8.01 Health Behavior PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 37

Copyright © 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

8.01
Health Behavior
MARK CONNER
University of Leeds, UK
and
PAUL NORMAN
University of Sheffield, UK

8.01.1 INTRODUCTION 2
8.01.2 KEY BEHAVIORS INFLUENCING HEALTH, MORBIDITY, AND MORTALITY 2
8.01.2.1 Introduction 2
8.01.2.2 Smoking 3
8.01.2.3 Diet 3
8.01.2.4 Exercise 3
8.01.2.5 Other Behaviors 4
8.01.3 UNDERSTANDING THE BASIS OF HEALTH BEHAVIOR 5
8.01.3.1 Introduction 5
8.01.3.2 Health Belief Model 6
8.01.3.2.1 Model description 6
8.01.3.2.2 Review of research 7
8.01.3.2.3 Commentary 8
8.01.3.3 Theory of Planned Behavior 8
8.01.3.3.1 Model description 8
8.01.3.3.2 Review of research 10
8.01.3.3.3 Commentary 11
8.01.3.4 Health Locus of Control 12
8.01.3.4.1 Model description 12
8.01.3.4.2 Review of research 12
8.01.3.4.3 Commentary 13
8.01.3.5 Protection Motivation Theory 14
8.01.3.5.1 Model description 14
8.01.3.5.2 Review of research 15
8.01.3.5.3 Commentary 17
8.01.3.6 Self-efficacy 17
8.01.3.6.1 Model description 17
8.01.3.6.2 Review of research 18
8.01.3.6.3 Commentary 18
8.01.4 ENCOURAGING THE ADOPTION OF HEALTH BEHAVIORS 19
8.01.4.1 Intervention Studies 19
8.01.5 LIMITATIONS AND EXTENSIONS OF EXISTING MODELS 21
8.01.5.1 Limitations of Current Models 21
8.01.5.2 Additional Theoretical Constructs 22
8.01.5.3 Processes by which Cognitions Influence Behavior 24
8.01.5.4 Stage Models of Health Behavior 24
8.01.5.5 Theoretical Integrations and Future Directions 27

1
2 Health Behavior

8.01.6 CONCLUSIONS 28
8.01.7 REFERENCES 28

8.01.1 INTRODUCTION Norman, 1996b; Marteau, 1989) and how


extrinsic factors produce behavior change
Interest in health behaviors is derived from (e.g., Rutter & Quine, 1996).
two assumptions; that a substantial proportion This chapter examines the key health beha-
of the mortality from the leading causes of death viors and ways in which their adoption might
is attributable to the behavior of individuals, be encouraged. The chapter then considers the
and that the behavior is modifiable (Stroebe & contribution psychology has made to under-
Stroebe, 1995). It is now widely recognized that standing and changing health behaviors
individuals can influence their own health and through the development of SCMs. The most
well-being through the adoption of health- widely applied SCMs are described and re-
enhancing behaviors (e.g., exercise) and the viewed along with recent developments and
avoidance of health-compromising behaviors future prospects for this field of research.
(e.g., smoking). The identification of the factors
which predict who engages in health behaviors
has become a focus of research in health 8.01.2 KEY BEHAVIORS INFLUENCING
psychology and other health-related disciplines HEALTH, MORBIDITY, AND
in recent years (e.g., Adler & Matthews, 1994; MORTALITY
Conner & Norman, 1996a; Glanz, Lewis & 8.01.2.1 Introduction
Rimmer, 1990; Hockbaum & Lorig, 1992;
Rodin & Salovey, 1989; Winett, 1985). We might define health behavior as any
The health behaviors examined have been activity taken for the purpose of preventing or
many and varied; from health enhancing detecting disease or for improving general well-
behaviors such as exercise and healthy eating, being (Conner & Norman, 1996b). The beha-
on the one hand, to avoidance of health harming viors within this definition include medical
behaviors such as smoking and excessive service usage (e.g., physician visits, vaccination,
alcohol consumption, on the other. Each of screening), compliance with medical regimens
these behaviors have immediate or long-term (e.g., dietary, diabetic, antihypertensive regi-
effects upon the individual's health and are to mens), and self-directed health behaviors (e.g.,
varying extents within the individual's control. diet, exercise, smoking, alcohol consumption).
Epidemiological studies reveal great variability Numerous studies have examined the rela-
in who performs these behaviors. The ap- tionship between health behaviors and health
proaches taken to understanding such indivi- outcomes (e.g., Black Report, 1988; Blane,
dual differences have been equally varied. One Smith, & Bartley, 1990; Blaxter, 1990; Cox,
can classify these approaches into those which Huppert, & Whichelow, 1993; Doll, Peto,
examine factors intrinsic to the individual (e.g., Wheatley, Gray, & Sutherland, 1994; Gottlieb
sociodemographic factors, personality, social & Green, 1984). Such studies demonstrate the
support, cognitions) and those which examine role of a variety of behaviors for both morbidity
factors extrinsic to the individual. This second and mortality. One of the first such studies in
group of approaches can be further divided into Alameda County identified seven features of
those based on incentive structures (e.g., taxing lifestyle: not smoking, moderate alcohol intake,
tobacco and alcohol, subsidizing sports facil- sleeping 7±8 hours per night, exercising reg-
ities) and those based on legal restrictions (e.g., ularly, maintaining a desirable body weight,
banning dangerous substances, financial penal- avoiding snacks, and eating breakfast regularly
ties for not wearing seat-belts). The first which together were associated with lower
approach (factors intrinsic to the individual) morbidity and higher subsequent long-term
has received most attention from psychologists, survival (Belloc, 1973; Belloc & Breslow, 1972;
with a particular focus on cognitive factors as Breslow & Enstrom, 1980). Such results have
the most important proximal determinants of been replicated in a variety of different popula-
behavior. A variety of models of how such tions (e.g., Brock, Haefner, & Noble, 1988;
cognitive factors produce various ªsocialº Metzner, Carman, & House, 1983). The impact
behaviors have been developed and are com- of health behaviors upon individuals' quality of
monly referred to as social cognition models life, via delaying the onset of chronic disease and
(SCMs). SCMs are recognized to have made a extending active life span, should also be noted
valuable contribution to the understanding of (Fries, Green, & Levine, 1989; Stroebe &
both who performs health behaviors (Conner & Stroebe, 1995). Smoking, alcohol consumption,
Key Behaviors Influencing Health, Morbidity, and Mortality 3

diet, gaps in primary care services and low are linked to excessive fat consumption and
screening uptake are all significant determi- insufficient fiber, fruit, and vegetable consump-
nants of poor health (Amler & Dull, 1987). Such tion. In addition, excess consumption of cal-
findings have led to a focus by those interested ories combined with insufficient exercise has
in health promotion on changing such beha- made obesity a major health problem. Diet has
viors in order to improve health. For example, been implicated in cardiovascular diseases
in the USA, Healthy People 2000 (USDHHS, (CVDs), strokes and high blood pressure,
1990) lists increased physical activity, changes in cancer, diabetes, obesity, osteoporosis, and
nutrition and reductions in tobacco, alcohol, dental disease.
and drug use as important for health promotion It is generally agreed that elevated blood
and disease prevention. cholesterol level is a major risk factor for the
Below we examine several health behaviors in development of CVD (Consensus Development
more detail, focusing on their prevalence and Conference on Lowering Blood Cholesterol to
relationship to health outcomes. Prevent Heart Disease, 1985). Nutbeam and
Catford (1990) estimate that 26% of men and
25% of women in the UK have cholesterol levels
8.01.2.2 Smoking greater than 6.5 mmol l71 (a level considered to
be excessive). While in the USA, it is estimated
Smoking is the behavior most closely linked
that 50% of the adult population is at risk of
with long-term negative health outcomes. Both
CHD by virtue of elevated blood cholesterol
morbidity and mortality from coronary heart
levels (Sampos, Fulwood, Haines et al., 1989).
disease (CHD) are increased among smokers
International studies have clearly demonstrated
(Doll et al., 1994; Friedman, Dales, & Ury,
an association between saturated fat consump-
1979). Moreover, there is a strong positive
tion (one source of cholesterol) and blood
relationship between the number of cigarettes
cholesterol levels. For example, Keys (1970)
smoked per day and the incidence of CHD
reported a correlation of 0.89 across the seven
(Friedman et al., 1979). In addition, smoking
countries studied. The reduction of blood
has been linked to a number of cancers in-
cholesterol via dietary change is now widely
cluding cancer of the lung, throat, stomach, and
accepted as an important way of tackling CHD
bowel as well as a number of more immediate
(Expert Panel, 1993). Drug treatment to reduce
negative health effects such as reduced lung
blood cholesterol levels is generally seen as only
capacity and bronchitis (Royal College of
advisable if dietary change is ineffective (War-
Physicians, 1983). Despite the array of negative
dle, 1995). Dietary recommendations include
health outcomes, smokers often report positive
reducing fat in the diet and increasing soluble
mood effects from smoking and the use of
fiber intake (Committee on Medical Aspects of
smoking as a coping strategy (Graham, 1987).
Food Policy, 1991; Expert Panel, 1993). How-
The number of people smoking in the USA
ever, their impact upon cholesterol levels may be
and UK has shown a steady decline over the
limited.
past 20 years. Data from the General House-
hold Survey (1992) showed that 28% of people
over the age of 16 smoke in the UK. Smoking is
more common among men and among un- 8.01.2.4 Exercise
skilled manual workers (General Household
Engaging in regular exercise is seen to be
Survey, 1994). A similar pattern is evident in the
another key component of a healthy lifestyle.
USA, with smoking more common among less
The potential health benefits of engaging in
educated, lower income, and minority groups
regular exercise are many and include reduced
(Rigotti, 1989). Those who quit smoking reduce
cardiovascular morbidity and mortality (Ober-
the risk to their health, particularly if they quit
man, 1985), lowered blood pressure (Blair,
before 35 years of age (Doll et al., 1994).
Goodyear, Gibbons, & Cooper, 1984) and the
increased metabolism of carbohydrates (Len-
8.01.2.3 Diet non et al., 1983) and fats (Rosenthal, Haskell,
Solomon, Widstrom, & Reavan, 1983), as well
The impact of various aspects of diet upon as a range of psychological benefits such as
health, morbidity, and mortality are well improved self-esteem (Sachs, 1984), positive
established (USDHHS, 1988). Whilst in the mood states (Folkins & Sime, 1981), reduced life
Third World the problems related to diet and stress (Brown, 1991), and reduced levels of
health are ones of undernutrition, in the First anxiety (Singer, 1992).
World, the problems are predominantly linked However, despite the various health benefits
to overconsumption of food. In Western of exercise, a significant proportion of the
industrialized countries the major problems population lead a sedentary lifestyle. For
4 Health Behavior

example, the General Household Survey (1989) used cannabis at some stage, while the Leitner
indicated that only one in three men and one in et al. (1993) study reported that 10% of the same
five women in the UK participate in any sport or age group had used it. In general, drug use in the
recreational physical activity. Moreover, the UK does not differ substantially from the rest of
Allied Dunbar Fitness Survey (1992) of 6000 the Western world. Reported lifetime usage in
English adults reported that one in six adults the USA is slightly higher than in the UK,
had done no exercise (i.e., for 20 minutes or although estimates for injecting use (1±2% of
more at a moderate or vigorous level) in the the general population), alcohol use (around
previous four weeks. Participation in regular 85%), and lifetime tobacco use (around 75%)
exercise is strongly related to a number of socio- are roughly similar.
demographic variables. In particular, young Sexual behaviors have also long been con-
people and males are more likely to engage in sidered health behaviors because of their impact
regular exercise. For example, the 1988 Welsh upon the spread of sexually transmitted diseases
Heart Health Survey (Health Promotion (STDs) such as gonorrhoea and syphilis. More
Authority for Wales, 1990) reported that among recently, the role of sexual behaviors in the
18±34 year olds, 61% of men engaged in spread of the human immunodeficiency virus
moderately vigorous exercise at least two times (HIV) has been a focus of attention (O'Leary &
a week compared with only 35% of women. For Raffaelli, 1996). Whilst early health education
35±64 year olds, the percentages drop to 37% campaigns emphasized the need to reduce the
for men and 17% for women. Overall, the number of sexual partners or avoid particular
typical exerciser is likely to be young, well- sexual practices (e.g., anal sex, penetrative sex),
educated, affluent, and male (King et al., 1992). more recently the focus has been upon the use of
condoms during penetrative sex to reduce the
risk of HIV transmission (Reiss & Leik, 1989).
8.01.2.5 Other Behaviors Condom use is particularly recommended for
those with multiple partners or those who do not
A number of other behaviors show clear links know their partners' sexual history. For these
to health. Below we briefly consider recreational reasons, much of the health advice concerning
drug use, safe sex, alcohol use, and health condom use has been focused on young people.
screening as behaviors with important health There seems to be considerable variation in
consequences. the use of condoms in response to the threat of
The use of recreational drugs has long been HIV/AIDS. For example, among heterosexuals,
recognized as a potential health problem which Richard and van der Pligt (1991) reported that
needs to be tackled (Aguirre-Molina & Gor- 50% of their sample of Dutch teenagers with
man, 1996). The most commonly used drugs are multiple partners consistently used condoms.
alcohol and tobacco, however, here we consider While other studies in the UK and USA report
what are usually referred to as psychoactive rates of between 24% and 58% (Fife-Schaw &
drugs (i.e., a drug that alters mood or behavior). Breakwell, 1992; Gerrard, Gibbons, & Bush-
There are a large number of such drugs man, 1996). Among homosexuals, Weather-
including hypnotic drugs such as barbiturates burn, Hunt, Davies, Coxon, and McManus
which reduce anxiety and produce sedation; (1991) reported that 39% of their sample always
stimulants such as amphetamines which elevate used a condom during anal sex. Whilst among
mood, increase wakefulness, and give an bisexuals, Boulton, Schram Evans, Fitzpatrick,
enhanced sense of mental and physical energy; and Hart (1991) report that 25% of their male
opiates such as opium and heroin which sample used condoms with their current male
produce pleasant mood states; antipsychotic partner and only 12% with their current female
agents such as chlorpromazine which diminish partner. The General Household Survey (1993)
the symptoms of psychoses; and psychedelics/ in the UK reported changes by age group in the
hallucinogens such as LSD, cannabis, and use of condoms for the period 1983±1991.
MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethampheta- Among 16±24-year-olds, condom use increase
mine) which cause visual and auditory halluci- from around 6% to around 12% during this
nations. Most of these drugs have medical uses period, whilst among 40±49-year-olds it
as well as being used recreationally. dropped from around 18% to around 12%
Data from various drug use surveys in the UK over the same period. Thus, health messages
(e.g., Leitner, Shapland, & Wiles, 1993) suggest aimed at increasing condom use may be having
that opiate use has remained stable (at around some impact among younger people, although
1%), but the use of cannabis and other the overall rates of use are still worrying low.
nonopiates has been on the increase over the High alcohol consumption has been linked to
last 25 years. The 1968 OPCS survey reported a range of negative health outcomes including
that 2% of the 16 years and over age group had high blood pressure (Shaper et al. 1981), heart
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 5

disease (Sherlock, 1982), and cirrhosis of the development of interventions to help indivi-
liver (Colliver & Malin, 1986), although there is duals gain the benefits of improved health and
some evidence to suggest that low levels of well-being. A variety of factors have been found
alcohol consumption may have slightly bene- to account for individual differences in the
ficial effects on health (Hennekens, 1983). High performance of various health behaviors,
levels of alcohol consumption have also been including demographic factors, social factors,
associated with accidents, injuries, suicides, emotional factors, perceived symptoms, factors
crime, domestic violence, rape, murder, and relating to access to medical care, personality
unsafe sex (British Medical Journal, 1982). factors, and cognitive factors (Adler & Mat-
While many of the adverse effects of high thews, 1994; Rosenstock, 1974; Taylor, 1991).
alcohol consumption are due to continued Demographic variables show reliable asso-
heavy drinking (e.g., cirrhosis of the liver, heart ciations with the performance of health beha-
disease), others are more specifically related to viors. For example, there is a curvilinear
excessive alcohol consumption in a single relationship between many health behaviors
drinking session (e.g., accidents, violence) and age, with high incidences of many health-
(Honkanen et al., 1983). risking behaviors such as smoking in young
The General Household Survey (1992) re- adults and much lower incidences in children
ported that the average weekly consumption of and older adults (Blaxter, 1990). Such behaviors
alcohol in the UK was 15.9 units (approxi- also vary by gender, with females being
mately 8 pints of beer) for men and 5.4 generally less likely to smoke, consume large
(approximately 2.5 pints of beer) for women. amounts of alcohol, engage in regular exercise
Of more interest was the finding that about 27% but more likely to monitor their diet, take
of men and 11% of women were drinking more vitamins, and engage in in dental care (Wal-
than the recommended weekly sensible limits dron, 1988). Differences by socioeconomic
(21 units for men, 14 units for women). Heavy status and ethnic group are also apparent for
drinking is also more likely among younger age behaviors such as diet, exercise, alcohol con-
groups. In a survey of 12 000 Welsh adults, sumption, and smoking (e.g., Blaxter, 1990).
Moore, Smith, and Catford (1994) reported that Generally speaking, younger, wealthier, better
31.1% of drinkers aged 18±24 engaged in binge educated individuals, under low levels of stress,
drinking (i.e., drinking half the recommended with high levels of social support, are more
weekly consumption of alcohol in a single likely to practice health enhancing behaviors.
session) at least once a week. Higher levels of stress and/or fewer resources
Finally, individuals may seek to protect their are associated with health-compromising beha-
health by participating in various screening viors such as smoking and alcohol abuse (Adler
programs which attempt to detect disease at an & Matthews, 1994).
early, or asymptomatic, stage. In the UK, Social factors seem to be important in
screening programs have been set up for various instilling health behaviors in childhood. Parent,
diseases including anemia (Ashworth, 1963), sibling, and peer influences are important, for
diabetes (Redhead, 1960), bronchitis (Gregg, example, in the initiation of smoking (e.g.,
1966), cervical cancer (Freeling, 1965), and McNeil et al., 1988). Cultural values also have a
breast cancer (Forrest, 1986). Considering major impact, for instance in determining the
breast cancer, it has been estimated that breast number of women exercising in a particular
screening programs which include mammo- culture (e.g., Steptoe & Wardle, 1996). For
grams can reduce breast cancer mortality by up example, Steptoe and Wardle (1992) report that
to 40% among women aged 50 and over (Strax, between 34% and 95% of women in their
1984). However, participation rates in breast European student sample had exercised in the
screening programs show great variability past 14 days. Perceived symptoms control
across different countries, ranging from 25% health habits when, for example, smokers
to 89% (Vernon, Laville, & Jackson, 1990). regulate their smoking on the basis of sensations
Participation tends to be negatively related to in the throat. Access to medical care has been
age and positively related to education level and found to influence the use of such health services
socioeconomic status (Vernon et al., 1990). (e.g., Black Report, 1988). Personality factors
have also been associated with health behaviors
(Adler & Matthews, 1994; Steptoe et al., 1994).
8.01.3 UNDERSTANDING THE BASIS OF Cognitive factors also determine whether or
HEALTH BEHAVIOR not an individual practices health behaviors.
8.01.3.1 Introduction Knowledge about behavior±health links is an
important factor in an informed choice con-
A clearer understanding of why individuals cerning a healthy lifestyle. Various other
perform health behaviors might assist in the cognitive variables have been studied including
6 Health Behavior

perceptions of health risk, efficacy of behaviors that they do not necessarily provide an adequate
in influencing this risk, social pressures to description of the way in which individuals
perform the behavior, and control over perfor- make decisions (e.g., Edwards, 1992; Feather,
mance of the behavior. The relative importance 1982; Frisch & Clemen, 1994; Jonas, 1993).
of various cognitive factors in determining who In the section which follows, the most widely
performs various health behaviors constitutes used of these models (HBM, TPB, HLOC,
the basis of different models. Such models have PMT, SE) are outlined, the research using them
been labeled SCMs because of their focus on described, and their use reviewed.
cognitive variables as the primary determinant
of individual social behaviors. 8.01.3.2 Health Belief Model
Two types of SCMs have been applied in
8.01.3.2.1 Model description
health psychology, predominantly to explain
health-related behaviors and response to treat- The HBM is probably the most widely used
ment (Conner, 1993). The first type focus on social cognition model in health psychology
individuals' understanding of the causes of (Becker, 1974; Rosenstock, 1966; Sheeran &
health-related events and are best typified by Abraham, 1996). It was originally developed by
attribution models (e.g., King, 1982). The US public health researchers attempting to
second type are more diverse in nature and develop models upon which to base health
attempt to predict future health-related beha- education programs (Hochbaum, 1958; Rosen-
viors and outcomes. These include the health stock, 1966). The model attempts to conceptua-
belief model (HBM; e.g., Becker, 1974; Janz & lize the health beliefs which make a behavior
Becker, 1984; Sheeran & Abraham, 1996), more or less attractive. In particular, the key
health locus of control (HLOC; Norman & health beliefs were seen to be the likelihood of
Bennett, 1996; Seeman & Seeman, 1983; Wall- experiencing a health problem, the severity of
ston, Wallston, & De Vellis, 1978), protection the consequences of the health problem, and the
motivation theory (PMT; e.g., Boer & Seydel, perceived costs and benefits of the health
1996; Rogers, 1983; van der Velde & van der behavior. Thus, the HBM employs two aspects
Pligt, 1991), theory of reasoned action/theory of of individuals' representations of health beha-
planned behavior (TRA/TPB; e.g., Ajzen, 1988; vior in response to threat of illness: perceptions
1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Conner & Sparks, of the threat of illness and evaluation of the
1996), and self-efficacy (SE; e.g., Bandura, 1982, effectiveness of behaviors to counteract this
1991; Schwarzer, 1992; Schwarzer & Fuchs, threat (see Figure 1). Threat perceptions depend
1996). Other models include self-regulation upon two beliefs: the perceived susceptibility to
theory (Leventhal, Nerenz, & Steele, 1984), the illness and the perceived severity of the
the transtheoretical model of change (Prochas- consequences of the illness. Together these two
ka & DiClemente, 1984), the precaution-adop- variables determine the likelihood of the
tion process (Weinstein, 1988), and the model of individual following a health-related action,
goal achievement (Bagozzi, 1992). However, although their effect is modified by individual
none of these latter models have been widely differences in demographic variables, social
applied to the prediction of health behaviors at pressure, and personality. The particular action
present. taken is determined by evaluation of the
These social cognition models provide a basis possible alternatives. This behavioral evalua-
for understanding the determinants of behavior tion depends upon beliefs concerning the
and behavior change. Each of these models benefits or efficacy of the health behavior and
emphasize the rationality of human behavior, the perceived costs or barriers to performing the
although they do not assume that all behavior is behavior. Hence, individuals are likely to follow
based upon careful thought (Ajzen, 1996). Most a particular health action if they believe
assume that behavior and decisions are based themselves to be susceptible to a particular
upon elaborate, but subjective, cost/benefit condition or illness which they consider to be
analysis of the likely outcomes of differing serious, and believe the benefits of the action
courses of action. As such they have roots going taken to counteract the condition or illness
back to expectancy-value theory (Peak, 1955) outweigh the costs. Cues to action and health
and subjective expected utility theory (SEU; motivation are two other variables commonly
Edwards, 1954). It is assumed that individuals included in the model. Cues to action include a
generally aim to maximize utility and so prefer diverse range of triggers to the individual taking
behaviors which are associated with the highest action and are commonly divided into factors
expected utility (Van der Pligt & de Vries, 1998). which are internal (e.g., physical symptom) or
Whilst such considerations may well provide external (e.g., mass media campaign, advice
good predictions of which behaviors are from others such as physicians) to the individual
selected, it has been noted by several authors (Janz & Becker, 1984).
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 7

EXTERNAL
VARIABLES Threat (motivation)
Demographic
variables Perceived susceptibility
Age, sex,
occupation,
socioeconomic status, Perceived severity
religion, education

Personality traits

Extraversion
Health motivation Behavior
Agreeableness

Conscientiousness

Neuroticism Response effectiveness


Openness
Perceived benefits
Other psychological
factors Cues to action

Peer pressure Perceived barriers


Self-efficacy

Figure 1 Health belief model.

Becker (1974) has argued that the HBM Sheeran and Abraham (1996) distinguish three
should also contain a measure of health broad areas of research. First, the HBM has
motivation (readiness to be concerned about been applied to various preventive health
health matters) because certain individuals may behaviors. These include health-risk behaviors
be predisposed to respond to cues to action such as smoking (Gianetti, Reynolds, & Rihen,
because of the value they place on their health. 1985; Stacey & Lloyd, 1990) and alcohol use (K.
Other influences upon the performance of H. Beck, 1981; Gottlieb & Baker, 1986), as well
health behaviors, such as demographic factors as health-promoting behaviors such as diet
or psychological characteristics (e.g., person- (Aho, 1979), exercise (Langlie, 1977), genetic
ality, peer pressure, perceived control over (Becker, Kaback, Rosenstock, & Ruth, 1975)
behavior), are assumed to exert their effect and health screening (Conner & Norman, 1994;
via changes in the six components of the HBM. King, 1982), vaccination (Oliver & Berger,
This is a potentially important issue if the HBM 1979), breast self-examination (Champion,
is to claim to be a complete model of health 1984; Ronis & Harel, 1989), contraceptive use
behavior. However, this has not been widely (Hester & Macrina, 1985), and dental behaviors
addressed in empirical studies and where it has (Chen & Land, 1986). A second area the HBM
the evidence has been equivocal. Orbell, has been applied to is various sick role behaviors
Crombie, and Johnson (1995), for example, which refer to compliance with professionally
reported that HBM components did mediate recommended medical regimens in response to
the effects of social class upon uptake of cervical illness. These include compliance with antihy-
screening, but did not mediate the effects of pertensive regimens (Taylor, 1979), diabetic
marital status or sexual experience. regimens (Harris & Lynn, 1985), and renal
disease regimens (Hartman & Becker, 1978),
and regimens adhered to by parents for a child's
8.01.3.2.2 Review of research
condition (Becker, Radius, & Eveland, 1978).
The HBM has been applied to a very broad Third, the HBM has been applied to clinic use,
range of health behaviors and populations. which includes physician visits for a variety of
8 Health Behavior

reasons including preventative (Aiken, West, mance of health behavior partly account for the
Woodward, Reno, & Reynolds, 1994), psychia- model's popularity.
tric (Connelly, 1984), and parent and child However, compared to other similar social
conditions (Kirscht, Becker, & Eveland, 1976). cognitive models of health behaviors, the HBM
There is no strong evidence that the HBM has suffers from a number of weaknesses. The way
been more predictive of behavior in any one of in which the variables in the HBM combine to
these behaviors compared with any other produce behavior has not been precisely
(Sheeran & Abraham, 1996). specified (but see Becker & Rosenstock, 1987)
There have been two quantitative reviews of and so the HBM is thus frequently tested as six
research with the HBM (Harrison, Mullen, & independent predictors of behavior. In addition,
Green, 1992; Janz & Becker, 1984). The first, various researchers have used somewhat differ-
conducted by Janz and Becker (1984), examined ent operationalizations of the six constructs (see
the proportion of times each of the HBM's Becker & Maiman, 1983; Rosenstock, 1974).
components showed a significant relationship Together these factors have weakened the status
with health behavior through the use of a of the HBM as a coherent SCM of health
ªsignificance ratio.º Across the 46 studies behavior (Conner, 1993; Sheeran & Abraham,
reviewed, the barriers component was found 1996). Moreover, key social cognitive variables,
to have the most consistent relationship with found to be highly predictive of behavior in
health behavior (89%), followed by the suscept- other models, are not incorporated in the HBM.
ibility (81%), benefits (78%), and severity For example, intentions to perform a behavior
(65%) components. However, while Janz and and social pressure are key components of the
Becker (1984) suggest that the HBM compo- TRA/TPB which do not appear in the HBM.
nents are consistent predictors of health Also, perceptions of personal control over the
behavior, they fail to estimate the strength of performance of the behavior (self-efficacy
the relationships. This question was addressed beliefs) which have been found to be such
in the second quantitative review, conducted by powerful predictors of behavior in models based
Harrison et al. (1992). Over 200 published upon social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1982;
studies on the HBM were identified although Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996) are not explicitly
only 16 of these were found to measure each of included in the HBM. In addition, in not
the components adequately. Harrison et al's specifying a causal ordering among the vari-
(1992) meta-analysis on these 16 studies pro- ables, as is done in other models, more powerful
duced a similar pattern of results to Janz and analysis of data and clearer indications of how
Becker's (1984) earlier review with the barriers interventions may have their effects are pre-
components having the highest average correla- cluded in the HBM. Several authors have noted,
tion with health behavior (r = 70.21), followed for example, that threat is perhaps best seen as a
by the susceptibility (r = 0.15), benefits (r = more distal predictor of behavior acting via
0.13), and severity (r = 0.08) components. The influences upon outcome expectancies. Finally,
predictive power of individual components is the model is static; there is no distinction
therefore relatively modest, accounting for only between a motivational stage dominated by
0.5±4% of variance in behavior. However, it cognitive variables and a volitional phase where
should be noted that it is the combined effects of action is planned, performed, and maintained
the six health beliefs which is generally of (Schwarzer, 1992). Such distinctions are
interest and this is commonly in excess of the thought to be important in understanding
sum of the effects of the individual components. various health behaviors. Hence, while an
extremely popular SCM for use in under-
standing health behavior, it is also in a number
8.01.3.2.3 Commentary
of ways limited and may receive relatively less
The HBM has provided a useful framework attention in the future.
for investigating health behaviors and identify-
ing key health beliefs, has been widely used, and
has met with moderate success in predicting a 8.01.3.3 Theory of Planned Behavior
range of health behaviors (for reviews see
8.01.3.3.1 Model description
Harrison et al., 1992; Janz & Becker, 1984;
Sheeran & Abraham, 1996). The strength of the The TPB was developed by social psycholo-
HBM lies in the fact that it was developed by gists and has been widely applied to the
researchers working directly with health beha- understanding of a variety of behaviors includ-
viors and so many of the concepts possess face- ing health behaviors (Ajzen, 1988, 1991; Conner
validity to those working in this area. This & Sparks, 1996) (see Figure 2). The TPB details
commonsense operationalization of a number how the influences upon an individual deter-
of cognitive variables relevant to the perfor- mine that individual's decision to follow a
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 9

particular behavior. This theory is an extension perceived behavioral control components are
of the widely applied TRA (Ajzen & Fishbein, also held to have determinants. The attitude
1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). The TPB component is a function of a person's salient
suggests that the proximal determinants of behavioral beliefs, which represent perceived
behavior are intentions to engage in that likely consequences of the behavior. Following
behavior and perceived behavioral control over expectancy-value conceptualizations (Peak,
that behavior. Intentions represent a person's 1955), the model quantifies consequences as
motivation in the sense of his or her conscious being composed of the multiplicative combina-
plan or decision to exert effort to perform the tion of the judged likelihood that performance
behavior. Perceived behavioral control is a of the behavior will lead to a particular outcome
person's expectancy that performance of the and the evaluation of that outcome. These
behavior is within his/her control. The concept expectancy-value products are then summed
is similar to Bandura's (1982) concept of self- over the salient consequences. It is not claimed
efficacy (see Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996). Control that individuals perform such calculations each
is seen as a continuum with easily-executed time they are faced with a decision about
behaviors at one end and behavioral goals whether to perform a behavior or not, but rather
demanding resources, opportunities, and spe- the results of such considerations are main-
cialized skills at the other. tained in memory and retrieved and used when
Intentions are determined by three variables. necessary (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). However, it
The first is attitudes, which are the overall is also possible for the individual to retrieve the
evaluations of the behavior by the individual. relevant beliefs and evaluations when necessary.
The second is subjective norms, which consist of Subjective norm is a function of normative
a person's beliefs about whether significant beliefs, which represent perceptions of specific
others think he/she should engage in the salient others' preferences about whether one
behavior. The third is perceived behavioral should or should not engage in a behavior. In
control (PBC), which is the individual's percep- the model, this is quantified as the subjective
tion of the extent to which performance of the likelihood that specific salient groups or
behavior is within his/her control. In addition, individuals (referents) think the person should
to the extent that PBC reflects actual control, it or should not perform the behavior, multiplied
is predicted to directly influence behavior. by the person's motivation to comply with that
Just as intentions are held to have determi- referent's expectation. Motivation to comply is
nants, so the attitude, subjective norm, and the extent to which the person wishes to comply

EXTERNAL
VARIABLES
Demographic
variables

Age, sex,
occupation, Belief Evaluation Attitude
socioeconomic about x of towards
status, outcomes outcomes behavior
religion, education

Personality traits

Extraversion Normative Motivation Subjective Behavioral


x Behavior
beliefs to comply norm intention
Agreeableness

Conscientiousness
Perceived Perceived
Neuroticism Perceived
likelihood facilitating/
x behavioral
Openness of inhibiting
control
occurrence power

Figure 2 Theory of planned behavior.


10 Health Behavior

with the specific wishes of the referent on this Several studies examined condom use (Bol-
issue. These products are then summed across dero, Moore, & Rosenthal, 1992; Wilson,
salient referents. Judgments of perceived beha- Zenda, McMaster, & Lavelle, 1992). Nucifora,
vioral control are influenced by beliefs concern- Gallois, and Kashima (1993), for example,
ing access to the necessary resources and examined undergraduates' use of condoms
opportunities to perform the behavior success- using the TPB. PBC was found to make a small
fully, weighted by the perceived power of each but significant contribution to the predictions of
factor (Ajzen, 1988, 1991). The perception of intentions to use condoms and actual condom
factors likely to facilitate or inhibit the use. However, intentions appeared to be
performance of the behavior are referred to as principally determined by attitudes and sub-
control beliefs. These factors include both jective norms, while behavior was mainly
internal control factors (information, personal influenced by intentions. Exercise has also been
deficiencies, skills, abilities, emotions) and examined in several studies (Dzewaltowski,
external control factors (opportunities, depen- Noble, & Shaw, 1990; Godin & Shepherd,
dence on others, barriers). People who perceive 1987; Norman & Smith, 1995). Dzewaltowski
they have access to the necessary resources and et al. (1990) reported the application of the TPB
perceive that there are the opportunities (or lack to exercise participation. Intentions were based
of obstacles) to perform the behavior are likely both upon attitudes and PBC, but not subjective
to perceive a high degree of behavioral control norms, whilst actual behavior seemed to be
(Ajzen, 1991). Ajzen (1991) has suggested that principally determined by intentions. Breast or
each control factor is weighted by its perceived testicle self-examination has been the focus of a
power to facilitate or inhibit performance of the couple of studies (McCaul, Sandgren, O'Neill,
behavior. The model quantifies these beliefs by & Hinsz, 1993; Young, Lierman, Powell-Cope,
multiplying the frequency or likelihood of & Kasprzyk, 1991). McCaul et al. (1993)
occurrence of the factor by the subjective showed the TRA components to predict
perception of the power of the factor to facilitate breast/testicle self-examination intentions and
or inhibit the performance of the behavior. behaviors, with PBC adding significantly to
So, according to the TPB, individuals are predictions of intentions but not behavior.
likely to follow a particular health action if they A range of other behaviors have been exam-
believe that the behavior will lead to particular ined using the TPB, including health screening
outcomes which they value, if they believe that attendance (DeVellis, Blalock, & Sandler, 1990;
people whose views they value think they should Norman & Conner, 1993), food choices (Beale
carry out the behavior, and if they feel that they & Manstead, 1991; Sparks & Shepherd, 1992),
have the necessary resources and opportunities kidney donation (Borgida, Conner, & Manteu-
to perform the behavior. fel, 1992), drug compliance (Hounsa, Godin,
Alihonou, & Valois, 1993), patient education
(Kinket, Paans, & Verplanken, 1992), and weight
8.01.3.3.2 Review of research
control (Netemeyer, Burton, & Johnston, 1991;
The TPB has been applied to the prediction of Schifter & Ajzen, 1985).
a number of different behaviors including The published studies applying the TRA have
health-relevant behaviors with varying degrees been reviewed by Sheppard, Hartwick, and
of success (Ajzen, 1991; see Conner & Sparks, Warshaw (1988) and van den Putte (1993), with
1996 for a review of the application of the TPB Ajzen (1991) reviewing 16 studies using the
to health behaviors). For example, smoking has TPB. The findings are generally supportive of
been a focus of several studies (Babrow, Black, the TRA/TPB. Ajzen (1991) reports the multiple
& Tiffany, 1990; Godin, Valois, Lepage, & correlation between intentions and attitude,
Desharnais, 1992). Godin et al. (1992) looked at subjective norm and PBC to be 0.71 across the
the prediction of the frequency of smoking in the 16 studies he reviewed. Van den Putte (1993)
general public over a six-month period. The computes a value of r = 0.64, but notes the large
prediction of intentions was significantly im- variation in results between behaviors. Ajzen
proved by the addition of the PBC component, reports the mean correlation between inten-
and actual smoking behavior appeared to be tions, PBC and behavior to be 0.51, while van
primarily related to PBC. One study has den Putte computes a value of 0.46. Ajzen (1991)
examined drinking alcohol (Schlegel, D'Aver- and Madden, Ellen, and Ajzen (1992) report
nas, Zanna, & DeCourville, 1992) and found empirical evidence that PBC significantly im-
that PBC contributed to the predictions of proves predictions of both intentions and
intentions but not the frequency of getting behavior. Hence, in summary, the evidence is
drunk in nonproblem drinkers, while in pro- broadly supportive of the TPB in helping to
blem drinkers, the PBC also contributed to understand and predict health behaviors. The
predictions of frequency of getting drunk. relative importance of the different predictors is
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 11

largely an empirical matter. However, in terms minant validity for the two concepts may be
of predictors of intentions, it has been argued weak. Bagozzi (1992) notes that the causal path
that attitude may be more important than may begin with the formation of desires which
subjective norms for health behaviors per- then develop into intentions, which in turn
formed in private (e.g., breast self-examina- inform self-predictions. However, Conner and
tion), while subjective norm may be more Sparks (1996) note that while theoretically these
important than attitudes where the behavior concepts may be distinguishable, empirically
is performed in public (e.g., safety helmet use) there is little to distinguish the three concepts.
(Quine, Rutter, & Arnold, 1998). Clearly, more work is needed to further
disentangle these and other related constructs
that have appeared in the literature such as
8.01.3.3.3 Commentary
planning and commitment (Bagozzi, 1992,
The TPB has been widely tested and success- 1993), need to change (Paisley & Sparks,
fully applied to the understanding of a variety of 1998), and behavioral willingness (Gibbons,
behaviors (for reviews see Ajzen, 1991; Conner Gerrard, Ouelette, & Burzette, 1998).
& Sparks, 1996; Sheppard et al., 1988). The Self-efficacy is a powerful predictor of the
theory incorporates a number of important performance of a range of health behaviors
cognitive variables which appear to determine (Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996). Ajzen (1991) argues
health behaviors (intentions, expectancy values, that the PBC and self-efficacy constructs are
perceived behavioral control). Also the role of interchangeable. However, several authors (e.g.,
social pressure from others is incorporated in Terry & O'Leary, 1995) have suggested that self-
the model in the form of subjective norms. efficacy and PBC are not entirely synonymous.
However, perhaps because the model was Bandura (1986), for example, argues that
developed outside the health arena, the model control and self-efficacy are quite different
does not make an assessment of health threat as concepts. Although Bandura (1986) accepts
is included in models such as the HBM. Finally, that some external factors (e.g., task difficulty)
the theory states a clear causal ordering among will have an influence on self-efficacy, it may
variables in how they relate to behavior, still be argued that self-efficacy is more
allowing sophisticated analysis techniques to concerned with perceptions of control based
be applied to assessing the model. on internal control factors. In contrast, PBC is
Sheppard et al. (1988), in a review of the likely to reflect more external factors, and may
TRA, have argued for the need to consider both be more usefully described as ªperceptions of
behavioral intentions and self-predictions when control over the behaviorº (Armitage & Con-
predicting behavior. Warshaw and Davis (1985) ner, in press). For example, Terry and O'Leary
noted a number of different ways in which (1995) measure self-efficacy over exercising by
intentions had been measured, and distin- items such as ªFor me to exercise would be . . .
guished measures of behavioral intentions easy±difficultº and perceived control over the
(e.g., ªI intend to perform behavior xº) and behavior by ªHow much control do you have
from measures of self-predictions (e.g., ªHow over exercising? no control±complete control.º
likely is it that you will perform behavior x?º). De Vries, Dijkstra, and Kuhlman (1988) have
This distinction is important when considering advocated the use of measures of self-efficacy as
the prediction of health behavior because while, opposed to PBC in the prediction of intentions
for example, David might intend to quit and behavior. Further, Dzewaltowski et al.
smoking, he might also think that it is unlikely (1990), in a comparison of the TRA, TPB, and
that he will do so. Sheppard et al. went on to Bandura's (1986) Social Cognitive Theory,
argue that self-predictions should provide better found that self-efficacy rather than PBC had
predictions of behavior as they are likely to a direct impact on behavior. Terry and O'Leary
include a consideration of those factors which (1995) examined exercise behavior and found
may facilitate or inhibit performance of a that self-efficacy only predicted intentions,
behavior as well as a consideration of the likely while PBC had main and interactive effects
choice of other competing behaviors. Sheppard on exercise behavior. Crucially, a combined
et al.'s meta-analysis supports this view; measure of PBC and self-efficacy failed to
measures of self-predictions were found to have moderate the effect of intention on behavior,
stronger relationships with behavior than suggesting that the two constructs are not
behavioral intentions. However, Norman and synonymous. This issue warrants further em-
Smith (1995) found no difference in the extent to pirical study. It seems plausible, however, that
which the two measures correlated with exercise perceptions of control and self-efficacy are two
behavior. Furthermore, the measures of beha- separable constructs which not only have
vioral intentions and self-predictions are differential effects on intentions and behavior
strongly correlated, suggesting that the discri- but may act differently for different behaviors.
12 Health Behavior

The TPB is correctly regarded as a theory of three dimensions. These measure the extent to
the proximal determinants of behavior. Indeed, which individuals believe their health is a
Ajzen (1991) describes the model as open to function of their own actions (i.e., internal
further elaboration if further important prox- HLOC), the actions of powerful others such as
imal determinants are identified. A number of health professionals (i.e., powerful others
potential candidate variables for addition to the HLOC), and the influence of chance or fate
TRA/TPB have been suggested. In each case (i.e., chance HLOC). According to HLOC
both theoretical and empirical justifications are theory, individuals who have strong internal
necessary (Fishbein, 1993). Some of the most HLOC beliefs should be more likely to engage in
promising of these ªadditional variablesº are health-promoting behaviors. Conversely, those
considered in Section 8.01.5.2. who believe that their health is due to chance or
fate should be less likely to engage in health-
promoting behaviors. The prediction for power-
8.01.3.4 Health Locus of Control ful others HLOC is less clear cut. Strong
powerful others HLOC beliefs may reflect a
8.01.3.4.1 Model description
receptivity to health messages endorsed by
The HLOC construct is one of the most health professionals. Alternatively, strong
widely researched constructs in relation to the powerful others HLOC beliefs may indicate a
prediction of health behavior (K. A. Wallston, strong belief in the ability of health profes-
1992). Its origins can be traced back to Rotter's sionals to cure subsequent illnesses and may be
(1954) social learning theory which states that unrelated or negatively related to the perfor-
the likelihood of a behavior occurring in a given mance of health-promoting behaviors.
situation is a function of the individual's According to social learning theory, the above
expectancy that the behavior will lead to a relationships should only hold for individuals
particular reinforcement and the extent to which who place a high value on their health as
the reinforcement is valued. As well as being behavior is a function of both expectancy beliefs
applied on a specific level, Rotter argued that (e.g., HLOC) and the value attached to certain
social learning theory could be applied on a outcomes (e.g., health value). As K. A. Wallston
general level such that individuals may have (1991) argues, individuals are unlikely to engage
generalized expectancy beliefs which cut across in health-promoting behaviors if they place a
situations. It was at this generalized level that low value on their health, whatever their HLOC
Rotter introduced the distinction between beliefs. Thus, health value should act as a
internal and external locus of control orienta- moderator of the relationship between HLOC
tions, with ªinternalsº believing that events are a and health behavior.
consequence of their own actions and thereby
under personal control and ªexternalsº believ-
8.01.3.4.2 Review of research
ing that events are unrelated to their actions and
thereby beyond their personal control. The majority of the research using the HLOC
Early work examining the relationship be- construct has correlated HLOC beliefs with the
tween locus of control and health behavior with performance of health behavior, without paying
Rotter's (1966) Internal±External (I±E) scale attention to the potential moderating role of
produced mixed results. However, the I±E scale health value. This may be due to a lack of
was criticized for being too generalized to appreciation of the complexity of social learning
predict health behavior and for conceptualizing theory (Wallston, 1991) and/or an unchallenged
locus of control as a unidimensional construct. assumption that all people value their health
In particular, Levenson (1974) argued that (Lau, Hartman, & Ware, 1986).
internal locus of control beliefs are orthogonal Several studies have examined the relation-
to external locus of control beliefs, and that ship between HLOC beliefs and general indices
within external locus of control a distinction can of health behavior. Most of these have reported
be made between external control exerted by a positive correlation between internal HLOC
powerful others and the influence of chance or beliefs and the performance of health-promot-
fate. The development of the multidimensional ing behaviours (Duffy, 1988; Mechanic &
health locus of control (MHLC) Scale (K. A. Cleary, 1980; Seeman & Seeman, 1983; Waller
Wallston et al., 1978) addressed both these & Bates, 1992; Weiss & Larsen, 1990), although
criticisms, and has since become the most other studies have failed to find such a
popular locus of control measure in research relationship (Brown, Muhlenkamp, Fox, &
on health behavior (Wallston & Wallston, 1981, Osborn, 1983; Muhlenkamp, Brown, & Sands,
1982). 1985; Norman, 1995; Steptoe et al., 1994;
The MHLC scale measures generalized ex- Wurtele, Britcher, & Saslawsky, 1985). Some
pectancy beliefs with respect to health along studies have found a negative relationship
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 13

between chance HLOC beliefs and health self-examination among women have found a
behavior indices (Brown et al., 1983; Duffy, positive relationship with internal HLOC beliefs
1988; Muhlenkamp et al., 1985; Steptoe et al., (Redeker, 1989) and a negative relationship
1994). Finally, powerful others HLOC beliefs with powerful others HLOC beliefs (Hallal,
have rarely been found to predict the perfor- 1982; Nemeck, 1990). The negative relationship
mance of health-promoting behaviors (Brown with powerful others HLOC beliefs may reflect
et al., 1983; Duffy, 1988; Muhlenkamp et al., a belief that breast examination is the respon-
1985; Steptoe et al., 1994; Waller & Bates, 1992; sibility of health professionals. In support of
Weiss & Larsen, 1990). The above results are this view, Bundek, Marks, and Richardson
generally in line with predictions, although the (1993) found a positive relationship between
relationship between HLOC and health beha- gynecological screening including physician
vior is typically a weak one. breast examination and powerful others HLOC
A similar pattern of results is obtained for the beliefs, and a positive relationship between self
relationship between HLOC beliefs and specific breast examination and internal HLOC beliefs.
health behaviors. For example, studies have However, other studies have failed to find any
reported a link between internal HLOC beliefs relationship between HLOC beliefs and breast
and exercise (Carlson & Petti, 1989; O'Connell self-examination (Lau et al., 1986; Liao et al.,
& Price, 1982; Slenker, Price, & O'Connell, 1995; Seeman & Seeman, 1983).
1985), while other studies have found only a Those studies that have tested the moderating
weak link or no link (Burk & Kimiecik, 1994; role of health value have generally produced
Calnan, 1989; Liao, Hunter & Weinman, 1995; positive results. Considering the prediction of
Norman, 1990, 1995; Speake, Cowart, & indices of health behavior first, Weiss and
Stephens, 1991). In a large-scale representative Larsen (1990) found a significant correlation
sample, Calnan (1989) found significant nega- between internal HLOC beliefs and a health
tive correlations between the powerful others behavior index among individuals placing a
and chance HLOC dimensions and exercise. high value on their health, but a nonsignificant
In relation to alcohol consumption, a number correlation among individuals placing a low
of early studies used the locus of control value on their health. Similar results have been
construct to compare alcoholics with nonalco- reported by a number of researchers (Lau, 1982;
holics producing mixed results (Butts & Cho- Lau et al., 1986; Seeman & Seeman, 1983; K. A.
tlas, 1973; Costello & Manders, 1974). More Wallston & Wallston, 1980), although other
recent work with the HLOC construct has studies have failed to find evidence for the
produced similar mixed results with a number of moderating role of health value (Norman, 1995;
studies finding no relationship between HLOC Wurtele et al., 1985). Studies looking at the
beliefs and drinking behavior (Dean, 1991; Liao interaction between health value and the
et al., 1995; Norman, 1990, 1995) and Calnan powerful others and chance dimensions have
(1989) only finding weak negative correlations generally produced nonsignificant results (Lau
between powerful others and chance HLOC et al., 1986; Wurtele et al., 1985 ).
beliefs and alcohol consumption. A similar pattern of results emerges when the
A number of studies have applied the HLOC performance of specific health behaviors is
construct to the prediction of smoking cessa- considered. Evidence for an interaction between
tion. A few of these studies have found internal internal HLOC beliefs and health value has
HLOC beliefs to be related to smoking cessation been found for a range of behaviors including
(Horwitz, Hindi-Alexander, & Wagner, 1985; dietary behavior (Hayes & Ross, 1987), smok-
Rosen & Shipley, 1983). Other studies have ing cessation (Kaplan & Cowles, 1978), breast
failed to find a relationship between internal self-examination (Lau et al., 1986), and in-
HLOC beliefs and smoking cessation (Kaplan formation seeking (K. A. Wallston, Maiders, &
& Cowles, 1978; Segall & Wynd, 1990; Wojcik, Wallston, 1976). However, other studies have
1988). Smokers who believe that their health is failed to find evidence for such an interaction
under the control of powerful others might be when considering cancer-preventive behavior
expected to be more successful in giving up (McCusker & Morrow, 1979), exercise (Burk &
smoking after attending a formal smoking Kimiecik, 1994), attendance at health checks
cessation program. However, a couple of (Norman, 1991), and information seeking (De
studies suggest that strong powerful others Vito, Bogdanowicz, & Reznikoff, 1982).
HLOC beliefs are related to a greater likelihood
of relapse following attendance at a smoking
8.01.3.4.3 Commentary
cessation program (Segall & Wynd, 1990;
Wojcik, 1988). Reviews of research with the HLOC con-
Studies examining the relationship between struct have concluded that HLOC is a weak
HLOC beliefs and the performance of breast predictor of health behavior, even when
14 Health Behavior

considered in conjunction with health value diet, etc.). As a result, the development of
(Norman & Bennett, 1996; K. A. Wallston, behavior-specific HLOC scales has been advo-
1991, 1992). There are a number of reasons for cated. Georgiou and Bradley's (1992) smoking-
this poor performance which are outlined specific locus of control scale is a good example
below. of such a scale. This scale was found to have
K. A. Wallston and Wallston (1981, 1982) stronger correlations with smokers' behaviors
have argued that certain combinations of and intentions than the more generalized
HLOC beliefs may be important in predicting MHLC scale. Other scales have been developed
health behavior. For example, when being in relation to exercise (Burk & Kimiecik, 1994),
advised to quit smoking by a health profes- AIDS risk behavior (Kelley et al., 1990), weight
sional, it may be advantageous to have a loss (Saltzer, 1982), alcohol use (Donovan &
combination of strong powerful others and O'Leary, 1978) and a range of specific condi-
internal HLOC beliefs. K. A. Wallston and tions including diabetes (Bradley et al., 1990),
Wallston (1981) have therefore proposed a 2 6 arthritis (Nicassio, Wallston, Callahan, Her-
2 6 2 typology based on median splits on the bert, & Pincus, 1985), cancer (Prwun et al.,
three HLOC dimensions (see also Waller and 1988), hypertension (Stanton, 1987) and heart
Bates, 1992). Using this typology, ªbelievers in and lung disease (Allison, 1987). Generally,
controlº (i.e., high internal high powerful these scales have been found to be more
others, low chance HLOC beliefs) have been predictive of health behavior than more general-
found to show better adjustment in relation to ized measures (Lefcourt, 1991).
rheumatoid arthritis (Roskam, 1986) and In conclusion, the amount of variance in
diabetes (Bradley, Lewis, Jennings, & Ward, health behavior explained by the HLOC
1990). To date, this approach has not been construct is low, even when considered in
applied to the prediction of health behavior. conjunction with health value (Norman &
However, despite the promise of this approach, Bennett, 1996; K. A. Wallston, 1991, 1992).
its utility may be limited given that large sample Nevertheless, the pattern of results obtained are
sizes are required in order to compare all eight generally in line with predictions suggesting that
types. In addition, this approach may lead to HLOC beliefs may have a distal influence on
results which are difficult to interpret, given the health behavior. In line with this position, K. A.
large number of comparisons. Wallston (1992) has proposed a ªmodified
A number of researchers have questioned the social learning theoryº in which health behavior
way in which health value is measured. is a function of HLOC beliefs, health value, and
Typically, health value has been measured as self-efficacy. Importantly, internal HLOC be-
an absolute value using Lau et al.'s (1986) four- liefs are seen to be a necessary, but not sufficient,
item scale. However, when deciding whether or condition for performing a health behavior. To
not to perform a specific health behavior, date, there have been no formal tests of this
individuals are often faced with more appealing modified theory, although the attempt to embed
alternatives. As a result, values other than HLOC beliefs into a broader theoretical
health may be important in determining perspective is an encouraging development.
behavior. In such cases it may be more
appropriate to use relative measures of health
value. Kristiansen (1986) followed this ap- 8.01.3.5 Protection Motivation Theory
proach and found that a measure of the value
8.01.3.5.1 Model description
placed on health relative to value of an exciting
life was more predictive of health behavior Protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1983)
among young people than an absolute measure was originally developed as a framework for
of health value. However, Wurtele et al. (1985) understanding the effectiveness of health-re-
reported the opposite pattern of results in a lated persuasive communications, although
sample of female undergraduates. Clearly, more more recently it has also been used to predict
work is needed comparing different approaches health protective behavior. It has its origins in
to the measurement of health value. early work on the persuasive impact of fear
The need to consider behavior-specific con- appeals, which was concerned with the condi-
trol beliefs has been highlighted by a number of tions under which fear appeals may influence
researchers. In particular it is possible to argue attitudes and behavior. In an extension of the
that one reason for the relatively poor perfor- fear-drive model, Janis (1967) proposed that if a
mance of the HLOC construct is that it persuasive communication successfully arouses
measures generalized expectancy beliefs with fear, usually through emphasizing the severity
respect to health. In other words, while HLOC is of a threat and the likelihood of its occurrence,
specific to a given goal (i.e., health), it cuts individuals will be motivated to reduce this
across many situations (e.g., smoking, exercise, unpleasant emotional state. If the message also
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 15

contains recommendations for action, then one performing an adaptive response will serve to
way in which individuals can reduce this state of inhibit such a response.
arousal is to follow the communicator's advice. Protection motivation results from the two
If the message does not contain effective appraisal processes and is a positive function of
behavioral advice, then maladaptive coping beliefs about severity, vulnerability, response
reactions may follow such as denial or avoid- efficacy, and self-efficacy, and a negative
ance. Janis proposed that fear appeals may be function of beliefs about the rewards associated
most effective when a medium level of fear is with the maladaptive response and the response
evoked. Under such conditions the cognitive costs of the protective behavior. Moreover, for
responses that promote adaptive reactions (e.g., protection motivation to be elicited, it is
following behavioral advice) outweigh those necessary for the rewards associated with the
that promote maladaptive reactions (e.g., maladaptive response to be outweighed by
denial). However, later work has failed to perceptions of severity and vulnerability, and
confirm this hypothesis (Sutton, 1982). the response costs of the protective behavior to
Leventhal (1970) made a similar distinction be outweighed by perceptions of response
between adaptive and maladaptive reactions in efficacy and self-efficacy. Protection motiva-
the parallel response model which differentiates tion, which is usually measured by behavioral
between two independent control processes that intentions, is seen to arouse, direct, and sustain
are initiated by a fear appeal. The first, fear protective behavior.
control, focuses on attempts to reduce the
emotional threat (e.g., avoidance) while the
8.01.3.5.2 Review of research
second, danger control, focuses on attempts to
reduce the threatened danger (e.g., following Protection motivation theory provides a
behavioral advice). The parallel response model framework for understanding both the effects
is important in proposing that protection of fear appeals and the social cognitive variables
motivation results from danger control pro- underlying health behavior. As a result, tests of
cesses (i.e., cognitive responses) rather than PMT have taken two forms. In the first, the key
from fear control processes (i.e., emotional components of PMT are manipulated in
responses). persuasive communications and their effects
Roger's (1983) (PMT) outlines the cognitive on protection motivation tested (see Section
responses resulting from fear appeals in more 8.01.4.1). In the second, PMT is considered as a
detail (see Figure 3). It is argued that various general attitude±behavior model and its com-
environmental (e.g., fear appeals) and intraper- ponents used as predictors of health behavior.
sonal (e.g., personality variables) sources of This work is reviewed below.
information can initiate two appraisal pro- One area in which PMT has been used as a
cesses: threat appraisal and coping appraisal. general attitude±behavior model is in relation to
Threat appraisal, which is similar to Lazarus cancer-related preventive behavior. In one of
and Launier's (1978) primary appraisal, focuses the few longitudinal tests of PMT, Hodgkins
on the source of the threat and the factors that and Orbell (1998) examined the social cognitive
may increase or decrease the probability of the predictors of breast self-examination (BSE) in a
maladaptive response. Both the perceived sample of young women (17±40 year olds) over
severity of the threat and the individual's a one-month period. Each of the main compo-
perceived vulnerability to the threat are seen nents of PMT was measured (i.e., severity,
to inhibit maladaptive responses. However, vulnerability, response efficacy, self-efficacy) as
there may be a number of intrinsic (e.g., well as the response costs of performing BSE
pleasure) and extrinsic (e.g., social approval) (e.g., I would feel awkward examining my
rewards which may serve to increase the breasts). In a path analysis, only self-efficacy
likelihood of maladaptive responses. Coping was related to intentions to perform BSE. Time
appraisal, which is similar to Lazarus and one behavioral intention was in turn found to be
Launier's (1978) secondary appraisal, focuses the most important predictor of performance of
on one's ability to cope with the threat and the BSE at one month follow-up. Similar results
factors that may increase or decrease the have been reported by Seydel, Taal, and
probability of an adaptive response. Both the Wiegman (1990) who found response efficacy
belief that the recommended action will be and self-efficacy to be predictive of intentions to
effective in reducing the danger (i.e., response engage in, and concurrent performance of, a
efficacy) and the belief that one is capable of number of cancer-related preventive behaviors.
performing the recommended action (i.e., self- Boer and Seydel (1996) also found response
efficacy) are likely to increase the probability of efficacy and self-efficacy to be predictive of
an adaptive response, although various re- intentions to participate in mammography
sponse costs (e.g., financial cost) associated with screening.
16 Health Behavior

SOURCES OF Threat appraisal


INFORMATION
Perceived vulnerability
Environmental

Fear appeals Perceived severity

Observation
Intrinsic rewards
Intrapersonal

Personality Extrinsic rewards

Protection
Behavior
Prior experience Motivation

Coping appraisal

Response costs
Other external
variables

Response efficacy
Age, sex, occupation,
socioeconomic status,
religion, education
Self-efficacy

Figure 3 Protection motivation theory.

A second area of application of PMT has behavioral intentions through a measure of fear.
been in relation to AIDS risk-reducing beha- Similar results were found with the homosexual
viors such as condom use. Aspinwall, Kemeny, sample with response efficacy, self-efficacy, and
Taylor, Schneider, and Dudley (1991) examined severity having a positive effect on behavioral
the ability of the PMT components to predict intentions to engage in safe sex. However,
reductions in a number of AIDS risk-reducing contrary to expectations, a negative relationship
behaviors in a sample of gay men over a six- was found between vulnerability and behavioral
month period. The results showed that strong intentions.
levels of self-efficacy and a high level of In a sample of male and female adolescents,
perceived vulnerability at time one were pre- Abraham, Sheeran, Abrams, and Spears (1994)
dictive of a reduction in the number of sexual found that self-efficacy had a positive influence
partners over the six-month follow-up period. and response costs (i.e., concern about reputa-
In addition, self-efficacy also emerged as the tion) had a negative influence on behavioral
most important predictor of reductions in the intentions to use a condom. In addition,
number of anonymous sexual partners. vulnerability had a negative effect on behavioral
Van der Velde and van der Pligt (1991) used intentions to limit the number of sexual
PMT as a framework for assessing the coping partners. In a cross-sectional study of male
responses of heterosexual men and women and and female heterosexuals, Bengel, Beltz-Merk,
homosexual men with multiple partners. Con- and Farin (1996) found that self-efficacy was
sidering the heterosexual sample first, it was related to a greater use of condoms and fewer
found that vulnerability, response efficacy, and sexual partners. However, perceptions of vul-
self-efficacy all had a direct positive effect on nerability were related to greater use of
behavioral intentions to use condoms. In condoms, but also higher number of sexual
addition, severity had an indirect effect on partners.
Understanding the Basis of Health Behavior 17

Taken together, the above results suggest that protective behavior, a number of researchers
PMT is a useful framework for understanding have commented on its lack of specification in
HIV-related protective behavior. Self-efficacy terms of the nature of its components and the
emerges as the most important predictor of such relationships between them (Bengel et al., 1996).
behavior, with response efficacy and severity For example, Rogers (1983) states that the
also emerging as significant predictors in some response costs of the protective behavior need to
studies. A conflicting pattern of results has been be outweighed by perceptions of response
found with the vulnerability component. Simi- efficacy and self-efficacy for protection motiva-
lar conclusions have been reached by Farin tion to be elicited. However, no guidance is
(1994) in a meta-analysis of PMT and HIV- given as to how these variables are to be
protective behavior, in which self-efficacy and measured and combined in order to predict
response efficacy emerged as the best predictors protection motivation.
of protective behavior. However, these two Overall, PMT identifies many of the social
components were only able to explain 2.2% and cognitive variables which are important pre-
1.8% of the variance in such behavior. Severity dictors of health behavior. It shares a number of
was seen to be less important, and vulnerability similarities with the HBM (i.e., measures of
had a conflicting pattern of results. perceived susceptibility, severity, benefits, and
barriers) although it also includes self-efficacy,
which has been found to be one of the most
8.01.3.5.3 Commentary
powerful explanatory constructs in relation to
Despite relatively few studies in the area, the health behavior (Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996), and
above review highlights the potential utility of a measure of behavioral intention, which is seen
PMT as a framework for considering the social to mediate the influence of threat appraisal and
cognitive predictors of health protective beha- coping appraisal. It is also important for
vior. However, there are a number of issues providing a synthesis between social cognitive
which future work needs to address. approaches and coping models as outlined by
First, some studies have reported a positive Lazarus and Launier (1978). However, to date
relationship between perceptions of vulnerabil- there have been relatively few longitudinal tests
ity and protection motivation, while others have of PMT in relation to health behavior. Such
reported a negative relationship. Seydel et al. studies should help clarify some of the issues
(1990) suggest that the negative relationship raised above.
between vulnerability and intentions to engage
in cancer-related preventive behavior in their
study may be due to a ªdefensive avoidanceº 8.01.3.6 Self-efficacy
style of coping, in which perceptions of vulner-
8.01.3.6.1 Model description
ability to cancer may lead to feelings of anxiety
which may inhibit adaptive responses and Self-efficacy is one of the most powerful
promote avoidance. However, it is more likely predictors of health behavior (K. A. Wallston,
that the mixed pattern of results is due to 1992). It has its origins in Bandura's (1977)
measurement issues. As Weinstein and Nicolich social cognitive theory which states that
(1993) argue, a negative correlation may be behavior is a function of both incentives (i.e.,
expected between perceptions of vulnerability reinforcements) and expectancies. Three kinds
and concurrent protective behavior given that of expectancies can be identified, these being
one's current behavior may be used to make situation-outcome expectancies which refer to
vulnerability judgments. In contrast, a positive beliefs about how events are connected, out-
correlation may be expected between percep- come expectancies which refer to beliefs about
tions of vulnerability and future protective the consequences of performing a behavior, and
behavior to the extent that perceptions of self-efficacy expectancies which refer to beliefs
vulnerability motivates protective behavior. about one's ability to perform the behavior.
Considering behavioral intentions, individuals Thus in order to perform a health behavior,
may feel vulnerable and therefore intend to individuals must value their health (i.e., in-
engage in a protective behavior (i.e., positive centive), believe that their current lifestyle poses
correlation), or may feel vulnerable because they a threat to their health (i.e., situation-outcome
do not intend to engage in a protective behavior expectancy), believe that adopting the new
(i.e., negative correlation). Clearly, more con- behavior will reduce the threat to their health
sideration needs to be given to the measurement (i.e., outcome expectancy) and believe that they
of perceptions of vulnerability and the potential are capable of performing the behavior (i.e.,
use of conditional measures of risk. self-efficacy expectancy). While all these beliefs
Second, while PMT provides a framework for are seen to be important in the initiation and
considering the social cognitive predictors of maintenance of health behavior, self-efficacy
18 Health Behavior

expectancies are seen to be the most important. Wandersman, 1991; Kok, de Vries, Mudde, &
Individuals with strong self-efficacy beliefs are Strecher, 1991; Morrison, Gillmore, & Baker,
believed to develop stronger intentions to act, to 1995). While the majority of studies examining
expend more effort to achieve their goals, and to the relationship between self-efficacy and AIDS
persist longer in the face of barriers and risk-reducing behaviors have reported signifi-
impediments (Bandura, 1991). cant results, a few studies have failed to do so
Self-efficacy beliefs are therefore believed to (Boyd & Wandersman, 1991; Morrison et al.,
play a crucial role in the determination of health 1995). Considering exercise behavior, self-
behavior. According to Bandura (1977, 1982), efficacy has been found to be an important
such beliefs can be conceptualized and measured predictor of both intentions to engage in regular
in terms of three parameters; magnitude, exercise and actual exercise behavior (Deshar-
strength, and generality. The first parameter nais, Bouillon, & Godin, 1986; McAuley, 1993;
refers to the level of difficulty of the behavior. Sallis, Howell, Hofsteffer, & Barrington, 1992).
Individuals with low-level expectations feel Finally, self-efficacy has also been related to
capable of performing only very simple beha- dieting and weight control (Bernier & Avard,
viors, whereas individuals with high-level ex- 1986; Hofstetter, Sallis, & Howell, 1990; Jeffrey
pectations feel capable of performing even the et al., 1984). For example, Jeffrey et al.
most difficult of behaviors. In this way it is examined the relationship between self-efficacy
possible to assess individuals' expectations and weight loss following participation in a
about their level, or magnitude, of performance. behavioral treatment program over a two-year
The second parameter refers to individuals' period. Making a distinction between ªemo-
confidence that they could perform a specific tionalº self-efficacy (i.e., confidence in one's
behavior, while the third parameter refers to the ability to refrain from eating during various
generality of expectations across situations or emotional states) and ªsituationalº self-efficacy
domains. The measurement of self-efficacy (i.e., confidence in one's ability to refrain from
usually focuses on the strength of the self- eating in various situations), they found pre-
efficacy expectation (e.g., ªI am confident that I treatment measures of both types of self-efficacy
can refrain from smokingº), although it will to be predictive of weight loss at one and two
often incorporate the magnitude of expectation years. However, post-treatment measures of
(e.g., ªI am confident that I can refrain from self-efficacy were not so powerful with only
smoking, even if someone offers me a cigaretteº). ªsituationalº self-efficacy predictive of weight
loss at one year.
8.01.3.6.2 Review of research
8.01.3.6.3 Commentary
The self-efficacy construct has been success-
fully applied to the prediction of a range of The self-efficacy construct has been found to
health behaviors (see Bandura, 1991; O'Leary, be one of the most important predictors of
1985; Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996). One of the health behavior (K. A. Wallston, 1992). How-
main areas of application has been in relation to ever, there are a number of issues which future
smoking cessation (Condiotte & Lichtenstein, work needs to address. First, the relationship
1981; Colletti, Supnick, & Payne, 1985; Kava- between outcome and self-efficacy expectancies
nagh, Piere, Lo, & Shelley, 1993). These studies has been a source of some debate (Corcorcan,
have found self-efficacy to be a consistent 1991; Kirsch, 1986, Maddux, 1993). As Schwar-
predictor of smoking cessation. For example, zer (1992) argues, there may be a temporal and
Condiotte and Lichtenstein (1981) found that causal order among the two types of expectancy
post-treatment self-efficacy beliefs were predic- beliefs inasmuch as individuals are unlikely to
tive of both the probability of relapse and the consider their ability to perform a behavior
amount of time before relapse. In addition, a before first considering the efficacy of the
close correspondence was noted between self- behavior. Bandura (1991) argues that the effect
efficacy beliefs for a range of specific tempting of outcome expectancies on intentions and
situations (e.g., after a meal, when drinking behavior are partly governed by self-efficacy
coffee) and the actual situation in which the expectancies; even if outcome expectancy is
relapse occurred. The results therefore indicate high, performance of a behavior is unlikely if
that self-efficacy is important not only in self-efficacy is low. As a result, when self-
predicting the likely success of smokers who efficacy is partialed out, any relationship
are trying to quit, but also the situations in between outcome expectancy and behavior
which they are most likely to relapse. should disappear. However, some research
Self-efficacy has also been related to AIDS has indicated that outcome and self-efficacy
risk-reducing behaviors, such as condom use expectancies can be independent predictors of
(Basen-Engquist & Parcel, 1992; Boyd & intentions and behavior (Maddux, 1993).
Encouraging the Adoption of Health Behaviors 19

Second, the role of incentives, or outcome we consider the use of these models in the design
values, has tended to be overlooked in research of interventions to encourage new health
with the self-efficacy construct. This is despite behaviors. As Fishbein (1993) has argued, the
the fact that a large body of research in ultimate test of the utility of these models lies in
expectancy value theory has found outcome their ability to inform the design of effective
value to be an important predictor (Kirsch, interventions. To the extent that these models
1986; McCelland, 1985). Unfortunately, those outline the key social cognitive determinants of
studies which have considered the role of health behavior, interventions which target
outcome and self-efficacy expectancies in con- these variables should lead to associated
junction with incentives or outcome values have changes in behavior. However, to date there
produced mixed results (Maddux, Norton, & have been relatively few theoretically driven
Stoltenberg, 1986; Manning & Wright; 1983). interventions. As a result there is still a need for
Third, there is some disagreement over more tests of these models ªin actionº (Fish-
whether it is appropriate to consider generalized bein, 1993).
self-efficacy expectancies. According to Ban- Brawley (1993) argues that we need to take
dura's (1977) original conceptualization, self- account of the practicality of employing SCMs
efficacy beliefs should focus on specific beha- when designing interventions. In short, a model
viors in specific situations. Self-efficacy is not which offers a high level of practicality must be
seen to be a personality trait as self-efficacy shown to have predictive utility, to describe the
beliefs can be seen to vary across behaviors and relationships between key constructs, to offer
situations, although self-efficacy beliefs may guidelines for the assessment of these con-
generalize to other behaviors and situations to structs, to allow the translation of these
the extent that the new behaviors require similar constructs into operational manipulations,
skills and the new situations have similar and to provide the basis for detecting the
features (Bandura, 1986). Nevertheless, reasons why an intervention succeeds or fails.
Schwarzer and Fuchs (1996) have argued that The SCMs considered here can be been seen to
self-efficacy can be viewed as a generalized trait have a high level of practicality and therefore
reflecting a personal resource factor, pointing to should provide a good framework for the design
studies which have successfully employed gen- of effective interventions.
eralized measures of self-efficacy to predict One model which has been used widely to
behavior (Mittag & Schwarzer, 1993; Snyder design interventions is the PMT. A good
et al., 1991; K. A. Wallston, 1992). Generalized example of the use of PMT in this respect is
self-efficacy may be closely related to disposi- provided by Wurtele and Maddux (1987) in
tional optimism (Scheier & Carver, 1992), their study on exercise intentions and behavior.
although on an empirical level Schwarzer In this study essays recommending beginning a
(1994) found a correlation of only 0.60 between regular exercise program were presented to a
dispositional optimism and generalized self- sample of nonexercising female undergradu-
efficacy. ates. The essays were designed so that each of
In conclusion, self-efficacy has been found to the PMT's main components (i.e., severity,
be one of the most powerful and consistent vulnerability, response-efficacy, and self-effi-
predictors of health behavior. For this reason cacy) were independently manipulated, result-
alone, it is not surprising that it has been ing in a 2626262 between-subjects factorial
incorporated into a number of the main social design with two levels (present vs. absent) of
cognition models of health behavior; for each factor. For example, the severity message
example, Rogers' (1983) PMT and Ajzen's emphasized the seriousness of the threat of a
(1991) TPB. In addition, Rosenstock, Strecher, sedentary lifestyle by vividly describing the
and Becker (1988) have called for the inclusion negative effects of a heart attack (e.g., ªNau-
of self-efficacy in the HBM. It is likely that self- seated, the victim vomits; pink foam comes out
efficacy will continue to attract considerable of the mouth. The face turns ashen grey, sweat
interest and continue to be a key predictor of rolls down the face . . .º). After reading an essay,
health behavior. subjects completed a questionnaire containing
measures of the main components of PMT and
were followed up two weeks later to chart any
8.01.4 ENCOURAGING THE ADOPTION changes in exercise behavior.
OF HEALTH BEHAVIORS Manipulation checks revealed that each of
8.01.4.1 Intervention Studies the messages successfully manipulated their
corresponding component from PMT, as is
The models presented earlier in this chapter the case in most PMT intervention studies.
outline some of the key social cognitive Of more interest was the effect of the messages
determinants of health behavior. In this section on protection motivation as measured by
20 Health Behavior

behavioral intentions to start a regular exercise that high levels of arousal or anxiety may
program. The results showed that only the indicate to the individual that he or she is not
vulnerability and self-efficacy messages had a capable of performing a given action. As a
significant effect on behavioral intentions, result, relaxation techniques may be employed
although a three-way interaction between to help maintain feelings of self-efficacy.
vulnerability, response efficacy, and self-effi- Each of these techniques have been used in
cacy was also found. In relation to changes in intervention studies to try to enhance feelings of
exercise behavior over the two-week follow-up self-efficacy. Maibach, Flora, and Nass (1991)
period, a significant interaction was found report the results of a year-long community
between severity and self-efficacy, such that health campaign to encourage the adoption of
the self-efficacy message only had a significant health behaviors. The campaign materials were
effect when the severity message was absent. all designed to reflect the main principles of
Overall, studies which have used PMT to Bandura's (1986) social cognitive theory and
design interventions have shown that self- used a number of strategies for enhancing
efficacy is the most powerful component that feelings of self-efficacy and encouraging beha-
can be manipulated in persuasive messages vior change attempts. These included encoura-
(Boer & Seydel, 1996). In addition, manipulat- ging participants to set behavior change goals,
ing response efficacy has been found to have a using community members who had success-
significant effect on intentions in a majority of fully changed their behavior as role models,
studies. These two variables have been shown to using health experts to give advice about
influence intentions to engage in a range of behavior change and focusing on the skills
health behaviors including exercise (Stanley & needed to support behavior change. The
Maddux, 1986), quit smoking (Maddux & campaign was found to successfully increase
Rogers, 1983), dietary intake (Wurtele, 1988), feelings of self-efficacy which, in turn, were
and breast self-examination (Rippetoe & Ro- related to the adoption of new health behaviors.
gers, 1987). Manipulating perceptions of vul- A number of interventions have focused on
nerability has been found to influence intentions more situation-specific feelings of self-efficacy.
in other studies (Maddux & Rogers, 1983; Stevens and Hollis (1989) designed an interven-
Wurtele & Maddux, 1987), while manipulating tion to help smokers quit smoking which built
perceptions of severity has not been found to on the results of earlier research which had
have an influence on behavioral intentions. shown that situation-specific ratings of self-
However, many PMT intervention studies have efficacy were predictive of the circumstances in
combined the vulnerability and severity com- which relapses occurred (Condiotte & Lichten-
ponents so that the potential threat of a stein, 1981). Abstinent smokers have identified
maladaptive behavior is emphasized. This has potential relapse situations in which they
been found to have a significant effect on perceived low levels of self-efficacy and then
intentions in relation to alcohol use (Stainback developed and rehearsed appropriate coping
& Roger, 1983), dental flossing (K. H. Beck & strategies over three weekly meetings. This
Lund, 1981), dietary behavior (Wurtele, 1988), intervention led to a greater abstinence rate at
information seeking (Brouwers & Sorrentino, one year than both a discussion-only interven-
1993), and breast self-examination (Rippetoe & tion and a no-treatment control. Other studies
Rogers, 1987). which have attempted to improve behavioral
Some studies have focused more specifically skills to enhance feelings of self-efficacy have
on enhancing feelings of self-efficacy as a means produced positive results in relation to alcohol
for encouraging health behavior change. As use (Baer et al., 1992) and dental hygiene
Bandura (1986) outlines, there are four main (McCaul, Glasgow, & O'Neil, 1992).
sources of self-efficacy, each of which could be Few studies have attempted to use the TRA/
addressed in interventions. First, individuals TPB as a framework for developing interven-
can develop feelings of self-efficacy from tions, despite quite clear guidelines outlined by
personal mastery experience. For example, it Ajzen and Fishbein (1980). In fact, van den Putte
may be possible to split a behavior into various (1993) reports a mere five studies which have
subgoals, such that the easiest subgoals are followed such an approach, with only limited
achieved before more difficult tasks are at- evidence for success compared to approaches
tempted. Second, individuals may develop not based upon the model. For example,
feelings of self-efficacy through observing other Brubaker and Fowler (1990) examined the effect
people succeed on a task (i.e., vicarious of persuasive messages upon men's intentions to
experience). Third, it is possible to use standard perform testicular self-examination. A persua-
persuasive techniques to try to instil feelings of sive message based on the theory of reasoned
self-efficacy. Finally, one's physiological state action was found to increase intentions to
may be used as a source of information, such perform testicular self-examination compared
Limitations and Extensions of Existing Models 21

to a no-message control, but was no more practical applications of these models have met
effective than a knowledge-only message. More with some success and suggest that health
recently, Parker, Manstead, and Stradling behavior change interventions may have a lot to
(1996) have developed intervention videos gain from using these models as a guiding
based on the TPB to discourage speeding in framework. As Lewin (1951) concludes, ªthere
residential areas by car drivers. The results is nothing so practical as a good theoryº (p. 169).
showed that the normative belief video had a
significant effect on a postintervention measure
of normative beliefs, while the behavioral belief 8.01.5 LIMITATIONS AND EXTENSIONS
video had no effect on behavioral beliefs, and OF EXISTING MODELS
the perceived behavioral control video had a 8.01.5.1 Limitations of Current Models
negative effect on perceptions of control.
Overall, the videos had no effect on subjects' The SCMs described above represent one
expectations of speeding in residential areas widely used approach to understanding health
over the next year. behaviors. Here we provide a critique of this
Finally, there is some evidence that tailoring approach to understanding health behavior and
interventions to fit in with individuals' existing outline ways in which research might develop
belief orientations may lead to more effective through consideration of additional theoretical
interventions. Chambliss and Murray (1979a) constructs, the processes by which cognitions
devised a weight control program in which influence behavior, stage models of behavior
participants were given placebo medication to change, potential integrations, and future
help control their metabolism. After two weeks directions for work in this area.
participants in one group were debriefed about There are several advantages of using social
the placebo medication and encouraged to cognition models in health psychology (Conner,
attribute any weight loss to their own efforts 1993; Conner & Norman, 1996b). First, they
over the previous two weeks. Participants in a provide a clear theoretical background to
second group were given further information research, guiding the selection of variables to
about the efficacy of the medication and measure, the procedure for developing reliable
encouraged to attribute any weight loss to the and valid measures, and how these variables are
medication. At two-week follow-up, a signifi- combined in order to predict health behaviors
cant interaction was found between the giving of and outcomes. Second, to the extent that the
information and participants' preprogram locus models identify the important variables in
of control orientation, such that ªinternalsº lost predicting health outcomes and behaviors, they
more weight than ªexternalsº in the self-efficacy enable us to develop effective behavioral
information group, while the opposite pattern interventions. Third, the models provide us
of results was found for the drug information with a description of the cognitive processes
group. Similar results have been reported by determining individuals' motivation to perform
Chambliss and Murray (1979b) in relation to different behaviors.
smoking cessation. These results led to further There are parallel disadvantages in too
work exploring the match between the control exclusive a focus upon social cognition models
orientation of the intervention and participants' as the way to understand health behaviors.
existing health locus of control beliefs. For First, in providing such an explicit general
example, Quadrel and Lau (1989) found an theoretical framework, these models may lead
interaction between health locus of control us to neglect variables (cognitive and noncog-
beliefs and the control orientation of a message nitive) potentially important in understanding a
to encourage breast self-examination among particular health behavior or outcome. For
female students. In particular, those females example, the decision to use a condom may be a
with strong internal health locus of control function of cognitions, emotional reactions, and
beliefs who received a message in a ªcontrolº also a complex interaction between the indivi-
frame were more likely to perform breast self- duals involved. Social cognition models on their
examination at follow-up, although this effect own are unlikely to provide considerable
was reversed if a neutral reminder was sent. predictive power in these situations. In addition,
Further evidence for a ªmatching hypothesisº SCMs are open to extension when empirically
has been provided in relation to weight and theoretically justified (Fishbein, 1993).
reduction (B. S. Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan, Second, while such models provide us with
& Maides, 1976) and smoking cessation (Best, targets for interventions to produce behavior
1975). change, they do not specify how such cognitions
Despite offering a high level of practicality, are best changed. Effective interventions need to
there have been relatively few studies testing consider both the targets (e.g., cognitions) and
SCMs ªin action.º As the above review shows, the persuasion process itself. This process of
22 Health Behavior

persuasion is described by other models of social social comparison processes and how the social
cognitive processes (e.g., the elaboration like- image or prototype of the person who performs
lihood model: Petty & Cacioppo, 1986; the a particular behavior influences the perfor-
systematic-heuristic model: Chaiken, Lieber- mance of various health behaviors (particularly
man, & Eagly, 1989). In addition, applications among young people). Other researchers have
of SCMs should not lead to neglect of suggested other forms of normative influence we
alternatives to persuasion in producing beha- might consider. These include descriptive norms
vior change such as extrinsic changes to the and personal or moral norms. Descriptive
rewards and costs of a given behavior. For norms are perceptions of the behavior of salient
example, increased taxation and legal restric- others. For example, Jane's eating behavior may
tions can be effective in producing change in be influenced not only by her perceptions of
health behaviors either in isolation or in tandem what others think she should eat, but also
with persuasion. perceptions of what they actually do eat. Several
Third, although SCMs have furthered our studies have reported that perceptions of others'
understanding of motivational processes and behavior contributed to the prediction of
their influence upon behavior, they have intentions independently of perceived injunctive
neglected other aspects of behavior change. norms (e.g., Conner et al., 1996; De Vries,
For example, few of the models consider Backbier, Kok, & Dijkstra, 1995; Grube,
volitional processes beyond attempting to Morgan, & McGee, 1986). Personal, or moral,
explain intentions (Bagozzi, 1993; Gollwitzer, norms are the individual's perception of the
1990). However, many individuals who intend moral correctness or incorrectness of perform-
to change fail to do so. Hence, we need to ing a behavior (Ajzen, 1991; Sparks, 1994) and
consider the other important volitional pro- take account of, ª. . . personal feelings of . . . re-
cesses associated with attempts to change and sponsibility to perform, or refuse to perform, a
maintain behavior change (see Norman & certain behaviorº (Ajzen, 1991, p. 199). Moral
Conner, 1996a; Schwarzer & Fuchs, 1996). norms should have an important influence on
the performance of those behaviors with a moral
or ethical dimension (L. Beck & Ajzen, 1991;
8.01.5.2 Additional Theoretical Constructs Gorsuch & Ortberg, 1983; Kurland, 1995). A
number of studies have found measures of
While the social cognition models outlined moral norms to be predictive of blood donating
here provide an important framework for behavior (Pomazal & Jaccard, 1976; Zuckerman
considering the social psychological determi- & Reiss, 1978) as well as intentions to donate
nants of health behavior, it is clear that in some organs (Schwartz & Tessler, 1972), eat geneti-
instances they only account for a modest cally produced food (Sparks, Shepherd, &
amount of the variance in health behavior. Frewer, 1995), buy milk (Raats, 1992), use
For example, Sheppard et al. (1988) noted that condoms (Nucifora et al., 1993), and commit
about 10% of studies they reviewed reported driving violations (Parker, Manstead, & Strad-
correlations between behavioral intentions and ling, 1995).
behavior below 0.2. This suggests that key Anticipated affective reactions to the perfor-
variables have failed to be included in these mance or nonperformance of a behavior may
models. We review here the most promising of also be an important determinant of behavior
such variables appearing in the literature: (Triandis, 1977; Van der Pligt & de Vries, 1998),
measures of norms, anticipated affective reac- especially in situations where the consequences
tions, self-identity, and past behavior. of the behavior are unpleasant or negatively
A number of researchers have argued that affectively laden. In the 1990s research has
further attention needs to be paid to the concept focused on the influence of anticipated regret
of normative influences (e.g., Cialdini, Reno & (Parker et al., 1995; R. Richard & van der Pligt,
Kallgren, 1990; Conner, Martin, Silverdale, & 1991; Richard, Van der Pligt, & de Vries, 1995,
Grogan, 1996). Of the major SCMs, only the 1996a, 1996b). It is argued that if individuals
TPB incorporates perceived social pressures to anticipate feeling regret after performing a
perform a behavior as a predictor of intentions. behavior then they will be unlikely to perform
Based upon social identification theory, Terry the behavior. Richard et al. (1995, 1996a)
and Hogg (1996) suggest that such (injunctive) investigated the role of anticipated regret in
normative measures might be more predictive of relation to condom use among adolescents and
behavior if they employed a measure of group found such feelings to be an important predictor
identification rather than motivation to comply of intentions. Richard et al. (1996b) examined
(e.g., I identify with my friends with regard to the influence of anticipated regret on subse-
smoking). Similarly, Gibbons and Gerrard quent behavior. Participants in their study were
(1995, 1997) have noted the need to consider asked to either focus on their anticipated
Limitations and Extensions of Existing Models 23

feelings following safe and unsafe sexual reported in relation to drug use (Bentler &
behavior or on their present feelings about Speckart, 1979; Huba, Wingard, & Bentler,
these behaviors. At follow-up, participants in 1981), exercise (Godin, Valois, & Lepage, 1993;
the anticipated feelings condition were more Norman & Smith, 1995; Valois, Desharnais, &
likely to have used condoms in casual sexual Godin, 1988), breast self-examination (Hodg-
encounters in the intervening five months. The kins & Orbell, 1998), attendance at health
effects of anticipated affective reactions have checks (Norman & Conner, 1993, 1996b) and
been confirmed in studies of driving (Parker, seat belt use (Sutton & Hallett, 1989). Such
Manstead, Stradling, Reason, & Baxter, 1992) results have led to calls for past behavior to be
and consumer behavior (Simonson, 1992). considered as an independent predictor of
However, in terms of developing social cogni- future behavior (Bentler & Speckart, 1979;
tion models of health behavior, it is possible to Fredricks & Dossett, 1983). However, there are
argue that anticipated affective reactions may problems with this view. Ajzen (1988) argues
be incorporated into constructs that focus on that the effects of past behavior on future
the consequences of behavior (e.g., behavioral behavior should be mediated by the variables
beliefs in the TPB; see Van der Pligt & de Vries, included in social cognition models; past
1998). behavior shapes individuals' beliefs about the
The concept of self-identity has also been behavior in question, and it is these cognitions
suggested as a predictor of behavior (Biddle, that determine subsequent behavior. When past
Bank, & Slavings, 1987; Charng, Piliavin, & behavior is found to have a direct effect on
Callero, 1988). For example, the extent to which future behavior it is because key social cognitive
individuals think of themselves as ªhealthy variables have not been considered (Ajzen,
eatersº should predict their dietary intentions 1991).
and behavior. In support, Sparks and Shepherd There has also been focus on the concept of
(1992) found that respondents who thought of habit (i.e., behaving in a way you have acted
themselves as ªgreen consumersº had stronger before without thinking about it). Eagly and
intentions to consume organic vegetables. Chaiken (1993) review numerous studies where
Sparks (1994) noted that self-identity may the addition of habit has added to the prediction
simply be a proxy for past behavior, although of future behavior over and above the influence
Sparks and Shepherd (1992) found that the of variables such as intention, attitude, and
relationship between self-identity and future subjective norm. Despite this evidence, few
intentions remained when past consumption of SCMs incorporate a measure of habit. A
organic vegetables was controlled for. Self- notable exception is Triandis (1977) who argues
identity as someone who is concerned about the that it is possible to make a distinction between
health consequences of one's diet has also been habitual and intentional behaviors. He argues
related to intentions to reduce fat consumption that novel behaviors will be primarily deter-
(Sparks, Shepherd, Wieringa, & Zimmermanns, mined by intention, while repeated behaviors
1995), although in an earlier study Sparks, will be primarily determined by habit. Ronis,
Shepherd, Wieringa, and Zimmermanns (1994) Yates, and Kirscht (1989) make a similar
failed to find an independent effect for self- distinction between habits and decisions, ar-
identity. Role identity (regarded as synonymous guing that the performance of repeated beha-
with self-identity) was measured by Theodor- vior is determined by habit rather than social
akis (1994) and found to be a significant cognitive variables. For example, Dishman
predictor of exercise behavior. Future work (1982) distinguished between the initiation
needs to assess the influence of self-identity and maintenance of behavior in relation to
across of range of behaviors as it may be the case clinical exercise programs and found that only
that self-identity is only important in a restricted the initiation of exercise behavior was predicted
range of situations. by social cognitive variables. Sutton (1994) has
The influence of past on current behavior in proposed a further distinction between habits
SCMs has attracted much attention. It is argued and routines. He argues that many health
that many health behaviors are determined by behaviors commonly considered habitual may
one's previous behavior rather than cognitions. be more appropriately considered routines.
The argument is based on the results of a Sutton (1994) describes a routine as a sequence
number of studies showing past behavior to be of behaviors which is repeated on a regular
the best predictor of future behavior. For basis. However, what distinguishes them from
example, Mullen, Hersey, and Iverson (1987) habits is their need to be supported by self-
found initial behavior to be the strongest reminders. It may be possible therefore to make
predictor of the consumption of sweet and the distinction between occasions when the
fried foods, smoking, and exercise over an eight- influence of past behavior is mediated by social
month period. Similar results have been cognitive variables and those occasions when it
24 Health Behavior

is seen to have a direct influence via habitual the accessibility of relevant attitudes influences
responses. In particular, future work should the strength of the relationship between atti-
develop measures of habit and routine that are tudes and behavior (Fazio & Williams, 1986)
discriminable from frequency of past behavior and that highly accessible attitudes can lead to
and outline the processes through which habit selective perception (Houston & Fazio, 1989;
and routine determine behavior. Fazio & Williams, 1986). Both findings are
consistent with the spontaneous processing
model. This above work has important implica-
8.01.5.3 Processes by which Cognitions tions for SCMs and health behaviors. The
Influence Behavior SCMs outlined here are deliberative processing
models inasmuch as they focus on the conscious
One important implication of Ronis et al.'s processing of information and fail to consider
(1989) distinction between habits and decisions spontaneous or automatic influences on beha-
is the suggestion that social cognition models vior. Hence, current SCMs may provide only a
may only predict health behavior under certain partial account of the social cognitive determi-
conditions. This issue has been addressed by nants of behavior, that is, they may only be
Fazio (1990) in the development of the MODE applicable in situations where the individual has
model of attitude±behavior relationships. He the ability and motivation to engage in
suggests that attitudes (and presumably other deliberative processing of information (Conner,
cognitions) influence behavior via two distinct 1993). For many behavioral decisions, simpli-
processes: a deliberative (or controlled) process fied or spontaneous decision-making rules may
and a spontaneous process. Most social cogni- be employed instead (Norman & Conner, 1993).
tion models can be labeled as deliberative Fazio's (1990) spontaneous model has consider-
processing models as they assume that behavior able potential in helping to provide a full
results from a controlled process of conscious account of the cognitive influences on behavior.
deliberation. However, Fazio (1990) argues that However, it is clear that most of the empirical
individuals may only make a behavioral work to date has focused on issues surrounding
decision in such a manner when they have the the activation of attitudes and their influence on
opportunity and motivation to do so. Under perception; later components of the model have
other conditions, attitudes which are highly received less attention.
accessible in memory may determine behavior
in a spontaneous fashion (Fazio, Powell, &
Williams, 1989). When the spontaneous process 8.01.5.4 Stage Models of Health Behavior
is operating, an attitude may be automatically
activated from memory following the presenta- A number of researchers have suggested that
tion of relevant cues, with the likelihood of there may be qualitatively different stages in the
activation determined by the accessibility of the initiation and maintenance of health behavior,
attitude. Once activated, the attitude shapes the and that to obtain a full understanding of the
perception of the attitude object in an auto- determinants of health behavior it is necessary
matic, attitude-congruent, fashion. For exam- to conduct a detailed analysis of the nature of
ple, if a positive attitude is activated then this these stages. From a social cognitive perspec-
will lead the individual to attend to and notice tive, an important implication of this position is
the positive qualities of the attitude object. This that different cognitions may be important at
automatic process of selective perception will different stages in promoting health behavior.
therefore shape the individual's definition of the One of the first stage models was put forward
event, and thus determine behavior. If the event by Prochaska and DiClemente (1984) in their
is defined on the basis of positive perceptions of transtheoretical model of change (TTM). Their
the attitude object, for example, then approach model has been widely applied to analyze the
behaviors will follow. In addition, it is argued process of change in alcoholism treatment
that normative guidelines (e.g., social norms or (DiClemente & Hughes, 1990), smoking cessa-
rules) may also influence the definition of the tion (DiClemente et al., 1991), head injury
event and thus may help to determine behavior rehabilitation (Lam, McMahon, Priddy, &
in some situations. Gehred-Schutlz, 1988), and psychotherapy
One important feature of the spontaneous (McConnaughly, DiClemente, Prochaska, &
processing model is that it outlines one way in Velicer, 1989). In its most recent form, DiCle-
which social cognitive variables (i.e., highly mente et al. (1991) identify five stages of change:
accessible attitudes) may determine behavior precontemplation, contemplation, preparation,
without systematic deliberation. To date, there action, and maintenance. Individuals are seen to
has been little research on Fazio's model, progress through each stage to achieve success-
although it has been successfully shown that ful maintenance of a new behavior. Taking the
Limitations and Extensions of Existing Models 25

example of smoking cessation, it is argued that important point is that these models are
in the precontemplation stage smokers are dynamic in nature; people move from one stage
unaware that their behavior constitutes a to another over time. Second, these stage
problem and have no intention to quit. In the models imply that different cognitions are
contemplation stage, smokers start to think important at different stages (Sandman &
about changing their behavior, but are not Weinstein, 1993). For example, in the earlier
committed to try to quit. In the preparation stages information may be processed about the
stage, the smoker has an intention to quit and costs and benefits of performing a behavior,
starts to make plans about how to quit. The while in the later stages cognitions become more
action stage is characterized by active attempts focused on the development of plans of action to
to quit, and after six months of successful initiate and support the maintenance of a
abstinence the individual moves into the behavior. This earlier motivational phase is
maintenance stage characterized by attempts assumed to end with the formation of an
to prevent relapse and to consolidate the newly intention and only when the level of motivation
acquired nonsmoking status. Whilst relative or intention reaches a particular level is the
widely applied, the evidence in support of the individual assumed to be likely to move on to
model and the different stages is at present later stages.
relatively weak (see Weinstein, Rothman, & The main SCMs of health behavior are
Sutton, in press). primarily concerned with people's motivations
Heckhausen (1991) has similarly identified to perform a health behavior and, as such,
phases in the initiation and maintenance of provide strong predictions of behavioral inten-
behavior change; these being the predecisional, tions (i.e., the end of a motivational stage).
postdecisional, actional, and evaluative phases, Ajzen (1991), for example, reports an average
which follow a similar progressive sequence as multiple correlation of 0.71 between variables in
that outlined by Prochaska and DiClemente the TPB and behavioral intention. However,
(1984). It is further suggested that different intentions do not always lead to corresponding
types of cognitions are important in each of actions. Studies examining the intention±
these phases. So in the predecisional phase, behavior relationship have reported a wide
cognitions about the desirability and feasibility range of correlations. For example, Sheppard
of the behavior are believed to be important et al. (1988) reported intention±behavior corre-
determinants of a desire to perform the behavior lations ranging from 0.10 to 0.94. Clearly, many
in question. This phase ends with the formation people who intend to perform a behavior fail to
of an intention to change. In contrast, the do so. However, the SCMs considered do not
decisional phase focuses on the development address the issue of translating intentions into
of plans and ends with the successful initiation action. They can be conceptualized as static
of the behavior. In the actional phase the models that stop at the formation of an
individual focuses on effectively achieving intention without distinguishing between in-
performance of the behavior and ends with tenders who become actors and those who do
the conlusion of the behavior. In the final, not. As Bagozzi (1993) argues, the variables
evaluative phase the individual compares outlined in the main social cognition models are
achieved outcomes with initial goals in order necessary but not sufficient determinants of
to regulate and maintain behavior. While this behavior. Clearly, a detailed analysis of the
four phase model of behavior was not developed social cognitive variables important in translat-
for the prediction of health behavior, the ing intentions into action is required to provide
potential for its application is clear (see a full account of the determinants of health
Gollwitzer, 1993). Other stage models have behavior.
been developed including the health action Relatively little detailed attention has focused
process approach (Schwarzer, 1992; Schwarzer on the cognitive processes underlying the
& Fuchs, 1996), the precaution-adoption pro- successful implementation of intentions. The
cess (Weinstein, 1988, Weinstein & Sandman, main social cognition models contain few
1992), and goal setting theory (Bagozzi, 1992, measures that account for the intention±
1993; Bagozzi & Edwards, 1998). behavior gap (Abraham & Sheeran, 1993).
There are two important themes in each of the The TPB attempts to do this by proposing a
stage models outlined above. First, they direct link between perceived behavioral control
emphasize a temporal perspective with different and behavior. Thus, people's perceptions about
stages of behavior change. While the models the amount of control they have over a behavior
postulate different numbers of stages, they all influence the likely performance of behavior
follow the same pattern from a precontempla- independently of their intentions, although an
tion stage through a motivation stage to the analysis of the volitional processes underlying
initiation and maintenance of behavior. The performance of a health behavior is required,
26 Health Behavior

and a number of researchers have focused any other environmental cue. This suggests that
attention on this issue (e.g., Kuhl, 1985; the making of an implementation intention can
Schwarzer, 1992; Weinstein, 1988). Here we significantly increase the performance of a
focus on Gollwitzer's (1993) work on imple- behavior. In a study on exercise behavior,
mentation intentions and Bagozzi's (1992) Kendzierski (1990) found that respondents were
model of goal achievement to highlight the more likely to implement their intentions to
social cognitive variables important in the exercise when they had engaged in some prior
initiation and maintenance of behavior. planning. Further work needs to establish the
Gollwitzer (1993) made the distinction be- utility of implementation intentions in predict-
tween goal intentions and implementation ing health behavior. However, initial findings
intentions. While the former are concerned are encouraging and suggest that those who
with intentions to perform a behavior or achieve make such plans of action are more likely to
a goal (i.e., ªI intend to achieve xº), the latter are initiate and maintain behavior.
concerned with plans as to when, where, and The work of Gollwitzer (1993) is important in
how the goal intention is to be translated into that it identifies one way in which goal
behavior (i.e., ªI intend to initiate the goal- intentions may be translated into behavior. A
directed behavior x when situation y is similar but more comprehensive approach has
encounteredº). Goal intentions are most like been put forward by Bagozzi (Bagozzi, 1992,
the intention construct in the TPB, although in 1993; Bagozzi & Edwards, 1998; Bagozzi &
the TPB such intentions usually refer to actions Warshaw, 1990) in his model of goal achieve-
or behaviors rather than goals. The important ment. He focuses on goal-directed behavior and
point about implementation intentions is that argues that to initiate behavior individuals need
they commit the individual to a specific course to form an ªintention to tryº to achieve their
of action when certain environmental condi- desired goal. Once an intention to try has been
tions are met; in so doing they help translate formed, the individual focuses on the means, or
goal intentions into action. Gollwitzer (1993) instrumental acts, by which to attempt to
argues that by making implementation inten- achieve the desired goal. Considering the
tions individuals pass over control to the example of weight loss, a number of instru-
environment. The environment acts as a cue mental acts can be identified, including restrict-
to action, such that when certain conditions are ing between-meal foods, reducing overall
met, the performance of the intended behavior calorie consumption, avoidance of high calorie
follows. These ideas have similarities with foods, exercise, and so on. Bagozzi (1993)
Weinstein's (1988) ªmessy deskº analogy, argues that for each of these instrumental acts,
whereby people may have intentions to achieve three appraisal tasks are performed. First, the
a number of goals (i.e., ªprojectsº) which get individual considers the extent to which they are
ªlostº on the ªmessy desk.º Which project is confident that they could perform the instru-
actually worked upon is determined by envir- mental act (i.e., specific self-efficacies). Second,
onmental factors in a similar way as outlined by the likelihood that the instrumental act will help
Gollwitzer (1993). in achieving the desired goal is assessed (i.e.,
Gollwitzer (1993, 1996) has compiled a range instrumental beliefs). Third, the individual
of experimental evidence to support the view considers an affective response towards the
that the making of implementation intentions instrumental act (i.e., affect towards means).
can aid the performance of intended behavior. Once an individual initiates efforts to achieve a
To date, the only application of implementation goal, there are a number of cognitive activities
intentions to the prediction of health behavior is that support the successful initiation and
a study by Orbell, Hodgkins, and Sheeran maintenance of goal-directed behavior. First,
(1997) on breast self-examination. At the end of the individual can develop plans in order to
a questionnaire about breast self-examination, ensure that instrumental acts are performed.
half the women were asked to indicate when and This involves identifying the situation or
where in the next month they intended to triggering conditions under which the instru-
perform breast self-examination. A one month mental act is performed (Bagozzi & Warshaw,
follow-up found that 64% of these women had 1990). This idea that certain environmental
performed breast self-examination that month conditions may trigger behavior has a clear
compared with only 16% of women who had overlap with Gollwitzer's (1993) work on
not made an implementation intention, despite implementation intentions and Weinstein's
no difference in goal intentions. In addition, (1988) ªmessy deskº analogy. One way in which
everyone in the implementation condition who plans are more likely to be acted upon is through
actually performed the behavior reported doing the development of scripts or cognitive rehear-
so in response to the environmental cue in the sal, whereby the individuals imagine themselves
implementation intention and not in response to performing the instrumental act (Anderson,
Limitations and Extensions of Existing Models 27

1983). Another is through the use of precom- making a change to their behavior. However,
mitting devices whereby the behavior is made this stage may be brought to an end by a range
more likely by precommitting oneself to it (e.g., of cues to action, as outlined in the HBM, which
avoiding eating butter at home by not having may motivate the individual to start thinking
butter in the house). Bagozzi (1993) also about performing a health-related behavior.
proposes that ongoing behavior has to be One such cue to action may be perceived threat
monitored to ensure, for example, that the (i.e., perceived susceptibility and perceived
instrumental acts achieve their objectives. If any severity). While perceived susceptibility and
unforeseen impediments are encountered then perceived severity are seen to be important
these need to be taken into consideration and determinants of behavior in the HBM, research
any future plans modified accordingly. These with these dimensions has tended to show that
ideas overlap with Kuhl's (1985) theory of they are relatively weak predictors of behavior.
action control which identifies a number of However, as Schwarzer (1992) has argued, it
processes by which individuals attempt to may be more appropriate to consider these
control their actions and achieve their goals. variables to have an indirect or more distal
These processes may be particularly important influence on behavior. Thus they may act as a
in allowing individuals to overcome tempta- cue to action, motivating the individual to start
tions to break their new behavior (see Loewen- deliberating over performing a health-related
stein, 1996). Finally, goal-directed behavior is behavior, and thus ensuring movement from the
likely to be stronger and more persistent if the first to the second stage.
individual has a strong sense of commitment to In the second stage, a decision-making or
the decision to try to achieve the goal and the motivation stage, the individual is thinking
means to achieve it. about adopting a new behavior, and the stage
ends when the individual forms an intention to
perform the behavior. To date, most social
8.01.5.5 Theoretical Integrations and Future cognition models have been primarily con-
Directions cerned with this stage. These models distinguish
between three distinct determinants of indivi-
The above research demonstrates some of the duals' intentions to perform a health behavior.
ways in which research into health behaviors First, are outcome expectancies, which focus on
based upon social cognitive approaches is the perceived consequences of performing a
developing. Here we outline possible directions behavior. These expectancies may also cover the
for the future development of an integrative notion of behavioral beliefs as considered in the
social cognition model of health behavior, TPB and include anticipated affective reactions.
outline some of the basic requirements for such Second, are normative influences which are
a model, and indicate some fruitful avenues for primarily tapped by the subjective norm and
future research. normative belief components of the TPB. This
It is clear that to fully explain health behavior group of variables could also include descriptive
it is necessary to develop a more dynamic model norms, moral norms, and perceived social
that examines different stages or phases in the support. The third influence on individuals'
contemplation, initiation, and maintenance of intentions to perform a behavior is control
behavior. What is being proposed is an beliefs (or self-efficacy expectancies) and may be
integration of current SCMs (such as the based on a consideration of perceived barriers
TPB) with stage models of health behavior (HBM) and control beliefs (TPB). In the
(such as the TTM). Several authors have motivation stage, it is likely that other variables
recommended such an integration (e.g., Cour- may have a more distal influence on behavioral
neya, Nigg, & Estabrooks, 1998; Godin, intention via the variables outlined above. For
Desharnais, Valois, & Bradet, 1995; Marcus, example, health locus of control beliefs may
Eaton, Rossi, & Harlow, 1994). Though the help shape self-efficacy expectancies, self-iden-
stage models considered in this chapter have tity and health values may influence the
suggested differing number of stages, it is likely interpretation of the potential consequences
that an integrative model should address at least of a behavior, and past behavior or experience
four or five main stages: precontemplation, may provide information which is used to
contemplation, planning, action, and mainte- determine the ease or difficulty of performing a
nance (Norman & Conner, 1996a). behavior (i.e., self-efficacy).
One implication of the identification of Once a behavioral intention has been formed,
different stages is that different cognitive it has to be translated into behavior. In the third
variables may be important in ensuring move- stage, the individual is therefore concerned with
ment from one stage to the next. In the first planning; focusing on the specific actions or
stage, the individuals are not thinking about instrumental acts that need to be performed and
28 Health Behavior

the resources required to support them. Thus, a the contention that important determinants of
number of authors have highlighted the im- health behaviors are identified in these models.
portance of action plans in this stage (Bagozzi & Further refinement and development of these
Warshaw, 1990; Schwarzer, 1992). Similarly, models along the lines we have suggested and
Gollwitzer (1993) focuses on implementation reviewed may lead to even better predictions of
intentions which help ensure performance of the behavior and greater understanding of health
target behavior. Despite slight differences behavior and how individuals may be encour-
between definitions of these two concepts, both aged to change.
emphasize the need to construct fairly detailed However, persuasive messages targeted at
plans of action in order to bridge the intention± relevant cognitions identified by SCMs may not
behavior gap. As Schwarzer (1992) argues, self- be sufficient to produce the major behavior
efficacy may have an important role to play in change necessary for health benefits to accrue. It
the development and implementation of such may be that strategies which employ multiple
plans, as might self-identity (Sparks & Shep- level interventions which take account not only
herd, 1992) and a sense of commitment of the psychosocial factors influencing perfor-
(Bagozzi, 1993). The planning stage is brought mance of the behavior (derived from SCMs) but
to an end when the individual initiates behavior. also models of the process of persuasion of how
In the fourth stage, the individual has to people change and the context in which changes
ensure that the behavior is successfully enacted. are made will be important (Glanz et al., 1990;
Various cognitive processes which are con- Hockbaum & Lorig, 1992; Winett, 1985).
cerned with the monitoring and controlling of
behavior may be important in this stage.
Schwarzer (1992), for example, highlights the 8.01.7 REFERENCES
need for action control in which the behavior is Abraham, C., & Sheeran, P. (1993). Inferring cognitions,
re-evaluated against initial goals in order to predicting behaviour: Two challenges for social cogni-
regulate and maintain behavior (see also tion models. Health Psychology Update, 14, 18±23.
Abraham, C. S., Sheeran, P., Abrams, D., & Spears, R.
Bagozzi & Warshaw, 1990; Heckhausen, (1994). Exploring teenagers' adaptive and maladaptive
1991). As with the development of action plans, thinking in relation to the threat of HIV infection.
self-efficacy, self-identity, and commitment may Psychology and Health, 9, 253±272.
all be important variables in ensuring that Adler, N., & Matthews, K. (1994). Health psychology:
Why do some people get sick and some stay well? Annual
behavior is maintained. In addition, Kuhl Review of Psychology, 45, 229±259.
(1985) has argued that there may be individual Aguirre-Molina, M., & Gorman, D. M. (1996).
differences in people's propensity to engage in Community-based approaches for the prevention of
monitoring activities that may account for why alcohol, tobacco, and other drug use. Annual Review of
Public Health, 17, 337±358.
behavior is not always maintained. These Aho, W. R. (1979). Smoking, dieting and exercise: Age
monitoring activities help to ensure that the differences in attitudes and behavior relevant to selected
behavior is successfully completed. The same health belief model variables. Rhode Island Medical
activities will be important in the fifth, main- Journal, 62, 95±102.
tenance stage for ongoing behaviors, where the Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., Woodward, C. K., Reno, R. R.,
& Reynolds, K. D. (1994). Increasing screening mam-
individual is concerned with ensuring that the mography in asymptomatic women: Evaluation of a
behavior is successfully repeated. second-generation, theory-based program. Health Psy-
chology, 13, 526±538.
Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality and behavior. Milton
Keynes, UK: Open University Press.
8.01.6 CONCLUSIONS Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
Health behaviors have been demonstrated to 50, 179±211.
have important consequences for both the Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on
quality and length of life through influencing behavior. In P. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.),
Psychology of action (pp. 385±403). New York: Guilford
various disease outcomes. We have attempted to Press.
justify the interest in understanding health Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes
behaviors as a basis for attempting to change and predicting social behavior. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ:
their occurrence in order to increase both length Prentice-Hall.
Allied Dunbar Fitness Survey (1992). London: Health
and quality of life. SCMs provide one approach Education Authority.
to understanding health behavior in describing Allison, K. R. (1987). Perceived control as a determinant of
the important social cognitive variables under- preventive health behaviour for heart disease and lung
lying such behaviors. We believe that these cancer. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
models provide an important way of achieving Toronto.
Amler, R. W., & Dull, H. B. (1987). Closing the gap. New
these aims by providing a means for identifying York: Oxford University Press.
useful targets for persuasion. It would seem that Anderson, C. A. (1983). Imagination and expectation: The
there is already sufficient literature to support effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal
References 29

intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, health, health care and the health professions (pp. 539±
45, 293±305. 568). New York: Free Press.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (in press). Distinguishing Becker, M. H., Radius, S. M., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1978).
perceptions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting Compliance with a medical regimen for asthma: A test of
consumption of a low fat diet using the theory of the health belief model. Public Health Reports, 93,
planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 268±277.
Ashworth, H. W. (1963). An experiment in presympto- Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1987). Comparing
matic diagnosis. Journal of the Royal College of General social learning theory and the health belief model. In W.
Practitioners, 6, 71. B. Ward (Ed.), Advances in health education and
Aspinwall, L. G., Kemeny, M. E., Taylor, S. E., Schneider, promotion (Vol. 2, pp. 245±249). Greenwich, CT: JAI
S. G., & Dudley, J. P. (1991). Psychosocial predictors of Press.
gay men's AIDS risk-reduction behavior. Health Psy- Belloc, N. B. (1973). Relationship of health practices to
chology, 10, 432±444. mortality. Preventive Medicine, 2, 67±81.
Babrow, A. S., Black, D. R., & Tiffany, S. T. (1990). Belloc, N. B., & Breslow, L. (1972). Relationship of
Beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and a smoking-cessation physical health status and health practices. Preventive
program: a planned behavior analysis of communication Medicine, 9, 409±421.
campaign development. Health Communication, 2, Bengel, J., Belz-Merk, M., & Farin, E. (1996). The role of
145±163. risk perception and efficacy cognitions in the prediction
Baer, J. S., Marlatt, G. A., Kivlahan, D. R., Fromme, K., of HIV-related preventive behavior and condom use.
Larimer, M. E., & Williams, E. (1992). An experimental Psychology and Health, 11, 505±525.
test of three methods of alcohol risk reduction with Bentler, P. M., & Speckart, G. (1979). Models of attitude-
young adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- behavior relations. Psychological Review, 86, 452±464.
ogy, 60, 974±979. Bernier, M. & Avard, J. (1986). Self-efficacy, outcome and
Bagozzi, R. P. (1992). The self-regulation of attitudes, attrition in a weight reduction program. Cognitive
intentions and behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, Therapy and Research, 10, 319±338.
55, 178±204. Best, J. A. (1975). Tailoring smoking withdrawal proce-
Bagozzi, R. P. (1993). On the neglect of volition in dures to personality and motivational differences.
consumer research: A critique and proposal. Psychology Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 1±8.
and Marketing, 10, 215±237. Biddle, B. J. Bank, B. J., & Slavings, R. L. (1987). Norms,
Bagozzi, R. P., & Edwards, E. A. (1998). Goal setting and preferences, identities and retention decisions. Social
goal pursuit in the regulation of body weight. Psychology Psychology Quarterly, 50, 322±337.
and Health, 13. Black Report (1988). Inequalities in health: the Black report
Bagozzi, R. P., & Warshaw, P. R. (1990). Trying to and the health divide. London: DHSS.
consume. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 127±140. Blair, S. N., Goodyear, N. N., Gibbons, L. W., & Cooper,
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying K. H. (1984). Physical fitness and incidence of hyperten-
theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, sion in healthy normotensive men and women. Journal of
191±215. the American Medical Association, 252, 487±490.
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human Blane, D., Smith, G. D., & Bartley, M. (1990). Social class
agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122±147. differences in years of potential life lost: Size trends, and
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and principal causes. British Medical Journal, 301, 429±432.
action: A cognitive social theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Blaxter, M. (1990). Health and lifestyles. London: Tavi-
Prentice-Hall. stock.
Bandura, A. (1991). Self-efficacy mechanism in physiolo- Boer, I., & Seydel, E. R. (1996). Protection motivation
gical activation and health-promoting behavior. In J. theory. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting
Madden (Ed.), Neurobiology of learning, emotion and health behaviour (pp. 95±120). Buckingham, UK: Open
affect (pp. 229±270). New York: Raven Press. University Press.
Basen-Engquist, K., & Parcel, G. S. (1992). Attitudes, Boldero, J., Moore, S., & Rosenthal, D. (1992). Intention,
norms, and self-efficacy: A model of adolescents' HIV- context, and safe sex: Australian adolescents' responses
related sexual risk behavior. Health Education Quarterly, to AIDS. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22,
19, 263±277. 1374±1396.
Beale, D. A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1991). Predicting Borgida, E., Conner, C., & Manteufel, L. (1992). Under-
mothers' intentions to limit frequency of infants' sugar standing living kidney donation: a behavioral decision
intake: Testing the theory of planned behavior. Journal perspective. In S. Spacapan & S. Oskamp (Eds.), Helping
of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 409±431. and being helped: Naturalistic studies (pp. 183±212).
Beck, K. H. & Lund, A. K. (1981). The effects of health Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
threat seriousness and personal efficacy upon intentions Boulton, M., Schramm Evans, Z., Fitzpatrick, R., & Hart,
and behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 11, G. (1991). Bisexual men: Women, safer sex, and HIV
401±415. infection. In P. Aggleton, P. M. Davies, & G. Hart
Beck, K. H. (1981). Driving while under the influence of (Eds.), AIDS: Responses, policy and care (pp. 55±67).
alcohol: Relationship to attitudes and beliefs in a college London: Falmer Press.
population. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Boyd, B., & Wandersman, A. (1991). Predicting under-
Abuse, 8, 377±388. graduate condom use with the Fishbein and Ajzen and
Beck, L., & Ajzen, I. (1991). Predicting dishonest actions the Triandis attitude-behavior models: Implications for
using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of public health interventions. Journal of Applied Social
Research in Personality, 25, 285±301. Psychology, 21, 1810±1830.
Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and sick role Bradley, C., Lewis, K. S., Jennings, A. M., & Ward, J. D.
behavior. Health Education Monographs, 2, 409±419. (1990). Scales to measure perceived control developed
Becker, M. H., Kaback, M. M., Rosenstock, I. M., & specifically for people with tablet-treated diabetes.
Ruth, M. (1975). Some influences on public participation Diabetic Medicine, 7, 685±694.
in a genetic screening program. Journal of Community Brawley, L. R. (1993). The practicality of using social
Health, 1, 3±14. psychological theories for exercise and health research
Becker, M. H., & Maiman, L. A. (1983). Models of health- and intervention. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 5,
related behavior. In D. Mechanic (Ed.), Handbook of 99±115.
30 Health Behavior

Breslow, L., & Enstrom, J. E. (1980). Persistence of health Colliver, J. D., & Mallin, H. (1986). State an national
habits and their relationship to mortality. Preventive trends in alcohol-related mortality, 1975±1982. Alcohol
Medicine, 9, 469±483. Health and Research World, 10, 60±64.
British Medical Journal (1982). Alcohol problems. London: Committee on Medical Aspects of Food Policy (1991).
BMJ Publications. Dietary reference values for food energy and nutrients
Brock, B. M., Haefner D. P., & Noble, D. S. (1988). for the United Kingdom. Report on Health and Social
Alameda County Redux: Replication in Michigan. Subjects, 41. London: HMSO.
Preventive Medicine, 17, 483±495. Condiotte, M. M., & Lichtenstein, E. (1981). Self-efficacy
Brouwers, M. C., & Sorrentino, R. M. (1993). Uncertainty and relapse in smoking cessation programs. Journal of
orientation and protection motivation theory: The role Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 648±658.
of individual differences in health compliance. Journal of Connelly, C. E. (1984). Compliance with outpatient lithium
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 102±112. therapy. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 22, 44±50.
Brown, J. D. (1991). Staying fit and staying well: Physical Conner, M. (1993). Pros and cons of social cognition
fitness as a moderator of life stress. Journal of Personality models in health behaviour. Health Psychology Update,
and Social Psychology, 60, 555±561. 14, 24±31.
Brown, N., Muhlenkamp, A., Fox, L., & Osborn, M. Conner, M., Martin, E., Silverdale, N., & Grogan, S.
(1983). The relationship among health beliefs, health (1996). Dieting in adolescence: An application of the
values, and health promotion activity. Western Journal theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Health
of Nursing Research, 5, 155±163. Psychology, 1, 315±325.
Brubaker, R. G., & Fowler, C. (1990). Encouraging College Conner, M., & Norman, P. (1994). Comparing the health
males to perform testicular self-examination: Evaluation belief model and the theory of planned behaviour in
of a persuasive message based on the revised theory of health screening. In D. R. Rutter & L. Quine (Eds.),
reasoned action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Social psychology and health: European perspectives
17, 1411±1422. (pp. 1±24). Aldershot, UK: Avebury.
Bundek, N. I., Marks, G., & Richardson, J. L. (1993). Role Conner, M., & Norman, P. (Eds.) (1996a). Predicting
of health locus of control beliefs in cancer screening of health behaviour. Buckingham, UK: Open University
elderly Hispanic women. Health Psychology, 12, Press.
193±199. Conner, M., & Norman, P. (1996b). The role of social
Burk, C., & Kimiecik, J. (1994). Examining the relationship cognition in health behaviours. In M. Conner & P.
among locus of control, health value and exercise. Health Norman (Eds.), Predicting health behaviour (pp. 1±22).
Values, 18, 14±23. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Butts, S. V., & Chotlas, J. A. (1973). A comparison of Conner, M., & Sparks, P. (1996). The theory of planned
alcoholics and nonalcoholics on perceived locus of behaviour and health behaviours. In M. Conner &
control. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 34, P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting health behaviour
1327±1332. (pp. 121±162). Buckingham, UK: Open University
Calnan, M. (1989). Control over health and patterns of Press.
health-related behaviour. Social Science and Medicine, Consensus Development Conference on Lowering Blood
29, 131±136. Cholesterol to Prevent Heart Disease (1985). Lowering
Carlson, B., & Petti, K. (1989). Health locus of control and blood cholesterol to prevent heart disease. Journal of the
participation in physical activity. American Journal of Australian Medical Association, 253, 2080±2086.
Health Promotion, 3, 32±37. Corcoran, K. J. (1991). Efficacy, ªskills,º reinforcement,
Chaiken, S., Lieberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). and choice behavior. American Psychologist, 46,
Heuristic and systematic information processing within 155±157.
and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. Costello, R. M., & Manders, K. R. (1974). Locus of control
A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212±252). New and alcoholism. British Journal of Addiction, 69, 11±17.
York: Guilford Press. Courneya, K. S., Nigg, C. R., & Estabrooks, P. A. (1998).
Chambliss, C. A., & Murray, E. J. (1979a). Efficacy Relationships among the theory of planned behavior,
attribution, locus of control, and weight loss. Cognitive stages of change, and exercise behavior in older persons
Therapy and Research, 3, 349±353. over a three year period. Psychology & Health, 13.
Chambliss, C. A., & Murray, E. J. (1979b). Cognitive Cox, B. D, Huppert, F. A., & Whichelow, M. J. (1993). The
procedures for smoking reduction: symptom attribution health and lifestyles survey: Seven years on. Aldershot,
versus efficacy attribution. Cognitive Therapy and Re- UK: Dartmouth.
search, 3, 91±95. Dean, K. (1991). Relationships between knowledge and
Champion, V. L. (1984). Instrument development of the belief variables and health maintenance behaviours in a
health belief model constructs. Advances in Nursing Danish population over 45 years of age. Journal of
Science, 6, 73±85. Ageing and Health, 3, 386±406.
Charng, H. W., Piliavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role Desharnais, R., Bouillon, J., & Godin, G. (1986). Self-
identity and reasoned action in the prediction of repeated efficacy and outcome expectations as determinants of
behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 303±317. exercise adherence. Psychological Reports, 59, 1155±
Chen, M. M., & Land, K. C. (1986). Testing the health 1159.
belief model: LISREL analysis of alternative models of DeVellis, B. M., Blalock, S. J., & Sandler, R. S. (1990).
the causal relationships between health beliefs and Predicting participation in cancer screening: the role of
preventive dental behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly. perceived behavioral control. Journal of Applied Social
49, 45±60. Psychology, 20, 639±660.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A DeVito, A. J., Bogdanowicz, J., & Reznikoff, M. (1982).
focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the Actual and intended health-related information seeking
concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. and health locus of control. Journal of Personality
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, Assessment, 46, 63±69.
1015±1026. DeVries, H., Backbier, E., Kok, G., & Dijkstra, M. (1995).
Colletti, G., Supnick, J. A., & Payne, T. J. (1985). The The impact of social influences in the context of attitude,
smoking self-efficacy questionnaire (SSEQ): Preliminary self-efficacy, intention, and previous behavior as pre-
scale development and validation. Behavioral Assess- dictors of smoking onset. Journal of Applied Social
ment, 7, 249±260. Psychology, 25, 237±257.
References 31

DeVries, H., Dijkstra, M., & Kuhlman, P. (1988). Self- training and mental health. American Psychologist, 36,
efficacy: The third factor besides attitude and subjective 373±389.
norm as a predictor of behavioural intentions. Health Forrest, A.P.M. (1986). Breast cancer screening. London:
Education Research, 3, 273±282. HMSO.
DiClemente, C. C., & Hughes, S. O. (1990). Stages of Fredricks, A. J., & Dossett, D. L. (1983). Attitude±
change profiles in outpatient alcoholism treatment. behavior relations: A comparison of the Fishbein±Ajzen
Journal of Substance Abuse, 2, 217±235. and Bentler±Speckart models. Journal of Personality and
DiClemente, C. C., Fairhurst, S. K., Velasquez, M. M., Social Psychology, 45, 501±512.
Prochaska, J. O., Velicer, W. F., & Rossi, J. S. (1991). Freeling, P. (1965). Candidates for exfoliative cytology of
The process of smoking cessation: An analysis of the cervix. Journal of the Royal College of General
precontemplation, contemplation, and preparation Practitioners, 10, 261.
stages of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Friedman, G. D., Dales, L. G., & Ury, H. K. (1979).
Psychology, 59, 295±304. Mortality in middle-aged smokers and non-smokers.
Dishman, R. K. (1982) Compliance/adherence in health- New England Journal of Medicine, 300, 213±217.
related exercise. Health Psychology, 1, 237±267. Fries, J. F., Green, L. W., & Levine, S. (1989). Health
Doll, R., Peto, R., Wheatley, K., Gray, R., & Sutherland, I. promotion and the compression of mortality. Lancet, i,
(1994). Mortality in relation to smoking: 40 years' 481±483.
observations on male British doctors. British Medical Frisch, D., & Clemen, R. T. (1994). Beyond expected
Journal, 309, 901±911. utility: rethinking behavioral decision making. Psycho-
Donovan, D. M., & O'Leary, M. R. (1978). The drinking- logical Bulletin, 116, 46±54.
related locus of control scale. Journal of Studies on General Household Survey (1989). London: OPCS.
Alcohol, 39, 759±784. General Household Survey (1992). London: OPCS.
Duffy, M. E. (1988). Determinants of health promotion in General Household Survey (1993). London: OPCS.
midlife women. Nursing Research, 37, 358±362. General Household Survey (1994). London: OPCS.
Dzewaltowski, D. A., Noble, J. M., & Shaw, J. M. (1990). Georgiou, A., & Bradley, C. (1992). The development of a
Physical activity participationÐsocial cognitive theory smoking specific locus of control scale. Psychology and
versus the theories of reasoned action and planned Health, 6, 227±240.
behavior. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., & Bushman, B. J. (1996).
388±405. Relation between perceived vulnerability to HIV and
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of precautionary sexual behavior. Psychological Bulletin,
attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 119, 390±409.
Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Gianetti, V. J., Reynolds, J., & Rihen, T. (1985). Factors
Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380±417. which differentiate smokers from ex-smokers among
Edwards, W. (1992). Utility theories: measurements and cardiovascular patients: A discriminant analysis. Social
applications. Boston: Kluwer. Science and Medicine, 20, 241±245.
Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Predicting young
High Cholesterol in Adults (1993). Summary of the adults' health risk behavior. Journal of Personality and
second report of the National Cholesterol Education Social Psychology, 69, 505±517.
Program (NCEP) expert panel on detection, evaluation, Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1997). Health images and
and treatment of high cholesterol in adults. Journal of the their effects on health behavior. In B. P. Buuuk & F. X.
American Medical Association, 269, 3015±3023. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping, and social comparisons
Farin, E. (1994). Eine Metaanalyse Empirischer Studien (pp. 63±94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
zum PraÈdiktiven Wert Kognitiver Variablen der HIV- Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Ouelette, J., & Burzette, R.
Bezogenen Risikowahrnehmung und-Verabeitung fuÈr (1998). Cognitive antecedents to adolescent health risk:
das HIV-Risikoverhalten. Dissertation. UniversitaÈt Frei- Discriminating between behavioral intention and beha-
burg. vioral willingness. Psychology and Health, 13.
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes Glanz, K., Lewis, F. M., & Rimmer, B. K. (Eds.) (1990).
guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative Health behavior and health education: Theory, research
framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi- and practice. San Francisco: Josey-Bass.
mental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75±109). San Godin, G., Desharnais, R., Valois, P., & Bradet, R. (1995).
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Combining behavioral and motivational dimensions to
Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessi- identify and characterize the stages in the process of
bility as a moderator of the attitude-perception and adherence to exercise. Psychology and Health, 10,
attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 333±344.
presidential election. Journal of Personality and Social Godin, G., & Shepherd, R. J. (1987). Psychosocial factors
Psychology, 51, 505±514. influencing intentions to exercise in a group of indivi-
Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Williams, C. J. (1989). The duals ranging from 45 to 74 years of age. In M. E.
role of attitude accessibility in the attitude-to-behavior Berridge & G. R. Ward (Eds.), International perspectives
process. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 280±288. on adapted physical activity (pp. 243±249). Champaign,
Feather, N. T. (1982). Expectations and actions: IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.
Expectancy-value models in psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Godin, G., Valois, P., & Lepage, L. (1993). The pattern
Erlbaum. of influence of perceived behavioral control upon
Fife-Schaw, C.R., & Breakwell, G. M. (1992). Estimating exercise behavior: An application of Ajzen's theory of
sexual behaviour parameters in the light of AIDS: A planned behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16,
review of recent UK studies of young people. AIDS 81±102.
Care, 4, 187±201. Godin, G., Valois, P., Lepage, L., & Desharnais, R. (1992).
Fishbein, M. (1993). Foreword. In D. J. Terry, C. Gallois, Predictors of smoking behaviourÐAn application of
& M. McCamish (Eds.). The theory of reasoned action: Ajzen's theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of
Its application to AIDS-related behavior (pp. xv±xxv). Addiction, 87, 1335±1343.
Oxford, UK: Pergamon. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
and behavior. New York: Wiley. motivation and cognition: foundations of social behaviour
Folkins, C. H., & Sime, W. E. (1981). Physical fitness (Vol. 2, pp. 53±92). New York: Guilford Press.
32 Health Behavior

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of theoretical specificity. Social Science and Medicine, 31,
intentions. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), 1051±1056.
European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141± Honkanen, R., Ertama, L., Kuosmanen, P., Linnoila, M.,
185). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Ahla, A., & Visuri, T. (1983). The role of alcohol in
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of accidental falls. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 44,
planning. In P. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), 231±245.
Psychology of action (pp. 287±312). New York: Guilford Horwitz, M. B., Hindi-Alexander, M., & Wagner, T. J.
Press. (1985). Psychosocial mediators of abstinence, relapse,
Gorsuch, R. L., & Ortberg, J. (1983). Moral obligation and continued smoking: A one-year follow-up of a
and attitudes: Their relation to behavioral intentions. minimal intervention. Addictive Behaviors, 10, 29±39.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, Hounsa, A. M., Godin, G., Alihonou, E., Valois, P., &
1025±1028. Girard, J. (1993). An application of Ajzen's theory of
Gottlieb, N. H., & Baker, J. A. (1986). The relative planned behaviour to predict mothers' intention to use
influence of health beliefs, parental and peer behaviours oral rehydration therapy in a rural area of Benin. Social
and exercise program participation on smoking, alcohol Science and Medicine, 37, 253±261.
use and physical activity. Social Science and Medicine, Houston, D. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1989). Biased processing
22, 915±927. as a function of attitude accessibility: Making objective
Gottlieb, N. H., & Green, L. W. (1984). Life events, social judgements subjectively. Social Cognition, 7, 51±66.
network, life-style and health: An analysis of the 1979 Huba, G. J., Wingard, J. A., & Bentler, P. M. (1981).
National survey of personal health practices and Intentions to use drugs among adolescents: A long-
consequences. Health Education Quarterly, 11, 91±105. itudinal analysis. Journal of the Additions, 16, 331±339.
Graham, H. (1987). Women's smoking and family health. Janis, I. L. (1967). Effects of fear arousal on attitude
Social Science and Family Health, 25, 47±56. change: recent developments in theory and experimental
Gregg, I. (1966). The recognition of early chronic research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in experimental
bronchitis. Journal of the American Medical Association, social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 166±224). New York:
176, 1114±1116. Academic Press.
Grube, J. W., Morgan, M., & McGee, S. T. (1986). Janz, N. K., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief
Attitudes and normative beliefs as predictors of smoking model: A decade later. Health Education Quarterly, 11,
intentions and behaviours: A test of three models. British 1±47.
Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 81±93. Jeffrey, R. W., Bjornson-Benson, W. M., Rosenthal, B. S.,
Hallal, J. (1982). The relationship of health beliefs, health Lindquist, R. A., Kurth, C. L., & Johnson, S. L. (1984).
locus of control, and self-concept to the practice of Correlates of weight loss and its maintenance over two
breast self-examination in adult women. Nursing Re- years of follow-up among middle-aged men. Preventive
search, 31, 137±142. Medicine, 13, 155±168.
Harris, R., & Lynn, M. W. (1985). Health beliefs, Jonas, K. (1993). Expectancy-value models of health
compliance and control of diabetes mellitus. Southern behaviour: An analysis by conjoint measurement.
Medical Journal, 2, 162±166. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 167±183.
Harrison, J. A., Mullen, P. D., & Green, L. W. (1992). Kaplan, G. D., & Cowles, A. (1978). Health locus of
A meta-analysis of studies of the Health Belief control and health value in the prediction of smoking
Model with adults. Health Education Research, 7, behavior. Health Education Monographs, 6, 129±137.
107±116. Kavanagh, D. J., Pierce, J., Lo, S. K., & Shelley, J. (1993).
Hartman, P. E., & Becker, M. H. (1978). Non-compliance Self-efficacy and social support as predictors of smoking
with prescribed regimen among chronic haemodialysis after a quit attempt. Psychology and Health, 8, 231±242.
patients. Journal of Dialysis and Transplantation, 7, Kelley, J. A., St. Lawrence, J. S., Brasfield, T. L., Lemke,
978±985. A., Amidei, T., Roffman, R. E., Hood, H. V., Smith, J.
Hayes, D., & Ross, C. E. (1987). Concern with appearance, E., Kilgore, H., & McNeill, C. (1990). Psychological
health beliefs and eating habits. Journal of Health and factors that predict AIDS high-risk versus AIDS
Social Behaviour, 28, 120±130. precautionary behavior. Journal of Consulting and
Health Promotion Authority for Wales (1990) Health in Clinical Psychology, 58, 117±120.
Wales. Cardiff, UK: Author. Kendzierski, D. (1990). Decision making versus decision
Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. Berlin, implementation: An action control approach to exercise
Germany: Springer-Verlag. adoption and adherence. Journal of Applied Social
Hennekens, C. H. (1983). Alcohol. In N. M. Kaplan & J. Psychology, 20, 27±45.
Stamler (Eds.) Prevention of coronary heart disease: Keys, A. (1970). Coronary heart disease in seven countries.
practical management of the risk factors (pp. 130±138). Circulation, 41(Suppl. 1), entire issue.
Philadelphia: Saunders. King, A. C., Blair, S. N., Bild, D. E., Dishman, R. K.,
Hester, N. R., & Macrina, D. M. (1985). The health belief Dubbert, P. M., Marcus, B. H. Oldridge, N. B.,
model and the contraceptive behavior of college women: Paffenbarger, R. S., Powell, K. E., & Yeager, K. K.
Implications for health education. Journal of American (1992). Determinants of physical activity and interven-
College Health, 33, 245±252. tions in adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Hochbaum, G. M. (1958). Public participation in medical Exercise, 24, 1079±1092.
screening programs: A socio-psychological study. Public King, J. (1982). The impact of patients' perceptions of high
Health Service Publication No. 572. Washington, DC: blood pressure on attendance at screening. Social Science
US Government Printing Office. and Medicine, 16, 1079±1091.
Hockbaum, G. M., & Lorig, K. (1992). Roles and uses of Kinket, B., Paans, L., & Verplanken, B. (1992). Patient
theory in health education practice. Health Education education by general practitioners: Application of the
Quarterly, 19, 289±412. theory of planned behavior. Gedrag and Gezondheid
Hodgkins, S. & Orbell, S. (1998). Can protection motiva- Tijdschrift voor Psychologie and Gezondheid, 20, 187±195.
tion theory predict behaviour? A longitudinal study Kirsch, I. (1986). Early research on self-efficacy: What we
exploring the role of previous behaviour. Psychology and already know without knowing we knew. Journal of
Health, 13. Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 339±358.
Hofstetter, C. R., Sallis, J. F., & Howell, M. F. (1990). Kirscht, J. P., Becker, M. H., & Eveland, P. (1976).
Some health dimensions of self-efficacy: analysis of Psychological and social factors as predictors of medical
References 33

behaviour. Journal of Medical Care, 14, 422±431. Maddux, J. E., Norton, L. W., & Stoltenberg, C. D. (1986).
Kok, G., de Vries, H., Mudde, A. N., & Strecher, V. J. Self-efficacy expectancy, outcome expectancy, and
(1991). Planned health education and the role of self- outcome value: relative effects on behavioral intentions.
efficacy: Dutch research. Health Education Research, 6, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
231±238. 783±789.
Kristiansen, C. M. (1986). A two-value model of preventive Maddux, J. E., & Rogers, R. W. (1983). Protection
health behaviour. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear
7, 173±184. appeals and attitude change. Journal of Experimental
Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognition-behavior Social Psychology, 19, 469±479.
consistency: Self-regulatory processes and action versus Maibach, E. W., Flora, J. A., & Nass, C. (1991). Changes
state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action in self-efficacy and health behavior in response to a
control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 101±128). New minimal community health campaign. Health Commu-
York: Springer. nication, 3, 1±15.
Kurland, N. B. (1995). Ethical intentions and the theories Manning, M. M. & Wright, T. L. (1983). Self-efficacy
of reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of expectancies, outcome expectancies, and the persistence
Applied Social Psychology, 25, 297±313. of pain control in childbirth. Journal of Personality and
Lam, C. S., McMahon, B. T., Priddy, D. A., & Gehred- Social Psychology, 54, 421±431.
Schutlz, A. (1988). Deficit awareness and treatment Marcus, B. H., Eaton, C. A., Rossi, J. S., & Harlow, L. L.
performance among traumatic head injury adults. Brain (1994). Self-efficacy, decision-making, and stages of
Injury, 2, 235±242. changeÐAn integrative model of physical exercise.
Langlie, J. K. (1977). Social networks, health beliefs and Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 489±508.
preventive health behaviours. Journal of Health and Marteau, T. M. (1989). Health beliefs and attributions. In
Social Behaviour, 18, 244±260. A. K. Broome (Ed.), Health psychology: processes and
Lau, R. R. (1982). Origins of health locus of control beliefs. applications (pp. 1±23). London: Chapman Hall.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, McAuley, E. (1993). Self-efficacy and the maintenance of
322±334. exercise participation in older adults. Journal of Beha-
Lau, R. R., Hartman, K. A., & Ware, J. E. (1986). Health vioral Medicine, 16, 103±113.
as value: methodological and theoretical considerations. McCaul, K. D., Glasgow, R. E., & O'Neill, H. K. (1992).
Health Psychology, 5, 25±43. The problem of creating habits: establishing health-
Lazarus, R. S. & Launier, R. (1978). Stress related protective dental behaviors. Health Psychology, 11,
transactions between person and environment. In 101±110.
L. A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.), Perspectives in McCaul, K. D., Sandgren, A. K., O'Neill, H. K., & Hinsz,
interactional psychology (pp. 287±327). New York: V. B. (1993). The value of the theory of planned
Plenum. behavior, perceived control, and self-efficacy expecta-
Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In J. P. tions for predicting health-protective behaviors. Basic
Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), and Applied Social Psychology, 14, 231±252.
Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes McCelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values
(pp. 661±753). New York: Academic Press. determine what people do. American Psychologist, 40,
Leitner, M., Shapland, J., & Wiles, P. (1993). Drug usage 812±825.
and drugs prevention: The views and habits of the general McConnaughly, E. A., DiClemente, C. C., Prochaska, J.
public. London: HMSO. O., & Velicer, W. F. (1989). Stages of change in
Lennon, D. L. F., Stratman, F. W., Shrago, E., Nagle, F. psychotherapy: A follow-up report. Psychotherapy, 26,
J., Hanson, P. G., Maddon, M., & Spennetta, T. (1983). 494±503.
Total cholesterol and DHL cholesterol changes during McCusker, J., & Morrow, G. (1979). The relationship of
acute, moderate intensity exercise in men and women. health locus of control to preventive health behaviors
Metabolism, 32, 244±249. and health beliefs. Patient Counselling and Health
Levenson, H. (1974). Multidimensional locus of control in Education, 1, 146±150.
psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical McNeil, A. D., Jarvis, M. J., Stapleton, J. A., Russell, M.
Psychology, 41, 397±404. A. H., Eiser, J. R., Gammage, P., & Gray, E. M. (1988).
Leventhal, H. (1970). Findings and theory in the study of Prospective study of factors predicting uptake of
fear communications. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in smoking in adolescents. Journal of Epidemiology and
experimental social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 119±186). Community Health, 43, 72±78.
New York: Academic Press. Mechanic, D., & Cleary, P. D. (1980). Factors associated
Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D. R., & Steele, D. F. (1984). Illness with the maintenance of positive health behavior.
representations and coping with health threats. In A. Preventive Medicine, 9, 805±814.
Baum & J. Singer (Eds.), A handbook of psychology and Metzner, H. L., Carman W. J., & House, J. (1983). Health
health (pp. 219±252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. practices, risk factors and chronic disease in Tecumeseh.
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Preventive Medicine, 12, 491±507.
Harper. Mittag, W. & Schwarzer, R. (1993). Interaction of
Liao, K. L., Hunter, M., & Weinman, J. (1995). Health- employment status and self-efficacy on alcohol con-
related behaviours and their correlates in a general sumption: A two-wave study on stressful life transitions.
population sample of 45-year old women. Psychology Psychology and Health, 8, 77±87.
and Health, 10, 171±184. Moore, L., Smith, C., & Catford, J. (1994). Binge drinking:
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences prevalence, patterns and policy. Health Education
on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- Research, 9, 497±505.
sion Processes, 65, 272±292. Morrison, D. M., Gillmore, M. R., & Baker, S. A. (1995).
Madden, T. J., Ellen, P. S., & Ajzen, I. (1992). A Determinants of condom use among high-risk hetero-
comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the sexual adults: A test of the theory of reasoned action.
theory of reasoned action. Personality and Social Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 651±676.
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 3±9. Muhlenkamp, A. F., Brown, N. J., & Sands, D. (1985).
Maddux, J. E. (1993). (Ed.) Self-efficacy, adaptation and Determinants of health promotion activities in nursing
adjustment: theory, research and application. New York: clinic clients. Nursing Research, 34, 327±333.
Plenum. Mullen, P. D., Hersey, J. C., & Iverson, D. C. (1987).
34 Health Behavior

Health behavior models compared. Social Science and cervical screening uptake. British Journal of Health
Medicine, 24, 973±981. Psychology, 1, 35±50.
Nemeck, M. A. (1990). Health beliefs and breast self- Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Translating
examination among black women. Health Values, 14, intentions into action. Personality and Social Psychology
41±52. Bulletin, 23, 953±962.
Netemeyer, R. G., Burton, S., & Johnston, M. (1991). A Paisley, C. M., & Sparks, P. (1998). Expectations of
comparison of two models for the prediction of reducing fat intake: The role of perceived need with the
volitional and goal-directed behaviors: A confirmatory theory of planned behaviour. Psychology and Health, 13.
analysis approach. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1995).
87±100. Extending the theory of planned behaviour: The role of
Nicassio, P., Wallston, K., Callahan, L., Herbert, M., & personal norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34,
Pincus, T. (1985). The measurement of helplessness in 127±137.
rheumatoid arthritis: The development of the arthritis Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1996).
helplessness scale. Journal of Rheumatology, 12, 462±467. Modifying beliefs and attitudes to exceed the speed limit:
Norman, P. (1990). Health locus of control beliefs and An intervention study based on the theory of planned
preventive health behaviours. Journal of the Institute of behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1±19.
Health Education, 28, 113±117. Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., Stradling, S. G., Reason, J.
Norman, P. (1991). Social learning theory and the T., & Baxter, J. S. (1992). Intention to commit driving
prediction of attendance at screening. Psychology and violationsÐAn application of the theory of planned
Health, 5, 231±239. behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 94±101.
Norman, P. (1995). Heath locus of control and health Peak, H. (1955). Attitude and motivation. In M. R. Jones
behaviour: an investigation into the role of health value (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 3,
and behaviour-specific efficacy beliefs. Personality and pp. 149±188). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska
Individual Differences, 18, 213±218. Press.
Norman, P., & Bennett, P. (1996). Health locus of control. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and
In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting health persuasion: Central and peripheral routes of attitude
behaviour (pp. 62±94). Buckingham, UK: Open Uni- change. New York: Springer-Verlag.
versity Press. Pomazal, R. P., & Jaccard, J. J. (1976). An informational
Norman, P., & Conner, M. (1993). The role of social approach to altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality
cognition models in predicting attendance at health and Social Psychology, 33, 317±326.
checks. Psychology and Health, 8, 447±462. Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1984). The
Norman, P., & Conner, M. (1996a). The role of social transtheoretical approach: Crossing traditional boundaries
cognition models in predicting health behaviours: future of therapy. Homewood, IL: Dow Jones Irwin.
directions. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting Prwun, J., van der Borne, H., de Reuver, R., de Boer, M.,
health behaviour (pp. 197±225). Buckingham, UK: Open Ter Pelkwijk, M., & de Jong, P. (1988). The locus of
University Press. control scale for cancer patients. Tijdscrift vour Sociale
Norman, P., & Conner, M. (1996b). Predicting health- Gezondherdszong, 66, 404±408.
check attendance among prior attenders and nonatten- Quadrel, M. J. & Lau, R. R. (1989). Health promotion,
ders: The role of prior behavior in the theory of planned health locus of control, and health behavior: Two field
behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, experiments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18,
1010±1026. 1497±1521.
Norman, P., & Smith, L. (1995). The theory of planned Quine, L., Rutter, D. R., & Arnold, L. (1998). Predicting
behaviour and exercise: An investigation into the role of safety helmet use among schoolboy cyclists: A compar-
prior behaviour, behavioural intentions and attitude ison of the theory of planned behavior and the health
variability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, belief model. Psychology and Health, 13.
403±415. Raats, M. M. (1992). The role of beliefs and sensory to milk
Nucifora, J., Gallois, C., & Kashima, Y. (1993). Influences in determining the selection of milks of different fat
on condom use among undergraduates: Testing the content. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of
theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour. In D. Reading.
J. Terry, C. Gallois & M. McCamish (Eds.), The theory Redeker, N. S. (1989). Health beliefs, health locus of
of reasoned action: Its application to AIDS-preventive control, and the frequency of practice of breast self-
behaviour (pp. 47±64). Oxford, UK: Pergamon. examination in women. Journal of Obstetrics, Gynaecol-
Nutbeam, D., & Catford, J. (1990). Modifiable risks for ogy and Neonatal Nursing, 18, 45±51.
cardiovascular disease among general practitioners in Redhead, I. H. (1960). The incidence of glycosuria and
Wales. Public Health, 104, 353±361. diabetes mellitus in general practice. British Medical
Oberman, A. (1985). Exercise and the primary prevention Journal, 1, 695.
of cardiovascular disease. American Journal of Cradiol- Reiss, I. L., & Leik, R. K. (1989). Evaluating strategies to
ogy, 55, 10D±20D. avoid AIDS: Numbers of partners versus use of
O'Connell, J. K., & Price, J. H. (1982). Health locus of condoms. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 411±433.
control of physical fitness program participants. Percep- Richard, R., & van der Pligt, J. (1991). Factors affecting
tual and Motor Skills, 55, 925±926. condom use among adolescents. Journal of Community
O'Leary, A. (1985). Self-efficacy and health. Behavioral and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 105±116.
Research and Therapy, 23, 437±451. Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1995).
O'Leary, A., & Raffaelli, M. (1996). Preventing the sexual Anticipated affective reactions and prevention of AIDS.
transmission of HIV: Current status and future direc- British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 9±21.
tions. Psychology and Health, 11, 75±94. Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1996a).
Office of Population Census and Surveys (1968). Drug use Anticipated affect and behavioral choice. Basic and
in the UK. London: Government Statistical Services. Applied Social Psychology, 18, 111±129.
Oliver, R. L. & Berger, P. K. (1979). A path analysis of Richard, R., Van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1996b).
preventive health care decision models. Journal of Anticipated regret and time perspective: Changing sexual
Consumer Research, 6, 113±122. risk-taking behavior. Journal of Behavioral Decision
Orbell, S., Crombie, I., & Johnson, G. (1996). Social Making, 9, 185±199.
cognition and social structure in the prediction of Rigotti, N. A. (1989). Cigarette smoking and body weight.
References 35

New England Journal of Medicine, 320, 931±933. overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and
Rippetoe, P. A., & Rogers, R. W. (1987). Effects of Research, 16, 201±228.
components of protection motivation theory on adaptive Schifter, D. E., & Ajzen, I. (1985). Intention, perceived
and maladpative coping with a health threat. Journal of control and weight loss: An application of the theory of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 596±604. planned behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social
Rodin, J., & Salovey, P. (1989). Health psychology. Annual Psychology, 49, 843±851.
Review of Psychology, 40, 533±579. Schlegel, R. P., D'Avernas, J. R., Zanna, M. P., DeCour-
Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological ville, N. H., & Mansue, S. R. (1992). Problem drinking:
processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised A problem for the theory of reasoned action? Journal of
theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. Applied Social Psychology, 22, 358±385.
E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: a source book Schwartz, S. H., & Tessler, R. C. (1972). A test of a model
(pp. 153±176). New York: Guilford Press. for reducing measured attitude-behavior discrepancies.
Ronis, D. L., & Harel, Y. (1989). Health beliefs and breast Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24,
self-examination behaviours: Analysis of linear structur- 225±236.
al relations. Psychology and Health, 3, 259±285. Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy in the adoption and
Ronis, D. L., Yates, J. F., & Kirscht, J. P. (1989). maintenance of health behaviors: Theoretical ap-
Attitudes, decisions and habits as determinants of proaches and a new model. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.),
repeated behavior. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & Self-efficacy: thought control of action (pp. 217±243).
A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure and function London: Hemisphere.
(pp. 213±239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schwarzer, R. (1994). Optimism, vulnerability, and self-
Rosen, T. J., & Shipley, R. H. (1983). A stage analysis of beliefs as health-related cognitions: a systematic over-
self-initiated smoking reductions. Addictive Behaviors, 8, view. Psychology and Health, 9, 161±180.
263±272. Schwarzer, R., & Fuchs, R. (1996). Self-efficacy and health
Rosenstock, I. M. (1966). Why people use health services. behaviour. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predict-
Millbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 44, 94±124. ing health behaviour (pp. 163±196). Buckingham, UK:
Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health Open University Press.
belief model. Health Education Monographs, 2, 1±8. Seeman, M., & Seeman, T. E. (1983). Health behaviour and
Rosenstock, I. M., Strecher, V. J., & Becker, H. M. (1988). personal autonomy: A longitudinal study of the sense of
Social learning theory and the health belief model. control in illness. Journal of Health and Social Behaviour,
Health Education Quarterly, 15, 175±183. 24, 144±160.
Rosenthal, M., Haskell, W. L., Solomon, R., Widstrom, Segall, M. E., & Wynd, C. A. (1990). Health conception,
A., & Reavan, G. M. (1983). Demonstration of a health locus of control, and power as predictors of
relationship between level of physical training and smoking behavior change. American Journal of Health
insulin stimulated glucose utilization in normal humans. Promotion, 4, 338±344.
Diabetes, 32, 408±411. Seydel, E., Taal, E., & Wiegman, O. (1990). Risk-appraisal,
Roskam, S. (1986). Application of health locus of control outcome and self-efficacy expectancies: Cognitive factors
typology toward predicting depression and medical ad- in preventive behaviour related to cancer. Psychology
herence in rheumatoid arthritis. Unpublished doctoral and Health, 4, 99±109.
dissertation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN. Shaper, A. G., Pocock, S. J., Walker, M., Cohen, N. M.,
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Wale, C. J., & Thomson, A. G. (1981). British Regional
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Heart Survey: Cardiovascular risk factors in middle-aged
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal men in 24 towns. British Medical Journal, 283, 179±186.
and external control of reinforcement. Psychological Sheeran, P., & Abraham, C. (1996). The health belief
Monographs: General and Applied, 80 (whole no. 609), model. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting
1±28. health behaviour (pp. 23±61). Buckingham, UK: Open
Royal College of Physicians (1983). Health or smoking: University Press.
Follow-up report. London: Pitman. Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988).
Rutter, D. R., & Quine, L. (1996). Social psychological The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past
mediators of the relationship between demographic research with recommendations for modifications and
factors and health outcomes: A theoretical model future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15,
and some preliminary data. Psychology and Health, 11, 325±343.
5±22. Sherlock, S. (1982). (Ed.) Alcohol and Disease. British
Sachs, M. L. (1984). Psychological well-being and vigorous Medical Bulletin, 38 (whole issue).
activity. In J. M. Silva & R. S. Weinberg (Eds.), Simonson, I. (1992). The influence of anticipating regret
Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 435±444). Cham- and responsibility on purchase decisions. Journal of
paign, IL: Human Kinetics. Consumer Research, 19, 105±118.
Sallis, J. F., Howell, M. F., Hofstetter, C. R., & Singer, R. N. (1992). Physical activity and psychological
Barrington, E. (1992). Explanation of vigorous physical benefits: A position statement. The Sport Psychologist, 6,
activity during two years using social learning variables. 199±203.
Social Science and Medicine, 34, 25±32. Slenker, S. E., Price, J. H., & O'Connell, J. K. (1985).
Saltzer, E. B. (1982). The weight locus of control (WLOC) Health locus of control of joggers and nonexercisers.
scale: A specific measure for obesity research. Journal of Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61, 323±328.
Personality Assessment, 46, 620±628. Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran, S. A.,
Sampos, C., Fulwood, R., Haines, C., Carroll, M., Anda, Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T., Yoshinobu, L., Gibb., J.,
R., Williamson, D. F., Remington, P., & Cleeman, J. Langelle, C., & Harney, P. (1991). The will and the ways:
(1989). The prevalence of high blood cholesterol levels Development and validation of an individual-differences
among adults in the United States. Journal of the measure of hope. Journal of Personality and Social
American Medical Association, 262, 45±52. Psychology, 60, 570±585.
Sandman, P. M., & Weinstein, N. D. (1993). Predictors of Sparks, P. (1994). Food choice and health: Applying,
home radon testing and implications for testing promo- assessing, and extending the theory of planned beha-
tion programs. Health Education Quarterly, 20, 471±487. viour. In D. R. Rutter & L. Quine (Eds.), Social
Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism psychology and health: European perspectives (pp. 25±
on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical 46). Aldershot, UK: Avebury.
36 Health Behavior

Sparks, P., & Shepherd, R. (1992). Self-identity and the Taylor, S. E. (1991). Health psychology. New York:
theory of planned behaviour: Assessing the role of McGraw-Hill.
identification with ªgreen consumerismº. Social Psychol- Terry, D. J., & O'Leary, J. E. (1995). The theory of planned
ogy Quarterly, 55, 388±399. behaviour: The effects of perceived behavioural control
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., & Frewer, L. J. (1995). Assessing and self-efficacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34,
and structuring attitudes toward the use of gene 199±220.
technology in food productionÐThe role of perceived Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1996). Group norms and the
ethical obligation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, attitude-behavior relationship: A role for group identi-
16, 267±285. fication. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., Wieringa, N., & Zimmermanns, 776±793.
N. (1994). Barriers to healthy eating: An examination of Theodorakis, Y. (1994). Planned behavior, attitude
perceived behavioural control and unrealistic optimism. strength, role identity, and the prediction of exercise
Paper presented at the BPS Social Psychology Section behavior. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 149±165.
Annual Conference, University of Hertfordshire, UK, Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey,
September 1994. CA: Brooks-Cole.
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., Wieringa, N., & Zimmermanns, US Department of Health and Human Services (1988). The
N. (1995). Perceived behavioural control, unrealistic Surgeon General's report on nutrition and health. Wa-
optimism and dietary change: An exploratory study. shington, DC: US Government Printing Office.
Appetite, 24, 243±355. US Department of Health and Human Services (1990).
Speake, D. L., Cowart, M. E., & Stephens, R. (1991). Health people 2000: National health promotion and
Healthy lifestyle practices of rural and urban elderly. disease prevention objectives. Washington, DC: US
Health Values, 15, 45±51. Government Printing Office.
Stacy, R. D., & Lloyd, B. H. (1990). An investigation of Valois, P., Desharnais, R., & Godin, G. (1988). A
beliefs about smoking among diabetes patients: Informa- comparison of the Fishbein and the Triandis attitudinal
tion for improving cessation efforts. Journal of Patient models for the prediction of exercise intention and
Education and Counselling, 15, 181±189. behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11, 459±472.
Stainback, R. D., & Rogers, R. W. (1983). Identifying Van den Putte, H. (1993). On the theory of reasoned action.
effective components of alcohol abuse prevention pro- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Am-
grams: effects of fear appeals, message style and source sterdam.
expertise. International Journal of Addictions, 18, Van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1998). Expectancy-value
393±405. models of health behaviours: The role of salience and
Stanley, M. A. & Maddux, J. E. (1986). Cognitive processes anticipated affect. Psychology and Health, 13.
in health enhancement: Investigation of a combined Van der Pligt, J., & Eiser, J. R. (1984) Dimensional
protection motivation and self-efficacy model. Basic and salience, judgement and attitudes. In J. R. Eiser (Ed.),
Applied Social Psychology, 7, 101±113. Attitudinal judgement (pp. 43±63). New York: Springer-
Stanton, A. (1987). Determinance of adherence to medical Verlag.
regimens by hypertensive patients. Journal of Behavioural Van der Velde, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1991). AIDS-related
Medicine, 10, 377±394. behavior: Coping, protection motivation, and previous
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1992). Cognitive predictors of behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 14, 429±451.
health behaviour in contrasting regions of Europe. Vernon, S. W., Laville, E. A., & Jackson, G. L. (1990).
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 485±502. Participation in breast screening programs: a review.
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1996). The European health and Social Science and Medicine, 30, 1107±1118.
behaviour survey: The development of an international Waldron, I. (1988). Why do women live longer than men?
study in health psychology. Psychology and Health, 11, Journal of Human Stress, 2, 2±13.
49±73. Waller, K. V., & Bates, R. C. (1992). Health locus of
Steptoe, A., Wardle, J., Vinck, J., Tuomisto, M., Holte, A., control and self-efficacy beliefs in a healthy elderly
& Wickstrom, L. (1994). Personality and attitudinal sample. American Journal of Health Promotion, 6,
correlates of healthy and unhealthy lifestyles in young 302±309.
adults. Psychology and Health, 9, 331±343. Wallston, B. S., Wallston, K. A., Kaplan, G. D., & Maides,
Stevens, V. J. & Hollis, J. F. (1989). Preventing smoking S. A. (1976). Development and validation of the health
relapse, using an individually tailored skills-training locus of control (HLC) scale. Journal of Consulting and
technique. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Clinical Psychology, 44, 580±585.
57, 420±424. Wallston, K. A. (1991). The importance of placing
Strax, P. (1984). Mass screening for control of breast measures of health locus of control beliefs in a theoretical
cancer. Cancer, 53, 665±670. context. Health Education Research: Theory and Prac-
Stroebe, W., & Stroebe, M. S. (1995). Social psychology and tice, 6, 251±252.
health. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press. Wallston, K. A. (1992). Hocus-pocus, the focus isn't strictly
Sutton, S. (1982). Fear-arousing communications: A on locus: Rotter's social learning theory modified for
critical examination of theory and research. In J. R. health. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 183±199.
Eiser (Ed.), Social psychology and behavioural medicine Wallston, K. A., Maiders, S., & Wallston, B. S. (1976).
(pp. 303±337). London: Wiley. Health-related information seeking as a function of
Sutton, S. (1994). The past predicts the future: Interpreting health-related locus of control and health value. Journal
behaviour±behaviour relationships in social psychologi- of Research in Personality, 10, 215±222.
cal models of health behaviours. In D. R. Rutter & L. Wallston, K. A., & Wallston, B. S. (1980). Health locus of
Quine (Eds.), Social psychology and health: European control scales. In H. Lefcourt (Ed.), Advances and
perspectives (pp. 71±88). Aldershot, UK: Avebury. innovations in locus of control research (pp. 198±234).
Sutton, S., & Hallett, R. (1989). Understanding seat-belt New York: Academic Press.
intentions and behavior: A decision-making approach. Wallston, K. A., & Wallston, B. S. (1981). Health locus of
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 1310±1325. control scales. In H. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the
Taylor, D. W. (1979). A test of the health belief model in locus of control construct (Vol. 1, pp. 189±243). New
hypertension. In R. B. Haynes, D. W. Taylor, & K. L. York: Academic Press.
Sackett (Eds.), Compliance in health care (pp. 103±109). Wallston, K. A., & Wallston, B. S. (1982). Who is
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. responsible for your health? The construct of health
References 37

locus of control. In G. Sanders & J. Suls (Eds.), Social behaviours. Social Behavior and Personality, 18,
psychology of health and illness (pp. 65±95). Hillsdale, 121±135.
NJ: Erlbaum. Wilson, D., Zenda, A., McMaster, J., & Lavelle, S. (1992).
Wallston, K. A., Wallston, B. S., & DeVellis, R. (1978). Factors predicting Zimbabwean students' intentions to
Development of multidimensional health locus of con- use condoms. Psychology and Health, 7, 99±114.
trol (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs, 6, Winett, R. A. (1985). Ecobehavioral assessment in health
160±170. life-styles: Concepts and methods. In P. Karoly (Ed.),
Wardle, J. (1995). Cholesterol and psychological well- Measurement strategies in health psychology (pp. 147±
being. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 39, 549±562. 181). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Warshaw, P. R., & Davis, F. D. (1985). Disentangling Wojcik, J. V. (1988). Social learning predictors of the
behavioral intention and behavioral expectation. Journal avoidance of smoking relapse. Addictive Behaviors, 13,
of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 213±228. 177±180.
Weatherburn, P., Hunt, A. J., Davies, P. M., Coxon, A. P. Wurtele, S. K. (1988). Increasing women's calcium intake:
M., & McManus, T. J. (1991). Condom use in a large the role of health beliefs, intentions and health value.
cohort of homosexually active men in England and Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 627±639.
Wales. AIDS Care, 3, 31±41. Wurtele, S. K., Britcher, J. C., & Saslawsky, D. A. (1985).
Weinstein, N. D. (1988). The precaution adoption process. Relationships between locus of control, health value and
Health Psychology, 7, 355±386. preventive health behaviors among women. Journal of
Weinstein, N. D. & Nicolich, M. (1983). Correct and Research in Personality, 19, 271±278.
incorrect interpretations of correlations between risk Wurtele, S. K., & Maddux, J. E. (1987). Relative
perceptions and risk behaviors. Health Psychology, 12, contributions of protection motivation components in
235±245. predicting exercise intentions and behavior. Health
Weinstein, N. D., Rothman, A. J., & Sutton, S. R. (in Psychology, 6, 453±466.
press). Stage theories of health behavior. Health Young, H. M., Lierman, L., Powell-Cope, G., Kasprzyk,
Psychology. D., & Bonoliel, J. Q. (1991). Operationalizing the theory
Weinstein, N. D., & Sandman, P. M. (1992). A model of of planned behaviour. Research in Nursing and Health,
the precaution adoption process: evidence from home 14, 137±144.
radon testing. Health Psychology, 11, 170±180. Zuckerman, M., & Reiss, H. T. (1978). Comparison of
Weiss, G. L., & Larsen, D. L. (1990). Health value, health three models for predicting altruistic behavior. Journal of
locus of control, and the prediction of health protective Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 498±510.

You might also like