PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

Overcoming the fear factor: How perceptions of supervisor openness


lead employees to speak up when fearing external threat
R. David Lebel
Katz School of Business, University of Pittsburgh, 364 Mervis Hall, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: While fear is generally assumed to powerfully limit employee voice, a functional view of emotions sug-
Received 26 March 2014 gests that responses to fear vary. Instead of assuming that fear is negatively associated with voice, I argue
Revised 26 April 2016 that this relationship may be more complex. Adopting a functional view of emotions, I hypothesize that
Accepted 5 May 2016
fears from external sources focus attention on shared threat to the organization and may be positively
Available online 18 May 2016
associated with employee voice. This effect is likely contingent: when employees perceive their supervi-
sors as open to input, they are motivated to speak up. Thus, perceptions of supervisor openness can help
Keywords:
transform other-focused motives resulting from fearing external threat into information-sharing. Results
Employee voice
Upward communication
from two studies suggest that fear of external threat positively relates to voice when employees perceive
Fear their supervisor as open to input. Additionally, results suggest that this interactive effect is mediated by
Discrete emotions prosocial motivation spurring employees to speak up when fearing external threat.
Layoffs Ó 2016 The Author. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
Supervisor openness (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Prosocial motivation

1. Introduction ‘‘whiner” or ‘‘trouble-maker”), damage to one’s image, not being


supported, or harming work relationships (Detert & Edmondson,
Given growing economic uncertainty and competitive threat at 2011; Kish-Gephart, Detert, Treviño, & Edmondson, 2009;
work, managers need feedback from employees at all levels in Milliken, Morrison, & Hewlin, 2003).
order to implement strategy and make informed decisions However, there is reason to believe that the relationship
(Morrison & Milliken, 2000; Senge, 1990). A flow of information between fear and voice may be more complex. A functional view
from employees facilitates organizational learning (Edmondson, of emotions suggests that negative emotions, such as fear, can be
1999) and enhances decision quality (Dooley & Fryxell, 1999). As adaptive, motivating and coordinating action to deal with potential
such, employee voice behavior—defined as discretionary commu- or existing threats (Elfenbein, 2007; Frijda, 1986). The experience
nication of ideas, suggestions, or concerns intended to benefit the of fear functions to protect a person from threat, often in the form
organization (Morrison, 2011)—can help improve the organiza- of ‘‘flight” responses such as withdrawal, freezing in place, or
tion’s responses to the external competitive environment, as well avoiding a situation (Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994; Shaver,
as its effectiveness and performance (Detert, Burris, Harrison, & Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987). Fear’s protective behavior,
Martin, 2013; Lam & Mayer, 2014; Mackenzie, Podsakoff, & however, may not always involve flight (Rosen & Schulkin, 1998;
Podsakoff, 2011). Öhman, 2008). Responses to fear depend on the situation: ‘‘careful-
Because employee voice is important for organizational func- ness when the threat is transitory; protective effort when there is
tioning, voice scholars have focused on the antecedents to this no immediate way of escape or immediate need to escape; escape
behavior (Morrison, 2014). While much of this research has when such is possible and the threat is more than protective
emphasized the role of cognitive processes that influence whether behavior can handle” (Frijda, 1986, p. 198). Therefore, whether
employees speak up, there is a lack of understanding as to how action is taken and the form this behavior takes depends on the
emotions influence voice (Grant, 2013). An exception is a body of nature of threat and what is instrumental for seeking protection
work suggesting that employees often withhold their ideas and from that threat (Blanchard & Blanchard, 2008; Frijda, Kuipers, &
suggestions, primarily due to fears of negative consequences, such ter Schure, 1989).
as supervisor retaliation or punishment, negative labels (e.g., Adopting a functional view of emotions, I explore the conditions
under which fear may lead to voice. First, responding to fear of
external threats, such as economic downturn, employees may
E-mail address: rdlebel@katz.pitt.edu make an effort to change the situation (George, 2011). Indeed,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.05.001
0749-5978/Ó 2016 The Author. Published by Elsevier Inc.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 11

employees fearing external changes often focus their attention on between these two types of fear is the originating source of threat.
shared threat to the organization, which sparks other-focused Additionally, whereas fear of speaking up focuses employee atten-
motives and potential action to protect the collective (Vuori & tion on the potential negative consequences of voice, fear of exter-
Huy, 2016). Second, cues from supervisors and leaders are particu- nal threat focuses attention on the negative consequences that
larly important during times of uncertainty (Kish-Gephart et al., may arise if the source of threat is not addressed. Because of the
2009; Rachman, 1990). When employees perceive that their super- perceived negative consequences of speaking up, voice scholars
visors are open to input, they believe they can approach them with have primarily focused on how employees often remain silent
suggestions or problems and that such input will be listened to, due to fears of speaking up (Kish-Gephart et al., 2009).
providing a means to cope with fears of external threat. Taken In this article, I focus on fears of external threat for two key the-
together, I argue that employees are motivated to speak up when oretical reasons. First, fear of external threat is commonly experi-
they are fearful of external organizational threats and when they enced in organizations (Huang, Zhao, Niu, Ashford, & Lee, 2013),
perceive high levels of supervisor openness. I further argue that with assumed negative effects on employee behavior and health
the effect of this interaction on voice is mediated by prosocial (Ashford et al., 1989). Second, employees may respond differently
motives. These hypotheses are tested across two studies: a field to fears of external threat than to fears of speaking up (Menon,
study in three organizations impacted by organizational and eco- Thompson, & Choi, 2006). In particular, research suggests that
nomic uncertainty, and, to explore possible mediating mecha- external threats can motivate information-sharing intended to pro-
nisms, a lagged survey study of full-time employees from a range tect the larger collective (Vuori & Huy, 2016), whereas fears of
of organizations. speaking up can motivate information-withholding for self-
This article makes several theoretical contributions to the liter- protective reasons (Kish-Gephart et al., 2009). This has important,
atures on employee voice and emotions in organizations. First, it yet unexplored implications for research on employee voice, sug-
examines when and why fear of external threat may motivate gesting that fear resulting from external sources may increase a
employees to speak up. Doing so adds complexity to the notion flow of information within an organization, and therefore the like-
that all fear decreases voice. Second, this article contributes by lihood of speaking up.
proposing a contingent relationship between fear and voice, argu-
ing that emotional factors interact with employee perceptions of 2.2. Fear of external threat and voice: a functional emotions
the situation. Despite indications from the emotions literature that perspective
fear’s impact on employee behavior may vary (e.g., Roseman et al.,
1994), the voice literature has yet to explore this relationship’s Emotions can be functional, helping people adapt to events,
possible contingencies. Third, this article extends a growing and changes, or threats (Frijda, 1986). Emotions coordinate behavior
nascent body of work exploring when negative emotions may have in social interactions and direct behavior toward addressing prob-
constructive outcomes at work (Bohns & Flynn, 2012; Geddes & lems or the demands of the environment (Elfenbein, 2007). For
Callister, 2007). The findings of this research also have practical example, the function of fear is to motivate protection from psy-
implications for understanding how supervisors and employees chological or physiological threats (Izard & Ackerman, 2000;
can effectively manage and respond to fears of external threat. Öhman, 2008). A functional perspective also suggests that emo-
tions prepare a person to take action, referred to as states of action
readiness (Frijda, 1986). States of action readiness involve auto-
2. Theory and hypotheses matic or learned behavioral patterns following emotional experi-
ence, such as moving away from or towards a person or object
2.1. Fear of external threat (Frijda et al., 1989). Discrete emotions, such as fear, are accompa-
nied by distinct patterns of action readiness (Frijda, 1986; Lazarus,
Fear is an emotional state involving a sense of uncertainty and 1991). The state of action readiness accompanying fear, for exam-
the threat of harm or an undesirable event (Frijda, 1986; Smith & ple, often results in withdrawing from a situation, avoidance
Ellsworth, 1985). As an experience, fear involves subjective feelings behavior, or freezing in place, whereas anger often results in
of apprehension, as well as associated physiological changes aggressive action towards another individual (Shaver et al., 1987).
including increased heart rate (Rachman, 1990). Fear is a discrete While fear generally motivates protection through flight or
emotion, which entails a distinct feeling state elicited by a specific withdrawal, a functional perspective suggests that the behavioral
stimulus or event (Lazarus, 1991). Fear is thus source-specific, and consequences of emotional experience vary. Thus, individuals
has an identifiable target (Öhman, 2008). Specifying the target is may respond to fear with withdrawal (i.e., flight), protective effort
important to understanding how people respond to fear, as the (i.e., fight), or increased attention towards a threat, depending on
source can influence the behavioral response to this emotion what is appropriate for ensuring safety (Frijda, 1986). Indeed, func-
(Blanchard & Blanchard, 2008). tional theorists argue that emotionally-directed behavior is contin-
In the workplace, employees can experience fear in response to gent and ‘‘dependent on the joint occurrence of an emotion and
a variety of sources (Basch & Fisher, 2000), both external and inter- specific external or internal stimulus conditions” (Roseman et al.,
nal (Menon & Pfeffer, 2003). Fear of external threat is defined as 1994, p. 216). That is, the experience of fear functions to direct a
feelings of uncertainty that result from sources outside an employ- person to seek safety, but the form that safety-seeking behavior
ee’s organization. These sources can include economic or industry takes can vary based on perceptions of the situation.
downturn, competitive threats, changes in technology, or mergers Thus, from a functional view of emotions, there are circum-
and acquisitions (Ashford, Lee, & Bobko, 1989; Schweiger & DeNisi, stances when fear leads to increased effort and aggressive action
1991). Fears stemming from external sources can also involve (fight), rather than flight (Blanchard & Blanchard, 2008). Following
shared feelings of threat to an organization and potential organiza- this, I argue that there are certain situations when fears of external
tional loss (Vuori & Huy, 2016). Fear of external threat is distinct threat can direct employees towards protecting the larger collec-
from fear of speaking up to one’s supervisor, one form of internal tive (fight). Different sources of threat invoke different patterns
threat, which involves interpersonal fears of retaliation, punish- of knowledge-sharing within an organization (Menon & Pfeffer,
ment, negative labels, or harming relationships with others 2003). For example, fear of external threats can motivate employ-
(Ashford, Rothbard, Piderit, & Dutton, 1998; Detert & ees to improve an organization’s position and performance relative
Edmondson, 2011; Milliken et al., 2003). A primary distinction to other organizations (Vuori & Huy, 2016). This motivation can
12 R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

spur employees to share information, innovative ideas, and sugges- ness is high, employees feel they can cope with fear of external
tions (Menon et al., 2006). Differing sources of threat also influence threat, which dampens pessimistic feelings of helplessness and
who or what an employee is willing to protect. For example, fear- enhances positive feelings that the situation can be changed by
ing external threat increases a person’s focus and attention on speaking up (Tangirala & Ramanujam, 2012). However, when per-
others facing similar threat (Gump & Kulik, 1997; Schachter, ceived supervisor openness is low, a relationship between fear of
1959). This suggests that employees may be more outwardly external threat and voice is unlikely, as employees are unlikely
focused and motivated to take action on behalf of others or the to view attempts to speak up as changing the situation or provid-
organization when fearing external threat. ing protection from external threats.
Extrapolating this to the domain of voice, employees fearing
external threat and concerned about protecting others or the orga- Hypothesis 1. Employee perceptions of supervisor openness mod-
nization may be motivated to offer suggestions to improve organi- erate the relationship between fear of external threat and voice,
zational functioning, or point out problems to help mitigate such that there is a positive relationship when perceived super-
potential harm (Hirschman, 1970; Liang, Farh, & Farh, 2012). In visor openness is high, and no relationship when perceived
other words, employees may speak up with the intention of help- supervisor openness is low.
ing the organization overcome potential threat. In this case, fearing
external threat can motivate employee efforts to change the status
quo, which is consistent with proactive conceptualizations of 2.2.2. The mediating role of prosocial motivation
employee voice (Van Dyne, Ang, & Botero, 2003). Beyond understanding the interactive relationship between fear
While fears of external threat may initially spark employee of external threat and perceptions of supervisor openness toward
motivation to speak up, a direct association between fear of exter- voice, it is important to understand potential mediating psycholog-
nal threat and voice is unlikely, given fear’s tendency towards ical processes. Fearing external threat can increase prosocial moti-
avoiding or withdrawing from a situation (Shaver, Schwartz, vation, defined as the desire to expend effort to benefit other
Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987). Moreover, the experience of fear damp- people (Batson, 1987). Prosocial motivation is conceptualized here
ens an individual’s perceptions that they can take action or deal as a psychological state arising from the work context, rather than
with the situation (Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003; Lerner & Keltner, as a stable individual difference (Grant, 2008). When employees
2001). From a functional perspective of emotions, then, certain fac- fear external sources, they often focus on shared threats to the
tors must be present to guide employees’ responses to fear towards organization, which increases their willingness and desire to help
approach and away from avoidance, and also to help employees protect the larger collective from threat (Vuori & Huy, 2016). For
cope with feelings of helplessness. example, Vuori and Huy found that managers fearing the introduc-
tion of new products from external rivals were motivated to take
2.2.1. Perceptions of supervisor openness influence voice when action to defend the organization’s competitive position. Further-
employees fear external threat more, fears of external threat can direct employee attention
An employee’s perceptions of the extent to which their immedi- towards other-focused motives, including taking action to protect
ate supervisor listens to, considers, and occasionally takes action other people (Taylor, 2006). This can occur as a result of emotional
based on their input and suggestions (Detert & Burris, 2007) likely contagion processes, whereby an individual facing external threat
moderate the fear of external threat-voice relationship for at least focuses on other people facing similar threats (Gump & Kulik,
two important theoretical reasons. First, in times of uncertainty 1997). Employees fearing external threat can thus be other-
marked by fear, employees look to cues from other individuals oriented, which is the hallmark of prosocial motivation (Grant &
for how to behave and respond (Gino, Brooks, & Schweitzer, Wrzesniewski, 2010).
2012; Schachter, 1959). Specifically, employees experiencing fear The experience of prosocial motivation following external
pay particular attention to their leader’s behavior (Rachman, threat is then enhanced by perceptions of supervisor openness.
1990), looking for guidance about what to do, and learning which Employees feel that they can take action on behalf of others when
actions are appropriate (Kish-Gephart et al., 2009). When supervi- they view their supervisors as open to input (Tangirala &
sors are open to input, they provide an important signal to employ- Ramanujam, 2012). That is, employees may believe a collective
ees that their suggestions and problems will be considered, and threat can be addressed by speaking up when perceiving that their
possibly lead to change (Detert & Burris, 2007). Furthermore, view- supervisor will listen to and incorporate ideas. Therefore, feeling
ing a supervisor as easily approachable can improve the likelihood high levels of external threat activates an initial desire to benefit
that an employee will speak up to change the situation (Saunders, others, which is strengthened when employees believe their super-
Sheppard, Knight, & Roth, 1992). Therefore, high levels of perceived visors are open to input. However, when employees perceive their
supervisor openness not only help to counteract fear’s tendency supervisor as closed to input, their prosocial motivation weakens;
toward withdrawal or avoidance, but also improve the likelihood they are less willing to expend effort on behalf of others because
of alternative responses. they view the situation as unlikely to change.
Second, in order to take constructive action following negative Increased prosocial motivation when fearing external threat
emotions, employees need a means to deal, or cope, with threat and perceiving supervisor openness is then likely to take the form
and fear’s characteristically intense emotional experience of employee voice. Employees experiencing fear and feeling a
(Lazarus, 1991). Leadership figures, such as supervisors, often pro- desire to benefit others often consider alternatives to withdrawing
vide the social resources employees need to cope (Lazarus & from the situation (Taylor, 2006). For example, employees fueled
Folkman, 1984). Supervisors who are open to input provide an out- by prosocial motives are likely to respond to external threat with
let for employees to express suggestions or concerns, and therefore extra effort to protect and promote the well-being of others or
a potential path to change the situation (Tangirala & Ramanujam, the organization (Grant & Wrzesniewski, 2010). In this case,
2012). Additionally, supervisors who are open to input help employees may try to protect the organization by pointing out
employees cope with fears of external threat by focusing them problems that may hinder performance, or by speaking up with
on positive rather than negative outcomes. Specifically, employees suggestions to enhance performance and mitigate potential com-
believe that there is a higher probability of successful change when petitive threat. Furthermore, employees with high levels of proso-
they perceive their supervisor to be high versus low in openness to cial motivation are willing to place the interests of others ahead of
input (Ashford et al., 1998). Thus, when perceived supervisor open- their own (Grant & Mayer, 2009), sparking action to benefit the
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 13

larger organization even despite personal risk, thereby increasing with 275 responses (50% overall employee survey response rate),
the likelihood of voice. and to 88 direct supervisors with 67 responses (76% overall super-
These arguments, combined with those for Hypothesis 1, sug- visor survey response rate). The recommendations of Rogelberg
gest a mediated moderation model. Fearing external threat and and Stanton (2007) were used to examine possible nonresponse
perceptions of supervisor openness are positively associated with bias. First, using data from ResearchCo and MilitaryCo, a wave
prosocial motivation, which in turn is positively associated with analysis was used to determine if early responders differed from
employee voice. late responders (the HealthCo paper surveys were not marked by
date, and thus could not be used in this analysis). A one-way
Hypothesis 2. Prosocial motivation mediates the moderated rela- ANOVA revealed no significant differences by wave for the inde-
tionship between fear of external threat and perceptions of pendent and dependent variables, all F’s(2, 172) < 1.25. Second,
supervisor openness predicting employee voice. the means for voice for responders vs. non-responders were com-
pared, as there was supervisor-rated data for some employees who
did not complete a survey. T-tests revealed no significant differ-
3. Overview of studies ence between responders (M = 3.82, SD = 1.57) versus non-
responders (M = 3.58, SD = 1.73, t = 1.48, p = 0.14).
These hypotheses were tested in two field studies. Study 1 was After combining data from these three organizations and
designed to test Hypothesis 1 using multisource data in a survey of accounting for missing data using listwise deletion, the final sam-
three organizations. Study 2 was designed to replicate and extend ple consisted of 183 employees (51% male). There were 56 super-
Study 1 by testing the interactive effect (Hypothesis 1), and also a visors in this sample who rated an average of 3.3 employees
possible mediating mechanism (Hypothesis 2), in a lagged survey (range 1–15 employees).
of full-time employees from a range of organizations.

4.2. Measures
4. Study 1
4.2.1. Fear of external threat
4.1. Method
Fear of external threat was measured using three items adapted
from Ashford et al. (1989) and Schweiger and DeNisi (1991).
4.1.1. Sample and procedure
Because the theoretical focus here is on the discrete emotion of
Study 1 involved a survey study of employees and their direct
fear, these items were adapted to specify fear and not other nega-
supervisors in three organizations: ResearchCo (a pseudonym), a
tive emotions (e.g., anger, frustration) associated with potential
medical research organization (e.g., cancer treatment and vaccine
organizational loss and layoffs (Jordan, Ashkanasy, & Hartel,
development) in the Northeast United States; HealthCo, a health
2002). Starting with ‘‘during the last month, how frequently have
services organization in the Northeast United States that provides
you felt fearful . . .,” the items were: ‘‘that the economic downturn
long-term inpatient nursing and rehabilitation care; and Mili-
would negatively impact this organization,” ‘‘that there would be
taryCo, a branch of the Army in the Western United States tasked
layoffs at this organization,” and ‘‘that this organization would lose
with managing the day-to-day functioning of military bases (e.g.,
sales or revenue,” a = 0.94. Participants rated the items on a 5-
public works, human resources, and logistics).
point scale where 1 = not at all and 5 = very often.
These three organizations share common characteristics that
These items were pre-tested to demonstrate internal reliability
are particularly relevant for testing hypotheses regarding fear of
and discriminant validity. The pre-test consisted of 244 full-time
external threat. Preliminary interviews revealed that the post-
employed individuals from a range of industries. Participants rated
2008 economic downturn negatively affected each organization.
the three items for fear of external threat along with items for
Specifically, ResearchCo received public funding to support a
other sources of fear at work. The three items for fear of external
majority of its research (e.g., from the National Institutes of
threat demonstrated high internal reliability in this sample,
Health), and possible funding reductions threatened to cut posi-
a = 0.85. As a test of discriminant validity, participants also rated
tions there. HealthCo was similarly affected, since it received pub-
items for fears when interacting with their immediate supervisor/-
lic funding at the state and county level. Finally, since MilitaryCo
boss (Shaver et al., 1987), and items rating the extent to which they
was funded by the federal government, announced federal budget
feared personal incompetence (Leary, 1983). I expected that these
cuts also meant plans for a reduced workforce. Taken together, this
survey items would form three distinct factors—fear of external
suggests that each organization’s employees had reason to fear
threat, fear of the boss, and fear of personal incompetence. A series
possible changes due to external sources.
of confirmatory factor analyses were used to test this structure.
Two voluntary surveys were administered at each organization:
The three-factor model demonstrated acceptable fit to the data
employees provided ratings of the independent variables and con-
(CFI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.09) based on standard cutoffs (Kline, 2005).
trols, and their direct supervisors provided ratings of the depen-
Furthermore, this three-factor model had superior fit to the one-
dent variable. Surveys were administered online via a
factor model (CFI = 0.81, RMSEA = 0.21), as well as the best-fitting
confidential survey link sent to employees at ResearchCo and Mil-
two-factor model, consisting of fear of external threat and of the
itaryCo, and administered via hard copy at HealthCo because sev-
boss on one factor, and fear of personal incompetence on the other
eral employees lacked access to email at work. Supervisor
factor (CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.11). Furthermore, a nested chi-square
surveys were electronically administered at all three organizations.
difference test indicated that the three-factor solution was a supe-
The same surveys were used at each location, with only minor
rior fit to both the one-factor (Dv2[3] = 191.30, p < 0.001) and best-
wording changes (e.g., ‘‘reduction in force” versus ‘‘layoffs”).
fitting two-factor (Dv2[2] = 23.38, p < 0.001) models, respectively.
Response rates were comparable across the three organizations.
At ResearchCo, 126 of 264 employees (47% response rate), and 38
of 50 (76%) direct supervisors completed the survey; at HealthCo, 4.2.2. Perceptions of supervisor openness
75 of 167 employees (45%), and 8 of 10 direct supervisors (80%) Each employee’s perceptions of supervisor openness were mea-
completed the survey; and at MilitaryCo, 74 of 117 (63%) man- sured with three items adapted from Detert and Burris (2007).
agers, and 21 of 28 (75%) direct supervisors (director-level person- Employees rated the following items on a 7-point agree/disagree
nel) completed it. In total, surveys were sent to 548 employees scale: ‘‘my supervisor uses my suggestions,” ‘‘my supervisor
14 R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

considers ideas from subordinates,” and ‘‘my supervisor rejects variables were grand-mean centered before creating the interac-
new ideas” (reverse-coded), a = 0.80. tion terms. The independent variables were theorized and ana-
lyzed at the individual level (i.e., level 1 predictors).
4.2.3. Voice
Supervisors rated each of their employee’s frequency of voice on 4.4. Results
a 7-point scale (1 = not at all and 7 = always) with three items
adapted from Van Dyne and LePine (1998) and Liang et al. Before testing the hypotheses, I evaluated the study variables’
(2012). Starting with ‘‘how often does this individual,” the items distinctiveness through a series of confirmatory factor analyses.
were: ‘‘speak up with ideas for new work-related policies and pro- The hypothesized six-factor model had acceptable fit to the data
cedures?,” ‘‘point out how we could make changes to make the (CFI = 0.92, SRMR = 0.07, RMSEA = 0.06, v2/df ratio = 1.84), and pro-
organization or group better?,” and ‘‘speak up with concerns about vided substantial improvement in fit indices over theoretically
work not being done effectively?,” a = 0.87. plausible alternatives (see Table 1).
Descriptive statistics and correlations can be found in Table 2.
4.2.4. Control variables Null random coefficient models with no predictors were used to
Negative affect, one’s trait disposition to experience negative examine the amount of variance accounted for by organization-
emotions, was used as a control because it could influence multiple and supervisor-level effects. For voice, the ICC[1] for
variables in the model. This variable has been shown to negatively organization-level effects was 0.19, and for supervisor-level effects
relate to voice (Venkataramani & Tangirala, 2010). Furthermore, it was 0.50, suggesting that 19% and 50% of the variance in voice
the effects of fear of external threat on voice could be explained resided between organizations and between supervisors, respec-
by one’s trait-level propensity to experience negative emotions, tively. Additionally, the ICC[1] for organizational-level effects of
such as fear, rather than the experience of fear of external threat fearing external threat was 0.38, indicating some level of agree-
itself. Employees thus reported their levels of trait negative affect ment among employees in each organization regarding a collective
using the ten-item scale from the PANAS (Watson, Clark, & threat from external sources.
Tellegen, 1988), a = 0.86. Table 3 presents the results of the random intercept models. I
The personality traits conscientiousness and extraversion were followed the moderated regression procedures recommended by
used as controls, because these traits generally positively associate Aiken and West (1991), entering the control variables and inde-
with voice (LePine & Van Dyne, 2001). These traits were self- pendent variables in Step 1 and the interaction term in Step 2.
reported using the Mini-IPIP scales (Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Hypothesis 1 predicted that employee perceptions of supervisor
Lucas, 2006). I also controlled for tenure (years in the organization), openness would moderate the relationship between fear of exter-
because higher levels of tenure associate with employee voice nal threat and voice. Model 2 shows a significant interaction for
(Botero, 2013). Additionally, because employees may be less likely supervisor openness and fear of external threat predicting voice
to speak up in larger groups (LePine & Van Dyne, 1998), group size (t = 2.25, p < 0.05). Simple slopes were plotted to better understand
was included as a control. the nature of this interaction (Aiken & West, 1991). This analysis
revealed a positive relationship approaching statistical significance
4.3. Analytical strategy between fear of external threat and voice when supervisor open-
ness is high (b = 0.16, p = 0.07), and no relationship when supervi-
Since individual employees were nested within supervisors, and sor openness is low (b = 0.08, p = 0.33). While neither simple
supervisor groups nested within the three organizations, I used slope is significantly different from zero, the significant interaction
random intercept models to control for differences at the supervi- coefficient indicates that the two slopes are significantly different
sor and organization level (Singer, 1998). Following Aiken and from one another (Aiken & West, 1991). Fig. 1 indicates that the
West’s (1991) recommendations, the independent and moderating form of the interaction is as predicted, with a positive relationship

Table 1
Study 1: Results of confirmatory factor analysis of survey items.

Model v2 df v2/df Dv2 RMSEA CFI SRMR


ratio
1. Fear of external threat, perceptions of supervisor openness, controls, and voice as distinct factors 567.70 309 1.84 0.06 0.92 0.07
2. Same as model 1 with fear of external threat and perceptions of supervisor openness as one factor 1048.25 314 3.34 96.11*** 0.11 0.68 0.12
3. One factor 1847.01 324 5.70 85.29*** 0.16 0.31 0.15
***
p < 0.001.

Table 2
Study 1: Means, standard deviations, and correlations.

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Tenure 5.84 5.51
2. Conscientiousness 5.69 1.02 0.03 (0.62)
3. Extraversion 4.87 1.11 0.20** 0.21** (0.66)
4. Group size 12.34 9.42 0.34** 0.01 0.07
5. Trait NA 2.23 0.69 0.10 0.21** 0.05 0.02 (0.86)
6. Fear of external threat 3.11 1.84 0.19** 0.07 0.03 0.01 0.07 (0.94)
7. Perceptions of supervisor openness 5.57 1.31 0.10 0.15* 0.25** 0.19** 0.14 0.02 (0.80)
8. Voice 3.92 1.63 0.10 0.06 0.08 0.05 0.16* 0.35** 0.04 (0.87)

Note. N = 183. Coefficients in the parentheses are Cronbach’s alpha values.


**
p < 0.01.
*
p < 0.05.
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 15

Table 3 mediators (Hypothesis 2) using a lagged survey design with


Study 1: Results of multilevel models predicting voice. employees from a range of organizations.
Variables DV: Voice
Model 1 Model 2 5. Study 2
Intercept 4.32** (0.36) 4.31** (0.34)
Controls 5.1. Method
Tenure 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02)
Group size 0.02 (0.03) 0.02 (0.03)
Conscientiousness 0.11 (0.09) 0.09 (0.09) 5.1.1. Sample and procedure
Extraversion 0.01 (0.08) 0.02 (0.08) Study 2 used lagged survey data at two points in time from
Trait NA 0.24* (0.12) 0.25* (0.12) alumni of a MBA program at a university in the Northeastern
Main effects United States. To participate, the alumni had to be employed at
Fear of external threat 0.02 (0.07) 0.04 (0.07) the time of the study. In Fall 2014 (Time 1), surveys were emailed
Perceptions of supervisor openness 0.05 (0.06) 0.04 (0.06)
to 3410 alumni who graduated between the years 2000–2014.1 As
Interaction effects an incentive to complete the survey, participants were entered into a
Supervisor openness  fear of external 0.08* (0.04)
raffle for an iPad, and could also elect to receive a confidential per-
threat
2 log likelihood 595.90 590.80 sonalized feedback report based on their survey responses. Initial
D 2 log likelihood (df) 5.10* (1) responses at Time 1 were received from 636 alumni (an 18.7%
response rate). A total of 16 participants were not currently
Note: N = 183. Unstandardized coefficients are reported, with standard errors in
parentheses.
employed, and 50 did not provide complete data, resulting in a sam-
**
p < 0.01. ple of 570 at Time 1 (16.7% effective response rate). This response
*
p < 0.05. rate is similar to previous studies using alumni samples. For exam-
ple, Seibert, Kraimer, and Crant (2001) obtained an effective
response rate of 18% using similar methodology. Additionally, the
5 response rate reported here is superior to the response rate obtained
by the university’s alumni office (approximately 9–10%).
4.5 To reduce common method and source biases associated with
Low Percepons
self-reported data, Time 2 surveys were emailed six weeks after
4 of Supervisor Time 1, because research demonstrates that lags of at least four
Voice

Openness weeks can reduce common method variance (Ostroff, Kinicki, &
3.5 High Percepons Clark, 2002). At Time 2, 421 initial responses were received
of Supervisor (66.2% response rate from Time 1). The final sample consisted of
3 Openness
391 respondents (72% male) who had complete data for both time
periods.2 The participants worked in a range of occupations, includ-
2.5 ing professional or information technology services (25%), manufac-
Low Fear of External High Fear of External
Threat Threat turing (20%), finance (17%), health care (8%), and education (8%).

Fig. 1. Interaction between fear of external threat and perceptions of supervisor


openness on voice, Study 1. 5.2. Measures

The independent variables, mediating and moderating vari-


between fear of external threat and voice when employees per- ables, and controls were assessed at Time 1, and the dependent
ceive high levels of supervisor openness, and no relationship when variable (voice) was assessed at Time 2. Unless otherwise noted,
they perceive low levels of supervisor openness. Thus, Hypothesis participants rated these scales using a 7-point strongly disagree/
1 was supported. agree scale.

5.2.1. Fear of external threat


4.5. Discussion
Employees reported how frequently (1 = not at all and 5 = very
often) they experienced fear of external threat at work using 5
This study revealed that fear of external threat was positively
items, a = 0.87. The scale included the same three items from
associated with improvement-oriented voice, but only when
Study 1, plus two additional items to enhance the measure’s con-
employees perceived their supervisor as open to input. In the mul-
tent validity in terms of involving competitive and industry threats
tilevel regression analyses, there was no main effect of fear of
(please see the Appendix A for the full list of items). As a check of
external threat on voice. This result is consistent with a functional
this measure’s validity, participants forwarded a separate survey to
view of emotions, suggesting that the fear-behavior relationship is
a significant other, friend, or family member to rate the extent to
contingent (Frijda, 1986; Roseman, 2011). In particular, Study 1’s
which the participant had ‘‘discussed being fearful about . . .” the
results identify supervisor openness as a key moderating factor
same five items. A total of 76 usable responses were received.
of this relationship, suggesting that employee perceptions of leader
behavior can be particularly important for shaping voice during 1
A power calculation revealed that using alumni graduating in these years would
times of economic and organizational uncertainty. Although the
provide a sufficient sample size to replicate the effects from Study 1, based on the
findings from Study 1 provide initial support for Hypothesis 1, it expected response rates the alumni office provided to the author.
was important to constructively replicate the finding in a different 2
Following the recommendations of Rogelberg and Stanton (2007), a wave analysis
sample in order to strengthen confidence in the result’s reliability was used to examine possible nonresponse bias by examining whether early
and generalizability. Furthermore, Study 1 involved a cross- responders at Time 1 differed from late responders at Time 1. A one-way ANOVA
revealed no significant differences by wave for the Time 1 independent and control
sectional design, making it difficult to explore mediating mecha- variables, all F’s(1, 570) < 1.12. Additionally, a comparison of the means of the
nisms and to make causal claims. Therefore, Study 2 was designed independent and control variables for Time 2 responders vs. Time 2 non-responders
to replicate and extend the results from Study 1 by testing for revealed no significant differences, all t’s(569) < 1.65.
16 R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

The self-report measure and the significant other measure were Hierarchical ordinary least squares (OLS) regression was used to
significantly correlated (r = 0.35, p < 0.001). test Hypothesis 1. As shown in Table 5 Model 2c, there was a sig-
nificant interaction between fear of external threat and supervisor
5.2.2. Perceptions of supervisor openness openness predicting employee voice (b = 0.08, t = 2.26, p < 0.05).
The 3-item scale from Detert and Burris (2007) was used to Following the recommendations of Aiken and West (1991), simple
measure each employee’s perceptions of supervisor openness, slopes analysis revealed a significant and positive relationship
a = 0.88. between fear of external threat and voice when supervisor open-
ness is high (b = 0.12, t = 2.09, p < 0.05), and no relationship when
5.2.3. Prosocial motivation supervisor openness is low (b = 0.06, t = 1.02, p = 0.31). As
Prosocial motivation was measured with the 4-item prosocial shown in Fig. 2, the form of the interaction is as predicted. Thus,
motivation scale from Grant (2008), a = 0.95. Starting with the Hypothesis 1 was supported, replicating the results of Study 1.
question ‘‘Why are you motivated to do your work?,” example To test for prosocial motivation’s mediating effects, I used medi-
items include ‘‘because I care about benefiting others through my ated moderation procedures using Model 8 from the PROCESS
work” and ‘‘because I want to have a positive impact on others.” macro developed by Hayes (2013). First, I examined the interactive
These items refer to prosocial motivation at work, rather than a effect of fear of external threat and perceptions of supervisor open-
stable individual difference. ness on prosocial motivation. The results shown in Model 1c of
Table 5 revealed a significant interaction predicting prosocial moti-
vation (b = 0.14, t = 2.41, p < 0.05). Next, the interactive effect of
5.2.4. Voice
fear of external threat and supervisor openness on voice was exam-
At Time 2, participants self-reported their frequency of voice at
ined with prosocial motivation included in the analysis. Model 2d
work using 4 items from Van Dyne and LePine (1998), a = 0.90. An
shows that the interactive effect was reduced in strength at con-
example item is, ‘‘How often do you speak up to your supervisor
ventional levels of statistical significance (b = 0.07, t = 1.97,
with ideas for new work-related policies and procedures?” Partic-
p = 0.05). In this model, prosocial motivation significantly and pos-
ipants rated the items using a 5-point scale where 1 = not at all and
itively predicted voice (b = 0.07, t = 2.31, p < 0.05). Finally, boot-
5 = very often.
strapping procedures were used to examine the significance of
the mediated (indirect) effect of fear of external threat and super-
5.2.5. Control variables visor openness on voice via prosocial motivation, constructing
In Study 2, extrinsic motivation was included in the analyses to bias-corrected confidence intervals based on 1000 random samples
control for self-protective motives—such as job security—in addi- (Hayes, 2013; Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007). The indirect effect
tion to the hypothesized other-focused motives that are character- is significant, and mediation present, if the 95% bias-corrected
istic of prosocial motivation. Starting with the question ‘‘Why are bootstrap confidence interval does not contain zero. The results
you motivated to do your work?,” example items include ‘‘because revealed that the 95% bias-corrected confidence interval for proso-
I need the income” and ‘‘because I need this job” (a = 0.88). cial motivation did not include zero (point estimate = 0.01, 95% CI
Additionally, as in Study 1, tenure (years in the current organi- [0.01, 0.03]). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was supported, as prosocial moti-
zation), conscientiousness, and extraversion (Gosling, Rentfrow, & vation mediated the moderating effects of fear of external threat
Swann, 2003) were included as controls. and supervisor openness on voice.

5.3. Results
5.4. Discussion
Correlations and descriptive statistics are provided in Table 4.
Before testing the hypotheses, we evaluated the Time 1 study vari- Study 2 replicated and extended the results of Study 1. Percep-
ables’ distinctiveness via confirmatory factor analysis. The hypoth- tions of supervisor openness moderated the relationship between
esized seven-factor model had strong fit to the data (CFI = 0.97, fear of external threat and voice. As in Study 1, fearing external
SRMR = 0.04, RMSEA = 0.04). The hypothesized model also demon- threat was positively associated with voice when employees per-
strated substantial improvement in fit indices over theoretically ceived their supervisors as open to input. Also, as predicted, proso-
plausible alternatives, such as a six-factor model with the indepen- cial motivation mediated the interactive effect of fear of external
dent and moderating variables on one factor (CFI = 0.86, threat and supervisor openness. The combination of fearing exter-
SRMR = 0.09, RMSEA = 0.10, nested Dv2(6) = 110.77, p < 0.001), nal threat and high perceived supervisor openness was positively
and a one-factor model (CFI = 0.17, SRMR = 0.21, RMSEA = 0.23, associated with prosocial motivation, which in turn was positively
nested Dv2(21) = 222.89, p < 0.001). associated with improvement-oriented voice.

Table 4
Study 2: Means, standard deviations, and correlations.

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Tenure 6.70 5.94
2. Conscientiousness 6.09 0.87 0.01 (0.50)
3. Extraversion 4.57 1.44 0.02 0.03 (0.77)
4. Extrinsic motivation 5.45 1.16 0.06 0.01 0.06 (0.88)
5. Prosocial motivation 5.19 1.36 0.08 0.15** 0.14** 0.06 (0.95)
6. Fear of external threat 2.22 0.99 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.02 0.08 (0.87)
7. Perceptions of supervisor openness 5.78 1.13 0.04 0.05 0.12* 0.02 0.13** 0.01 (0.88)
8. Voice (Time 2) 3.51 0.83 0.07 0.03 0.14** 0.15** 0.15** 0.04 0.19** (0.90)

Note: N = 391. Coefficients in the parentheses are Cronbach’s alpha values.


**
p < 0.01.
*
p < 0.05.
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 17

Table 5
Study 2: Results of regression analyses predicting voice.

Variables DV: Prosocial motivation DV: Voice


Model 1a Model 1b Model 1c Model 2a Model 2b Model 2c Model 2d
Intercept 5.08*** (0.10) 5.07*** (0.10) 5.07*** (0.10) 3.46*** (0.06) 3.46*** (0.06) 3.47*** (0.06) 3.47*** (0.06)
Controls
Tenure 0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01)
Conscientiousness 0.21** (0.08) 0.20* (0.08) 0.20** (0.08) 0.02 (0.05) 0.02 (0.05) 0.01 (0.05) 0.00 (0.05)
Extraversion 0.13** (0.05) 0.11* (0.05) 0.11* (0.05) 0.07* (0.03) 0.06* (0.03) 0.06* (0.03) 0.05 (0.03)
Extrinsic motivation 0.10 (0.06) 0.09 (0.06) 0.09 (0.06) 0.10** (0.04) 0.10** (0.04) 0.11** (0.04) 0.11* (0.04)
Main effects
Fear of external threat 0.14* (0.07) 0.13 (0.07) 0.03 (0.04) 0.03 (0.04) 0.04 (0.04)
Perceptions of supervisor openness 0.13* (0.06) 0.17** (0.06) 0.13** (0.04) 0.15*** (0.04) 0.14** (0.04)
Interaction effects
Supervisor openness  fear of external 0.14* (0.06) 0.08* (0.04) 0.07* (0.04)
threat
Mediator
Prosocial motivation 0.07* (0.03)
R2 0.05 0.07 0.09 0.04 0.08 0.09 0.10
D R2 0.02* 0.01* 0.03** 0.01* 0.01*
F value 5.11** (4, 385) 4.95*** (6, 383) 5.12*** (7, 382) 4.42** (4, 385) 5.15*** (6, 383) 5.18*** (7, 382) 5.26*** (8, 381)
DF 5.11** (4, 385) 4.45** (2, 383) 5.78* (1, 382) 4.42** (4, 385) 6.34** (2, 383) 5.08* (1, 382) 5.35* (1, 381)

Notes: N = 391. Unstandardized coefficients are reported, with standard errors in parentheses.
***
p < 0.001.
**
p < 0.01.
*
p < 0.05.

4.5 results from two studies suggest that there are situations when
fears of external threat may positively, rather than negatively,
4 influence employee voice. In so doing, this article provides evi-
dence that there are instances when fearing external threat can
3.5 Low Percepons of lead to constructive patterns of communication within organiza-
Voice

Supervisor Openness
tions (Vuori & Huy, 2016).
3
High Percepons of A related contribution lies in extending previous voice research
2.5 Supervisor Openness by suggesting that experienced emotions—such as fear of external
threat—interact with cognitions and perceptions of supervisor
2 behavior to promote employee voice. Doing so follows calls for
Low High research exploring how cognitive and emotional processes may
Fear of External Threat combine to influence voice (Morrison, 2011). While fear can cer-
tainly produce deeply rooted automatic or taken-for-granted
Fig. 2. Interaction between fear of external threat and perceptions of supervisor behavioral responses that lead to silence (Detert & Edmondson,
openness on voice, Study 2. 2011), the results here suggest that perceptions of the situation
can modify how fear is processed and acted upon. Thus, this
6. General discussion research highlights the value in taking an emotional and cognitive
perspective when exploring factors that influence voice (Grant,
Across two studies, fear of external threat was positively related 2013), suggesting that future theory and research should simulta-
to voice when employees perceived their supervisor as open to neously consider both perspectives and potential interactions.
input. Additionally, there was no main effect of fearing external Similarly, this article contributes by identifying a contingent
threat in either study’s regression analyses, supporting a functional relationship between fear of external threat and voice. The interac-
view of emotions that this relationship is contingent on other fac- tion between supervisor openness and fear of external threat sup-
tors. Furthermore, Study 2’s findings provide insight into a psycho- ports the notion that supervisor behavior is particularly important
logical mechanism driving the interactive effect, suggesting that in shaping how employees respond to fear (Detert & Burris, 2007).
prosocial motivation can lead employees to speak up when fearing Conceptually positioning supervisor openness as a moderator
external threat. diverges from previous research that treats this construct as a pre-
dictor variable with mediating effects on voice via psychological
6.1. Theoretical contributions safety (Detert & Burris, 2007). The rationale for doing so follows
from a functional view of emotions, with fear’s impact contingent
This article’s primary contribution lies in theoretically and on the presence or absence of certain stimulus conditions
empirically identifying when employees speak up while experienc- (Roseman et al., 1994). In this case, an employee’s perception of
ing external threat. Previous research has focused on how fears of his/her supervisor’s receptiveness to input is an important contex-
speaking up reduce voice, with employees generally remaining tual stimulus that can spark voice when fearing external threat.
silent when fearing potential repercussions from supervisors Thus, the results of this article support conceptualizations of lead-
(Burris, Detert, & Chiaburu, 2008; Milliken et al., 2003). However, ership as influencing employee behavior at multiple points in the
not all sources of fear influence voice in the same way. Complicat- causal chain, serving moderating or mediating roles (Avolio,
ing the notion that fear always has a negative impact on voice, the 2007; de Vries, Roe, & Taillieu, 2002).
18 R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

This article also contributes by identifying a psychological partially offset in Study 1 with the use of multisource data, and
mechanism for explaining why the combination of fear of external also by employing a lagged design in Study 2, which has been
threat and supervisor openness facilitates voice. The results sug- shown to mitigate common method and source biases (Ostroff
gest that prosocial motivation to protect and promote the well- et al., 2002).
being of co-workers and the organization can spark voice when Although lagged survey data was collected in Study 2, alterna-
employees fear external threat and perceive their supervisor as tive causal pathways cannot be ruled out. Furthermore, data for
open to input. This also suggests that employees who sense a the independent, moderating, and mediating variables in Study 2
shared threat to the organization may focus their attention on were collected from the same source at the same time. Therefore,
other- versus self-protective motives. Additionally, supervisors future research could employ experimental designs to more rigor-
who are open to input help transform prosocial motives into voice, ously test for causality, temporally separating fear of external
signaling to employees that active efforts on behalf of the organi- threat from mediating mechanisms. Doing so may present an eco-
zation will be positively received. logical validity challenge, designing manipulations such that fear
This research also builds on previous work exploring the link of external threat impacts participants in a lab setting in the same
between job insecurity—defined as an awareness of, or emotional way as employees facing economic threat and uncertainty in orga-
reactions to, the possibility of job loss (Huang, Niu, Lee, & nizational settings. Moreover, manipulating high levels of fear of
Ashford, 2012)—and proactive and citizenship behavior. Huang external threat in a lab setting, such as creating a scenario whereby
et al. (2013) found that job insecurity was positively related to students learn they may suffer actual consequences (e.g., a reduc-
the proactive use of impression management tactics. Interestingly, tion in student scholarships) may pose ethical and moral dilem-
while their findings suggest that fears of job insecurity can moti- mas. Despite these challenges, I, along with other voice scholars
vate action to proactively protect the self, my findings suggest that (Kish-Gephart et al., 2009), argue that technological advances such
fears of external threat can motivate action to protect the organi- as physiological measures may provide a means to detect fine-
zation. Additionally, the results of the studies reported here are grained differences in the experience of fear (Öhman, 2008). Such
consistent with the notion that job insecurity can have variable measures may help detect differences between scenarios involving
effects on citizenship behavior contingent on certain factors different fear sources.
(Probst, 2008). For example, job insecurity may be positively or Additionally, there may be potential mediators beyond proso-
negatively associated with citizenship behaviors, such as helping cial motivation that direct fear of external threat towards voice.
and conscientiousness, depending on the nature of the employ- For example, employees fearing external threat may speak up
ment contract (full-time vs. contract employees) (Feather & based on self-interested, rather than, or in addition to, prosocial
Rauter, 2004). This article demonstrates that perceptions of super- motives. In this case, employees may speak up to improve organi-
visor behavior play an important role in shaping whether job inse- zational functioning, but with self-protective intent to secure their
curity resulting from external threat is positively related to job. Such motives may be particularly relevant when supervisors
citizenship behavior, extending previous work by linking fears of are open to and value voice. From this perspective, employees
external threat to challenging (e.g., voice) rather than affiliative speak up as a way of standing out and enhancing their image
forms of citizenship behavior. and value to the organization (Bolino, 1999), hoping to secure
Although the main contributions are to the literature on their job should the need for layoffs from economic downturn
employee voice, this research also contributes to the nascent and arise. Another possibility is that employees fearing external
growing body of work exploring when negative emotions such as threat speak up when they are highly identified with their organi-
anger and guilt produce constructive outcomes in organizations zation. Such identification may motivate action to protect the
(Bohns & Flynn, 2012; Lebel, 2016; Stickney & Geddes, 2016). In larger organization, such as pointing out problems and proposing
particular, this article suggests that fear produces constructive solutions. Future research should explore both of these
forms of communication under certain circumstances. While emo- possibilities.
tions research suggests that fear precludes individuals from taking Study 1’s results demonstrate a positive relationship between
risks, leading to risk-averse choices and cautious behavior (Lerner fear of external threat and voice when supervisor openness is high,
& Keltner, 2001; Maner & Gerend, 2007; Raghunathan & Pham, consistent with Hypothesis 1. However, a close examination of
1999), my findings suggest that employees may take a risk and Fig. 1 reveals that perceptions of supervisor openness make more
speak up because of fear. This supports a functional view of emo- of a difference in voice at low levels of fear of external threat,
tions, whereby fear of external threat signals that action is needed which is inconsistent with the logic that stronger effects would
to remedy the situation (George, 2011). Furthermore, identifying be found at high, rather than low, levels of fear of external threat.
the specific circumstances when fear may lead to behaviors other It could be the case that employees in this sample who experienced
than flight provides a more precise understanding of the contin- low external threat had less motivation to speak up about or iden-
gent nature of the negative emotion-behavior relationship tify problems, even when their supervisors were open to input. Or,
(Roseman, 2011). this could be driven by the lack of a positive relationship between
perceptions of supervisor openness and voice, a potential artifact of
6.2. Limitations, strengths, and future directions Study 1 consistent with recent research also finding a null relation-
ship (Liu, Tangirala, & Ramanujam, 2013). It is also important to
The results of this research should be qualified in light of its note that Study 2’s results support the logic for Hypothesis 1
limitations, many of which suggest potential avenues for future (Fig. 2), in terms of a positive relationship between fearing external
research. Study 1 was a cross-sectional field study of three organi- threat and voice when supervisor openness is high, and also sup-
zations, preventing tests of causal inference. Study 2 involved port the prediction that perceptions of supervisor openness would
self-report measures of voice, and could therefore be subject to have more of an impact on employee voice at higher external
common method and source biases. A strength of the paper is threat levels.
the multi-study approach used to help offset the limitations of While outside the scope of this article, future research should
each study. Specifically, Study 2 employed a lagged survey design explore other possible proactive behaviors following fear of exter-
of employees from a range of organizations to better make causal nal threat. There could be circumstances when fearing external
inferences and also to replicate the findings from Study 1 in a sep- threat leads to action that prevents change, such as proactively
arate sample. Additionally, potential common method bias was speaking up in support of organizational strategy and policies
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 19

(Maynes & Podsakoff, 2014). For example, supportive voice follow- Appendix A
ing external threat may be more likely when employees highly
identify with their organization and do not want to see it change. Fear of external threat survey items (adapted from Ashford
There may also be instances when employees without other job et al., 1989; Schweiger & DeNisi, 1991).
alternatives may be more self-focused, and thus increase task During the last month, how frequently have you felt fearful . . .
effort or seek feedback in efforts to secure their job. Furthermore, (1 = not at all, 5 = very often).
employees may use other forms of proactive behavior, such as
impression management tactics, to ingratiate themselves with 1. That the economic downturn would negatively impact
their supervisor and secure their job status (Huang et al., 2013). this organization
Therefore, future research could benefit by identifying when fear 2. That this organization would lose sales or revenue
of external threat leads to self-focused rather than other- or 3. That there would be layoffs at this organization
organizationally-focused proactive behavior. 4. That your organization would lose business to a
Moreover, while this article takes a first step towards under- competitora
standing when employees might speak up fearing external threat, 5. That an industry downturn would negatively impact this
it did not explore how employees cope with fear of speaking up organizationa
to express their voice. Future research could thus explore factors
that lead employees to voice even if they fear speaking up. For a
Items used in Study 2 only.
example, it could be that fears of negative reactions from super-
visors can positively affect other forms of employee voice, such as
speaking up defensively to protect themselves by directing atten-
tion away from the self and towards other employees (Van Dyne References
et al., 2003).
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
interactions. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
Ashford, S. J., Lee, C., & Bobko, P. (1989). Content, causes, and consequences of job
6.3. Practical contributions and conclusion insecurity: A theory-based measure and substantive test. Academy of
Management Journal, 32(4), 803–829.
Ashford, S. J., Rothbard, N. P., Piderit, S. K., & Dutton, J. E. (1998). Out on a limb: The
This research provides practical implications for managers and role of context and impression management in selling gender-equity issues.
leaders in organizations. The results from two studies suggest that Administrative Science Quarterly, 43(1), 23–57.
leader behavior is particularly important during times of economic Avolio, B. J. (2007). Promoting more integrative strategies for leadership theory-
building. American Psychologist, 62(1), 25–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-
crisis and uncertainty. Specifically, supervisors can take steps to 066X.62.1.25.
motivate employees to constructively respond despite fear. Super- Basch, J., & Fisher, C. D. (2000). Affective events-emotions matrix: A classification of
visors can encourage employees to act and speak up when experi- work events and associated emotions. In N. M. Ashkanasy, C. E. J. Hartel, & W.
Zerbe (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice
encing fear of external threat by being more open and receptive to
(pp. 36–48). Westport, CT: Quorum Books.
ideas and suggestions. Doing so signals to employees that they can Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Berkowitz
manage their fears by taking action to help improve their organiza- (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 65–122). New York,
tion or work unit. At the same time, the results do not suggest that NY: Academic Press.
Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (2008). Defensive behaviors, fear, and anxiety. In
leaders should instill a sense of fear in their employees. Indeed, a R. J. Blanchard, D. C. Blanchard, G. Griebel, & D. Nutt (Eds.). Handbook of anxiety
large body of research suggests that when leaders or supervisors and fear (Vol. 17, pp. 63–79). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Academic Press.
are viewed as the source of fear, employees respond by withdraw- Bohns, V. K., & Flynn, F. J. (2012). Guilt by design: Structuring organizations to elicit
guilt as an affective reaction to failure. Organization Science, 24(4), 1157–1173.
ing or by remaining silent. Therefore, in order to encourage http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1120.0787.
employees to speak up during times of economic uncertainty, Bolino, M. C. (1999). Citizenship and impression management: Good soldiers or
managers must recognize when employees fear external threat good actors? Academy of Management Review, 24(1), 82–98. http://dx.doi.org/
10.2307/259038.
and help them cope with this fear, and not create fear themselves. Botero, I. C. (2013). Individual correlates of employee voice: What do we know so
Such efforts from managers may help organizations capture the far? Where should we go next? In R. J. Burke & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Voice and
benefits of voice. whistleblowing in organizations: Overcoming fear, fostering courage and
unleashing candour (pp. 72–91). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.
This article suggests that there are times when employees who Burris, E. R., Detert, J. R., & Chiaburu, D. S. (2008). Quitting before leaving: The
fear external threat can be motivated to speak up with mediating effects of psychological attachment and detachment on voice. Journal
improvement-oriented voice. The challenge for supervisors and of Applied Psychology, 93(4), 912–922.
de Vries, R. E., Roe, R. A., & Taillieu, T. C. B. (2002). Need for leadership as a
leaders is to recognize when these fears result from external
moderator of the relationships between leadership and individual outcomes.
sources, and then to remain open to input as well as foster employ- The Leadership Quarterly, 13(2), 121–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843
ees’ prosocial motivation to help the organization. (02)00097-8.
Detert, J. R., & Burris, E. R. (2007). Leadership behavior and employee voice: Is the
door really open? Academy of Management Journal, 50(4), 869–884. http://dx.
doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2007.26279183.
Acknowledgements Detert, J. R., Burris, E. R., Harrison, D. A., & Martin, S. R. (2013). Voice flows to and
around leaders: Understanding when units are helped or hurt by employee
I would like to thank my dissertation committee members – voice. Administrative Science Quarterly, 58(4), 624–668. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0001839213510151.
Sigal Barsade, Adam Grant, Katherine Klein, and Nancy Rothbard Detert, J. R., & Edmondson, A. C. (2011). Implicit voice theories: Taken-for-granted
– as well as Tim Maynes, Elizabeth McClean, Elizabeth Morrison, rules of self-censorship at work. Academy of Management Journal, 54(3),
John Sumanth, and the three anonymous reviewers for their 461–488.
Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The mini-IPIP
insightful advice and feedback on previous versions of this paper. scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality.
Funding was generously provided by the Center for Leadership Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 192–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-
and Change Management at the Wharton School, University of 3590.18.2.192.
Dooley, R. S., & Fryxell, G. E. (1999). Attaining decision quality and commitment
Pennsylvania, as well as the David Berg Center for Ethics and Lead- from dissent: The moderating effects of loyalty and competence in strategic
ership of the Katz Graduate School of Business, through the BNY decision-making teams. Academy of Management Journal, 42(4), 389–402. http://
Mellon Foundation of Western Pennsylvania. dx.doi.org/10.2307/257010.
20 R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21

Edmondson, A. C. (1999). Psychological safety and learning behavior in work Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and
teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(2), 350–383. Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Elfenbein, H. A. (2007). Emotion in organizations. Academy of Management Annals, 1 3514.81.1.146.
(1), 315–386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/078559812. Liang, J., Farh, C. I. C., & Farh, J.-L. (2012). Psychological antecedents of promotive
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. J. and prohibitive voice: A two-wave examination. Academy of Management
Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences Journal, 55(1), 71–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0176.
(pp. 572–595). New York: Oxford University Press. Liu, W., Tangirala, S., & Ramanujam, R. (2013). The relational antecedents of voice
Feather, N. T., & Rauter, K. A. (2004). Organizational citizenship behaviours in targeted at different leaders. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(5), 841–851.
relation to job status, job insecurity, organizational commitment and http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032913.
identification, job satisfaction and work values. Journal of Occupational and Mackenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2011). Challenge-oriented
Organizational Psychology, 77(1), 81–94. organizational citizenship behaviors and organizational effectiveness: Do
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press. challenge-oriented behaviors really have an impact on the organization’s
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, bottom line? Personnel Psychology, 64(3), 559–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 j.1744-6570.2011.01219.x.
(2), 212–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212. Maner, J. K., & Gerend, M. A. (2007). Motivationally selective risk judgments: Do fear
Geddes, D., & Callister, R. R. (2007). Crossing the line(s): A dual threshold model of and curiosity boost the boons or the banes? Organizational Behavior and Human
anger in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 32(3), 721–746. http:// Decision Processes, 103(2), 256–267.
dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2007.25275495. Maynes, T. D., & Podsakoff, P. M. (2014). Speaking more broadly: An examination of
George, J. M. (2011). Dual tuning: A minimum condition for understanding affect in the nature, antecedents, and consequences of an expanded set of employee
organizations? Organizational Psychology Review, 1(2), 147–164. http://dx.doi. voice behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(1), 87–112. http://dx.doi.org/
org/10.1177/2041386610390257. 10.1037/a0034284.
Gino, F., Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2012). Anxiety, advice, and the ability to Menon, T., & Pfeffer, J. (2003). Valuing internal vs. external knowledge: Explaining
discern: Feeling anxious motivates individuals to seek and use advice. Journal of the preference for outsiders. Management Science, 49(4), 497–513.
Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 497–512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Menon, T., Thompson, L., & Choi, H.-S. (2006). Tainted knowledge vs. tempting
a0026413. knowledge: People avoid knowledge from internal rivals and seek knowledge
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr., (2003). A very brief measure of the from external rivals. Management Science, 52(8), 1129–1144. http://dx.doi.org/
Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528. 10.2307/20110589.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1. Milliken, F. J., Morrison, E. W., & Hewlin, P. F. (2003). An exploratory study of
Grant, A. M. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational employee silence: Issues that employees don’t communicate upward and why.
synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and productivity. Journal of Journal of Management Studies, 40(6), 1453–1476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
Applied Psychology, 93(1), 48–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93. 1467-6486.00387.
1.48. Morrison, E. W. (2011). Employee voice behavior: Integration and directions for
Grant, A. M. (2013). Rocking the boat but keeping it steady: The role of emotion future research. Academy of Management Annals, 5(1), 373–412. http://dx.doi.
regulation in employee voice. Academy of Management Journal, 56(6), org/10.1080/19416520.2011.574506.
1703–1723. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0035. Morrison, E. W. (2014). Employee voice and silence. Annual Review of Organizational
Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good actors: Prosocial and Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1(1), 173–197. http://dx.doi.org/
impression management motives as interactive predictors of affiliative 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091328.
citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 900–912. http://dx. Morrison, E. W., & Milliken, F. J. (2000). Organizational silence: A barrier to change
doi.org/10.1037/a0013770. and development in a pluralistic world. Academy of Management Review, 25(4),
Grant, A. M., & Wrzesniewski, A. (2010). I won’t let you down. . . or will I? Core self- 706–725.
evaluations, other-orientation, anticipated guilt and gratitude, and job Öhman, A. (2008). Fear and anxiety: Overlaps and dissociations. In M. Lewis, J. M.
performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 108–121. http://dx.doi.org/ Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.,
10.1037/a0017974. pp. 709–729). New York: Guilford Press.
Gump, B. B., & Kulik, J. A. (1997). Stress, affiliation, and emotional contagion. Journal Ostroff, C., Kinicki, A. J., & Clark, M. A. (2002). Substantive and operational issues of
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 305–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ response bias across levels of analysis: An example of climate-satisfaction
0022-3514.72.2.305. relationships. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(2), 355–368. http://dx.doi.org/
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process 10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.355.
analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation
Hirschman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: Responses to decline in firms, hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral
organizations, and states. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Research, 42(1), 185–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273170701341316.
Huang, G.-H., Niu, X., Lee, C., & Ashford, S. J. (2012). Differentiating cognitive and Probst, T. (2008). Job insecurity. In C. L. Cooper & J. Barling (Eds.), Handbook of
affective job insecurity: Antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Organizational organizational behavior (pp. 178–195). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Behavior, 33(6), 752–769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.1815. Rachman, S. J. (1990). Fear and courage (2nd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman and
Huang, G.-H., Zhao, H. H., Niu, X.-Y., Ashford, S. J., & Lee, C. (2013). Reducing job Company.
insecurity and increasing performance ratings: Does impression management Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal:
matter? Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(5), 852–862. http://dx.doi.org/ Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making.
10.1037/a0033151. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77.
Izard, C. E., & Ackerman, B. P. (2000). Motivational, organizational, and Rogelberg, S. G., & Stanton, J. M. (2007). Introduction: Understanding and dealing
regulatory functions of discrete emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland- with organizational survey nonresponse. Organizational Research Methods, 10
Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 253–265). New York: (2), 195–209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428106294693.
Guilford Press. Roseman, I. J. (2011). Emotional behaviors, emotivational goals, emotion
Jordan, P. J., Ashkanasy, N. M., & Hartel, C. E. J. (2002). Emotional intelligence as a strategies: Multiple levels of organization integrate variable and consistent
moderator of emotional and behavioral reactions to job insecurity. Academy of responses. Emotion Review, 3(4), 434–443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Management Review, 27(3), 361–372. 1754073911410744.
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Edmondson, A. C. (2009). Silenced by Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals
fear: The nature, sources, and consequences of fear at work. Research in differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67
Organizational Behavior, 29, 163–193. (2), 206–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.48.4.384.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Rosen, J. B., & Schulkin, J. (1998). From normal fear to pathological anxiety.
Guilford. Psychological Review, 105(2), 325–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
Lam, C. F., & Mayer, D. M. (2014). When do employees speak up for their customers? 295x.105.2.325.
A model of voice in a customer service context. Personnel Psychology, 67(3), Saunders, D. M., Sheppard, B. H., Knight, V., & Roth, J. (1992). Employee voice to
637–666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/peps.12050. supervisors. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 5(3), 241–259.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation.Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Press.
Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Schweiger, D. M., & DeNisi, A. S. (1991). Communication with employees following a
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(3), 371–375. merger: A longitudinal field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 34(1),
Lebel, R. D. (2016). Moving beyond fight and flight: A contingent model of how 110–135.
anger and fear spark proactivity. Academy of Management Review. http://dx.doi. Seibert, S. E., Kraimer, M. L., & Crant, J. M. (2001). What do proactive people do? A
org/10.5465/amr.2014.0368. longitudinal model linking proactive personality and career success. Personnel
LePine, J. A., & Van Dyne, L. (1998). Predicting voice behavior in work groups. Journal Psychology, 54(4), 845–874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.
of Applied Psychology, 83(6), 853–868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021- tb00234.x.
9010.83.6.853. Senge, P. M. (1990). The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the learning
LePine, J. A., & Van Dyne, L. (2001). Voice and cooperative behavior as contrasting organization. New York: Doubleday.
forms of contextual performance: Evidence of differential relationships with Big Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge:
Five personality characteristics and cognitive ability. Journal of Applied Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 86(2), 326–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.2.326. Psychology, 52(6), 1061–1086. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061.
R.D. Lebel / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 135 (2016) 10–21 21

Singer, J. D. (1998). Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical Van Dyne, L., Ang, S., & Botero, I. C. (2003). Conceptualizing employee silence and
models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral employee voice as multidimensional constructs. Journal of Management Studies,
Statistics, 23(4), 323–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1165280. 40(6), 1359–1392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6486.00384.
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. (1998). Helping and voice extra-role behaviors: Evidence
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. http://dx.doi.org/ of construct and predictive validity. Academy of Management Journal, 41(1),
10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813. 108–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256902.
Stickney, L. T., & Geddes, D. (2016). More than just ‘‘Blowing off Steam”: The roles of Venkataramani, V., & Tangirala, S. (2010). When and why do central employees
anger and advocacy in promoting positive outcomes at work. Negotiation and speak up? An examination of mediating and moderating variables. Journal of
Conflict Management Research, 9(2), 141–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Applied Psychology, 95(3), 582–591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018315.
ncmr.12071. Vuori, T. O., & Huy, Q. N. (2016). Distributed attention and shared emotions in the
Tangirala, S., & Ramanujam, R. (2012). Ask and you shall hear (but not always): innovation process: How Nokia lost the smartphone battle. Administrative
Examining the relationship between manager consultation and employee voice. Science Quarterly, 61(1), 9–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0001839215606951.
Personnel Psychology, 65(2), 251–282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744- Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
6570.2012.01248.x. measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273–277. 0022-3514.54.6.1063.

You might also like