Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618

DOI 10.1007/s10578-016-0687-7

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Child Abuse and Psychiatric Co-morbidity Among Chinese


Adolescents: Emotional Processing as Mediator and PTSD
from Past Trauma as Moderator
Man Cheung Chung1 · Zhuo Sheng Chen2

Published online: 4 October 2016


© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract  This study investigated whether child abuse Introduction


was associated with psychiatric co-morbidity in a group of
Chinese adolescents, and whether this association would The prevalence rate for child abuse among adolescents can
be mediated by emotional processing difficulties and mod- range from 17 to 71 % [1–3], some of whom (13 % in one
erated by the severity of PTSD from other traumas in the study) have experienced multiple abuses (e.g. physical and
past. Four hundred seventy-four adolescents participated in sexual abuse and neglect) [4]. Child abuse is associated with
the study. They completed the Childhood Trauma Ques- a range of psychiatric symptoms [5] including depression,
tionnaire–Short Form, General Health Questionnaire-28, suicidal tendency, hopelessness, hostility, substance abuse,
the Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale, and Emotional conduct disorder and general anxiety disorder [2, 6, 7].
processing scale-25. The results showed that after adjusting Adolescents who have a history of childhood abuse tend to
for the total number of traumatic events and how long ago have a low level of health-related quality of life [8], family
the most traumatic event occurred, child abuse was associ- cohesive problems, attention difficulty and daily stress [9].
ated with psychiatric co-morbidity. This association was They also tend to be more prone to delinquency, posttrau-
not moderated by the severity of PTSD from past traumas matic stress disorder and dissociation than those who have
but mediated by emotion processing difficulties. To con- not [10].
clude, adolescents who experience child abuse can develop Types of abuse seem to be related to specific psychiat-
emotional processing difficulties which in turn impact on ric co-morbidity. For example, emotional abuse and neglect
psychiatric symptoms. Experience of past trauma does not are associated with anxiety, depression, anger, poor psy-
influence these psychological processes. chological functioning [11], interpersonal problems [12]
and dissociation [13]. Sexual abuse is also associated with
Keywords  Child abuse · Emotional processing · depression. Additionally, it is associated with posttraumatic
PTSD · Mediation · Moderation stress disorder, conduct disorder, eating disorder, substance
abuse, panic disorder, borderline personality disorder and
suicidality [14, 15].
Two observations are noteworthy in examining the effect
of child abuse on psychiatric co-morbidity. The first is that
in addition to child abuse, based on data from both Western
and Eastern (Malaysia) samples, adolescents experiencing

Man Cheung Chung potentially traumatic events of other kinds (Polyvictimiza-
man.chung@cuhk.edu.hk tion) is common [1, 16, 17] ranging from 25 to 60 % or more
1 in the community and 60 to 90 % within clinical outpatients
Department of Educational Psychology, Faculty of
Education, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Ho Tim [18]. Before turning16, 1 in 4 children is likely to experience
Building, Shatin, NT, Hong Kong traumas other than abuse including assault, domestic vio-
2
Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, lence, and bullying [19]. Before entering adulthood, almost
Beijing, China 80 % of adolescents will have experienced at least one other

123
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618 611

trauma including accident (20 %), and death of someone


close (almost 20 %) [20]. Polyvictimization is associated Emotional
processing
with elevated psychiatric symptoms, risk-taking behaviour PTSD from
past trauma
[2, 17–19, 21, 22], physical health symptoms, obesity and
impaired immunity [23]. Compared with those of single
traumas, adolescents who had experienced multiple abuses
and other traumas were worse in symptom severity [6].
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) can result from Child abuse
these other traumatic events for adolescents [24]. The
Psychiatric
co-
morbidity
prevalence rates for full-PTSD were 22 and 51 % and for
partial-PTSD were 32 and 35 % in two separate studies [25,
26]. The number of traumatic events was positively corre- Fig. 1  The hypothesized model with PTSD from past traumas mod-
erating and emotional processing mediating the path between child
lated with the severity of PTSD symptoms [25]. In essence, abuse and psychiatric co-morbidity
this amounts to a cumulative traumatic effect. When trauma
types increase, the odds ratios for psychiatric symptoms
also increase [27]. PTSD is not that which can be ignored form of premature inhibition of processing in which victims
among adolescents and its severity may moderate the effect avoid thinking about the abuse and try to escape the emo-
of child abuse on psychiatric symptoms. To the best of our tional arousal associated with it. Repetitive processing like
knowledge, this has not been investigated systematically in this will lead to the process becoming automatic. Conse-
literature. quently, emotional arousal and deliberate attempts to avoid
Secondly, there is a paucity of research looking at the it are no longer necessary [42]. However, the unresolved and
effect of emotional processing difficulties among abused unprocessed distressing feelings accumulate in the body and
adolescents, with the exception of one study alluding to later cause disturbance in the physiological and neurologi-
the fact that child abuse and neglect were associated with cal system giving rise to health or psychological difficulties
increased emotional problems among Chinese adolescents [43], anxiety and depression [44], and negative affects [45].
[28]. Child abuse can change the neural system governing This study aimed to examine (1) whether there would be
emotion regulation [29, 30] thereby increasing sensitivity to a link between child abuse and psychiatric co-morbidity,
emotional conflict [31]. As adults, they continue to have dif- (2) whether this link would be mediated by emotional pro-
ficulties regulating and processing emotion [32–34]. They cessing difficulty and moderated by the severity of PTSD
also tend to avoid negative emotional expressions [35]. from other traumas among a group of secondary school
These emotional difficulties would, in turn, lead to problems adolescents in China. Guided by the preceding literature,
in interpersonal skills [32], self-concept and the concept of we hypothesized that the effect of child abuse would be
others [36], psychological disorders (e.g. disordered eating) positively associated with psychiatric co-morbidity. We also
[37], obesity [38] and extreme stress not otherwise specified hypothesized that the severity of PTSD from other trauma
[39]. In other words, child abuse leads to emotional pro- and emotional processing difficulty would moderate and
cessing difficulties which in turn affect the severity of psy- mediate respectively the effect of child abuse on psychiatric
chiatric co-morbidity among adolescents. Put another way, co-morbidity.
emotional processing difficulties could mediate the effect
of child abuse on psychiatric symptoms. Systematic stud-
ies investigating this are also lacking in literature, although Methods
evidence exists to support emotional processing difficulty
acting as a mediator among the adult population [40]. Procedure
What has been said sets the theoretical model for the cur-
rent investigation. Figure 1 depicts that the effect of child Four hundred and seventy-four adolescents (M  = 291,
abuse on psychiatric symptoms is moderated by the severity F = 183) were recruited from three vocational schools in a
of PTSD from other traumas and is mediated by emotional city in China. These schools were located in three different
processing difficulty. It echoes the self-trauma theory [41] areas of the city. Thus, the adolescents in the schools came
suggesting that child abuse affects self-capacities especially from families with diverse socio-economic backgrounds.
in the area of emotional regulation or self-regulation of Upon the approval of the schools, the research assistant
emotion. As a result, victims of abuse are at risk for ele- identified the classes that students meeting the inclusion
vated emotional distress which motivates them to protect criteria attended and presented the purpose of the study to
themselves by avoiding or dissociating themselves from it, them. Students were reassured that information collected
in effect not processing the distressing emotion. This is a would be kept anonymous and that they were entitled to

123
612 Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618

withdraw from the study any time they wished without giv- throughout life that adolescents may have experienced.
ing a reason. Students provided their assent to the study If adolescents had experienced more than one traumatic
before participation. They completed the questionnaires event, they were asked to identify the one which was most
described in the “Measures” section. The inclusion criteria traumatic. The second part assesses PTSD symptoms that
were that (1) they were between 13 and 19 years old, and (2) resulted from this most traumatic event. Adolescents were
they were not receiving special needs education. The first asked to rate the severity of the symptoms according to the
criterion was based on Erikson’s age range for adolescence. rating scale: 0 = not at all, 1 = once a week or less/once in a
The second criterion was based on the school records and while, 2 = 2 to 4 times a week/half the time, 3 = 5 or more
confirmed by the class teacher. All the questionnaires went times a week/almost always. Based on the PDS diagnostic
through the back translation procedure. Ethical approval criteria, adolescents were classified into three groups: full-
was sought from the Panyu Education Center in China. PTSD (meeting the threshold for all criteria), partial-PTSD
(meeting at least one symptom in each of the three PTSD
Measures symptom clusters [re-experiencing, avoidance and hyper-
arousal symptoms]) [53] or no-PTSD (not meeting the diag-
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form (CTQ) [46] nostic criteria). This scale has shown good reliability and
provides a brief, standardized, validated instrument for validity and good agreement with the Structured Clinical
assessing abuse and neglect, with five subscales: emotional Interview for Diagnosis (kappa = 0.65, agreement = 82 %,
abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect and sensitivity = 0.89 and specificity = 0.75). PDS was used and
physical neglect. Adolescents were asked to answer on a validated in a recent study based on a group of Chinese ado-
5-point Likert scale from “never true” [1] to “very often lescents (Cronbach’s α score for the total score = 0.95) [51].
true” [5]. CTQ has been validated based on adolescent and Based on the sample of the current study, the Cronbach’s α
adult populations. Good internal consistency was reported for PTSD total was 0.95.
with alpha reliabilities ranging from 0.70 to 0.93 and retest Emotional processing scale-25 (EPS-25) [54] aims to
reliabilities ranging from 0.66 to 0.94. One study looking identify emotional processing difficulties. The scale contains
at Chinese college students demonstrated a good retest reli- five factors with 3 (avoidance, suppression, unregulated
ability (0.71) and Cronbach’s α (0.60) for the total score emotion) assessing emotional control and dysregulation.
[47]. Based on the current sample, the Cronbach’s α for the Impoverished emotion is another factor examining difficul-
total score was 0.85. ties including differentiating feelings from illness and flat
The General Health Questionnaire-28 (GHQ-28) [48] emotion. Unprocessed emotions are felt to be intrusive,
aims to estimate the likelihood of adolescents being diag- controlling and overwhelming. Adolescents’ responses
nosed as suffering from general psychiatric morbidity were scored according to their experiences of emotional
at interview. The questions are scored using the scoring processes on a scale of 0 = completely disagree to 9 = com-
method of 0-0-1-1, which counts the number of symptoms pletely agree. EPS-25 was validated from a whole range of
and avoids the problems of middle-user response bias. As samples including youths. Internal reliability for the overall
the total GHQ score exceeds the recommended cutoff point scale is high (α = 0.92) and the reliability for the subscales
of 4, the probability for adolescents to be diagnosed as suf- ranged from (α = 0.70 to 0.80). Based on Chinese univer-
fering from a general psychiatric disorder increases. As we sity students, one study revealed good internal consistency
analysed the mean scores of the GHQ-28, however, the rat- (0.85), Cronbach’s α (0.65 to 0.77) for the subscales and
ing scale of 1-2-3-4 was used for preventing the problem of test–retest reliability (0.75 to 0.87) [55]. The Cronbach’s α
information loss. In PTSD research, the GHQ-28 has been for the total score of the current sample was 0.92.
recommended as a standardized questionnaire to measure
global dysfunction and diagnose co-morbid psychological Data Analysis Plan
disorders associated with PTSD [49]. The questionnaire
yields four subscales which are somatic problems, anxiety, Descriptive statistics were used to describe the data initially.
social dysfunction and depression. Reliability coefficients Within-subject ANOVA was used to examine the within-
ranged from 0.78 to 0.95 [50]. GHQ-28 has recently been subject effects between the subscales of questionnaires.
validated in a study based on a sample of Chinese adoles- Correlation coefficients including point biserial correlations
cents (Cronbach’s α score for the total score = 0.94) [51]. (rbp) were carried out to examine the relationship between
Based on the current sample, the Cronbach’s α was 0.91 for adolescents’ demographic variables and psychiatric co-mor-
the total score. bidity. PROCESS, a modelling tool for mediation and mod-
The Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS) [52] eration analysis, was used [56]. Bias-corrected bootstrapping
aims to assess PTSD, based on DSM-IV criteria. The first was used to generate confidence intervals which addressed
part of PDS focuses on a range of previous traumatic events the problem of power resulting from the asymmetric and

123
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618 613

non-normal sampling distributions of an indirect effect [57]. differed significantly from each other (p < 0.001) except for
The bootstrapping sampling (n = 1000) distributions of the unprocessed and improvised emotions.
indirect effects were produced by selecting a sample of cases In terms of PTSD from past traumas, sudden death of
from the complete data set and calculating the indirect effects someone close was the traumatic event reported most fre-
in the resamples. Point estimates and confidence intervals quently (17 %). Otherwise, events included accident (9 %),
(95 %) were estimated for the indirect effects. When zero physical assault (4 %), natural disaster (3 %) and life threat-
was not contained in the confidence interval, point estimates ening illness (3 %). On average, the most traumatic event
of indirect effects were then considered to be significant. occurred just over 1 year ago (mean = 13.27 in months,
Expectation Maximization (EM) algorithm [58] was used to SD = 32.58). The total number of traumatic events that hap-
replace the missing data. Less than 5 % of responses were pened in their lives was 0.83 (SD = 1.67).
missing due to participants omitting questionnaire items. On the basis of the Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale,
Regression imputation has been shown to be a valid method 19 % met the diagnostic criteria for full-PTSD; 22 % partial
in dealing with missing data [59]. and 59 % no-PTSD. At the time of the study, the adoles-
cents reported that the severity of PTSD symptoms had been
persistent for 3 months or more. Looking at psychiatric co-
Results morbidity, there were significant within-subject effects for
symptoms [Greenhouse-Geisser: F(2.85, 1352.38) = 166.70,
Four hundred and seventy-four adolescents (M = 291, F = 183) p < 0.001, η2 = 0.26]. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni
were recruited from 3 vocational schools in a city in China. adjustment showed that social dysfunction and depression
They were on average 15.39 (SD = 0.67) years old ranging differed significantly from each other (p < 0.001). Ninety-
from 14 to 18. Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations two percent met the cutoff for GHQ-28 cases. It is notewor-
of measures on child abuse, emotional processing difficulties, thy that PTSD was a distinctively different psychological
PTSD following past traumas and psychiatric co-morbidity. construct from psychiatric co-morbid symptoms (Collinear-
There were significant within-subject effects for types of ity: tolerance = 0.55, 0.44, 0.75, 0.61 and VIF = 1.80, 1.24,
child abuse [Greenhouse-Geisser: F(1.96, 931.01) = 290.42, 1.33 and 1.62 for somatic problems, anxiety, social dysfunc-
p < 0.001, η2 = 0.38] and emotional processing difficulties tion and depression respectively).
[Greenhouse-Geisser: F(3.60, 1706.91)  = 88.62, p < 0.001, To address the two research questions, correlation coeffi-
2
η  = 0.15]. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni adjustment cients including point biserial correlations (rbp) were carried
showed that all child abuse and emotional processing types out to ascertain whether demographic variables would relate
to outcomes. PTSD research suggests that “victim variables”
are related to PTSD severity which in turn might influence
Table 1  The means and standard deviations of measures on child distress outcomes [60, 61]. The results showed that the total
abuse, posttraumatic stress following past traumas, psychiatric co-
morbidity and emotional processing
number of traumatic events experienced was correlated with
psychiatric co-morbidity (r = 0.18, p < 0.001). Summing up
Mean SD the total number of traumatic events meant losing sight of
Physical abuse 6.12 2.31 qualitative differences in trauma responses. To make an
Emotional abuse 7.03 3.62 attempt to address this issue, following recent literature
Sexual abuse 5.34 1.26 [62], the traumatic events were classified into interpersonal
Emotional neglect 11.86 6.14 and single or non-interpersonal trauma. Adopting the defi-
Physical neglect 8.18 3.18 nitions from recent studies [63, 64], interpersonal trauma
Re-experiencing 1.65 2.92 was defined as an assault experienced directly from another
Avoidance 2.09 3.86
person, examples of which were robbery, physical or sexual
Hyperarousal 1.55 2.96
abuse/assault, assault with weapon, and domestic violence.
Somatic problem 18.31 3.10
Type of trauma in this study was, however, not significantly
correlated with psychiatric co-morbidity (r  = 0.16, ns).
Anxiety 18.02 3.39
Neither were gender (rpb = 0.08, ns) and age (r = 0.06, ns).
Social dysfunction 19.87 2.73
On the other hand, how long ago the most traumatic event
Depression 16.65 3.26
occurred (r = 0.11, p < 0.05) was significantly correlated
Suppression 19.67 8.10
with psychiatric co-morbidity.
Unprocessed emotion 17.73 8.63
Thus, the total number of traumatic events and how long
Unregulated emotion 15.41 8.88
ago the most traumatic event occurred were treated as co-
Avoidance 21.32 8.73
variates in the mediational and moderation analyses. The
Impoverished emotion 18.01 8.78
initial analysis was based on the total scores of PTSD from

123
614 Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618

past trauma (moderator) and emotional processing difficulty difficulty and moderated by the severity of PTSD symptoms
(mediator). The results showed that child abuse predicted from past traumas among a group of adolescents in China.
psychiatric co-morbidity. The cross-product term (child After adjusting for the total number of traumatic events and
abuse X PTSD total) was not significant when the outcome how long ago the most traumatic event occurred, consis-
was psychiatric co-morbidity. The total amount of emotional tent with the first hypothesis, child abuse was associated
processing difficulty mediated the effects of child abuse on with psychiatric co-morbidity. The second hypothesis was
psychiatric co-morbidity (Effect: 0.11, Boot SE: 0.02, Boot partially confirmed in that emotional processing difficulties
LLCI: 0.07, Boot ULCI: 0.16). These results were consistent (unprocessed emotion, unregulated emotion and impov-
even without these two co-variates adjusted (Effect: 0.09, erished emotion) mediated the relationship between child
Boot SE: 0.02, Boot LLCI: 0.05, Boot ULCI: 0.13). abuse and psychiatric co-morbidity. However, contrary to
Further analyses were then carried out to examine which the hypothesis, the severity of PTSD symptoms following
types of emotion processing difficulty acted as significant past trauma did not moderate the preceding relationship.
mediators. The results showed that unprocessed, unregulated The finding on the association between child abuse and
and impoverished emotions were the significant mediators. psychiatric co-morbidity provided further evidence for the
Increased child abuse was positively correlated with the detrimental effects of child abuse. Although trauma symp-
three types of emotion processing difficulty (unprocessed, toms can vary depending on cultural differences, e.g. col-
r = 
0.19; unregulated, r  = 0.26, impoverished, r  = 0.23) lectivism versus individualism [65, 66], the link between
which were in turn associated with increased psychiatric the effect of child abuse on psychiatric co-morbidity in our
co-morbidity (unprocessed, r = 0.17; unregulated, r = 0.18, Chinese adolescents was consistent with the link established
impoverished, r = 0.37). These analyses also confirmed that mostly based on Western samples described in the introduc-
PTSD from past trauma did not moderate the effect of child tion and suggests that the effect of child abuse is somewhat
abuse on psychiatric co-morbidity (see Table 2). universal.
These Chinese adolescents were presumably influenced
by a culture of collectivism which emphasizes the impor-
Discussion tance of groups such as families. Group membership is
important for self-concept. Well-being results from carrying
This study aimed to investigate whether there would be a out social roles and meeting social obligation. Meanwhile,
link between child abuse and psychiatric co-morbidity, and restraint in emotional expression is encouraged to ensure
whether this link would be mediated by emotional processing group harmony [65]. Thus, child abuse by a group member

Table 2  Results on moderating and mediating the effect of child abuse on psychiatric co-morbidity
Effect SE t p LLCI ULCI Boot SE Boot LLCI Boot ULCI

Model
Emotion processing – 0.01 11.43 0.00 0.10 0.14 – – –
Child abuse – 0.04 2.87 0.00 0.03 0.19 – – –
PTSD – 0.14 0.77 0.44 −0.17 0.39 – – –
Child abuse X PTSD – 0.00 −0.48 0.62 −0.00 0.00 – – –
No of events – 0.26 1.91 0.05 −0.01 1.04 – – –
Onset of trauma – 0.01 0.01 0.99 −0.02 0.02 – – –
Conditional direct effects of X (child abuse) on Y (psychiatric co-morbidity) at values of the moderator (PTSD)
PTSD
0.00 0.06 0.04 1.54 0.12 −0.01 0.14 – – –
5.40 0.05 0.03 1.62 0.10 −0.01 0.12 – – –
14.51 0.04 0.04 1.16 0.24 −0.03 0.13 – – –
Indirect effect of X on Y
Suppression 0.00 – – – – – 0.00 −0.01 0.01
Unprocessed emotion 0.03 – – – – – 0.01 0.01 0.06
Unregulated emotion 0.05 – – – – – 0.02 0.01 0.10
Avoidance −0.00 – – – – – 0.00 −0.03 −0.00
Impoverished emotion 0.08 – – – – – 0.02 0.05 0.13

123
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618 615

(such as a caregiver) would mean a major assault to the include the distortion of regulating bodily and emotional
development of the self-concept and well-being for these processes, anxiety and arousal modulation and ultimately
adolescents. Betrayal trauma is a way to capture this strug- psychological well-being [74].
gle [67]. In addition, adolescents’ psychological distress Focusing on the three emotional processing difficulties
could be exacerbated by being in a sensitive developmen- (unprocessed emotion, unregulated emotion and impover-
tal period. According to Erikson [68, 69], this is a period ished emotion) which mediated the effect of child abuse on
of psychological confusion and instability in which they psychiatric co-morbidity, arguably, what underpinned the
try to develop their own identity. Betrayal from their own first two is lack of control. What underpinned the third emo-
caregiver during this sensitive developmental period could tion is an attempt to cope with distress, i.e. coping by not
disrupt their self-concept and personality [67] and heighten allowing oneself to get in touch with one’s internal feelings.
sensitivity to emotional conflict [31] and ultimately the The lack of control is understandable given what we said
severity of psychiatric co-morbidity [67]. earlier about these Chinese adolescents being discouraged
The lack of moderating effect of PTSD from past trauma to express distress for the sake of keeping group harmony.
on the path between child abuse and psychiatric co-morbid- In other words, they found themselves having to endure the
ity was surprising. This does not mean, however, that the effect of these overwhelmingly distressing emotions rather
phenomenon of cumulative trauma or polyvictimization was than allow themselves to gain control over their emotions by
irrelevant for these adolescents, but that it was expressed confronting and processing them. Without emotional pro-
unexpectedly. Bivariate correlation showed that PTSD from cessing, the effect of the abuse lingered over a long period
past trauma was associated with psychiatric co-morbidity of time.
(r = 0.21, p < 0.001). However, it became non-significant This could lead to the emergence of the third emotion
when it was entered into the moderation analyses. In other which is a coping strategy. They might have developed a kind
words, for these adolescents, it was not the case that the of “alexithymic” state [75] in order to protect themselves by
effect of their abuse on psychiatric co-morbidity was severe endorsing an inhibitory mechanism that regulates the flow
when the degree of PTSD was high. Neither was it the case of traumatic information [76]. Being unable to differenti-
that the effect of abuse had little effect on psychiatric co- ate feelings is an attempt to inhibit difficult, intensive and
morbidity when the degree of PTSD was low. The results negative emotions [77, 78] and avoid frightening or intoler-
seemed to support an additive model in that the effects of able feelings [79]. It is therefore not surprising to learn that
PTSD from past trauma and child abuse made independent betrayal trauma leads to the emergence of dissociation and
or unique contributions to distress outcomes; the clinical difficulties in emotional processing [67]. As was mentioned
phenomenon of pathogenetic comorbidity [70]. in the introduction, the consequence of not allowing these
Arguably, this is consistent with one assumption behind emotions to be processed is the unresolved and unprocessed
distinguishing externalization and internalization in child- distressing feelings accumulating in the body, causing dis-
hood disorder symptoms [71]. The two broad-band clinical turbance in the physiological and neurological system and
features are separate psychological constructs contributing later giving rise to health or psychological difficulties [43],
to the genesis of each other [72]. Similarly, it is plausible anxiety and depression [44] and negative affects [45].
that symptoms resulting from the abuse and from past trau- It is noteworthy that the emotional processing difficulties
mas other than child abuse could have specific, unique and of suppression and avoidance had a direct, as opposed to an
direct impacts on the genesis of psychiatric co-morbidity. indirect, impact on psychiatric co-morbidity (suppression,
In terms of the mediation results, unprocessed emo- r = 0.30, p < 0.001; avoidance, r = 0.28, p < 0.001). This
tion, unregulated emotion and impoverished emotion were opens up questions about the roles of emotional processing
mediators for the effect of child abuse on psychiatric co- strategies. Some seemed to carry the effects of child abuse
morbidity. In other words, for these adolescents, the abuse onto the distress outcome but others did not. Perhaps this
impacted their psychiatric symptoms indirectly through the reflects individual differences in emotional processing. For
severity of intrusive and overwhelming distressing emotion example, it has been argued that the way in which we reg-
associated with the abuse, difficulty controlling or regulating ulate our emotions is manifested through culture [80] and
emotion, and differentiating feelings. This is consistent with influenced by our temperament [81] and personality [82].
the postulate that traumas can affect self-regulatory ability It is therefore not inconceivable that certain emotional pro-
and increase symptom complexity [73]. Developmentally cessing difficulties may or may not affect the effect of abuse
adverse interpersonal traumas of which child abuse is one on psychiatric co-morbidity among adolescents depending
type can distort psychological development especially when on their individual differences.
occurring during periods (like infancy or adolescence) in One implication from the findings of the current study for
which foundational self-regulatory capacities are develop- school principals and parents is that screening for possible
ing and being consolidated. These self-regulatory capacities child abuse among adolescent pupils is paramount. Such

123
616 Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618

abuse could have a long term impact on their psychologi- References


cal well-being which, in turn, could affect their academic
performance. For those who have been identified as having   1. Brown EJ (2005) Correlates and treatment of stress disorder in
children and adolescents. Psychiatr Ann 35(9):759–765
been abused in the past, opportunity to process distressing   2. Greger HK, Myhre AK, Lydersen S, Jozefiak T (2015) Previous
emotions associated with the abuse should be given. What maltreatment and present mental health in a high-risk adolescent
entails in the procedure for processing these emotions is population. Child Abuse Neglect 45:122–134
beyond the scope of this paper. However, one main com-   3. Manoj J, Thomas J (2014) Prevalence of maltreatment of adoles-
cents among high school children in Kerala. In: Merrick J (ed)
ponent is that it be carried out in line with the Chinese col- Child and adolescent health yearbook 2013. Nova Science Pub-
lective culture. Otherwise, an adverse effect may occur as lishers, New York, pp 317–322
a result of the emotional processing. Also, opportunities  4. Arata CM, Langhinrichsen-Rohling J , Bowers D, O’Farrill-
for processing traumatic emotions should not be viewed Swails L (2005) Single versus multi-type maltreatment: an exam-
ination of the long-term effects of child abuse. J Aggress Maltreat
as opportunities for treating psychopathologies for adoles- Trauma 11:29–52
cents, but as attempts to confront and address maladaptive   5. Kaplan SJ, Pelcovitz D, Labruna V (1999) Child and adolescent
coping or protective strategies that these adolescents might abuse and neglect research: a review of the past 10 years. Part
have adopted to deal with emotional exhaustion. I: physical and emotional abuse and neglect. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 38:1214–1222
Limitations of the study need to be mentioned. Firstly,   6. Arata C, Langhinrichsen-Rohling J, Bowers D, O’Brien N (2007)
although the PDS is a widely used tool for measuring the Differential correlates of multi-type maltreatment among urban
severity of PTSD based on a given list of events, adolescents youth. Child Abuse Neglect 31:393–415
could have under-reported the number or type of traumatic   7. Brewin CR (1996) Cognitive processing of adverse experiences.
Int Rev Psychiatry 8(4):333–339
events. Unfortunately, we had no way of verifying this. They   8. Weber S, Jud A, Landolt M (2016) Quality of life in maltreated
might not have recalled certain traumas since repression of children and adult survivors of child maltreatment: a systematic
traumatic memories has been shown to be prevalent dur- review. Qual Life Res 25:237–255
ing childhood or adolescence, although the psychological   9. Williamson JM, Borduin CM, Howe BA (1991) The ecology of
adolescent maltreatment: a multilevel examination of adolescent
mechanisms underlying these memories are controversial physical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect. J Consult Clin Psychol
[83, 84]. In other words, the present results could have been 59:449–457
exacerbated by these covert traumatic effects. Secondly, all 10. Wolfe DA, Scott K, Wekerle C, Pittman A-L (2001) Child
adolescents in the present study were selected from voca- maltreatment: risk of adjustment problems and dating vio-
lence in adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
tional schools. Traditionally speaking, these students tend 40(3):282–289
to have lower academic ability than those in non-vocational 11. van Vugt E, Lanctot N, Paquette G, Collin-Vezina D, Lemieux
schools. This could have affected the way in which they A (2014) Girls in residential care: from child maltreatment to
understood some of the questions on the measures. Thirdly, trauma-related symptoms in emerging adulthood. Child Abuse
Neglect 38(1):114–122
we did not control for the degree of perceived daily stress 12. Ometto M, Oliveira PA, Milioni AL, Santos B, Scivoletto S,
among these adolescents. Whether they appraised their cur- Busatto GF et al (2016) Social skills and psychopathic traits in mal-
rent academic situation as unpredictable, uncontrollable and treated adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 25:397–405
overloading might have affected the degree of psychiatric 13. Sar V, Onder C, Kilincaslan A, Zoroglu SS, Alyanak B (2014)
Dissociative identity disorder among adolescents: prevalence in
co-morbidity. These adolescents might have also encoun- a university psychiatric outpatient unit. J Trauma Dissociation
tered stressful issues pertaining to their parents, school and 15(4):402–419
wider community as advocated by the developmental con- 14. McLean CP, Rosenbach SB, Capaldi S, Foa EB (2013) Social
textualism framework [85]. Again, whether these factors and academic functioning in adolescents with child sexual abuse-
related PTSD. Child Abuse Neglect 37(9):675–678
might have contributed to overall psychological well-being 15. O’Brien BS, Sher L (2013) Child sexual abuse and the pathophys-
is unknown. Lastly, it would have been useful to have some iology of suicide in adolescents and adults. Int J Adolesc Med
information on the adolescents’ individual differences, in Health 25(3):201–205
terms of cultural beliefs and personality. This might shed 16. Flannery DJ, Wester KL, Singer MI (2004) Impact of exposure
to violence in school on child and adolescent mental health and
light onto some of their psychological reactions, particularly behavior. J Community Psychol 32(5):559–573
concerning emotional processing. 17. Ford JD, Elhai JD, Connor DF, Frueh B (2010) Poly-victimiza-
tion and risk of posttraumatic, depressive, and substance use dis-
orders and involvement in delinquency in a national sample of
adolescents. J Adolesc Health 46(6):545–552
Summary 18. Fisak B (2014) Trauma-related problems and disorders. Com-
prehensive evidence based interventions for children and adoles-
To conclude, child abuse can impact on emotional process- cents. Wiley, Hoboken, pp 287–300
ing difficulties which can then affect psychiatric symptoms. 19. Gerson R (2015) Child abuse and trauma. Helping kids in crisis:
managing psychiatric emergencies in children and adolescents.
The severity of other past traumas had a limited influence American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc, Arlington, pp 91–108
on this finding.

123
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618 617

20. Ghazali SR, Elklit A, Balang RV, Sultan M, Kana K (2014) Pre- 39. Zlotnick C, Zakriski AL, Shea M, Costello E (1996) The long-
liminary findings on lifetime trauma prevalence and PTSD symp- term sequelae of sexual abuse: support for a complex posttrau-
toms among adolescents in Sarawak Malaysia. Asian J Psychiatry matic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 9(2):195–205
11:45–49 40. Carpenter L, Chung MC (2011) Childhood trauma in obsessive
21. Adams ZW, Moreland A, Cohen JR, Lee RC, Hanson RF, Dan- compulsive disorder: the roles of alexithymia and attachment.
ielson CK et al (2016) Polyvictimization: latent profiles and men- Psychol Psychother 84(4):367–388
tal health outcomes in a clinical sample of adolescents. Psychol 41. Briere J (2002) Treating adult survivors of severe childhood
Violence 6:145–155 abuse and neglect: future development of an integrative model.
22. Gustafsson PE, Nilsson D, Svedin CG (2009) Polytraumatization In: Myers LB JFB, Briere J (eds) The APSAC handbook on child
and psychological symptoms in children and adolescents. Eur maltreatment. Sage Publications, Newbury Park, pp 175–204
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 18(5):274–283 42. Brewin CR, Dalgleish T, J oseph S (1996) A dual represen-
23. Clark DB, Thatcher DL, Martin CS (2010) Child abuse and other tation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Rev
traumatic experiences, alcohol use disorders, and health prob- 103(4):670–686
lems in adolescence and young adulthood. J Pediatr Psychol 43. Pennebaker JW (1995) Emotion, disclosure & health. American
35(5):499–510 Psychological Association., Washington DC
24. Brosky BA, Lally SJ (2004) Prevalence of trauma, PTSD, and 44. Amstadter AB, Vernon LL (2008) A preliminary examination of
dissociation in court-referred adolescents. J Interpers Violence thought suppression, emotion regulation, and coping in a trauma-
19:801–814 exposed sample. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma 17:279–295
25. Ndetei DM, Ongecha-Owuor FA, Khasakhala L, Mutiso V, 45. Gross JJ, John OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion
Odhiambo G, Kokonya DA (2007) Traumatic experiences of regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and
Kenyan secondary school students. J Child Adolesc Ment Health well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:348–362
19(2):147–155 46. Bernstein DP, Stein J A, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D,
26. Silva RR, Alpert M, Munoz DM, Singh S, Matzner F, Dummit S Ahluvalia T et al (2003) Development and validation of a brief
(2000) Stress and vulnerability to posttraumatic stress disorder in screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child
children and adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 157(8):1229–1235 Abuse Neglect 27:169–190
27. Layne CM, Greeson J K, Ostrowski SA, Kim S, Reading S, 47. Fu W-Q, Yao S-Q (2005) Initial reliability and validity of Child-
Vivrette RL et al (2014) Cumulative trauma exposure and high hood Trauma Questionnaire(CTQ-SF) applied in chinese college
risk behavior in adolescence: findings from the National Child students. Chin J Clin Psychol 13(1):40–42
Traumatic Stress Network Core Data Set. Psychol Trauma 48. Goldberg D, Hillier V (1979) A scaled version of the general
6(Suppl 1):S40–S49 health questionnaire. Psychol Med 9:139–145
28. Li Y, Cao F-l, Cui N-x, Li Y-l (2012) Child abuse and neglect, 49. Raphael B, Lundin T, Weisaeth L (1989) A research method for
executive function, and emotional and behavioral problems in the study of psychological and psychiatric aspects of disaster.
rural adolescents. Chin J Clin Psychol 20(6):813–815 Acta Psychiatr Scand 353:1–75
29. McLaughlin KA, Peverill M, Gold AL, Alves S, Sheridan MA 50. Goldberg D, Bridges KW (1987) Screening for psychiatric illness
(2015) Child maltreatment and neural systems underlying emotion in general practice: the general practitioner versus the screening
regulation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 54(9):753–762 questionnaire. J R Coll Gen Pract 37:15–18
30. Owens M, Goodyer IM, Wilkinson P, Bhardwaj A, Abbott R, 51. Chen ZS, Chung MC (2016) The relationship between gender,
Croudace T et al (2012) 5-HTTLPR and early childhood adversi- posttraumatic stress disorder from past trauma, alexithymia and
ties moderate cognitive and emotional processing in adolescence. psychiatric co-morbidity in Chinese adolescents: a moderated
PLoS ONE 7(11):e48482 mediational analysis. Psychiatr Q 87:689–701
31. Marusak HA, Martin KR, Etkin A, Thomason ME (2015) Child- 52. Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K (1997) The validation of
hood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emo- a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder. The post-
tional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology 40(5):1250–1258 traumatic diagnostic scale. Psychol Assess 9:445–451
32. Levitt JT, Cloitre M (2005) A Clinician’s guide to STAIR/MPE: 53. Stein MB, Walker JR, Hazen AL, Forde DR (1997) Full and par-
treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse. Cogn Behav Pract tial posttraumatic stress disorder: findings from a community sur-
12(1):40–52 vey. Am J Psychiatry 154:1114–1119
33. Powers A, Cross D, Fani N, Bradley B (2015) PTSD, emotion 54. Baker R, Thomas S, Thomas WP, Owens M (2007) Development
dysregulation, and dissociative symptoms in a highly traumatized of an emotional processing scale. J Psychosom Res 62:167–178
sample. J Psychiatr Res 61:174–179 55. Wang H-b, Liu T, Lu J-m, Zhang J-j (2013) Reliability and valid-
34. Young J C, Widom CS (2014) Long-term effects of child abuse ity of the emotional processing scale in Chinese undergraduates.
and neglect on emotion processing in adulthood. Child Abuse Chin J Clin Psychol 21(2):209–212
Neglect 38(8):1369–1381 56. Hayes AF (2013) Introduction to mediation, moderation and con-
35. Bell KM, Naugle AE (2008) The role of emotion recognition ditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. The Guil-
skills in adult sexual revictimization. J Behav Anal Offender Vict ford Press, New York
Treat Prev 1(4):93–118 57. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J (2004) Confidence
36. Arntz A (1994) Treatment of borderline personality disorder: a limits for the indirect effect. Distribution of the produce and resa-
challenge for cognitive-behavioural therapy. Behav Res Ther mpling methods. Multivar Behav Res 39:99–128
32(4):419–430 58. Enders CK (2011) A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms
37. Mitchell KS, Mazzeo SE (2005) Mediators of the association available for use with missing data. Struct Equ Model 8:128–141
between abuse and disordered eating in undergraduate men. Eat 59. Schafer J L, Graham J W (2002) Missing data: our view of the
Behav 6(4):318–327 state of the art. Psychol Methods 7:147–177
38. Zeller MH, Noll J G, Sarwer DB, Reiter-Purtill J , Rofey DL, 60. Friedman M, Keane T, Resick P (eds) (2007) Handbook of PTSD:
Baughcum AE et al (2015) Child maltreatment and the adoles- science and practice. Guilford, New York
cent patient with severe obesity: implications for clinical care. J 61. Vogt D, King D, King L (2007) Risk pathways in PTSD: making
Pediatr Psychol 40(7):640–648 sense of the literature. In: Friedman M, Kean T, Resick P (eds)

123
618 Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2017) 48:610–618

Handbook of PTSD: science and practice. Guildford, New York, 74. Ford J D (2013) Enhancing emotional regulation with complex
pp 99–116 trauma survivors. In: Murphy D, Joseph S (eds) Trauma and the
62. Ehring T, Quack D (2010) Emotion regulation difficulties in therapeutic relationship. Palgrave, Basingstoke
trauma survivors: the role of trauma type and PTSD symptom 75. Bagby RM, Parker J DA, Taylor GJ (1994) The twenty-item
severity. Behav Ther 41:587–598 toronto alexithymia scale-I: item selection and cross-validation
63. Lilly MM, Lim BH (2013) Shared pathogeneses of posttrauma of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res 38:23–32
pathologies: attachment, emotion regulation and cognitions. J 76. Horowitz MJ (1976) Stress response syndromes. Aronson,
Clin Psychol 69:737–748 Northvale
64. Westphal M, Olfson M, Bravova M, Gameroff MJ, Gross R, al 77. Helmes E, McNeill PD, Holden RR, Jackson C (2008) The con-
e (2013) Borderline personality disorder, exposure to interper- struct of alexithymia: associations with defense mechanisms. J
sonal trauma, and psychiatric comorbidity in urban primary care Clin Psychol 64:318–331
patients. Psychiatry 76:365–380 78. Meganck R, Vanheule S, Desmet M (2013) Affective processing
65. J ayawickreme N, J ayawickreme E, Foa EB (2013) Using the and affect regulation: a clinical interview study. J Am Psychoanal
individualism-collectivism construct to understand cultural dif- Assoc 61:NP12-NP6
ferences in PTSD. In: Gow KM, Celinski MJ (eds) Mass trauma: 79. Busch FN (2014) Clinical approaches to somatization. J Clin Psy-
impact and recovery issues. Nova Science Publishers, Hauppauge, chol 70:419–427
pp 55–76 80. Mesquita B, Albert D (2007) The cultural regulation of emotions.
66. Yeomans PD, Forman EM (2009) Cultural factors in traumatic In: Gross JJ (ed) Handbook of emotion regulation. The Guildford
stress. In: Eshun S, Gurung RAR (ed) Culture and mental health. Press, New York, pp 486–503
Wiley-Blackwell., Chichester, pp 221–244 81. Rothbart MK, Sheese BE (2007) Temperament and emotion regu-
67. Kaehler LA, Babcock R, DePrince AP, Freyd JJ (2013) Betrayal lation. In: Gross J J (ed) Handbook of emotion regulation. The
trauma. Treating complex traumatic stress disorders in children Guildford Press, New York, pp 331–350
and adolescents: scientific foundations and therapeutic models. 82. John OP, Gross JJ (2007) Individual differences in emotion regu-
Guilford Press, New York, pp 62–78 lation. In: Gross J J (ed) Handbook of emotion regulation. The
68. Erikson EH (1963) Childhood and society. Norton, New York Guildford Press, New York, pp 351–372
69. Erikson EH (1968) Identity: youth and crisis. Norton, New York 83. Greenhoot AF, Tsethlikai M (2009) Repressed and recovered
70. Kaplan MH, Feinstein AR (1974) Clinical symptoms and comor- memories during childhood and adolescence. In: Kuehnle K,
bidity in evaluation the outcome of diabetes mellitus. Journal of Connell M (eds) The evaluation of child sexual abuse allega-
Chronic Disease 27:387–404 tions: a comprehensive guide to assessment and testimony, vol
71. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS (1978) The classification of child 25. Wiley, Hoboken, pp 203–244
psychopathology: a review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psy- 84. Geraerts E (2010) Posttraumatic memory. In: Rosen GM, Frueh
chol Bull 85:1275–1301 BC (eds) Clinician’s guide to posttraumatic stress disorder, vol
72. Lilienfeld SO (2003) Comorbidity between and within childhood 14. Wiley, Hoboken, pp 77–95
externalizing and internalizing disorders: reflections and direc- 85. Lerner RM, Castellino DR (2002) Contemporary developmen-
tions. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31:285–291 tal theory and adolescence: developmental systems and applied
73. Cloitre M, Stolbach BC, Herman JL, van der Kolk B, Pynoos R, developmental science. J Adolesc Health 31:122–135
Wang J et al (2009) A developmental approach to complex PTSD:
childhood and adult cumulative trauma as predictors of symptom
complexity. J Trauma Stress 22:399–408

123

You might also like