Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY 30:515–527 (2013)

Review
COGNITIVE DEFICITS AND FUNCTIONAL OUTCOMES
IN MAJOR DEPRESSIVE DISORDER: DETERMINANTS,
SUBSTRATES, AND TREATMENT INTERVENTIONS
Roger S. McIntyre, MD, FRCPC,1,2,3,4 ∗ Danielle S. Cha, HBSc,3,4 Joanna K. Soczynska, HBSc,3,4
Hanna O. Woldeyohannes, HBSc,2 Laura Ashley Gallaugher, HBSc,2 Paul Kudlow, BSc, MD,2 Mohammad
Alsuwaidan, MD, MPH, FRCPC, Dip ABPN,3
and Anusha Baskaran, HBSc, MSc2,5

Background: Few reports have aimed to describe the mediational effect of cog-
nitive deficits on functional outcomes in major depressive disorder (MDD), and
relatively few interventions are demonstrated to mitigate cognitive deficits in
MDD. Methods: Studies enrolling subjects between the ages of 18–65 were se-
lected for review. Bibliographies from identified articles were reviewed to identify
additional original reports aligned with our objectives. Results: Cognitive deficits
in MDD are consistent, replicable, nonspecific, and clinically significant. The ag-
gregated estimated effect size of cognitive deficits in MDD is small to medium.
Pronounced deficits in executive function (≥1 SD below the normative mean) are
evident in ∼20–30% of individuals with MDD). Other replicated abnormalities
are in the domains of working memory, attention, and psychomotor processing
speed. Mediational studies indicate that cognitive deficits may account for the
largest percentage of variance with respect to the link between psychosocial dys-
function (notably workforce performance) and MDD. No conventional antide-
pressant has been sufficiently studied and/or demonstrated robust procognitive
effects in MDD. Conclusions: Cognitive deficits in MDD are a principal me-
diator of psychosocial impairment, notably workforce performance. The hazards
posed by cognitive deficits in MDD underscore the need to identify a consensus-
based neurocognitive battery for research and clinical purposes. Interventions
(pharmacological, behavioral, neuromodulatory) that engage multiple physio-
logical systems implicated in cognitive deficits hold promise to reduce, reverse,
and prevent cognitive deficits. Depression and Anxiety 30:515–527, 2013.

C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Key words: major depressive disorder; cognitive dysfunction; cognitive deficits;


dementia; duloxetine; vortioxetine; functional outcome

1 Institute
of Medical Science, University of Toronto, Toronto, Contract grant sponsor: Takeda Pharmaceutical Company, Ltd.
Canada ∗ Correspondence to: Roger S. McIntyre, MD, FRCPC, Mood Dis-
2 Mood Disorders Psychopharmacology Unit, University Health
orders Psychopharmacology Unit, University Health Network, Uni-
Network, Toronto, Canada versity of Toronto, 399 Bathurst Street, Toronto, ON, Canada M5T
3 Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, 2S8. E-mail: roger.mcintyre@uhn.ca
Canada Received for publication 5 November 2012; Revised 4 January
4 Department of Pharmacology, University of Toronto, Toronto, 2013; Accepted 4 January 2013
Canada
5 Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University,
DOI 10.1002/da.22063
Published online 6 May 2013 in Wiley Online Library
Kingston Canada (wileyonlinelibrary.com).


C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
516 McIntyre et al.

INTRODUCTION fundamentally re-think and perhaps redefine how “re-


mission” should be conceptualized.[6] Studies assessing
M ajor depressive disorder (MDD) is a multidimen- temporality of onset provide supporting evidence that
sional mental disorder affecting approximately one in in some individuals, cognitive dysfunction may predate
seven individuals at some time in their life.[1] MDD is the onset of the first MDE.[12] Cognitive dysfunction
associated with a high rate of nonrecovery and recur- can be disaggregated into two broad interrelated cate-
rence, with chronicity rates estimated at approximately gories: (1) deficits in one or more cognitive domain(s)
20%.[2] The estimated annual costs attributable to MDD or (2) cognitive bias: representing attentional alloca-
are approximately $83 billion, with indirect costs due to tion toward negatively valenced stimuli and/or aberrant
decreased psychosocial function (notably workforce per- interpretation of social cues. It could be further sur-
formance) being a major contributor.[3] For example, it is mised that cognitive bias represents a trait characteristic
estimated that MDD is associated with an annual loss of in individuals who are temperamentally predisposed to
27.2 workdays per ill worker.[4] The human capital cost MDD (e.g., neuroticism). The preponderance of evi-
attributable to MDD provides the impetus for identi- dence indicates that cognitive deficits are a consequence
fying determinants of functional impairment. Available and/or residual phenomenon of MDD and in relatively
evidence indicates that cognitive dysfunction is a crit- fewer cases may be an antecedent to MDD. (The no-
ical mediator of adverse psychosocial outcomes in this tion that cognitive deficits predate the onset of MDD
population.[5–7] is more frequently reported in older individuals with
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis- MDD).
orders – Fourth Edition – Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) Relatively few studies have evaluated the effect
identifies cognitive impairment (i.e., poor concentra- of conventional antidepressants on cognitive perfor-
tion or indecisiveness) as a criterion item of a major mance in nongeriatric, adult MDD samples. Selective
depressive episode (MDE). Cognitive complaints dur- serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), selective nore-
ing the symptomatic and remitted phases are commonly pinephrine inhibitors (SNRIs), dopamine modulators
reported by individuals with MDD.[3, 4, 6, 8] When com- (bupropion), and norepinephrine inhibitors (reboxe-
pared to other severe mental disorders (e.g., schizophre- tine) have been reported to improve cognitive per-
nia, bipolar disorder), relatively fewer studies have formance in adults with MDD.[13–18] There is also
critically reviewed the affected cognitive domains and a paucity of evidence comparing the effects of dif-
estimated magnitude of cognitive impairment in MDD. ferent classes of antidepressants.[16–18] Questions per-
In addition, there have been fewer original reports taining to the degree to which the improvement in
that have primarily aimed to parse out the neurobi- cognitive deficits can be dissociated from the ef-
ological substrate(s) of cognitive dysfunction in this fect of other MDD psychopathology domains (e.g.,
population, to determine the contribution of cognitive chronicity, subtype of MDD, number and duration of
dysfunction to psychosocial impairment, and to primar- MDEs) and/or comorbidities have been insufficiently
ily evaluate the procognitive effects of treatment (regard- addressed.
less of modality). The overarching aim of this review is to describe the
Replicated evidence indicates that cognitive dysfunc- role of cognitive dysfunction as a determinant of func-
tion causes and maintains psychosocial impairment in tional outcome in MDD. Toward this aim, we succinctly
severe mental disorders.[5–7] Moreover, deficits in cog- review (1) cognitive deficits and their determinants in
nitive function in adults under the age of 65 with MDD adults with MDD 65 years old or younger (i.e., 18–65
cannot be sufficiently explained by age-related cogni- years) and (2) the effect of treatment on cognitive per-
tive changes. Available evidence suggests that cognitive formance. The secondary objectives of this review are to
dysfunction is a critical determinant of functional out- identify underlying substrate(s) that subserve cognitive
come in MDD.[7] It also remains to be determined if deficits and to propose treatment approaches that pri-
the neurobiological substrate(s) that subserve cognitive marily aim to mitigate, reverse, and prevent deficits in
dysfunction in MDD are discrete and/or if they overlap cognitive function.
with substrate(s) implicated in other mental disorders.
Questions regarding which, if any, treatment modality is
more effective in mitigating cognitive deficits, enhancing
cognitive function and/or preventing their occurrence in METHOD
MDD remain unanswered. A general impression, albeit
based on relatively few empirical studies, is that cogni- We conducted a review of computerized databases (i.e., PubMed,
tive deficits are suboptimally treated with conventional Google Scholar) from 1980 to 2012. “MDD” was cross-referenced
with the following terms: cognitive dysfunction, cognitive deficits, de-
treatment approaches [e.g., selective serotonin reuptake
mentia, and functional outcome. Studies enrolling subjects between
inhibitors (SSRIs)].[9–11] the ages of 18–65 were primarily selected for review: select studies in
The persistence of cognitive dysfunction in MDD late life depression in the absence of studies in adults 65 years of age or
beyond resolution of the acute episode suggests that less were also reviewed were also reviewed. Bibliographies from iden-
it may represent a trait or a residual phenomenon in tified articles were also reviewed in order to identify any other original
many individuals. Moreover, it provides the impetus to reports that were aligned with the objectives of this paper.

Depression and Anxiety


Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 517

RESULTS TABLE 1. Common neurocognitive tests

NEUROCOGNITIVE TESTS Cognitive domain Neurocognitive tests


Table 1 contains a list of conventional instru- Executive function • Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
ments and batteries frequently administered for as- (concept • Trail-Making Test Part B (TMT B)
sessing cognitive function. The available tests measure formation, • Stroop Colour-Word Interference Test
intellectual functioning, working memory, verbal learn- abstraction, set (SCWT)
ing and memory, attention and information processing shifting, set • Categories Test
speed, executive function, psychomotor performance, maintenance, • Block Design (WAIS-R)
planning, • Picture Completion (WAIS-R)
and global cognitive function. None of the foregoing
self-monitoring, • Concept Shifting Task (CST)
cognitive measures have been specifically developed to divided attention) • Tower of London (TOL; CANTAB)
evaluate cognitive performance and/or its functional im- • Stockings of Cambridge (SOC; CANTAB)
plications in the workforce. • Intra/Extradimensional Shift Test
(CANTAB; IED)
• Spatial Span (SSP; CANTAB)
COGNITIVE DEFICITS IN MDD • Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT)
The interpretation of study outcomes as they pertain • Verbal Fluency–Letter Fluency & Category
to cognitive deficits in MDD begins with a review of Fluency
methodological factors that may affect the interpreta- • Controlled Oral Word Association Test
(COWAT)
tion of the data. First, available studies are often inclu-
• The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function
sive of heterogeneous sample compositions that have in- System (D-KEFS)
cluded mixed ages, disparate definitions of MDD, and Attention and • Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST;
illness characteristics (e.g., age at onset, duration, num- processing speed WAIS-R)
ber of episodes, depression subtype, presence or absence • Digit Span Forwards and Backwards
of psychotic features) as well as treatments. Additionally, (WAIS-R)
the co-occurrence of medical and/or psychiatric condi- • Continuous Performance Task (CPT)
tions has not always been controlled. The relevance of • Reaction Time (RTI; CANTAB)
this latter observation is underscored by evidence in- • Choice Reaction Time (CRT; CANTAB)
dicating that comorbid conditions in MDD may exert • Simple Reaction Time (SRT; CANTAB)
• Trail-Making Test Part A (TMT A)
direct effects on cognitive performance (e.g., attention
• Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
deficit/hyperactivity disorder, obesity).[19, 20] For exam- (PASAT)
ples of determinants of cognitive deficits in MDD, see • Serial Sevens Subtraction Test (SSST)
Table 2. Working memory • Arithmetic (WAIS-R)
Most studies evaluating cognitive dysfunction in • Digit Span Forwards and Backwards
MDD have enrolled symptomatic and treated individ- (WAIS-R)
uals whereas relatively fewer studies enrolled “asymp- • Delayed Recognition Span Test (DRST)
tomatic” and/or individuals not receiving medication. • Spatial Working Memory (SWM;
Most studies have evaluated cognitive function follow- CANTAB)
ing the onset of MDD. However, there is little docu- • Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS; WMS-R)
• Logical Memory (WMS-R)
mentation reporting on cognitive performance prior to
• n-Back Test
the onset of an index MDE. A separate understudied is- Verbal learning and • California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)
sue relates to the impact of early childhood adversity on memory • Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
cognitive measures in adult samples with MDD and its (RAVLT)
moderational effect.[21] • Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test
A notable factor affecting interpretation of study re- (RBMT)
sults is the heterogeneity of neurocognitive tests and bat- • Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised
teries. Most available studies include tests that evaluate (HVLT-R)
performance across several principal cognitive domains • Logical Memory (WMS-R)
(see Table 1). There is, however, no accepted “gold • Verbal Paired Associates (VPA; WMS-R)
• Visual Verbal Learning Test (VVLT)
standard” psychometric of cognitive function in MDD
• Digit Span Forwards and Backwards
that has been embraced by the research/clinical com- (WAIS-R)
munity. In contradistinction, there have been efforts to • Luria Verbal Learning Test (LVLT)
agree upon a common set of tests for individuals with • Serial Sevens Subtraction Test (SSST)
primary psychotic disorders for both descriptive and • Verbal Recognition Memory Test (VRM;
interventional research [e.g., measurement and treat- CANTAB)
ment in research to improve cognition in schizophrenia Visual learning and • Visual Reproduction (WMS-R)
(MATRICS)]. In the clinical setting, subjective memory • Benton Visual Retention Test (VRT)
complaints of cognitive deficits are common.[22–24] • Benton Visual Form Discrimination (VFD)
Notwithstanding the ubiquity of cognitive complaints,
Depression and Anxiety
518 McIntyre et al.

TABLE 1. Continued TABLE 2. Determinants of cognitive deficits in major


depressive disorder (MDD)
Cognitive domain Neurocognitive tests
Age[86]
• Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test Age at onset[87]
(ROCF) Educational attainment[88]
• Kimura’s Recurring Figures Test (RFT) Baseline depression severity[89]
• Visual Verbal Learning Test (VVLT) MDD subtype[90]
• Pattern Recognition Memory (PRM; Symptomatic status (i.e., remission vs. nonremission)[14, 30, 91]
CANTAB) Psychiatric comorbidity[92]
• Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM; Medical comorbidity[93, 94]
CANTAB) Illness duration[9]
• Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS; Episode frequency[32]
CANTAB) Treatment[66]
• Paired Associates Learning (PAL; Childhood adversity[95, 96]
CANTAB)
• Matching Familiar Figures Test 2
(MFFT-20)
Language and verbal • Controlled Oral Word Association Test the congruence between subjectively reported and ob-
comprehension (COWAT) jectively measured cognitive deficits is not consistent in
• Verbal Fluency–Category Fluency & Letter MDD.[25]
Fluency
A common convention among extant studies is the
• Similarities (WAIS-R)
• Vocabulary (WAIS-R)
reporting and comparing of group means rather than
• Information (WAIS-R) an emphasis on individuals who fall below a pre-
• Comprehension (WAIS-R) specified cut score for cognitive dysfunction [i.e., 1–
• Token Test 2 standard deviations (SD) below the norm].[26] This
Visuospatial/ • Judgement of Line Orientation (JOLO) convention may inadvertently decrease the “assay sensi-
perceptual • Benton Visual Form Discrimination (VFD) tivity” and not provide a sufficient estimate of the mag-
processing • Block Design (WAIS-R) nitude of deficits, and/or the functional implications, in
• Visuospatial Span Forwards and Backwards MDD. Moreover, the use of mean scores in parametric
(WMS-R) statistics assumes normal distribution of the dependent
Brief mental status • Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE)
variable. As the mean scores influence extreme values,
General intelligence • Raven’s Progressive Matrices
• Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised
the emphasis on mean scores rather than the SD from
(WAIS-R) the normative population mean could underestimate the
• National Adult Reading Test (NART) magnitude of deficits that affect a subgroup of individu-
• Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) als with MDD.[26] For example, individuals with MDD
• Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-3 (TONI-3) fall, on average, 0.5–1 SD below the normal population
Cognitive battery • Cambridge Neuropsychological Test mean.[14]
Automated Battery (CANTAB) Notwithstanding it is reported that approximately 25–
• Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised 50% of patients with MDD exhibit deficits that are
(WMS-R) more than 1 SD below the mean on at least one cog-
• Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised
nitive domain and as many as 48% score more than 2
(WAIS-R)
• Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT)
SD below the mean.[26] A summary statement regarding
• California Computerised Assessment cognitive deficits in MDD is that cognitive deficits are
Package (CalCAP) consistent, replicable, nonspecific, clinically significant,
• CNS Vital Signs and of small to medium in effect size. The most repli-
• Massachusetts General Hospital Cognitive cated cognitive deficits are in the domains of executive
and Physical Functioning Questionnaire function, working memory, attention, as well as general
(CPFQ) and psychomotor processing speed (e.g., sensorimotor,
• The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function cognitive).[27] A limitation of the approach that defines
System (D-KEFS) deficits based on deviations from the standard popula-
Psychomotor • Finger Tapping
tion means is that it is not inclusive of individuals who
performance • Grooved Pegboard Test
• Purdue Pegs
remain above the mean, possibly due to cognitive re-
Decision making • The Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT; serve (e.g., education, occupation, intelligence),[28] but
and response CANTAB) report deterioration compared with baseline. It could be
control • Information Sampling Task (IST; hypothesized that these “uncaptured” deficits likewise
CANTAB) contribute significantly to relative functional decline in
• Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT; these individuals.
CANTAB) The conventional assumption has been that cognitive
Induction • Big/Little Circle (BLC; CANTAB) deficits in MDD are a consequence or a residual fea-
ture of an acute MDE. For example, individuals with
Depression and Anxiety
Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 519

remitted MDD often exhibit persisting cognitive deficits composition, treatment regimens, patterns of comorbid-
in measures of attention, executive function, and verbal ity, definitions of depression and cognition measures, as
memory.[29, 30] There have been relatively few studies well as insufficient adjustment for early childhood ad-
evaluating the temporality of onset, wherein cognitive versity and colinearity, are major methodological lim-
deficits are documented prior to the onset of MDD. itations. Notwithstanding cognitive deficits in MDD
Notwithstanding the paucity of data, a longitudinal, are a consistent observation exhibiting trait, state, and
population-based study has reported a significant associ- progressive features.
ation between episodic memory function and emergent
depressive symptoms during a 2-year follow-up.[31]
A separate study that broadly aimed to identify pre- COGNITIVE DEFICITS IN MDD: LINK TO
morbid markers of depression in a population-based FUNCTIONAL OUTCOME
sample of nondepressed individuals (N = 708) evalu- Results from the Global Alliance and Chronic Disease
ated prospectively over a 3-year period reported that Report extend and replicate the observation that MDD
female gender, lower educational attainment, low so- is associated with the highest level of disability-adjusted
cial support, financial strain, and poor episodic memory life years among all mental, neurological, and substance
(but not verbal fluency, psychomotor speed, or men- use disorders.[36] A significant component of the overall
tal speed) were associated with an increased risk of disability and cost associated with depression relates to
depression.[12] These results suggest that hippocampal- impaired workplace performance.[4]
dependent memory may be abnormal prior to the clinical The effect of neurocognitive deficits as a mediator
presentation of depression.[12] The persistence of cog- of disability in individuals with MDD has been insuffi-
nitive deficits in asymptomatic individuals as well as the ciently studied. Jaeger et al. reported that measures of at-
identification of cognitive deficits prior to the presence tention, ideational fluency, nonverbal (i.e., visuo-spatial)
of symptomatic depression indicates that in many in- and learning domains were highly associated with dis-
dividuals, cognitive deficits can be conceptualized as a ability 6 months following hospitalization for a MDE in
core deficit rather than an epiphenomenon of a MDE a relatively small cohort (N = 48) of adults with MDD.[5]
(i.e., secondary to diminished initiative and effortful Naismith et al. sought to determine whether sub-
processing). jective and/or objective measures of cognitive per-
Replicated evidence indicates that episode frequency formance were related to measures of psychomotor
and illness duration are significantly associated with performance.[25] A small sample (N = 21) of adults
the presence of cognitive deficits.[32, 33] For example, treated for MDD exhibited a moderate relationship
a large study (N = 8,229) of outpatients with MDD between objectively measured psychomotor speed and
reported that declarative memory (measured with the physical disability.[25] Functional disability was mod-
delayed paragraph recall index from the Wechsler erately correlated to objective measures of memory
Memory Scale—Revised), a surrogate marker of hip- retention.[25] Measures of mental health disability (i.e.,
pocampal function, decreases approximately 2–3% with Brief Disability Questionnaire Mental Health and SF-12
each MDE, up to four episodes.[32] This decrement Well-Being) were not associated with any neuropsycho-
in memory parallels results from morphometric stud- logical measure.[25] These results replicate and extend
ies that have documented volumetric reduction in the observations of an association between cognitive deficits
hippocampus in MDD with a more pronounced de- and functional outcomes in a separate small sample of
crease observed during the first several episodes.[32, 34] adults with MDD as well as individuals with late-life
Current illness severity was the major determinant of MDD.[29, 37, 38]
performance, as opposed to the intensity of their pre- Buist-Bouwman et al. conducted a mediational anal-
vious depressive history (i.e., the number and length ysis of the European Study of the Epidemiology of
of past episodes). However, following clinical response Mental Disorders (ESEMeD), a cross-sectional sur-
at the second visit, the length of previous depres- vey representative of the adult population in Belgium,
sive history became more significant than current France, Germany, Italy, Netherlands, and Spain
symptoms.[32, 34] (N = 21,425, age ≥ 18).[7] The mediating effects of
Not all reports identify episode frequency as a de- six activity limitations (Mobility, Self Care, Cogni-
terminant of cognitive deficit. For example, a meta- tion/Concentration/Attention/Memory, Social Interac-
analysis of cognitive deficits in first episode MDD tion, Discrimination, Embarrassment) articulated in the
reported small-to-moderate effect size decreases in psy- WHO International Classification of Functioning, Dis-
chomotor speed, attention, visual and learning memory ability, and Health were evaluated.[7] Cognition and em-
as well as all aspects of executive function.[27] The pres- barrassment were the only determinants significantly
ence of cognitive deficits during the first MDE as part associated with both MDEs and work functioning.[7]
of MDD indicates that cognitive performance repre- Moreover, cognition and embarrassment accounted for
sents a target for early identification, measurement, and approximately half of the association between a MDE
intervention.[27, 35] and work loss, underscoring the salience of cognitive
Taken together, there are multiple determinants of function in mediating and reducing role functioning in
cognitive outcome in MDD: heterogeneity of sample MDEs.[7]
Depression and Anxiety
520 McIntyre et al.

Taken together, cognitive deficits in MDD are not with greater neuritic plaque density in neuropatho-
only characterized by poor performance on objective and logically confirmed Alzheimer’s brain.[55] Moreover,
subjective measures, but also appear to be a principal me- cognitively intact adults with MDD manifest lower
diator of functional impairment.[39, 40] This observation cerebrospinal fluid concentration of beta-amyloid,
underscores the prioritization of cognitive deficits in the providing indirect evidence for parenchymal deposition
evaluation and management of individuals with MDD. of beta-amyloid-42.[56]
A bidirectional relationship between MDD and
metabolic disorders has been amply documented.[57] Ac-
COGNITIVE DYSFUNCTION IN MDD: cumulating evidence supports the hypothesis that al-
NEUROBIOLOGICAL SUBSTRATE terations in insulin signaling may be relevant to neu-
Contemporary models of disease pathoetiology in rocognitive decline in subpopulations of individuals with
MDD have been extensively reviewed elsewhere.[41] MDD. For example, insulin resistance and diabetes mel-
Progress in molecular and cellular biology as well as litus type 2 are associated with cognitive deficits in
results from neuroimaging investigations have served younger adults as well as those with mild cognitive
to refine the hypothesis that disparate domains of impairment (MCI) and AD.[58] An imbalance between
psychopathology in MDD are subserved by abnor- insulin and counterregulatory neurohormonal systems
malities in the structure, function, and chemical (i.e., glucocorticoids) may alter pro-apoptotic intracellu-
composition of fronto-subcortical circuitry.[42, 43] A pre- lar signaling cascades thereby resulting in neuronal/glial
liminary study also suggests that brain activation pat- loss and neurocognitive decline.[59, 60]
terns suggestive of functional disconnectivity in MDD The default mode network (DMN), a proposed net-
may be different in individuals who are on long-term sick work of neural circuits that connects cortical and subcor-
leave when compared with those who are able to main- tical structures, may be relevant to cognitive function.[61]
tain workplace performance.[44] Moreover, several nodal The DMN comprises several nodal structures including
structures (e.g., hippocampus, amygdala, anterior cin- the ventral–medial–prefrontal cortex, posterior cingu-
gulate cortex) are susceptible to volumetric/functional late/retrosplenial cortex, and bilateral inferior parietal
changes as a consequence of illness duration, episode lobe.[61] The DMN is usually more active during the
frequency, and severity.[34, 43, 45] The neurochemical ab- “resting state” than it is during a cognitive, emotional,
normalities that are well documented in individuals with and/or motor task.[62] Abnormalities in the DMN have
MDD can be conceptualized as a consequence of aber- been implicated in pathological disease states of which
rant cellular functioning within the implicated neural cognitive deficits are a defining feature (e.g., AD).[63]
circuits.[46–48] For example, disturbances in monoamine An integrated translational research report indicated that
systems, notably catecholamine systems, have been doc- abnormalities in insulin resistance are accompanied by
umented to play a role in attentional and executive disturbances in the DMN in individuals at risk for AD.
function deficits.[49] Moreover, modafinil, an agent with established procog-
A derivative of this hypothesis is that the neurobiolog- nitive effects by enhancing catecholamine neurotrans-
ical correlates (i.e., neural circuits) of cognitive function mission, augments deactivation in the major nodal struc-
and deficits, overlap with those implicated in other com- tures of the DMN.[61]
ponents of MDD.[47] The most replicated volumetric In summary, the pathoetiological model in MDD
abnormality in MDD is bilateral hippocampal reduction, posits structural and functional disturbances in inter-
which is a consequence of loss of neuropil, decreased connected neural circuits and distributed networks. The
dendritic density, and reduced neuronal soma size.[34, 50] substrates that subserve cognitive performance in MDD
Hypofrontality of the prefrontal cortex and overactivity have overlapping and discrete components from the
of the anterior cingulate cortex are hypothesized to be substrates subserving affective processing; refining the
the correlate of a functional disconnection between cor- model subserving domains of psychopathology in MDD
tical and subcortical structures.[43, 51] These alterations hold promise to inform interventional strategies for cog-
may mediate deficits in measures of executive function, nitive deficits in MDD.
attention, learning and memory, as well as information
processing speed.
The mediators of aberrant neural circuitry, structure THE EFFECT OF INTERVENTION ON
and function are hypothesized to include alterations COGNITIVE DEFICITS IN MDD
in regulatory–counterregulatory hormone balance All commercially available and approved conventional
(i.e., glucocorticoid-signaling abnormalities, insulin antidepressants augment central indolamine and/or cat-
resistance), immunoinflammatory activation, neu- echolamine neurotransmission.[64, 65] This pharmacody-
rotrophins [e.g., Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor namic profile provides the basis for hypothesizing that
(BDNF)], and oxidative stress.[46, 52–54] A separate, and antidepressants, in addition to mitigating both the mood
possibly related, observation is that recurrent MDD and vegetative symptoms of an MDE, would also be ex-
is associated with an increased hazard for Alzheimer’s pected to ameliorate cognitive deficits. There are, how-
disease (AD).[44] For example, results from postmortem ever, relatively few studies that have primarily aimed
studies indicate that a history of MDD is associated to evaluate the effect of conventional antidepressants
Depression and Anxiety
Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 521

on cognitive performance in nongeriatric, adult MDD the DSST) in a group of elderly (N = 453) nonde-
samples.[13–15] Available evidence indicates that SSRIs, mented individuals with MDD.[68] In this study, duloxe-
SNRIs, dopamine modulators (bupropion), and nore- tine, the active control, also demonstrated improvement
pinephrine inhibitors (reboxetine) improve cognitive on RAVLT, but not DSST.[68] A path analysis concluded
performance in adults with MDD.[16–18] There are also that 83% of the improvement on DSST with vortioxe-
relatively few studies that have compared the cognitive tine was a direct effect (duloxetine 26%), while the direct
effects of different classes of antidepressants on cogni- effect on acquisition and delayed recall (i.e., RAVLT)
tive function.[16–18] Insufficiently answered questions in- was 71% and 72%, respectively (for duloxetine 65%
clude the degree to which the improvement in cognitive and 66%, respectively).[68] The beneficial effects of vor-
deficits can be dissociated from the effect on other MDD tioxetine in this study on cognitive performance were a
psychopathology domains. secondary outcome, providing an empirical basis for
Herrera-Guzman et al. reported on the cognitive ef- evaluating the effect of this agent on cognition and con-
fects of escitalopram and duloxetine in adults (aged 20– sequently functional outcomes as a primary outcome in
50) with MDD.[16] They observed that both treatments younger adults with MDD.[68]
improved working memory, attention, and executive Notwithstanding the paucity of studies, a signal that
function as well as mental processing speed and mo- emerges from the foregoing studies is that treatment in-
tor performance.[16, 66] Duloxetine was superior to es- terventions that engage multiple neurochemical systems
citalopram on episodic and working memory whereas simultaneously may be more likely to improve cogni-
no significant differences between the antidepressants tive performance when compared to interventions that
were observed on measures of attention and executive principally target a single system (e.g., SSRIs). In keep-
function.[16, 66] ing with this view, it is not uncommon for practition-
Raskin et al. sought to determine the effect of dulox- ers to coprescribe mechanistically diverse agents (e.g.,
etine versus placebo on several measures of cognitive modafinil, psychostimulants) and/or suggest behavioral
performance.[10] A study in older adults (62–90 years of interventions (e.g., aerobic exercise) in addition to con-
age) is reviewed as, to our knowledge, it is the only study ventional antidepressant treatment with an aim to pri-
that primarily aimed to evaluate a conventional antide- marily target the cognitive domain. Hitherto, no Food
pressant’s ability to mitigate cognitive deficits in a cohort and Drug Administration (FDA) approved agent for de-
of adults with MDD.[10] They reported that when com- pression has been purposefully evaluated to mitigate cog-
pared to placebo, 8 weeks of treatment with duloxetine nitive deficits and improve psychosocial functioning in
significantly improved the cognitive composite score.[10] MDD.
The improvement in the duloxetine-treated group was
largely accounted for by improvement in measures of
verbal learning and recall with trending between-group DISCUSSION
differences noted on attention [i.e., Digit Symbol Sub- Several observations emanate from the synthesis of the
stitution Test (DSST)], visual attention and executive data reviewed herein.[3, 36, 71, 72] The functional impair-
function (i.e., Two-Digit Cancellation Test), and work- ments (notably workforce impairment) associated with
ing memory/executive function (i.e., Letter-Number Se- MDD disproportionately account for the overall costs
quencing Test).[10] Path analyses showed that for the attributable to MDD.[3, 73] This observation provides
improvement of the cognitive composite score, there the basis for elucidating determinants of psychosocial
was a 90.9% direct effect and 90.1% indirect effect impairment in MDD.[3, 73] A global shift in the work-
through improvement in a Geriatric Depression Scale force away from primary industry toward a skilled hu-
Total Score.[10] man capital economy will only amplify the importance of
Vortioxetine (Lu AA21004) is a novel multi-modal diseases/disorders of the central nervous system (CNS)
antidepressant that exhibits 5-HT3 and 5-HT7 re- and their actuarial costs.[3, 72, 74]
ceptor antagonism, 5-HT1B receptor partial agonism, Cognitive deficts in MDD are prevalent and may
5-HT1A receptor agonism, and inhibition of the 5-HT represent a core dimension of psychopathology in
transporter. In addition to its effect on indolamine sig- MDD.[31, 32, 75] Cognitive deficits are often a persist-
naling, vortioxetine also increased central neurotrans- ing abnormality in individuals with MDD and in some
mission of noradrenaline, dopamine, acetylcholine, and cases may predate the onset of clinically significant
histamine.[67] Vortioxetine has demonstrated antide- depressive symptoms.[12, 27] The observation that cog-
pressant efficacy in MDD in adult and elderly pop- nitive deficits mediate psychosocial impairment (i.e.,
ulations as well as in adults with generalized anxiety accounting for a significant percentage of variance) and
disorder.[68, 69] In addition to antidepressant proper- intervention.[7, 26, 74] When compared with severe men-
ties, vortioxetine has demonstrated cognitive-enhancing tal disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), rela-
properties in animal and human studies.[70] For ex- tively less emphasis has been given to the hazards posed
ample, vortioxetine showed statistically significant im- by cognitive deficits in MDD. Along with the method-
provement on acquisition and delayed recall (as mea- ological limitations that affect inferences and interpre-
sured by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test), as tations that may be drawn from the extant data per-
well as information processing speed (as measured by taining to cognitive deficits in MDD, there is no “gold
Depression and Anxiety
522 McIntyre et al.

TABLE 3. Effects of pharmacological treatment on cognitive deficits in major depressive disorder

Cognitive
Author Year Population Treatment Cognitive tests deficits/improvements

Ferguson et al. 2003 74 MDD (age range = Reboxetine (8–10 mg/day; SRT, Digit Vigilance, Reboxetine significantly
18–65) N = 25) CRT, Numeric WM, improved sustained
Paroxetine (20–40 Word Recognition, and attention and speed of
mg/day; N = 23) Critical Flicker cognitive functioning
Placebo (N = 26) Frequency compared with baseline
No significant changes or
trends in this direction
were observed in
individuals receiving
either paroxetine or
placebo
Constant et al. 2005 20 MDD (mean age = Sertraline (50–75 mg/day) Phasic Alertness Task, MDDs had psychomotor
47.67) Classic SCWT, and the slowing associated with
26 healthy controls (mean Supraliminal and attentional and
age = 48.85) Subliminal Emotional executive disturbance
Stroop test Following first weeks of
treatment, a beneficial
effect on psychomotor
slowing on attentional
and executive functions
was observed
Gualtieri et al. 2006 38 drug-free MDD (mean Citalopram (N = 1) CNS Vital Signs Untreated MDDs had
age = 38.11, SD = 9.95) Fluoxetine (N = 3) global
31 MDD antidepressant Escitalopram (N = 8) neuropsychological
monotherapy Paroxetine (N = 3) impairment.
responders (mean age = Mirtazapine (N = 1) Successfully treated
43.55, SD = 10.68) Trazodone (N = 1) MDD`s performance
69 healthy controls (mean Venlafaxine (N = 5) improved, but not
age = 41.30, SD = Bupropion (N = 6) normalized
11.40) Sertraline (N = 3) Specific
depression-related
deficits were observed
in executive function
and processing speed,
but not in memory,
psychomotor speed, or
reaction time
Gualtieri et al. 2007 81 MDD (mean age = Bupropion (N = 27) CNS Vital Signs SSRI group scored
43.85) Venlafaxine (N = 27) significantly below
27 healthy controls (mean SSRI (N = 27) controls in tests of
age = 43.85) psychomotor speed,
cognitive flexibility, and
reaction time
Venlafaxine group scored
worse than controls in
reaction time
Bupropion group did not
differ from controls in
any of the cognitive
domains
Raskin et al. 2007 311 MDD Duloxetine (60 mg/day; N Verbal Learning and Duloxetine demonstrated
Duloxetine (N = 207; = 207) Recall Test, DSST, significantly greater
mean age = 72.6, SD = Placebo (N = 104) Cancellation Test, LNS improvement versus
5.7) placebo. MDD with
Placebo (N = 104; mean duloxetine treatment
age = 73.3, SD = 5.7) improved verbal
learning and memory

Depression and Anxiety


Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 523

TABLE 3. Continued

Cognitive
Author Year Population Treatment Cognitive tests deficits/improvements

Herrera-Guzman 2010 73 MDD Escitalopram (10 mg/day; Vocabulary, Digit Span SSRI and SNRI
et al. SSRI (N = 36; mean age N = 36) and SWM, Rapid treatments both
= 32.91, SD = 8.73) Duloxetine (60 mg/day; N Visual Information improved WM,
SNRI (N = 37; mean age = 37) Processing, DMS, attention, and all
= 33.21, SD = 8.61) SCWT, IED, SOC executive functions.
37 healthy controls (mean However, MDD`s
age = 33.05, SD = 8.04) cognitive function did
not improve enough to
reach levels of control
subjects
Herrera-Guzman 2010 73 MDD Escitalopram (10 mg/day; Vocabulary, Digit Span, SSRI and SNRI
et al. SSRI (N = 36; mean age N = 36) AVLT, PRM, PAL, treatments both
= 32.91, SD = 8.73) Duloxetine (60 mg/day; N DMS, SRM, RTI, improved episodic
SNRI (N = 37; mean age = 37) SCWT memory and, to a lesser
= 33.21, SD = 8.61) extent, WM, mental
37 healthy controls (mean processing speed and
age = 33.05, SD = 8.04) motor performance.
SNRI was superior to
SSRI at improving
episodic memory and
WM
Herrera-Guzman 2010 73 MDD Escitalopram (10 mg/day; Vocabulary, SWM, MDDs in remission
et al. SSRI (N = 36; mean age N = 36) RAVLT, PAL, SOC, showed deficits in verbal
= 32.91, SD = 8.73) Duloxetine (60 mg/day; N Rapid Visual and visual episodic
SNRI (N = 37; mean age = 37) Information Processing memory, sustained
= 33.21, SD = 8.61) attention, mnemonic
37 healthy controls (mean and strategic aspects of
age = 33.05, SD = 8.04) WM, and planning.
MDDs in recovery
showed the same
neuropsychological
deficit pattern. MDDs
treated with SSRI
showed more
impairment in episodic
visual and verbal
memory than those
treated with SNRI
Hinkelmann et 2012 N = 52 MDD with SSRI Baseline RAVLT, Digit Span F and MDD performed worse
al. treatment and add-on Escitalopram (10–20 B, ROCF, Cancellation compared with healthy
treatment modulating mg/day; N = 52) test, TMT A and B controls (Digit Span
the mineralocorticoid Add-on treatment Forward, ROCF,
receptor MR-agonist Cancellation Test),
N = 50 healthy subjects fludrocortisone (0.2 indicating ongoing
mg/day; N = 19) relative cognitive
MR-antagonist deficits in these
spironolactone (100 domains. No differences
mg/day; N = 22) between the three
Placebo (N = 11) treatment groups were
observed over time

standard” to measure cognitive deficits in research reliable, and have utility for personalizing treatment
settings and/or to succinctly and comprehensively mea- interventions.[76]
sure cognitive deficits in the clinical ecosystem. More- MDD is a heterogeneous phenotype in phenomenol-
over, as interest in patient-reported outcomes in psy- ogy and pathoetiology. There is no single disease model
chiatric and medical disorders increases, there will be that will sufficiently explain the panoply of abnormal-
a need to identify not only objective, but also sub- ities observed across disparate populations. Admittedly
jective measures of cognitive functions that are valid, complex, a conceptual framework of disease modeling in

Depression and Anxiety


524 McIntyre et al.

MDD that would be comprehensive and coherent would distorted socio-emotional information processing in chronic de-
posit that there are multiple (i.e., hundreds, if not thou- pression. J Affect Disord 2012.
sands) disease pathways in MDD. Succinct neurobio- 3. Greenberg PE, Kessler RC, Birnbaum HG, et al. The eco-
logical substrate(s) of neurocognitive dysfunction is not nomic burden of depression in the United States: how did it
change between 1990 and 2000? J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64(12):
currently available. Notwithstanding available evidence
1465–1475.
implicates altered cellular, synaptic, and circuitry abnor-
4. Kessler RC, Akiskal HS, Ames M, et al. Prevalence and effects
malities in MDD manifesting as disturbances in central of mood disorders on work performance in a nationally repre-
neurotransmitter signaling.[46, 52–54, 77] sentative sample of U.S. workers. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163(9):
Robust therapeutic interventions specifically tar- 1561–1568.
geting cognitive deficits in MDD are not currently 5. Jaeger J, Berns S, Uzelac S, Davis-Conway S. Neurocognitive
available, largely due to insufficient empirical obser- deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res
vation. Notwithstanding available evidence indicates 2006;145(1):39–48.
that targeting multiple monoamine systems mitigates 6. Conradi HJ, Ormel J, de JP. Presence of individual (residual)
cognitive deficits independent of its effect on other symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission:
depressive symptoms.[10, 16, 66] The complex pathoeti- a 3-year prospective study. Psychol Med 2010 Oct. 8:1–10.
7. Buist-Bouwman MA, Ormel J, de GR, et al. Mediators of the as-
ology of cognitive deficits provides the basis for hy-
sociation between depression and role functioning. Acta Psychiatr
pothesizing that interventions capable of engaging Scand 2008;118(6):451–458.
multiple physiological systems may be differentially ef- 8. Reppermund S, Zihl J, Lucae S, et al. Persistent cognitive im-
fective for this domain of psychopathology. Moreover, pairment in depression: the role of psychopathology and altered
it remains a testable hypothesis that mitigating cog- hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system regulation.
nitive deficits in MDD would result in an improve- Biol Psychiatry 2007;62(5):400–406.
ment in overall psychosocial performance, notably in the 9. Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Herrera-Abarca JE, et al.
workforce. Major depressive disorder in recovery and neuropsychological
Refining disease models in MDD will provide a plat- functioning: effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor and
dual inhibitor depression treatments on residual cognitive deficits
form for identifying genuinely novel, pharmacological
in patients with major depressive disorder in recovery. J Affect
treatment approaches to treat, reverse, and prevent cog- Disord 2010;123(1–3):341–350.
nitive deficits in MDD (Table 3). Extrapolating from re- 10. Raskin J, Wiltse CG, Siegal A, et al. Efficacy of duloxetine on cog-
sults reported in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, it is nition, depression, and pain in elderly patients with major depres-
reasonable to expect that some adults with MDD would sive disorder: an 8-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial.
be candidates for cognitive remediation and neurore- Am J Psychiatry 2007;164(6):900–909.
habilitative activities/endeavors.[78–82] Moreover, novel 11. Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brune M, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psy-
neuromodulatory approaches [e.g., repetitive Transcra- chiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for im-
nial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS)], complementary al- proved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2012;11(2):141–168.
ternative medicine (e.g., N-acetyl-cysteine), and behav- 12. Airaksinen E, Wahlin A, Forsell Y, Larsson M. Low episodic
memory performance as a premorbid marker of depression: evi-
ioral interventions (e.g., aerobic exercise) may also have
dence from a 3-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007;115(6):
a place in the armamentarium against cognitive deficits 458–465.
in MDD beyond resolution of the acute episode.[13, 83–85] 13. Furtado CP, Hoy KE, Maller JJ, Savage G, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzger-
ald PB. An investigation of medial temporal lobe changes and
Acknowledgments. This review was sponsored by cognition following antidepressant response: a prospective rTMS
the Takeda Pharmaceutical Company, Ltd., as part of study. Brain Stimul 2012 Jul 6;1e9.
a joint clinical development program with H. Lund- 14. Wagner S, Doering B, Helmreich I, Lieb K, Tadic A. A meta-
beck A/S. Dr. McIntyre drafted and reviewed successive analysis of executive dysfunctions in unipolar major depressive
disorder without psychotic symptoms and their changes during
versions of the manuscript. Editorial support, including
antidepressant treatment. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2012;125(4):281–
styling and editing for journal submission, was provided 292.
by The Medicine Group, New Hope, Pennsylvania. 15. McLennan SN, Mathias JL. The depression-executive dys-
function (DED) syndrome and response to antidepressants:
a meta-analytic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010;25(10):
Conflict of interest. The authors have no conflicts of 933–944.
interest. Joanna K. Soczynska is a recipient of the Eli 16. Herrera-Guzman I, Herrera-Abarca JE, Gudayol-Ferre E, et al.
Lilly Canada Fellowship Award. Effects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic-
noradrenergic reuptake treatments on attention and executive
functions in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry
REFERENCES Res 2010;177(3):323–329.
17. Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Lira-Mandujano J, et al.
1. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen Cognitive predictors of treatment response to bupropion and cog-
HU. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid nitive effects of bupropion in patients with major depressive dis-
risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J order. Psychiatry Res 2008;160(1):72–82.
Methods Psychiatr Res 2012. 18. Ferguson JM, Wesnes KA, Schwartz GE. Reboxetine versus
2. van RA, Huffmeier J, Victor D, Klocke K, Borlinghaus J, Pawelzik paroxetine versus placebo: effects on cognitive functioning in de-
M. Contrasting chronic with episodic depression: an analysis of pressed patients. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2003;18(1):9–14.

Depression and Anxiety


Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 525

19. Larochette AC, Harrison AG, Rosenblum Y, Bowie CR. 38. Kim JM, Stewart R, Glozier N, et al. Physical health, depression
Additive neurocognitive deficits in adults with attention- and cognitive function as correlates of disability in an older Korean
deficit/hyperactivity disorder and depressive symptoms. Arch Clin population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005;20(2):160–167.
Neuropsychol 2011;26(5):385–395. 39. McCall WV, Dunn AG. Cognitive deficits are associated with
20. Cserjesi R, Luminet O, Poncelet AS, Lenard L. Altered executive functional impairment in severely depressed patients. Psychiatry
function in obesity. Exploration of the role of affective states on Res 2003;121(2):179–184.
cognitive abilities. Appetite 2009;52(2):535–539. 40. Sweeney JA, Kmiec JA, Kupfer DJ. Neuropsychologic im-
21. Aas M, Steen NE, Agartz I, et al. Is cognitive impairment fol- pairments in bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the
lowing early life stress in severe mental disorders based on spe- CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biol Psychiatry 2000;48(7):
cific or general cognitive functioning? Psychiatry Res 2012 Apr 1; 674–684.
198(3):495–500. 41. Krishnan V, Nestler EJ. Linking molecules to mood: new
22. Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Colom F, et al. Cognitive dysfunctions insight into the biology of depression. Am J Psychiatry
in bipolar disorder: evidence of neuropsychological disturbances. 2010;167(11):1305–1320.
Psychother Psychosom 2000;69(1):2–18. 42. Jiao Q, Ding J, Lu G, et al. Increased activity imbalance in fronto-
23. Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Reinares M, et al. Cognitive function subcortical circuits in adolescents with major depression. PLoS
across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in One 2011;6(9):e25159.
bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161(2):262–270. 43. Pizzagalli DA. Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward
24. Grut M, Jorm AF, Fratiglioni L, Forsell Y, Viitanen M, Winblad biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology
B. Memory complaints of elderly people in a population survey: 2011;36(1):183–206.
variation according to dementia stage and depression. J Am Geri- 44. Sandstrom A, Sall R, Peterson J, et al. Brain activation patterns in
atr Soc 1993;41(12):1295–1300. major depressive disorder and work stress-related long-term sick
25. Naismith SL, Longley WA, Scott EM, Hickie IB. Disability in leave among Swedish females. Stress 2012;15(5):503–513.
major depression related to self-rated and objectively-measured 45. Malykhin NV, Carter R, Hegadoren KM, Seres P, Coupland NJ.
cognitive deficits: a preliminary study. BMC Psychiatry 2007;7:32. Fronto-limbic volumetric changes in major depressive disorder. J
26. Gualtieri CT, Morgan DW. The frequency of cognitive impair- Affect Disord 2012;136(3):1104–1113.
ment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: an 46. Andreazza AC. Combining redox-proteomics and epigenomics to
unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry explain the involvement of oxidative stress in psychiatric disorders.
2008;69(7):1122–1130. Mol Biosyst 2012 Jun 18; 8(10):2503–2512.
27. Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A 47. Drevets WC, Price JL, Furey ML. Brain structural and func-
meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive tional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocir-
disorder. J Affect Disord 2012;140(2):113–124. cuitry models of depression. Brain Struct Funct 2008;213(1–2):93–
28. Jones RN, Manly J, Glymour MM, Rentz DM, Jefferson AL, Stern 118.
Y. Conceptual and measurement challenges in research on cogni- 48. Yucel K, Nazarov A, Taylor VH, Macdonald K, Hall GB, Mac-
tive reserve. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011 Mar 17; 17(4):593–601. Queen GM. Cerebellar vermis volume in major depressive disor-
29. Godard J, Baruch P, Grondin S, Lafleur MF. Psychosocial and der. Brain Struct Funct 2012; Jun 14.
neurocognitive functioning in unipolar and bipolar depression: 49. Stahl SM, Zhang L, Damatarca C, Grady M. Brain circuits deter-
a 12-month prospective study. Psychiatry Res 2012;196(1):145– mine destiny in depression: a novel approach to the psychophar-
153. macology of wakefulness, fatigue, and executive dysfunction in
30. Preiss M, Kucerova H, Lukavsky J, Stepankova H, Sos P, major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64(Suppl 14):6–
Kawaciukova R. Cognitive deficits in the euthymic phase of unipo- 17.
lar depression. Psychiatry Res 2009;169(3):235–239. 50. Malykhin NV, Carter R, Seres P, Coupland NJ. Structural
31. Simons CJ, Jacobs N, Derom C, et al. Cognition as predictor of changes in the hippocampus in major depressive disorder: con-
current and follow-up depressive symptoms in the general popu- tributions of disease and treatment. J Psychiatry Neurosci
lation. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;120(1):45–52. 2010;35(5):337–343.
32. Gorwood P, Corruble E, Falissard B, Goodwin GM. Toxic ef- 51. Zeng LL, Shen H, Liu L, et al. Identifying major depression using
fects of depression on brain function: impairment of delayed re- whole-brain functional connectivity: a multivariate pattern analy-
call and the cumulative length of depressive disorder in a large sis. Brain 2012;135(Pt 5):1498–1507.
sample of depressed outpatients. Am J Psychiatry 2008;165(6): 52. Li M, Soczynska JK, Kennedy SH. Inflammatory biomarkers in
731–739. depression: an opportunity for novel therapeutic interventions.
33. Singh-Manoux A, Akbaraly TN, Marmot M, et al. Persistent Curr Psychiatry Rep 2011;13(5):316–320.
depressive symptoms and cognitive function in late midlife: the 53. Ryan JP, Sheu LK, Critchley HD, Gianaros PJ. A neural cir-
Whitehall II study. J Clin Psychiatry 2010;71(10):1379–1385. cuitry linking insulin resistance to depressed mood. Psychosom
34. MacQueen GM, Campbell S, McEwen BS, et al. Course of ill- Med 2012;74(5):476–482.
ness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major 54. Kessing LV, Jorgensen OS. Apolipoprotein E-epsilon 4 fre-
depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003;100(3):1387–1392. quency in affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1999;45(4):430–
35. Snyder HR. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad im- 434.
pairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: 55. Rapp MA, Schnaider-Beeri M, Grossman HT, et al. Increased hip-
a meta-analysis and review. Psychol Bull 2012 May 28; 139(1):81– pocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease
132. with a lifetime history of major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
36. Collins PY, Patel V, Joestl SS, et al. Grand challenges in global 2006;63(2):161–167.
mental health. Nature 2011;475(7354):27–30. 56. Pomara N, Bruno D, Sarreal AS, et al. Lower CSF amyloid
37. Kiosses DN, Klimstra S, Murphy C, Alexopoulos GS. Executive beta peptides and higher F2-isoprostanes in cognitively intact
dysfunction and disability in elderly patients with major depres- elderly individuals with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychia-
sion. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001;9(3):269–274. try 2012;169(5):523–530.

Depression and Anxiety


526 McIntyre et al.

57. McIntyre RS, Rasgon NL, Kemp DE, et al. Metabolic syndrome major depressive disorder: mental health services utilization and
and major depressive disorder: co-occurrence and pathophysio- costs, and work performance. Depress Anxiety 2010;27(1):78–
logic overlap. Curr Diab Rep 2009;9(1):51–59. 89.
58. Baker LD, Cross DJ, Minoshima S, Belongia D, Watson GS, Craft 75. van EP, van WG, Fernandez G, et al. Amygdala responsiv-
S. Insulin resistance and Alzheimer-like reductions in regional ity related to memory of emotionally neutral stimuli consti-
cerebral glucose metabolism for cognitively normal adults with tutes a trait factor for depression. Neuroimage 2011;54(2):
prediabetes or early type 2 diabetes. Arch Neurol 2011;68(1):51– 1677–1684.
57. 76. Fava M, Graves LM, Benazzi F, et al. A cross-sectional study of the
59. McIntyre RS, Vagic D, Swartz SA, et al. Insulin, insulin- prevalence of cognitive and physical symptoms during long-term
like growth factors and incretins: neural homeostatic regula- antidepressant treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 2006;67(11):1754–
tors and treatment opportunities. CNS Drugs 2008;22(6):443– 1759.
453. 77. Blank T, Prinz M. Microglia as modulators of cognition and neu-
60. Craft S, Watson GS. Insulin and neurodegenerative disease: ropsychiatric disorders. Glia 2012; Jun 27; 61(1):62–70.
shared and specific mechanisms. Lancet Neurol 2004;3(3):169– 78. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments,
178. determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatr Clin North
61. Minzenberg MJ, Yoon JH, Carter CS. Modafinil modulation Am 2005;28(3):613–633, 626.
of the default mode network. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 79. Bowie CR, McGurk SR, Mausbach B, Patterson TL, Har-
2011;215(1):23–31. vey PD. Combined cognitive remediation and functional skills
62. Mars RB, Neubert FX, Noonan MP, Sallet J, Toni I, Rushworth training for schizophrenia: effects on cognition, functional
MF. On the relationship between the “default mode network” and competence, and real-world behavior. Am J Psychiatry 2012;
the “social brain.” Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:189. May 11.
63. Kenna H, Hoeft F, Kelley R, et al. Fasting plasma insulin and the 80. Bowie CR, Depp C, McGrath JA, et al. Prediction of real-
default mode network in women at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a com-
Neurobiol Aging 2012; Jul 5; 34(3):641–649. parison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry
64. Muller N, Schwarz MJ. Immunological aspects of depressive dis- 2010;167(9):1116–1124.
orders. Nervenarzt 2007;78(11):1261–1273. 81. Goodwin GM, Martinez-Aran A, Glahn DC, Vieta E. Cognitive
65. Xu Z, Zhang Z, Shi Y, et al. Influence and interaction of genetic impairment in bipolar disorder: neurodevelopment or neurode-
polymorphisms in catecholamine neurotransmitter systems and generation? An ECNP expert meeting report. Eur Neuropsy-
early life stress on antidepressant drug response. J Affect Disord chopharmacol 2008;18(11):787–793.
2011;133(1–2):165–173. 82. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipo-
66. Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Herrera-Guzman D, lar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in eu-
Guardia-Olmos J, Hinojosa-Calvo E, Herrera-Abarca JE. Ef- thymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord
fects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic- 2009;113(1–2):1–20.
noradrenergic reuptake treatments on memory and mental 83. Chan A, Shea TB. Effects of dietary supplementation with N-
processing speed in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psy- acetyl cysteine, acetyl-L-carnitine and S-adenosyl methionine on
chiatr Res 2009;43(9):855–863. cognitive performance and aggression in normal mice and mice
67. Bang-Andersen B, Ruhland T, Jorgensen M, et al. Discovery of 1- expressing human ApoE4. Neuromolecular Med 2007;9(3):264–
[2-(2,4-dimethylphenylsulfanyl)phenyl]piperazine (Lu AA21004): 269.
a novel multimodal compound for the treatment of major depres- 84. Vasques PE, Moraes H, Silveira H, Deslandes AC, Laks J. Acute
sive disorder. J Med Chem 2011;54(9):3206–3221. exercise improves cognition in the depressed elderly: the effect of
68. Katona C, Hansen T, Olsen CK. A randomized, double-blind, dual-tasks. Clinics (Sao Paulo) 2011;66(9):1553–1557.
placebo-controlled, duloxetine-referenced, fixed-dose study com- 85. Knochel C, Oertel-Knochel V, O’Dwyer L, et al. Cognitive and
paring the efficacy and safety of Lu AA21004 in elderly pa- behavioural effects of physical exercise in psychiatric patients. Prog
tients with major depressive disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol Neurobiol 2012;96(1):46–68.
2012;27(4):215–223. 86. Thomas AJ, Gallagher P, Robinson LJ, et al. A comparison of
69. Alvarez E, Perez V, Dragheim M, Loft H, Artigas F. A double- neurocognitive impairment in younger and older adults with major
blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, active reference study of depression. Psychol Med 2009;39(5):725–733.
Lu AA21004 in patients with major depressive disorder. Int J Neu- 87. Herrmann LL, Le MM, Ebmeier KP. White matter hyperintensi-
ropsychopharmacol 2012;15(5):589–600. ties in late life depression: a systematic review. J Neurol Neurosurg
70. Mork A, Pehrson A, Brennum LT, et al. Pharmacological ef- Psychiatry 2008;79(6):619–624.
fects of Lu AA21004: a novel multimodal compound for the 88. Gildengers AG, Butters MA, Chisholm D, et al. Cognition in
treatment of major depressive disorder. J Pharmacol Exp Ther older adults with bipolar disorder versus major depressive disor-
2012;340(3):666–675. der. Bipolar Disord 2012;14(2):198–205.
71. Kessler RC, Barber C, Birnbaum HG, et al. Depression in the 89. McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression
workplace: effects on short-term disability. Health Aff (Millwood) severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord 2009;119(1–3):1–
1999;18(5):163–171. 8.
72. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al. The epidemiology of 90. Withall A, Harris LM, Cumming SR. A longitudinal study of
major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity cognitive function in melancholic and non-melancholic subtypes
Survey Replication (NCS-R). JAMA 2003;289(23):3095–3105. of major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord 2010;123(1–3):150–
73. Kessler RC, Merikangas KR, Wang PS. The prevalence and cor- 157.
relates of workplace depression in the national comorbidity survey 91. Gallagher P, Robinson LJ, Gray JM, Porter RJ, Young AH.
replication. J Occup Environ Med 2008;50(4):381–390. Neurocognitive function following remission in major depressive
74. Birnbaum HG, Kessler RC, Kelley D, Ben-Hamadi R, Joish VN, disorder: potential objective marker of response? Aust N Z J Psy-
Greenberg PE. Employer burden of mild, moderate, and severe chiatry 2007;41(1):54–61.

Depression and Anxiety


Review: Cognitive Deficits and Functional Outcomes in MDD 527

92. Liu IC, Chiu CH, Yang TT. The effects of gender and syndromes be reclassified as “metabolic syndrome type II”? Ann
a co-occurring depressive disorder on neurocognitive func- Clin Psychiatry 2007;19(4):257–264.
tioning in patients with alcohol dependence. Alcohol Alcohol 95. Lemos-Miller A, Kearney CA. Depression and ethnicity as in-
2010;45(3):231–236. termediary variables among dissociation, trauma-related cogni-
93. McIntyre RS, Kenna HA, Nguyen HT, et al. Brain volume abnor- tions, and PTSD symptomatology in youths. J Nerv Ment Dis
malities and neurocognitive deficits in diabetes mellitus: points of 2006;194(8):584–590.
pathophysiological commonality with mood disorders? Adv Ther 96. Riso LP, Miyatake RK, Thase ME. The search for determinants
2010; Apr 13; 27(2):63–80. of chronic depression: a review of six factors. J Affect Disord
94. McIntyre RS, Soczynska JK, Konarski JZ, et al. Should depressive 2002;70(2):103–115.

Depression and Anxiety


Copyright of Depression & Anxiety (1091-4269) is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

You might also like