Berryman 1992

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

The Orgins and Evolution of Predator-Prey Theory

Author(s): Alan A. Berryman


Reviewed work(s):
Source: Ecology, Vol. 73, No. 5 (Oct., 1992), pp. 1530-1535
Published by: Ecological Society of America
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1940005 .
Accessed: 26/12/2012 15:50

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Ecological Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Ecology.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Ecology, 73(5). 1992. pp. 1530-1535
? 1992 by the Ecological Society of America

THE ORIGINS AND EVOLUTION OF


PREDATOR-PREY THEORY'

ALAN A. BERRYMAN
Departmentsof Entomologyand Natural Resource Sciences,
WashingtonState University,
Pullman, Washington99164 USA

Abstract. Predator-preytheoryis traced fromits originsin the Malthus-Verhulstlo-


gisticequation, throughthe Lotka-Volterraequations, logisticmodificationsto both prey
and predatorequations,incorporationoftheMichaelis-Menten-Hollingfunctionalresponse
into the predator and prey equations, and the recent development of ratio-dependent
functionalresponses and per-capita rate of change functions.Some of the problems of
classical predator-preytheory,includingthe paradoxes of enrichmentand biological con-
trol,seem to have been caused by theapplicationoftheprincipleofmass action to predator-
prey interactions.Predator-preymodels that evolved fromlogistictheoryor that incor-
porate ratio-dependentfunctionalresponsesdo not have these problemsand also seem to
be more biologicallyplausible.
Key words: logistic;Lotka- Volterra,predator-preyinteractions;ratio dependence.

INTRODUCTION burg 1989, Arditi and Berryman1991). In this paper


The dynamical relationshipbetween predatorsand I brieflysketchthe originand evolution of population
theirprey is one of the dominant themes in ecology. theoryand, in particular,predator-preymodels. My
Yet the theoryof predator-preyinteractionshas some objectivesare twofold:First,to show how conventional
notableproblems:One is the"paradox ofenrichment," predator-preymodels deviated significantly fromear-
where classical models predictthat enrichingthe sys- lier theory,and how this may have led to some of their
tem will cause an increase in the equilibrium density problems.Second, to show how ratio-dependentpred-
of the predatorbut not in that of the prey (Hairston, ator-preytheoryfollowslogicallyfrombasic concepts
Smith, and Slobodkin 1960), and will destabilize the of single-speciespopulation dynamics, and how this
community equilibrium (Rosenzweig 1969). These viewpointsolves many of the problemsand paradoxes
predictions,however,are not always in line with field of traditionalpredator-preytheory.
observations(Arditi and Ginzburg 1989, Arditi et al.
1991, Ginzburg and Akqakaya 1992). Another is the MALTHUS-VERHULST LOGISTICTHEORY
"biological control paradox" (Luck 1990, Arditi and The firstdefinitivetheoreticaltreatmentof popula-
Berryman1991), where classical models predict that tion dynamicswas Thomas Malthus' (1798) Essay on
you cannot have both a very low and a stable pest thePrincipleofPopulation. Malthus arguedthat,while
(prey)equilibriumdensity,yetthereare numerousex- populations grow logarithmically,the resources on
amples ofpredatorand parasiteintroductions(classical which they depend remain constant or only increase
biological control) that have resulted in exotic pests arithmetically.Thus, the demand for resources must
being maintainedat sparse and apparentlystable den- eventuallyexceed the supply and population growth,
sities(Turnbulland Chant 1961, DeBach 1974, Hagen being dependent on the resource supply, must then
and Franz 1973). cease. Fortyyears later,Verhulst(1838) formedMal-
Recentlytherehas been renewedinterestin what is thus' "principle of population" into a mathematical
beingcalled ratio-dependentpredator-preytheory(Ar- model-the logisticequation
diti and Ginzburg 1989, Berryman 1990). Although
ratio-dependent predator-prey models are notnew,they dN/dt = aN(1 - N/K), (1)
have not previouslyoccupied a central place in eco- where N is the biomass densityof the population in
logical theory.Yet theysolve many of the problemsof question, a is its maximum per-capitarate of change,
more conventionalmodels, includingthe paradoxes of or the instrinsicrate of increase,and K is the equilib-
enrichmentand biological control (Arditi and Ginz- rium density,oftencalled the carryingcapacityof the
I For reprintsof this Special Feature, see footnote 1, p. environment.Althoughthisequation is oftencriticized
1529. forits oversimplicity,it remainsthe centraltheoretical

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
October 1992 RATIO-DEPENDENT PREDATOR-PREY THEORY 1531

constructforsingle-speciespopulation dynamics and, terferencebetweensearchingpredators,spatial hetero-


when generalizedto account fordiscretegrowthpro- geneities,polyphagy,etc. (Hassell 1978).
cesses, time-delayed and nonlinear density depen-
dence, and multipledomains of attraction,it describes INCURSIONS OF LOGISTIC THEORY

the dynamics of many single-speciespopulations in In the original Lotka-Volterraequations, the prey


both laboratory and field (Gause 1934, Allee et al. populationgrowsinfinitelyin theabsence of predators.
1949, Thomas et al. 1980, Berrymanand Millstein To correctthisunreasonableassumption,a logisticself-
1990). limitationtermis oftenadded to the preyequation,
LOTKA-VOLTERRA PREDATOR-PREY THEORY dN/dt = aN(I - N/K) - bNP. (3a)
Lotka's Elements of Physical Biology (Lotka 1925) This modificationproduces an isocline structuresim-
was the next major advance in population dynamics ilar to that shown in Fig. lb and stabilizes the system
theory.Not onlydid Lotka derivethe logisticequation, (the equilibrium is now a stable focus).
which he called the "law of population growth,"from It is interestingthatVolterra(1928, as translatedin
firstprinciples,but he also proposed the firstmodel of Chapman 1931) and Gause (1934) both used the lo-
trophic(predator-prey)interactions.However, instead gisticequation as theunderlyingstructurefortheirtwo-
of developing the predator-preymodel by extending species competitionmodels, but failed to consider it
the logistic"law" to two species he, and soon after- as a suitable frameworkfor modeling predator-prey
wardsVolterra(1928, as translatedin Chapman 1931), interactions.Leslie (1948) seems to have been the first
adopted thechemical principleof mass action. In other to consider a logisticpredatorequation
words,he assumed thattheresponseofthepopulations
dP/dt = cP(1 - eP/N), (3b)
would be proportionalto the productof theirbiomass
densitiesso that where e is the densityof prey required to maintain a
single predator and to replace it with one offspring
dN/dt = aN - bNP, (2a) when it dies. In otherwords, e is the marginalsubsis-
tencedemand forprey,I /eis themarginalreproductive
dP/dt = cNP - dP, (2b)
value of the resource,and N/e is the carryingcapacity
of predatorswhen provided witha constantsupply of
where N and P are the biomass densities of prey and prey.Leslie's equation seems to be the firsttime that
predator,respectively,a and d are theirper-capitarates predator/preyratios (P/N) ratherthan products (NP)
of changein the absence of each other,and b and c are are seen in models oftrophicrelationships.The system
theirrespectiveratesof changedue to interaction.This of predator-preyequations (Eqs. 3) has an isocline
application of the principle of mass action seems to structuresimilar to that shown in Fig. 1c, with the
have been the point where predator-preytheoryde- communityequilibriuma stable focus.Notice thatthe
viated fromclassical (logistic)thinking,and wheresub- predatorisocline is slantingratherthan vertical.This
sequent theorizingmay have been misled. new isocline structureseems to be intuitivelyreason-
ShortlyafterpublicationoftheLotka-Volterraequa- able because predator equilibrium densities are ex-
tions, Nicholson and Bailey (1935) proposed a dis- pected to be dependent on prey abundance (see e.g.,
crete-timemodel of the interactionbetween insect Berryman1981, Arditiand Ginzburg 1989). In addi-
parasitoids and theirhosts. Althoughthis model was tion,theslantingpredatorisocline solves theparadoxes
developed fromthe more mechanisticperspectiveof of enrichmentand biological control(Arditiand Ginz-
parasitoid searchbehavior,it is identicalin concept to burg 1989, Arditiand Berryman1991).
the Lotka-Volterramodel (Royama 1971).
PREDATOR FUNCTIONAL RESPONSES
The zero-growthisoclines of the Lotka-Volterra
equations, obtained by settingtheirleft-handsides to The next major contributionto the theoryof pred-
zero, are perpendicularto the axis of the otherspecies ator/preyinteractionswas the addition of a predator
(Fig. la). Solutions of the differential
equations (Eqs. functionalresponse.Solomon (1949) and Holling(1959,
2) forma seriesof closed ellipses thatdepend critically 1966) argued that,because predatorscan only handle
on the initialconditions(neutrallystable limitcycles). a finitenumberof preyin a unitof time,thepreydeath
In the more reasonable discrete-time(Nicholson-Bai- rate should be a nonlinear functionof prey density;
ley)form,however,themodel has an unstablesolution; i.e.,
i.e., the communityequilibrium is an unstable focus
dN/dt = aN(1 - N/K) - b(N)P, (4a)
(similar to the trajectoryshown in Fig. la). A great
deal of theoreticalefforthas gone into stabilizingthe where b(N) is the functionalresponse of the predator
Nicholson-Bailey equations; i.e., by incorporatingin- to preydensity.Based on a seriesof elegantbehavioral

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1532 SPECIAL FEATURE Ecology, Vol. 73, No. 5

100 a oo .
a b

8320 80

< <
u.J wU
mr 40 Cm 40

20 20

0 0
200 400 600 800 1000 200 400 600 800 1000

PREY PREY
100 100
C d

80 80

60 60

w W
ci: 40 Cr: 40

20 20

0'
~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~0
200 400 600 800 100 200 400 600 800 1000

PREY PREY
100 100
e f
830 80

0 60 0 6

cl 40 ,' = 0: 40
al-

20 '20

0 0
200 400 600 800 1000 200 400 600 800 1i00

PREY PREY
FIG. 1. Zero-growthisoclines for models of interactingprey
(i)hand=. =
predator -) populations. The thin line is-
trajectorypredictedby the discrete-timeper-capitatrophicmodel, Eqs. 8 and 9 (see Berryman1990); i.e., N, = N,,_exp~ai
+bMNa t I + c , where i = 1 for prey and i = 2 for predator,Z, is the predator/prey ratio,
N2modl/(ws +N(,,-), in ratio
models and Z, = N,2-1 , = Nl, Iin Lotka-Volterramodels. (a) Lotka-Volterra-Nicholson-Bailey model: a, = 0.2, b, = 0,
cl= -0.004, a, = 0.1, b,= 0, c, = 0.0002. (b) L-V-N-B model with logisticself-limitationon the prey: prey model with
parametersthe same as (a) except a, = 0.3 and b, = -0.0004; predatormodel the same as (a) except C2= 0.0005. (c) Logistic-
Leslie predatorequation: preymodel as in (b); ratio predatorwitha, = 0.2, b, = 0, c, = - 1, w,= 0. (d) Holling-Rosenzweig-
MacArthurmodel: ratio preymodel with a, = 0.3,-b = - 0.0004, c, = -1, w, = 0; predatormodel as in (a), a, = - 0. 5,-b2
= 0, c2 = 0.001, wl= 0. (e) Logistic predator-preymodel with no predator self-limitation: prey model as in (d); predator
model as in (c). (f) Logistic predator-preymodel with predatorself-limitation:preymodel as in (d); predatormodel as in (c)
except b2 = -0.001.

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
October 1992 RATIO-DEPENDENT PREDATOR-PREY THEORY 1533

i~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~
ii ff SE S j i .fiiii FfEf .f i ii .iFE j f Eiii g EEE i Ei L i.EiSi .

experiments,in which predators (sometimes blind- by DeAngelis et al. (1975). When insertedintoclassical
foldedstudents)searchedfordifferentdensitiesofprey predator-preymodels, this ratio-dependentfunctional
(sometimes sandpaper disks), Holling derived his fa- response produces a parabolic preyisocline and right-
mous "disk" equation whichturnedout to be identical slantingpredatorisocline (Fig. 1e), therebysolvingthe
to the well-knownMichaelis-Menten equation of en- paradoxes ofenrichmentand biological control(Arditi
zyme kinetics(Real 1977); i.e., and Ginzburg 1989, Arditiand Berryman1991).
b(N) = mN/(w + N), (5)
THE PER-CAPITA VIEWPOINT
where m is the maximum predatorattack rate and w
is the prey densitywhere the attack rate is half-satu- Berryman(1981) and Getz (1984) have argued that,
rated. The Michaelis-Menten-Hollingequation can be because population dynamics arise frominteractions
extended to account forgeneral predatorsthat switch betweenindividual organisms,theequations should be
fromone prey species to another sigmoiddfunctional derived as per-capitaratesof change. For example, we
responses) (Real 1977). When the functionalresponse could writethe followinggeneralratio-dependentper-
is included in the prey equation, we obtain the para- capita trophicequation,
bolic (humped) preyisoclines (Rosenzweig 1971) that
are characteristicof ratio-dependentprey equations dNI/Njdt = Ri = a, - f(NI/N,) - g,(N?+I/N,), (7)
(Fig. Id); note thatthe functionalresponse introduces
ratio-dependenceintothepreyequation because, when whereN, is the biomass densityof the ith species in a
w is set to zero as in Fig. 1, the per-capitadeath rate trophicchain,R, is theper-capitarateof changeof that
of the preybecomes mP/N. species, a, is its maximum per-capitarate of change in
On theargumentthatthe preydeaths can be directly a givenphysicalenvironments definesthe interaction
translatedinto predator births,functionalresponses between the species and the lower trophic level (its
are oftenemployed in predatorequations; i.e., prey) as a functionof the predator/preyratio, and g,
definesits interactionwiththe highertrophiclevel (its
dP/dt= cP[mN/(w + N)] - dP. (4b) predator),also as a functionof the predator/prey ratio.
However, this formulationgives rise to the primitive Using the typeII functionalresponse (Eq. 5) forfand
rectilinearLotka-Volterrapredatorisocline fromwhich g, we can obtain an explictper-capitatrophicequation
arise the paradoxes of enrichmentand biological con- (see Appendix),
trol(cf. Fig. 1a and d). Nevertheless,the isocline struc-
R, = a - bNj/(wjI + N_,) - cNj+1/(w,+ N,), (8)
ture shown in Fig. Id has been employed extensively
in the development of modem predator-preytheory wherebi definesthe effectof intra-specific competition
(Rosenzweig and MacArthur 1963, MacArthur and forfood on the per-capitarate of change (freduces to
Connell 1966). the classical logisticwhen w,_1+ N,-, = a constant;
see Appendix), ci is a coefficient
of vulnerabilitydefin-
ing the effectof predation on the per-capita rate of
RATIO-DEPENDENT FUNCTIONAL RESPONSES change,and w,is the biomass densityof all otherfood
Althoughthe inclusion of a functionalresponse in species in theith trophiclevel. Notice thattheper-capita
the predator-preymodel is intuitivelyappealing, be- viewpointclarifiesthe ecological meaningof the func-
cause it conservativelycouples the preyand predator tional response parameter wi, previously called the
equations, thereare some notable problems with this "half-saturation"point; i.e., it definesthe quantityof
approach. For instance, the functionalresponse de- alternativefood available to the predator.Because the
scribes the behavior of searchingpredatorson a fast denominators of the predator/preyratios in (Eq. 8)
(behavioral) timescale (minutesor hours),whereasthe contain all the preyspecies available to each predator,
population equation, into which it is inserted,often while the predatorsutilizingthat food are contained
operateson a slower(population dynamical)timescale in the numerators,it is fairlystraightforward to extend
(days or years). To overcome thisproblem,Arditiand this equation to food webs with many species in each
Ginzburg(1989) suggestthat,in cases where the time trophiclevel.
scales are incongruent,the functionalresponse should It is worthnoting that, although simple per-capita
be expressedin termsof the ratio of preyto predators; equations are nonlinear in respect to their variables,
e.g., the Holling Type II functionalresponseshould be theyare linear in theirratios and, therefore,can be fit
written to data withstandardregressiontechniques(Berryman
1990). In thisway, model parameterscan be estimated
b(N/P) = m(N/P)/(w+ N/P) = mN/(wP + N). (6)
a posteriorifrom time-seriesdata, such as mightbe
A similar feedingequation was proposed previously obtained fromannual surveysor harvestrecords.For

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1534 SPECIAL FEATURE Ecology, Vol. 73, No. 5

example, the per-capitarate of change of each species LITERATURE CITED

can be estimatedfromthe relationship


Allee, W. C., A. E. Emerson, 0. Park, T. Park, and K. P.
R, = 1/N dN/dt= d In N/dt= ln(N,/N,-,), (9)Schmidt. 1949. Principles of animal ecology. Saunders,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania,USA.
and this can be regressedagainst the predator/prey Arditi,R., and A. A. Berryman. 1991. The biologicalcontrol
ratios(Eq. 8). These models can thenbe used to predict paradox. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 6:32.
futurepopulationtrendsor to simulateresourceor pest Arditi, R., and L. R. Ginzburg. 1989. Coupling in predator-
prey dynamics: ratio-dependence.Journal of Theoretical
managementpolicies (Berryman1991); i.e., theyhave Biology 139:311-326.
practicalutility. Arditi,R., L. R. Ginzburg,and H. R. Akcakaya. 1991. Vari-
ation in plankton densities among lakes: a case forratio-
CONCLUSIONS dependent models. American Naturalist138:1287-1296.
A. A. 1981. Population systems.Plenum, New
Unlike the theoreticalmodels of inter-specificcom- Berryman,
York, New York, USA.
petitionand mutualism,which emergeddirectlyfrom 1990. Population Analysis System:POPSYS Series
the theoryof logisticsingle-speciespopulation dynam- 2, Two-Species Analysis (Version 1.0). Ecological Systems
ics, the original Lotka-Volterrapredator-preyequa- Analysis,Pullman, Washington,USA.
1991. Population theory:an essential ingredientin
tions were built around the principleof mass action;
pest prediction,management,and policy-making.Ameri-
i.e., the responseswere assumed to be proportionalto can Entomologist37:138-142.
the productratherthan the ratio of population densi- Berryman,A. A., and J. A. Millstein. 1990. Population
ties. Predatorequations derived in this way have ver- Analysis System: POPSYS Series 1, One-Species Analysis
tical isoclines,leading to the paradoxes of enrichment (Version 2.5). Ecological SystemsAnalysis,Pullman,Wash-
ington,USA.
and biological control. DeAngelis, D. L., R. A. Goldstein,and R. V. O'Neill. 1975.
The inclusion of a predator functionalresponse in A model fortrophicinteractions.Ecology 56:881-892.
the Lotka-Volterramodel correctedthe unreasonable DeBach, P. 1974. Biological control by natural enemies.
assumptionof unsaturatingattackrates.However, the Cambridge UniversityPress, Cambridge,England.
primitiverectilinearpredator isocline remained, and Gause, G. F. 1934. The struggleforexistence.Williams &
Wilkins,New York, New York, USA.
so also the paradoxes of enrichmentand biological Getz, W. M. 1984. Population dynamics: a per-capita re-
control.This problem was solved by makingthe func- source approach. Journalof Theoretical Biology 108:623-
tional response depend on prey/predator ratios rather 643.
than preydensitiesalone. Ginzburg,L. R., and H. R. Akcakaya. 1992. Consequences
of ratio-dependentpredation forsteadystate propertiesof
Per-capita predator/preymodels evolve naturally ecosystems.Ecology 73:1536-1543.
fromclassical logistictheory.Such models have slant- Gutierrez,A. P. 1992. The physiologicalbasis of ratio-de-
ing predatorisoclines, contain implicit functionalre- pendent predator-preytheory:a metabolic pool model of
sponses (see Appendix), and solve the paradoxes of Nicholson'sblowfliesas an example.Ecology73:1552-1563.
enrichmentand biological control. In addition, they Hagen, K. S., and J. M. Franz. 1973. A historyof biological
control. Pages 433-476 in R. F. Smith, T. E. Mittler,and
offerparsimonious descriptionsof predator/preyin- C. N. Smith, editors. Historyof entomology.Annual Re-
teractions,and can easily be extendedto multi-species views, Palo Alto, California,USA.
food webs. It may also be significant thatlogisticmod- Hairston, N. G., F. E. Smith, and L. B. Slobodkin. 1960.
els emerge from the reduction and simplificationof Community structure,population control, and competi-
tion. American Naturalist94:421-425.
detailed physiological and behavioral predator-prey Hassell, M. P. 1978. The dynamicsof arthropodpredator-
models (Rapport and Turner 1975, Gutierrez 1992). preysystems.PrincetonUniversityPress, Princeton,New
The main criticismthathas been levelled at logistic Jersey,USA.
(per-capita)predator/prey models is thattheymay not Holling, C. S. 1959. The components of predation as re-
strictlyconformto the laws of conservation(Ginzburg vealed by a studyof small mammal predation of the Eu-
ropean pine sawfly.Canadian Entomologist91:293-320.
and Akqakaya 1992); i.e., the equations are not ex- 1966. The functionalresponseof invertebratepred-
plicitlycoupled by the biomass of prey killed, as are atorsto preydensity.Memoirs oftheEntomologicalSociety
conventional models. However, it may be overly re- of Canada 48: 1-86.
strictiveto insist that predator/preymodels adhere Leslie, P. H. 1948. Some furthernoteson theuse ofmatrices
in population mathematics.Biometrica 35:213-245.
strictlyto thelaws ofconservation.Afterall, preydeath Lotka, A. J. 1925. Elements of physical biology. Williams
is not always necessaryforpredatorreproduction.For & Wilkins,Baltimore,Maryland,USA.
example, trueparasites and most organismsthat feed Luck, R. F. 1990. Evaluation of natural enemies for bio-
on plants do not kill theirhosts, but theymay reduce logical control:a behavioral approach. Trends in Ecology
fecundity, and growthrates.Insistenceon con- and Evolution 5:196-199.
fertility,
MacArthur,R. H., and J. H. Connell. 1966. The biologyof
formityto the laws of conservation may, therefore, populations. JohnWiley,New York, New York, USA.
unnecessarilyconstrainthe development of a general Malthus, T. R. 1959. An essay on the principleof popula-
theoryof predator-preyinteractions. tion. Reprinted from 1798 edition, Johnson,London, as

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
October 1992 RATIO-DEPENDENT PREDATOR-PREY THEORY 1535

:: : : :: ::::::::::::::::::::::::::L:::::::::::::::::::::::E:~::
::::::::: :::::::::: ::::: ::::::::::::::: :::::::::::::: ::::i:E : ~
~~~:N0:

Malthus-Population: the firstessay. Ann Arbor Paper- dation and parasitism.Researches on Population Ecology,
backs,UniversityofMichigan,Ann Arbor,Michigan,USA. SupplementNumber 1: 1-91.
Nicholson, A. J., and V. A. Bailey. 1935. The balance of Solomon, M. E. 1949. The natural control of animal pop-
animal populations. Proceedingsof the Zoological Society ulation. Journalof Animal Ecology 18:1-35.
of London 3:551-598. Thomas, W. R., M. J. Pomerantz,and M. E. Gilpin. 1980.
Rapport, D. J., and J. E. Turner. 1975. Feeding rates and Chaos, asymmetricgrowthand group selectionfordynam-
population growth.Ecology 56:942-949. ical stability.Ecology 61:1312-1320.
Real, L. 1977. The kineticsof functionalresponse. Amer- Turnbull,A. L., and D. A. Chant. 1961. The practiceand
ican Naturalist111:289-300. theoryof biological controlof insectsin Canada. Canadian
Rosenzweig, M. L. 1969. Paradox of enrichment:destabi- Journalof Zoology 3:697-753.
lization of exploitationsystemsin ecological time. Science Verhulst,P. F. 1838. Notice sur la loi que la population
(Washington,D.C.) 171:385-387. suite dans son accroissement.Correspondence Mathema-
1971. Why the prey curve has a hump. American tique et Physique 10: 113-121.
Naturalist103:81-87. Volterra,V. 1931. Variationsand fluctuationsofthenumber
Rosenzweig,M. L., and R. H. MacArthur. 1963. Graphical of individuals in animal species livingtogether.Translated
representationand stabilityconditionsofpredator-preyin- from 1928 edition by R. N. Chapman. Animal ecology.
teraction.American Naturalist97:209-223. Arno, New York, New York, USA.
Royama, T. 1971. A comparative studyof models forpre-

APPENDIX
The type II functionalresponse (Eq. 5) definesthe attack dP/Pdt= c(l - eP/N),
rateper predatorsupplied withN prey,so forP predatorsthe
preydeath rateis mNP/(w+ N). Dividing by N gives the per- Rp= c- bP/N, with b = ce.
capita death rate mP/(w + N) which,afterrelabelling,can be For a single predatorforagingforprey in an arena, this be-
substitutedforf and g, in Eq. 7 to give Eq. 8. It is important comes
to note that the firstper-capitafunctionalresponse (f,in Eq.
7) reducesto the classical logisticwhen the lowertrophiclevel RP = c - b/N,
is constant;e.g.,a constantinputof sunlightforplants.Under whichhas an identicalformto thetypeII functionalresponse;
this condition, w, l + N, I = a constant = E, l, and E Ilb- i.e., R, - -o as N -O and Rp- c as N -oo. Equivalency
= K, -, the equilibrium densityor carryingcapacity of the betweenthe logisticequation and the functionalresponseoc-
environment.Leslie (1948) demonstratedthis implicitfunc- cursifthe per-capitarateofchangeofthe predatoris assumed
tional response withinthe logisticequation; e.g., if we write to be directlyproportionalto the numberof preyeaten, a not
the per-capitalogisticpredatorequation (Eq. 3b) unreasonable proposition.

This content downloaded on Wed, 26 Dec 2012 15:50:47 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like