Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Motor Learning and

T
wo propositions are central to theories of occupa-
tional therapy: (a) the client must be an active

Occupational Therapy: participant in the therapeutic process, and (b) in-


tervention should consist of purposeful activities in a
structured environment (Ayres, 1972; Breines, 1986; Co-
The Organization of hen, 1989; King, 1978; Mosey, 1981; Trombly, 1989). Both
of these propositions focus on the interaction of the cli-
Practice ent with the surrounding environment, an approach that
is relevant to occupational therapy practice (Gliner,
1985). The therapist's skillful structuring of the client-
Tal Jarus environment interaction facilitates development of new
skills (Ayres, 1972; Clark, 1979; Fidler & Fidler, 1978;
Gilfoyle, Grady, & Moore, 1981; Llorens, 1986; Mosey,
Key Words: memory. motor control 1981; Trombly, 1989). Adaptation of the environment and
task presentation should produce desired responses in
the client (Trombly, 1989).
There are common threads between occupational
This article addresses implications for the practice of therapy and the field of motor learning and control (Bur-
occupational therapy when that therapy is guided by gess, 1989; Gliner, 1985; Poole, 1991a; Schmidt, 1991).
theories of motor learning. In occupational therapy, Motor learning refers to a set of internal processes that
clients must learn or relearn motor skills through the are associated with practice or experience and that lead
use of activities. The occupational therapist must pre- to relatively permanent (i.e., long-lasting) changes in
sent activities in a manner that elicits the retention motor behavior (Schmidt, 1988). Because permanent
and transfer of the desired skills for use in functional changes are the desired outcome, the ability to retain the
settings. Therefore, the therapist should strive for ac-
learned task is of great importance. Practice conditions
quisition conditions that facilitate retention and
transfer of the learned skills. The processes that under- affect the transfer of training (Salmoni, Schmidt, & Wal-
lie motor learning should guide therapy. Three major ter, 1984; Sage, 1984; Schmidt, 1988). Transfer enables a
factors that affect motor learning are environmental client to draw on past experience to perform a new task
conditions, cognitive processes, and movement organ- (Schmidt, 1988). For example, a client who learns to put
ization. Examination of the clinical implications of on a hospital shirt should be able to transfer this learning
these factors, however, results in contradictmy predic- and put on different shirts after being discharged. The
tions regarding optimal practice conditions for motor acquisition phase may indicate performance, but the re-
skills. This article explores the successful integration of tention and transfer phases may indicate learning (Magill,
these factors and its application in occupational ther- 1989; Sage, 1984; Schmidt, 1988, 1991).
apy practice. It is suggested that increasing the diffi- Clients in rehabilitation must relearn voluntary con-
culty of the learning context during practice is benefi-
trol over injured muscles or learn new motor skills
cial for retention.
through the use of functional activities (Gliner & Davis,
1984; Katz, 1985; Mosey, 1981; Mulder & Hulstyn, 1984;
Trombly, 1989). Therefore, the occupational therapist
must structure functional activities to enable the client to
learn and retain the desired skill. Principles of motor
learning can assist the therapist in structuring these ac-
tivities. For occupational therapy, in which the client
must learn or relearn motor skills and perform them effi-
Ciently later in different contexts, the therapist should
strive for acquisition conditions that produce optimal re-
tention and transfer of the learned skills. For example,
during practice of a motor task with a client, giving limit-
ed feedback will facilitate maximum retention, even
though feedback would enhance performance in this
Tal Jarus, PhD. OTR, is Assistant Professor, Sackler Faculty of
Medicine, Occupational Therapy Department, Tel Aviv Univer-
practice phase (Schmidt, 1988). Clients will achieve maxi-
sity, POB 39040, Ramat Aviv, 69978, Tel Aviv, Israel. mal progress when therapy is guided by principles of
motor learning. This article discusses how a therapist
This article was accepted for publication March 28, 1994.
should structure the activities in a treatment session.

810 September 1994, votume 48, Number 9

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Factors That Influence Motor Learning Studies investigating the effects of practice variability
Movement Organization (e,g., Kerr, 1982; Magill & Reeve, 1978; McCracken &
Stelmach, 1977; Newell & Shapiro, 1976; Wrisberg &
In the past, motor learning theories focused on descrip- Ragsdale, 1979) compared constant practice of a single
tions of movement organization, stressing the coordina- task to variable practice in either a predictable or unpre-
tion of the neuromuscular systems that control move- dictable presentation order. Studies in which variable
ments (Adams, 1971, 1977; Arbib, 1985; Schmidt, 1975). practice tasks were presented in a predictable order
From this perspective evolved the theory that movement found limited support for the theory that variability en-
patterns are stored in memory as a schema or an abstrac- hance.'; retention and transfer (Kerr, 1982; Magill & Reeve,
tion of the general characteristics of a movement 1978; Newell & Shapiro, 1976), Studies in which variable
(Schmidt, 1975). However, Schmidt (1988) referred to practice was presented in an unpredictable order
the schema as a generalized motor program that serves as (McCracken & Stelmach, 1977; Wrisberg & Ragsdale,
the memory representation for a class of movements, 1979), however, did support the theory,
rather than for anyone action or movement.
A movement class is defined by the invariant charac-
Environmental Factors
teristics of actions. Invariant characteristics are "aspects
of movements that appear to be fL,ed even though other, Environmental factors influence motor learning, The
more superficial features can change" (Schmidt, 1988, therapist must consider the nature of the environment
p. 266), When a group of actions have common invariant because different environmental factors elicit different
characteristics, they are considered to be in the same motor reactions (Gentile, 1972, 1987). To be successful,
movement class and are therefore represented in and movements should correspond with certain factors of the
controlled by the same generalized motor program. For environment. For example, the movement of picking up a
example, signing one's name is a generalized motor pro- cup to drink must be adapted to the environmental fac-
gram, Whether one signs on paper or on a blackboard, tors of the shape of the cu p, its distance from the person,
using hands or feet, the gUiding rules in this action are and the volume of liquid. The object, in this case the cup,
identical. The pattern of the signature or the shape of the determines the spatial arrangement of the movement, In
letters, which will be similar in any of these actions, are some cases, timing of the movement is im portant, such as
invariant, yet the size of the letters and the specific mus- in the act of catching a ball. The goal of the action deter-
cles used vary, mines the environmental factors (regulatory conditions)
Several different movement characteristics have that are critical for performance (Gentile, 1972, 1987)
been proposed to be the invariant characteristics of a Motor skills may be classified according to their spa-
motor program, such as the relative timing of the compo- tial and temporal environmental factors (Gentile, 1972,
nents of the action, the relative force produced by the 1987), When temporal environmental factors are station-
components of the action, the sequence of events in- ary, only the spatial factor of the movement is controlled
volved in the action (Schmidt, 1988), and spatial configu- by the environment, For example, the spatial factors of
rations (Bernstein, 1967), Currently, however, there is no picking up a newspaper are different for lifting a Sunday
general consensus concerning the characteristics of mo- newspaper than for lifting a weekday newspaper. Timing
tor programs (e.g., Gentner, 1987, 1988; Heuer, 1988; is not specified -lifting the paper is a self-paced action,
Heuer & Schmidt, 1988). Furthermore, the generalized Tasks in which temporal environmental factors remain
motor program theory has been criticized for its failure to stationary and fixed from trial to trial are termed closed
prove the existence of additional invariant features tasks Tasks in which the temporal factors of the environ-
(Horak, 1991; Poole, 1991b). ment are stationary but the spatial factors of the task,
From Schmidt'S (1975; 1988) viewpoint, the strength such as the size or location of objects, vary from trial to
of a generalized motor program is directly related to the trial, are called variable motionless tasks, These include
variability in practice of different tasks that belong to the walking on different surfaces, picking up a cup from dif-
program's movement class. For example, practicing ferent locations on a table, and buttoning a shirt with
transfer to different types of chairs allows the client to different types of buttons.
practice the motor program of transferring to a chair, When environmental factors include objects or per-
whereas the timing of the action, the force produced by sons that ar":: moving, both spatial and temporal factors of
the muscles during the motion, and the size of the motion the movement are determined by the environment. For
(the spatial configurations) differ according to the width, example, when catching a ball or stepping onto an escala-
height, and shape of the chairs. Hence, Schmidt predict- tor, a person needs to predict where the object will be.
ed that variability in practice is beneficial for retention Tasks in which these environmental factors change from
and transfer of a motor skill. Catching halls of varying size trial to trial are termed open tasks For some tasks, the
or grasping objects of different shapes are two other ex- pace of the movement is constant while the environment
amples of variable practice, is moving, These tasks, termed consistent motion tasks~

The American ]oumat o[ Occupationat Therapy 811

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


are associated with mechanical devices such as escalators client to forget certain aspects of the way he or she per-
and assembly line belts. formed the tasks before. The time lapse between the
Skill acquisition, according to Gentile (1972, 1987), presentation of different tasks (e.g., different chairs)
involves two stages: (a) trial and error, in which the move- causes the client to forget the way he or she performed
ment p~tterns ~re not consistent; and (b) task dependent, the movements before, forcing the client to reconstruct
in which the four types of tasks are learned differently and movements for each subsequent attempt. As a result,
therefore should be taught differently. As practice pro- further practice is imposed on the client, which facilitates
gresses in closed and consistent motion tasks, movement retention. Several studies supported this reconstruction
patterns are refined and retained. The movement pattern view (Lee & Magill, 1983a, 1985; Magill, Meeuwson, Lee, &
should become consistent because the environmental Mathews, 1992; Shea & Wright, 1991). The findings of Del
conditions are static (Higgins & Spaeth, 1972; Hobart, Rey, Wughalter, and Carnes (1987) did not suPPOrt Lee
Vorro, & Dotson, 1978). Therefore, Gentile (1972, 1987) and Magill's (1983b, 1985) explanations, because the in-
recommended using a predictable environment to teach terfering activity they used to facilitate this type of forget-
persons to develop a consistent motOr pattern in re- ting did not influence retention or acquisition.
sponse to stationary input. The easy learning context, termed one with low con-
As practice progresses in the open and variable mo- textual interference, such as a predictable situation, does
tionless tasks, movement patterns should become diver- not force the client to use elaborate encoding processes,
sified. The client must respond to the varied environmen- leaVing weaker memory representations of the move-
tal stimuli (Higgins & Spaeth, 1972; Hobart et aI., 1978). ments practiced (Battig, 1979; Shea & Zimny, 1983; 1988).
Therefore, Gentile (1972, 1987) recommended teaching As a result, although low contextual interference may
open and variable motionless tasks in an unpredictable, speed the process of acquiring a skill, it does not help
changing environment to develop diversification of the retention to the same extent as high contextual interfer-
motOr pattern. ence. Low contextual interference can be created by pre-
senting tasks in a blocked order, so that practice of the
first task must be completed before the client advances to
Cognitive Processes
the second task, and so on (i.e., AAABBBCCC). For exam-
Motor learning theories have been criticized for their ple, in the activity of transferring to a chair, the client
tendency to focus on internal neuromuscular movement would practice transferring to a specific chair repeatedly
organization while underestimating the importance of until that task was mastered. Only then would the client
cognitive processes in retention and transfer of motor practice transferring to another type of chair, and so on.
behavior (Battig, 1979; Lee & Magill, 1983b; Salmoni et aI., The feedback given to the client about the conse-
1984). Learning theory suggests that skill acquisition in quences of a movement, such as the extent to which the
unpredictable environmental conditions is beneficial for intended goal was achieved, can be referred to as knowl-
memory and transfer (Battig, 1979; Craik & Lockhart, edge a/results (Sage, 1984; Schmidt, 1988). Knowledge of
1972; Shea & Zimny, 1983; Tulving, 1979). Battig (1979) results can be telling the client "You burned the egg,"
claimed that presenting several motor tasks tOgether in "You put the peg in the right hole," "You missed the
certain contexts can facilitate cognitive processes benefi- target by 2 inches," or "You buttoned your shirt wrong."
cial to memory improvement. He termed the difficult Jarus (1988) and Lee and Magill (1983b) suggested that
learning context, such as an unpredictable situation, a the effect of the level of knowledge of results during the
context with high contextual interference. Performing a acquisition phase may be similar to the effect of high
motor task in a difficult learning context forces the client contextual interference. Several studies have found that
to use multiple and variable processes to overcome the clients given constant knowledge of results in the acquisi-
difficulty of practice (Battig, 1979; Shea & Zimny, 1983; tion phase do not do as well in the retention phase, when
1988). Despite its sloWing of the acquisition phase, per- knowledge of results is eliminated, as do clients given less
forming in a difficult context leads to the development of frequent knowledge of results in the acquisition phase
more elaborate and distinctive memory representations (Baird & Hughes, 1972; Ho & Shea, 1978; Jarus, in press;
of the movements practiced, which is beneficial for reten- Schmidt, Young, Swinnen, & Shapiro, 1989; Winstein &
tion (Battig, 1979; Shea & Zimny, 1983; 1988). The teach- Schmidt, 1990; Wulf & Schmidt, 1989). Schmidt (1988)
er can create high contextual interference by presenting stated that knowledge of results can be viewed as a crutch
trials of several tasks in a random order. That is, if each that guides performance. Under conditions with limited
task is assigned a different letter, the order of tasks might knowledge of results, the client is forced to rely on other
be ABACBACABBC. For example, random practice with relevant cues of the task, such as sensory feedback, to
transferring to a chair would involve introducing a differ- improve performance (Salmoni et aI., 1984; Schmidt,
ent chair to the client in different trials. 1988). The client can subsequently perform the acquired
Lee and Magill (1985) suggested that the presenta- task without knowledge of results.
tion of motor tasks in an unpredictable context causes the This reasoning is consistent with the contextual vari-

812 September 1994, Volume 48, Number 9


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
ety hypothesis (Battig & Shea, 1978). That is, predictable ingly, when the skill variations to be learned require
practice in a blocked order, or with constant knowledge different motor programs, manipulating the practice
of results, is beneficial for acquisition, yet detrimental to schedule creates different levels of contextual interfer-
retention. Unpredictable practice in a random order, or ence, which results in varying retention and transfer ef-
with lower knowledge of results frequency, is detrimental fects (i.e., the higher the level of interference, the better
to acquisition, but beneficial for retention and transfer. the effect). Similarly, when the skill variations involve
Thus, the level of contextual interference and the level of parameter modifications of the same motor program, the
knowledge of results during acquisition of motor skills contextual interference effect wiJl not be found. For ex-
influences the abilities to retain the learned material and ample, if a person practices throwing a baJ] at a target and
to perform a transfer task (Battig, 1979; Magill & HaJ], uses three different thrOWing patterns, such as overhand,
1990). underhand, and sidearm, then three different motor pro-
grams are involved and the contextual interference effect
would be found (MagiJ] & Hall, 1990). However, if only
Discussion
one throWing pattern is used but three different ball
Both Schmidt's (1988) generalized motor program the- speeds are practiced, then only one motor program is
ory and the contextual interference effect theory address involved although the overall limb speed parameter must
the concept that variability in practice is related to im- be modified to produce the different ball speeds. In this
proved retention and transfer. According to Gentile case, the contextual interference effect would not be
(1972, 1987), however, the acquisition of motor skills found (Magill & Hall, 1990). This hypothesis was explored
depends on the type of task being learned. Therefore, it is by Magill and Hall (1990) and requires further research.
relevant to compare the concepts of variability in practice,
practice schedule, and task classification, and their re-
Practice Schedule and Task Classification
spective implications for clinical practice.
The effects of blocked versus random practice have been
studied considerably in the past decade (Magill & Hall,
Variability in Practice and Practice Schedule
1990). However, researchers who tested the premise of
Schmidt (1975, 1988) made no predictions about the contextual interference theory in motor learning (Del
practice schedules that can be developed for variability in Rey, 1982; Goode & Magill, 1986; Lee & Magill, 1983b;
practice. In the traditional approach to testing the prac- Young, Cohen, & Husak, 1993) used either a closed task
tice variability hypothesis, one group (the constant or an open task exclusively. None of these studies investi-
group) learned only one task, whereas the second group gated whether the effect of contextual interference differs
(the variable group) learned several tasks. Both groups between open and closed tasks. Studies in which the
had the same total number of trials. The number of tasks subjects learned a closed task (Shea & Morgan, 1979;
practiced by subjects differed (i.e., the amount ofvariabil- Young et aI., 1993) supported the contextual interference
ity differed), and little or no attention was given to the theory. Subjects in the group that practiced variations of a
order of the tasks (i.e., schedule of practice). In the con- closed task in random order retained the task better than
textual interference approach, however, the number of subjects in the group that practiced variations of a closed
tasks was held constant for all clients (i.e., the amount of task in blocked order. Studies in which the subjects
variability was constant) but the order in which these learned an open task (Del Rey, 1982; Del Rey, Whitehurst,
tasks were presented (i.e., schedule of practice) varied. & Wood, 1983; Del Rey, Whitehurst, Wughalter, & Barn-
That is, one group learned several tasks in a random well, 1983; Del Rey, Wughalter, Du Bois, & Carnes, 1982;
order, whereas the other group learned the same tasks in Del Rey, Wughalter, & Whitehurst, 1982) partially sup-
a blocked order. ported the contextual interference theory. Subjects who
Several studies (Lee, Magill, & Weeks, 1985; Turnbull learned an open task in the random group did not always
& Dickinson, 1986; Wulf & Schmidt, 1988) have tried to differ significantly from subjects who learned the task in
assess whether the advantages of variability in practice the blocked group in their ability to retain the task.
result from generalized motor program formation or To differentiate between the effects of contextual
from enhanced information processing (caused by higher interference on the two types of tasks, a study must in-
contextual interference), but these studies were either clude both open and closed tasks, which can be present-
inconclusive or involved questionable experimental de- ed under conditions of either low (blocked practice) or
signs (Magill & Hall, 1990). There is apparently insuffi- high (random practice) contextual interference. Holding
cient empirical evidence of the relationship between vari- all other factors constant will enable an investigation of
able practice and practice schedule. the interaction between contextual interference and mo-
MagiJl and Hall (1990) hypothesized that the contex- tor tasks of different classifications.
tual interference effect differs for skill variations con- In a study that manipulated both the task classifica-
trolled by the same or different motor program. Accord- tion and the contextual interference, subjects performed

The Amen·can Journal of Occupational Therapy 813


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
a tracking task by moving their heads as they watched a an open task if the target or the client is placed on a
16-in. computer-generated video display containing a cur- moving scooter. Climbing the stairs can become an open
sor and a target (Tarus, 1988) The subject, who controlled task if practiced on moving escalators. Throwing a ball to
the cursor by performing head movements, had to posi- the client is an open task whose unpredictability can be
tion the cursor in the target. In the open task, the target increased if the client sits on an equilibrium board. Using
was in motion throughout the 5-sec trial. In the closed a wheelchair in a busy corridor rather than an empty one
task, the target remained stationary. Three target loca- is an open task.
tions were provided for both the open and closed tasks, During treatment, the therapist should try not only
to enable manipulation of the practice schedule. The sub- to present open tasks but also to organize the session so
jects were divided randomly into four groups: open task- that several tasks are learned simultaneously and ran-
random order, open task-blocked order, closed task- domly, avoiding repetitions. For example, the session
random order, and closed task-blocked order. Results could involve throwing a ball to the client from different
indicated that the high level of difficulty encountered angles, asking the client to throw bean bags to several
when practicing the open task (in both random and targets, practicing transfers from a wheelchair to a bed
blocked order) created high contextual interference, from different angles or to beels of different heights or
which is beneficial for retention and transfer (Tarus, firmness, or alternating between practicing activities of
1988). Subjects who learned the open task appeared to daily living and eye-hand coordination. The therapist can
be less dependent on the context in which the initial make the client do different tasks during each trial, alter-
encoding occurred. It seems that the open task better nate tasks, and introduce interfering activities that cause
prepared subjects to develop movement strategies or pat- the client to forget the solutions so that the client must
terns in an unpredictable environment, allowing better reconstruct the solutions when he or she tries the task
retention and transfer to a new task. The closed task again. Although this random presentation may be detri-
facilitated the development of fixed movement patterns, mental to performance, research suggests that it is benefi-
which resulted in inflexibility when a new task was en- cial for retention and transfer of the acquired task (Magill
countered (Tarus, 1988). & Hall, 1990). The exception to this guideline might be in
These findings illustrate that there is no unique the very early stages of practice, when the client is just
method of creating high contextual interference to facili- learning the basic concept of the task. At this time,
tate retention and transfer. In addition to presenting tasks blocked practice might be slightly more effective than
in an unpredictable order or with lower knowledge of random practice (Shea, Kohl, & Indermill, 1990). Once
results frequency, open tasks, in which critical factors in the client can perform the movement at all, random prac-
the environment are moving, can also create high contex- tice can be initiated (Schmidt, 1991).
tual interference. That is, predictable practice, such as Educating clients about these principles can height-
practice in a closed environment, in a blocked order, or en their understanding that these types of activities are
with constant knowledge of results, is beneficial for acqui- beneficial for their long-term treatment goals. According-
sition yet detrimental to retention and transfer, whereas ly, the clients may be less frustrated by a challenging or
unpredictable practice in an open environment, in a ran- difficult therapy session.
dom order, or with lower knowledge of results frequency
is detrimental to acquisition but beneficial for retention
Conclusion
and transfer.
Knowledge of effects of changing the order of presenta-
tion of different motor tasks, based on the different motor
Clinical implications
learning theories reviewed, can be used to facilitate reten-
Contextual variety, open environment, and low knowl- tion and transfer of motor skills. Occupational therapists
edge of results frequency apparently facilitate cognitive- may use this information when planning treatment. The
motor functioning during motor skill acquisition, thereby treatment goal should not necessarily be to achieve the
enhancing retention and transfer. Open tasks, unpredict- most effective performance during therapy, but to maxi-
able schedules, and limited knowledge of results reljuire mize retention and transfer of the acquired skill. When
clients to adapt strategies and solve problems, which de- the treatment goal is to enhance motor skill retention or
mand greater cognitive effort. Such activities facilitate re- transfer, increasing the difficulty of the learning context
tention and transfer of the acquired skill, because clients during practice is beneficial. Increased difficulty can be
are forced to draw on information other than knowledge achieved by teaching an open task, presenting tasks in a
of results and must either develop an elaborate encoding random order, or providing infrequent knowledge of
process or reconstruct movement plans for each practice results.
trial. Use of difficult practice contexts in occupational
For example, difficulty can be increased by teaching therapy is beneficial for the treatment process. In addi-
an open task. Throwing bean bags at a target can become tion, such situations correspond more closely to the

814 September 1994, Votume 48, Number 9

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


kinds of everyday situations clients encounter, which are effects of contextual interference on females with varied experi-
the core concern of occupational therapy (Breines, 1986; ence in open spon skills. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
King, 1978; Meyer, 1977). This teaching method might Sport, 53, 108-115
DiJoseph, L. M. (1982). Independence through activity:
support the fundamental assumptions of occupational Mind, body, and environment interaction in therapy. American
therapy, which suggest that the focus of treatment should journal of Occupational Therapy, 36, 740-744.
be the types of activities encountered in daily life (Dijo- Fidler, G. 5., & Fidler,]. W. (1978). Doing and becoming:
seph, 1982; Katz, 1985; King, 1978). These activities usu- Purposeful action and self-actualization. American journal of
Occupational Therapy, 32, 305-310.
ally require the ability to respond to unpredictable events
Gentile, A, M. (1972). A working model of skill acqUisition
on the basis of limited information.• with appliCation to teaching. Quest, 17, 3-23.
Gentile, A. M. (1987). Skill acquisition: Action, movement,
References
and neuromotor processes. In]. H. Carr, R. B. Shepherd, ].
Adams,]. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Gordon, &]. M. Held (Eds.), Movement science: Foundations
journal of NIotor BehaVior, 3, 111-150. for physical therapy in rehabilitation (pp. 93-154). Gaithers-
Adams,]. A. (1977). Feedback theory of how joint receptors burg, MD: Aspen.
regulate the timing and positioning of a limb. Psychological Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor perform-
Review, 84, 504-523. ance: Tests of the proportional duration model. Psychological
Arbib, M. A. (1985). Schemas for the temporal organization Review, 94, 225-276.
of behavior. Human Neurobiology, 4, 63-72. Gentner, D. R. (1988). Observed movements reflect both
Ayres, A.]. (1972). Sensory integration and learning dis- central and peripheral mechanisms: Reply to Heuer. Psychologi-
orders. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services, cal Review, 94, 558.
Baird, 1. S., & Hughes, G. H. (1972). Effects of frequency Gilfoyle, E M, Grady, A. P., & Moore,]. C. (1981). Children
and specificity of information feedback on the acquisition and adapt. Thorofare, NJ: Slack.
extinction of a positioning task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Gliner, ]. A, (1985). Purposeful activity in motor learning
34, 567-572 theory: An event approach to motor skill acquisition. Amen'can
Battig, W. F. (1979). The flexibility of human memory, In L. journal of Occupational Therapy, 39, 28-34.
S, Cermak & F. 1. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing and Gliner,]. A., & Davis, C. G. (1984). The effects of movement
human memory (pp. 23-44), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, speed and initial position on movement reproduction accuracy.
Battig, W. F., & Shea,] .B. (1978, November). Levels of Occupational Therapy journal of Research, 4(3), 181-192
processing as related to motor skills. Paper presented at the Goode,S., & Magill, R. A. (1986). The contextual interfer-
meeting of the Canadian Society for Psychomotor Learning and ence effects in learning three badminton serves. Research
Sport Psychology, Toronto, Quarter~y for Exercise and Sport, 57, 308-314.
Bernstein, A. (1967). The coordination and regulation of Heuer, H. (1988), Testing the invariance of relative timing:
movement. New York: Pergamon. Comment on Gentner, 1987. Psychological Review, 97, 492-
Breines, E. (1986). Ongins and adaptations, A philosophy 497
of practice. Lebanon, NJ: Geri-Rehab. Heuer, H. & Schmidt, R. A. (1988). Transfer of learning
Burgess, M. K. (1989). Motor control and the role of occu- among motor patterns with different relative timing.journal of
pational therapy: Past, present, and future. American journal Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Pelfonn-
of Occupational Therapy, 43, 345-348, ance, 14, 249-252
Clark, P. N. (1979), Human development through occupa- Higgins]. R., & Spaeth, R. K. (1972). Relationship between
tion: Theoretical frameworks in contemporary occupational consistency of movement and environmental condition. Quest,
therapy practice, pan 1. Amen'can jounwl of Occupational 17, 61-69
Therapy, 33, 505-514 Ho, L., & Shea,]. B. (1978). Effects of relative frequency of
Cohen, H. (1989). Nationally Speaking - Occupational knowledge of resultS on retention of a motor skill. Perceptual
therapy and motor COntrol. Amen'canjournal ofOccupational and NIotor Skills, 46, 859-866.
Therapy, 43, 289-290 Hobart, D]., Vorro,]. R., & Dotson, C. O. (1978). Synchro-
Craik, F. 1. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972) Levels of process- nized myoelectric and cinematographic analySis of skill acquisi-
ing: A framework for memory research. journal 0/ Verbal tion. journal of Human Movement Studies, 4, 155-166.
Learning and Verbal BehaVior, 11, 671-684. Horak, F. B. (1991), Assumptions underlying motor control
Del Rey, P. (1982), Effects of contextual interference on the for neurologic rehabilitation. Jn M.]. Lister (Ed.), Proceedings of
memory of older females differing in levels of physical activity. the 11 STEP Conference on Contemporary Management of Mo-
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 171-180, tor Control Problems (pp. 11-27). Alexandria, VA: Foundation
Del Rey, P., Whitehurst, M., & Wood,]. M. (1983). Effects of for Physical Therapy.
experience and contextual interference on learning and transfer Jarus, T. (1988). Contextual intelference and task classifi-
by boys and girls. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 581-582. cation effects on acquisition retention, and transfer of motor
Del Rey, P., \Xfhitehurst, M" Wughalter, E., & Barnwell,]. skills by female adults. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New
(1983). Contextual interference and experience in acquisition York University, New York.
and transfer. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 241-242. Jarus, T. (in press). Is more always better' Optimal
Del Rey, P" Wughalter, E., & Carnes, IVI. (1987). Level of amounts of feedback in learning to calibrate sensory awareness.
expertise, interpolated aCtivity and contextual interference ef- Occupational Therapy journal of Research.
fects on memory and transfer. Perceptual and /1;[otOl' Skills. 64, Katz, N. (1985). Occupational therapy's domain of concern:
257-284 Reconsidered, American journal of Occupational Therapy,
Del Rey, P., Wughalter, E., Du Bois, D" & Carnes, M. M. 39, 518-524
(1982). Effects of contextual interference and retention intervals Kerr, R. (1982), Practice variability and longer and short
on transfer. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 467-476. retention intervals. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 243-250.
Del Rey, P., Wughalter, E., & Whitehurst, M. (982). The King, L.]. (1978). Toward a science of adaptive responses.

The American Joumal of Occupational Therapy 815

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


American journal of Occupational Tberapy, 32, 429-437. behavioral emphasis (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human
Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1983a) Activity cluring the post- Kinetics.
KR interval: Effects U[1on performance of learning? Research Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Motor learning principles for phys-
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 54, 340-345. ical therapy. In M. j. Lister (Ed.), Proceedings of the II STEP
Lee, TO., & Magill, R. A. (1983b). The locus of contextual Conference on ContemporCllJl Management qf Motor Control
interference in motor-skills acquisition. journal ojExperimen- Problems (p[1 49-63). Alexandria, VA: Foundation for Physical
tal Psychology. Learning, Mem01J1, and Cognition, 9, 730-746 Therapy.
Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1985). Can forgetting facilitate Schmidt, R. A., Young, D. E., Swinnen, S., & Shapiro, D. C.
skill acquisition? In D. Goodman, R. B. Wilberg, & I. M. Frank (1989). Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Sup-
(Eds.), Differingperspectives in motor learning, memory, and POrt for the guidance hypothesis.journal ofExperimental Psy-
control (pp. 3-22). Amsterdam: North HolJand. chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 352-359.
Lee, T. D., Magill, R. A., & Weeks, D. j. (1985). Influence of Shea, C. H., Kohl, R., & Indermill, C. (1990). Contextual
practice schedule on testing schema theory predictions in interference: Contributions of practice. Acta Psychological
adults. journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 282-299. (amst), 73, 145-157.
Llorens, L. A. (1986). Activity analysis: Agreement among Shea,]. B, & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference
factors in a sensory processing model. American journal of effects on the acqUisition, retention, and transfer of a motor
Occupational Therapy, 40, 103-110. skill. journal of Experimental Psycbology: Human Learning
Magill, R. A. (1989). Motor learning: Concepts and appli- and MemoTJI, 5, 179-187.
cations (3rt! ed.). Dubuque, lA: William C. Brown. Shea,{B., & Wright, D. L. (1991). When forgetting benefits
Magill, R. A., & Hall, K. G. (1990). A review ofthe contextual motor retention. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
interference effect in motor skill acqUisition. Human Movement 62, 293-301.
Science, 9, 241-289. Shea,]. B., & Zimny, S. T. (1983). Context effects on mem-
Magill, R. A., Meeuwson, H.]., Lee, T. D., & Mathews, R. C. ory and learning movement information. In R. A. Magill (Ed.),
(1992). Is the contextual interference effect in motol' skill Memory and control oj action (pp. 345-366). Amsterdam:
learning a spacing e.ffecf? Unpublished manuscript. North Holland.
MagiJl, R. A., & Reeve, T. G. (1978). Variability of prior Shea,]. B., & Zimny, S. T. (1988). Knowledge incorporation
practice in learning and retention of a novel motor response. in motor representation. In 0. G. Meijer & K. Roth (Eds.),
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 107-110. Complex motor behavior: The motor-action controversy (pp.
McCracken, H. D., & Stelmach, G. E. (1977). A test of the 289-314). New York: North Holland.
schema theory of discrete motor learning. journal of Motor Tromhly, C. A. (Ed.). (1989). Occupational therapy for
Behavior, 9, 193-201. physical dYfifunction (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Meyer, A. (1977). The philosophy of occupational therapy. Tulving, E. (1979). Relation between encoding specificity
Americanjournal ofOccupational Therapy, 31, 639-642. (Re- and levels of processing. In L S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.),
printed from Archives qfOccupational TberapJI, 1922,1, 1-10.) Levels qf processing in human memory (pp. 405-428). Hills-
Mosey, A. C. (1981). Configuration of a profeSSion. New dale, N]: Erlbaum.
York: Raven. Turnbull, S. D., & Dickinson,]. (1986) Maximizing variabij:..
Mulder, T., & Hulsryn, W. (1984). Sensory feedback therapy ity of practice: '\ test of schema theory and contextual inter-
and theoretical knowledge of motor control and learning. ference theory. journal oj Human Movement Studies, 12,
American Journal qf Physical Medicine, 63, 226-244. 201-213.
Newell, K. M., & Shapiro, D. C. (1976). Variability of prac- Winstein, c.]., & Schmidt, R. A (1990). Reduced frequency
tice and transfer of training: Some evidence toward a schema ofknowJedge of result enhances motor skill learning.journal of
view of motOr learning.journal ofMotor BehaVior, 8, 233-243. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Poole, j. L. (1991a). Application of motor learning princi- 16, 677-691.
ples in occupational therapy. Americanjournal ojOccupation- Wrisberg, C. A., & Ragsdale, M. R. (1979). Further tests of
al Therapy, 45, 531-537. Schmidt's schema theory: Development of a schema rule for a
Poole, j. L. (1991b). Motor control. In C. B. Royeen (Ed.), coincident timing task.journal ofMotor BehaVior, 11, 159-166.
AOTA self study series: Neuroscience foundations of human Wulf, G., & Schmidt, R. A. (1988). Variability in practice:
perfonnance (pp. 1-23). Rockville, MD: The American Occupa- Facilitation in retention and transfer through schema formation
tional Therapy Association. or context effects? journal of /VIotor Behavior, 20, 133-149.
Sage, G. H. (1984). Motor learning and control: A new Wulf, G., & Schmidt, R. A. (1989). The learning of general-
neuropsychological approacb. Dubuque, lAo William C. Brown. ized motor programs: Reducing the relative frequency of knowl-
Salmoni, A. W., Schmidt, R. A., & Walter, C. B. (1984). edge of results enhances memory. journal of Experimental
Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 748-756.
reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 355-386. Young, D. E., Cohen, M.]., & Husak, W. S. (1993). Contex-
Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theOly of discrete motor tual interference and motor skill acquisition: On the proces-
skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225-260. ses that influence retention. Human Movement Science, 12,
Schmidt, R. A. (Ed.). (1988). Motor control and learning A 577-600

816 September 1994, Volume 48, Number 9

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930206/ on 06/03/2017 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like