Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance anxiety and


dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem
Viren Swami a,b,∗
a
Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK
b
Department of Psychology, HELP University College, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Previous studies on psychosocial aspects of tattooing have not examined prospective changes in self- and
Received 10 March 2011 body-related attitudes as a result of obtaining a tattoo. In the present study, 82 British residents obtain-
Received in revised form 16 April 2011 ing their first tattoo completed measures of state appearance anxiety and dissatisfaction prior to, and
Accepted 21 April 2011
immediately after, obtaining a tattoo. They also completed measures of trait body appreciation, distinc-
tive appearance investment, self-ascribed uniqueness, social physique anxiety, and self-esteem before
Keywords:
obtaining a tattoo and three weeks later. Results showed that both women and men had significantly
Tattoo
lower appearance anxiety and dissatisfaction immediately after obtaining their tattoo, and significantly
Body art
Uniqueness
higher body appreciation, distinctive appearance investment, self-ascribed uniqueness, and self-esteem
Body image after three weeks. Women reported greater social physique anxiety after three weeks, whereas men
Appearance anxiety reported lower anxiety. These results are discussed in relation to the positive impacts of obtaining body
Self-esteem art and the mainstreaming of tattooing in Western societies.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction psychosocial aspects of tattooing had, for a time at least, remained


relatively piecemeal (Fisher, 2002; Tate & Shelton, 2008). More
The art of tattooing may have an incredibly long history among recently, it has been possible to discern at least three broad areas
many cultural groups, but the growth of its popularity and accep- of interest (Swami & Harris, 2011), relating to sociocultural and
tance in Western, industrialised societies since the early 1990s has individual motivations for obtaining tattoos, interpersonal percep-
been particularly dramatic (Caplan, 2000; DeMello, 1995, 2000; tions of individuals with tattoos, and psychological and behavioural
Rubin, 1988; Swami & Harris, 2011). For instance, the incidence correlates of tattooing.
of tattooing among respondents in North America and Europe is In terms of the former, some sociologists have attempted to
approximately 25% (Armstrong, Roberts, Owen, & Koch, 2004; Greif, understand the contemporary popularity of tattooing as a means
Hewitt, & Armstrong, 1999; Laumann & Derick, 2006; Makkai & of reclaiming some agency over corporeal experiences in post-
McAllister, 2001; Mayers & Chiffriller, 2008; Stieger, Pietschnig, industrialised societies where the body is violently commodified
Kastner, Voracek, & Swami, 2010) and at least one commentator (e.g., Benson, 2000). Others have highlighted the appropriation
believes it will reach 40% in the next decade (Anderson, 2006). of counter-cultures by twentieth-century capitalism alongside the
Moreover, the historic trend of higher prevalence among men editing out of discourse on traditional body art populations, such
appears to have been overturned, with women now being just as that tattoos are now packaged as a bourgeois means of self-styling
likely to have tattoos (Laumann & Derick, 2006; Mayers & Chiffriller, within cultures that require constant work on the body (Blanchard,
2008; Stieger et al., 2010). 1994; Kosut, 2006; Sanders, 1989). In addition to these macro-
Where tattoos were once more common among out-groups that level factors, the contemporary popularity of tattooing may also
were stigmatised as being deviant or aggressive (e.g., bikers, prison- have benefited from advances in procedures (e.g., in terms of safety
ers, organized crime syndicates), they now appear to have achieved and faster recovery), the influx of new artists into the industry,
an unprecedented degree of mainstreaming across socioeconomic and higher disposable incomes and the lower cost of tattooing
and other demographic boundaries (DeMello, 2000; Forbes, 2001). (DeMello, 2000).
Yet, despite this apparent mainstreaming, scholarly interest in the Concurrently, a raft of recent studies has focused on individual
motivations for obtaining a tattoo (for a review, see Wohlrab, Stahl,
& Kappeler, 2007). Broadly speaking, these studies have centred on
∗ Correspondence address: Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, fashion-related motivations versus meaning-related motivations,
309 Regent Street, London W1B 2UW, UK. Tel.: +44 2079115000. while also downplaying the role of tattoos in signalling simple out-
E-mail address: v.swami@wmin.ac.uk group affiliation. In the first instance, the rapid mainstreaming of

1740-1445/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.005
238 V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244

tattooing has led some scholars to dismiss tattoos as passé or as ferences between those who have tattoos and those who do not
an example of the incorporation of the ‘exotic’ into the fashion (Tate & Shelton, 2008). Thus, several studies have suggested that
system (Falk, 1995; Steele, 1996). In this perspective, tattoos are tattoo possession is associated with being sexually active (Carroll,
viewed as meaningless cultural ephemera or ‘fashion accessories’ Riffenburgh, Roberts, & Myhre, 2002; Roberts & Ryan, 2002). Among
that serve purely aesthetic functions and thus signal an individual’s college students, for instance, tattoo possession predicts being sex-
participation in the fashion system (Steele, 1996). ually active and, among men, becoming sexually active at a younger
Certainly, qualitative research by Sweetman (1999, p. 55) sug- age (Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2005; Koch, Roberts,
gests that lightly tattooed respondents view their tattoos as little Armstrong, & Owen, 2010).
more than fashion accessories, similar to “more standard forms A larger body of research has suggested that tattoo posses-
of jewellery or other items intended to enhance a particular sion among adults is correlated with greater risk-taking behaviour,
‘look”’. Other work has similarly suggested that common motiva- including the use of illegal substances, violent behaviours directed
tions for obtaining tattoos include “just wanting one” (Greif et al., at others, and illegal activities such as shoplifting (Armstrong et al.,
1999) or because “they look good” (Forbes, 2001; Tiggemann & 2004a; Brooks, Woods, Knight, & Shrier, 2003; Burger & Finkel,
Hopkins, 2011). However, Sweetman (1999) also problematises 2002; Drews, Allison, & Probst, 2000; Forbes, 2001; Nathanson,
the argument that tattoos are fashionable per se by highlight- Paulhus, & Williams, 2006). Among adolescents, too, tattoo posses-
ing the permanence, planning, and pain involved in tattooing. His sion is associated with drug use and sexual activity (Braithwaite,
research showed how both heavily and lightly tattooed respon- Robillard, Woodring, Stephens, & Arriola, 2001; Carroll et al., 2002;
dents viewed tattooing as a form of ‘anti-fashion’, employed as a Roberts & Ryan, 2002), as well as school truancy, illegal activi-
means of preserving individual or social identities, while advertis- ties, gang affiliation, and problem gambling (Deschesnes, Fines, &
ing the permanence of their experiences, values, or beliefs. Demers, 2006).
Indeed, it appears to be the case that many tattooed individuals Finally, some scholars have examined the personality, mood,
emphasise their tattoos as an individual means of self-expression or psychopathology of tattooed individuals, although the results
or as a means of constructing self-identity by drawing attention of their work remain equivocal. For example, some studies report
to the body (Armstrong et al., 2004a; Millner & Eichold, 2001). that tattooed individuals score higher than non-tattooed individu-
For instance, recent studies have discussed tattooing as a means als on measures of extraversion and related traits, such as sensation
through which individuals are able to accentuate their self-identity seeking (Copes & Forsyth, 1993; Drews et al., 2000; Stirn, Hinz,
or achieve uniqueness in the appearance domain (Tiggemann & & Brahler, 2006; Wohlrab, Stahl, et al., 2007; see also Roberti,
Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). In this view, individuals Storch, & Bravata, 2004), but other studies have returned non-
with a need for uniqueness and distinctive appearance investment significant findings (Forbes, 2001; Tate & Shelton, 2008). Similarly,
may be more likely to obtain tattoos, and the resulting perception some studies report that tattooed individuals have lower scores
of uniqueness may help improve an individual’s self-esteem, par- on measures of depression than non-tattooed individuals (Fredrick
ticularly in contemporary industrialised societies where the ‘body & Bradley, 2000), while others have shown that they have higher
project’ remains ubiquitous. scores (Roberti & Storch, 2005; see also Nathanson et al., 2006). In
However, any positive impact of tattoos on the psychological summing up this area of research, Tate and Shelton (2008, p. 281)
well-being of respondents may be tempered by negative inter- cautioned that the effect sizes of reported differences have tended
personal attributions they experience (Swami & Furnham, 2007). to be very small and “most likely reflect inconsequential real world
For example, several studies have reported that children are more differences” between those with and without tattoos.
likely to attribute negative acts (e.g., drug use) to tattooed figures Although studies on tattoos and their outcomes have bur-
(Durkin & Houghton, 2000) and that non-tattooed adults perceive geoned in recent years, Swami and Harris (2011) identified two
tattooed individuals as more neurotic, less agreeable, less con- specific shortcomings of this literature. Specifically, these authors
scientious, and less open to experience than themselves (Forbes, noted, firstly, that there have been no in-depth examinations of
2001; Wohlrab, Fink, Kappeler, & Brewer, 2009). Within occupa- the associations between possession of tattoos and measures of
tional settings, employers are more likely to hold negative attitudes body image. Secondly, they highlight the fact that there have been
toward tattooed employees compared with non-tattooed employ- no longitudinal studies examining the effects of obtaining tat-
ees (Bekhor, Bekhor, & Gandrabur, 1995; Stuppy, Armstrong, & toos, particularly in relation to body-related attitudes in pre- and
Casals-Ariet, 1998). post-modification states (especially among individuals who are
Such negative attributions may be more readily elicited for obtaining tattoos for the first time). This dearth in the literature is
women with visible tattoos compared to tattooed men (Armstrong, important because corporeal experiences may not only be motivat-
1991; Degelman & Price, 2002; Hawkes, Senn, & Thorn, 2004). ing factors in tattoo procurement (Armstrong, 1991), but may also
In one study, line-drawn figures of women with visible tattoos change markedly once a tattoo has placed on the body (Schildkrout,
were rated as less physically attractive, more sexually promiscu- 2004). In addition, it has been estimated that prevalence rates of
ous, and heavier consumers of alcohol than the same figure without individuals dissatisfied with their tattoos is about 20% (Anderson,
tattoos (Swami & Furnham, 2007). Another study using computer- 2006; Laumann & Derick, 2006), with 6% seeking removal (Mayers,
generated images showed that tattooed women were rated as more Judelson, Moriarty, & Rundell, 2002), and negative corporeal expe-
sexually uninhibited and less motivated, honest, and healthy than riences may be an important reason for dissatisfaction (Anderson,
non-tattooed women (Wohlrab, Stahl, Rammsayer, & Kappeler, 2001; Armstrong, Roberts, Owen, & Koch, 2004; Huxley & Grogan,
2007). This gendered pattern of findings has been discussed in rela- 2005). The present study, then, was conceived as an attempt to
tion to norms of behaviour ascribed to men and women, such that overcome the afore-mentioned shortcomings in the literature.
women bearing tattoos are viewed as transgressing conservative Specifically, the current study examined participants’ state body
gender attitudes, which in turn results in greater stigmatisation image and appearance anxiety in their pre-modification state and
(Armstrong, 1991; Hawkes et al., 2004). immediately after obtaining a tattoo. During the first testing ses-
A third avenue that has received sustained attention from sion, participants also completed measures of motivations for
researchers interested in body art concerns behavioural and per- obtaining a tattoo, trait body image and appearance anxiety, per-
sonality differences between body-modified and non-modified ceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem, which were re-examined
individuals. This body of work is based on the premise that the three weeks after respondents had obtained their tattoo (by which
agency involved in obtaining body art reflects, or results in, dif- time the tattoo would have healed completely). Given the dearth
V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244 239

of research on temporal changes in body-related attitudes follow- is computed as the mean of all 16 items, with higher scored indi-
ing tattooing, the present study was largely exploratory, although cating greater state anxiety about appearance. Reed et al. (1991)
it was expected that obtaining a tattoo would generally result in reported that PASTAS scores have good internal consistency, high
more positive body-related attitudes in the short term. test-retest reliability over a two-week period, and are sensitive to
situational differences. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha for
Method this scale was .88 at Time 1 and .90 at Time 2.

Design Body Appreciation Scale (BAS; Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-


Barcalow, 2005). The BAS, completed at Times 1 and 3, is a 13-item
The present study employed a prospective design in which measure of trait body image in which items are rated on a 5-point
respondents completed a questionnaire-based survey immediately Likert-type scale (1 = Never, 5 = Always). The scale measures related
before (Time 1) and after (Time 2) obtaining a tattoo, as well as three aspects of positive body image and, among Western samples, it
weeks after obtaining their tattoo (Time 3). At Time 1, participants has been shown to have a one-dimensional structure (Avalos et al.,
completed state and trait measures of body image, appearance anx- 2005; Swami, Stieger, Haubner, & Voracek, 2008). An overall BAS
iety, motivations for obtaining a tattoo, perceptions of uniqueness, score is calculated as the mean of all 13 items, with higher scores
and self-esteem. At Time 2, participants completed state measures indicating more positive body image. Avalos et al. (2005) reported
of body image and appearance anxiety, as well as a measure of that the BAS has good internal consistency, good discriminant, con-
their satisfaction with the new tattoo. Finally, at Time 3, partic- struct, and incremental validities, and good test-retest reliability
ipants completed trait measures of body image and appearance after three weeks. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha for the
anxiety, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Key measures BAS was .91 at Time 1 and .88 at Time 3.
are described below along with details of testing sessions.
Self-Attributed Need for Uniqueness Scale (SANU; Lynn &
Study Site and Participants Harris, 1997). The SANU, which participants completed at Times
1 and 3, is a 4-item measure of self-perceived feelings of being or
Participants of this study were 82 British residents recruited at thinking differently from others. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-
a tattoo parlour in Camden Town, an inner city district in London, type scale (1 = Not at all, 5 = Extremely) and an overall SANU score
England. In the past several decades, Camden Town has witnessed a is computed as the mean of all four items (higher scores reflect
rapid increase in retail, tourist, and entertainment outlets, many of greater self-attributed uniqueness). Previous work has shown that
which are associated with alternative cultures. Part of this change the SANU has good internal consistency as well as good construct
from its earlier industrial heritage has been the proliferation of tat- validity (Lynn & Harris, 1997; Lynn & Snyder, 2002). In the present
too and body piercing parlours. The present study was conducted at study, Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .92 at Times 1 and 3.
a licensed tattoo parlour with a complement of five tattoo artists.
The participants of the study were 39 women and 43 men rang-
Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS; Hart, Leary, & Rajeski,
ing in age from 18 to 50 years (M = 24.89, SD = 6.38) and of whom
1989). The SPAS, completed at Times 1 and 3, is a 12-item mea-
a majority were of British White descent (89.0%). In terms of edu-
sure of anxiety associated with perceived evaluation of one’s body
cational qualifications, 35.4% had completed secondary education,
or physical appearance. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type
42.7% had a post-secondary qualification, 19.5% had an undergrad-
scale (1 = Not at all like me, 5 = Like me a lot) and an overall score
uate degree, and 2.4% had a postgraduate degree.
is computed as the mean of all items. For the SPAS, higher scores
indicate greater social physique anxiety. The SPAS has been shown
Measures
to have adequate construct validity, internal consistency, and test-
retest reliability (Hart et al., 1989). In the present study, Cronbach’s
Demographics. Participants provided their demographic
alpha for this scale was .90 at Time 1 and .92 at Time 3.
details at Time 1, consisting of gender, age, ethnicity, and highest
educational qualification.
Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). The
Visual Analogue Scale (Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). A visual RSES, completed by participants at Times 1 and 3, is the most
analogue scale was used to measure state feelings about over- widely used measure of an individual’s feeling of self-worth. The
all appearance at Times 1 and 2. Respondents were asked to rate RSES consists of 10 items that are rated on a 4-point Likert-type
their satisfaction with their “overall physical appearance” on a scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 4 = Strongly agree). An overall score is
100-millimetre line, anchored by two extremes, namely No dissat- computed as the mean of all items, with higher scores reflecting
isfaction and Extreme dissatisfaction. Responses were measured to greater self-esteem. Scores on the RSES have been shown to have
the nearest millimetre, with higher scores indicating greater dis- high internal consistency and good convergent validity (Whiteside-
satisfaction with overall appearance at a given moment. Visual Mansell & Corwyn, 2003). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha for
analogue scales of this type have been used to determine the effects the RSES was .86 at Time 1 and .88 at Time 3.
of experimental manipulation on body image and have been shown
to have good construct validity (e.g., Cattarin, Thompson, Thomas, Distinctive Appearance Investment Scale (DAI; Tiggemann
& Williams, 2000; Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). & Golder, 2006). The DAI is a 6-item measure of an individual’s
desire to look different and stand out specifically in the appear-
Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale (PASTAS; ance domain, which participants completed at Times 1 and 3.
Reed, Thompson, Brannick, & Sacco, 1991). The PASTAS, com- Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = Strongly dis-
pleted at Times 1 and 2, is an affective measure assessing anxiety agree, 5 = Strongly agree) and an overall score is computed as the
about 16 different body sites. The present study used the state ver- mean of all items (higher scores reflect greater distinctive appear-
sion of the PASTAS, in which participants are asked to describe how ance investment). Tiggemann and Golder (2006) reported that DAI
anxious, tense, or nervous they feel at that particular moment about scores have a one-dimensional structure with high internal con-
the different body sites. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type sistency and good construct validity, insofar as scores are only
scale (0 = Not at all, 4 = Exceptionally so) and an overall PASTAS score moderately correlated with scores on need for uniqueness and
240 V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244

Table 1
Mean and standard deviations for reasons for obtaining tattoos.

Item Women (n = 39) Men (n = 43) Total (N = 82)

M SD M SD M SD

They celebrate an occasion/person 1.77 0.84 2.26 1.14 2.02 1.09


To feel independent 1.46 0.72 1.63 0.95 1.55 0.84
To look attractive 1.79 0.89 1.81 0.73 1.80 0.81
To express myself 3.97 1.09 4.32 0.92 4.16 1.01
To be an individual 2.74 0.99 2.65 0.92 2.70 0.95
To be unique 3.69 0.83 3.81 1.10 3.76 0.98
To control my body 1.82 1.05 1.58 0.76 1.70 0.91
To be fashionable 1.67 0.87 1.28 0.55 1.46 0.74
To be creative 1.95 0.69 2.16 1.02 2.06 0.88
Because they look good 3.89 1.14 3.67 1.21 3.78 1.18
Because my friend are tattooed 1.23 0.48 1.51 0.82 1.37 0.70
To rebel 1.25 0.64 1.74 1.09 1.51 0.93
To look tough 1.13 0.41 1.30 0.74 1.22 0.61
To feel better about myself 2.07 0.77 1.81 1.01 1.94 0.91
To stand out in a crowd 2.02 1.13 2.21 1.21 2.12 1.17
Because I like to take risks 1.52 0.60 1.60 0.93 1.56 0.79
To feel mature 1.12 0.34 1.27 0.59 1.21 0.49
To have a beauty mark 1.31 0.69 1.44 0.55 1.38 0.62
To show commitment to a group 1.41 0.72 1.44 0.55 1.42 0.63

Note: Means above 3.00 are shown in bold.

appearance investment. In the present study, internal consistency approached and agreed to the study taking place on their premises.
was .88 at Time 1 and .84 at Time 3. Because of the large number of tourists who frequent Camden
Town, participation was limited to British residents of an adult
Reasons for obtaining tattoos (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006). age and those who were obtaining their first tattoo. A receptionist
At Time 1, participants were asked to rate 19 possible reasons for working at the tattoo parlour initially screened potential partic-
obtaining their tattoo (see Table 1 for a full list). The list was origi- ipants on the day they made their booking (which was usually
nally devised by Tiggemann and Golder (2006) to reflect the range between two and four weeks prior to their appointment) and noti-
of motivations for obtaining body art, such as tattoos and pierc- fied the author of appointment dates for potential participants.
ings. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not a reason, On the day of the appointment, the author (who has readily vis-
5 = Very strong reason) and are treated individually (Tiggemann & ible tattoos) invited potential participants to take part in a survey
Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). ostensibly on consumer behaviours and attitudes, and informed
participants that they would be retested immediately after their
Visibility and extent of tattoos (Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). tattoo had been completed. Of a total of 92 participants who were
In order to get an accurate description of participants’ tattoos, par- invited to take part in the study, 84 agreed, representing a response
ticipants were provided with schematic outlines of the front and rate of 91.3%.
back view of the human figure. At Time 2, participants were asked Once participation had been agreed and participants provided
to draw a likeness of their tattoo on the figures. The drawings were informed consent, they completed a paper-and-pencil question-
then used to ascertain tattoo visibility and percentage of the body naire containing the measures described above, as well as filler
covered by the new tattoo. Tattoo visibility was classified on a 5- scales related to consumer behaviours and attitudes. The order
point scale (1 = Rarely visible, 2 = Only visible in underwear, 3 = Visible of the scales in the questionnaire was semi-randomised for each
in shorts, t-shirt, and open shoes, 4 = Visible in trousers, long sleeves, participant, with the demographic items always appearing first.
and covered shoes, 5 = Always visible). For the present purposes, four Participants completed the questionnaire in the waiting room of the
independent judges coded both visibility and percentage of the tattoo parlour while the tattoo artist prepared for the appointment.
body covered by tattoos. In the present study, inter-rater reliability Completed questionnaires were returned to the experimenter in
for both measures was good (visibility, r = .94; percentage of body an envelope. Immediately after obtaining their tattoo, partici-
covered, r = .96). Following Tiggemann and Hopkins (2011) and for pants were invited to take part in a second survey, containing
the purposes of analyses, visibility ratings were collapsed as ‘easily the measures described above as well as filler scales not included
concealed’ (visibility ratings of 1 or 2) or ‘readily visible’ (visibility during the first testing session. Two participants declined to take
ratings of 3 of more). part at this stage, citing a lack of time, leaving a final sample
of 82 respondents. The order of presentation of scales was fully
Satisfaction with tattoo. At Times 2 and 3, participants were randomised and completed questionnaires were returned to the
asked to rate their overall satisfaction with their tattoo using a experimenter.
single item with a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = Very dissatisfied, Three weeks after the appointment, all 82 participants were
7 = Very satisfied). invited by email to take part in a third survey, again ostensibly
on consumer behaviour. Where there was no response, invitations
Likelihood of obtaining future tattoos. At Time 3, participants were sent again a week later. All 82 participants from the initial test-
were asked to indicate how likely they were to obtain tattoos in ing agreed to take part in the third survey. Once participation had
the future on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = Very unlikely, 7 = Very been agreed, participants were sent the questionnaire containing
likely). the scales described above as well as filler scales. The question-
naires were sent by email and returned in a sealed envelope to the
Procedure experimenter. As required by the ethics committee, nominal codes
were used to link the data from the third testing session with ear-
Once ethical approval had been obtained from the relevant uni- lier data without breaching the participants’ right to anonymity.
versity ethics committee, the tattoo parlour’s management were Once testing was complete, all participants who had been sur-
V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244 241

Table 2
Means and standard deviations for all factors included in the present study at different testing sessions.

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Women Men Women Men Women Men

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

VAS 60.18 20.32 44.09 17.60 39.21 25.07 29.49 16.69 – – – –


PASTAS 3.27 1.10 2.52 0.97 2.78 1.09 2.12 0.82 – – – –
Satisfaction with tattoo – – – – 6.49 0.68 6.35 0.72 4.62 1.94 5.53 1.39
BAS 2.79 0.90 3.87 0.58 – – – – 3.26 0.90 4.27 0.66
DAI 2.45 1.08 2.42 0.96 – – – – 2.74 0.86 2.67 0.86
SANU 3.70 0.50 3.75 0.53 – – – – 4.00 0.52 4.21 0.62
SPAS 2.45 0.89 2.36 0.83 – – – – 3.04 0.92 1.92 0.49
RSES 3.03 0.53 2.89 0.62 – – – – 3.45 0.60 3.07 0.66

Note: VAS = Visual Analogue Scale, PASTAS = Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale, BAS = Body Appreciation Scale, DAI = Distinctive Appearance Investment,
SANU = Self-Ascribed Need for Uniqueness, SPAS = Social Physique Anxiety Scale, RSE = Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale.

veyed were fully debriefed via email and were provided with the Analyses of Scores at Times 1 and 2
experimenter’s contact details.
To examine possible differences between participants’ appear-
ance anxiety and dissatisfaction at Times 1 and 2, a mixed ANOVA
was conducted with testing session as a within-subjects factor,
Results
participant gender as a between-subjects factor, and the VAS and
PASTAS scores, respectively, as dependent variables. The results
Preliminary Analyses
for the VAS scores showed that there was a significant decrease in
dissatisfaction with overall appearance at Time 2, F(1, 80) = 66.58,
Missing data (<2.0%) were replaced using the mean replace-
p < .001, 2p = .45, and that women were significantly more dissatis-
ment method. Descriptive statistics for all variables measured
fied with their appearance across testing sessions, F(1, 80) = 11.18,
during each testing session are reported in Table 2. Prelimi-
p = .001, 2p = .12. There was no significant interaction between
nary analysis showed that there was no significant difference in
testing session and gender, F(1, 80) = 2.13, p = .148, 2p = .03. The
age between women (M = 25.46, SD = 6.06) and men (M = 24.37,
same pattern of results was observed for PASTAS scores, with a sig-
SD = 6.69), t(80) = 0.77, p = .443, d = .17. Among men, the most com-
nificant decrease in appearance anxiety at Time 2, F(1, 80) = 31.57,
mon position for the tattoo was on the upper arm (n = 16), followed
p < .001, 2p = .28, and women having significantly higher anxiety at
by the lower arm (n = 11), and chest (n = 7), whereas among women
the most common position was the foot or ankle (n = 11), lower back both testing sessions, F(1, 80) = 11.67, p = .001, 2p = .13. There was
(n = 8), or back of the shoulder (n = 8). no significant interaction between testing session and gender, F(1,
Among men, 25.6% had a tattoo that was easily concealed (visi- 80) = 0.43, p = .516, 2p < .01.
bility ratings of 1 or 2) and 74.4% had a tattoo that was readily visibly A series of 2 × 2 ANOVAs (visibility: easily concealed versus
(visibility ratings of 3 or more). Among women, 43.6% had a tattoo readily visible; gender: women versus men) were conducted to
that was easily concealed and 56.4% had a tattoo that was readily examine the influence of these variables of state body image and
visible. A Chi-squared test showed that women were significantly appearance anxiety at Time 2. Results showed that, for the VAS,
more likely to have a tattoo that was easily concealed compared there was no significant main effect of visibility, F(1, 78) = 1.61,
with men, 2 (1) = 8.24, p = .004. Finally, there was no significant p = .208, 2p = .02, and no significant visibility by gender inter-
difference between women (4.4%) and men (5.6%) in the average action, F(1, 78) = 0.83, p = .365, 2p = .01. There was, however, a
percentage of the body covered by the new tattoo, t(80) = 1.42, significant main effect of gender, with men having significantly
p = .161, d = 0.32. lower appearance dissatisfaction than women (see Table 2), F(1,
78) = 3.99, p = .049, 2p = .05. The same pattern was repeated for
PASTAS scores at Time 2: there was no significant main effect of
visibility, F(1, 78) = 0.13, p = .722, 2p < .01, and no significant inter-
Reasons for Obtaining Tattoos
action, F(1, 78) = 0.05, p = .818, 2p < .01, but there was a significant
main effect of gender (see Table 2), F(1, 78) = 7.28, p = .009, 2p = .09.
Descriptive statistics for reasons for obtaining tattoos are
Examination of the data for satisfaction with participants’ tat-
reported in Table 1. As can be seen, the most common rea-
toos at Time 2 suggests that there were floor effects. Of the total
sons for obtaining tattoos were ‘To express myself’, ‘Because
sample, 90.2% recorded a score of 6 or 7, 8.5% gave a score of 5,
they look good’, and ‘To be unique’. To examine whether there
and 1.2% gave a score of 4. Further analysis showed that, although
were significant gender differences in responses to the items, a
the VAS and PASTAS scores were significantly correlated at Time 2
multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted with
(r = .43, p < .001), neither VAS (r = −.05, p = .632) nor PASTAS scores
participant gender as the independent variable and responses
(r = .15, p = .178) were significantly correlated with satisfaction with
to the 19 reasons for obtaining a tattoo as the dependent vari-
the new tattoo. The most likely explanation for this lack of sig-
ables. The results showed that there was a significant omnibus
nificant findings is that the floor effects in relation to satisfaction
effect of participant gender, F(19, 62) = 1.99, p = .022, 2p = .38.
ratings attenuated any possible association with state appearance
Examination of the analysis of variance (ANOVA) results showed
dissatisfaction or appearance anxiety.
that men had higher scores than women on obtaining a tat-
too because they celebrate an occasion or person, F(1, 80) = 4.25,
p = .042, 2p = .05, and to rebel, F(1, 80) = 5.93, p = .017, 2p = .07. Analyses of Scores at Times 1 and 3
Women, on the other hand, had significantly higher scores than
men on obtaining a tattoo to be fashionable, F(1, 80) = 5.95, p = .017, The same set of analyses as above was repeated for partici-
2p = .07. pants’ trait body appreciation, distinctive appearance investment,
242 V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244

self-attributed uniqueness, self-esteem, and social physique anx- in this instance reflect a greater decrease in satisfaction with the
iety at Times 1 and 3. Results for body appreciation showed that tattoo between Time 2 and Time 3. Bivariate correlations were
there was a significant increase in body appreciation at Time 3, then computed between this difference score and body appreci-
F(1, 80) = 20.48, p < .001, 2p = .20, and that women had significantly ation, distinctive appearance investment, self-attributed need for
lower body appreciation than men at both testing sessions, F(1, uniqueness, social physique anxiety, and self-esteem scores from
80) = 56.48, p < .001, 2p = .41. There was, on the other hand, no Time 3, as well as visibility and size of the tattoo. The only significant
significant interaction between testing session and gender, F(1, correlates that emerged in this analyses were self-esteem (r = body
80) = 0.10, p = .759, 2p < .001. For distinctive appearance invest- appreciation (r = −.37, p = .001) and body appreciation (r = −.24,
ment, there was a significant increase in scores at Time 3, F(1, p = .033).
80) = 13.46, p < .001, 2p = .14, but no significant main effect of gen-
der, F(1, 80) = 0.07, p = .791, 2p < .01, and no significant interaction,
F(1, 80) = 0.04, p = .843, 2p < .01. Discussion
The results for the mixed ANOVA with self-ascribed uniqueness
showed that there was a significant increase in scores at Time 3, F(1, Studies on the psychosocial aspects of tattooing have not previ-
80) = 28.03, p < .001, 2p = .26. There was, on the other hand, no sig- ously examined changes in self- and body-related attitudes among
nificant main effect of gender, F(1, 80) = 2.00, p = .161, 2p = .02, and individuals who have obtained their first tattoo. The present study
sought to overcome this dearth in the literature by examining
no significant interaction, F(1, 80) = 1.23, p = .270, 2p = .02. Results
changes in a range of self- and body-related attitudes immediately
for self-esteem showed that there was a significant improvement
before and after obtaining a tattoo, as well as three weeks later
in self-esteem at Time 3, F(1, 80) = 25.76, p < .001, 2p = .24, and that
when the tattoo would have healed completely. Broadly speak-
women generally had higher self-esteem than men, F(1, 80) = 4.50,
ing, the results of the present work suggest that obtaining a tattoo
p = .037, 2p = .05. There was, on the other hand, no significant
results in positive changes to corporeal attitudes both immediately
interaction between testing session and participant gender, F(1,
after the tattoo is complete as well as three weeks later. Indeed, the
80) = 3.91, p = .052, 2p = .05.
effect sizes of most of the changes were moderate-to-large, sug-
Finally, the ANOVA for social physique anxiety scores showed
gesting that the positive impact of tattooing is fairly pronounced.
that there was a significant main effect of gender, F(1, 80) = 18.87,
Immediately after the tattoo had been completed, both women
p < .001, 2p = .19, but importantly also a significant interaction, F(1,
and men reported significant reductions in dissatisfaction with
80) = 22.98, p < .001, 2p = .22. A paired samples t-test showed that,
their appearance and anxiety over their appearance. This signifi-
among men, there was a significant decrease in social physique anx-
cant decrease in appearance dissatisfaction and anxiety may be a
iety at Time 3, t(42) = 3.86, p < .001, d = 1.19, whereas among women
result of participants focusing on different aspects of their bodies
there was a significant increase at Time 3, t(38) = 3.14, p < .001,
at the two time points when contemplating their appearance (e.g.,
d = 1.02. For SPAS scores, there was no significant main effect of
a greater relative focus on weight and shape at Time 1 and a greater
testing session, F(1, 80) = 0.45, p = .503, 2p = .01.
focus on their tattoo at Time 2), but importantly, participants also
reported significant improvements to body appreciation after three
Further Analyses weeks. As discussed by Benson (2000), in societies where the body
is commodified and work on the body is expected, tattoos may offer
As before, a series of 2 × 2 (visibility and gender as indepen- individuals a means of reclaiming or reappropriating their corpo-
dent variables) ANOVAs were conducted with the trait measures real selves. As she writes, contemporary tattooing is a “statement
above. In all cases, there was no significant main effect of visibility of ownership over the flesh. . . the only possession of the self in
(all Fs = 0.05–1.34, all ps = .250 to .822, all 2p = .01 to .02) and no a world characterized by accelerating commodification and unpre-
significant interactions (all Fs = 0.16–2.33, all ps = .130 to .689, all dictability” (Benson, 2000, p. 205). The present results suggest that,
2p = .01 to .03). A mixed ANOVA showed that there was a sig- not only do tattoos afford individuals a means of reclaiming some
nificant decrease in satisfaction with the tattoos between Times agency of their bodies, but also that this process results in more
2 and 3, F(1, 80) = 58.17, p < .001, 2p = .42, but no significant main positive body image in the short term.
effect of gender, F(1, 80) = 3.08, p = .083, 2p = .04. Importantly, how- Interestingly, however, while there was no gender difference
ever, there was a significant interaction between testing session in state anxiety over appearance (with both women and men
and gender, F(1, 80) = 9.03, p = .004, 2p = .10. Paired samples t-tests reporting a reduction in anxiety), there was a clear gender dif-
showed that the decrease in satisfaction was larger among women, ference after three weeks. Specifically, while men experienced a
t(38) = 6.00, p < .001, d = 1.95, than it was among men, t(42) = 4.52, decrease in social physique anxiety at Time 3 compared to Time
p < .001, d = 1.39. 1, women appeared to experience a significant increase in social
In addition, bivariate correlations were carried out between physique anxiety. One explanation for this finding is based on work
satisfaction with the tattoo and body appreciation, distinctive suggesting that women with tattoos are more likely to elicit nega-
appearance investment, self-attributed uniqueness, self-esteem, tive attributions than their male counterparts (Armstrong, 1991;
social physique anxiety, and likelihood of getting future tattoos Hawkes et al., 2004; Swami & Furnham, 2007; Wohlrab, Stahl,
measured at Time 3. The only significant correlation with satis- et al., 2007). In such a scenario, women may experience greater
faction scores was with self-esteem (r = .31, p < .001) and likelihood appearance anxiety in the slightly longer term precisely because of
of getting future tattoos (r = .27, p = .013). An independent samples their tattoos. Nevertheless, the present results suggest that, while
t-test showed that there was no significant difference in likelihood women may report greater appearance anxiety, they are also more
of getting future tattoos between women (M = 4.64, SD = 1.99) and respectful and appreciative of their bodies. The negotiation of these
men (M = 4.67, SD = 1.74), t(80) = 0.08, p = .936, d = 0.02. In terms of different experiences is one aspect of tattooing that will require
the distribution of responses on the likelihood of getting future tat- further attention.
toos, the majority of participants gave scores of 5 or above (56.1%). The present results also showed that, at Time 3, both women
Finally, to examine potential correlates of the decrease in tat- and men reported significant increases in distinctive appearance
too satisfaction scores between Time 2 and Time 3, a difference investment and self-ascribed uniqueness. This set of results cor-
score was first computed by substracting Time 3 satisfaction scores roborates the work of Tiggemann and colleagues (Tiggemann &
from Time 2 satisfaction scores (M = 1.32, SD = 1.67). Higher scores Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011), who suggested that
V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244 243

tattoos allow individuals to achieve a sense of distinctiveness, cies between expectations and realities, such as that respondents’
particularly in the appearance domain. In contrast to their work, selected design may not have been realised in the manner they
which showed that tattooed individuals have higher scores on expected) (Armstrong, Murphy, Sallee, & Watson, 2000; Armstrong
need for uniqueness and distinctive appearance investment than & Pace-Murphy, 1997; Sanders, 1985). This is an aspect of the cur-
non-tattooed individuals, the present work showed that the act rent study that needs further research.
of obtaining a tattoo increased an individual’s sense of unique- A number of limitations of the present study need to be
ness prospectively. The present results are important because they acknowledged. First, although steps were taken to mask the study’s
highlight self-expression and a sense of identity or uniqueness as objectives (e.g., by inviting participants to take part in a study on
outcomes, rather than just predictors, of obtaining a tattoo. consumer behaviours and through the use of filler scales), it is quite
In the view of Tiggemann and Golder (2006), by achieving a possible that participants discerned the nature of the study and
moderate sense of perceived self-distinctiveness, tattooed indi- responded in socially desirable ways. The likelihood of this occur-
viduals may experience more positive self-regard. That is, tattoos ring may have been heightened by the fact that the questionnaire
appear to enhance perceptions that the self is unique in compari- was initially administered in a tattoo parlour and by an experi-
son to others, which in turn may improve self-esteem. Indeed, the menter with visible tattoos. Second, the sample size in the present
present study found that obtaining a tattoo resulted in a significant study was relatively small, although the select nature of the sam-
improvement in self-esteem over a three-week period among both pling population (i.e., British residents seeking to obtain their first
women and men. This finding is notable because it highlights the tattoo at a single tattoo parlour) should be noted. In a similar vein,
positive impact of obtaining a tattoo on an individual’s overall sense the sample in the present work was one of convenience and it is
of self. Because of the nature of the present design, however, it is not not clear whether the recruitment technique introduced sampling
possible to say whether this improvement in self-esteem is a result biases (e.g., in comparison to individuals who visited the tattoo
of obtaining a tattoo specifically or due to improvements in related parlour but did not make an appointment).
domains, such as greater body appreciation (which is known to be Future work could improve on the present design by including
associated with self-esteem; Swami et al., 2008). a wider array of measures that tap an individual’s body attitudes
A number of other aspects of the present results are worthy (e.g., internalisation of societal ideals of appearance) or personal-
of comment. First, the current results showed that there were no ity (e.g., the Big Five personality traits). Additionally, although the
significant differences in any of the included variables between present work relied on a previously used scale to measure motiva-
individuals with easily concealed and readily visible tattoos. This tions for obtaining a tattoo (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006), this scale
finding is consistent with previous work among tattooed individ- may not tap the full range of potential motivations for obtaining a
uals (Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011) and suggests that the corporeal first tattoo. For instance, the scale does not contain items relating
meaning of obtaining a first tattoo is more important than its visi- to obtaining a tattoo in order to understand traumatic experiences,
bility. This is not to say that visibility of a tattoo may not play a more to aid healing processes (Jeffreys, 2000), or for religious or spiritual
important role in shaping body attitudes among, say, heavily tat- reasons (Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2004). Certain items in
tooed individuals (Sweetman, 1999). For Tiggemann and Hopkins this scale may also need to be carefully modified in order to avoid
(2011), the lack of differences between individuals with readily any negative connotation (e.g., the item ‘To be fashionable’ may
concealed and visible tattoos indicates that the act of obtaining a hold negative connotations, particularly for individuals involved in
tattoo may not necessarily be about appearance per se. counter-cultures, despite its intended positive meaning).
Additionally, the present study showed that two primary moti- Taking into account the above limitations, the present work
vations for obtaining a first tattoo were to express one’s self suggests that the act of obtaining a tattoo is associated with
and to be unique. This provides further evidence for Tiggemann improvements in self- and body-related attitudes, at least in the
and Golder’s (2006) suggestion that tattoos allow an individual short term. Although the present work has begun the task of exam-
to achieve distinctiveness in the appearance domain. Another ining prospective changes as a result of obtaining a tattoo, there
strongly rated motivation for obtaining a tattoo was because they remains scope for future studies in the area. For instance, future
look good, which is consistent with previous findings (Forbes, 2001; work could examine changes in self- and body-related attitudes
Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011). Although the latter result may be over a much longer period of time. Such work should also examine
interpreted as evidence of tattoos being mere ‘fashion accessories’ changes in satisfaction with the first tattoo as individuals obtain
(Falk, 1995; Steele, 1996), such a view is problematic because of the further body art and also whether tattoo removal results in any
permanence, planning, and pain involved in tattooing (Sweetman, decrease in perceived uniqueness or changes in body-related atti-
1999), as well as the role of tattoos in self-expression. It is also tudes more generally (Armstrong, Roberts, Koch, Saunders, Owen, &
worth noting that obtaining a tattoo ‘to be fashionable’ was rated Anderson, 2008). The present work suggests that tattooed individ-
poorly in the present study, as well as in previous work (Tiggemann uals who obtain a first tattoo experience significant changes in their
& Hopkins, 2011). self- and body-related attitudes in the short term, but it remains to
Finally, the present results showed that a majority of partici- be seen whether these positive changes are maintained over the
pants self-reported as being likely to obtain further tattoos in the longer term.
future. Likelihood of obtaining future tattoos was correlated with
satisfaction with their first tattoo at Time 3, although there was a
significant decrease in satisfaction with tattoos over time. The latter References
finding is of note because it highlights the fact that some partici-
Anderson, R. R. (2001). Regarding tattoos: Is that sunlight, or an oncoming train at
pants, at least, were not entirely satisfied with their tattoo. Reasons the end of the tunnel? Archives of Dermatology, 137, 210–212.
for this may include poorer body appreciation and self-esteem at Anderson, R. R. (2006). Commentary: Tattoos and body piercing. Journal of the Amer-
Time 3, which suggests complex associations between corporeal ican Academy of Dermatology, 55, 422.
Armstrong, M. L. (1991). Career-oriented women with tattoos image. Journal of
experiences, self-perceptions, and tattoo possession. More gener-
Nursing Scholarship, 23, 215–220.
ally, reduced satisfaction with the tattoo may be a result of purchase Armstrong, M. L., Murphy, K. P., Sallee, A. S., & Watson, M. G. (2000). Tattooed army
risks (e.g., pain, expense, problems with the artist, or poor knowl- soldiers: Examining the incidence, behavior, and risk. Military Medicine, 165,
edge about the healing process, which requires a good deal of 37–40.
Armstrong, M. L., & Pace-Murphy, K. (1997). Tattooing: Another risk-behavior in
effort and commitment on the part of the tattooed individual once adolescents warranting national health teaching. Applied Nursing Research, 10,
they leave the tattoo parlour) or possession risks (e.g., discrepan- 181–189.
244 V. Swami / Body Image 8 (2011) 237–244

Armstrong, M. L., Roberts, A. E., Koch, J. R., Saunders, J. C., Owen, D. C., & Anderson, Kosut, M. (2006). An ironic fad: The commodification and consumption of tattoos.
R. R. (2008). Motivation for contemporary tattoo removal: A shift in identity. The Journal of Popular Culture, 39, 1035–1048.
Archives of Dermatology, 144, 878–884. Laumann, A. E., & Derick, A. J. (2006). Tattoos and body piercings in the United States:
Armstrong, M. L., Roberts, A. E., Owen, D. C., & Koch, J. R. (2004a). Contemporary A national data set. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 55, 413–421.
college students and body piercing. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35, 58–61. Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997). Individual differences in the pursuit of self-uniqueness
Armstrong, M. L., Roberts, A. E., Owen, D. C., & Koch, J. R. (2004b). Toward building a through consumption. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1861–1883.
composite of college student influences with body art. Issues in Comprehensive Lynn, M., & Snyder, C. R. (2002). Uniqueness seeking. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez
Pediatric Nursing, 27, 277–295. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 395–419). New York, NY: Oxford
Avalos, L., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale: University Press.
Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2, 285–297. Makkai, T., & McAllister, I. (2001). Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing in the
Bekhor, L., Bekhor, P. S., & Gandrabur, M. (1995). Employer attitudes toward persons Australian community. Communicable Diseases Intelligence, 25, 67–72.
with visible tattoos. Australian Journal of Dermatology, 36, 75–77. Mayers, L. B., & Chiffriller, S. H. (2008). Body art (body piercing and tattooing) among
Benson, S. (2000). Inscriptions of the self: Reflections on tattooing and piercing in undergraduate university students: Then and now. Journal of Adolescent Health,
contemporary Euro-America. In J. Caplan (Ed.), Written on the body: The tat- 42, 201–203.
too in European and American history. (pp. 234–254). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Mayers, L. B., Judelson, D. A., Moriarty, B. W., & Rundell, K. W. (2002). Prevalence
University Press. of body art (body piercing and tattooing) in university undergraduates and
Blanchard, M. (1994). Post-bourgeois tattoo: Reflections on skin writing in late cap- incidence of medical complications. Mayo Clinical Proceedings, 77, 29–34.
italist societies. In L. Taylor (Ed.), Visualizing theory: Selected essays from V.A.R., Millner, V. S., & Eichold, B. H. (2001). Body piercing and tattooing perspectives.
1990–1994. New York, NY: Routledge. Clinical Nursing Research, 10, 424–441.
Braithwaite, R., Robillard, A., Woodring, T., Stephens, T., & Arriola, J. (2001). Tattoo- Nathanson, C., Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2006). Personality and misconduct
ing and body piercing among adolescent detainees: Relationship to alcohol and correlates of body modification and other cultural deviance markers. Journal of
other drug use. Journal of Substance Abuse, 13, 5–16. Research in Personality, 40, 779–802.
Brooks, T. L., Woods, E. R., Knight, J. R., & Shrier, L. A. (2003). Body modification and Reed, D. L., Thompson, J. K., Brannick, M. T., & Sacco, W. P. (1991). Development and
substance use in adolescents: Is there a link? Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, validation of the Physical Appearance State and Trait Anxiety Scale (PASTAS).
44–49. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 323–332.
Burger, T. D., & Finkel, D. (2002). Relationships between body modifications and very Roberti, J. W., & Storch, E. A. (2005). Psychosocial adjustment of college students
high risk behaviors in a college population. College Student Journal, 36, 203–213. with tattoos and piercings. Journal of College Counseling, 8, 14–19.
Carroll, S. T., Riffenburgh, R. H., Roberts, T. A., & Myhre, E. B. (2002). Tattoos and Roberti, J. W., Storch, E. A., & Bravata, E. A. (2004). Sensation seeking, exposure to psy-
body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk taking behaviors. Pediatrics, 109, chological stressors, and body modifications in a college population. Personality
1021–1027. and Individual Differences, 37, 1167–1177.
Caplan, J. (Ed.), (2000). Written on the body: The tattoo in European and American Roberts, T. A., & Ryan, S. A. (2002). Tattooing and high-risk behavior in adolescents.
history Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Pediatrics, 110, 1058–1063.
Cattarin, J. A., Thompson, J. K., Thomas, C. M., & Williams, R. (2000). Body image, Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
mood, and televised images of attractiveness: The role of social comparison. University Press.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 220–239. Rubin, A. (1988). Marks of civilization. Los Angeles, CA: Museum of Cultural History.
Copes, J. H., & Forsyth, C. J. (1993). The tattoo: A social psychological explanation. Sanders, C. R. (1985). Tattoo consumption: Risk and regret in the purchase of a
International Review of Modern Sociology, 23, 83–89. socially marginal service. In E. Hirschman & M. Holbrook (Eds.), Advances in
Degelman, D., & Price, N. D. (2002). Tattoos and ratings of personal characteristics. consumer research (pp. 17–22). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.
Psychological Reports, 90, 507–514. Sanders, C. R. (1989). Customizing the body: The art and culture of tattooing. Philadel-
DeMello, M. (1995). Not just for bikers anymore: Popular representations of Amer- phia, PA: Temple University Press.
ican tattooing. Journal of Popular Culture, 29, 37–52. Schildkrout, E. (2004). Inscribing the body. Annual Review of Anthropology, 33,
DeMello, M. (2000). Bodies of inscription: A cultural history of the modern tattoo com- 319–344.
munity. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Steele, V. (1996). Fetish: Fashion, sex, and power. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Deschesnes, M., Fines, P., & Demers, S. (2006). Are tattooing and body piercing Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Kastner, C., Voracek, M., & Swami, V. (2010). Prevalence
indicators of risk-taking behaviours among high school students? Journal of and acceptance of tattoos and piercings: A survey of young adults from the
Adolescence, 29, 379–393. southern German-speaking area of Central Europe. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Drews, D. R., Allison, C. K., & Probst, J. R. (2000). Behavioral and self-concept differ- 110, 1065–1074.
ences in tattooed and nontattooed college students. Psychological Reports, 86, Stirn, A., Hinz, A., & Brahler, E. (2006). Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing
475–481. in Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking
Durkin, K., & Houghton, S. (2000). Children’s and adolescent’s stereotypes of tattooed among tattooed and body-pierced individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
people as delinquents. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 5, 153–164. 60, 531–534.
Falk, P. (1995). Written in the flesh. Body and Society, 1, 95–106. Stuppy, D. J., Armstrong, M. L., & Casals-Ariet, C. (1998). Attitudes of health care
Fisher, J. A. (2002). Tattooing the body, marking culture. Body and Society, 8, 91–107. providers and students toward tattooed people. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 27,
Forbes, G. B. (2001). College students with tattoos and piercings: Motives, family 1165–1170.
experiences, personality factors and perception by others. Psychological Reports, Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2007). Unattractive, promiscuous, and heavy drinkers:
89, 774–796. Perceptions of women with tattoos. Body Image, 4, 343–352.
Fredrick, C. M., & Bradley, K. A. (2000). A different kind of normal? Psychological and Swami, V., & Harris, A. S. (2011). Body art (tattoos and piercings). To appear. In T. Cash
motivational characteristics of young adult tattooers and body piercers. North (Ed.), Encylopedia of human appearance and body image. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
American Journal of Psychology, 2, 379–389. Swami, V., Stieger, S., Haubner, T., & Voracek, M. (2008). Translation and validation
Greif, J., Hewitt, W., & Armstrong, M. L. (1999). Tattooing and body pierc- of the German Body Appreciation Scale. Body Image, 5, 122–127.
ing: Body art practices among college students. Clinical Nursing Research, 8, Sweetman, P. (1999). Anchoring the (post-modern) self? Body modification, fashion,
368–385. and identity. Body and Society, 5, 51–76.
Hart, E. H., Leary, M. R., & Rajeski, W. J. (1989). The measurement of social physique Tate, J. C., & Shelton, B. L. (2008). Personality correlates of tattooing and body piercing
anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 94–104. in a college sample: The kids are alright. Personality and Individual Differences,
Hawkes, D., Senn, C. Y., & Thorn, C. (2004). Factors that influence attitudes toward 45, 281–285.
women with tattoos. Sex Roles, 50, 593–604. Tiggemann, M., & Golder, F. (2006). Tattooing: An expression of uniqueness in the
Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness appearance domain. Body Image, 3, 309–315.
and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of Social and Tiggemann, M., & Hopkins, S. (2011). Tattoos and piercings: Bodily expressions of
Clinical Psychology, 14, 325–338. uniqueness? Body Image, 8, 245–250.
Huxley, C., & Grogan, S. (2005). Tattooing, piercing, health behaviors, and health Whiteside-Mansell, L., & Corwyn, R. F. (2003). Mean and covariance structures anal-
value. Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 831–841. yses: An examination of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale among adolescents
Jeffreys, S. (2000). ‘Body art’ and social status: Cutting, tattooing, and piercing from and adults. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 163–173.
a feminist perspective. Feminism and Psychology, 10, 409–429. Wohlrab, S., Fink, B., Kappeler, P. M., & Brewer, G. (2009). Differences in personality
Koch, J. R., Roberts, A. E., Armstrong, M. L., & Owen, D. C. (2004). Correlations of attributions toward tattooed and non-tattooed virtual human characters. Journal
religious belief and practice on college students’ tattoo-related behavior. Psy- of Individual Differences, 30, 1–5.
chological Reports, 94, 425–430. Wohlrab, S., Stahl, J., & Kappeler, P. M. (2007). Modifying the body: Motivations for
Koch, J. R., Roberts, A. E., Armstrong, M. L., & Owen, D. (2005). College students, getting tattooed and pierced. Body Image, 4, 87–95.
tattoos, and sexual activity. Psychological Reports, 97, 887–890. Wohlrab, S., Stahl, J., Rammsayer, T., & Kappeler, P. M. (2007). Differences in person-
Koch, J. R., Roberts, A. E., Armstrong, M. L., & Owen, D. C. (2010). Body art, deviance, ality characteristics between body modified and nonmodified individuals and
and American college students. Social Science Journal, 47, 151–161. possible evolutionary implications. European Journal of Personality, 21, 931–951.

You might also like