ........................................................................................................ 48

Primary Analyses ....................................................................................................... 49


vii

Maternal versus paternal favoritism............................................................... 49

Physical resemblance and favoritism............................................................. 52

Health ............................................................................................................ 53

Social competitiveness................................................................................... 54

Summary and Conclusions ........................................................................................ 59

DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................ 61

Support for hypotheses .............................................................................................. 61

Hypotheses that were not supported .......................................................................... 63

Concluding remarks ................................................................................................... 70

REFERENCES ...................................................................................................................... 72

APPENDIX A. CONSENT FORM ....................................................................................... 88

APPENDIX B. INSTRUCTIONS ......................................................................................... 90

APPENDIX C. SIBLING INVENTORY OF DIFFERENTIAL EXPERIENCE (SIDE)..... 91

APPENDIX D. FORCED-CHOICE AND OBVIOUSNESS ITEM..................................... 94

APPENDIX E. RESEMBLANCE SCALE ........................................................................... 95

APPENDIX F. GENERAL HEALTH................................................................................... 97

APPENDIX G. INTELLIGENCE MEASURE .................................................................... 98

APPENDIX H. AMBITION/INDUSTRIOUSNESS MEASURE ........................................ 100

APPENDIX I. PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS MEASURE............................................ 102

APPENDIX J. DEMOGRAPHICS FORM ........................................................................... 103

APPENDIX K. POST-EXPERIMENTAL QUESTIONNAIRE........................................... 104

APPENDIX L. DEBRIEFING FORM .................................................................................. 105

APPENDIX M. EXPERIMETRIX ANNOUNCEMENT ..................................................... 107


viii

APPENDIX N. EXPLORATORY ANALYSES................................................................... 108


ix

LIST OF FIGURES/TABLES

Figure/Table Page

1 Means and Standard Deviations on Outcome Measures as a Function of Participant Sex

.............................................................................................................................. 111

2 Means and Standard Deviations of Physical Health Composite Index as a Function of

Participant Sex and Sibling Sex................................................................................. 112

3 Means and Standard Deviations of Intelligence Composite Index as a Function of

Participant Sex and Sibling Sex................................................................................. 113

4 Means and Standard Deviations on Dependent Measures as a Function of Sibling

Hierarchy ............................................................................................................ 114

5 Means and Standard Deviations of Re-coded SIDEs as a Function of Parental Sex. 115

6 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Physical Resemblance and Original SIDE Items

............................................................................................................ 116

7 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Physical Health and Original Maternal and

Paternal SIDE Items................................................................................................... 117

8 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Intelligence and Original

Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items ............................................................................ 118

9 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Intelligence and Original

Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items ............................................................................ 119

10 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Ambition/Industriousness

and Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items ....................................................... 120

11 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Ambition/Industriousness

and Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items ....................................................... 121


x

12 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Physical Attractiveness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items .............................................................. 122

13 Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Physical Attractiveness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items .............................................................. 123

14 Bivariate Correlations Between Outcome and Predictor Variables across all Participants

............................................................................................................ 124

15 Means and Standard Deviations of Original SIDE Items as a Function of Parental Sex

............................................................................................................ 125
Parental Favoritism 1

INTRODUCTION

Parental favoritism, defined as the preference of a parent for one child over another

(Harris & Howard, 2001), has been a source of vexation for children throughout history. Famous

cases of parental favoritism have been noted throughout literature (e.g., the Biblical story of Cain

and Abel) as well as film (e.g., Cinderella). However, parental favoritism is not a phenomenon

limited to the past. Perceptions of parental favoritism, whether accurate or illusory, continue to

be reported by both parents and children (e.g., Suitor & Pillemer, 2007). For example, 36% 1

(Harris & Howard, 1985) to 92% (Suitor, Sechrist, Steinhour, & Pillemer, 2006) of adolescent

and adult children report perceiving parental favoritism. Furthermore, many parents report

differentially displaying affection, pride, and disappointment towards their children (Brackbill,

Kitch, & Noffsinger, 1988; Brody & Stoneman, 1994). For example, Suitor et al. (2006) found

that in a sample of over 700 mother-child dyads, 68% of mothers reported that they were

emotionally closer to one of their children, and 84% identified a child they would first approach

when confronted with a personal problem. Thus, parental favoritism is a prevalent phenomenon

that is reported by both parents and children.

Despite the prevalence of parental favoritism, few theories have been offered to explain

it. Alfred Adler (1932) has proposed the most prominent theory of parental favoritism. He

theorized that an individual’s psychological position 2 into which they are born and during which

they experienced their formative years of childhood influenced their psychological make-up. He

1
Harris and Howard’s lower prevalence rate (36%) was found only for one particular subgroup (two-child families).
When examining larger families, their prevalence rates are in accordance with other estimates.
2
Adler believed that one’s psychological position into which they were born was more important than their ordinal
position. He believed five psychological positions existed: only child, first child, second child, middle child, and
youngest child (For Review, See Kiracofe and Kiracofe, 1990).
Parental Favoritism 2

believed that oldest children were most likely to develop neuroticism due to feeling responsible

for younger siblings, as well as feeling “dethroned” by the birth of a sibling. Adler also believed

that youngest children were most likely to develop grandiose feelings about themselves due to

never being “dethroned” by the birth of a sibling, and that parents were most likely to

overindulge the youngest child. Finally, Adler believed that middle children were most likely to

develop even-tempered personalities and become successful individuals because they would

neither feel “dethroned” nor overindulged.

Adler’s theorizing on this topic has prompted a number of researchers to examine the role

of birth order regarding parental favoritism. Yet, findings from more than 50 years of birth order

research demonstrate few meaningful results beyond some indication that last-born children are

most likely to be favored by parents (Brody, Stoneman, & Burke, 1987; Bryant & Crockenberg,

1980; Dunn & Plomin, 1991; Tucker, McHale, & Crouter, 2003). Despite the dearth of findings

from this line of research, researchers continue to explore the relationship between birth order

and parental favoritism. This state of affairs is problematic because birth order is thought to

account for only 1-2% of the variance regarding parental favoritism (Daniels, Dunn,

Fursternberg, and Plomin, 1985).

In the current research, I investigated parental favoritism from an evolutionary

perspective. In doing so, I hoped to provide a more modern and useful framework for examining

parental favoritism. For the purposes of this study, I defined parental favoritism as a child’s

perception of systematic bias by a parent in the direction of one child in the areas of financial

(e.g., father buys me gifts) and psychological (e.g., mother supports me) assistance.

In my review of the literature, I focus on two factors identified by evolutionary

psychologists that might influence the activation and expression of psychological mechanisms
Parental Favoritism 3

associated with parenting behaviors. First, I review the role of genetic relatedness (i.e., degree to

which parent and offspring are genetically related), focusing specifically on the relationship

between paternal certainty and genetic relatedness. Based upon this review, I hypothesized that

mothers and fathers possess different psychological mechanisms associated with assessing the

genetic relatedness (i.e., paternal certainty) of offspring. In particular, I focus on psychological

mechanisms distinct to males that appear to be activated by cues associated with genetic

relatedness (e.g., resemblance of child to father) that influence parenting-related judgments and

decisions. I examine how these relatedness-assessment mechanisms might result in more

favoritism being displayed by fathers than mothers.

Second, I discuss the notion that an offspring’s ability to convert parental care into fitness

benefits for the parent (via later reproductive success) might predict various forms of parenting

behavior. Along these lines, I will review the literature on characteristics (e.g., physical health)

known to be associated with fitness. I will also discuss a previously un-researched child

characteristic, social competitiveness, which might indicate a greater likelihood of a child

converting parental care into fitness benefits for the parent. I end the section by proposing

several hypotheses regarding how characteristics of children that indicate (a) parental certainty or

(b) their ability to convert parental care into fitness, might be associated with parental favoritism.
Parental Favoritism 4

AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE OF PARENTAL CARE

Richard Dawkins (1972) coined the phrase selfish 3 gene to highlight the point that natural

selection occurs at the level of the gene, rather than at the level of the organism. Genes that

account for traits that tend to benefit the organism’s survival or reproductive success will be

passed on to future generations. Organism’s are the cumulative products of their genes in

interaction with the environment and, metaphorically speaking, serve as vehicles for their genes.

Because offspring share 50% of a parent’s genes (r = genetic relatedness; in this case r = .50) a

child, too, might be thought of as a parent’s “gene vehicle.” In fact, offspring represent optimal

“genetic vehicles” for parents because offspring share the largest percentage of a parent’s genes

compared with other kin. For example, grandchildren, nieces, and nephews share 25% of one's

genes; first cousins share 12.5% of genes, etc. 4 . Therefore, offspring survival and reproductive

success is paramount to a parent's continued fitness. Thus, natural selection typically favors

mechanisms associated with the desire for children and for mechanisms associated with

thoughts, feelings, and, ultimately, behaviors that increase the likelihood of offspring survival

and later reproductive success.

Parents contribute to the fitness of their offspring in a variety of ways. Parents provide

material resources (e.g., food, shelter), as well as psychological resources (e.g., attention,

protection). Any contribution made by parents to their children that increases the fitness of

offspring can be categorized under the term “parental care” (Clutton-Brock, 1991). Because they

are born in an altricial state and have an extended childhood and adolescence, human offspring

3
Dawkins did not choose the word “selfish” in order to imply that genes have motivations. Metaphorically, he is
stating that genes are designed, by natural selection, to do what is in their best interest in terms of being replicated.
4
See William Hamilton’s (1964) inclusive fitness theory for further understanding of this matter.
Parental Favoritism 5

require significant parental care over a rather extended time period. As a result, parents allocate a

substantial amount of tangible (e.g., food) and intangible (e.g., attention) resources towards

offspring. In terms of time, energy, and resource allocation, caring for offspring can be a very

costly endeavor for parents. In fact, few species invest in the growth and development of

offspring beyond the production of ova and sperm due to the costs associated with parental care

(Clutton-Brock, 1991). Humans, though, represent one of the few species that heavily invest in

the development of offspring.

Robert Trivers (1972) coined the term parental investment to highlight the costs

experienced by parents due to parental care. Trivers originally defined parental investment as any

cost associated with offspring that reduces a parent’s ability to produce or invest in other

offspring. The definition was subsequently broadened to include costs invested in a child that

reduce a parent's ability to invest in any additional fitness-related pursuits (Clutton-Brock, 1991).

For example, time spent towards childcare is time that cannot be spent on acquiring additional

resources, or attracting and securing additional mates. Due to costs associated with parental

investment, as well as limited investment quantities (e.g., time, food), parents necessarily and

regularly make decisions about how to distribute resources amongst themselves, their offspring,

and other kin.

Because each child shares 50% of a parent’s genes; offspring appear to possess equal

fitness opportunities for the parent. Therefore, parents might be expected to invest equally in all

offspring. However, this is not the case. Reproductive value refers to the expected probability of

reproduction of an individual from their current age onward, given the fact that they have

survived to their current age (Fisher, 1930). If offspring were of identical reproductive value

natural selection would favor an equitable distribution of parental care (Daly & Wilson, 1990). In
Parental Favoritism 6

other words, if all offspring were equally likely to bear the same number of equally fit children

(equal reproductive success), then parents would tend to invest equally amongst all offspring.

However, based upon a number of factors (e.g., physical health), offspring within a family often

vary in their reproductive values. For example, a child with a congenital abnormality who is

unlikely to reach sexual maturity possesses a much lower reproductive value than a child who is

born healthy. Greater investment by the parent in the healthier children is beneficial to the parent

because the healthier child is more likely to capitalize on the parents' investment via later

reproductive success.

Because reproductive success of offspring increases the fitness of parents, parents might

invest more in offspring who are more likely to be reproductively successful. Were parents to

indiscriminately invest in all offspring equally, they would experience significant cost and less-

than-optimal fitness benefits. Thus, through the process of natural selection, psychological

mechanisms are thought to have evolved in parents that favor offspring who are likely to provide

a higher reproductive return on the investment (Daly & Wilson, 1995). These mechanisms result

in the condition known as parental favoritism. These ideas served as the theoretical basis for the

current research.

Genetic Relatedness to Offspring

Genetically-related offspring represent maximally efficient “vehicles” of genetic

transmission for parents because they share 50% of a parent’s genes. Because of the importance

(from an evolutionary perspective) of passing one’s genes on to future generations, evolutionary

psychologists have hypothesized that parents will invest more, on average, in genetically-related

than genetically-unrelated children.


Parental Favoritism 7

In support of this hypothesis, research has repeatedly demonstrated a relationship

between the r between parents and offspring and the amount of parental care delivered by

parents to offspring (See Buss, 2008). For example, in one study, fathers reported providing

more than five times the amount of financial assistance to genetically-related offspring than to

step-children (Anderson, Kaplan, & Lancaster, 1999). Differences regarding resource allocation

are found amongst emotional resources as well. In another study, 53% of stepfathers and only

25% of stepmothers reported having “parental feelings” towards their stepchildren (Duberman,

1975) 5 . Thus, research has repeatedly supported the evolutionary psychology prediction that

parents invest more in children with whom they share a genetic relatedness. Therefore, it appears

that genetic relatedness influences parental investment with individuals of higher genetic

relatedness being more likely to receive parental investment.

Paternal certainty. Among humans, ovulation is concealed (relative to many other

primate species), and the fertilization process occurs internally within women. Thus, human

males cannot fully ascertain if and when females are fertile or if and when fertilization has

occurred. Further complicating this matter for males is that a male’s romantic partner may

become pregnant by another male through acts of infidelity (cuckoldry). Due to concealed

ovulation, internal fertilization, and female infidelity, males, unlike females, can never be 100%

certain regarding paternity (i.e., the genetic relatedness of the child). For example, in a random

sample of 35 to 45-year-old American women, 20% reported engaging in at least one extra

5
One may be surprised to see that females reported fewer parental feelings than males, as females, on average,
provide more care to children than males. However, this study only examined “parental feelings.” Therefore, one
should not conclude that stepmothers are more likely to treat children more harshly than stepfathers. To highlight
this point, research has repeatedly demonstrated that stepfathers are, on average, more likely than stepmothers to
commit more severe forms of parental neglect, such as physical abuse of stepchildren (Adler-Baeder, 2006).
Parental Favoritism 8

martial affair (Essock-Vitale & McGuire, 1988). Furthermore, current estimates of paternity by

someone other than the putative and domestic father are between 1 and 30% (Baker & Bellis,

1995). According to an analysis of 280,000 paternity tests conducted in 1999 by the American

Association of Blood Banks, approximately 30% of children were fathered by extra-pair

copulations (Platek & Shackelford, 2006).

Thus, cuckoldry and paternal uncertainty represent significant adaptive problems for

males. Failing to detect cuckoldry might result in the male investing substantial resources in a

child with whom he shares no genetic relatedness. A male’s reproductive success and fitness

level would decrease were he to invest limited, valuable resources in another man’s child (Buss,

2008). For example, resources invested in another man’s child can no longer be invested in one’s

own offspring. Furthermore, a cuckolded male who invests in a child with whom he shares no

genetic relatedness increases the fitness of rival males.

Because cuckoldry and paternal uncertainty represent adaptive problems that males have

always faced, natural selection has favored psychological and physiological mechanisms in

males that have helped solve these problems. Psychological mechanisms that might have evolved

in males to help combat the adaptive problem of cuckoldry include sexual jealousy, mate

guarding, and a preference for chaste women (Buss, 2007). For example, a male who regularly

guards his mate, especially when rival males are nearby, is less likely to fall victim to cuckoldry

than a male who is unaware of his mate’s general whereabouts. Physiological mechanisms, such

as those that increase sperm volume and, thus, aid in sperm competitions, have also evolved to

help solve the problem of cuckoldry (see Shackelford & Goetz, 2006).

Kin-discrimination mechanisms are mechanisms that help individuals distinguish

between offspring that are more or less likely to be theirs. These mechanisms are also thought to
Parental Favoritism 9

have evolved in males to combat the adaptive problem of paternal uncertainty. A male who is

successful in inferring the relatedness of putative offspring is more likely to invest valuable

resources in those children with whom he shares a genetic relatedness and less likely to invest

resources in those children with whom he is not genetically related. Men who were able to make

such distinctions were likely to have greater reproductive fitness than men who were unable to

do so. Over many generations, these beneficial kin-discrimination mechanisms would spread

throughout the male population.

Along these lines, Platek, Burch, Panyavin, Wasserman, and Gallup (2002) provide

evidence that males possess kin-discrimination mechanisms that might influence their investment

in children. These authors investigated the relationship of facial resemblance between adults and

children and judgments regarding parental investment. Platek et al. (2002) noted that one method

used by males to assess the probability that children they are caring for are, in fact, their

genetically-related children is to infer relatedness via degree of facial resemblance between

themselves and the children. With parental uncertainty not representing an adaptive problem for

females, they were not expected to have evolved mechanisms associated with inferring

relatedness of offspring.

Based on this logic, Platek et al. (2002) hypothesized that perceived paternal resemblance

with a child would influence parental investment more strongly than would perceived maternal

resemblance with a child. In their study, 20 male and 20 female participants were exposed to

photo arrays of children’s faces. Unbeknownst to participants, one photo in the array was a

morphed image of the participant’s face and a child’s face (self-child morph). The remaining

photos were of other participants’ faces that had been morphed with a photo of a child’s face

(other-child morphs). During exposure to a group of these photos, participants were asked a
Parental Favoritism 10

series of questions. They were asked to indicate which of the children (a) would you be most

willing to adopt?; (b) do you find to be most attractive?; (c) would you be comfortable spending

the most time with?; (d) would you spend the least time with?; (e) would you spend fifty dollars

on if you could only spend on one child?; (f) would you spend fifty dollars on last?; (g) would

you punish most if they damaged something valuable of yours?; (h) would you punish least?; (i)

would you most resent having to pay child support for?; and (j) would you least resent having to

pay child support for?

Consistent with predictions, results indicated that males were significantly more likely

than females to select their self-child morphed image on the following questions: (a) which child

would you most likely adopt (males = 90%, females = 35%; p < .001), (b) find most attractive

(males = 85%, females = 35%; p < .001), (c) be most comfortable spending time with (males =

70%, females = 35%; p < .01), and (d) spend fifty dollars on if you could only spend it on one

child (males = 80%, females = 40%; p < .01). Furthermore, on each of the remaining items males

were more likely than females to choose their own self-child morph, though these results did not

achieve statistical significance. Thus, men, compared to women, appeared to be more sensitive to

cues related to genetic relatedness, communicated in the form of facial resemblance, when

making judgments pertaining to parental investment. Interestingly, no participant when queried

following the experiment, cited child resemblance as a factor in their judgments. This suggests

that whereas physical resemblance appears to influence males’ investment decisions, they are not

necessarily aware of this influence.

In a second study, Platek, Keenan, and Mohammed (2005) demonstrated a neural basis

for the proposed sex differences regarding kin discrimination via facial resemblance. Using

fMRI, they showed that males display greater cortical activity than females in response to
Parental Favoritism 11

children’s faces that resembled them. In particular, males displayed greater brain activation in

areas associated with attentional and decision-making processes (e.g., the anterior cingulate).

Together, these findings suggest that males might possess unique psychological mechanisms

regarding the ability to detect physical resemblance of self to a child, and that these mechanisms

appear to influence judgments related to paternal investment.

This laboratory research by Platek and his colleagues is complemented by other more

ecologically-valid research that also suggests that the degree to which a putative father believes a

child looks like him influences paternal investment (Burch & Gallup, 2000). For example, 53

men who were currently participating in a domestic violence treatment program completed

questionnaires wherein they rated the degree of physical similarity between themselves and their

child. Results indicated a significant positive correlation (r = .60) between the degree to which a

male believed the child looked like him and the self-reported quality of the relationship between

father and child. In other words, fathers reported having more positive relationships with

children with whom they perceived a greater self-resemblance. Additionally, fathers who

reported having heard more often that the child looked like him reported more positive

relationships with the child (r = .54). Furthermore, when these same participants were asked

about their own relationship with their father, participants who reported looking more like their

father reported closer relationships with their father (r = .40). Thus, it appears that a man’s

perception of the physical similarity between himself and a child is associated with his

investment in the child, and that this investment translates into the quality of the relationship

between the parties.

In sum, genetically-related offspring represent optimal “vehicles” for transmission of

one's genes. Consequently, parents are expected to and have been shown to invest greater
Parental Favoritism 12

resources in children with whom they share a greater genetic relatedness. For females, genetic

relatedness of offspring is a certainty. For males, genetic relatedness is not certain. Instead males

must infer relatedness. Therefore, women know that any parental investment they provide is

given to genetically-related offspring, whereas males are uncertain of this. For these reasons, I

expected participants to report that mothers are, on average, more equitable regarding parental

investment than fathers. In other words, I expected participants to report that fathers show more

parental favoritism than mothers.

Males have always faced the adaptive problems of cuckoldry and paternal certainty. If

males were to indiscriminately invest equally amongst all putative offspring they could

experience significant costs because some offspring might be the product of cuckoldry. It has

been argued that males have evolved psychological and physiological mechanisms that assist

them in solving these problems. In particular, males appear to possess unique kin-discrimination

mechanisms, assisting them in inferring the relatedness of a child. Findings from Platek et al’s.

(2002) "hypothetical investment" study and Burch and Gallup’s (2000) examination of fathers

indicate that facial resemblance serves as a valuable indicator of relatedness for males. Males

who perceive facial resemblance in a child appear to invest more in the child than males who

perceive little facial resemblance with the child. In the current research, I extended this line of

reasoning by examining favoritism from the perspective of the child. Specifically, I expected

participants' perceptions regarding degree of physical resemblance between themselves and

parent (father and mother, respectively) to be more strongly associated with paternal than

maternal favoritism. Furthermore, I expected those who reported a greater degree of paternal

physical resemblance to more often report receiving paternal favoritism. I did not expect a
Parental Favoritism 13

significant correlation between degree of maternal physical resemblance and maternal

favoritism.

Offspring Ability to Convert Parental Care into Fitness

One’s reproductive success is arguably the most critical measure of one’s fitness.

Survival, for example, contributes to fitness only as a necessary precondition for mating and

child rearing. Parental investment is associated with an offspring’s later reproductive success,

because offspring are unlikely to survive and less likely to thrive and be reproductively

successful without receiving many forms of investment from parents. However, children within

the same family are not equally likely to convert a parent’s investment into fitness benefits for

the parent via their later reproductive success. In other words, children, based on their unique

personal characteristics, vary regarding their potential reproductive success.

Certain aspects of children, such as their health, personality, and intelligence might

influence their ability to convert parental investment into fitness gains for the parent. Stated

differently, certain children are more likely to survive and be reproductively successful based on

the characteristics (e.g., health, physical attractiveness) they possess. Investment of limited,

valuable resources in children who are unlikely to survive or be reproductively successful comes

at a substantial cost to the parent. However, investment in children who are most likely to

capitalize on the investment (i.e., translate investment into fitness benefits for the parents) often

represents an optimal investment strategy for parents. Therefore, selection would have favored

adaptations in parents that result in greater parental investment (favoritism) in children who are

more likely to convert investment into fitness benefits for the parent(s) (Buss, 2008).

Health. Health of the child appears to be a primary factor associated with a child’s ability

to convert investment into fitness benefits for the parent (Daly & Wilson, 1988). Children with
Parental Favoritism 14

congenital abnormalities are severely compromised regarding reproductive success, because they

are less likely to survive childhood and reach sexual maturity. Compared to children with

congenital abnormalities, “healthy” children are more likely to survive beyond childhood and

early adolescence, reach sexual maturity, find suitable mating opportunities, and be

reproductively successful. Consequently, compared to children with congenital abnormalities,

healthy children are more likely to benefit from parental investment and translate the investment

into fitness benefits for the parent.

Along similar lines, the literature on mate preferences suggests that healthy individuals,

compared to less healthy individuals, are more likely to be more desired as a mate and ultimately

be more reproductively successful. Cross culturally, both men and women consider health to be

an important and highly desired trait in a mate. In an international sample, both men and women

ranked health fourth most important among a list of 13 traits that may characterize a long-term

mate (Buss, 1989). Individuals might desire healthy mates because healthy mates are less likely

to transfer communicable diseases or genes associated with poor health to offspring (Buss,

Abbott, Angleitner, Asherian, Biaggio, & 45 other co-authors, 1990). Healthy mates are also

desirable because, compared to unhealthy individuals, they are more likely to be able to provide

adaptive benefits such as food and protection over an extended time period. Healthy mates are

also less likely to impose costs in the form of being a drain of resources (e.g., time, care,

attention). Because being in good health is highly desired by both sexes regarding mates, healthy

individuals can afford to be more selective in their mate choices and, through the process of

assortative mating (see Shackelford, Schmitt, & Buss, 2005), might therefore attract highly

desirable mates. Thus, health status serves as an important indicator of potential reproductive

success. Therefore, from a fitness perspective, it is typically more beneficial for parents to
Parental Favoritism 15

allocate resources to healthier children, as these children are more likely to capitalize on the

resource investment via survival and reproductive success 6 .

Studies examining the relationship between a child’s health and parental care indicate

that healthier children are, indeed, more likely to receive parental investment than less healthy

children (Daly & Wilson, 1988; Mann, 1992). Several researchers have demonstrated that

premature infants and children with health problems receive less attention and affection from

parents (Baratt, Roach, & Leavitt, 1996; Hagen, 1999; Mann, 1992). Mann (1992) examined

maternal responses to twins who were born prematurely. Her results indicated that mothers

kissed, held, soothed, talked to, and played with the “healthier” twin significantly more so than

with the other child. Furthermore, Mann found that the different maternal responses received by

the twins increased over time and that when the infants were eight months old, all mothers in the

study directed more positive maternal behaviors towards the healthier twin.

In a study examining hypothetical willingness to adopt, Volk, Lukjanczuk, and Quinsey

(2005) found that participants reported a significantly lower likelihood of "adopting" children

whose faces were digitally altered to simulate cues of low body weight (an indicator of poor

health), than children whose faces were not digitally altered. Research also demonstrates that

children with congenital abnormalities are often institutionalized, wherein they are never or

rarely visited 7 (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1978), and that children with congenital

6
Parents might not be fully aware of the reasons behind their resource allocation decisions. Many parents would
deny intentionally allocating fewer resources to a child just because the child is sick. Evolved psychological
mechanisms that become activated by the context of an unhealthy child occur rapidly, outside of one’s awareness
and are most likely mediated by affect (e.g., feeling more frustrated by an unhealthy child).
7
The 1976 United States Census found that roughly 12% (16,000) of institutionalized children were never visited by
parents and, an additional 22% (30,000) were visited by parents only once a year or less.
Parental Favoritism 16

abnormalities are more likely to be abused than are children without any abnormalities (Daly &

Wilson, 1988).

This body of research on children’s health and its relationship with parenting behaviors is

important and compelling. However, this research (e.g., Mann, 1992) has typically treated health

status of a child in a dichotomous manner, in which people are categorized as being either

healthy or unhealthy. Moreover, this research has focused on the tail ends of the health-status

distribution, as it has compared the treatment of healthy children with noticeably and often

severely unhealthy children. In the current research, I measured participant's health on a

continuous rather than a dichotomous scale. Doing so allowed health status to be measured in a

manner that more accurately reflects the manner in which health status is distributed among the

general population. Furthermore, treating health status on a continuum allowed me to examine

the potentially more subtle role of a child's health status as a predictor of how that child is treated

by his/her parents. Additionally, because I examined the full range of health outcomes

(unhealthy to healthy) my study demonstrated a good degree of ecological and external validity.

Moreover, the current research differed from previous research on this topic because I examined

the relationship between a child's health and his/her self-reports regarding parental favoritism,

which, to date, had not been done.

In sum, it is typically more beneficial for parents to invest in children who are likely to

translate the investment into fitness benefits for the parent (via later reproductive success).

Research suggests that health is a primary factor associated with a child’s ability to convert

investment into fitness benefits for the parent (Daly & Wilson, 1988). Children without health

problems are more likely to survive childhood and adolescence, thus reaching their sexual

maturity, than are children with health concerns. Additionally, health is a desirable trait in a
Parental Favoritism 17

mate. Consequently, healthy individuals are, on average, more likely to be reproductively

successful than are less healthy individuals. Therefore, parents are expected to invest more in

healthier children, because these children are more likely to be reproductively successful. For

these reasons, I expected healthier children to be more likely to report having been favored by

their parents.

Social competitiveness. Compared with other mammals, humans possess a

disproportionately large brain (relative to body mass), as well as a long developmental period.

Geary and Flinn (2001) argue that there is an association between brain size, length of

developmental period, and an organism’s social network. They cogently argue that one function

of a lengthy developmental period is to allow offspring to learn the complexities of social life

and to refine strategies that are necessary to enhance one’s status and, therefore, one’s

reproductive success. Essentially, Geary and Flinn argue that individuals need to be socially

competitive (i.e., possess characteristics likely to elevate one’s hierarchical position within a

group) in order to be reproductively successful.

All known human groups are characterized by social hierarchies (Lund, Tamnes,

Moestue, Buss, & Vollrath, 2007), wherein people are ranked according to their ability or status.

Specific characteristics are valued within each type of social hierarchy. For example, intelligence

is valued in academia; athleticism is valued in sports. Individuals who possess the characteristics

that are valued within their social niche will rise in status within the hierarchy, whereas those

lacking these characteristics will find themselves relegated to positions of lower status. Status is

important because one’s hierarchical position is related to how one is treated by others. For

example, individuals higher in status are treated with more respect and deference, and they

obtain better food, larger territory, and, most importantly for the purposes of this study, have
Parental Favoritism 18

access to more desirable mates (Buss, 2007). In other words, a number of benefits are available

to individuals who hold high status within the hierarchy. It appears that a host of characteristics

that vary amongst individuals besides health, namely those associated with social

competitiveness, might strongly influence an individual’s reproductive success.

However, the socially competitive characteristics associated with reproductive success

differ somewhat between the sexes due to differences in the reproductive biology of men and

women and the different reproductive adaptive problems faced by men and women. For

example, unlike men, women have a finite number of gametes and a limited time period of

fertility. Because of the differences in their reproductive biology (e.g., number and size of

gametes, internal gestation in women), males and females have faced different adaptive problems

regarding mating and reproduction. Consequently, males and females have evolved different

psychological mechanisms that, on average, demonstrated effectiveness in helping to solve these

adaptive problems and increase one’s reproductive success. Therefore, certain characteristics

associated with social competitiveness that might increase one’s reproductive success, are likely

to differ in importance between the sexes as well. My examination of mate preferences, an

important class of evolved psychological mechanisms, will further elucidate this point.

Characteristics associated with male reproductive success. Pregnancy is an extremely

costly process for women. The processes involved in pregnancy (e.g., internal gestation) require

women to initially provide a substantial number of resources to the fetus. Following birth,

offspring require a substantial number of resources (e.g., food, shelter, protection) in order to

survive. Females without a committed male partner to help provide protection, food, and other

important resources during and following pregnancy would have children who would be unlikely

to survive. Thus, for females an important adaptive problem they experienced was finding a mate
Parental Favoritism 19

who was willing and able to assist in resource provisioning (e.g., providing shelter, protection

etc). Through selection pressures, mechanisms that assisted females in identifying males able to

acquire and willing to share resources evolved. Consequently, females seek and prefer long-term

mates who have the ability and willingness to provide resources (Buss, 1989; Buss & Schmitt,

1993).

Buss’s (1989) massive cross-cultural examination of mating preferences across 37

cultures demonstrated that women around the world, more so than men, prefer mates with high

social status and good earning potential. Within the same study women rated ambition and

industriousness as highly desirable traits in long-term male partners (Buss, 1989). In another

study, Li, Kenrick, Bailey, and Linsenmeier (2002) demonstrated that for many women,

intelligence in a mate is considered a “necessary” trait. 8 In other words, males needed to reach an

intelligence threshold before women will even consider their standing on other characteristics

related to mate selection.

Intelligence, ambition, and industriousness predict social status and earning potential

(Kyl-Heku & Buss, 1996). Males who possess these traits are more likely to rise in social status

and possess greater earning potential. Males with higher earning potentials are more likely to be

able to provide resources, and thus are more likely to be perceived as desirable mates by women.

Therefore, it appears that intelligent males who are ambitious and industrious are more likely to

be reproductively successful than males lacking these characteristics (Evans & Brase, 2007).

8 Li et al. (2002) asked participants to design their ideal marriage partner. Participants were allotted a particular
budget (e.g., 100 mate dollars) and were instructed to assign a percentile level for each identified trait. Each decile
level corresponded to a number level (e.g., 80th percentile = 8 mate dollars). From this, Li et al. were able to
categorize mate preferences of the participants as being either a “necessity” or a “luxury.” “Necessity” traits were
traits an individual must possess in order to even be considered as a mate. “Luxury” traits were not necessarily
needed in order to still be considered as a mate.
Parental Favoritism 20

In sum, because of the physical burdens imposed on women by pregnancy, securing

resources was a more profound adaptive problem for women than men. Consequently,

mechanisms evolved in women resulting in women preferring long-term mates who are willing

and able to provide resources. Men, who possess traits like intelligence, ambition, and

industriousness, are preferred by women because these traits indicate a male’s ability to acquire

and provide resources. Therefore, males who possess these preferred traits are more likely to be

reproductively successful than individuals who lack these characteristics. In the current research,

I expected parents to favor male children who possessed the traits (e.g., intelligence, ambition)

associated with social competitiveness among males, because these traits are indicators of a male

child's later reproductive success.

There is some existing support for a relationship between child intelligence, ambition,

and industriousness and parental favoritism (Kiracofe, 1992; Zervas & Sherman, 1994). Kiracofe

(1992) examined the relationship between child-perceived favoritism and the development of

psychosocial characteristics (e.g., self-esteem). Participants in this study were 417 individuals,

ranging in age from 14 to 66, who were currently receiving Adlerian-focused therapy. As part of

their treatment, participants were asked to provide a description of each of their siblings, to rate

each sibling on a list of characteristics (e.g., intelligence), and to answer a series of questions

regarding sibling inter-relationships within their family. One question in particular asked

participants if they believed that either parent had a favorite. Results indicated that siblings who

were perceived to be most intelligent, perceived to have the highest standards of achievement,

received the best grades, and were perceived to be the hardest worker were perceived to be

favored by the father. Results also indicated that mothers, too, were perceived as favoring the

child who was reported as being most intelligent and had the highest standards of achievement.
Parental Favoritism 21

Thus, according to participants’ reporting on parental behaviors, it appears that parents are more

likely to favor children who are intelligent and industrious/ambitious.

Along similar lines, Zervas and Sherman (1994) investigated the relationship between

self-esteem and participants’ perceptions of parental favoritism. Participants in this study were

91 college students with a mean age of 18.5 years, who had at least one biological sibling and

lived in a home consisting of two biological parents who were currently living together.

Participants completed a self-esteem questionnaire, a demographic sheet, and a favoritism

questionnaire that consisted of six items. Questionnaire items included, “Do you think your

mother/father has a favorite? and “If so, who?”. Based on responses, participants were divided

into three categories: favored (participant was favored by at least one parent), non-favored

(neither parent favored the participant and at least one parent favored a sibling of the participant)

or no favoritism (neither parent favored any child). Participants who reported a belief that one or

both parents had a favorite were then asked to circle from a list of 65 adjectives (e.g.,

intelligence, sense of humor) what they believed were possible reasons for that parent’s

favoritism. Relevant to the theoretical framework of the current study, intelligence was the

reason most commonly circled by participants, with 44% of participants circling this item. Thus,

as reported from the perspective of adult children, intelligence appeared to be a primary reason

for which they or their sibling was favored.

Neither Kiracofe (1992) or Zervas and Sherman (1994) examined the relationships

between intelligence and industriousness/ambition and parental favoritism separately for male

and female participants. This lack of attention to potential sex differences by both sets of

researchers might have occurred because Kiracofe worked from an Adlerian framework, which

offers no theoretical reason for why a child's sex may influence parental favoritism. Additionally,
Parental Favoritism 22

Zervas and Sherman simply investigated the general relationship between favoritism and self-

esteem and did not appear to have a basis on which to predict a different pattern of results for

male and female participants. In the current research, sex differences in the predictors of

parental favoritism were a central focus.

In the current research, I argued that, from a fitness perspective, traits such as

intelligence, ambition, and industriousness are more important characteristics for males to

possess than females, because these traits are more strongly associated with a male’s

reproductive success. Therefore, I expected males who reported possessing these traits to a

higher degree than a sibling to more often report being favored by parents. I did not expect these

traits to predict parental favoritism for female participants because these traits are not as strongly

associated with a female’s reproductive success.

Characteristics associated with female reproductive success. As noted, in the domain of

mating, men and women have faced different adaptive problems. Therefore, males have evolved

a different set of psychological mechanisms in the form of mate preferences than have females. I

now review the characteristics that men desire in long-term mates, how these characteristics are

associated with female reproductive success, and how they may be associated with parental

treatment.

Males, unlike females, have a continually replenishing supply of gametes (i.e., sperm

cells) and thus are capable of reproducing throughout most of their adult life. Furthermore,

fertilization and gestation of offspring occur internally to women, not men. For these reasons,

over evolutionary history, male reproductive success has hinged primarily on the ability to

identify and attract female mates who are fertile and able to bear children. Males who

successfully identified and attracted fertile mates were more likely to produce offspring and were
Parental Favoritism 23

thus more likely to pass their genes on to future generations. Males who were less successful in

identifying and attracting fertile mates were less likely to produce offspring and pass their genes

on to future generations. Thus, through selection pressures, mechanisms that assisted males in

identifying and mating with fertile females evolved.

However, males cannot directly observe and assess a woman’s fertility. Instead, they

infer a woman’s fertility indirectly via a host of visual and behavioral cues. A woman’s age is the

most important determinant of conception. Fertility is steeply age-graded in women, with peak

fertility occurring between the ages of 20 - 28 and then gradually declining after approximately

age 35 (Maheshwan, Hamilton, & Bhatacharya, 2008). Consistent with this line of reasoning,

cross-cultural research indicates that males generally prefer younger women.

More important for the current study, research also indicates that men prefer women who

demonstrate cues associated with fertility (Buss, 1989), such as physical beauty (Evans & Brase,

2007), clear, smooth skin, full lips, and a bouncy gait (Symons, 1995) and a specific waist-to-hip

ratio (Singh, 1993a). Essentially, physical features that are considered attractive are those that are

correlated with youth, good health, and fertility. Moreover, men generally view physical beauty

as a “necessary” requirement when they are considering various women as a potential mate (Li et

al., 2002). In other words, women might need to reach a certain attractiveness threshold before

men will even consider their other characteristics in a context of mate selection. Thus, it appears

that physically attractive females are more likely to be reproductively successful than females

lacking these characteristics, because these characteristics are highly desired by men (Evans &

Brase, 2007).

In sum, identifying, attracting, and mating with fertile female mates constituted a set of

important adaptive problems for men. Consequently, mechanisms evolved in men that result in
Parental Favoritism 24

men preferring long-term mates who are fertile, as indexed, in part, by cues of physical beauty.

Men prefer characteristics in women associated with physical beauty because these

characteristics serve as a proxy for a women’s fertility. Therefore, physically beautiful women,

(and young girls who show the signs of later developing such features) are more likely to be

reproductively successful than less attractive women (and young girls who seem less likely to

develop attractive features). Consequently, parents might be expected to favor female children

who are physically attractive, because physical attractiveness is an indicator of a female child's

later reproductive success.

Research has demonstrated a link between child attractiveness and parental favoritism.

For example, laboratory studies have shown that mothers pay more attention to and play more

often with attractive infants than unattractive infants (Langlois, Ritter, Casey, & Sawin, 1995).

Another study showed that mothers of infants with craniofacial abnormalities, compared to

mothers of infants without craniofacial abnormalities, smiled less, vocalized to less, and were

generally less responsive to their children (Barden, Ford, Jensen, Rogers-Salyer, & Salyer,

1989). Langlois et al. (1995) found that mothers with an unattractive child were more likely to

view the child as interfering with her life than were mothers with attractive children.

Furthermore, adults generally treat attractive children less harshly (Langlois, Ritter, Casey, &

Swain, 1995). For example, Elder, Van Nguyen, and Caspi (1985) found that fathers were more

punitive toward their unattractive daughters than toward their attractive daughter following a job

loss. Overall, these findings suggest that parents tend to favor children, especially female

children, who are physically attractive.

In sum, characteristics such as intelligence, ambition, industriousness, and physical

attractiveness are associated with increased potential for reproductive success for those who
Parental Favoritism 25

possess them. In particular, intelligence, ambition, and industriousness are associated with a

male’s reproductive success because these characteristics tend to be preferred by females

regarding male mates. Physical attractiveness is associated with a female’s reproductive success

because these set of characteristics tend to be preferred by males regarding female mates.

Therefore, I expected parents to invest more in (favor) children who possess these characteristics

because these children are more likely to translate parental investment into fitness benefits for

the parent(s).

The Present Study

Evolutionary psychologists have identified two contexts that appear to influence

psychological mechanisms associated with parenting 9 . These contexts include the genetic

relatedness between offspring and parent, and the ability of offspring to convert parental

investment into fitness benefits for the parent. In my study, I tested several theoretically-derived

hypotheses regarding the genetic relatedness between parent and offspring, specifically the

association between parental certainty and favoritism, and the ability of offspring to convert

parental investment into fitness benefits for the parent as predictors of parental favoritism.

I defined parental favoritism as child-participant-reported systematic bias by a parent(s)

that is manifested financially (e.g., gives us money) and psychologically (e.g., proud of things

we’ve done) in the direction of a particular offspring. I measured perceptions of parental

favoritism with the Differential Parent Treatment Scale on the Sibling Inventory of Differential

Experience (SIDE; Daniels & Plomin, 1985). This scale was designed to assess the extent to

which a parent(s) favor or punish one sibling more than the other(s). I used this scale to assess

9
A third context, alternative usage of resources (e.g., mating pursuits), will not be examined in my study because no
predictions relevant to parental favoritism may be offered for this context.
Parental Favoritism 26

parental favoritism because it is one of the few scales that can specifically assess differential

parental treatment. Previous researchers either coded mother and child observations (Brody,

Stoneman, & Burke, 1987), simply asked participants, “Who is your mother/father’s favorite)

(Harris & Howard, 2001; Kiracofe, 1992; Zervas & Sherman, 1994), or interviewed mothers

about their relationship with their children (Suitor & Pillemer, 2007). The SIDE allowed for

assessment of both maternal and paternal favoritism from the perspective of the child across a

number of facets inherent to the parent-child relationship (support, pride, discipline, etc.). Thus,

it more fully assesses parental favoritism than brief observations, and, compared to interviewing

the parents, it is not as subject to social desirability concerns. Furthermore, I created and added

several new items, especially regarding issues of financial assistance, in order to more fully

assess parental favoritism.

Participants completed a SIDE for each biological parent, as well as completed a physical

resemblance scale (self with parent) for each parent. The resemblance scale was designed for the

current research. Participants also completed a measure regarding their physical health, and a

measure of social competitiveness, namely intelligence, ambition/industriousness, and physical

attractiveness. All measures were based on self-perception and were relative in nature, in that

participants compared themselves to a sibling on all of these characteristics. These constructs

were assessed using a collection of previously established items and scales, items from the

International Personality Item Pool (IPIP; http://ipip.ori.org/), and some items designed

originally for the purposes of the current study.


Parental Favoritism 27

Hypotheses

H1: I expected paternal favoritism to be reported by participants as more common,

intense, and obvious than maternal favoritism. I expect mothers to invest in children in a

more equitable manner than fathers.

Mothers, unlike fathers, are certain regarding their genetic relatedness to

offspring. In other words, mothers know without a doubt that all their children

share 50% of their genes. Fathers, on the other hand, face the problem of paternal

uncertainty. For these reasons, from a fitness perspective, mothers benefit more

than fathers from investing their valuable and finite resources relatively equally

across their offspring. Fathers might benefit, from a fitness perspective, from

favoring certain children over other children.

H2: I expected participants who, compared to a sibling, reported a greater degree of

physical resemblance with their putative biological father to more often report receiving

paternal favoritism than participants who reported a lower degree of facial resemblance

with their father. I did not expect a significant correlation between physical resemblance

with biological mother and maternal favoritism.

Fathers are not certain of their genetic relatedness to a child due to conditions

such as internal fertilization and gestation in women, as well as the threat of

cuckoldry. Consequently, males do not know with certainty whether putative

offspring share 50% of their genes. Due to the adaptive problems of cuckoldry

and investment of resources in a child with whom they share no genetic

relatedness, psychological mechanisms evolved in males that on average, helped

males avoid the costs associated cuckoldry. One such mechanism in males is a
Parental Favoritism 28

kin-discrimination mechanism that is sensitive to the degree of facial resemblance

between themselves and a child. For males, physical resemblance to the child

functions as an indicator of genetic relatedness. Thus, males who perceive a facial

resemblance to offspring are more certain of genetic relatedness than those who

perceive no resemblance, and as such are expected to invest in the offspring.

Because females did not face the adaptive problems associated with maternal

uncertainty they would not have evolved mechanisms associated with determining

which children are or are not genetically related to them and therefore parenting

behaviors would not be factors associated with parental certainty (e.g., physical

resemblance).

H3: I expected participants (of both sexes) who, compared to a sibling, reported being

relatively healthier to be more likely to report receiving parental favoritism than

individuals who reported being less healthy.

An offspring’s ability to convert parental investment into fitness benefits for the

parent is expected to be of primary importance regarding a parent’s “decision” to

invest in a child. Parents will tend to invest more in children who are likely to

translate the investment into fitness benefits for them. Reproductive success is

arguably the most critical measure of one’s fitness. Individuals who possess traits,

such as health, that are desired by mates tend to be more reproductively successful

than individuals who do not possess the desired traits. Health is a highly desired

trait in mates for both men and women. Therefore, healthier individuals are

expected to be more reproductively successful than less healthy individuals.

Parents are expected to invest more and favor healthier children because health is
Parental Favoritism 29

associated with reproductive success and offspring reproductive success

ultimately translates into fitness benefits for parents.

H4: I expected that male participants who, compared to a sibling, reported having greater

intelligence to be more likely to report being favored by both mothers and fathers. I did

not expect a significant relationship between female participants reporting of intelligence

and their reporting of parental favoritism.

Females prefer to mate with men who are intelligent. Therefore, males who

possess this trait are more likely to be reproductively successful than males who

do not have this trait. Due to the increased potential reproductive success of these

offspring, parents are expected to invest more and favor the son who is more

intelligent. Because intelligence is more associated with a male’s, rather than a

female’s reproductive success, I did not expect a significant relationship with

females reporting of intelligence and their reporting of parental favoritism.

H5: I expected that male participants who, compared to a sibling, reported having greater

ambition/industriousness to be more likely to report being favored by both mothers and

fathers. I did not expect a significant relationship between female participants reporting

of ambition/industriousness and their reporting of parental favoritism.

Females prefer to mate with men who are ambitious/industrious. Therefore, males

who possess this trait are more likely to be reproductively successful than males

who do not have this trait. Due to the increased potential reproductive success of

these offspring, parents are expected to invest more and favor the son who is more

ambitious/industrious. Because ambition/industriousness is more associated with

a male’s, rather than a female’s reproductive success, I did not expect a


Parental Favoritism 30

significant relationship with females reporting of ambition/industriousness and

their reporting of parental favoritism.

H6: I expected that female participants who, compared to a sibling, reported being more

physically attractive to be more likely to report being favored by both mothers and

fathers. I did not expect a significant relationship between male participants reporting of

physical attractiveness and their reporting of parental favoritism.

Males prefer to mate with physically attractive females. Therefore, physically

attractive females are more likely to be reproductively successful than less

attractive females. Due to the increased potential reproductive success of these

offspring, parents are expected to invest more in and favor physically attractive

daughters. Because physical attractiveness is more associated with female’s rather

than male’s reproductive success, I did not expect a significant a relationship

between males reporting being physically attractive and their reporting of parental

favoritism.
Parental Favoritism 31

METHOD

Participants

Undergraduate students attending a university in Northwest Ohio (n = 451) participated

in the study. Students from psychology classes were recruited through Experimetrix, an online

website where students can select from a number of experiments currently being conducted.

Participants were required to have at least one fully-biological sibling and to have lived with

both biological parents for at least 17 years. All students received course credit for participating.

Two hundred and eighty-eight students participated during the Fall Semester (November

– December), and 163 participated at the beginning of the Spring Semester (January – February).

Of these 451 participants, 418 students (93%) completed the study in its entirety. There were

more female (66%) than male participants (34%). Eighty-eight percent of participants were

White; 7% were Black; 2.5% were multi-racial; 1% were Latino/a; and, 1.5% indicated an ethnic

identity of “Other.” Participants averaged 19.2 years of age (SD = 2.1), ranging in age from 18

(which was the modal age, n = 158,) to 43 (n = 1). Regarding socioeconomic status, 3% reported

combined family income of $20,000 or less, 7% reported income ranging from $20,001 -

$40,000, 12% reported income ranging from $40,001 - $60,000, 13% reported income ranging

from $60,001 - $80,000, 15% reported income ranging from $80,001 - $100,000, 20% reported

income of over $100,000, and 30% reported not knowing their parents' income.

Approximately half (53%) of participants reported having just one fully-biological sibling

(the rest reported having more than one fully-biological sibling). Thirty-six per cent of the data

comes from a female participant reporting about herself and a male sibling; 31% comes from a

female participant reporting about herself and a sister; 19% comes from a male participant

reporting about himself and a brother; and 14% comes from a male participant reporting about
Parental Favoritism 32

himself and a sister. Thus, half of the data comes from same-sex sibling pairs, and half comes

from mixed-sex sibling pairs. The average age of participants’ sibling was 19.3 years of age (SD

= 4.6, mode = 17 years of age), ranging in age from 5 to 45. The participant was the older sibling

in 50% of the cases; the sibling was older than the participant in 48% of the cases, and

participant and sibling were the same age in 2% of the cases. In 68% of the cases participants

were within three years of age of their sibling; in 24% of the cases participants were within four

to six years of their sibling, and in 8% of cases there was an age difference between participant

and sibling of seven years or more.

Materials and Measures

Online questionnaires. All materials were completed on-line through surveymonkey.com.

Surveymonkey is a software program that allows researchers to design web-based surveys.

Informed consent. Participants signed up for a study concerned with family dynamics.

They were informed that they would be asked to provide demographic information about

themselves (e.g., age, sex), as well as complete several personality and behavioral measures

about themselves and other family members (e.g., a sibling, mother, father; See Appendix A).

Participants were informed that participation was voluntary, that there were no foreseeable risks

associated with participation, and that they could withdraw at any time without penalty. The

contact information of the primary researcher and his faculty advisor was provided to

participants. Participants were instructed that clicking on a “YES” link indicated their consent.

Instructions. Participants were instructed to compare themselves to the same fully-

biological sibling for all questions (See Appendix B). Participants with more than one fully

biological sibling were instructed to compare themselves to the fully biological sibling who was

closest in age. Participants with biological siblings equidistant to them in age (e.g., one sibling
Parental Favoritism 33

who is 2 years older and one who is 2 years younger) were instructed to report on the sibling

with whom they most closely associate.

Differential parental treatment sub-scale of the Sibling Inventory of Differential

Experience (SIDE). Participants completed a modified version of the Differential Parental

Treatment subscale of the Sibling Inventory of Differential Experience (SIDE; Daniels, 1986;

See Appendix C) for each biological parent. This subscale assesses whether a parent favors or

punishes one child more than another. The original SIDE subscale included 9 items, and it

assessed a child's perceptions of his or her social-affective experiences with a parent (e.g., parent

enjoyed doing things with us). I removed one item, the “has favored” item, because this item

represents the global construct of interest and a similar item, to be described, was used to help

assess the validity of the SIDE measure.

To allow for an assessment of parental favoritism expressed through additional (e.g.,

financial) means I created and added 12 items (has disciplined, given money, bought things for,

given freedom over actions, enjoyed talking with, given what was requested, done favors for,

done things with, been supportive of, criticized, listened to the opinions of, respected). Thus, the

modified scale contained 20 items.

Each set of 20 items was completed twice, once in reference to the mother and once in

reference to the father. For each item (e.g., has been strict with), participants selected an option

from a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = In general, my mother/father has been much more this way

towards my sibling than me, 2 = In general, my mother/father has been somewhat more this way

towards my sibling than me, 3 = In general, my mother/father has been the same towards my

sibling and me, 4 = In general, my mother/father has been somewhat more this way towards me

than my sibling , 5 = In general, my mother/father has been much more this way towards me than
Parental Favoritism 34

my sibling. For 15 of the 20 items (e.g., has given money) higher scores indicated favoritism in

the direction of the participant. For five items (e.g., has been strict with, has punished for

misbehavior, has blamed for what another family member did, has disciplined, has criticized)

higher scores indicated favoritism in the direction of a sibling. Prior to analysis, these five items

were reverse-scored, so that for all items higher scores indicated favoritism in the direction of the

participant. The 20 items comprising the maternal favoritism scale were internally consistent

(Cronbach's α = .86), as were the 20 items comprising the paternal favoritism scale (Cronbach's α

= .88). Therefore, I averaged the 20 items from each scale to form a maternal and paternal

favoritism composite index.

Forced-choice and obviousness parental favoritism items. On a separate page,

participants were asked twice, once in reference to each parent, about their general perception of

parental favoritism (See Appendix D). For each parent participants selected one of three response

options (1 = My mother/father has favored me over my sibling, 2 = My mother/father has

favored my sibling over me, 3 = My mother/father has not favored me or my sibling).

Participants who indicated favoritism in either direction also reported the “obviousness” of the

favoritism on a Likert-type scale (1 = My mother’s/father’s favoritism was very subtle, 7 = My

mother’s/father’s favoritism was very obvious).

To help assess the validity of the maternal and paternal SIDE composite indices, I

submitted the maternal and paternal favoritism composite indices to a one-way ANOVA using

the target of favoritism (participant, sibling, no one), as identified on the single-item measure, as

a quasi-independent variable. Results indicated significant differences on the maternal SIDE

composite index between the groups, F(2, 422) = 111.70, p = .00. Follow-up t-tests indicated

significant differences between all groups, all p’s = .00. Participants who indicated on the forced-
Parental Favoritism 35

choice item that their mother favored them, scored higher on the maternal SIDE composite index

(M = 3.31, SD = .38) than those who indicated no maternal favoritism (M = 3.01, SD = .23), as

well as those who indicated maternal favoritism in the direction of the sibling (M = 2.59, SD =

.49).

Results indicated significant differences on the paternal SIDE composite index between

the groups, F(2, 423) = 104.40, p = .00. Follow-up t-tests indicated significant differences

between all groups, all p’s = .000. Participants who indicated on the forced-choice item that their

father favored them, scored higher on the paternal SIDE composite index (M = 3.37, SD = .40)

than those who indicated no paternal favoritism (M = 3.02, SD = .25), as well as those who

indicated paternal favoritism in the direction of the sibling (M = 2.62, SD = .48). Together, these

findings suggest that the maternal and paternal SIDE composite indices were measuring

perceptions of parental favoritism.

Relative physical resemblance to parents. Participants completed a perceived physical

resemblance scale for each parent (See Appendix E). These scales were created originally for the

current research and consisted of six items. Four items asked participants to indicate the degree

to which they believed that they, relative to a sibling, physically resembled a parent on specific

facial features (e.g., Whose eyes/hair/nose/mouth more closely resembles your

mother’s/father’s?).

For each item, participants selected one of five response options (1 = My sibling is much

more similar to my mother/father than I am, 2 = My sibling is somewhat more similar to my

mother/father than I am, 3 = My sibling and I are equally similar to my mother/father, 4 = I am

somewhat more similar to my mother/father than my sibling, 5 = I am much more similar to my


Parental Favoritism 36

mother/father than my sibling). Higher numbers indicate that the participant perceived him- or

herself to be more physically similar to his/her parent than the sibling.

The fifth item asked participants about their overall facial resemblance to the respective

parent (Overall, whose face more closely resembles your mother’s/father’s?). The sixth item

asked participants how much they believed they physically resembled each parent overall,

ostensibly on all features including and in additional to facial features (Overall, who more

physically resembles your mother/father?). Response options were: 1 = My sibling much more

physically resembles my mother/father, 2 = My sibling somewhat more physically resembles my

mother/father, 3 = My sibling and I physically resemble my mother/father equally, 4 = I

physically resemble my mother/father somewhat more than does my sibling 5 = I physically

resemble my mother/father much more than does my sibling). This item was added to allow for a

more complete assessment of physical resemblance between parent and child. These six physical

resemblance items were averaged to form two relative physical resemblance composite indices

(Cronbach's α = .82 for maternal resemblance and Cronbach's α = .85 for paternal resemblance).

Relative Health. Participants completed a five-item health scale designed to assess

whether the participant or his or her sibling was perceived to be healthier (See Appendix F).

Scale items were borrowed from the General Health Scale on the Short Form - 36 Health Survey

(see Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Items were modified in order to allow for a comparison

between the participant and his/her sibling. The first four items included: I seem to get sick more

often than does my sibling; I seem to suffer from aches/pains more often than does my sibling; I

seem to visit a doctor or health center more often than does my sibling; and, I seem to take

medication for illnesses more often than my sibling.


Parental Favoritism 37

For each item, participants selected one of five response options (1 = Definitely False, 2=

Somewhat False, 3 = Neither True nor False, 4 = Somewhat True, 5 = Definitely True). The first

four items were reverse-coded so that higher scores always indicated that participants perceived

themselves to be healthier than their sibling.

I added a fifth item that asked participants their perception of their overall health

(Overall, I seem to be healthier than my sibling). Participants selected one of five response

options (1 = Definitely False, 2= Somewhat False, 3 = Neither True nor False, 4 = Somewhat

True, 5 = Definitely True). These five items were averaged to form a relative health composite

index (Cronbach's α = .82).

Relative social competitiveness. I was interested in the following characteristics of social

competitiveness: intelligence, ambition/industriousness, and physical attractiveness. To measure

these facets of social competitiveness I borrowed and modified items from the on-line

International Personality Item Pool (IPIP; http://ipip.ori.org/). The IPIP website consists of over

2,000 items (e.g., work hard, use difficult words) that cover many facets of the Big-Five factor

structure (Goldberg, 1999) 10 . The IPIP was developed in response to the perception that

personality assessment has progressed at a slow pace due to the fact that many personality

inventories are proprietary instruments, and thus not readily subjected to comparative validity

studies (Goldberg, 1999).

10
Regarding the IPIP’s development, the IPIP’s original 1,252 items hoped to cover many facets of the Big-Five
factor structure (Goldberg, 1999). The original items, 360 trait-descriptive adjectives and 525 person-descriptive
adjectives, were administered to an adult community sample. Participants also completed a number of commercial,
empirically demonstrated personality inventories (e.g., NEO-Personality Inventory-Revised). IPIP items were
correlated with items from the proprietary personality instruments believed to measure similar constructs. Results
indicated that the average reliability coefficients for the IPIP scales were at least as high as the values found on the
proprietary instruments, such as the NEO-PI-R (See http://ipip.ori.org/ for a detailed description of scale
construction).
Parental Favoritism 38

Through its availability on-line, the IPIP allows for continual development and

refinement of personality inventories found in the public domain. The IPIP website includes

information on the psychometric characteristics of the current set of IPIP scales, keys for scoring

the current set of scales, and the current total set of IPIP items (Goldberg, Johnson, Eber, Hogan,

Ashton, Cloninger, & Gough, 2006). The IPIP web site individually lists a number of constructs

(e.g., ambition, intelligence) that researchers might assess. These items are similar but not

identical to items from proprietary instruments measuring similar constructs.

Relative intelligence. Participants completed an 8-item measure designed to assess

whether the participant or his or her sibling was perceived to be more intelligent (e.g., compared

to my sibling, I have a richer vocabulary, am more likely to solve complex problems; see

Appendix G). These items, selected from the IPIP, were designed to assess intellect in a manner

similar to how intellect is conceptualized as part of the Openness scale of the NEO-PI-R 11 (Costa

& McRae, 1992).

For each item, participants selected one of five response options (1 = Definitely False, 2

= Somewhat False, 3 = Neither True nor False, 4 = Somewhat True, 5 = Definitely True). Higher

numbers indicate that participants perceived themselves to be more intelligent than their sibling.

Reliability analysis indicated that the eight items were internally consistent (Cronbach's α = .89).

Therefore, I averaged the items to create a relative intelligence composite index.

Relative ambition and industriousness. Participants completed a 17-item measure

designed to assess whether participants perceived themselves or their sibling to be more

ambitious/industrious (e.g., compared to my sibling, I am more likely to succeed; See Appendix

11The NEO-PI-R is one of the most prominent and best-researched personality measures (De Fruyt, Aluja, Garcia,
Rolland, & Jung, 2006).
Parental Favoritism 39

H). Eight items were borrowed and modified from items similar to achievement-striving items

found in the NEO-PI-R (Costa &McRae, 1992; e.g., Plunge into tasks with all my heart). Eight

items were borrowed and modified from items similar to the industriousness items from the Six

Factor Personality Questionnaire 12 (6FPQ; Jackson, Paunonen, & Tremblay, 2000; e.g., Put work

above pleasure). I created one item (Compared to my sibling, I am more ambitious).

For each item, participants selected one of five response options (1 = Definitely False, 2

= Somewhat False, 3 = Neither True nor False, 4 = Somewhat True, 5 = Definitely True). Higher

numbers indicate that participants perceived themselves to be more ambitious/industrious than

their sibling. Reliability analysis indicated that the 17 items were internally consistent

(Cronbach's α = .93). Therefore, I averaged them to create a relative ambition/industriousness

composite index.

Relative physical attractiveness. Participants completed a 6-item measure designed to

assess whether participants perceived themselves or their sibling to be more physically attractive

(e.g., I like my body more than does my sibling, I like to show off my body more than does my

sibling; See Appendix I). These items were borrowed and modified from the IPIP. Specifically,

these items were designed to be similar to attractiveness items found on the Personal Attributes

Survey. Items were modified so that each item allowed for a relative comparison between

participants and a sibling.

For each item, participants selected one of five response options (1 = Definitely False, 2

= Somewhat False, 3 = Neither True nor False, 4 = Somewhat True, 5 = Definitely True). Higher

12 The 6FPQ provides a measure of six dimensions of personality: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Independence,
Openness to Experience, Methodicalness, and Industriousness. The Industriousness subscale consists of three facet
scales: Achievement, Endurance, and Seriousness. The 6FPQ demonstrates solid psychometric properties with
internal consistency reliability ranging from .54 to .84 (Jackson, Paunonen, & Tremblay, 2000).
Parental Favoritism 40

numbers indicate that the participants perceived themselves to be more physically attractive than

their sibling. Reliability analysis indicated that the six items were internally consistent

(Cronbach's α = .87). Therefore, I averaged the 6 items to create a relative physical attractiveness

composite index.

Demographics. Participants indicated their age, sex, race, height, and weight, waist-hip-

ratio (WHR), the sex of their sibling, the age of their sibling, their GPA, as well as their parent’s

yearly household income (See Appendix J). Additionally, participants’ body mass indices (BMI)

were calculated.

Post-study questionnaire. The post-study questionnaire consisted of two items (See

Appendix K). The first item asked participants to write in their own words what they believed

the study’s purpose was. This way, I could assess the degree to which participants became aware

of my hypotheses. The second item asked participants to provide any comments/feedback they

might have regarding the study. This question was designed to help identify problems or issues

with the study that were not recognized prior to the start of data collection.

Debriefing. The on-line debriefing form informed participants that the study investigated

parental favoritism from an evolutionary perspective (See Appendix L). The debriefing sheet

described my hypotheses. The sheet also included a reference related to the study’s topic, as well

as contact information of the primary researcher. The page also contained contact information for

counseling services within the area that were readily available to participants should the

completion of the study have triggered any emotional distress.

Questionnaire order. Participants completed the surveys in one of two orders.

Participants were assigned to an order based on the first letter of their last name (Order 1 was

completed by those whose last name began with a letter A – L; Order 2 was completed by those
Parental Favoritism 41

with last names beginning with a letter M - Z). Order 1 was as follows: maternal SIDE, relative

health, relative intelligence, relative ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness,

paternal SIDE, relative maternal physical resemblance, and relative paternal physical

resemblance. Order 2 was as follows: paternal SIDE, relative health, relative intelligence,

relative ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness, maternal SIDE, relative

paternal physical resemblance, and relative maternal physical resemblance. Demographics and

the post-study questionnaire were completed last by participants in both Order 1 and 2.

Procedure

Participants registered for the study on-line through Experimetrix. The title of the

experiment was “Thoughts on Myself and My Family.” Participants were informed that the

purpose of the study was to obtain a better understanding of people’s personal attributes, as well

as their family relationships. Although accurate, this statement was intentionally vague in order

to minimize demand characteristics. Furthermore, conducting the survey on-line might have

reduced social desirability because participants could complete the questionnaire in the privacy

of their home without being directly observed by the researcher who would be reviewing his/her

answers, thus fostering a sense of anonymity and privacy.

From the Experimetrix synopsis, students were directed to a website. At this site,

participants were instructed to click one of two possible links based upon the first letter in their

last name. Participants with last names starting with the letters A – L selected one link, whereas

participants with last names starting with the letters M – Z, selected a different link. Links

directed participants to their respective surveys which varied only in their order of appearance.

Participants were then directed to the informed consent page. The informed consent

highlighted that there were no right or wrong answers for the questions, that the researchers were
Parental Favoritism 42

interested only in the participant’s opinions, and that all answers were anonymous and

confidential. After reading the informed consent sheet, participants could either click the “NO”

button if they did not wish to participate in the study whereupon they were directed to a page

thanking them for their initial interest, or the “YES” button indicating participation consent.

Upon participation consent, all participants were directed to the “instructions” webpage.

Here, participants were instructed that for all questions they were to refer to the same, fully-

biological sibling throughout the entire survey. Participants with more than one fully biological

sibling were instructed to select the fully biological sibling who was closest to them in age. If

more than one equidistant in age fully biological sibling existed, participants were instructed to

select the sibling with whom they most interacted. After reading the instructions, participants

then indicated whether they had one or more than one fully biological sibling. Next, siblings

reported the age and sex of the sibling they were to use for comparison purposes. Participants

then completed the measures in the order that corresponded with their last name.

Debriefing. Following study completion, the participants were directed to a webpage for

debriefing. On average, participated completed the survey in 17 minutes.


Parental Favoritism 43

RESULTS

Overview

I was interested in examining whether paternal favoritism occurs more frequently and to

a greater degree than maternal favoritism, as reported by college-student participants. I was also

interested in examining hypotheses regarding child characteristics (physical health, intelligence,

ambition/industriousness, physical attractiveness, maternal and paternal physical resemblance)

that might be associated with parental favoritism. My primary outcome measures were maternal

and paternal favoritism. First, I report descriptive statistics pertaining to my measures.

Specifically, I examine the prevalence of parental favoritism as reported by participants, as well

as conduct analyses to determine whether certain variables (e.g., sibling sex) are generally

associated with parental favoritism, despite not being predicted. Second, I report results that

pertain to my hypotheses. Lastly, exploratory analyses are located in the Appendix.

Counterbalancing. As noted, participants completed the various surveys in one of two

orders. I performed a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), with Order serving as the

independent variable, on the six predictor variables (relative health, relative intelligence, relative

ambition/industriousness, relative attractiveness, relative maternal and paternal resemblance) and

the two outcome variables (maternal and paternal favoritism) in order to assess potential order

effects. Results indicate no order effects, all p’s > .05. Thus, the Order variable is not included in

subsequent analyses.

Descriptive Statistics

Parental favoritism. In order to get a general idea of how common maternal and paternal

favoritism were within this sample, I reviewed participants’ responses on the forced-choice item

(Check the statement that is most true for you: My mother/father favored me over my sibling,
Parental Favoritism 44

My mother/father favored my sibling over me, My mother/favored has not favored me or my

sibling). Forty two percent of participants reported experiencing paternal favoritism (with 24%

reporting that their father favored them, and 18% reporting that their father favored their sibling);

38% of participants reported experiencing maternal favoritism (with 21% of participants

reporting that their mother favored them, and 17% reporting that their mother favored their

sibling; See Table 1, e.g., for details regarding participant sex).

At first blush, such numbers might seem low if one compares to the rate obtained by

Suitor et al., 2006 who reported a favoritism rate of 92%. However, the current study’s reported

prevalence of parental favoritism falls within the range found in previous studies. Furthermore,

this percentage does not present a full picture regarding parental favoritism. In order to obtain a

fuller picture, I calculated the overall percentage of participants who indicated that at least one

parent displayed favoritism. Overall, 57% of participants indicated that at least one of their

parents displayed favoritism. This rate is more in accordance with those found in the extant

parental favoritism literature.

In cases where participants reported maternal (n = 162) or paternal favoritism (n = 179), I

assessed the level of obviousness of the favoritism (1 = Very Subtle, 7 = Very Obvious). As

noted in Table 1, participants reported that both fathers (M = 3.56, SD = 1.75) and mothers (M =

3.46, SD = 1.54) manifested favoritism in a moderate manner. Again, casual review of these data

may not present a full picture regarding the obviousness of parental favoritism. For this scale, a

score of four reflects the midpoint between a parent being very subtle or being very obvious

regarding favoritism displays. Thus, scores ranging from 1 to 3 on the item indicate subtle

favoritism, whereas scores ranging from 5 to 7 indicate obvious favoritism. Using these ranges as

cutoffs for dichotomizing the scale data, it appears that paternal favoritism, when it was reported,
Parental Favoritism 45

was described as being rather obvious (scores of 5 -7 on the scale) 33% of the time. When

maternal favoritism was reported it was described as rather obvious 27% of the time. Thus,

among those who reported the presence of maternal or parental favoritism, the favoritism was

obvious approximately one-third of the time.

Possible Correlates of Parental Favoritism

Sex differences and dyad composition effects. To assess whether participant or sibling sex

were associated with favoritism, I performed a series of 2 (Participant Sex: male, female) X 2

(Sibling Sex: male, female) ANOVAs with Maternal and Paternal SIDE composite indices

serving as the dependent variables. The SIDE composite indices are relative measures (i.e.,

participant compared to a sibling), wherein scores greater than three indicate favoritism in the

direction of the sibling and scores below three indicate favoritism in the direction of the sibling.

Because these effects (participant sex and dyad composition) were not predicted I used the more

conservative alpha level of .01 to interpret these findings. Any effects involving participant or

sibling sex that were significant at the p < .01 level were controlled for in subsequent analyses.

Regarding the maternal favoritism composite index, there was no main effect of sibling

sex, F(1, 420) = 4.87, p = .03. Furthermore, there was no main effect for participant sex, F(1,

420) = 3.25, p = .07; nor was there a Participant Sex by Sibling Sex interaction, F(1, 420) = .07,

p = .79. As for the paternal favoritism composite index, there was no main effect for sibling sex,

F(1, 421) = .09, p = .77, participant sex, F(1, 421) = 1.66, p = .20; nor was there a significant

Participant Sex by Sibling Sex interaction, F(1, 421) = .11, p = .74.

I also explored whether participant or sibling sex might be associated with relative health,

relative intelligence, relative ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness, and

relative physical resemblance. Thus, I performed a series of 2 (Participant Sex: male, female) X
Parental Favoritism 46

2 (Sibling Sex: male, female) ANOVAs with health, intelligence, ambition/industriousness,

physical attractiveness, and physical resemblance serving as the dependent variables.

Results indicated that, overall, male participants reported being healthier (M = 3.36, SD =

.87) than their sibling more so than female participants (M = 2.93, SD = .85), F(1, 421) = 28.85,

p = .00. Overall, participants also reported being significantly healthier than their sisters (M =

3.21, SD = .91), as compared to their brothers (M = 2.97, SD = .84), F(1, 421) = 11.25, p = .00.

There was no Participant Sex x Sibling Sex significant interaction regarding health, F(1, 421) =

2.39, p = .12 (See Table 2). Based on these results, I controlled for participant and sibling sex in

subsequent analyses examining health.

Regarding relative intelligence, overall, males (M = 3.57, SD = .82) reported being more

intelligent than their siblings more so than females (M = 3.32, SD = .84), F(1, 421) = 8.53, p =

.00. There was no main effect for sibling sex, F(1, 421) = 2.68, p = .10, nor was there a

significant Participant Sex by Sibling Sex interaction regarding relative intelligence, F(1, 421) =

.66, p = .42 (See Table 3). Therefore, I controlled for Participant Sex in subsequent analyses

examining relative intelligence.

For relative ambition/industriousness, there was no main effect for participant sex, F(1,

421) = 5.94, p = .02 nor was there a main effect for sibling sex, F(1, 421) = 6.09, p = .01.

Additionally, there was no Participant Sex by Sibling Sex interaction, F(1, 421) = .33, p = .57.

Regarding relative physical attractiveness, no significant effects were obtained regarding

physical attractiveness, all p’s > .01.

No significant effects were obtained regarding relative maternal resemblance, all p’s >

.01. As for paternal resemblance, results indicated no main effect for participant sex, F(1, 411) =
Parental Favoritism 47

3.90, p = .05, sibling sex, F(1, 411) = 4.95, p = .03 nor was there a Participant Sex by Sibling

Sex significant interaction regarding relative paternal resemblance, F(1, 411) = .821, p = .37.

Number of fully biological siblings. As noted earlier, 53% of participants possessed only

one fully biological sibling, whereas the remaining 47% possessed more than one fully biological

sibling and were, therefore, instructed to select one sibling for comparison purposes. I conducted

a series of independent samples t-tests comparing participants who had only one fully biological

sibling with those who had multiple fully biological siblings on my outcome (maternal SIDE and

paternal SIDE) and predictor variables (relative health, relative intelligence, relative

ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness, relative maternal resemblance, and

relative paternal resemblance) with number of fully biological siblings serving as a quasi-

independent variable. Results indicated no differences between the groups on any of these

variables, all p’s > .01. Thus, this variable is not included in subsequent analyses.

Sibling hierarchy. Because I asked participants to report their age as well as their sibling's

age, I was able to classify each participant as the older or younger sibling of the dyad. After

doing so, I performed a series of independent samples t-tests 13 on my outcome (maternal SIDE

and paternal SIDE) and predictor variables (relative health, relative intelligence, relative

ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness, relative maternal resemblance, and

relative paternal resemblance) with older or younger sibling status serving as the quasi-

independent variable (See Table 4). Results indicated that older siblings (M = 3.61, SD = .77)

reported greater intelligence than their siblings compared to younger siblings (M = 3.19, SD =

.86), t(401) = 5.25, p = .00. Older siblings (M = 3.55, SD = .59) also reported being more

13
In only seven cases were participants and sibling the same age. As such, they were not included in these analyses.
Parental Favoritism 48

ambitious/industrious than their siblings compared to younger siblings (M = 3.23, SD = .73),

t(401) = 4.89, p = .00. Lastly, younger siblings reported being more physically attractive (M =

3.21, SD = .87) than their sibling compared to older siblings (M = 2.91, SD = .81), t(401) = -3.67,

p = .00. Therefore, I controlled for sibling hierarchy in subsequent analyses examining relative

intelligence, relative ambition/industriousness, and relative physical attractiveness.

Age gap between siblings. Because I asked participants to report their age as well as their

sibling's age, I was able to categorize the sibling dyads based upon gaps in ages between

participants and their siblings. After doing so, I performed a series of one-way ANOVAs to

examine participants’ responses on my outcome and predictor measures with sibling age gap

serving as the quasi-independent variable (within three years, 4 – 6 years, 7 – 9 years, over 10

years 14 ). Results indicated no significant differences on my outcome or predictor measures based

upon age gap, all p’s > .01.

Summary. A majority (57%) of participants reported the presence of some form of

parental favoritism (maternal or paternal). Furthermore, such favoritism was manifested in a

rather obvious manner nearly 33% of the time. I conducted analyses to determine whether certain

variables (e.g., sibling sex) were generally associated with parental favoritism, despite not being

predicted. Several main effects for nominal variables that were not originally expected to be

associated with favoritism were identified. Specifically, main effects for participant sex and

sibling sex were found regarding health. Male participants reported greater health than female

participants and participants reported being healthier than female siblings, as compared to male

siblings. Additionally, a main effect for sibling hierarchy was found regarding relative

14
Previous literature notes greater conflict between siblings when the age gap is three years or less (Dunn &
McGuire, 1992).
Parental Favoritism 49

intelligence, relative ambition/industriousness, and relative physical attractiveness. In the cases

of relative intelligence and relative ambition/industriousness, participants who were older

siblings reported greater possession of intelligence and ambition/industriousness than did

participants who represented younger siblings. In the case of physical attractiveness, participants

who were younger siblings reported being more physically attractive than did participants who

represented older siblings. Additionally, there was a main effect for participant sex regarding

relative intelligence, wherein male participants reported greater intelligence than did female

participants. Therefore, I controlled for these variables in subsequent analyses.

Primary Analyses

Maternal versus paternal favoritism. For Hypothesis 1, I predicted that participants

would report greater favoritism by fathers than by mothers. To test this hypothesis, I first re-

coded items comprising the SIDE scales, because original items reflected both the presence and

direction of favoritism (toward self or sibling). For my first hypothesis, I was only interested

originally in the presence of favoritism; direction of favoritism (toward self or sibling) was of no

relevance.

I re-coded all favoritism items from the original five-item response set (1 = In general,

my mother/father has been much more this way towards my sibling than me, 2 = In general, my

mother/father has been somewhat more this way towards my sibling than me, 3 = In general, my

mother/father has been the same way towards my sibling and me, 4 = In general, my

mother/father has been somewhat more this way towards me than my sibling, 5 = In general, my

mother/father has been much more this way towards me than my sibling), to a three-item

response set (1 = no favoritism, 2 = some favoritism, 3 = a lot of favoritism). In other words,

items originally scored as either a one or five (high favoritism to either sibling or participant)
Parental Favoritism 50

were re-coded as a score of three (high favoritism present); items scored as either two or four

(some favoritism to either sibling or participant) were re-coded as a score of two (some

favoritism present); and items scored as three (no favoritism) were re-coded as one (no

favoritism present).

Additionally, I created maternal SIDE and paternal SIDE composite indices following

item re-coding, which I will refer to as Re-coded SIDEs in order to avoid confusion with the

original SIDE measures that were used for my remaining hypotheses. (Note that re-coding the

scales in the aforementioned manner also prevented the exclusion of cases where favoritism

existed but would not be detected if scale items were averaged. For example, a parent could

strongly favor the participant on 10 items (a score of 5 on each item), as well as strongly favor

the participant’s sibling on 10 items (a score of 1 on each item). Averaging these items would

result in a score of three indicating no favoritism towards either sibling or participant on the

original scale. However, in this hypothetical situation, it appears that this parent manifests

substantial displays of favoritism. In order for such data not to be lost, I re-coded items so that

the total degree of favoritism, regardless of direction, could be assessed.)

I examined Hypothesis 1 in several ways. First, I tested the hypothesis by conducting a

paired-samples t-test comparing the newly created maternal favoritism and paternal favoritism

Re-coded SIDE composite indices. Additionally, and with the purpose of fully analyzing the

data, I performed a series of paired-samples t-test comparing each item on the Re-coded maternal

SIDE composite index with the corresponding item on the Re-coded paternal SIDE composite

index. Lastly, I compared participants reporting of favoritism between mothers and fathers on the

forced-choice item, as well as on the obviousness item using paired samples t-test analyses.
Parental Favoritism 51

First, I compared the overall maternal Re-coded SIDE composite index 15 with the overall

Re-coded paternal SIDE composite index using a paired-samples t-test. Contrary to expectations,

the result suggested that participants report equal favoritism by mothers (M = 1.42, SD = .32)

and fathers (M = 1.43, SD = .36), t(425) = -.97, p = .33.

Comparisons of individual items on the Re-coded maternal and paternal SIDE composite

indices identified specific areas wherein one parent appeared to manifest favoritism more than

the other parent (See Table 5). Participants reported greater maternal versus paternal favoritism

on the dimension of punishing the children for misbehavior, t(425) = 3.91, p = .00. However,

participants reported greater paternal versus maternal favoritism on the dimensions of interest

shown in children, t(425) = -4.52, p = .00, doing things with children, t(425) = -3.71, p = .00, and

protecting children, t(425) = -2.26, p = .02. Thus, fathers showed greater favoritism in more

specific areas (e.g., shown interest) than did mothers. Furthermore, a casual inspection of the

overall pattern of means suggests that fathers received higher favoritism scores than mothers on

13 of the 20 items comprising the overall Re-coded SIDE composite indices.

To further examine Hypothesis 1, I examined participants’ responses to the forced-choice

item (My mother/father favored: me, my sibling, or neither of us) using a series of paired-

samples t-test. Results indicated that participants reported similar frequencies of maternal and

paternal favoritism directed toward themselves (21% versus 24%) and their sibling (17% versus

18%). Furthermore, there was also no difference regarding maternal versus paternal equal

treatment of the siblings (58% versus 62%), all p’s > .05.

15
For exploratory purposes, identical analyses were conducted on original SIDE composite indices (See Appendix
N and Table 15).
Parental Favoritism 52

Next, I examined potential differences between mothers and fathers regarding the

obviousness of parental favoritism. Results from a paired-samples t-test indicated that when

participants reported that a parent did display favoritism towards a particular child, mothers (M =

3.47, SD = 1.50) and fathers (M = 3.64, SD = 1.76) did not differ in terms of its obviousness,

t(103) = -1.02, p = .31. However, the means were in the expected direction, with higher scores

for fathers than mothers. Overall, minimal support was found for Hypothesis 1 in that

participants reported three specific areas wherein fathers displayed significantly greater

favoritism than did mothers.

Physical resemblance and favoritism. For Hypothesis 2, I predicted a positive

correlational relationship between relative physical/facial resemblance to one’s father (relative to

sibling) and paternal favoritism (relative to sibling). No relationship was expected to exist

between relative maternal physical/facial resemblance and maternal favoritism.

To test this hypothesis, I performed a bivariate correlation of relative paternal

resemblance and paternal favoritism. As expected, there was a significant, positive correlation

between the relative paternal physical/facial resemblance composite index and the original

paternal SIDE composite index, r(414) = .11, p = .02. Participants who reported looking more

like their father than their sibling were also more likely to report being favored by their father.

To examine this hypothesis more closely, I correlated relative paternal physical

resemblance with each of the individual items on the original paternal SIDE. By doing so, I was

able to identify the specific areas wherein fathers manifested favoritism towards the children

who shared a greater resemblance to them. Participants reported that fathers manifested

favoritism towards the children with a greater shared resemblance in the areas of (a) pride, (b)

enjoying doing things together, (c) freedom over one’s actions, (d) giving what was requested,
Parental Favoritism 53

(e) doing favors for, (f) being supportive, g) listening, and h) respecting, all p’s < .05 (See Table

6).

In the second part of Hypothesis 2, I predicted that no relationship would exist between

relative maternal physical/facial resemblance and maternal favoritism. To examine this

prediction, I performed a bivariate correlation between reported relative maternal physical

resemblance and maternal favoritism as assessed by the original maternal SIDE composite index

score. As expected, relative maternal resemblance and maternal favoritism were not significantly

correlated, r(424) = .06, p = .25. In other words, no relationship existed between participants'

self-perceived greater resemblance to their mother, relative to their sibling, and receiving

maternal favoritism. As before, I correlated relative maternal physical resemblance with each of

the individual items on the original maternal SIDE, in order to identify any specific domains

wherein relative maternal resemblance correlated with maternal favoritism. Results indicated

three specific areas wherein mothers manifested favoritism towards children who shared a

greater resemblance: (a) pride, (b) enjoying doing things together, and (c) doing things with (See

Table 6). Overall, relative maternal resemblance did not predict maternal favoritism. Therefore,

Hypothesis 2 was supported.

Health. For Hypothesis 3, I predicted that participants claiming to be healthier than their

sibling would also be more likely to report receiving favoritism from their mother and father. To

test this hypothesis, I performed partial correlations, controlling for participant sex and sibling

sex (as indicated by earlier ANOVA analyses) between the relative physical health composite

index score and the original SIDE composite indices (maternal and paternal).

The partial correlation between the relative physical health composite scores and the

original maternal SIDE composite index did not reach significance, r(420) = -.07, p = .17.
Parental Favoritism 54

Similarly, the partial correlation between relative physical health composite scores and the

paternal SIDE composite index was not significant, r(421) = .04, p = .40. Thus, it appears that

self-perceived relative health was not associated with maternal or paternal favoritism.

Despite the lack of significant results involving the original SIDE composite indices, I

correlated the relative physical health composite index with each of the individual items on the

SIDEs to identify any specific areas wherein physical health predicted parental favoritism.

Unexpectedly, mothers appeared to display favoritism towards less healthy children regarding (a)

enjoying doing things with, (b) being sensitive to, (c) giving money to, (d) buying things for, and

(e) doing things with, all p’s < .05. Fathers appeared to favor less healthy children in the area of

discipline. However, in support of the hypothesis, fathers manifested favoritism towards the

healthier child in the areas of (a) pride, (b) doing things with, (c) listening to, and (d) respecting,

all p’s < .05 (See Table 7). Therefore, Hypothesis 3 was partially supported with fathers

appearing to favor healthier children on several dimensions but interestingly, mothers appearing

to favor less healthy children on several dimensions.

Social competitiveness. In the remaining hypotheses I made predictions regarding the

correlations between specific aspects of social competitiveness (intelligence,

ambition/industriousness, and physical attractiveness) and parental favoritism. For these

characteristics, I made sex-differentiated predictions, in that the relationships between these

predictors and parental favoritism would vary depending on participant sex.

For Hypothesis 4, I expected that male participants who reported being more intelligent

than their sibling to report being the recipient of parental favoritism. I did not expect such a

relationship regarding female participants. To test this hypothesis, I performed separate


Parental Favoritism 55

correlations for male and female participants between original maternal and paternal SIDE

indices and the relative intelligence composite index.

I performed a partial correlation between original SIDE composite indices and the

relative intelligence composite index, controlling for sibling hierarchy 16 (older or younger).

There was no significant correlation between the relative intelligence composite index, as

reported by male participants, and the maternal favoritism composite index, r(132) = .12, p = .17.

Additionally, there was not a significant correlation between the relative intelligence composite

index and the original paternal favoritism composite index, r(132) = -.04, p = .65, after

conducting a partial correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy.

In the second part of this hypothesis I predicted that no significant correlation would be

obtained between female participants’ relative intelligence and parental favoritism. Results,

contrary to expectation, revealed a significant correlation between daughter's relative intelligence

and maternal favoritism, r(267) = .13, p = .03. In other words, mothers favored more intelligent

daughters. There was not a significant correlation between daughters' relative intelligence and

paternal favoritism, r(267) = .11, p = .10, after conducting a partial correlation controlling for

sibling hierarchy. Thus, findings were mixed with intelligence for female participants being

associated with maternal favoritism but not paternal favoritism.

To more fully explore the data, I ran correlations between the relative intelligence

composite indices and each item on the original maternal and paternal SIDEs. Mothers displayed

favoritism towards more intelligent sons in the areas of (a) pride, (b) enjoyment in talking with,

16
A main effect for participant sex had been found as well. Due to making specific predictions based upon
participant sex, I could not control for participant sex for these analyses. However, in Appendix N, I control for
participant sex by running bivariate correlations comparing only same-sex dyads.
Parental Favoritism 56

and (c) listening to opinions, all p’s < .05. Fathers showed favoritism towards less intelligent

sons regarding sensitivity to thoughts and feelings, p = .02 (See Table 8). Mothers showed

favoritism toward more intelligent daughters in the areas of (a) pride, (b) enjoyment in talking

with, (c) giving what was requested, (d) doing favors for, (e) doing things with, (f) being

supportive, (g) listening to the opinion of, and (h) respect, all p’s < .05. Fathers favored more

intelligent daughters in the areas of (a) pride and (b) being supportive of, and (c) listening to

opinions, all p’s < .05 (See Table 9). Thus, it appears that for female participants, greater

intelligence compared to a sibling was associated with parental favoritism more so than was the

case for male participants. Therefore, the data did not support my hypothesis.

For Hypothesis 5, I expected that male participants who reported being more

ambitious/industrious than their sibling to also report being the recipient of parental favoritism. I

did not expect such a relationship regarding female participants. To test this hypothesis, I

performed separate correlations for male and female participants between original maternal and

paternal SIDE composite indices and the ambition/industriousness composite index.

I performed a partial correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy 17 (older or younger).

There was not a significant correlation between relative male ambition/industriousness and

maternal favoritism, r(134) = .03, p = .77. Additionally, there was no correlation between

ambition/industriousness and paternal favoritism, r(134) = .12, p = .16, after conducting a partial

correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy.

17
A main effect for participant sex had been found as well. Due to making specific predictions based upon
participant sex, I could not control for participant sex for these analyses. However, in Appendix N, I control for
participant sex by running bivariate correlations comparing only same-sex dyads.
Parental Favoritism 57

In order to more fully examine the data, I correlated the ambition/industriousness

composite index with each original maternal and paternal SIDE item. Mothers favored more

ambitious/industrious sons in the areas of (a) pride, (b) listening to opinions, and (c) respecting,

all p’s < .05. Additionally, mothers favored less ambitious/industrious sons in the area of

sensitivity to feelings and thoughts, p < .05. Fathers favored more ambitious/industrious sons in

the areas of (a) pride, (b) enjoyment in talking with, and (c) respect, all p’s < .05 (See Table 10).

Therefore, the data did not support my hypothesis because ambition/industriousness was not

associated with parental favoritism for male participants.

The second part of my hypothesis consisted of the prediction that no significant

correlation existed between female participants’ relative ambition/industriousness and parental

favoritism. Analyses indicated that there was no significant correlation between

ambition/industriousness and maternal favoritism, r(269) = .11, p = .06. Additionally, there was

no significant correlation between relative ambition/industriousness and paternal favoritism,

r(269) = .08, p = .20 after conducting a partial correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy.

In order to more fully examine the data, I correlated the relative ambition/industriousness

composite index with each original maternal and paternal SIDE item. Mothers showed favoritism

toward more ambitious/industrious daughters in the areas of (a) pride, (b) enjoyed doing things

with, (c) showing interest in, (d) enjoying talking with, (e) doing favors for, (f) doing things

with, (g) being supportive, (h) listening to opinions, and (i) respecting, all p’s < .05. Fathers

showed favoritism toward more ambitious/industrious daughters in the areas of (a) pride, (b)

being supportive, (c) listening to opinions, and (d) respecting, all p’s < .05 (See Table 11). Thus,

it appears that for female participants, greater ambition/industriousness compared to a sibling


Parental Favoritism 58

was associated with parental favoritism more so than was the case for male participants.

Therefore, my hypothesis was not supported.

For my sixth and final hypothesis, I expected that female participants who reported being

physically more attractive than their sibling would also report being the recipient of parental

favoritism. I did not expect such a relationship regarding male participants. To test this

hypothesis, I performed separate correlations for male and female participants between original

maternal and paternal SIDE composite indices and the relative physical attractiveness composite

index.

I performed a partial correlation between SIDEs and relative physical attractiveness,

controlling for sibling hierarchy 18 (older or younger), as indicated by earlier analyses. There was

not a significant correlation between relative female attractiveness and maternal favoritism,

r(269) = .03, p = .62. Additionally, there was no correlation between relative attractiveness, and

paternal favoritism, r(269) = .07, p = .23, after conducting a partial correlation controlling for

sibling hierarchy.

In order to more fully examine the data, I correlated relative physical attractiveness with

each original maternal and paternal SIDE item. As seen in Table 12, both mothers and fathers

displayed favoritism towards physically more attractive daughters in the areas of a) giving

money to and b) buying things for, all p’s < .05. Fathers also showed favoritism in the area of

enjoying doing things with, p = .04.

18
Exploratory analyses found in Appendix N, examine the association of physical attractiveness and parental
favoritism when looking at only same-sex sibling dyads. Such examinations eliminate potential confounds that
might exist in mixed-pair sibling dyads.
Parental Favoritism 59

The second part of my hypothesis consisted of the prediction that no significant

correlation existed between male participants’ relative physical attractiveness and parental

favoritism. There was not a significant correlation between relative physical attractiveness and

maternal favoritism, r(134) = -.06, p = .48, nor for paternal favoritism, r(134) = .06, p = .51, after

conducting a partial correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy.

In order to more fully examine the data, I correlated physical attractiveness with each

original maternal and paternal SIDE item. Mothers showed favoritism towards less physically

attractive sons in the areas of a) strictness, b) punishing, and c) disciplining, whereas fathers

showed favoritism towards less physically attractive sons in the area of respect, all p’s < .05 (See

Table 13). Therefore, Hypothesis 6 was partially supported. Mothers and fathers appear to favor

physically more attractive female participants on several dimensions, whereas both mothers and

fathers appear to favor less attractive male participants on several dimensions.

Summary and Conclusions

Overall, mixed findings occurred with my hypotheses being supported, partially

supported, as well as not supported. Paternal resemblance data demonstrated strongest support

for my hypotheses. As expected, it appeared that the child who was identified as striking a

greater facial/physical resemblance to the father also tended to be identified as the recipient of

paternal favoritism. Additionally, and as I predicted, there was no significant correlation between

maternal facial/physical resemblance and being identified as the recipient of maternal favoritism.

Although participants reported that mothers and fathers displayed favoritism at

approximately equal rates, participants reported a significant difference between mothers and

fathers in specific areas comprising parental favoritism. In fact, and as expected, participants

reported that fathers significantly manifested favoritism in more areas than mothers. Specifically,
Parental Favoritism 60

fathers displayed favoritism to a greater degree than mothers in the areas of (a) showing interest

in children, (b) doing things with children, and (c) protecting children. Participants also reported

that mothers demonstrated favoritism to a greater degree than fathers regarding the punishment

of children.

Analyses of the socially competitive characteristics provided the least support for my

hypotheses. Partial support for my hypothesis regarding relative health was reached with fathers

demonstrating greater favoritism towards healthier children on several dimensions. However,

mothers were found to favor less healthy children on several dimensions, and there was no

overall significant correlation between health and parental favoritism. Similar findings occurred

regarding relative physical attractiveness, wherein no significant correlations were reached

between relative physical attractiveness and parental favoritism, however, both mothers and

fathers showed favoritism towards physically more attractive female children across several

dimensions.

Results regarding the association of relative intelligence and relative

ambition/industriousness with parental favoritism did not support my hypotheses. No overall

correlations between either male participants’ relative intelligence or ambition/industriousness

with parental favoritism were reached (See Table 14 for correlations between all measures).

Furthermore, both mothers and fathers displayed greater favoritism towards more intelligent

and/or more ambitious/industrious female participants than more intelligent and/or more

ambitious/industrious male participants across several dimensions.


Parental Favoritism 61

DISCUSSION

Parental favoritism is a prevalent phenomenon that is reported by both parents and

children (Harris & Howard, 2001; Suitor & Pillemer, 2007). Despite its prevalence, limited

research has examined potential predictors of parental favoritism. Evolutionary psychologists

have identified two contexts that appear to influence psychological mechanisms associated with

parenting. These contexts include the genetic relatedness between offspring and parent, which is

associated with parental certainty/uncertainty, and the ability of offspring to convert parental

investment into fitness benefits for the parent (e.g., reproductive success). From these identified

contexts, I tested several theoretically-derived hypotheses to identify possible predictors of

parental favoritism. Specifically, I examined whether characteristics believed to be associated

with paternal uncertainty or reproductive success correlated with parental favoritism.

First, I review the major findings from the study and discuss some implications of the

findings. Next, I offer possible explanations for results that were not consistent with my

hypotheses. In the final section, I highlight some of the study’s limitations, and make

recommendations for future studies.

Support for hypotheses. The strongest support for my hypotheses comes from finding a

significant correlation between paternal resemblance and paternal favoritism. Individuals,

whether they be male or female, who purported to share a greater paternal facial resemblance

(relative to their sibling) also tended to be identified as the recipient of paternal favoritism. As

expected, no correlation, however, was obtained between maternal resemblance and maternal

favoritism. Paternal favoritism appeared to be manifested through both psychological (e.g.,

respects us) and financial (e.g., buys us things) means. Evolutionary psychologists posit that

males have developed kin-discrimination mechanisms because they have helped to solve the
Parental Favoritism 62

adaptive problem of cuckoldry. Thus, it appears that physical resemblance plays a unique role for

fathers regarding their parental responses to putative offspring.

Obtaining a relationship between paternal resemblance and paternal favoritism extends

previous research on this topic and contributes to this literature in an important manner. Previous

studies, relying on hypothetical situations, have demonstrated that males might utilize physical

resemblance as a kin-discrimination mechanism (Platek et al., 2002). In Platek et al.’s (2002)

study, participants were asked to assume the role of a caregiver and make judgments on children

who were depicted in photos; participants were not actual mothers or fathers. Thus, one must

project findings from this study towards expected behavior of male participants once they in fact

become fathers. No such "leap of faith" is required for my results because my data were collected

in a more ecologically valid manner, in that actual children reported the behaviors of their actual

parents (i.e., the study contained no hypothetical elements). Additionally, the current research

complements the few studies wherein ecological validity is less of a concern (Burch and Gallup,

2000). In this study, fathers’ relationships with their children were assessed via one question

(quality of the father-child relationship). The current research presents a more comprehensive

assessment of parenting behaviors because I assessed a wider range of parental responses.

Moreover, the Burch & Gallup study relied on a very circumscribed and potentially atypical

sample of men enrolled in a domestic violence treatment program. My sample in contrast is

likely to have produced results that are more generalizable. Lastly, the current research is one of

the few, if not the only, to examine both maternal and paternal physical resemblance in an

ecologically valid manner.

Clinical implications exist regarding the finding that fathers tend to favor children who

more closely resemble them. Differential parental treatment is a commonly discussed matter
Parental Favoritism 63

during psychotherapy (Harris & Howard, 1985). The perception of unfair treatment from parents

appears to impact one’s psychological development and well-being. For example, non-favored

children report greater feelings of anger and depression (Altus, 1970; Elicker, England, &

Sroufe, 1992; Gecas, 1971; Harris & Howard, 1985). Additionally, the non-favored status has

been associated with problems relating to self-esteem and self-worth (Rosenberg, 1965; Ross,

Dalton, & Milgram, 1980; Sears, 1970). Paternal favoritism, in particular, has been associated

with the non-favored child more likely to view oneself as being incompetent and boring (Harris

& Howard, 1985). Specific to males, paternal absence has been associated with greater risk

taking and higher mortality rates in adulthood (Geary, 2000). A non-favored status for daughters

regarding the father has been associated with a number of costs as well including greater mistrust

of others (Erikson, 1998; Fuori, 2005).

Knowledge of the relationship between paternal resemblance and paternal favoritism

might aid in the clinical treatment of non-favored individuals seeking therapy. Mental health

professionals who are aware of the correlation between resemblance and favoritism can pursue

this line of questioning in treatment. From the clinical discussion and exploration of this topic,

clients may develop insight into some underlying contributing factors towards their feelings of

mistrust, lowered self-esteem, etc. noted in the research examining the consequences of

favoritism. Insight is believed by many to serve a crucial role regarding the change process

(Carey, Carey, Stalker, Mullan, Murray, & Spratt, 2007).

Hypotheses that were not supported. In general, my prediction that fathers would

manifest more favoritism than mothers was not strongly supported. Due to the severe paucity of

research examining paternal favoritism, such a finding cannot be contrasted with the previous

literature. Thus, my study is one of the few to specifically examine fathers and compare their
Parental Favoritism 64

parenting behaviors to those of mothers. Although results pertaining to this hypothesis were not

overwhelmingly consistent, the current study did indicate that certain differences might exist

between mothers and fathers regarding their parenting behaviors when one examines specific

behaviors. For example, fathers were shown to demonstrate more favoritism than mothers in

several activities: showing interest in children, protecting children, and engaging in activities

with children. In addition, mothers were shown to display more favoritism than fathers regarding

punishing children. Such findings suggest that parenting behaviors might be rather circumscribed

with specific behaviors being relegated to either mothers or fathers, and thus each parent

manifests favoritism through different avenues. Consequently, a strength of this study is that I

examined favoritism as a multi-faceted, complex construct rather than as a general or uni-

dimensional one.

Viewing parental favoritism as a multi-faceted, rather than uni-dimensional construct

holds numerous implications for future research. Previous research typically viewed favoritism

as uni-dimensional (Suitor et al., 2006). Because the current study’s findings appear to

demonstrate that parental favoritism is a complex construct comprised of numerous behaviors,

some of which appear to be more relevant for a particular parent (mother or father), viewing

favoritism in a uni-dimensional or general way strongly handicaps one’s ability to state that

favoritism in fact, was assessed. In fact, it appears more prudent, as well as might result in

potentially greater findings, if future studies highlight parental favoritism’s complex nature by

developing novel measures wherein favoritism is not assessed by composite scores or uni-

dimensional measures but rather is assessed across several dimensions.

Overall, my predictions regarding the roles of relative health, relative intelligence,

relative ambition/industriousness, and relative physical attractiveness were not strongly


Parental Favoritism 65

supported by the data. The lack of strong support for the hypothesis that healthy children would

be favored is particularly surprising because previous studies demonstrate that health appears to

influence parental treatment (Baratt, Roach, & Leavitt, 1996; Hagen, 1999; Mann, 1992). A

potential reason why findings were not supportive might be the fact that limited differences on

health existed between participants and siblings. The data show that on average, participants

indicated equitable health with siblings. Therefore, it would be unlikely for health to serve as a

correlate of parental favoritism because no differences existed between the two groups

(participant and sibling). In the previous studies wherein health was shown to be a predictor of

favoritism, rather drastic health differences existed between siblings (e.g., one sibling has a

congenital abnormality whereas the other does not). Such drastic differences might explain why

findings indicated health to be a predictor of parental favoritism because it was a much more

salient issue.

A priori, I expected participants and siblings to be relatively healthy, in part because it is

unlikely for individuals with severely compromised health (e.g., cerebral palsy) to attend college.

Therefore I felt my study offered potentially unique contributions in that I could assess rather

subtle influences of health regarding parental favoritism. However, I might have used a measure

that was not sensitive enough to measure subtle health differences. The current study’s health

measure consisted of only a few general items, thus reducing my likelihood of assessing subtle

health differences. Future studies might wish to develop a more comprehensive health measure

in order to more effectively assess for potential health differences, especially if participants are

likely to be relatively healthy.

A second unexpected finding on the topic of health included several findings

demonstrating both mothers and fathers favoring less healthy children on certain parenting
Parental Favoritism 66

dimensions (e.g., enjoy doing things with). At first blush, this finding is unexpected due to its

counterintuitive nature. It would seem counterproductive for a parent to invest limited resources

in a child who might not reach sexual maturity and be reproductively successful. However,

recent research indicates that when resources are plentiful, parents tend to invest more in less

healthy children as compared to healthier children (Beaulieu & Bugental, 2008; Bugental &

Beaulieu, 2003). The logic is that parents with abundant resources can afford to invest a greater

amount of resources in a child with lower reproductive, compared to a healthier (i.e., higher

reproductive value) child because the parent still has an adequate amount of resources remaining

to invest in other children. In so doing, the reproductive value of healthy children is not

compromised and the parent might receive an added reproductive success benefit in aiding a

reproductively compromised child in reaching a status wherein reproductive success is possible.

Thus, the parent now has a greater number of children who are reproductively successful than a

parent who invests all resources in healthy children, neglecting the health-compromised children

(Beaulieu & Bugental, 2008). Thus, one reason for the current study’s finding of parents

favoring less healthy children is that these children are from families where the parents have the

luxury of investing greater resources in less healthy children without compromising the

reproductive success of the healthier children. In fact, 20% of participants reported family

incomes of over $100,000. Families with this much earning power might have enough resources

to be able to invest more heavily in less healthy children without compromising the reproductive

success of healthier children. Therefore, my study provides added support to the theory being

posed by Bugental and Beaulieu.

Previous studies indicate that intelligence and ambition/industriousness are associated

with parental favoritism. The current literature reinforced, as well as made such findings more
Parental Favoritism 67

precise, because I examined sex differences whereas the previous literature had not. Both

mothers and fathers were reported to favor the more intelligent and more ambitious/industrious

daughter across more domains than was true for sons. Such a finding was not expected because I

believed that intelligence and ambition/industriousness were more closely associated with male,

rather than female, reproductive success.

Review of the mate preferences literature might shed light on why parents appeared to

favor the more intelligent daughter. Buss et al.’s (1990) cross-cultural study examining mate

preferences found that males rated intelligence in females as a desirable trait. Specifically, males

ranked intelligence second in their listing of desirable mate characteristics. As such, It would

appear that intelligence might be associated with female reproductive success. Taking this into

account, it is then not unexpected for parents to favor more intelligent daughters because

intelligence is associated with female reproductive success.

However, intelligence was also highly ranked as a desirable mate characteristic for males

(Buss et al., 1990). Therefore, it remains uncertain why the intelligence of males was not

associated with maternal or paternal favoritism. Regarding male reproductive success, stronger

correlates than intelligence or ambition/industriousness might exist. Based upon evolutionary

theory, I held the belief that intelligence correlated with resource acquisition, which then

correlated with male reproductive success. Therefore, women find intelligent, ambitious males to

be desirable mates because such traits are associated with a male’s ability to acquire important

resources. With my findings indicating that male intelligence did not significantly correlate with

favoritism, it is possible that stronger correlates for male reproduce exist. In other words,

additional characteristics exist that are more predictive of a male’s ability to achieve dominance,

high social status, and thus, greater resource acquisition.


Parental Favoritism 68

Research indicates that “toughness” and athleticism are associated with social status for

males (Vaillancourt & Hymel, 2006). As noted earlier, people of higher social status are treated

with more respect and deference, obtain better food, larger territory, and have access to more

desirable mates (Buss, 2007). It is possible that factors such as “toughness” and athleticism are

more predictive of male reproductive success than is intelligence. Additionally, characteristics

associated with “toughness,” such as physical strength and height might also serve as better

correlates for male reproductive success. Therefore, inclusion of these characteristics in the

current study might have produced more robust findings than utilizing intelligence and

ambition/industriousness as predictors for social status and male reproductive success.

Several reasons exist for why my physical attractiveness predictions were not supported.

First, my relative attractiveness measure might have poorly assessed physical attractiveness. Few

in-depth, self-report attractiveness measures seemingly exist. The majority of studies examining

physical attractiveness appear to utilize rating groups, wherein a number of individuals examine

photographs and then an attractiveness score for the photographed individual is determined

(Brown, Cash, & Noles, 1986; Cunningham, Roberts, Barbee, Druen, & Wu, 1995; Mathes,

Brenna, Haugen, & Rice, 1985). My study solely relied on self-report. The fact that the majority

of studies assessing physical attractiveness do not use self-report, suggests that self-report is a

less ideal measure.

BMI, an objective measure, might have served as a better measure for physical

attractiveness. Recent research indicates that BMI is a significant predictor of attractiveness

(Cornelissen, Tovee, & Bateson, 2009). Thus, using BMI to assess physical attractiveness seems

promising, and the BMI’s reported by participants might be accurate, objective predictors of

attractiveness. Unfortunately, participants were not asked to report the BMI’s of their respective
Parental Favoritism 69

sibling. This failure rendered the current study’s author unable to compare participants’ BMI’s

with those of their siblings’. Therefore, BMI could not be utilized as a measure for physical

attractiveness. Due to the recent evidence that BMI serves as a measure for physical

attractiveness, it is recommended that future studies utilize this measure.

Besides the specific speculations I have offered to explain a lack of support for several of

my hypotheses, several global reasons exist for why I did not achieve all expected findings. First,

my study relied on self-report. A plethora of social psychology research has repeatedly

demonstrated that a majority of people tend to view themselves as better than average (Alicke &

Govorun, 2005; Taylor and Brown, 1988). The current study does not appear to be immune from

the “better-than-average” effect. For example, 66% of participants reported being more

intelligent than their sibling; compared to only 28% of participants indicating that their sibling

was more intelligent. Sixty-two percent reported being more ambitious/industrious than their

sibling; compared to only 23% who reported that their sibling was more ambitious/industrious.

Differences remained consistent regarding physical attractiveness but were less drastic. Thus,

one must review the self-report findings skeptically, especially for factors deemed socially

desirable (e.g., intelligence, physical attractiveness).

Two specific methodological changes could be implemented to address the problem of

the “better-than-average” effect. First, future studies might wish to seek corroborating evidence.

For example, the current study would have benefited by also obtaining data from participants’

parents, as well as from their compared-to sibling. Inclusion of these two parties would have

allowed me to compare responses across parties, therefore allowing me to assess the veracity of

self-report data from participants. For example, the case where both parents and both children

(participant and compared-to sibling) indicate child X as being more intelligent is more
Parental Favoritism 70

compelling than when only the participant self-identifies as being more intelligent, whereas

parental and sibling report indicate otherwise.

Second, future studies should include more objective measures than those utilized by the

current study. For example, intelligence could be determined via standardized intelligence

measures (e.g., Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition). Regarding attractiveness,

participants could provide photos of themselves and their siblings. Photos would be rated by a

panel of independent raters, thus establishing a more “objective” determination of attractiveness.

Usage of more objective measures reduces bias, as well as would allow for the researcher to

reach greater confidence regarding the veracity of reporting.

Concluding remarks. I wish to end the discussion highlighting the strengths of the current

study. First, my study viewed parental favoritism as a complex construct and as such, I made

efforts to assess its complexities. Typically, studies define parental favoritism in a rather narrow

manner such as simply asking participants if their parents displayed favoritism (Zervas & Martin,

1994 ) or asking parents if they felt emotionally closer to a particular child (Suitor, Sechrist,

Steinhour, & Pillemer, 2006). By measuring favoritism across a range of psychological and

financial areas, I was able to move beyond simply reporting prevalence rates. Second, I included

fathers in the study. The majority of studies focus solely on mothers, thus ignoring 50% of the

parent dyad. Additionally, I generated theoretically-driven hypotheses of potential correlates of

parental favoritism. Many studies attempt to identify such correlates post-hoc (Zervas &

Sherman, 1994). By generating theoretically-driven hypotheses, my findings, particularly those

regarding paternal resemblance are more compelling than in the cases where such findings are

simply “discovered” upon the study’s completion. Finally, the current study’s findings regarding

paternal resemblance, the favoring of less healthy children, and sex differences offer additional
Parental Favoritism 71

avenues for future studies to examine, as well as demonstrates the potentially valuable

contribution to the literature that can be made by applying evolutionary-based theories towards

unexplained, yet highly salient phenomenon such as parental favoritism.


Parental Favoritism 72

REFERENCES

Ackerman, P. & Heggestadt, E. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for

overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219 – 245.

Adler, A. (1932). What life should mean to you. A Porter (Ed.). London: George Allen and

Uwin.

Adler-Baeder, F. (2006). What do we know about the physical abuse of stepchildren? A review

of the literature. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 44(3), 67 – 81.

Alicke, M., Smith, R., & Klotz, M. (1986). Judgments of physical attractiveness: The role of

faces and bodies: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 381 – 389.

Alicke, M. & Govorun, O. (2005). The better-than-average effect. In M.D. Alicke, D.A.

Dunning, & J.I. Krueger (Eds.), The self in social judgment (pp. 85 – 106). New York:

Psychology Press.

Alley, T. & Scully, K. (1994). The impact of actual and perceived changes in body weight on

women’s physical attractiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 15(4), 535 – 542.

Anderson, K., Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers

I: Reports from Albuquerque men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 405 – 431.

Anderson, K., Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers

II: Reports by Xhosa high school students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 433 –

451.

Ashwell, M., Cole, T., & Dixon, A. (1985). Obesity: New insight into the anthropometric

classification of fat distribution shown by computed tomography. British Medical

Journal, 290, 1692 – 1694.


Parental Favoritism 73

Baker, R. & Bellis, M. (1995). Human Sperm Competition. London: Chapman & Hall.

Baker, L. & Daniels, D. (1990). Nonshared environmental influences and personality

differences in adult twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 103 – 110.

Baratt, M., Roach, M., & Leavitt, L. (1996). The impact of low-risk prematurity on maternal

behavior and toddler outcomes. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19,

581-602.

Barden, R., Ford, M., Jensen, A., Rogers-Salyer, M., & Salyer, K. (1989). Effects of

craniofacial deformity in infancy on the quality of mother-infant interactions. Child

Development, 60, 819-824.

Beaulieu, D. & Bugental, D. (2008). Contingent parental investment: An evolutionary

framework for understanding early interaction between mothers and children. Evolution

and Human Behavior, 29, 249 – 255.

Bell, D. (2001). Evolution of parental caregiving. Personality and Social Psychology Review,

5(3), 216 – 229.

Bjorklund, D. & Shackelord, T. (1999). Differences in parental investment contribute to

important differences between men and women. Current Directions in Psychological

Science, 8(3), 86 – 90.

Bjorklund, D. & Yunger, J. (2001). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A useful

framework for evaluating the evolution of parenting. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1,

63 – 66.

Brackbill, Y., Kitch, D., & Noffsinger, W. (1988). The perfect child (from an elderly parent’s

point of view). Journal of Aging Studies, 2, 243 – 254.


Parental Favoritism 74

Bredart, S. & French, R. (1999). Do babies resemble their fathers more than their mothers? A

failure to replicate Christenfeld and Hill. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 129 – 135.

Brody, G. & Burke, M. (1994). Sibling relationships and their association with parental

differential treatment. In E. M. Hetherington, D. Reiss, & R. Plomin (Eds.). Separate

social worlds of siblings: The impact of nonshared environment on development (pp. 129

– 142). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brody, G., Stoneman, Z., & Burke, M. (1987). Child temperaments, maternal differential

behavior, and sibling relationships. Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 354 – 362.

Brown, T., Cash, T., & Noles, S. (1986). Perceptions of physical attractiveness among college

students: Selected determinants and methodological matters. Journal of Social

Psychology, 126(3), 305 – 317.

Bugental, D. & Beaulieu, D. (2003). A bio-social cognitive approach to understanding and

promoting the outcomes of children with medical and physical disorders. In R. Kail (Ed.),

Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, 229 – 258). New York: Academic

Press.

Burch, R. & Gallup, G. (2000). Perceptions of paternal resemblance predict family violence.

Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 429 – 435.

Buss, D. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses testing

in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1-49.

Buss, D. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Pearson

Education, Inc.
Parental Favoritism 75

Buss, D., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Asherian, A., Biaggio, A., & 45 other co-authors. (1990).

International preferences in selecting mates. A study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-

Cultural Psychology, 21, 5 – 47.

Buss, D., & Schmitt, D. (1993). Sexual strategies: An evolutionary perspective on human

mating. Psychological Review, 2, 204 – 232.

Carey, T., Carey, M., Stalker, K., Mullan, R, Murray, L., & Spratt, M. (2007). Psychological

change from the inside looking out: A qualitative investigation. Counseling and

Psychotherapy Research, 7(3), 178 – 187.

Chalfant, D. (1994). Birth order, perceived parental favoritism, and feelings toward parents.

Individual Psychology, 50, 53 – 57.

Clutton-Brock, T. & Vincent, A. (1991). Sexual selection and the potential reproductive rates of

males and females. Science, 351, 58-60.

Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (1999).

The functional anatomy of inhibition processes investigated with the Hayling task.

Neuroimage, 14, 258-267.

Cornelissen, P., Tovee, M., & Bateson, M. (2009). Patterns of subcutaneous fat deposition and

the relationship between body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio: Implications for models

of physical attractiveness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 256, 343 – 350.

Cornell, D. & Grossberg, I. (1987). Family environment and personality adjustment in gifted

program children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 31, 59 – 64.

Costa, P. & McRae, R (1992). Professional manual: Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-

PI-R) and NEO five-factor-inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment

Resources.
Parental Favoritism 76

Cox, C., Dupont, E., Whitehead, G., Ebert, M., Nguyen, K, Peltz, E., Peckham, D., Cantor, A,

Reintgen, D. (2002). Age and body mass index may increase the chance of failure in

sentinel lymph node biopsy for women with breast cancer. Breast Journal, 8(2), 88 -91.

Crase, S., Foss, C., Colbert, K. (1981). Children’s self-concept and perception of parents’

behavior. The Journal of Psychology, 108, 297 – 303.

Crouter, A., Manke, B., & McHale, S. (1995). The family context of gender intensification in

early adolescence. Child Development, 66, 317 – 329.

Cunningham, M., Roberts, A., Barbee, A., Druen, P., & Wu, C. (1995). “Their ideas of beauty

are, on the whole, the same as ours”: Consistency and variability in the cross-cultural

perception of female physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 68(2), 261 – 279.

Daniels, D., Dunn, J., Furstenberg, F., & Plomin, R. (1985). Environmental differences within

the family and adjustment differences within pairs of adolescent siblings. Child

Development, 56, 764 – 774.

Daniels, D., & Plomin, R. (1985). Differential experience of siblings in the same family.

Developmental Psychology, 21, 747 – 760.

Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1982). Whom are newborn babies said to resemble? Ethology and

Sociobiology, 3, 69 -78.

Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.

Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1990). Is parent – offspring conflict sex linked? Freudian and

Darwinian models. Journal of Personality, 58, 163 – 189.

Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford, England: Oxford Univerity Press.
Parental Favoritism 77

De Fruyt, F., Aluja, A., Garcia, L., Rolland, J., & Jung, S. (2006). Positive presentation

management and intelligence and the personality differentiation by intellignece

hypothesis in job applicants. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14(2),

101 – 112.

Despres, J., Prudhomme, D., Pouliot, M., Tremblay, A., & Bouchard, C. (1991). Estimation of

deep abdominal adipose-tissue accumulation from simple anthropometric measurements

in men. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 54, 471 – 477.

Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285 – 290.

Duberman, L. (1975). The reconstituted family. A study of remarried couples and their

children. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.

Dunn, J. & McGuire, S. (1992). Sibling and peer relationships in childhood. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 67-106.

Dunn, J. & Plomin, R. (1991). Why are siblings so different? The significance of differences in

sibling experiences within the family. Family Processes, 30, 271-283.

Eagly, A., Makhijani, M., Ashmore, R., & Longo, L. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but…:

A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype.

Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109 -128.

Elder, G., Van Nguyen, T., & Caspi, A. (1985). Linking family hardship to children’s lives.

Child Development, 56, 361-375.


Parental Favoritism 78

Essock-Vitale, S. & Mcguire, M. (1988). What 70 million years hath wrought: Sexual histories

and reproductive success of a random sample of American women. In L. Betzig, M.

Borgerhoff Mulder, & P. Turke (Eds.), Human reproductive behavior: A Darwinian

perspective (pp. 221-235). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Evans, K. & Brase, G. (2007). Assessing sex differences and similarities in mate preferences:

Above and beyond demand characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,

24(5), 781-791.

Fan, J., Dai, W., Liu, F., & Wu, J. (2005). Visual perception of male body attractiveness.

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 219 – 226.

Fisher, R. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Fisher, M. & Voracek, M. (2006). The shape of beauty: determinants of female physical

attractiveness. Journal of Cosmetic Dermatology, 5(2), 190 – 194.

Flinn, M., Quinlan, R., Decker, S., Turner, M., & England, B. (1996). Male-female differences

in effects of parental absence on glucocorticoid stress response. Human Nature, 7, 125-

162.

Freud, S. (1970). A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Geary, D. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment.

Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55 – 77.

Geary, D. & Flinn, M. (2001). Evolution of parental behavior and the human family.

Parenting: Science and Practice 1, 5 – 61.

Gecas, V. (1971). Parental behavior and dimensions of adolescent self-evaluation. Sociometry,

34(4), 466 – 482.


Parental Favoritism 79

Goldberg, L. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality inventory measuring the

lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, &

F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe, 7, (pp. 7 – 28). Tilburg, The

Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.

Goldberg, L., Johnson, J., Eber, H., Hogan, R., Ashton, M., Cloninger, R., & Gough, H. (2006).

The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality

measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84 – 96.

Gross, M. (2005). The evolution of parental care. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 37 –

45.

Grych, J. (2001). On the origins of fathering: Implications of evolutionary perspective for

understanding links among marriage, divorce, and men’s parenting. Parenting: Science

and Practice, 1, 67 – 70.

Hagen, E. (1999). The functions of postpartum depression. Evolution and Human Behavior,

20, 325-359.

Hamilton, W. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. I and II. Journal of

Theoretical Biology, 7, 1 – 52.

Harris, I. & Howard, K. (2001). Correlates of perceived parental favoritism. The Journal of

Genetic Psychology, 146, 45 – 56.

Hart, S., Carrington, H., Tronick, E., & Carroll, S. (2004). When infants lose exclusive

maternal attention: Is it jealousy? Infancy, 6(1), 57-78.

Henss, R. (1995). Waist-to-hip ratio and attractiveness. Replication and extension. Personality

and Individual Differences, 19, 479 – 488.


Parental Favoritism 80

Henss, R. (2000). Waist-to-hip ratio and female attractiveness. Evidence from photographic

stimuli and methodological considerations. Personality and Individual Differences, 28,

501 – 513.

Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. (1996). Ache life history: The ecology and demography of a foraging

people. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Howell, D. (1992). Statistical methods for psychology. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.

International Personality Item Pool: A Scientifc Collaboratory for the Development of

Advanced Measures of Personality Traits and Other Individual Differences

(http://ipip.ori.org/). Internet Web Site.

Jackson, D., Paunonen, S., & Tremblay, P. (2000). Six Factor Personality Questionnaire. Port

Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems.

Jenkinson, C., Coulter, A., & Wright, L. (1993). The SF – 36 Health Survey Questionnaire:

Normative data from a large random sample of working age adults. British Medical

Journal, 306, 1435 – 1440.

Keenan, J., Nelson, A., O’Connor, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the

right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305.

Kiracofe, N. (1992). Child-perceived parental favoritism and self-reported personal

characteristics. Individual Psychology, 48(3), 350 – 356.

Kiracofe, N. & Kiracofe, H. (1990). Child-perceived parental favoritism and birth order.

Individual Psychology, 46, 74 – 81.

Korchmaros, J. & Kenny, D. (2001). Emotional closeness as a mediator of the effect of genetic

relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science, 12(3), 262 – 265.


Parental Favoritism 81

Kyl-Heku, L., & Buss, D. (1996). Tactics as units of analysis in personality psychology: An

illustration using tactics of hierarchy negotiation. Personality and Individual Differences,

21, 497-517.

Lamb, M. & Elster, A. (1985). Adolescent mother-infant-father relationships. Developmental

Psychology, 21, 768-773.

Langlois, J., Ritter, J., Casey, R., & Sawin, D. (1995). Infant attractiveness predicts maternal

behaviors and attitudes. Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 464-472.

Leibel, R., Edens, N., Fried, S. (1989). Physiologic basis for the control of body fat distribution

in humans. Annual Reviews of Nutrition, 9, 417 – 443.

Lund, C., Tamnes, C., Moestue, C., Buss, D., & Vollrath, M. (2007). Tactics of hierarchy

negotiation. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 25 – 44.

MacDonald, K. (1993). Parent-child play: An evolutionary perspective. In K. MacDonald

(Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications (pp. 113-143). Albany: State

University of New York Press.

Maheshwan, A., Hamilton, M., & Bhattacherya, S. (2008). Effect of female age on the diagnostic

categories of infertility. Human Reproduction, 23(3), 538 – 542.

Mann, J. (1992). Nurturance of negligence: Maternal psychology and behavioral preference

among preterm twins. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind

(pp. 367-390). New York: Oxford University Press.

Markey, C., Tinsley, B., Ericksen, A., Ozer, D., & Markey. (2002). Preadolescent’s perception of

female’s body size and shape: evolutionary and social learning perspective. Journal of

Youth and Adolescents, 31, 137 – 147.


Parental Favoritism 82

Marti, B., Tuomilehto, J., Saloman, V., Kartovaara, L., Korhonen, H., & Pietinen. (1991). Body

fat distribution in the Finnish population: Environmental determinants and predictive

power for cardiovascular risk factor levels. Journal of Epidemiology and Community

Health, 45, 131 – 137.

Mathes, E., Brennan, S., Haugen, P., & Rice, H. (1985). Ratings of physical attractiveness as a

function of age. Journal of Social Psychology, 125(2), 157 – 169.

McHale, S. & Pawletko, T. (1992). Differential treatment of siblings in two family contexts.

Child Development, 63, 68 – 81.

McLain, D., Setters, D., Moulton, M., & Pratt, A. (2000). Ascription of resemblance of

newborns by parents and nonrelatives. Evolution and Human Behavior 21, 11 – 23.

Nielsen, L. (2007). College daughters’ relationships with their fathers: A 15 year study.

College Study Journal, 41, 112 – 121.

Myers, S. (2004). Religion and intergenerational assistance: Distinct differences by adult

children’s gender and parent’s marital status. The Sociological Quarterly, 45, 67 –89.

Nevid, J. (1984). Sex differences in factors of romantic attraction. Sex Roles, 11, 401 – 411.

Newman, J. (1996). The more the merrier? Effects of family size and sibling spacing on

sibling relationships. Childcare Health Development, 22, 285-302.

Pagel, M. (1997). Desperately concealing father: A theory of parent – infant resemblance.

Animal Behavior, 53, 973 – 981.

Pallone, N. & Hennessy, J. (1989 - 1993). Review of the SF – 36 Health Survey. Located in

Mental Measurements Yearbook Database.


Parental Favoritism 83

Perusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the

relationship at proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 267-

322.

Platek, S., Burch, R., Panyavin, I., Wasserman, B., Gallup, G. (2002). Reactions to children’s

faces: Resemblance affects males more than females. Evolution and Human Behavior,

23, 159 -166.

Platek, S., Keenan, J., & Mohamed, F. (2005). Sex differences in the neural correlates of child

facial resemblance: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 25, 1336 – 1344.

Platek, S. & Shackelford, T. (2006). Female infidelity and paternal uncertainty: Evolutionary

perspectives on male anti-cuckoldry tactics. Cambridge University Press.

Pleck, J. (1997). Paternal involvement: Levels, sources, and consequences. In M.E. Lamb

(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (3rd ed., pp. 66-103). New York: Wiley.

Price, G. (1970). Selection and covariance. Nature, 227, 520-521.

Puhl, R. & Boland, F. (2001). Predicting female physical attractiveness. Psychology, Evolution,

and Gender, 3, 27 – 46.

Read, J., Quinn, R., & Hoefer, M. (1987). Measuring overall health: An evaluation of three

important approaches. Journal of Chronic Diseases(Supp.), 75 – 215.

Reid, A. (1997). Locality or class. Spatial and social differentials in infant and child mortality

in England and Wales, 1895-1911. In C.A. Corsini and P.P. Viazzo (Eds.), The decline of

infant and child mortality (pp. 129-154). The Hague, the Netherlands: martinus Nijhoff.
Parental Favoritism 84

Rogers, R., Rammani, N., Mackay, C., Wilson, J., Jezzard, Pl, Carter, C., Smith, S. (2002).

Distinct portions of anterior cingulated cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated

by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biological

Psychiatry, 55, 594-602.

Sapolsky, R. (1986). Glucocorticoid toxicity in the hippocampus: Reversal by supplementation

with brain fuels. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 2240-2244.

Sears, R. (1970). A treatment of worth. Contemporary Psychology, 14, 146 – 147.

Shackelford, T. & Goetz, A. (2007). Adaptation to sperm competition in humans. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 47 – 50.

Shackelford, T., Schmitt, D., & Buss, D. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate

preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), 447 – 458.

Singh, D. (1993a). Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip

ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 293 – 307.

Singh, D. (1993b). Body shape and women’s attractiveness. The critical role of waist-to-hip

ratio. Human Nature, 4, 297 – 321.

Singh, D. (1995). Female judgment of male attractiveness and desirability for relationships: Role

of waist-to-hip ratio and financial status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

69(6), 1089 – 1101.

Singh, S. & Randall, P. (2007). Beauty is in the eye of the plastic surgeon: Waist-hip ratio

(WHR) and women’s attractiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 329 –

340.
Parental Favoritism 85

Suitor, J. & Pillemer, K. (2000). Did mom really love you best? Developmental histories,

status transitions, and parental favoritism in later life families. Motivation and Emotion,

24(2), 105 – 120.

Suitor, J. & Pillemer, K. (2006). Choosing daughters: Exploring why mothers favor adult

daughters over sons. Sociological Perspectives, 49(2), 139 – 160.

Suitor, J. & Pillemer, K. (2007). Mothers’ Favoritism in Later Life. The role of children’s birth

order. Research on Aging, 29, 32-55.

Suitor, J., Sechrist, J., Steinhour, M., & Pillemer, K. (2006). “I’m sure she chose me!” Accuracy

of children’s report of mother’s favoritism in later life families. Family Relations, 55, 526

– 538.

Swami, V., Miller, R., Furnham, A., Penke, L., Tovee, M. (2008). The influence of men’s sexual

strategies on perceptions of women’s bodily attractiveness, health, and fertility.

Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 98 – 107.

Swami, V., Smith, J., Tsiokris, A., Georgiades, C., Sangareau, Y., Tovee, M., & Furnham, A.

(2007). Male physical attractiveness in Britain and Greece: A cross-cultural study. The

Journal of Social Psychology, 147, 15 – 26.

Swami, V. & Tovee, M. (2008). The muscular male: A comparison of the physical attractiveness

preferences of gay and heterosexual men. International Journal of Men’s Health, 7, 59 –

71.

Taylor, P. & Glenn, N. (1976). The utility of education and attractiveness for females’ status

attainment through marriage. American Sociological Review, 41, 484-498.

Taylor, S. & Brown, J. (1988). Illusions and well-being: A social psychological perspective on

mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193 – 210.


Parental Favoritism 86

Tooley, G., Karakis, M., Stokes, M., & Ozanne-Smith, J. (2006). Generalizing the Cinderella

effect to unintentional childhood fatalities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 224-230.

Tovee, M. & Cornelissen. (2001). Female and male perceptions of female physical attractiveness

in front-view and profile. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 391 – 402.

Tovee, M., Mason, S., Emery, J., McClusky, S., & Cohen-Tovee, E. (1997). Super models: Stick

insects or hour glasses. The Lancet, 350, 1474 – 1475.

Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual

selection and the descent of man: 1871 – 1971 (pp. 136 – 179). Chicago: Aldine.

Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249 – 264.

Updergraff, K., McHale, S., Crouter, A., & Kupanoff, K. (2001). Parents’ involvement in

adolescents’ peer relationships: A comparison of mothers’ and fathers’ roles. Journal of

Marriage and Family, 63, 655 – 668.

U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1978). 1976 survey of institutionalized persons: A study of

persons receiving long-term care. Current population reports. (Special Studies Series P –

23, No. 69). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Vaillancourt, T. & Hymel, S. (2005). Aggression and social status: The moderating roles of sex

and peer-valued characteristics. Aggressive Behavior, 32(6), 486 – 495.

Volk, A., Lukjanczuk, J., & Quinsey, V. (2005). Influence of infant and child facial cues of

low body weight on adults’ ratings of adoption preferences, cuteness, and health. Infant

Mental Health Journal, 26(5), 459-469.

Ware, J. & Sherbourne, C. (1992). The MOS36-item short-form health survey (SF – 36).

Medical Care, 30(6), 473 – 483.


Parental Favoritism 87

Wilson, J., Tripp, D., & Boland, F. (2005). The relative contributions of waist-to-hip ratio and

body mass index to judgments of attractiveness. Sexualities, Evolution, and Gender, 7(3),

245 – 267.

Zervas, L. & Sherman, M. (1994). The relationship between perceived parental favoritism and

self-esteem. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155, 25 – 33.


Parental Favoritism 88

APPENDIX A

CONSENT FORM

Department of Psychology
Bowling Green, OH 43403-0228
Bowling Green State University Phone: (419) 372-2301
FAX: (419) 372-6013
Web Page:http//www.bgsu.edu/departments/psych/

Thoughts on Myself and My Family Fall, 2008


My name is Anthony Lauricella and I am the primary investigator for this study being conducted
at Bowling Green State University. You are invited to be in a research study on family
relationships. You must be at least 18 years old to participate in this study. This study is my
Dissertation Project for the Psychology department. In this study, I am interested in better
understanding personal attributes of people, as well as their family relationships.

You will also be asked to provide some demographic information about (e.g., age, sex) yourself,
and answer some questions about yourself (e.g. your personality, work habits).

• Before the study begins, there are several things for you to note:

1. There are no right or wrong answers for the questionnaires used in this study. I am
interested solely in your personal opinions. The information you provide will
remain anonymous and others will not know of the answers you provided.

2. There are no known risks associated with you participating in this study.

3. Taking part in this study is your choice. If at any time during the study you would like to
quit for any reason please tell the experimenter. You will experience no penalty should
you decide not to participate or quit. Your participation in this study is expected to take
about 30 - 45 minutes.
Parental Favoritism 89

4. You will receive 1 research credit for participating in this study. Your credit will be
noted on the experimetrix website which you can access at
http://www.experimetrix.com/bgsu/.

5. At the end of the study you will be given information about what I am trying to learn by
conducting this study. You will also receive information about how you may obtain the
results of this study when they are available.

6. I hope that by participating in this research you are able to gain an increased
understanding of psychological theory and how theory testing is conducted.

7. If you have any questions or comments concerning this research you should
contact Anthony Lauricella at 419-308-5578 or alauric@bgsu.edu, or my advisor Dr.
Anne Gordon at 419- 372-8161 or akg@bgsu.edu. Additionally, if you have any
questions about the conduct of this study or any concerns about your rights as a
research participant, you may contact the Chair of the Human Subjects Review Board
at 419-372-7716 or hsrb@bgnet.bgsu.edu.

If what is going to be asked of you is clear and you agree to voluntarily participate in
this study please click on the “OK” button to indicate your informed consent.

• If you wish to decline from participating in this study please click on the “NO” button
and close the webpage.
Parental Favoritism 90

APPENDIX B

INSTRUCTIONS

You will now be asked a series of questions. Some questions will ask you about how you and a
fully-biological brother or sister were treated by your parent(s). Other questions will ask about
features, such as your personality and your behaviors.

For ALL questions, we would like you to compare yourself to the same fully-biological sibling.
In this context, a fully-biological sibling refers to a brother or sister who has the same biological
mother and biological father as you. As you answer the questions throughout the survey, please
respond to each question by comparing yourself to the same sibling for the entirety of the study.
Finally, please do NOT compare yourself to a step-, half-, or adopted sibling. For this phase of
this research, we are only interested in the relationship between fully-biological siblings.

For those of you with more than one sibling, we would like you to answer every question on
every survey in reference to the one fully-biological sibling who is CLOSEST to you in age. For
example, if you are 18 and you have a brother who is 10 (an 8-year age difference) and a sister
who is 20 (a 2-year age difference), you will answer every question in reference to yourself and
your 20-year-old sibling, because that sibling is the one who is closer to you in age. If you are 18
and you have a brother who is 13 (a 5-year difference) and one who is 23 (also a 5-year
difference), think about which sibling you spent more time at home with and answer the
questions in reference to yourself and that sibling.

Remember: It is important that for all of the questions, you compare yourself to one sibling and
that you compare yourself to the same sibling for all questions.

1. Check the statement that is true for you.

I have one full sibling.

I have more than one full sibling.

2. Type in the age of your full sibling.

3. My fully biological sibling is:

male

female
Parental Favoritism 91

APPENDIX C

SIBLING INVNTORY OF DIFFERENTIAL EXPERIENCE (SIDE)

This questionnaire is to be completed in reference to your BIOLOGICAL MOTHER/FATHER.


It is not to be completed for stepmothers/fathers or for adoptive mothers/fathers. This
questionnaire is designed to ask you how similarly or differently your mother/father has
generally treated you and your fully biological sibling (brother or sister). For each item below,
compare yourself to your sibling during the time when you were growing up and living at home
and indicate which response most closely matches your opinion. If you have more than one full
sibling, please compare yourself with the sibling who is closest to you in age.

1 = In general, my mother/father has been much more this way towards my brother/sister than
me.

2 = In general, my mother/father has been somewhat more this way towards my brother/sister
than me.

3 = In general, my mother/father has been the same towards my brother/sister and me.

4 = In general, my mother/father has been somewhat more this way towards me than my
brother/sister.

5 = In general, my mother/father has been much more this way towards me than my
brother/sister.

1) Has been 1 2 3 4 5
strict with
2) Has been 1 2 3 4 5
proud of
3) Has enjoyed 1 2 3 4 5
doing things
together
4) Has been 1 2 3 4 5
sensitive to
thoughts and
feelings
Parental Favoritism 92

5) Has 1 2 3 4 5
punished for
misbehavior
6) Has shown 1 2 3 4 5
interest in
7) Has blamed 1 2 3 4 5
for what
another family
member did
8) Has 1 2 3 4 5
protected
9) Has 1 2 3 4 5
disciplined
10) Has given 1 2 3 4 5
money
11) Has bought 1 2 3 4 5
things for
12) Has given 1 2 3 4 5
us freedom
over actions
13) Has 1 2 3 4 5
enjoyed talking
with
14) Has given 1 2 3 4 5
what was
requested
15) Has done 1 2 3 4 5
favors for
16) Has done 1 2 3 4 5
things with
Parental Favoritism 93

17) Has been 1 2 3 4 5


supportive of
18) Has 1 2 3 4 5
criticized
19) Has 1 2 3 4 5
listened to the
opinions of
20) Has 1 2 3 4 5
respected
Parental Favoritism 94

APPENDIX D

FORCED-CHOICE ITEM AND OBVIOUSNESS ITEM

1. Check the one that is most true for you.

My mother/father has favored me over my sibling.

My mother/father has favored my sibling over me.

My mother/father has not favored my sibling or me.

2. To what extent was your mother’s/father’s favoring of you or your sibling subtle or obvious.

1 = My mother’s/father’s favoritism was very subtle.

7 = My mother’s/father’s favoritism was very obvious.


Parental Favoritism 95

APPENDIX E

RESEMBLANCE SCALE

For the following items record how similar you are with your father/mother when comparing
yourself to your brother/sister. In other words, for the following characteristics who looks more
like your mother/father, you or your sibling.

1 = My sibling is much more similar to my mother/father than I am.

2 = My sibling is somewhat more similar to my mother/father than I am.

3 = My sibling and I are equally similar to my mother/father.

4 = I am somewhat more similar to my mother/father than my sibling.

5 = I am much more similar to my mother/father than my sibling.

1. Whose eyes more 1 2 3 4 5


closely resemble your
mother’s/father’s
eyes?
2. Whose hair color 1 2 3 4 5
and texture more
closely resembles your
mother’s/father’s hair?
3. Whose nose more 1 2 3 4 5
closely resembles your
mother’s/father’s
nose?
4. Whose mouth more 1 2 3 4 5
resembles your
mother’s/father’s
mouth?
5. Overall, whose face
more closely
resembles your
mother’s/father’s face?
Parental Favoritism 96

1= My sibling much more physically resembles my mother/father.

2= My sibling somewhat more physically resembles my mother/father.

3 = My sibling and I physically resemble my mother/father equally.

4 = I physically resemble my mother/father somewhat more than does my sibling.

5 = I physically resemble my mother/father much more than does my sibling.

6. Overall, who more physically 1 2 3 4 5


resembles your mother/father?
Parental Favoritism 97

APPENDIX F

GENERAL HEALTH

This questionnaire is designed to ask you how similar or different you and your sibling (brother
or sister) are in terms of general health. For each item below, compare yourself to your sibling
and indicate which response most closely matches your opinion.

Remember that you are to compare yourself to the same sibling that you used for the previous
questions.

Definitely Somewhat Neither True Somewhat Definitely


False False nor False True True
1. I seem to get 1 2 3 4 5
sick more often
than does my
sibling.
2. I seem to 1 2 3 4 5
suffer from
aches/pain more
often than does
my sibling.
3. I seem to visit 1 2 3 4 5
a doctor or
health center
more often than
does my sibling.
4. I seem to take 1 2 3 4 5
medication for
illnesses more
often than my
sibling.
5. Overall, I 1 2 3 4 5
seem to be
healthier than
my sibling.
Parental Favoritism 98

APPENDIX G

INTELLIGENCE MEASURE

On the following pages, there are phrases that describe people's behavior. Please use the rating
scale below to describe how accurately each statement describes you, in comparison to your
sibling.

Respond to each item as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Try to
respond to the items as honestly as possible, and please read each statement carefully.

Remember that you are to compare yourself to the same sibling that you used for the previous
questions.

Definitely Somewhat Neither True Somewhat Definitely


False False nor False True True
1. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I have
a richer
vocabulary
2. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I have
less difficulty
understanding
abstract ideas.
3. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I can
better remember a
lot of information.
4. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more likely to
have
philosophical
discussions.
5. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more likely to
enjoy reading
challenging
material.
6. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more interested in
Parental Favoritism 99

abstract ideas.
7. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more likely to
solve complex
problems.
8. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more intelligent.
Parental Favoritism 100

APPENDIX H

AMBITION/INDUSTRIOUSNESS MEASURE

On the following pages, there are phrases that describe people's behavior. Please use the rating
scale below to describe how accurately each statement describes you, in comparison to your
sibling.

Respond to each item as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Try to
respond to the items as honestly as possible, and please read each statement carefully.

Remember that you are to compare yourself to the same sibling that you used for the previous
questions.

Definitely Somewhat Neither True Somewhat Definitely


False False nor False True True
1. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I work
harder.
2. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I am more
likely to turn plans
into actions.
3. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I put more
time and effort into
my work.
4. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I do more
than what’s
expected of me.
5. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I set higher
standards for myself
and others.
6. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I am less
likely to do just
enough work to get
by.
7. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I am more
likely to plunge into
tasks with all my
heart.
Parental Favoritism 101

8. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I am more
motivated to
succeed.
9. Compared to my 1 2 3 4 5
sibling, I place
more pressure on
myself to get things
done.
10. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I have a
faster pace to my
life.
11. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more likely to put
work above
pleasure.
12. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I have
less extra time on
my hands.
13. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, work is
a more important
part of my life.
14. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more likely to
complete tasks
successfully.
15. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I have
more things to do.
16. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am
more ambitious.
17. Compared to 1 2 3 4 5
my sibling, I am a
harder work.
Parental Favoritism 102

APPENDIX I

PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS MEASURE

On the following pages, there are phrases that describe people's behavior. Please use the rating
scale below to describe how accurately each statement describes you, in comparison to your
sibling.

Respond to each item as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Try to
respond to the items as honestly as possible, and please read each statement carefully.

Remember that you are to compare yourself to the same sibling that you used for the previous
questions.

Definitely Somewhat Neither True Somewhat Definitely


True True nor False False False
1. I am 1 2 3 4 5
considered more
attractive than
my sibling.
2. I like my 1 2 3 4 5
body more than
does my sibling.
3. I have a more 1 2 3 4 5
pleasing body
type than my
sibling.
4. I like to show 1 2 3 4 5
off my body
more than does
my sibling.
5. I like to look 1 2 3 4 5
at myself in the
mirror more
than does my
sibling.
6. I am 1 2 3 4 5
physically more
attractive than
my sibling.
Parental Favoritism 103

APPENDIX J

DEMOGRAPHICS FORM

1. Please enter your current age.

2. I am a ______ male ______female

3. Please enter your race.

4. Please indicate your height in feet and inches.

5. Please write down your weight in pounds.

6. My current GPA or best estimate is:

If you have no GPA, please record your high school GPA.

7. My family’s yearly household income is:

$0 – 20,000
$20,000 – 40,000
$40,000 – 60,000
$60,000 – 80,000
$80,000 – 100,000
over $100,000
I don’t know
Parental Favoritism 104

APPENDIX K

POST-EXPERIMENTAL QUESTIONNAIRE

On the following lines, please describe in your own words, what you believe to have been
the purpose(s) of this study.

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

Please describe any additional comments regarding anything that was asked of you during your
participation in this study. Any feedback you provide may be very helpful in terms of how we
design future experiments.
__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________
Parental Favoritism 105

APPENDIX L

DEBRIEFING FORM

Department of Psychology
Bowling Green, OH 43403-0228
Bowling Green State University Phone: (419) 372-2301
FAX: (419) 372-6013
Web Page:http//www.bgsu.edu/departments/psych/

THOUGHTS ON MYSELF AND MY FAMILY Fall 2008

Thank you for participating in this study!

Research suggests that parental favoritism is a very common phenomenon. This study was
designed to examine whether certain characteristics (e.g., ambition) influence the likelihood of
being favored by a parent(s). This research is important in that the results may lead to further
understanding of parental favoritism.

All participants completed the same questionnaires as you did. The numerous questionnaires
assessed your beliefs about your intelligence, ambition, industriousness, health, physical
attractiveness, and parental relations.

The theory behind this study is based on evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary psychologists
posit that certain contexts, such as genetic relatedness between parent and child and specific
characteristics related to fitness that are possessed by children may influence parenting
behaviors. Due to natural selection pressures, we believed that parents would be more likely to
favor children who possessed certain characteristics (e.g., ambition) that are likely to maximize
their ability to survive, thrive, and be reproductively successful (pass their genes on to future
generations).

• If you are interested in learning more about this topic, you may wish to read:

Geary, D. & Flinn, M. (2001). Evolution of parental behavior and the human family.
Parenting: Science and Practice 1, 5 – 61.

• If you are interested in learning the results of this study when they are available or have
any questions about this research, please contact:

Anthony Lauricella
alauric@bgnet.bgsu.edu
(419) 308-5578
Parental Favoritism 106

Sometimes reflecting on one’s personal characteristics or relationship with family triggers


sensitive emotional material. If upon completing this survey, you find yourself experiencing
some form of emotional distress, a number of existing psychological service agencies are
available to assist you in further coping/exploring these issues. Some recommended agencies
include:

1) The Bowling Green State University Counseling Center


Location: 422 Saddlemire at North Conklin
Phone: 419 372 2081
Walk in hours are Monday – Thursday from 2:00 – 3:30 pm.
Or you can schedule an appointment
Regular hours: Mondays 8:00 am to 7:00 pm
Tuesday – Friday 8:00 am to 5:00 pm
Services are free for students

2)Psychological Services Center


Location: Psychology Building, Suite 300
Phone: 419 372 2540
Must call to schedule an appointment
Office hours are Monday, Thursday, and Friday 8:00 am to 5:00 pm
Tuesday and Wednesdays 8:00 am to 8:00 pm
Services are free for students

3)Psychological Resources Ltd.


Bowling Green Location: 970 W. Wooster St., Suite 124 Located in the Medical Building by
the Wood County Hospital
Phone: 419 352 6666

Toledo Location: 4841 Monroe Street, Suite 100


Phone: 419 475 0881

Must call to schedule an appointment.


Insurance may cover part of services
Parental Favoritism 107

APPENDIX M

EXPERIMETRIX ANNOUNCEMENT

MUST BE 18 OR OLDER, HAVE TWO CURRENTLY MARRIED BIOLOGICAL PARENTS,


AND HAVE AT LEAST ONE FULLY BIOLOGICAL SIBLING (BROTHER OR SISTER) TO
PARTICIPATE.

This study examines characteristics about yourself and your family. If you decide to participate,
you will be asked to complete nine short questionnaires. I anticipate that it will take
approximately 30 - 45 minutes for most people to complete this study. Please note that your
answers will be anonymous. You will receive 1.0 research credits for this study.
Parental Favoritism 108

APPENDIX N

EXPLORATORY ANALYSES

Maternal versus Paternal Favoritism

To further examine for differences between maternal favoritism and paternal favoritism, I

performed exploratory analyses (paired samples t-tests) by examining individual items on the

Original-coded maternal and Original-coded paternal SIDE composite indices. Similar to my

earlier analyses performed on Re-coded SIDE composite indices, results identified specific areas

wherein one parent appears to manifest favoritism more than the other parent (See Table 15).

Exploratory analyses indicated that mothers displayed greater favoritism than fathers in the areas

of (a) criticizing, (b) punishing, (c) enjoy talking with, (d) being strict with, (e) blaming for what

another family member did, and (f) doing things with, all p’s < .05. Fathers displayed greater

favoritism than mothers in the areas of (a) protecting and (b) giving money to. Thus, exploratory

analyses further demonstrated that mothers and fathers appear to manifest favoritism in unique

areas.

Sociocompetitive characteristics.

As noted earlier, I expected sex differences between children to exist regarding parental

favoritism and the possession of intelligence, ambition/industriousness, and physical

attractiveness. To more fully explore these hypotheses, I performed partial correlations,

controlling for sibling hierarchy, between parental SIDE composite indices and each construct

(relative intelligence, relative ambition/industriousness, relative physical attractiveness) between

the two same-sex sibling dyads (brothers and sisters). I believe such analyses allow for a “purer”

examination of the association of each construct with parental favoritism because comparison of
Parental Favoritism 109

same-sex dyads allows for a “tighter” comparison than is true regarding comparison of mixed-

sibling dyads since both participant and sibling sex are controlled in same-sex sibling dyads.

Regarding relative intelligence and in the case of brothers (male participant-male sibling),

there was no significant correlation between relative intelligence and either maternal or paternal

favoritism after running a partial correlation controlling for sibling hierarchy, all p’s > .05.

However, mothers show favoritism towards the more intelligent son in the areas of (a) pride and

(b) listening to the opinion of, all p’s < .05. Fathers did not show favoritism towards the more

intelligent son in any area, all p’s > .05.

In the case of sisters, however, both mothers and fathers show favoritism towards the

more intelligent daughter, all p’s < .05. Specifically, mothers show favoritism in the areas of (a)

pride, (b) freedom over one’s actions, (c) enjoy talking with, (d) granting requests, (e) doing

favors for, (f) doing things with, (g) being supportive, (h) listening to opinions, and (i)

respecting. Fathers show favoritism in the areas of (a) pride, (b) freedom over one’s actions, (c)

enjoy talking with, (d) granting requests, (e) listening to opinions, and (f) respecting. Thus, it

would appear that both mothers and fathers favor the more intelligent daughter more so than the

more intelligent son.

In the case of brothers regarding relative ambition/industriousness, there was no

significant correlation between relative ambition/industriousness and either maternal or paternal

favoritism, all p’s > .05. However, mothers show favoritism towards the more

ambitious/industrious son in the areas of (a) pride and (b) listening to opinions, all p’s < .05.

Additionally, mothers show favoritism towards the less ambitious/industrious son in the areas of

(a) buying things for and (b) sensitivity towards thoughts and feelings, all p’s < .05. Fathers did

not show favoritism towards the more ambitious/industrious son in any area, all p’s > .05.
Parental Favoritism 110

In the case of sisters, however, both mothers and fathers show favoritism towards the

more ambitious/industrious daughter, all p’s < .05. Specifically, mothers show favoritism in the

areas of (a) discipline, (b) pride, (c) enjoying doing things with, (d) showing interest in, (e)

freedom over one’s actions, (f) enjoy talking with, (g) granting requests, (h) doing favors for, (i)

doing things with, (j) support, (k) respect, and (l) listening to opinions, all p’s < .05. Fathers

show favoritism towards the more ambitious/industrious daughter in the areas of (a) pride, (b)

freedom over one’s actions, (c) enjoy talking with, (d) support, (e) listening to opinions, (f)

respecting, all p’s < .05. Thus, it would appear that both mothers and fathers favor the more

ambitious/industrious daughter more so than the more ambitious/industrious son.

Lastly, neither mothers nor fathers show favoritism towards the more attractive daughter,

in the case of sisters, all p’s > .05. However, mothers showed favoritism towards the more

attractive daughter in the areas of (a) giving money to, (b) freedom over one’s actions, and (c)

listening to opinions, all p’s < .05. Fathers did not favor the more attractive daughter in any area,

all p’s > .05.

In the case of brothers, neither mothers nor fathers show favoritism towards the more

attractive son, p’s > .05. However, mothers show favoritism towards the more attractive son in

the areas of (a) protection and (b) listening to opinions. Fathers show favoritism towards the less

attractive son in the area of respect, p’s = .03.

Summary

These findings corroborate, as well as deepen the original findings. It appears that

mothers and fathers manifest favoritism towards children in circumscribed areas. Additionally,

intelligence and ambition/industriousness appear to be more related to favoritism towards

daughters than sons, especially in the case of same-sex sibling dyads (brothers and sisters).
Parental Favoritism 111

Table 1.

Means and Standard Deviations on Outcome Measures as a Function of Participant Sex

Males Females Total


(n = 145) (n = 280) (n = 425)
Forced-Choice Paternal Favoritism Item

Self favored 23% 25% 24%

Sibling Favored 20% 16% 18%

No favoritism 57% 59% 58%

Forced-Choice Maternal Favoritism Item

Self favored 26% 18% 21%

Sibling Favored 14% 19% 17%

No favoritism 60% 63% 62%

Maternal Favoritism Obviousness Item 3.41 (1.31) 3.49 (1.66) 3.46 (1.54)

Paternal Favoritism Obviousness Item 3.38 (1.77) 3.65 (.92) 3.56 (1.75)

Note. Obviousness ranges from 1 (very subtle) to 7 (very obvious).


Parental Favoritism 112

Table 2.

Means and Standard Deviations of Physical Health Composite Index as a Function of

Participant Sex and Sibling Sex

_________________________________________________________________
Sex of Participant

Male Female Row Means


____________________________________________________________________
Sex of Sibling

Male 3.19 (.84) 2.86 (.83) 2.97 (.84)**

Female 3.62 (.86) 3.01 (.87) 3.21 (.91)**

Column Means 3.36 (.87)** 2.93 (.85)**

______________________________________________________________________
Note. ** p < .01.
Parental Favoritism 113

Table 3.

Means and Standard Deviations of Intelligence Composite Index as a Function of Participant

Sex and Sibling Sex

_________________________________________________________________
Sex of Participant

Male Female Row Means


____________________________________________________________________
Sex of Sibling

Male 3.60 (.84) 3.42 (.81) 3.48 (.82)

Female 3.53 (.79) 3.21 (.86) 3.30 (.85)

Column Means 3.57 (.82)** 3.32 (.84)**

______________________________________________________________________
Note. ** p < .01.
Parental Favoritism 114

Table 4.

Means and Standard Deviations on Dependent Measures as a Function of Sibling Hierarchy

Participant is Older Sibling Participant is Younger Sibling


(df = 204 – 207) (df = 190 – 196)

Maternal Favoritism 2.97 (.36) 3.04 (.38)


Paternal Favoritism 3.01 (.40) 3.09 (.43)
Health 3.13 (.86) 3.02 (.92)
Intelligence 3.61 (.78)** 3.19 (.86)**
Ambition/Industriousness 3.55 (.59)** 3.23 (.73)**
Physical Attractiveness 2.91 (.81)** 3.21 (.87)**
Maternal Resemblance 2.99 (.87) 3.02 (.89)
Paternal Resemblance 3.11 (.91) 3.05 (.94)
Note. ** p < .01.
Parental Favoritism 115

Table 5.
Means and Standard Deviations of Re-coded SIDEs as a Function of Parental Sex

Maternal Favoritism Paternal Favoritism P

Has punished for 1.76 (.69) 1.60 (.70) .00**


misbehavior
Has shown interest in 1.24 (.51) 1.39 (.61) .00**
Has done things with 1.38 (.57) 1.51 (.67) .00**
Has protected 1.34 (.59) 1.42 (.65) .02*
Has bought things for 1.44 (.59) 1.38 (.62) .07
Has given freedom 1.65 (.66) 1.59 (.65) .08
over actions
Has done favors for 1.24 (.49) 1.29 (.54) .09
Has respected 1.15 (.40) 1.18 (.46) .14
Has enjoyed doing 1.53 (.61) 1.58 (.68) .15
things together
Has blamed for what 1.39 (.60) 1.35 (.57) .19
another family
member did
Has been supportive 1.19 (.45) 1.21 (.48) .31
of
Has given what was 1.27 (.52) 1.30 (.54) .35
requested
Has disciplined 1.49 (.61) 1.52 (.66) .52
Has given money 1.55 (.66) 1.53 (.69) .58
Has been strict with 1.76 (.69) 1.78 (.73) .69
Has listened to the 1.24 (.47) 1.25 (.52) .74
opinions of
Has enjoyed talking 1.48 (.63) 1.47 (.64) .76
with
Has criticized 1.41 (.59) 1.42 (.62) .89
Has been proud of 1.42 (.60) 1.47 (.63) .167
Has been sensitive to 1.41 (.59) 1.41 (.58) 1.000
thoughts and feelings
Overall 1.42 (.32) 1.43 (.36) .33
Note. Items in italics were reverse-coded so that higher score always indicated favoritism. Scores
ranged from 1 = no favoritism to 3 = high favoritism. * p < .05, ** p < .01.
Parental Favoritism 116

Table 6.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Physical Resemblance and Original SIDE Items

Maternal Resemblance Paternal Resemblance

Has been strict with .03 -.03


Has been proud of .10* .14**
Has enjoyed doing things .18** .13*
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.02 .02
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior .03 -.01
Has shown interest in .01 .05
Has blamed for what another .03 -.01
family member did
Has protected .05 .02
Has disciplined -.03 -.03
Has given money -.02 .07
Has bought things for .03 .04
Has given freedom over actions -.04 .12*
Has enjoyed talking with .09 .04
Has given what was requested -.03 .15**
Has done favors for .03 .17*
Has done things with .10* .09
Has been supportive of .09 .12*
Has criticized .03 .00
Has listened to the opinions of .02 .13**
Has respected -.09 .22*
Overall Favoritism .06 .11*
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 117

Table 7.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Physical Health and Original Maternal and Paternal

SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with .00 -.04


Has been proud of .02 .15**
Has enjoyed doing things -.11* .03
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.16** -.09
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior .00 -.07
Has shown interest in .00 .08
Has blamed for what another -.02 .01
family member did
Has protected -.05 .02
Has disciplined -.01 -.11*
Has given money -.11* .00
Has bought things for -.11* .03
Has given freedom over actions -.01 .00
Has enjoyed talking with -.05 .09
Has given what was requested -.07 .01
Has done favors for -.06 .02
Has done things with -.10* .10*
Has been supportive of .02 .09
Has criticized .08 .06
Has listened to the opinions of .04 .10*
Has respected .05 .11*
Overall Favoritism -.07 .04
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 118

Table 8.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Intelligence and Original Maternal

and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with .00 .02


Has been proud of .21* .09
Has enjoyed doing things .14 -.05
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.12 -.20*
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior .00 -.12
Has shown interest in -.04 -.08
Has blamed for what another -.05 -.09
family member did
Has protected .04 -.09
Has disciplined .00 -.12
Has given money .05 .02
Has bought things for -.08 -.05
Has given freedom over actions .14 .09
Has enjoyed talking with .19* .10
Has given what was requested .05 -.01
Has done favors for .04 -.05
Has done things with .03 -.02
Has been supportive of -.02 -.01
Has criticized .12 -.07
Has listened to the opinions of .38** .15
Has respected .13 .11
Overall Favoritism .12 -.04
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 119

Table 9.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Intelligence and Original

Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with -.08 .00


Has been proud of .27** .23**
Has enjoyed doing things .02 -.02
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts .03 .04
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior -.08 .06
Has shown interest in .10 -.02
Has blamed for what another .04 -.03
family member did
Has protected .07 .03
Has disciplined .05 .07
Has given money .06 .06
Has bought things for .04 .00
Has given freedom over actions .09 .10
Has enjoyed talking with .15* .09
Has given what was requested .17** .08
Has done favors for .17** .03
Has done things with .13* .00
Has been supportive of .18** .15*
Has criticized .02 .02
Has listened to the opinions of .18** .20**
Has respected .16* .12
Overall Favoritism .13* .10
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 120

Table 10.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Ambition/Industriousness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with -.02 .07


Has been proud of .31** .20*
Has enjoyed doing things -.02 .02
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.18* .01
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior -.02 .01
Has shown interest in -.03 .06
Has blamed for what another -.06 -.03
family member did
Has protected .11 .00
Has disciplined -.06 -.07
Has given money -.06 -.04
Has bought things for -.08 -.02
Has given freedom over actions -.03 .11
Has enjoyed talking with .09 .17*
Has given what was requested -.14 .02
Has done favors for -.10 .04
Has done things with .02 .13
Has been supportive of .03 .15
Has criticized .08 .08
Has listened to the opinions of .27** .16
Has respected .23** .22*
Overall Favoritism .03 .12
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 121

Table 11.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Ambition/Industriousness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with -.10 .01


Has been proud of .28** .24**
Has enjoyed doing things .15* .02
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts .06 .01
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior -.10 .05
Has shown interest in .15* -.03
Has blamed for what another -.06 .02
family member did
Has protected .03 .10
Has disciplined .06 .05
Has given money -.07 -.01
Has bought things for -.01 -.02
Has given freedom over actions .04 .03
Has enjoyed talking with .17** .05
Has given what was requested .07 .02
Has done favors for .15* .04
Has done things with .18** -.06
Has been supportive of .17** .14*
Has criticized .08 .06
Has listened to the opinions of .12* .21**
Has respected .21** .14*
Overall Favoritism .11 .08
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 122

Table 12.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Female Participants’ Physical Attractiveness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with .04 -.02


Has been proud of -.01 .06
Has enjoyed doing things -.06 .12*
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.08 -.06
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior .04 .02
Has shown interest in -.10 .01
Has blamed for what another -.01 .01
family member did
Has protected .02 .02
Has disciplined .02 .05
Has given money .14* .18**
Has bought things for .18** .14*
Has given freedom over actions .03 -.01
Has enjoyed talking with -.04 .00
Has given what was requested .02 .02
Has done favors for -.01 .07
Has done things with .01 .01
Has been supportive of .00 .02
Has criticized .01 .11
Has listened to the opinions of .05 .06
Has respected .00 .03
Overall Favoritism .03 .07
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 123

Table 13.

Pearson’s Correlational Values Between Male Participants’ Physical Attractiveness and

Original Maternal and Paternal SIDE Items

Maternal Paternal

Has been strict with -.23** .03


Has been proud of .03 -.08
Has enjoyed doing things .07 .10
together
Has been sensitive to thoughts -.02 .04
and feelings
Has punished for misbehavior -.23** -.01
Has shown interest in -.05 .12
Has blamed for what another .04 .12
family member did
Has protected .16 .04
Has disciplined -.21* -.06
Has given money .00 .09
Has bought things for .04 .13
Has given freedom over actions -.16 .09
Has enjoyed talking with .08 .12
Has given what was requested -.10 .07
Has done favors for -.02 -.14
Has done things with .04 .03
Has been supportive of .04 .02
Has criticized .04 .09
Has listened to the opinions of .13 -.16
Has respected -.03 -.26**
Overall Favoritism -.06 .06
*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 124

Table 14.

Bivariate Correlations Between Outcome and Predictor Variables across all Participants

Maternal Paternal Physical Intelligence Ambition Physical


Favoritism Favoritism Health Attractiveness

Maternal -
Favoritism

Paternal .22** -
Favoritism

Physical -.06 .03 -


Health

Intelligence .11* .03 .12* -

Ambition .05 .06 .06 .49** -

Physical .02 .08 .97 .00 .03 -


Attractiveness

Maternal .06 -.02 -.07 -.06 -.09 .02


Resemblance

Paternal .04 .11* .12* .09 .05 -.06


Resemblance

*Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)


**Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
Parental Favoritism 125

Table 15.

Means and Standard Deviations of Original SIDE Items as a Function of Parental Sex

Maternal Favoritism Paternal Favoritism P

Has criticized 2.86 (.71) 3.01 (.75) .00**


Has punished for 2.81 (1.01) 2.97 (.92) .01*
misbehavior
Has protected 2.98 (.68) 3.11 (.76) .01*
Has been strict with 2.81 (1.01) 2.96 (1.07) .02*
Has enjoyed doing 3.21 (.78) 3.07 (.89) .02*
things together
Has blamed for what 2.86 (.70) 2.95 (.67) .02*
another family
member did
Has given money 3.05 (.85) 3.15 (.85) .02*
Has enjoyed talking 3.25 (.75) 3.12 (.78) .02*
with
Has disciplined 2.91 (.78) 2.99 (.84) .06
Has done things with 3.10 (.68) 3.00 (.85) .07
Has been sensitive to 2.97 (.72) 3.04 (.71) .08
thoughts and feelings
Has done favors for 2.99 (.54) 3.04 (.61) .18
Has given what was 2.97 (.58) 3.02 (.62) .23
requested
Has given freedom 2.99 (.92) 3.04 (.88) .26
over actions
Has been supportive 3.03 (.49) 3.00 (.52) .38
of
Has bought things for 3.09 (.73) 3.12 (.71) .53
Has shown interest in 2.98 (.57) 2.97 (.73) .74
Has been proud of 3.12 (.72) 3.11 (.77) .79
Has respected 3.03 (.42) 3.04 (.50) .93
Has listened to the 3.07 (.53) 3.07 (.57) 1.00
opinions of
Items in italics were reverse-coded so that higher score always indicated favoritism. Scores
ranged from 1 = sibling strongly favored to 5 = participant strongly favored.
* p < .05
** p < .01

You might also like