Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Original Article Obesity

CLINICAL TRIALS AND INVESTIGATIONS

Bariatric Surgery Does Not Affect Food Preferences,


but Individual Changes in Food Preferences May Predict
Weight Loss
1,2, Simone Rasmussen1, Bodil Just Christensen3, Christian Ritz1, Carel W. le Roux 4,5,
Mette Søndergaard Nielsen
1 and Anders Sjödin1
Julie Berg Schmidt

Objective: Using an ad libitum buffet meal targeting direct behavior, the authors of the current study previ-
ously reported no effect of Roux-en-Y gastric bypass (RYGB) and sleeve gastrectomy (SG) surgery on food
preferences 6 months after surgery. The current study investigated changes in food preferences at 18
months after surgery and whether changes in food preferences at 6 months predicted weight loss.
Methods: Twenty food items separated into the following food categories were served at the buffet meal:
high-fat, low-fat, sweet, savory, high-fat savory, high-fat sweet, low-fat savory, and low-fat sweet. Energy
intake and intake from each of the food items were registered. Energy intake prior to the meal was
standardized.
Results: Thirty-nine subjects completed visits before surgery and 18 months following RYGB (n = 29) and
SG (n = 10) surgery. Energy intake decreased 41% (4,470 ± 209 kJ vs. 2,618 ± 209 kJ, P < 0.001), but no change
occurred in relative energy intake from any of the food categories (all P ≥ 0.23), energy density (P = 0.20), or
macronutrient intake (all P ≥ 0.28). However, changes in high-fat food intake, protein intake, energy intake,
and energy density at 6 months predicted weight loss at 18 months (P ≤ 0.02).
Conclusions: RYGB surgery and SG surgery do not affect food preferences. However, changes in food
preferences seem to be predictive of weight loss.
Obesity (2018) 0, 00-00. doi:10.1002/oby.22272

Introduction beyond an effect of dietary counseling or increased motivation for


weight loss (19).
Roux-en-Y gastric bypass (RYGB) and sleeve gastrectomy (SG) sur-
gery have been associated with changes in food preferences toward
The evidence from human studies relies on verbal reports, such as
less energy-dense foods (1-12). Studies generally report decreased food records, questionnaires, and interviews, which are prone to
consumption of sugary and fatty foods (1-3,5,7,10-12) and increased reporting bias and underestimation of intake of unhealthy food (20).
consumption of fruit and vegetables and protein-rich foods (3,5,7,12), To address this limitation, we previously targeted direct behavior
although conflicting results exists (13). Animal studies seems to by investigating postoperative changes in food preferences using an
replicate results from human studies (4,14-18), suggesting that the ad libitum buffet meal (13). Six months following RYGB and SG
mechanisms driving the potential changes in food preferences are surgery, total energy intake decreased 54%; however, we found no

1
Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sports, Faculty of Science (Obesity Research),  University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark.
Correspondence: Mette Søndergaard Nielsen (msn@nexs.ku.dk) 2 The Danish Diabetes Academy,  Odense University Hospital, Odense,
Denmark 3 Department of Food and Resource Economics, Faculty of Science, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark 4 Investigative
Science, Imperial College London, London, UK 5 Diabetes Complications Research Centre, Conway Institute, University College Dublin, Dublin,
Ireland.

Funding agencies: This study was carried out as part of the research program “Governing Obesity” funded by the University of Copenhagen Excellence Programme for
Interdisciplinary Research (www.go.ku.dk). Furthermore, the Danish Diabetes Academy supported by the Novo Nordisk Foundation, the Lundbeck Foundation, and the
Aase and Ejnar Danielsens Foundation funded this study.
Disclosure: CWlR was supported by grants from Science Foundation Ireland (ref 12/YI/B2480), the Health Research Board (USIRL-2016-2), and the Irish Research Council
during the conduct of the study. Furthermore, CWlR reported being on the advisory boards for Novo Nordisk and GI Dynamics, receiving a research grant from AnaBio,
and receiving honoraria for lectures and advisory work from Eli Lily, Johnson and Johnson, Sanofi Aventis, Astra Zeneca, Janssen, Bristol-Myers Squibb, and Boehringer-
Ingelheim. The other authors declare no conflict of interest.
Clinical trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT02070081.
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.
Received: 3 May 2018; Accepted: 22 June 2018. Published online 0 Month 2018. doi:10.1002/oby.22272

www.obesityjournal.org  Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018    1


Obesity Food Preferences Assessed by Buffet Meal Test  Søndergaard Nielsen et al.

changes in food preferences, macronutrient intake, or energy density preferences and for as long as they wanted. They ate unmonitored
of the selected meal (13). and unaccompanied, in order to diminish social desirability bias. The
staging and placement on the table of the 20 food items were identi-
The prevailing view is that changes in food preferences following bar- cal at each visit.
iatric surgery are due to a learned adjustment to postingestive responses
following intake of high-fat and sweet foods, commonly termed “con- The 20 food items varied along two dimensions associated with food
ditioned taste avoidance” (21). At 6 months, patients might still be in a choices, fat (high or low) and taste (sweet or savory) (23), thereby
“learning period,” in which they adjust food intake to avoid postinges- making it possible to organize the food items into separate catego-
tive responses (22). Thus, we speculate whether 6 months postsurgery ries (high-fat and low-fat or sweet and savory) or combined food cat-
was too early for behavioral changes to set in. egories (high-fat savory, low-fat savory, high-fat sweet, and low-fat
sweet) (13). Total energy intake, intake from each of the food cate-
The aim of this study was to investigate food preferences using an gories, and the duration of the meal were registered. The participants
ad libitum buffet meal targeting direct behavior 18 months following were unaware of this registration. In order to divert the attention of
RYGB and SG surgery. We hypothesized that energy intake at 18 the participants away from making food choices to please the investi-
months after surgery was increased compared with 6 months after gators, a blood sample was drawn after the meal, and the participants
surgery, thereby reaching a threshold at which enough postinges- were informed that the aim was to investigate how “eating as much as
tive responses could be generated, resulting in a relative reduction they wished of the food they liked best” affected gut hormone levels
in intake of high-fat and sweet foods and a corresponding rela- in the blood.
tive increase in intake of low-fat and savory foods. Furthermore,
because of the large variation we observed in food preferences at Picture display test.  Participants were presented with standardized
6 months, we investigated whether changes in food preferences for pictures of the following 20 food items: pork rib roast, turkey strips,
high-fat and sweet foods, macronutrient intake, energy density, and nuggets, Danish omelet, French fries, crispbread, salty crackers, skyr
total energy intake at 6 months predicted weight loss outcomes 18 with berries, carrots, cut fruit, cheese, smoked fillet (cold cuts), vanilla
months after surgery. ice cream, cocoa meringues, sweet licorice, Danish pastries, milk choc-
olate, pound cake, cookies, and wine gum. Pictures were displayed in
a randomized order, and the participants were instructed to choose the
food item they would most like to eat.
Methods
The study population, design, and methods have been described Previous experience of unpleasant postingestive responses. Previous
previously (13). Briefly, food preferences were assessed approxi- experience of unpleasant postingestive responses was assessed by a sem-
mately 3 months before surgery (prior to a mandatory 8% presur- istructured qualitative interview 6 weeks, 6 months and 18 months after
gical weight loss), as well as 6 months and 18 months following surgery. Data from the interviews were quantified into a 4-point score
RYGB and SG surgery. Data from the visits before surgery and 6 (0-3), with 0 indicating no experience of discomfort related to eating after
months after surgery have been reported previously (13). At each surgery, while 1, 2 and 3 indicate low, medium and high levels of discom-
visit, anthropometric data were collected in the fasted state at 9:00 fort, respectively.
am. At 3:30 pm, a picture display test was carried out, and at 4:30
pm, an ad libitum buffet meal was served. Energy intake prior to Premeal hunger.  Premeal hunger was assessed by a visual analog
the test meal was standardized (a 954 kJ meal and a 1,675 kJ liquid scale (100 mm) approximately 15 minutes prior to the buffet meal.
meal were served at 10:00 am and 1:15 pm, respectively; Cambridge
Weight Plan). The liquid meal came in five different flavors: choc-
olate, chocolate mint, strawberry, wild berry, and caramel and Anthropometric measures.  Body weight and height were measured
walnut. after an overnight fast. Percent total weight loss (%TWL) was defined
as the presurgical weight minus the weight at 18 months after surgery,
divided by the presurgical weight, and multiplied by 100.
The study was approved by the Scientific Ethic Committees of
the Capital Region of Denmark (Journal number H-3-2013-138)
and registered in the database www.ClinicalTrials.gov (identi- Statistical analysis
fier NCT02070081). All study participants gave written informed
Descriptive data summaries are presented as mean ± SD. Premeal hun-
consent.
ger, total energy intake, energy density, eating time, eating rate, and
weight changes were analyzed using linear mixed models with visit as
Outcomes fixed effect. Intake from the buffet meal was analyzed using a two-stage
Buffet meal test.  The following 20 food items were served at the approach to accommodate for semicontinuous data (consisting of zero
buffet meal: pork rib roast, chicken fillet strips, fish cakes, nuggets, intake and positive intake; (24)): First, a logistic mixed-effects model
Danish omelet, French fries, creamy potato gratin, bread (rye bread, was fitted to the binary data indicating if intake was 0 or not. Second, a
baguette, and crispbread), ketchup, remoulade, mayonnaise, skyr linear mixed model was fitted to the square root–transformed positive
with berries, cut raw vegetables, cut fruit, vanilla ice cream, choc- intake values only. Both models included the food category-visit in-
olate sauce, cocoa meringues, biscuit cones with chocolate, sweet teraction as fixed effect. Estimates from these two analyses were then
licorice, and Danish pastries. The only liquid served at the meal combined and back-transformed to obtain unconditional estimates of
was water. The participants were instructed to eat according to their mean intakes per category and visit and differences in mean intakes

2    Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018 www.obesityjournal.org


Original Article Obesity
CLINICAL TRIALS AND INVESTIGATIONS

R Foundation, Vienna, Austria) and RStudio version 1.1.383 (www.


TABLE 1 Characteristics of the subjects (n = 39)
rstudio.com).
Male/female 6/33
Age (y) 40.3 ± 9.2
Body weight (kg) 130.5 ± 22.0 Results
BMI (kg/m2) 45.0 ± 6.8
RYGB/SG surgery 29/10
Participants
Sixty-one subjects were originally recruited and 41 completed the visit
Patients with type 2 diabetes 10 at 6 months post surgery. Results were reported previously (13). Prior
Time from baseline to surgery (d) 92 ± 37 to the 18 months follow-up visit at, two subjects were excluded due to
Data are shown as mean ± SD or frequency.
pregnancy, leaving a total sample of 39 subjects with complete data
SG, sleeve gastrectomy; RYGB: Roux-en-Y gastric bypass. from all three visits. Twenty-nine subjects underwent RYGB surgery,
and ten subjects underwent SG surgery. Baseline characteristics are
summarized in Table 1.
between combinations of category and visit. Differences between type
of surgery and between the groups with highest and lowest energy
intake were analyzed using linear mixed models with a visit-group
Weight loss
interaction or a food category-visit-group interaction (intake from the Eighteen months after surgery, %TWL was 31% (40.9 ± 2.2 kg,
buffet meal) as fixed effect. Binary outcomes from the picture display P < 0.001) (Supporting Information Figure S1). Weight loss was larger
test were analyzed using logistic mixed-effects models (one model per in RYGB compared with SG patients (44.8 ± 3.4 kg vs. 30.0 ± 4.4 kg,
category) with visit as fixed effect. All models were adjusted for age P = 0.01) and in subjects without diabetes compared with subjects with
and gender and included subject-specific random effects. Associations diabetes (45.7 ± 3.1 kg vs. 26.4 ± 5.8 kg, P = 0.01).
between changes in relative intake at the buffet meal at 18 months and
previous experience of postingestive responses were evaluated using
simple linear regression adjusted for age and gender. The same model Buffet meal test and premeal hunger
was used to evaluate associations between changes in relative intake From before surgery to 18 months after surgery, total energy intake
at the buffet meal at 6 months and 18 months and weight loss out- at the buffet meal decreased 41% (4,470 ± 209 kJ vs. 2,618 ± 209 kJ,
come at 18 months after surgery. Results are reported as mean ± SE P < 0.001) and eating rate decreased 18% (242 ± 13 kJ/min vs. 198 ± 13
(intake as mean percentages). P values < 0.05 were considered signif- kJ/min, P = 0.01). No changes in hunger or energy density of the meal
icant. Statistical analyses were carried out using R (version 3.3.2; The were observed (Table 2).

TABLE 2 Intake, eating time, and eating rate at buffet meal (n = 39)

Postsurgery, Postsurgery, P value, P value,


Presurgery 6 mo 18 mo 6 mo 18 mo
Premeal hunger (cm) 5.7 ± 0.5 5.0 ± 0.5 5.5 ± 0.5 0.20 0.68
Total energy intake (kJ) 4,470 ± 209 2,096 ± 209 2,618 ± 209 < 0.001 < 0.001
Energy density (kJ/g) 7.2 ± 0.3 7.3 ± 0.3 7.5 ± 0.3 0.72 0.20
Eating time (min) 19.5 ± 1.1 13.5 ± 1.1 14.9 ± 1.1 < 0.001 < 0.001
Eating rate (kJ/min) 279 ± 16 204 ± 16 236 ± 16 < 0.001 0.01
Intake from combined categories
High-fat savory (%) 48 ± 2 48 ± 4 51 ± 3 0.95 0.53
High-fat sweet (%) 7 ± 1 7 ± 1 7 ± 1 0.91 0.73
Low-fat savory (%) 40 ± 2 37 ± 3 36 ± 3 0.50 0.31
Low-fat sweet (%) 5 ± 1 8 ± 2 6 ± 1 0.16 0.62
Intake from separate categories
High-fat (%) 55 ± 3 56 ± 4 61 ± 4 0.88 0.23
Low-fat (%) 45 ± 3 44 ± 4 39 ± 4
Sweet (%) 20 ± 2 21 ± 3 19 ± 3 0.71 0.89
Savory (%) 80 ± 2 79 ± 3 81 ± 3
Intake macronutrients
E% carbohydrate 35 ± 1 34 ± 2 36 ± 2 0.59 0.75
E% fat 44 ± 2 45 ± 2 46 ± 2 0.70 0.59
E% protein 20 ± 1 21 ± 2 18 ± 2 0.88 0.28
Data are shown as mean ± SE. Changes compared with presurgery were found using linear mixed models adjusted for age and gender and with subject as random effect.
E%, energy percentage.

www.obesityjournal.org  Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018    3


Obesity

TABLE 3 Intake at buffet meal stratified by type of surgery


RYGB patients (n = 29) SG patients (n = 10) Overall
P value,
Postsurgery, Postsurgery, P value, P value, Postsurgery, Postsurgery, P value, P value, between
Presurgery 6 mo 18 mo 6 mo 18 mo Presurgery 6 mo 18 mo 6 moa 18 moa groups
Premeal hunger 5.6 ± 0.4 5.5 ± 0.4 5.8 ± 0.4 0.98 0.60 6.5 ± 1.6 4.0 ± 1.6 5.0 ± 1.6 < 0.001 < 0.01 0.12
(cm)

4    Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018


Total energy intake 4,718 ± 243 2,252 ± 243 2,782 ± 244 < 0.001 < 0.001 3,754 ± 400 1,643 ± 399 2,140 ± 401 < 0.001 < 0.001 0.16
(kJ)
Energy density 7.6 ± 0.4 7.6 ± 0.4 8.1 ± 0.4 1.00 0.06 6.4 ± 1.3 6.8 ± 1.3 6.5 ± 1.2 0.56 0.97 0.52
(kJ/g)
Eating time (min) 18.9 ± 1.4 14.3 ± 1.4 16.2 ± 1.4 < 0.001 < 0.001 19.2 ± 2.7 9.4 ± 2.7 9.2 ± 2.6 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.01
Eating rate (kJ/min) 299 ± 16 203 ± 16 238 ± 16 < 0.001 < 0.001 212 ± 34 193 ± 33 224 ± 32 0.18 0.52 0.048
Intake combined categories
High-fat savory (%) 50 ± 3 51 ± 4 52 ± 4 0.79 0.71 43 ± 5 37 ± 7 47 ± 6 0.45 0.59 0.44
High-fat sweet (%) 6 ± 1 6 ± 2 5 ± 1 0.91 0.64 8 ± 2 10 ± 4 14 ± 4 0.78 0.20
Low-fat savory (%) 40 ± 3 34 ± 4 36 ± 4 0.25 0.40 42 ± 5 49 ± 7 36 ± 6 0.41 0.46
Low-fat sweet (%) 5 ± 1 9 ± 2 7 ± 2 0.83 0.20 6 ± 2 4 ± 3 2 ± 2 0.55 0.09
Intake separate categories
High-fat (%) 56 ± 3 59 ± 5 63 ± 4 0.66 0.19 52 ± 6 52 ± 9 64 ± 7 0.99 0.21 0.54
Low-fat (%) 44 ± 3 41 ± 5 37 ± 4 48 ± 6 48 ± 9 36 ± 7
Sweet (%) 19 ± 3 21 ± 4 21 ± 3 0.57 0.63 22 ± 4 19 ± 6 16 ± 4 0.69 0.26 0.86
Savory (%) 81 ± 3 79 ± 4 79 ± 3 78 ± 4 81 ± 6 84 ± 4
Intake macronutrients
E% carbohydrate 34 ± 2 34 ± 2 36 ± 2 0.95 0.36 40 ± 3 34 ± 5 35 ± 4 0.26 0.37 0.20
E% fat 46 ± 2 45 ± 3 45 ± 2 0.90 0.87 40 ± 3 46 ± 5 47 ± 5 0.35 0.20
E% protein 20 ± 1 21 ± 2 18 ± 2 0.94 0.36 20 ± 3 21 ± 4 17 ± 4 0.85 0.59
Data are shown as mean ± SE.
a
Changes compared with presurgery were found using linear mixed models adjusted for age and gender and with subject as random effect.
E%, energy percentage; RYGB, Roux-en-Y gastric bypass; SG, sleeve gastrectomy.

www.obesityjournal.org
Food Preferences Assessed by Buffet Meal Test  Søndergaard Nielsen et al.
TABLE 4 Body weight and intake, eating time, and eating rate at buffet meal in subjects with high energy intake and low energy intake at buffet meal 18 mo after
surgery
Low energy intake group (n = 13) High energy intake group (n = 13)

Postsurgery, Postsurgery, Overall P value,


Presurgery 18 mo P value Presurgery 18 mo P value between groupsa
Original Article

Female/male 12/1 - - 9/4 - - -

www.obesityjournal.org 
RYGB/SG surgery 7/6 - - 11/2 - - -
Body weight (kg) 124.0 ± 4.1 83.8 ± 4.1 < 0.001 135.9 ± 5.6 96.1 ± 5.6 < 0.001  > 0.99
Premeal hunger (cm) 5.5 ± 0.8 4.6 ± 0.8 0.43 5.6 ± 0.8 6.0 ± 0.8 0.62 0.32
Total energy intake (kJ) 3,463 ± 292 1,295 ± 292 < 0.001 5,513 ± 407 3,931 ± 407 < 0.01 0.37
CLINICAL TRIALS AND INVESTIGATIONS

Energy intake (kJ/kg body 31.5 ± 4.1 19.2 ± 4.0 < 0.001 42.1 ± 4.0 43.6 ± 4.0 0.79 0.02
weight)
Energy density (kJ/g) 6.6 ± 0.4 6.0 ± 0.4 0.16 7.6 ± 0.5 9.0 ± 0.5 < 0.01 < 0.01
Eating time (min) 19.9 ± 1.8 9.7 ± 1.8 < 0.001 19.7 ± 2.0 19.2 ± 2.0 0.79 < 0.01
Eating rate (kJ/min) 186 ± 17 150 ± 17 0.06 299 ± 33 238 ± 33 0.18 0.61
Intake from combined categories
High-fat savory (%) 41 ± 4 27 ± 6 0.048 48 ± 4 57 ± 5 0.20 < 0.01
High-fat sweet (%) 4 ± 1 2 ± 1 0.20 9 ± 2 16 ± 3 0.05
Low-fat savory (%) 48 ± 4 65 ± 6 0.02 40 ± 4 22 ± 4 < 0.001
Low-fat sweet (%) 7 ± 2 6 ± 3 0.75 3 ± 1 6 ± 2 0.21
Intake from separate categories
High-fat (%) 46 ± 5 33 ± 8 0.16 59 ± 5 73 ± 5 0.048 0.03
Low-fat (%) 54 ± 5 67 ± 8 41 ± 5 27 ± 5
Sweet (%) 17 ± 3 13 ± 4 0.40 15 ± 3 24 ± 4 0.10 0.39
Savory (%) 83 ± 3 87 ± 4 85 ± 3 76 ± 4
Intake macronutrients
E% carbohydrate 37 ± 3 42 ± 5 0.39 35 ± 3 36 ± 3 0.81 < 0.001
E% fat 42 ± 3 38 ± 5 0.52 45 ± 3 48 ± 3 0.47
E% protein 22 ± 3 20 ± 4 0.78 20 ± 2 16 ± 2 0.21
Data are shown as mean ± SE.
aChanges compared with presurgery were compared using linear mixed models adjusted for age and gender and with subject as random effect.

E%, energy percentage; RYGB, Roux-en-Y gastric bypass; SG, sleeve gastrectomy.

Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018    5


Obesity
Obesity Food Preferences Assessed by Buffet Meal Test  Søndergaard Nielsen et al.

TABLE 5 Mean frequency of food choices at picture display test from each combined food category

Postsurgery, Postsurgery, P value, P value,


Presurgery 6 mo 18 mo 6 mo 18 mo
High-fat savory (%) 31 ± 10 21 ± 9 31 ± 10 0.39 1.00
High-fat sweet (%) 7 ± 4 0 ± 0 0 ± 0 1.00 1.00
Low-fat savory (%) 36 ± 8 63 ± 9 51 ± 9 0.04 0.23
Low-fat sweet (%) 12 ± 7 6 ± 5 8 ± 5 0.30 0.50
Data are shown as mean ± SE. Changes compared with presurgery were found using linear mixed models adjusted for age and gender and with subject as random effect.

Figure 1 Associations between percent total weight loss (%TWL) at 18 mo after surgery and changes in (A) energy intake from before surgery to 6 mo after surgery, (B)
energy density from before surgery to 6 mo after surgery, (C) relative intake of protein from before surgery to 6 mo after surgery, (D) relative intake of high-fat food from
before surgery to 6 mo after surgery, and (E) relative intake of high-fat food from before surgery to 18 mo after surgery. Associations were evaluated using simple linear
regressions adjusted for age and gender.

6    Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018 www.obesityjournal.org


Original Article Obesity
CLINICAL TRIALS AND INVESTIGATIONS

No postoperative changes were observed in relative energy intake for changes between presurgery and 18 months postsurgery for
from the combined food categories (high-fat savory, high-fat sweet, total energy intake (P = 0.09, R 2 = 0.18) and energy density (P = 0.07.
low-fat savory, and low-fat sweet), the separate food categories (high- R 2 = 0.20). No associations were found between intake of sweet foods
fat versus low-fat and sweet versus savory), the different macronutri- and %TWL at 18 months after surgery (P ≥ 0.35) or between intake
ents (Table 2), or from any of the 20 food items served at the buffet from the remaining macronutrients and %TWL at 18 months after
(Supporting Information Table S1). surgery (all P ≥ 0.19).

Experiences of unpleasant postingestive responses assessed at 6


weeks, 6 months, and 18 months after surgery were not associated
with changes in relative intake of high-fat or sweet foods at the buffet
Discussion
meal (all P ≥ 0.07). All of the 39 subjects (100%) reported having one We confirmed our previous findings, that RYGB surgery and SG sur-
or more experiences with unpleasant physiological responses to food gery do not affect food preferences when assessed by an ad libitum
after surgery. buffet meal targeting direct behavior. Total energy intake at the meal
decreased after surgery, but there were no changes in food preferences.
Thus, the reduction in calories was simply caused by eating smaller
Similar decreases in energy intake were seen in RYGB and SG patients
portions of the same food items. However, changes in energy intake
(41% vs. 43%, respectively; RYGB: 4,718 ± 243 kJ vs. 2,782 ± 244
and food preferences seem to be predictive of the postoperative weight
kJ, P < 0.001 and SG: 3,754 ± 400 kJ vs. 2,140 ± 401 kJ, P < 0.001,
loss at 18 months.
P = 0.16). Furthermore, the stratified analysis showed no changes in
relative energy intake from the combined and separate food categories
and macronutrients in RYGB (all P ≥ 0.19) or SG patients (all P ≥ 0.09), This is the first study to investigate changes in food preferences fol-
and there was no differences between surgical groups (all P ≥ 0.20) lowing bariatric surgery using an ad libitum buffet meal test. Our
(Table 3). However, we found a trend for a reduced relative intake results contradict those of previous studies using verbal report mea-
of low-fat sweet foods in SG patients (6 ± 2% vs. 2 ± 2%, P = 0.09). sures, emphasizing the need for further investigation of food pref-
Furthermore, relative energy intake from the combined and separate erences targeting direct behavior. Mechanistic studies investigating
food categories did not differ between patients with or without diabetes the effect of surgery on taste sensitivity report decreased (25-28) or
(all P ≥ 0.44). unchanged (29,30) sucrose detection following surgery. Furthermore,
appetitive behavior for sweet and fatty food also seems to be reduced
following surgery (31), and the mechanisms have further been elu-
In a post hoc analysis separating data into the highest (n = 13) and cidated by functional neuroimaging studies showing a reduced
lowest (n = 13) tertile of total energy consumed at the buffet meal reward-related response to pictures of palatable foods (32-34). These
18 months after surgery, we found a difference in relative intake changes are all consistent with studies reporting changes in food
from the combined categories (P < 0.01), the separate categories preferences following bariatric surgery. However, it is uncertain if
(P = 0.03), and macronutrients (P < 0.001) across tertiles. In the sub- changes in taste sensitivity and food reward translate to a modifica-
group of patients with the lowest energy intake at 18 months, the tion of food selection and preferences; thus, implications for actual
relative intake from the high-fat savory category decreased, whereas food consumption behavior are not possible to infer from these stud-
relative intake from the low-fat savory category increased (P = 0.048 ies. Animal models, however, allow for assessment of more direct
and P = 0.02, respectively). In contrast, in the subgroup with the behavior, using the two-bottle preference test and a cafeteria diet
highest energy intake at 18 months, the relative intake from the low- paradigm. Using animal models, it is possible to examine changes in
fat savory category decreased after surgery (P < 0.001). Furthermore, food preferences in strictly controlled experiments, while excluding
pre- to postoperative changes in eating time, energy density of the the potential impact of social stigma associated with unhealthy eat-
meal, and total energy consumed per kilogram body weight dif- ing behavior seen in humans. Interestingly, animal studies replicate
fered between groups (P < 0.01, P < 0.01, and P = 0.02, respectively) previous findings in humans, reporting a decrease in preferences for
(Table 4). high-fat and sweet foods following surgery (4,14-18). These results,
however, are not in line with the findings in present study target-
ing direct behaviors of food intake in humans. The limited exposure
Picture display test of rats to different diets compared with the large range of different
The subjects’ choice of picture from the combined groups, high- foods humans are exposed to throughout life could be a potential
fat savory, high-fat sweet, low-fat savory and low-fat sweet, did not explanation for these discrepancies. Furthermore, rodent models
change from before surgery to 18 months after surgery (all P ≥ 0.23) have much less biological variations than human models, increasing
(Table 5). the likelihood of detecting differences in food preferences on the
group level.

Prediction of weight loss outcomes We hypothesized that 18 months after surgery, energy intake at
Changes between presurgery and 6 months postsurgery for total the buffet meal would increase compared with 6 months after sur-
energy intake (P = 0.02, R 2  = 0.24), energy density (P < 0.001, gery, thereby potentially reaching a threshold at which postinges-
R 2 = 0.39), protein intake (P = 0.01, R 2 = 0.26), and intake of high-fat tive responses would be sufficient to lead to a substantial shift in
foods (P = 0.01, R 2 = 0.26) predicted %TWL at 18 months (Figure 1). food preferences for most patients. This hypothesis was based on
Furthermore, changes between presurgery and 18 months postsur- the view that the effect of RYGB and SG surgery on food prefer-
gery for intake of high-fat foods (P = 0.01, R 2 = 0.26) were associated ences is caused by a learned adjustment to postprandial visceral sig-
with %TWL at 18 months (Figure 1). A tendency was also found nals rather than by a change in experienced palatability (35). This

www.obesityjournal.org  Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018    7


Obesity Food Preferences Assessed by Buffet Meal Test  Søndergaard Nielsen et al.

learned adjustment was hypothesized to be caused by conditioned decrease in intake from before surgery to after surgery was compa-
taste avoidance in which the triggering foods can still be consumed, rable between RYGB and SG patients (41% and 43% respectively).
as long as the patients learn to reduce consumption to avoid certain Studies comparing the effect of RYGB and SG surgery on food
postingestive signals or responses (36). Thus, one possibility was preferences are scarce. In a pilot study, El Labban et al. did not
that energy intake at 6 months was too low to trigger these postpran- detect any major differences in food intake and preferences between
dial visceral signals and, consequently, a shift in food chosen at the RYGB and SG operated patients (37). Nance et al. compared pre-
buffet meal or that learned adjustment to conditioned taste avoidance to postoperative changes in eating behavior and detected similar
progresses over time so that 6 months postsurgery was too early for changes after RYGB and SG surgery (30). Thus, in spite of different
behavioral changes to set in. However, we were not able to confirm anatomic changes following RYGB and SG surgery, these proce-
our hypothesis, even though energy intake increased 25% between 6 dures seem to have similar outcomes on dietary intake and food
and 18 months after surgery. preferences.

To fully explore our hypothesis, we divided the subjects into tertiles When measuring food preferences by means of verbal report, we
according to total energy consumed after surgery. Food preferences found no change in subjects’ choice of picture from the different
changed in both the low and high energy intake group in opposite direc- food categories between presurgery and 18 months postsurgery.
tions. However, these changes were contrary to our hypotheses. In the However, at the assessment 6 months after surgery, patients reported
high energy intake group, patients consumed more food from the high- an increased preference for low-fat savory foods compared with
fat category; hence, this group may either have had a higher threshold before surgery, which is in line with the dietary advice received as
for triggering postprandial visceral signals or a less profound biological part of the bariatric surgical program. These results could indicate
response to these visceral signals compared with the group with lower that patients are more aware of the nutritional recommendations in
energy intake. the early postoperative period, whereas this awareness seems to fade
with time after surgery.
Thus, we can preclude that our previous findings 6 months after sur-
gery were caused by a low energy intake or an early assessment of Assessing food preferences by targeting direct behavior is a major
food preferences occurring before adaptation to conditioned taste strength of this study. However, the experimental setting of the meal
avoidance. has some limitations, which have previously been described (13).
Briefly, we cannot preclude that the social desirability of healthy
We found no associations between postoperative experiences of nega- eating might influence the choice of food at the buffet meal, and
tive responses related to eating and food preferences at the buffet meal. whether the assessed behavior at the buffet meal test reflects actual
This result is not caused by a low incidence of subjects reporting post- behavior outside the experimental setting can be questioned. The
prandial discomfort since all patients reported one or more experiences standardized liquid meals consumed prior to the buffet meal were
with unpleasant physiological responses to food after surgery. As early predominantly “sweet meals” (e.g., chocolate and chocolate mint
as 6 weeks after surgery, 95% of the subjects reported postprandial flavor). This could have reduced intake of sweet foods at the buf-
discomfort, indicating that patients, despite receiving nutritional coun- fet meal because of reduced liking for sweetness caused by sen-
seling as part of the bariatric surgery program, encountered food that sory-specific satiety (38). Such an effect would be present at all
caused visceral malaise. visits, and also before surgery, potentially limiting the ability to
detect a decrease in preference for sweet foods following surgery.
Additionally, the low number of SG participants (n = 10) limited the
Even though we found no consistent changes in food preferences
ability to make specific conclusions regarding the effect of SG sur-
for the entire cohort of patients, changes in high fat intake, protein
gery on food preferences.
intake, total energy intake, and energy density seem to be predic-
tive of the postoperative weight loss. The decrease in energy intake,
energy density, and intake of high-fat foods and the increase in pro-
tein intake from before surgery to 6 months after surgery predicted Conclusion
the extent of %TWL at 18 months. The association between changes Data from this 18-month follow-up study confirmed our previous
in high-fat food and %TWL at 18 months after surgery was still sig- findings that RYGB surgery and SG surgery do not consistently af-
nificant when analyzing changes in intake from before surgery to fect food preferences in humans. Energy intake was still consider-
18 months after surgery. These results suggest that changes in food ably decreased; however, we found no overall changes in relative
preferences, especially in the early postoperative period, could be intake of high-fat, low-fat, sweet, and savory foods; macronutrient
important markers for weight loss success following surgery because intake; or energy density of the meal. These results conflict with
they explained up to 39% of the variability in later weight loss. With previous results based on verbal report measures, emphasizing the
the potential of identifying patients in need of additional support to need for further studies investigating changes in food preferences by
improve their weight loss success, this association should be further targeting direct behavior. This is especially important if changes in
investigated. energy intake and food preferences can predict postoperative weight
loss, as our data indicate, because this will strengthen identification
In spite of a larger weight loss in RYGB compared with SG patients, of subjects in need of additional early support to optimize weight
we found no changes in food preferences when stratifying subjects loss.O
according to surgery or differences between surgical groups. Energy
consumed at the meal was higher in RYGB patients; however, the © 2018 The Obesity Society

8    Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018 www.obesityjournal.org


Original Article Obesity
CLINICAL TRIALS AND INVESTIGATIONS

References 20. Poppitt SD, Swann D, Black AE, Prentice AM. Assessment of selective under-re-
porting of food intake by both obese and non-obese women in a metabolic facility.
1. Miller GD, Norris A, Fernandez A. Changes in nutrients and food groups in- Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 1998;22:303-311.
take following laparoscopic Roux-en-Y gastric bypass (RYGB). Obes Surg 21. Kapoor N, Al-Najim W, le Roux CW, Docherty NG. Shifts in food preferences after
2014;24:1926-1932. bariatric surgery: observational reports and proposed mechanisms. Curr Obes Rep
2. Laurenius A, Larsson I, Melanson KJ, et al. Decreased energy density and 2017;6:246-252.
changes in food selection following Roux-en-Y gastric bypass. Eur J Clin Nutr 22. Mathes CM, Spector AC. Food selection and taste changes in humans after
2013;67:168-173. Roux-en-Y gastric bypass surgery: a direct-measures approach. Physiol Behav
3. Olbers T, Björkman S, Lindroos A, et al. Body composition, dietary intake, and en- 2012;107:476-483.
ergy expenditure after laparoscopic Roux-en-Y gastric bypass and laparoscopic ver- 23. Finlayson G, King N, Blundell JE. Is it possible to dissociate “liking” and “want-
tical banded gastroplasty: a randomized clinical trial. Ann Surg 2006;244:715-722. ing” for foods in humans? A novel experimental procedure. Physiol Behav
4. le Roux CW, Bueter M, Theis N, et al. Gastric bypass reduces fat intake and prefer- 2007;90:36-42.
ence. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol 2011;301:R1057-R1066. 24. Smith VA, Preisser JS, Neelon B, Maciejewski ML. A marginalized two-part model
5. Ullrich J, Ernst B, Wilms B, Thurnheer M, Schultes B. Roux-en Y gastric bypass for semicontinuous data. Stat Med 2014;33:4891-4903.
surgery reduces hedonic hunger and improves dietary habits in severely obese sub- 25. Bueter M, Miras AD, Chichger H, et al. Alterations of sucrose preference after
jects. Obes Surg 2013;23:50-55. Roux-en-Y gastric bypass. Physiol Behav 2011;104:709-721.
6. Ammon BS, Bellanger DE, Geiselman PJ, Primeaux SD, Yu Y, Greenway FL. 26. Altun H, Hanci D, Altun H, et al. Improved gustatory sensitivity in morbidly
Short-term pilot study of the effect of sleeve gastrectomy on food preference. Obes obese patients after laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol
Surg 2015;25:1094-1097. 2016;125:536-540.
7. Ernst B, Thurnheer M, Wilms B, Schultes B. Differential changes in dietary habits 27. Burge JC, Schaumburg JZ, Choban PS, DiSilvestro RA, Flancbaum L. Changes in
after gastric bypass versus gastric banding operations. Obes Surg 2009;19:274-280. patients’ taste acuity after Roux-en-Y gastric bypass for clinically severe obesity. J
8. Coluzzi I, Raparelli L, Guarnacci L, et al. Food intake and changes in eating behav- Am Diet Assoc 1995;95:666-670.
ior after laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy. Obes Surg 2016;26:2059-2067. 28. Scruggs DM, Buffington C, Cowan GSM. Taste acuity of the morbidly obese before
9. Molin Netto BD, Earthman CP, Farias G, et al. Eating patterns and food choice and after gastric bypass surgery. Obes Surg 1994;4:24-28.
as determinant of weight loss and improvement of metabolic profile after RYGB. 29. Pepino MY, Bradley D, Eagon JC, Sullivan S, Abumrad NA, Klein S. Changes in
Nutrition 2017;33:125-131. taste perception and eating behavior after bariatric surgery-induced weight loss in
10. Halmi KA, Mason E, Falk JR, Stunkard A. Appetitive behavior after gastric bypass women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2014;22:E13-E20.
for obesity. Int J Obes 1981;5:457-464. 30. Nance K, Eagon J, Klein S, Pepino M Effects of sleeve gastrectomy vs. Roux-en-Y
11. Coughlin K, Bell RM, Bivins BA, Wrobel S, Griffen WO. Preoperative and postop- gastric bypass on eating behavior and sweet taste perception in subjects with obe-
erative assessment of nutrient intakes in patients who have undergone gastric bypass sity. Nutrients 2017;10. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10010018
surgery. Arch Surg 1983;118:813-816. 31. Miras AD, Jackson RN, Jackson SN, et al. Gastric bypass surgery for obesity de-
12. Kenler HA, Brolin RE, Cody RP. Changes in eating behavior after horizontal gas- creases the reward value of a sweet-fat stimulus as assessed in a progressive ratio
troplasty and Roux-en-Y gastric bypass. Am J Clin Nutr 1990;52:87-92. task. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;96:467-473.
13. Nielsen MS, Christensen BJ, Ritz C, et al. Roux-En-Y gastric bypass and sleeve 32. Scholtz S, Miras AD, Chhina N, et al. Obese patients after gastric bypass sur-
gastrectomy does not affect food preferences when assessed by an ad libitum buffet gery have lower brain-hedonic responses to food than after gastric banding. Gut
meal. Obes Surg 2017;27:2599-2605. 2014;63:891-902.
14. Shin AC, Zheng H, Pistell PJ, Berthoud HR. Roux-en-Y gastric bypass surgery 33. Ochner CN, Stice E, Hutchins E, et al. Relation between changes in neural respon-
changes food reward in rats. Int J Obes (Lond) 2011;35:642-651. sivity and reductions in desire to eat high-calorie foods following gastric bypass
15. Mathes CM, Letourneau C, Blonde GD, le Roux CW, Spector AC. Roux- surgery. Neuroscience 2012;209:128-135.
en-Y gastric bypass in rats progressively decreases the proportion of fat calo- 34. Ochner CN, Kwok Y, Conceição E, et al. Selective reduction in neural responses to
ries selected from a palatable cafeteria diet. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol high calorie foods following gastric bypass surgery. Ann Surg 2011;253:502-507.
2016;310:R952-R959. 35. Gero D, Steinert RE, le Roux CW, Bueter M. Do food preferences change after
16. Mathes CM, Bohnenkamp RA, Blonde GD, et al. Gastric bypass in rats does not de- bariatric surgery? Curr Atheroscler Rep 2017;19:38.
crease appetitive behavior towards sweet or fatty fluids despite blunting preferential 36. Abdeen G, le Roux CW. Mechanism underlying the weight loss and complications
intake of sugar and fat. Physiol Behav 2015;142:179-188. of Roux-en-Y gastric bypass. Review. Obes Surg 2016;26:410-421.
17. Chambers AP, Wilson-Perez HE, McGrath S, et al. Effect of vertical sleeve 37. El Labban S, Safadi B, Olabi A. The effect of Roux-en-Y gastric bypass and sleeve
gastrectomy on food selection and satiation in rats. Am J Physiol Metab gastrectomy surgery on dietary intake, food preferences, and gastrointestinal symp-
2012;303:E1076-E1084. toms in postsurgical morbidly obese Lebanese subjects: a cross-sectional pilot
18. Wilson-Perez HE, Chambers AP, Sandoval DA, et al. The effect of vertical sleeve study. Obes Surg 2015;25:2393-2399.
gastrectomy on food choice in rats. Int J Obes (Lond) 2013;37:288-295. 38. He W, Boesveldt S, Delplanque S, de Graaf C, de Wijk RA. Sensory-specific sati-
19. Manning S, Pucci A, Batterham RL. Roux-en-Y gastric bypass: effects on feeding ety: Added insights from autonomic nervous system responses and facial expres-
behavior and underlying mechanisms. J Clin Invest 2015;125:939-948. sions. Physiol Behav 2017;170:12-18.

www.obesityjournal.org  Obesity | VOLUME 00 | NUMBER 00 | MONTH 2018    9

You might also like