Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

r Academy of Management Journal

2017, Vol. 60, No. 3, 1071–1093.


https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.1040

THE MEANING OF MY FEELINGS DEPENDS ON WHO I AM:


WORK-RELATED IDENTIFICATIONS SHAPE EMOTION
EFFECTS IN ORGANIZATIONS
SAMANTHA A. CONROY
Colorado State University

WILLIAM J. BECKER
Virginia Tech

JOCHEN I. MENGES
WHU – Otto Beisheim School of Management

Theory and research on affect in organizations has mostly approached emotions from
a valence perspective, suggesting that positive emotions lead to positive outcomes and
negative emotions to negative outcomes for organizations. We propose that cognition
resulting from emotional experiences at work cannot be assumed based on emotion
valence alone. Instead, building on appraisal theory and social identity theory, we
propose that individual responses to discrete emotions in organizations are shaped by,
and thus depend on, work-related identifications. We elaborate on this proposition
specifically with respect to turnover intentions, theorizing how three discrete
emotions—anger, guilt, and pride—differentially affect turnover intentions, depending
on two work-related identifications: organizational and occupational. A longitudinal
study involving 135 pilot instructors reporting emotions, work-related identifications,
and turnover intentions over the course of one year provides general support for our
proposition. Our theory and findings advance emotion and identity theories by
explaining how the effects of emotions are dependent on the psychological context in
which they are experienced.

The ongoing “affective revolution” has produced employee behavior (Butt & Choi, 2006; Butts, Becker,
greater appreciation for the importance of emotions & Boswell, 2015; Lench, Flores, & Bench, 2011; Scott
in understanding organizational behavior (Barsade, & Judge, 2006; Weiss, Suckow, & Cropanzano, 1999).
Brief, & Spataro, 2003: 3). Research in this area has Despite these advances, understanding about how
focused primarily on positive and negative affect emotions affect outcomes in organizations is still
(i.e., pleasant and unpleasant emotions or moods), underdeveloped. There is little knowledge con-
and has been guided by the “symmetrical assump- cerning cases when the same discrete emotion has
tion” that employees’ positive feelings are beneficial different effects, depending on the circumstances.
and negative feelings are detrimental for organiza- Yet, recent work points to this possibility. For ex-
tions (Lindebaum & Jordan, 2012: 1027). More re- ample, research on the discrete emotion of envy
cently, there has been growing recognition that theorizes that it can motivate individuals to improve
organizational life is rife with more specific discrete their own efforts, to help others, or to sabotage the
emotions (e.g., pride, anger, guilt), and that each efforts of others (Tai, Narayanan, & McAllister, 2012;
discrete emotion has unique implications for van de Ven, Zeelenberg, & Pieters, 2009). A fur-
ther complexity is the possibility that different dis-
crete emotions, even those with opposite valence
We would like to gratefully acknowledge our action
(i.e., positive and negative), may have similar effects
editor, Brent Scott, and three anonymous reviewers whose
feedback improved our work immensely. We would also under some conditions. For example, negative feel-
like to thank Laura Little, Dan Ganster, and Lynn Shore for ings have been found to increase absenteeism while
providing feedback on prior drafts of this paper. This re- positive feelings have been found to decrease ab-
search was also supported by the TCU Neeley School of senteeism (Pelled & Xin, 1999)—yet, it has also been
Business research committee. argued that positive feelings may, in some cases
1071
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express
written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
1072 Academy of Management Journal June

(i.e., when experienced in nonwork contexts), lead year, depending on employees’ work-related identi-
people to be absent from work as a mood control fication levels.
mechanism (George, 1989: 322). Taken together, Our research offers three main contributions to the
there are indications that responses to discrete management literature. First, we develop boundary
emotions are not as simple as previously thought; conditions around the effects of discrete emotions on
theory based on the symmetrical assumption cannot outcomes important to organizations. Based on the
accommodate such complexities. importance of identifications in meaning-making
To advance understanding of how emotions un- processes (Weick, Sutcliffe, & Obstfeld, 2005), we
fold at work and influence organizationally relevant integrate social identity theory with emotion ap-
outcomes, we examine the psychological context praisal theory to identify these boundaries and clar-
within which emotions develop and transform into ify the process of emotion appraisals at work. Prior
cognitive responses. We draw on and integrate two work has addressed the importance of studying dis-
major theories: emotion appraisal theory (Lazarus, crete emotions to move beyond the valence-only
1991a), which provides a general process account of (i.e., positive and negative) treatment of emotion
how discrete emotions and response tendencies (e.g., Butts et al., 2015; Lench et al., 2011; Scott &
emerge from individuals’ interpretations of events, Judge, 2006). We take this further by theorizing the
and social identity theory (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; conditions under which the same discrete emotion
Tajfel & Turner, 1985), which explains how in- can produce different responses and can push the
dividuals’ self-definitions (i.e., identities) relate to employee to either engage with or disengage from the
group membership and influence individual needs, workplace. For example, we predict and empirically
beliefs, and behaviors. We propose that the appraisal show that anger can in some cases—when organi-
process elicited by work events is shaped by the zational identification is high—decrease turnover
psychological context that employees’ social iden- intentions, and in other cases—when organizational
tities provide. identification is low—increase turnover intentions.
Self-definitions are a critical contextual factor for An uncontextualized approach to discrete emotions
emotions, given their role in individual motives and (i.e., one that specifies a concrete action tendency for
sense-making processes (Lazarus, 1991a). Indeed, in a discrete emotion; Frijda, Kuipers, & ter Schure,
his influential work on emotions, the psychologist 1989) would not be able to account for this pattern of
Richard S. Lazarus noted that identity or self “is effects.
involved in every emotion” (Lazarus, 1991a: 100). Second, we move beyond the assumptions of
Moreover, research suggests that social identifications emotion–outcome symmetry. We provide evidence
have “orienting effects,” influencing how events are that positive and negative discrete emotions can lead
interpreted and guiding action (Hekman, Steensma, to similar outcomes. For example, we theorize and
Bigley, & Hereford, 2009: 1327). We apply these ideas find that anger and pride are positively related to
to appraisal theory to explain how employees’ work- turnover intentions when organizational identifica-
related identifications influence the relationship tion is low and negatively related to turnover in-
between three discrete emotions—anger, guilt, and tentions when organizational identification is high.
pride—and turnover intentions. A symmetrical assumption about emotions could not
Two group memberships carry particular impor- explain such a finding (Lindebaum & Jordan, 2012).
tance for employees’ social identities: (1) the mem- Third, by integrating appraisal theory with social
bership with the organization for which they work, identity theory, we are able to explain why emotions,
and (2) the membership with the occupational group at times, have asymmetric effects in organizations
to which they belong (Gouldner, 1957). We suggest and demonstrate the power of social identifications
that the extent to which employees identify with in transforming responses to emotions. Specifically,
their organization or their occupation (or with both we suggest that emotion appraisals are influenced by
or neither) will affect how affective events are ap- the need to protect identity-based motives, and, re-
praised, and, in turn, how the resulting emotion will latedly, the meaning of emotion can be transformed
affect employees’ turnover intentions. We test our by identifications. Our findings suggest that both
theory in a flight training and instruction firm with organizational and occupational identifications
an experience sampling study. Specifically, we as- psychologically contextualize responses to work
sess how within-person variations in anger, guilt, emotions. For example, although guilt is tradition-
and pride triggered by job and organizational events ally thought to engage individuals with the target of
relate to turnover intentions over a period of one their guilt (Kitayama, Mesquita, & Karasawa, 2006),
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1073

we find that guilt reduces turnover intentions only while experiences that threaten or pull one further
under certain conditions. from one’s goals invoke negative emotions. “Goal
In sum, this research answers calls from emotion content” (or ego involvement) is the appraisal of the
researchers to dive deeper into our understand- event’s specific relevance to the individual’s goals,
ing of discrete emotions, remove assumptions of though the type of emotion to be experienced as well
emotion–outcome symmetry, and better contextu- as the consequent appropriate response is yet to be
alize the experience of emotion at work (Gooty, determined (but is determined as part of the sec-
Gavin, & Ashkanasy, 2009; Lindebaum & Jordan, ondary appraisal). For example, if an event is ap-
2012). Although, experientially, positive emotions praised to have goal content involving the self- or
are pleasant and negative emotions unpleasant, our social esteem part of the identity, the emotion pos-
theory and findings suggest that their respective sibilities narrow to a set of emotions (i.e., anger,
effects for organizations may not follow the same anxiety, and pride) rather than to a specific emotion.
pattern. The primary appraisal process is important be-
cause it assesses whether an event is relevant to the
person–environment relationship and whether a re-
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND sponse is necessary. However, the primary appraisal
HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT points mainly to the valence and intensity of the
emotion. As such, predictions made regarding basic
Appraisal Theory
positive and negative emotions address responses to
Appraisal theory concerns the process and content primary appraisals. Primary appraisals are immedi-
of emotions, and suggests that emotions result from ate and automatic, and appraisal theory posits that
appraisals of the person–environment relationship emotional experiences quickly evoke secondary ap-
(Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003; Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, praisals as individuals seek to interpret and respond
1991a; Roseman & Smith, 2001). Appraisals are eli- to the emotion-evoking event (Lazarus, 1991a).
cited by affective events––significant incidents that Secondary appraisals determine the discrete
signal or precipitate change relevant to the person’s emotion and the appropriate forms of coping with
well-being (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Multiple the emotion through three considerations (Lazarus,
appraisal frameworks have been proposed, but ap- 1991a). One is “blame or credit”—to what extent
praisal criteria tend to be similar across frameworks should blame (in the case of negative emotions) or
(Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer, & Frijda, 2013). Our should credit (in the case of positive emotions) be
theory is based on Lazarus’s (1991a) appraisal directed at the self, at another, or be non-directed.
framework, as it includes generally accepted ap- The other two secondary appraisals are “coping
praisal criteria, often references identity, self, and potential”––beliefs that one can influence the envi-
ego, and provides a thorough account of emotion ronment for change––and “future expectations”––
responses. beliefs that things will be favorable or unfavorable
Lazarus’s (1991a) appraisal framework splits the going forward.
appraisal process into primary and secondary ap- An important distinction to be made about ap-
praisals. Primary appraisal relates to the intensity praisal is that it is different from knowledge (Lazarus,
and nature of the emotion through appraisals of 1991a). “Knowledge” is an understanding of the way
goal relevance, goal congruence, and goal content. the world works in general and in specific encoun-
“Goals,” also called motives, needs, or concerns, ters, whereas “appraisal” is the personal significance
are defined as anything important to an indi- of what is known or believed about such encounters.
vidual (Moors et al., 2013). Goals can be narrow Specific encounters provide knowledge, which is
(e.g., feedback provided on a project) and broad appraised based on “generalized patterns (cognitive
(e.g., what such feedback means about one’s poten- and motivational traits) that transcend a specific
tial) (Lazarus, 1991a). Emotions are experienced if an encounter but that may be regarded by a person as
event is viewed as “goal relevant” (i.e., a goal is at relevant to it” (Lazarus, 1991a: 140). We propose that
stake), and the intensity of the emotion is related to one’s identifications with social groups influence the
the distance between one’s experience and one’s specific nature of secondary appraisals because
goals. “Goal congruence” pertains to whether the identifications produce a set of innate motive pat-
encounter brings one closer to one’s goals or further terns relevant to personal significance. For example,
away from them. Experiences appraised as bringing identifications can have an influence on beliefs
one closer to one’s goals invoke positive emotions, about whom or what we define as part of our self,
1074 Academy of Management Journal June

whether coping within the work environment can (Cooper & Thatcher, 2010; Shamir, 1991). Self-
produce change, and whether similar experiences consistency motives involve the need to think, feel,
will continue to occur in the future within the work and behave in ways consistent with one’s self-
environment. definition (Shamir, 1991). “Self-enhancement mo-
tives” are driven by the need to feel positively about
oneself (Ashforth, 2001; Ashforth et al., 2008; Hogg,
Responses to Emotion at Work
Terry, & White, 1995; Tajfel & Turner, 1985). Ful-
Because emotion provides important information filling self-enhancement needs through social iden-
about goal pursuit, there is an immutable impetus to tification requires that the social group with which
respond to emotion. Some scholars have focused on one identifies be held in high regard (Ashforth &
action tendency responses (i.e., the immediate urge Kreiner, 1999; Ellemers, Van Rijswijk, Roefs, &
to act upon emotion; Frijda, 1986). Others have Simons, 1997; Elsbach & Kramer, 1996). Because
treated responses as “coping,” described by Lazarus these identity motives provide the psychological
as “the psychological analogue of action tendencies context within which emotions are experienced, we
more psychological, complex, deliberate, and plan- suggest that they hold important implications for
ful” (Lazarus, 1991b: 830). Lazarus (1991b) proposed how secondary appraisals and coping responses
coping as a critical outcome of emotion, in particular unfold.
for negative emotions. Positive emotions have action
tendencies (Lazarus, 1991a) and influence cognition
Discrete Emotions
as well (Fredrickson, 1998), primarily in the form of
a desire to continue or enhance the positive emotion One challenge of studying discrete emotions is
experience, though these effects may not be as strong selecting a limited set of emotions to consider. The
or differentiated as for negative emotions. As such, selection process, in light of multiple discrete emo-
emotions always produce some response, cognitive tion frameworks and over 100 descriptive feeling
or behavioral or both. words indicating discrete emotions (Izard, 2009;
Emotion responses can be engaging or disengag- Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987), must
ing, meaning that the emotion affirms either “in- be guided by clear criteria. Therefore, we established
terdependence of the self” or “independence of the four such criteria. First, we chose emotions that are
self” (Kitayama et al., 2006: 891). In the context of commonly experienced in work environments; spe-
organizations, turnover intentions reflect the extent cifically, achievement emotions (i.e., “emotions tied
to which employees seek to engage with or disengage directly to achievement activities or achievement
from their organization. Thus, turnover intentions outcomes”; Pekrun, 2006: 317; Perry, Stupnisky,
are a relevant response to emotions experienced Hall, Chipperfield, & Weiner, 2010) that are likely to
at work. When emotions affirm interdependence occur in organizations where individuals are pur-
within the organization environment, turnover in- suing goals at multiple levels (e.g., goals for oneself,
tentions are expected to decrease, but, when emo- goals for the organization). Second, we chose emo-
tions affirm independence, turnover intentions are tions likely to facilitate engagement and disengage-
expected to increase. ment responses relevant to turnover intentions.
The extent to which a discrete emotion engages or Third, we chose emotions that were different expe-
disengages an individual with work-related groups, rientially such that individuals would be able to
and thus leads to a decrease or increase in turn- readily discern between them. Fourth, we chose
over intentions, is likely to be influenced by an in- to investigate both negative and positive discrete
dividual’s motives and ego structure. Identification emotions.
is an ideal contextual variable, given its central role Based on these criteria, we chose to examine an-
in thoughts and behaviors through identity motives ger, guilt, and pride. Each of these emotions are
(Ashforth, 2001; Ashforth, Harrison, & Corley, 2008). frequently experienced in organizational contexts
Identity motives represent the needs, goals, or con- (Basch & Fisher, 2000; Lazarus & Cohen-Charash,
cerns associated with self-definition. Two of the 2001) and have been identified as achievement
most studied and accepted identity motives are self- oriented (Pekrun, 2006; Perry et al., 2010). Anger,
consistency and self-enhancement (Ashforth et al., guilt, and pride also have responses pertinent to
2008; Cooper & Thatcher, 2010; Shamir, 1991). “Self- organizational exit (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009;
consistency” refers to the need to view oneself as Kitayama et al., 2006; Schmader & Lickel, 2006;
consistent across time and across facets of the self Shaver et al., 1987). These three emotions are unique
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1075

in their appraisals (Lazarus & Cohen-Charash, 2001) effects of negative emotions. Staying with the orga-
and readily distinguishable from one another nization fulfills the self-consistency motive; there-
(Shaver et al., 1987). These emotions have also been fore, to the extent that individuals identify with the
the focus of prior organizational research on dis- organization, they should be less likely to respond to
crete emotions (Butt & Choi, 2006; Weiss et al., negative feelings by disengaging. One cannot easily
1999). Anger and guilt are considered negative go elsewhere and find an organization with the same
emotions, whereas pride is seen as a positive emo- organizational identity. Leaving the organization for
tion. It is notable that positive emotions are less those highly identified is likely to lead to a strong
differentiated in comparison to negative emotions sense of loss (Conroy & O’Leary-Kelly, 2014), as the
(Dasborough, 2006; Ellsworth & Smith, 1988; individual’s work-related identity features a spe-
Fredrickson, 1998), and few positive emotions have cific, non-transferrable component that is tied to an
the same specificity in appraisals and responses organization. This suggests that the positive re-
(Fredrickson, 1998). As such, we chose one positive lationship between negative emotions and turnover
emotion: pride, an emotion with clear appraisal intentions as a means of coping with undesirable
characteristics, achievement orientation, and a high feelings will be weakened as organizational identi-
level of ego relevance (Lewis, 2008; Roseman, fication increases. However, the effect of identifica-
Antoniou, & Jose, 1996). Thus, we develop hy- tion may act more fundamentally to the emotion
potheses regarding the relationship between anger, experience than a simple buffering effect, and as-
guilt, and pride and turnover intentions depending pects of secondary appraisal (i.e., blame/credit,
on work-related identifications. coping potential, future expectations) speak to this
effect.
The blame aspect of secondary appraisal for anger
Organizational Identification
involves the intentional actions of another to block
As noted, identifications are important to meaning- one’s goals (Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1991a; Ortony,
making processes likely to be engaged when emo- Clore, & Collins, 1988). Self-definitions influence
tions are experienced. Research indicates that who or what is defined as “other.” For an individual
“organizational identification”—that is, the overlap highly identified with the organization, anger directed
of an individual’s self-definition with an organiza- toward the organization is similar to self-blame,
tion’s central characteristics (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; because the organization is part of one’s self-
Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994)—is particularly definition. Self-blame is inconsistent with the feeling
relevant to one’s interpretations of and responses to of anger (Roseman et al., 1996). It is also inconsistent
affective events at work (Hekman, Bigley, Steensma, & with self-enhancement motives, because it suggests
Hereford, 2009; Hekman, Steensma et al., 2009). the organization has done something wrong, thus
Furthermore, organizational identification is one of lowering the regard for the organization. As a result,
the most commonly studied work-related identifica- high organizational identification should bias in-
tions and has an empirical relationship with turnover dividuals away from attributing responsibility for
intentions (Abrams, Ando, & Hinkle, 1998). Given the anger to the organizational in-group, and divert at-
centrality of organizational identification to both tention toward an ostensibly blameworthy out-group
turnover intentions and event interpretation, we de- target (e.g., customers, competitors). As an example,
velop our hypotheses by starting with examining how last-minute scheduling changes may anger an em-
it affects the relationship between each of the three ployee because they prevent that person from going
emotions and turnover intentions. to a personal gathering. The target of blame can vary,
The primary appraisal for anger is that an event is though, even given the same anger-invoking event. If
goal relevant and goal incongruent, meaning that organizational identification is low, blaming the or-
anger is experienced as a negative emotion. In pre- ganization for irresponsible scheduling is a natural
vious studies, higher intensity negative emotions response. However, if organizational identification
have been associated with increased turnover in- is high, blame could be turned toward clients who
tentions (Cropanzano, James, & Konovsky, 1993; did not communicate needs properly or toward co-
Maertz & Griffeth, 2004), because leaving the orga- workers who called in sick.
nization allows the employee to avoid the discomfort Anger is also associated with “coping potential”
of the negative emotion (Maertz & Griffeth, 2004). appraisals that action will be beneficial and “fu-
When organizational identification is high, self- ture expectancy” appraisals that are favorable
consistency motives should buffer the undesirable (Lazarus, 1991a). For those highly identified with
1076 Academy of Management Journal June

the organization, anger should be engaging (as op- 355). Thus, despite being a negative emotion, guilt
posed to distancing) with the firm, because the or- may bring an individual closer to the organization.
ganization is viewed with a positive bias, likely to Whether the guilt response is a stronger bond with
provide coping support and favorable future expe- the organization or a greater distance from it depends
riences. To continue with the aforementioned ex- on the nature of coping potential and future expec-
ample, the organizational scheduling event, the tations secondary appraisals. Lazarus (1991a) de-
individual may think, “The firm will correct the scribed apologies (a form of engagement at work) as
customer problems that led to this problem.” Pro- most likely to occur when guilty individuals believe
viding further evidence that anger engages the indi- the apology will rectify their mistakes (i.e., coping
vidual, given high identification, research has potential is high) and when this belief is accompa-
shown that conflict directed against one’s group en- nied by expectations that such mistakes can be
courages group solidarity and commitment (Tajfel, avoided in the future (i.e., future expectations are
1982; Tajfel & Turner, 1985). Poor treatment of the positive). Individuals with high organizational
group by an external source strengthens the impor- identification, given their positive bias toward the
tance and value assigned to the group among those organization and their motivation to stay in the or-
who identify with the group (Ashforth & Kreiner, ganization’s good graces, are expected to have sec-
1999; Branscombe, Schmitt, & Harvey, 1999; Leach, ondary appraisals in favor of remaining in the
Mosquera, Vliek, & Hirt, 2010). In the context of organization. For example, an employee may feel
work, the bias created by high organizational iden- guilty because of a mistake made with a client at
tification should divert blame from the organization work. If the employee is highly identified, a need to
and, in turn, engage the individual with his or her correct the mistake to ensure the organization is not
organization in the face of an anger-evoking event. perceived poorly is expected. Beliefs that the orga-
Thus, we propose that, when organizational identi- nization will support reconciliation attempts and
fication is high, anger will decrease turnover in- will provide an environment conducive to pre-
tentions. We note that these effects are likely to be venting future mistakes will be high, facilitating
stronger for some events than others. In particular, reconciliation.
when events leave room for interpretation, these ef- Guilt in a low- or non-identified individual is ex-
fects would be most prominent, while the effects may pected to increase considerations of organizational
be weaker for events that clearly involve organiza- exit. In these cases, coping potential appraisals and
tional leaders or unmistakably implicate the orga- future expectancy appraisals associated with
nization’s responsibility. remaining in the organization are not favorable.
Thus, individuals may cope with guilt by avoiding
Hypothesis 1. Anger intensity is positively re-
those they feel guilty toward and/or reframing blame
lated to turnover intentions when organiza-
around another party (forms of disengagement)
tional identification is low and negatively
(Lazarus, 1991a). To remedy the feelings of guilt and
related to turnover intentions when organiza-
prevent future occurrences, leaving the organization
tional identification is high.
may be seen as a desirable option. Low organiza-
Similar to anger, guilt is a negative emotion. Per tional identification should precipitate these sec-
extant theory and research, it is expected to increase ondary appraisals, because the organization can be
turnover intentions (Cropanzano et al., 1993; Maertz reframed as the target of blame (e.g., the individual
& Griffeth, 2004), because leaving the organization may think the organization could have prevented
frees individuals from the negative emotions created him or her from making the mistake).
by work events. However, in light of secondary ap-
Hypothesis 2. Guilt intensity is positively related
praisals, guilt is quite different from other negative
to turnover intentions when organizational
emotions in the work context (Ilies, Peng, Savani, &
identification is low and negatively related to
Dimotakis, 2013). For guilt, blame appraisals are di-
turnover intentions when organizational iden-
rected toward the self. It is a self-conscious emotion
tification is high.
(Tangney, 1999) resulting from individuals’ aware-
ness of their own shortcomings or wrong-doing Pride is distinct in the primary appraisal process
(Lewis, 2008; Schmader & Lickel, 2006). Guilt can from both anger and guilt in that it is experienced due
be an emotion that “binds the person to the source of to goal congruence rather than goal incongruence.
guilt and does not subside without reconciliation As such, pride is a positive emotion. Positive emo-
that tends to restore social harmony” (Izard, 1991: tions have commonly been associated with increased
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1077

employee retention (Cropanzano et al., 1993) be- the highly identified teacher, pride will be experi-
cause they create “psychological comfort,” which enced in the context of the valued social group
motivates staying (Maertz & Griffeth, 2004: 669). But, (e.g., the culture of this school facilitates student
as with the other emotions, we theorize that the re- success), but, without this identification, pride will
lationship between pride and turnover intentions be attributed to the self (e.g., “I am incredibly tal-
will be influenced by organizational identification. ented!”), facilitating inferences in favor of turnover
Identification seems especially relevant, given the (e.g., “I am too good for this place”). Thus, in-
core relational theme of pride to enhance “one’s dividuals unmoored from the organization are likely
ego identity by taking credit for a valued object to experience pride as an emotion that distances
or achievement, either our own or that of some- them from the firm, increasing one’s belief that
one or group with whom we identify” (Lazarus, leaving the non-valued group, given the individual’s
1991a: 271). superiority, is a desirable option.
Primary appraisals for pride include goal content
Hypothesis 3. Pride intensity is positively re-
involving enhancement of one’s self- or social es-
lated to turnover intentions when organiza-
teem, and secondary appraisal regarding credit at-
tional identification is low and negatively
tributed to oneself.1 As such, pride’s effects will
related to turnover intentions when organiza-
depend on how the self is defined. Research suggests
tional identification is high.
there is a crucial difference between those who in-
terpret pride in relation to the self as a group member Despite the differences among anger, guilt, and
compared to those who experience pride as an in- pride, our theorizing leads to similar predictions
dividual (Helm, 2013). Pride can be engaging, bind- for each. Emotion intensity increases turnover in-
ing a social group together (Helm, 2013; Tyler, 1999), tentions among those with low organizational iden-
but it can also be disengaging, elevating the self tification, but decreases turnover intentions among
above others (Kitayama et al., 2006, Smith, 2000). those with high organizational identification, notably
When individuals are highly identified with the independent of emotion valence. Yet, individuals
organization, we expect pride to be experienced as can identify with more than one work-related group
self-enhancing, due to membership in a valued (Ramarajan, 2014) and make distinctions among
group. Remaining with this group, which has pro- different foci for identification (Ashforth et al.,
vided fertile conditions for success, should lead to 2008). We have proposed that identifications mat-
self-enhancement through future success and pride ter because they psychologically contextualize
experiences. Thus, the relationship between pride the appraisal process and channel the coping re-
and turnover intentions is expected to be negative sponses. It would be overly simplistic to assume
when organizational identification is high. that only one identification is influencing these
However, if organizational identification is low, processes, given the multitude of social groups
pride may actually increase turnover intentions. from which individuals may derive self-definition.
When pride is attributed to oneself independently, it However, theorizing and testing around many
serves to reinforce one’s value (Smith, Tong, & identities leads to very complex theory and, sta-
Ellsworth, 2014) and increases self-confidence tistically, to four- or five-way interactions. Thus,
(Tracy & Robins, 2007). Individuals for whom the we limited ourselves to occupational identification
organization is not central are expected to experi- as an additional contextual factor.
ence pride as elevating the self above the organiza-
tion and to socially disengage as part of the pride
Occupational Identification
response (Kitayama et al., 2006, Smith, 2000). As an
example, if a teacher has a successful experience “Occupational identification” captures the extent to
teaching a student, he or she may experience feelings which employees define themselves through mem-
of pride, but the response will depend on the bership in their current occupational group (i.e., in
teacher’s identification with the organization. For terms of the work that they do; Ashforth & Kreiner,
1999; Gümüs, Hamarat, Çolak, & Duran, 2012; Leavitt,
1
Secondary appraisals of coping potential and future Reynolds, Barnes, Schilpzand, & Hannah, 2012). Oc-
expectations are not included in Lazarus’s (1991a) ap- cupational identification provides a valuable juxta-
praisal framework for pride; however, Lazarus (1991a) position because it may complement or conflict with
does suggest that a pride response includes an urge to organizational identification. For example, Gouldner
publicly note successes. (1957) proposed organizational and occupational/
1078 Academy of Management Journal June

professional bases as the primary work-focused iden- blame directed toward the organization especially
tifications.2 Two types of employees were identified likely when anger is experienced.
by Gouldner (1957): locals (identified with the or- Furthermore, anger combined with high occupa-
ganization) and cosmopolitans (identified with the tional and low organizational identification facili-
occupation). Subsequent research in this vein has tates conclusions that coping potential and future
emphasized either the conflict between these two expectations in the current work environment are
bases of identification (e.g., when one identification low. Specifically, the current environment may
is high and the other is low; Hekman, Bigley et al., be viewed as preventing the individual from fulfill-
2009; Hekman, Steensma et al., 2009) or the benefits of ing the role of a prototypical occupational member
high identification with both bases (e.g., work–identity (i.e., an exemplary member; Hogg & Terry, 2000) and
complexity; Caza & Wilson, 2009). from meeting occupational expectations. Because
Anger, as noted earlier, will be appraised with fulfilling the goals of occupation identification does
blame directed outside of the organization when not require the organization and the organization has
organizational identification is high, leading re- demonstrated itself as a problematic context for
sponses to involve engaging with the organization. motive fulfillment, exiting the organization may al-
This should be the case regardless of occupational low for more promising coping potential and future
identification since the focus is on other-blame expectations to enact the occupational identity and
and high organizational identification redirects fulfill identity motives. We expect that those low in
blame. However, when organizational identifica- occupational identification will still respond to an-
tion is low, occupational identification is expected ger with higher turnover intentions when organiza-
to shape responses to anger. In this case, there is tional identification is low, because blame may turn
not a need to protect the organization from blame, toward the organization and leaving the organization
so anger may leave individuals feeling that their may have more favorable coping potential and future
goals are being blocked from within the organiza- expectations. However, we propose that the rela-
tion. We propose that blame appraisals incorpo- tionship will be strongest when occupational iden-
rating the organization will be especially salient tification is high because of the “us [occupation]
when occupational identification is high because versus them [organization]” mentality that is cre-
occupational goals may be at odds with organiza- ated in this case.
tional goals.
Hypothesis 4. The relationship between anger
In support of this view, Hekman and colleagues
intensity and turnover intentions is dependent
theorized that, when individuals were particularly
on organizational and occupational identifica-
identified with their work but not their organization,
tion, such that, when organizational identifica-
they would be especially sensitive to workplace
tion is high, anger intensity is negatively related
events because the organization’s management could
to turnover intentions, and, when organiza-
be viewed as an enemy of good occupational activ-
tional identification is low, anger intensity is
ity (Hekman, Bigley et al., 2009; Hekman, Steensma
positively related to turnover intentions, with
et al., 2009). Employees with high levels of occu-
the strength of this relationship increasing as
pational identification and low levels of organiza-
occupational identification increases.
tional identification are expected to use their
occupational identification as a frame of reference, Guilt is distinct from anger with regard to sec-
leading employees to frame behaviors of adminis- ondary appraisals. When organizational identifica-
trators (i.e., organizational representatives) nega- tion is high, guilt should lead an employee to engage
tively (Hekman, Steensma et al., 2009). This makes with the organization; in contrast, low organizational
identification can lead the individual to cognitively
separate the self from the guilt-creating environ-
2
ment, and even reframe the situation to push guilt
We note that occupational and professional identifi-
toward the organization (Lazarus, 1991a). When both
cation are similar constructs often used interchangeably
organizational and occupational identification are
though some distinctions exist. Professional identification
is distinctive in the unique knowledge, skillsets, and high, the negative relationship between guilt and
prestige associated with the work one does (e.g., medicine; turnover intentions should be especially strong,
see Pratt, Rockmann, & Kaufmann, 2006). However, both because guilt will bind the individual to both
identifications are tied to the same theme of self-overlap the organization and the occupation. The occupa-
with a group pursuing the same work. tional and organizational motives of self-consistency
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1079

and self-enhancement encourage reconciliation. engaging when both identifications are high (e.g., “I
Remaining with the organization provides the must correct this mistake with the client and be
greatest coping potential for rectifying the mistake, a better occupational and organizational member in
since the organization and the occupation are both the future”). Guilt should be especially disengaging
valued groups. Future expectations should also be when both identifications are low (e.g., “The orga-
favorable, because the organization and occupation nization should have prevented this. I should leave,
are held in high regard and are expected to support so I don’t get into a similar situation in the future”).
the individual and create conditions to prevent Finally, when occupational identification is high but
future mistakes. organizational identification is low, the employee
When individuals hold neither their occupation will feel conflicted (e.g., “The organization is par-
nor their organization in high regard, experiences of tially to blame, but I need to make up for this with my
guilt may involve less need to make amends for client”), leading to a weak engagement response.
failures within the organizational environment due
Hypothesis 5. The relationship between guilt
to unfavorable appraisals of coping potential and
intensity and turnover intentions is dependent
future expectations. This is particularly true because
on organizational and occupational identifica-
the individual’s failure involves the workplace,
tion, such that, when organizational identifica-
which is not central to the individual’s identity
tion is high, guilt intensity is negatively related
needs. As a result, guilt is likely to be coped with
to turnover intentions, with the strength of this
either through separation and distancing from the
relationship increasing as occupational identi-
work where the negative feelings of guilt were cre-
fication increases; when organizational identi-
ated (following a coping strategy known as “situation
fication is low, guilt intensity is positively
selection”; see Elfenbein, 2007; Gross, 1998, 2008),
related to turnover intentions when occupational
or through reframing the guilt-inducing situation.
identification is low and weakly negatively re-
Cognitive reframing is likely to redirect blame to-
lated to turnover intentions when occupational
ward the occupation or the organization or both, in-
identification is high.
creasing the desire to disengage. Thus, higher guilt
intensity increases turnover intentions when both Pride involves goal relevance, goal congruence,
identifications are low. and enhancement of one’s self- or social esteem. Ex-
Predicting the response to guilt becomes most periences of pride at work indicate that the organi-
complicated when organizational identification is zational context provides an environment where self-
low and occupational identification is high. On the enhancement is possible. As such, we have proposed
one hand, low organizational identification may lead that a negative relationship exists between pride and
to reframing blame secondary appraisals toward the turnover intentions when the individual is highly
organization. On the other hand, self-enhancement identified with the organization. When both organi-
and self-consistency motives are likely to drive zational and occupational identification are high, the
a need to make up for work failures that relate to two identities reinforce the self-enhancing value of
one’s occupation. Coping potential with members of the current environment and the engaging nature of
the occupational group at work may be favorable. But pride. Attributions of credit in the pride secondary
future expectations, given the lack of identification appraisal are expected to be shared with the organi-
with the organization, may be less favorable: while zation for providing a context for successful occupa-
the current transgression may be dealt with through tional enactment. Because social pride is engaging,
apology, there may be an expectation that guilt will we expect individuals with high levels of both iden-
reoccur because the organizational environment tifications to be brought closer to others in the or-
was partly responsible for the inducement of guilt. ganization and occupation. Thus, the relationship
Taken together, this suggests that the engaging (with between pride and turnover intentions is expected to
occupational members) and disengaging (from the be negative when organizational identification is
organization) responses to guilt will be competing high, with this relationship strengthened by increases
influences. Thus, we propose a weak negative re- in occupational identification.
lationship between guilt and turnover intentions When organizational identification is low, whether
when occupational identification is high and orga- pride is engaging within the work context or dis-
nizational identification is low. engaging will depend on occupational identifica-
To illustrate, if an employee commits an error in an tion. Because pride involves giving credit rather
interaction with a client, guilt should be especially than assigning blame, a lack of organizational
1080 Academy of Management Journal June

identification when experiencing pride may make Hypothesis 6. The relationship between pride
sharing credit with the organization less likely. intensity and turnover intentions is dependent
However, secondary appraisals of credit for pride- on organizational and occupational identification,
invoking work experiences may extend to occu- such that, when organizational identification is
pational members at work when occupational high, pride intensity is negatively related to turn-
identification is high. Thus, when pride is experi- over intentions, with the strength of this relation-
enced at work and occupational identification is ship increasing as occupational identification
high, the emotion engages employees with their increases; when organizational identification is
colleagues. This suggests that staying with the or- low, pride intensity is positively related to turnover
ganization is a function of increased attachment intentions when occupational identification is low
to colleagues more so than the organization. Thus, and is weakly negatively related to turnover in-
while there may be a negative relationship, it is not tentions when occupational identification is high.
expected to be as strong as when organizational
identification is high. METHODS
However, when both organizational and occupa-
tional identification are low, the pride goal content Participants and Procedure
appraisals related to self- or social esteem can be ex- We tested our hypotheses in the training business
pected to lean toward self-esteem. Secondary ap- unit of a large company in the transportation in-
praisals of credit will be attributed to the individual’s dustry. The company employed 284 full-time em-
personal merits divorced from the current organization ployees whose primary role was pilot training and
and occupation. Pride can elevate individuals above certification. Employees were located at four differ-
their organization and occupation (Kitayama et al., ent sites in the United States and in Europe. Before
2006; Smith, 2000). This seems especially likely be- beginning our data collection, members of the re-
cause pride can be a “competitive emotion” (Lazarus, search team visited each site and met with employees
1991a: 273), and is disengaging when self-focused to better understand the work conditions and to en-
(Kitayama et al., 2006). In turn, because neither the courage participation in the study. Following these
organization nor the occupation is central to future meetings, data collection occurred over a period of
experiences of self-enhancement or self-consistency, one year via online surveys, including an initial sur-
the current environment may be seen as irrelevant or vey and multiple follow-up surveys. We employed
even as a hindrance rather than as a vehicle to future experience sampling methodology to assess within-
success. person variations in affective events across time. This
To take an example, a teacher who is responding to approach allowed us to capture emotional reactions
pride experiences at work will be influenced by iden- to affective events and intentions to turnover over the
tification levels. A teacher highly identified with both course of a year.
the organization and the occupation is expected to In the United States, employees were sent an ini-
experience pride with credit given to important social tial survey and four follow-up surveys at intervals of
groups (e.g., “My school features a positive student– two months. In Europe, employees were sent an
teacher environment, my colleagues are supportive, initial survey and two follow-up surveys. The initial
and this has provided me the opportunity to succeed”). survey included identification measures, a baseline
If the teacher is not identified with the school but is measure of turnover intention, and demographic
highly identified with the occupation of teaching, the control variables (i.e., age, tenure, marital status). For
pride experience will engage the teacher with other each of the follow-up surveys, we used an event-
members of the occupation (e.g., “My colleagues are contingent sampling strategy, whereby participants
supportive and they have pushed me to provide a better were asked to complete the survey based upon work
learning experience for my students”), but also link the events they reported experiencing in the past few
teacher to the setting within which self-enhancement days. Each follow-up survey included questions
of the occupational identity is possible (i.e., the school). concerning the discrete emotions that were elicited
However, if the teacher is unidentified with the occu- by work events and turnover intentions.
pation, pride secondary appraisals involve self-credit.
Measures
In this case, the teacher may think about alternative
options (e.g., “I am so talented, I could make much Discrete emotions. We measured feelings of
more money in business”). anger, guilt, and pride in response to work-related
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1081

events using a single emotion word (Shaver et al., would have the lowest values for anger; and those
1987; Weiss et al., 1999). The work events were cho- reporting only one high-intensity anger experience
sen based on management and employee feedback or multiple moderate-intensity anger experiences
during our pre-data collection meetings. Specifically, would have moderate values for anger.3
two types of events appeared to be relevant in this Turnover intentions. We measured turnover in-
particular organization. One set of work events was tentions in both the initial survey and all follow-up
focused on general organizational issues, such as surveys using the three-item scale from Cropanzano,
schedule and pay issues. The second set of events Howes, Grandey, and Toth (1997). A sample item
was focused on experiences of respect and meaning was “I would leave my job if a similar position were
with regard to the job. It was relatively simple to ask available at another company.” Responses were on
about the general organization-related events, but it a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to
was less clear what to ask with regard to job issues. 5 (“strongly agree”). Coefficient a was .76 in the
Thus, we developed a list of positive and negative initial survey and .80 in the follow-up surveys.
events based on the work of Vignoles, Regalia, Organizational identification. Individual-level
Manzi, Golledge, and Scabini (2006) regarding variables were assessed in the initial survey. Orga-
emotions and identity-related experiences. In the nizational identification was measured with Mael
end, there were two sets of work-related events: and Ashforth’s (1992) six-item scale. A sample item
organizational (e.g., pay-related event, schedule- was “The company’s successes are my successes,”
related event, organizational value) and job events with higher scores indicating a stronger identifica-
(e.g., event that made the employee feel good at tion with the organization. The response scale was
their job, event that made the employee feel dis- 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 5 (“strongly agree”).
respected). See Table 1 for a list of events that were Coefficient a was .87.
included in the follow-up surveys. Occupational identification. Occupational iden-
In each follow-up survey, participants were asked, tification was measured using three items from the
“To what extent did this event cause you to experi- social identity importance and centrality scale
ence the following emotions?” with regard to each (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992), in which the occupation
work-related event. Responses were on a 5-point of “instructor” was inserted as the target of social
scale ranging from 0 (“not at all”) to 4 (“very identification. A sample item was “Being an in-
strongly”). We then coded each emotion as the sum structor is an important reflection of who I am,” with
of the specific emotion reported across the events, higher scores indicating a stronger identification
such that each participant had a value for anger, with the instructor occupation. The response scale
guilt, and pride for that survey. As an example of this was 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 5 (“strongly agree”).
coding, those reporting multiple, high-intensity an- Coefficient a was .83.
ger experiences would have the highest values for Control variables. We controlled for baseline
anger; those who experienced no anger experiences turnover intentions as reported in the initial survey,
so that our results would reflect those changes in
TABLE 1
turnover intentions that were due to affective work-
Event Descriptions and Types related events rather than those due to some stable
turnover tendency. We also controlled for three
Event Description Event Type
3
There are a number of ways we could have coded the
Pay-related event Organization
Schedule-related event Organization data (i.e., the sum, the maximum, or the average of each
Event that made you feel devalued Organization emotion). Sum was the most theoretically appropriate
by your organization since multiple high-intensity affective events should have
Event that made you feel important Job stronger overall effects than one high-intensity affective
as a pilot instructor event. To ensure the robustness of our results, we analyzed
Event that made you feel good at your job Job the data using each coding approach, and findings were
Event that made you feel close to other pilot Job generally consistent. Specifically, the maximum produced
instructors very similar results, but the three-way interaction for guilt
Event that made you feel that you were not Job
was stronger while the interaction for pride was weaker.
effective at your job
Using the average greatly decreased the variance of the
Event that reminded you of what it means to be Job
a pilot emotion variables, and, hence, while the magnitude of the
Event that made you feel disrespected as a pilot Job coefficients was similar, there were fewer significant con-
ditional effects.
1082 Academy of Management Journal June

demographic variables—tenure, age, and marital shows the means, standard deviations, and correla-
status. Each of these variables is frequently included tions for all study variables.
in turnover studies and has shown modest predictive
strength (Griffeth, Hom, & Gaertner, 2000). Age was
Analytical Strategy
also of potential interest because emotional experi-
ences can vary across one’s lifespan (Carstensen, The data contained a hierarchical structure in
Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000). In addition, which episodic responses were nested within in-
tenure has long been considered to be related to or- dividuals. To account for these dependencies in the
ganizational identification (Dutton et al., 1994). Ten- data, we used random coefficient modeling with HLM
ure was reported in years and months of continuous 6 (Scientific Software International, Skokie, Illinois)
employment with the company. Age was reported in (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1996) to test our hypotheses.
increments of five, starting at 30, as all participants Because we took an episodic approach, we analyzed
were over 30 years old. Marital status was coded as emotions and turnover intentions in the same follow-
“0” for not currently married and “1” for currently up survey, with identifications as the moderators and
married. Gender was not controlled because the controlling for baseline turnover intentions. To sep-
sample was almost entirely male. We also repeated arate within-person and between-person variance, we
the analyses without the controls and found that this centered Level 1 variables around each individual’s
did not change the direction or significance of any of mean and Level 2 variables around the grand mean of
the results of interest reported here. the sample (Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin, 2000). Before
testing our hypotheses, we ran a series of uncondi-
tional multilevel models to ensure there was suffi-
RESULTS
cient within-person variance. Results indicated that
One hundred and sixty-eight pilot instructors (59% 55% of the variance in anger, 63% in pride, and 57%
response rate) completed the initial survey, and 135 of in guilt was within-person. Finally, 58% of the vari-
those instructors completed follow-up surveys. The ance in turnover intention was within-person.
final dataset consisted of 135 respondents, most of
whom were male (95%). The median reported age
Hypothesis Testing
range was between 51 and 55 years old (ages ranged
from 30 to 70 years). In total, the 135 pilot instructors We started our analyses with an examination of the
completed 334 usable follow-up surveys. Table 2 main effects of discrete emotions (anger, pride, and

TABLE 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among Study Variables
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Level 1 (within-person)
1. Anger 2.86 3.52
2. Guilt .45 1.25 .21**
3. Pride 3.97 4.70 2.12 2.01
4. TOI 2.55 .75 .30** 2.07 2.27** (.80)
Level 2 (between-person)
5. Age 6.27 2.08 2.12* 2.17** .11 2.13*
6. Marital status .78 .42 2.05 2.06 .08 2.10 .20*
7. Tenure 5.90 5.05 .03 2.01 .00 2.01 .39** 2.03
8. Initial TOI 2.27 .97 2.13* 2.08 2.13* .48** 2.02 2.09 .03 (.76)
9. Organizational 3.69 .81 2.21** .03 .08 2.34** .03 .06 .01 2.48** (.87)
Identification
10. Occupational 3.44 1.02 .02 .07 .27** 2.22** .27** .11 .12 2.17* .26** (.83)
Identification

Notes: NLevel 1 5 334. NLevel 2 5 135. Coefficient a provided along the diagonal. Anger, guilt, and pride were coded as 0 to 4 and summed
across events. TOI 5 turnover intentions. Age was coded in 5-year windows beginning at 30. Marital status was coded “1” if married and “0” if
not. Tenure was coded in years. Initial TOI 5 turnover intentions in initial survey.
**5 p , .01
*5 p , .05
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1083

guilt) on turnover intentions. Table 2 shows that both positive when organizational identification was low
anger (r 5 .30, p , .01) and pride (r 5 2.27, p , .01) (1 SD below mean, t 5 2.43, p 5 .02). These findings
were correlated with turnover intentions. However, support Hypothesis 1.
when multilevel considerations were taken into ac- Hypothesis 2 proposed that guilt intensity is pos-
count, there was no longer a significant relationship itively related to turnover intentions when organi-
between any of the discrete emotions and turnover zational identification is low and negatively related
intentions, as Model 1 of Table 3 shows. to turnover intentions when organizational identifi-
Next, we investigated whether organizational cation is high. Model 2 of Table 3 shows that the
identification moderated the within-person effects of cross-level interaction of guilt and organizational
anger, guilt, and pride on turnover intentions (see identification on turnover intentions approached
Table 3). Hypothesis 1 proposed that anger intensity is significance (b 5 2.07, p , .10). Simple slopes ana-
positively related to turnover intentions when orga- lyses using the tools for probing multilevel in-
nizational identification is low and negatively related teractions suggested by Preacher, Curran, and Bauer
to turnover intentions when organizational identifi- (2006) showed that the relationship between guilt
cation is high. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, as shown and turnover intentions was negative when organi-
in Model 2 of Table 3, there was a significant cross- zational identification was high (t 5 22.18, p , .05),
level interaction between anger and organizational as hypothesized, but was not significantly different
identification (b 5 2.07, p , .01). Figure 1 provides from zero when it was low. Therefore, Hypothesis 2
a graphical depiction of this moderated relationship. received marginal support.
Simple slopes analyses showed that this relationship Hypothesis 3 proposed that pride intensity is
was negative when organizational identification was positively related to turnover intentions when or-
high (1 SD above mean, t 5 23.16, p , .01) and ganizational identification is low and negatively

TABLE 3
Results for Moderated Effects of Emotions on Turnover Intentions
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

Intercept (b00) 2.50** 2.52** 2.51** 2.52** 2.48** 2.50**


Level 1
Anger (b10) .01 2.02 2.01 2.02
Guilt (b20) 2.06 2.03 2.03 2.08†
Pride (b30) 2.02† .00 2.01 2.01
Level 2
Age (b01) 2.04† 2.04* 2.04* 2.04* 2.03 2.04*
Marital status (b02) .06 .04 .05 .04 .08 .05
Tenure (b03) .01 .01 .01 .01 .01 .01
Initial TOI (b04) .28** .28** .28** .27** .29** .29**
Org ID (b05) 2.13* 2.12* 2.12* 2.12* 2.11† 2.11†
Occ ID (b06) 2.10† 2.09† 2.09† 2.09† 2.10† 2.09†
Org ID 3 Occ ID (b07) .01 .01 .01 .01
Cross-Level Interactions
Anger 3 Org ID (b11) 2.07** 2.06** 2.06**
Anger 3 Occ ID (b12) .01 .01 .01
Anger 3 Org ID 3 Occ ID (b23) 2.01 2.02
Guilt 3 Org ID (b21) 2.07† 2.10† 2.12*
Guilt 3 Occ ID (b22) .06 .04 .03
Guilt 3 Org ID 3 Occ ID (b23) .06 .20**
Pride 3 Org ID (b31) 2.04** 2.05** 2.05**
Pride 3 Occ ID (b32) 2.03* 2.02** 2.05**
Pride 3 Org ID 3 Occ ID (b33) .02** .02**
s2 .30 .27 .27 .29 .32 .30

Notes: NLevel 1 5 334. NLevel 2 5 135. Initial TOI 5 turnover intentions in initial survey. Org ID 5 organizational identification. Occ ID 5
occupational identification.
**5 p , .01
*5 p , .05

5 p , .10
1084 Academy of Management Journal June

FIGURE 1 FIGURE 2
Moderating Effect for Organizational Moderating Effect for Organizational
Identification on Anger Identification on Pride
3.5 3.5
Low Org ID
Low Org ID
High Org ID
High Org ID
3 3

Turnover Intentions
Turnover Intentions

2.5 2.5

2 2

1.5 1.5
Low High Low High
Anger Pride
Note: Org ID 5 Organizational Identification. Note: Org ID 5 Organizational Identification.

related to turnover intentions when organizational on organizational and occupational identification.


identification is high. Model 2 of Table 3 shows that Specifically, we expected the positive relationship
the cross-level interaction effect of pride and orga- between anger and turnover intentions to be stron-
nizational identification on turnover intentions was gest when organizational identification was low and
significant (b 5 2.04, p , .01). As shown in Figure 2, occupational identification high. The three-way in-
the interaction was consistent with our predictions. teraction for anger in Model 3 was not significant.
Simple slopes showed that this relationship was Model 4 shows that, when anger was tested sepa-
negative when organizational identification was rately, the three-way interaction was still not signifi-
high (t 5 22.74, p , .01) and positive when organi- cant. Simple slopes analysis found some conditional
zational identification was low (t 5 3.31, p , .01). effects for occupational identification; however, these
Therefore, Hypothesis 3 was supported. effects did not alter the basic relationships of the two-
Next, we investigated the conditional effects on way interaction between anger and organizational
the relationships when occupational identification identification. Therefore, we found little evidence
is considered in addition to organizational identifi- that the moderated relationship between anger and
cation. We took three steps to complete this analysis, turnover intentions was also conditional on occupa-
because three-way interactions are especially diffi- tional identification.
cult to detect in field studies (McClelland & Judd, Hypothesis 5 stated that the relationship between
1993). As an initial test, we tested the three-way in- guilt intensity and turnover intentions depends on
teractions of all emotions simultaneously in Model 3, both organizational and occupational identification.
knowing that power issues can be expected when Once again, as evident in Model 3 in Table 3, the
estimating all of the parameters based on a sample three-way cross-level interaction effect of guilt and
size that is constrained (Dawson & Richter, 2006). organizational and occupational identification on
Next, we tested each of the emotions individually, in turnover intentions was not significant. However,
order to increase the power of the omnibus test Model 5 shows that the three-way interaction was
(Models 4, 5, and 6). Finally, we performed simple significant (b 5 .20, p , .01) when tested separately.
slopes analyses on the three-way interactions for Simple slopes analysis revealed conditional effects
each emotion, using the tools provided by Preacher on the relationship between guilt and turnover in-
et al. (2006) and Dawson and Richter (2006). Simple tentions that are plotted in Figure 3. The relationship
slopes tests and plots are based on the individual between guilt and turnover intentions was negative
emotion regressions. This set of steps provided us when organizational identification was high and
with a thorough and consistent set of analyses. occupational identification was low (t 5 23.87, p ,
Hypothesis 4 proposed that the relationship be- .01) and when organizational identification was low
tween anger intensity and turnover intentions depends and occupational identification was high (t 5 22.40,
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1085

FIGURE 3 FIGURE 4
Moderating Effect of Organizational and Moderating Effect of Organizational and
Occupational Identification on Guilt Occupational Identification on Pride
3.5 3.5 Low Org ID, Low Occ ID
High Org ID, Low Occ ID
Low Org ID, High Occ ID
3
3 High Org ID, High Occ ID

Turnover Intentions
Turnover Intentions

2.5
2.5
2
Low Org ID, Low Occ ID
High Org ID, Low Occ ID 2
1.5
Low Org ID, High Occ ID
High Org ID, High Occ ID
1 1.5
Low High Low High
Guilt Pride
Notes: Org ID 5 Organizational Identification, Occ ID 5 Occu- Notes: Org ID 5 Organizational Identification, Occ ID 5 Occu-
pational Identification. pational Identification.

p , .05). Simple slope comparisons indicated that was high and occupational identification was low,
the relationship was stronger when organizational there was a negative relationship between pride
identification was high and occupational identifi- and turnover intentions (t 5 22.50, p , .05). When
cation was low (t 5 25.06, p , .01). This suggests that organizational identification was low and occupa-
the within-person relationship between guilt and tional identification was high, there was no re-
turnover intentions was conditional on the relative lationship between pride and turnover intentions
levels of both organizational and occupational (t 5 20.50, ns). When both were high, there was
identification. As predicted, the relationship was a negative relationship between pride and turnover
negative when organizational identification was intentions (t 5 23.50, p , .01). As predicted, the
high and occupational identification was low and conditional relationship between pride and turn-
when organizational identification was low and over intentions depended on the relative levels of
occupational identification was high. When both both organizational and occupational identifica-
identifications were low, we predicted that the re- tion. The results suggest that the effect of organiza-
lationship would be positive; the trend was positive, tional identification alone was sufficient to make
but it was not significant. There was not a relation- pride-evoking events beneficial for employee re-
ship between guilt and turnover intentions when tention, while the buffering effect of occupational
both organizational and occupational identification identification was at least sufficient to prevent it
were high. This pattern of results provides moderate from being harmful. Overall, these results support
support for Hypothesis 5. Hypothesis 6.
Our last tests concerned Hypothesis 6, which
stated that the relationship between pride intensity
Supplemental Analyses
and turnover intentions is dependent on organiza-
tional and occupational identification. This omnibus Event types. The results reported above are based
test of the interaction was significant in both Model 3 on all affective event responses with no distinctions
(simultaneous, b 5 .02, p , .01) and Model 6 (indi- among the events. However, as noted earlier, two
vidual, b 5 .02, p , .01). Once again, we performed types of events were included in our data collection:
simple slopes analyses to probe this interaction fur- organizational and job-related events. It is possible
ther. Figure 4 depicts the conditional relationships that different findings could emerge across different
graphically. When both organizational and occupa- types of events. Specifically, we might expect the
tional identification were low, there was a positive three-way interactions to emerge more strongly for
relationship between pride and turnover intentions job-related events because they are particularly rel-
(t 5 4.50, p , .01). When organizational identification evant to the individual’s occupation. Thus, we ran
1086 Academy of Management Journal June

a supplementary analysis based on the two different a positive relationship between anger and turnover
categories of events (see Table 1 for the categoriza- intentions when organizational identification was
tion of events and Table 4 for the analyses). The low and occupational identification was high (t 5
findings of this analysis were generally consistent 3.38, p , .01). When occupational identification was
with our prior analysis. Still, there were some addi- low, the slopes were reversed, but neither was sig-
tional findings with regard to job-related events. nificantly different from zero.
For job-related events, the three-way interaction In addition, the two-way interaction for guilt and
for anger was significant (Hypothesis 4, b 5 2.18, p , organizational identification (Hypothesis 2) was
0.01). We performed simple slopes analyses and significant for job-related events. In contrast to all
plotted this interaction (see Figure 5). In line with our events, there was a positive relationship between
expectations, we found a negative relationship be- guilt and turnover intentions (t 5 2.61, p 5 .01) for
tween anger and turnover intentions when both job events when both identifications were low. The
identifications were high (t 5 22.10, p , .05) and findings for pride were consistent with those for all
events.
Turnover intentions and objective turnover. Our
TABLE 4 focus in this paper is on turnover cognitions; thus, we
Supplemental Results for Moderated Effects studied and found support for the effects of discrete
of Emotions on Turnover Intentions emotions and identifications on turnover intentions.
Organization Yet, it is important from an organizational perspec-
Job Events Events tive to confirm that these intentions are relevant to
Model Model Model Model actual turnover outcomes. The organization in our
Variable 1 2 3 4 study provided archival data of actual turnover ap-
proximately six months after the last follow-up sur-
Intercept (b00) 2.52** 2.51** 2.50** 2.50** vey that could be linked to our data on turnover
Level 1
intentions. Employees who exited the organization
Anger (b10) 2.01 .00 2.03 2.02
Guilt (b20) .26** .27** 2.09 2.11†
were coded as “1” and those who did not exit were
Pride (b30) .01 .01 2.08* 2.08* coded as “0.” Correlational analysis demonstrated
Level 2 that turnover intentions from our last survey were
Age (b01) 2.04* 2.04* 2.04 2.04† significantly correlated with actual turnover (r 5 .27,
Marital status (b02) .04 .05 .06 .06 p , .01). We also ran survival analyses using Cox
Tenure (b03) .01 .01 .01 .01
Initial TOI (b04) .28** .28** .29** .29**
Org ID (b05) 2.12* 2.11* 2.11* 2.11*
Occ ID (b06) 2.09† 2.09† 2.09† 2.09† FIGURE 5
Org ID 3 Occ ID (b07) .01 .01
Cross-Level
Moderating Effect of Organizational and
Interactions Occupational Identification on Anger (for Job
Anger 3 Org ID (b11) 2.11* .01 2.09** 2.09** Events)
Anger 3 Occ ID (b12) .05 .03 .01 .00
Anger 3 Org ID 3 Occ 2.18** 2.02 4.5 Low Org ID, Low Occ ID
ID (b23) High Org ID, Low Occ ID
Guilt 3 Org ID (b21) 2.30** 2.43** 2.08 2.15 4 Low Org ID, High Occ ID
Guilt 3 Occ ID (b22) 2.05 .12 .10 .04 High Org ID, High Occ ID
Turnover Intentions

Guilt 3 Org ID 3 Occ .40 .19 3.5


ID (b23)
Pride 3 Org ID (b31) 2.04** 2.05** 2.11** 2.15** 3
Pride 3 Occ ID (b32) 2.03** 2.03** 2.04 2.05
Pride 3 Org ID 3 Occ .03** .12** 2.5
ID (b33)
s2 .27 .27 .27 .27 2
Pseudo-R2 .33 .33 .32 .34
1.5
Notes: NLevel 1 5 334. NLevel 2 5 135. Initial TOI 5 turnover
intentions in initial survey. Org ID 5 organizational identification. 1
Occ ID 5 occupational identification. Low High
Anger
**5 p , .01
*5 p , .05 Notes: Org ID 5 Organizational Identification, Occ ID 5 Occu-

5 p , .10 pational Identification.
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1087

regression with time-varying covariates (Andersen & a more fine-grained understanding of the work-
Gill, 1982), and found support for the relationship ings and consequences of affective experiences in
between turnover intentions and actual turnover organizations.
(b 5 .59, p , .01). These findings underscore the rel- We drew on emotion-appraisal processes (Lazarus,
evance and importance of our dependent variable. 1991a) and social identity theory (Ashforth, 2001;
Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Tajfel & Turner, 1985) to show
that it is not enough to characterize specific discrete
DISCUSSION
emotions as beneficial or detrimental, particularly
The intent of this research was to advance un- with respect to turnover cognitions. Rather, these
derstanding of how discrete emotions unfold in orga- emotions must be investigated with consideration of
nizational environments to influence organizationally how emotion secondary appraisals are shaped. Our
relevant outcomes. We proposed that the pattern of theoretical integration points to the importance of
identifications that individuals had with their or- identifications to this process. For example, the
ganization and occupation were likely to be key findings for anger show that an employee with high
factors in the secondary appraisal process, because organizational identification may respond to feelings
identifications provide a context in which em- of anger with low rather than high withdrawal cog-
ployees’ feelings are processed. In an experience nitions. In addition, the positive emotion of pride
sampling study of anger, guilt, and pride emotions generally is associated with lower turnover in-
at work, we found that their respective influence on tentions. However, for employees lacking in work-
turnover intentions depended on the extent to related identifications, feelings of pride contributed to
which employees identified with their organization increased turnover intentions, and, in this case,
and occupation. These findings provide evidence a positive emotion was associated with a negative
that the effects of affective events at work have organizational outcome.
much to do with the psychological context in which Overall, our research suggests that future explo-
the specific emotions are experienced. rations into other discrete emotions as well as other
variables that influence secondary appraisals would
be promising. For example, future endeavors could
Implications for Theory and Research
address other discrete emotions with distinct sec-
The current study makes several theoretical con- ondary appraisal characteristics, such as hope
tributions. Past empirical research has often relied (which has unfavorable but not hopeless future ex-
on symmetrical assumptions to investigate affective pectations) and sadness (which has no blameworthy
events at work (Elfenbein, 2007; Grandey, Tam, & party and low coping potential) (Lazarus, 1991a).
Brauburger, 2002; Wegge, van Dick, Fisher, West, & Variables beyond identifications may also influence
Dawson, 2006). This research concludes that, gen- secondary appraisals. For example, leader–member
erally, positive emotions are desirable and nega- exchange may be relevant to coping potential and
tive emotions are undesirable for organizations future expectations, or organizational culture may
(Elfenbein, 2007; Maertz & Griffeth, 2004). However, influence how blame/credit appraisals are formed.
there is growing evidence that discrete positive and Our treatment of identification as a moderator also
negative emotions can have unique effects that run has implications for identity researchers, particu-
counter to this basic premise (e.g., Ilies et al., 2013). larly for studies that incorporate more than one
Our study addresses this issue by considering the identity. Self-consistency and self-enhancement
appraisal characteristics of discrete emotions. In motives are relevant when an identification is high,
doing so, we show that discrete emotions can influ- but the approach to fulfilling these motives depends
ence work-related cognitions in ways that suggest on the focal social group with which an individual
a dark side of positive emotion and a bright side of identifies, as well as the set of the identifications.
negative emotion. More generally, our theory and This is particularly interesting in the case of the self-
findings suggest that, when identifications are con- enhancement motive for pride. We found that the
sidered, the effects of discrete emotions can differ effect of organizational identification alone was
among emotions with similar valence (e.g., anger and sufficient to make pride-evoking events conducive to
guilt), and be similar for emotions with different employee retention, and that occupational identifi-
valence (e.g., guilt and pride). Our theory and results cation had a buffering effect on turnover intentions
thus demonstrate that recognizing the factors that for those experiencing pride. The results suggest that
can influence discrete emotion appraisals offers there is somewhat of a substitution effect, with high
1088 Academy of Management Journal June

levels of organizational identification or high levels Rather than focusing on suppressing workplace
of occupational identification being sufficient for emotions, organizations should employ practices
the organization context to fulfill self-enhancement that encourage greater organizational identification.
needs. So long as either organizational or occupa- Sense-giving is a key contributor to identification
tional identification were high, pride did not in- processes (Ashforth et al., 2008; Gioia & Chittipeddi,
crease turnover intentions. 1991), and organizational communication is key to
The interactions across all three emotions with sensegiving (DiSanza & Bullis, 1999; Pratt, 2000).
identification provide evidence that work-related Managers can communicate organizational informa-
identifications are powerful factors in how people tion that enhances the prestige of the organization,
sense-make around their emotions. Identification thereby making identification with the organization
has frequently been associated with cognition, affect, a contributor to the employee’s self-enhancement
and behavior (e.g., Ashforth et al., 2008). We clarify needs (Smidts, Pruyn, & Van Riel, 2001). Organi-
and extend these relationships in an organizational zations can also sense-give through policies. For
context. In our model, identification constrains be- example, compensation systems that encourage
havioral intentions of leaving the organization, and employees to see themselves as members of an in-
we propose this occurs due to a relationship between group working against out-group organizations
work-related identification and the cognitive fram- should encourage identification (Long, 1978; Worchel,
ing of affective events. Furthermore, in our supple- Rothgerber, Day, Hart, & Butemeyer, 1998). Over-
mentary analysis, we confirm that these intentions all, this study emphasizes the value of encourag-
do lead to actual turnover outcomes. In sum, work- ing identifications, especially those tied to the
related identifications provide the psychological organization.
context within which people give meaning to what
they do and experience at work. And, discrete emo-
Limitations
tions and their consequences cannot be understood
within organizational settings unless we take such Our theory is based on the fulfillment of self-
contextual variables into account. enhancement and self-consistency motives as pri-
mary explanatory mechanisms for the specific
nature of appraisal cognitions and turnover inten-
Implications for Practice
tions; however, we were unable to measure sec-
This study also presents interesting practical im- ondary appraisal components or motive fulfillment
plications. The first of which is that company prac- cognitions. Also, we chose two commonly noted
tices and policies informed by a positive/negative individual motives for identity and identification;
affect approach (rather than discrete emotions), however, there are other motives. For example,
which seek to engender positive emotions and mini- Ashforth (2001) noted six identity motives: knowl-
mize negative emotions, may not have the intended edge, expression, coherence, continuity, distinc-
effects. Among those not highly identified with their tiveness, and enhancement. Clearly, identity in
organizations or their work, positive employee emo- general, and identification with social groups in
tions may actually increase turnover intentions while particular, is important with respect to the fulfill-
negative emotions can sometimes reduce the risk of ment of several individual needs. More detailed
turnover. Organizations have limited control over work addressing the two primary motives we se-
emotions in the workplace (Basch & Fisher, 2000; lected (i.e., measuring the motives within the context
Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Our findings suggest of emotions) and expanding the research to include
a promising alternative for inoculating employees other motives has great potential to inform our un-
against the potentially turnover-inducing effect of derstanding of how identification colors employee
emotions. Enhancing work-related identification interpretations of the work environment.
may actually turn affective events into turnover- The results indicate that occupational identifica-
reducing experiences. In particular, organizational tion may be a particularly important area for addi-
identification was generally beneficial for buffering tional development in regard to how identification
the negative effects of work-related emotions. Our influences interpretation of emotion. Organizational
theory and our empirical results also suggest that, identification acted as a predictable moderator in
while occupational identity is not as powerful as our analyses. Both pride and anger interacted with
organizational identity in turnover, it does play organizational identification to increase turnover
a complementary role. intentions when identification was low, but to
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1089

decrease turnover intentions when identification was research on individuals’ emotional responses (Mauss,
high. Guilt also interacted somewhat predictably with Levenson, McCarter, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2005). Anger,
organizational identification, as the relationship was guilt, and pride are familiar and commonly felt emo-
negative when organizational identification was tions, which most people are able to identify within
high. However, when occupational identification themselves (Barrett, Gross, Christensen, & Benvenuto,
was added to our analyses, some results did not line 2001; Izard, 1991; Shaver et al., 1987). The likelihood
up with predictions. For example, in the case of that individuals were capable of identifying their felt
guilt, when organizational and occupational iden- emotions using one well-known emotion word sug-
tification were both high, the relationship between gests that the use of one-item emotion measures does
guilt and turnover intentions trended slightly up- not undermine the findings. In addition, group-
wards (though not significantly). Perhaps the emotion centered emotions minimize the potential for com-
of guilt turned to shame (a more negative holistic self- mon source biases such as social desirability, response
focused emotion; Lewis, 2008) when employees felt tendencies, and trait affectivity. Still, it would be
a strong self-overlap with both the organization and worthwhile in the future to consider other discrete
the occupation, thus counteracting the need to repair emotions that have their own unique characteristics
with a need for withdrawal. Further work is necessary and to use multi-item emotion measures.
to specify the effects of occupational identification
more precisely.
CONCLUSION
We briefly addressed the nature of work events,
and the results of the supplementary analysis suggest This paper contributes to our understanding of
that additional research is needed. When splitting affective events as complex phenomena that depend
out job-related events, responses to anger were not as not simply on the positive or negative nature of
predicted when occupational identification was emotions but rather on the emotion appraisal pro-
low. Though the slope was not significant, when cesses. For organizations, emotions are sometimes
both identifications were low, anger appeared to re- viewed as a problem that must be regulated (Ashforth
duce turnover intentions for job-related events. This & Kreiner, 2002). However, our findings suggest the
raises questions about other identifications or con- “problem of emotions” at work is not necessarily
textual factors that may shape the relationship be- a problem at all; by fostering identifications, emo-
tween an emotion and work-related cognitions. We tions at work may actually help organizations retain
have identified an important piece of the puzzle, but employees. In short, we feel and interpret the world
there is more to explore. through our self-definitions.
From a methodological perspective, the sample
used here was relatively homogeneous, primarily
involving middle-aged men. The employees of this REFERENCES
organization tended to be at later stages in their ca- Abrams, D., Ando, K., & Hinkle, S. 1998. Psychological
reer. Although our theory is not contingent upon attachment to the group: Cross-cultural differences in
gender or age, empirically, we cannot be certain that organizational identification and subjective norms as
the findings generalize to women or younger em- predictors of workers’ turnover intentions. Personal-
ployees (the youngest participants were in their ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24: 1027–1039.
thirties). Furthermore, this study was conducted in Andersen, P. K., & Gill, D. R. 1982. Cox’s regression model
one organization, meaning there could have been for counting processes: A large sample study. Annals
other specific contextual characteristics that influ- of Statistics, 10: 1100–1120.
enced the relationships we found. In other settings, Ashforth, B. 2001. Role transitions in organizational
employees may have greater reasons to stay with an life: An identity-based perspective. New York, NY:
organization based on financial and personal needs Routledge.
and may respond differently to affective events. Al- Ashforth, B. E., Harrison, S. H., & Corley, K. G. 2008.
though our findings provide evidence that discrete Identification in organizations: An examination of
emotions are given meaning in relation to identifi- four fundamental questions. Journal of Management,
cation, the limitations of the sample emphasize the 34: 325–374.
importance of additional research developing our Ashforth, B. E., & Kreiner, G. E. 1999. “How can you do
understanding of these effects. it?”: Dirty work and the challenge of constructing
Finally, the use of one-item measures of emotions a positive identity. Academy of Management Re-
is a limitation, though it is consistent with prior view, 24: 413–434.
1090 Academy of Management Journal June

Ashforth, B. E., & Kreiner, G. E. 2002. Normalizing emotion in Cooper, D., & Thatcher, S. M. 2010. Identification in orga-
organizations: Making the extraordinary seem ordinary. nizations: The role of self-concept orientations and
Human Resource Management Review, 12: 215–235. identification motives. Academy of Management
Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. 1989. Social identity theory and Review, 35: 516–538.
the organization. Academy of Management Review, Cropanzano, R., Howes, J. C., Grandey, A. A., & Toth, P.
14: 20–39. 1997. The relationship of organizational politics and
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. support to work behaviors, attitudes, and stress.
2001. Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18: 159–180.
to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion Cropanzano, R., James, K., & Konovsky, M. A. 1993. Dis-
differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition and positional affectivity as a predictor of work attitudes
Emotion, 15: 713–724. and job performance. Journal of Organizational Be-
Barsade, S. G., Brief, A. P., & Spataro, S. E. 2003. The af- havior, 14: 595–606.
fective revolution in organizational behavior: The Dasborough, M. T. 2006. Cognitive asymmetry in em-
emergence of a paradigm. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Orga- ployee emotional reactions to leadership behaviors.
nizational behavior: The state of the science: 3–52. The Leadership Quarterly, 17: 163–178.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. 2006. Probing three-way
Basch, J., & Fisher, C. D. 2000. Affective events – emotion interactions in moderated multiple regression: De-
matrix: A classification of work events and associated velopment and application of a slope difference test.
emotions. In N. M. Ashkanasy, C. E. J. Hartel & The Journal of Applied Psychology, 91: 917–926.
W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Re-
DiSanza, J. R., & Bullis, C. 1999. “Everybody identifies with
search, theory and practice: 36–48. Westport, CT:
Smokey the Bear”: Employee responses to newsletter
Quorum Books.
identification inducements at the U.S. Forest Ser-
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. 1999. vice. Management Communication Quarterly, 12:
Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African 347–399.
Americans: Implications for group identification and
Dutton, J. E., Dukerich, J. M., & Harquail, C. V. 1994. Or-
well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ganizational images and member identification. Ad-
chology, 77: 135–149.
ministrative Science Quarterly, 39: 239–263.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. 1996. HLM: Hierarchical
Elfenbein, H. A. 2007. Emotion in organizations: A review
linear and nonlinear modeling with the HLM/2L and
and theoretical integration. The Academy of Man-
HLM/3L programs. Chicago, IL: SSI, Inc.
agement Annals, 1: 315–386.
Butt, A. N., & Choi, J. N. 2006. The effects of cognitive
Ellemers, N., Van Rijswijk, W., Roefs, M., & Simons, C.
appraisal and emotion on social motive and negotia-
1997. Bias in intergroup perceptions: Balancing group
tion behavior: The critical role of agency of negotiator
identity with social reality. Personality and Social
emotion. Human Performance, 19: 305–325.
Psychology Bulletin, 23: 186–198.
Butts, M., Becker, W., & Boswell, W. 2015. Hot buttons and
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. 2003. Appraisal processes in
time sinks: The effects of electronic communica-
emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer &
tion during nonwork time on emotions and work–
H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sci-
nonwork conflict. Academy of Management Journal,
ences: 572–595. New York, NY: Oxford University
58: 763–788.
Press.
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade,
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. 1988. Shades of joy: Patterns
J. R. 2000. Emotional experience in everyday life
of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cog-
across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79: 644–655. nition and Emotion, 2: 301–331.

Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. 2009. Anger is an Elsbach, K. D., & Kramer, R. M. 1996. Members’ responses
approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. to organizational identity threats: Encountering and
Psychological Bulletin, 135: 183–204. countering the Business Week rankings. Administra-
tive Science Quarterly, 41: 442–476.
Caza, B. B., & Wilson, M. G. 2009. Me, myself, and I: The
benefits of work-identity complexity. In L. M. Roberts Fredrickson, B. L. 1998. What good are positive emotions?
& J. E. Dutton (Eds.), Exploring positive identities and Review of General Psychology, 2: 300–319.
organizations: 99–123. New York, NY: Routledge. Frijda, N. H. 1986. The emotions. London, England:
Conroy, S. A., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M. 2014. Letting go and Cambridge University Press.
moving on: Work-related identity loss and recovery. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. 1989. Relations
Academy of Management Review, 39: 67–87. among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1091

readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. 2000. Social identity and self-
chology, 57: 212–228. categorization processes in organizational contexts.
George, J. M. 1989. Mood and absence. The Journal of Academy of Management Review, 25: 121–140.
Applied Psychology, 74: 317–324. Hogg, M. A., Terry, D. J., & White, K. M. 1995. A tale of two
Gioia, D. A., & Chittipeddi, K. 1991. Sensemaking and theories: A critical comparison of identity theory with
sensegiving in strategic change initiation. Strategic social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly,
Management Journal, 12: 433–448. 58: 255–269.
Gooty, J., Gavin, M., & Ashkanasy, N. M. 2009. Emotions Ilies, R., Peng, A. C., Savani, K., & Dimotakis, N. 2013.
research in OB: The challenges that lie ahead. Journal Guilty and helpful: An emotion-based reparatory
of Organizational Behavior, 30: 833–838. model of voluntary work behavior. The Journal of
Applied Psychology, 98: 1051–1059.
Gouldner, A. W. 1957. Cosmopolitans and locals: Toward
an analysis of latent social roles. Administrative Sci- Izard, C. E. 1991. The psychology of emotions. New York,
ence Quarterly, 2: 281–306. NY: Springer.
Grandey, A. A., Tam, A. P., & Brauburger, A. L. 2002. Af- Izard, C. E. 2009. Emotion theory and research: Highlights,
fective states and traits in the workplace: Diary and unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual
survey data from young workers. Motivation and Review of Psychology, 60: 1–25.
Emotion, 26: 31–55. Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. 2006. Cultural
Griffeth, R. W., Hom, P. W., & Gaertner, S. 2000. A meta- affordances and emotional experience: Socially en-
analysis of antecedents and correlates of employee gaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the
turnover: Update, moderator tests, and research im- United States. Journal of Personality and Social
plications for the next millennium. Journal of Man- Psychology, 91: 890–903.
agement, 26: 463–488. Lazarus, R. S. 1991a. Emotion and adaption. New York,
Gross, J. J. 1998. The emerging field of emotion regulation: NY: Oxford University Press.
An integrative review. Review of General Psychol- Lazarus, R. S. 1991b. Progress on a cognitive-motivational-
ogy, 2: 271–299. relational theory of emotion. The American Psy-
Gross, J. J. 2008. Emotion regulation. In M. Lewis, chologist, 46: 819–834.
J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook Lazarus, R. S., & Cohen-Charash, Y. 2001. Discrete emotions
of emotions, vol. 3: 497–513. New York, NY: Guilford. in organizational life. In R. Payne & C. L. Cooper (Eds.),
Gümüs, M., Hamarat, B., Çolak, E., & Duran, E. 2012. Or- Emotions at work: Theory, research and applications
ganizational and occupational identification: Re- for management: 45–81. Chichester, England: Wiley.
lations to teacher satisfaction and intention to early Leach, C. W., Mosquera, P. M. R., Vliek, M. L., & Hirt, E.
retirement. Career Development International, 17: 2010. Group devaluation and group identification.
300–313. The Journal of Social Issues, 66: 535–552.
Hekman, D. R., Bigley, G. A., Steensma, H. K., & Hereford, Leavitt, K., Reynolds, S., Barnes, C., Schilpzand, P., &
J. F. 2009. Combined effects of organizational and
Hannah, S. 2012. Different hats, different obligations:
professional identification on the reciprocity dynamic
Plural occupational identities and situated moral
for professional employees. Academy of Manage-
judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 55:
ment Journal, 52: 506–526.
1316–1333.
Hekman, D. R., Steensma, H. K., Bigley, G. A., & Hereford,
Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. 2011. Discrete
J. F. 2009. Effects of organizational and professional
emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, ex-
identification on the relationship between adminis-
perience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis
trators’ social influence and professional employees’
of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological
adoption of new work behavior. The Journal of Ap-
Bulletin, 137: 834–855.
plied Psychology, 94: 1325–1335.
Lewis, M. 2008. Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment,
Helm, S. 2013. A matter of reputation and pride: Associa-
pride, shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-
tions between perceived external reputation, pride in
Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions:
membership, job satisfaction and turnover intentions.
742–756. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
British Journal of Management, 24: 542–556.
Lindebaum, D., & Jordan, P. J. 2012. Positive emotions,
Hofmann, D. A., Griffin, M. A., & Gavin, M. B. 2000. The
application of hierarchical linear modeling to organiza- negative emotions, or utility of discrete emotions?
tional research. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33: 1027–1030.
Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organi- Long, R. J. 1978. The effects of employee ownership on
zations: 423–468. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. organizational identification, employee job attitudes,
1092 Academy of Management Journal June

and organizational performance: A tentative frame- Ramarajan, L. 2014. Past, present and future research on
work and empirical findings. Human Relations, 31: multiple identities: Towards an intrapersonal net-
29–48. work approach. The Academy of Management An-
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. 1992. A collective self-esteem nals, 8: 589–659.
scale: Self-evaluation of one’s social identity. Person- Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. 1996. Appraisal
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18: 302–318. determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accu-
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. E. 1992. Alumni and their alma rate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emo-
mater: A partial test of the reformulated model of or- tion, 10: 241–277.
ganizational identification. Journal of Organiza- Roseman, I. J., & Smith, C. A. 2001. Appraisal theory:
tional Behavior, 13: 103–123. Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In
Maertz, C. P., & Griffeth, R. W. 2004. Eight motivational K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Ap-
forces and voluntary turnover: A theoretical synthesis praisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, re-
with implications for research. Journal of Manage- search: 3–19. New York, NY: Oxford University
ment, 30: 667–683. Press.
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. 2006. The approach and
& Gross, J. J. 2005. The tie that binds? Coherence avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions:
among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused
Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 5: 175–190. wrongdoing. Motivation and Emotion, 30: 42–55.
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. 1993. Statistical difficul- Scott, B. A., & Judge, T. A. 2006. Insomnia, emotions, and
ties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. job satisfaction: A multilevel study. Journal of Man-
Psychological Bulletin, 114: 376–390. agement, 32: 622–645.
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. Shamir, B. 1991. The charismatic relationship: Alternative
2013. Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and explanations and predictions. The Leadership
future development. Emotion Review, 5: 119–124. Quarterly, 2: 81–104.
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. 1988. The cognitive Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. 1987.
structure of emotions. Cambridge, England: Cam- Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a pro-
bridge University Press. totype approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52: 1061–1086.
Pekrun, R. 2006. The control–value theory of achievement
emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications Smidts, A., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Van Riel, C. B. 2001. The
for educational research and practice. Educational impact of employee communication and perceived
Psychology Review, 18: 315–341. external prestige on organizational identification.
Academy of Management Journal, 44: 1051–1062.
Pelled, L. H., & Xin, K. R. 1999. Down and out: An in-
vestigation of the relationship between mood and Smith, R. H. 2000. Assimilative and contrastive emo-
employee withdrawal behavior. Journal of Manage- tional reactions to upward and downward social
ment, 25: 875–895. comparisons. In J. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.), Hand-
book of social comparison: 173–200. New York,
Perry, R. P., Stupnisky, R. H., Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G.,
NY: Springer.
& Weiner, B. 2010. Bad starts and better finishes: At-
tributional retraining and initial performance in Smith, C. A., Tong, E. M., & Ellsworth, P. C. 2014. The
competitive achievement settings. Journal of Social differentiation of positive emotional experience as
and Clinical Psychology, 29: 668–700. viewed through the lens of appraisal theory. In
M. M. Tugade, M. N. Shiota & L. D. Kirby (Eds.),
Pratt, M. G. 2000. The good, the bad, and the ambivalent:
Handbook of positive emotions: 11–27. New York,
Managing identification among Amway distributors.
NY: Guilford.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 45: 456–493.
Tai, K., Narayanan, J., & McAllister, D. J. 2012. Envy as
Pratt, M. G., Rockmann, K. W., & Kaufmann, J. B. 2006.
pain: Rethinking the nature of envy and its implica-
Constructing professional identity: The role of work
and identity learning cycles in the customization of tions for employees and organizations. Academy of
identity among medical residents. Academy of Man- Management Review, 37: 107–129.
agement Journal, 49: 235–262. Tajfel, H. 1982. Social psychology of intergroup relations.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. 2006. Compu- Annual Review of Psychology, 33: 1–39.
tational tools for probing interactions in multiple lin- Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1985. The social identity theory of
ear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin
analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations: 7–24.
Statistics, 31: 437–448. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.
2017 Conroy, Becker, and Menges 1093

Tangney, J. P. 1999. The self‐conscious emotions: Shame, Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Cropanzano, R. 1999. Effects of
guilt, embarrassment and pride. In T. Dalgleish & justice conditions on discrete emotions. The Journal
M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emo- of Applied Psychology, 84: 786–794.
tion: 541–568. New York, NY: Wiley. Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E. A., Hart, D., & Butemeyer,
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. 2007. The psychological J. 1998. Social identity and individual productivity
structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of within groups. British Journal of Social Psychology,
Personality and Social Psychology, 92: 506–525. 37: 389–413.
Tyler, T. R. 1999. Why people cooperate with organiza-
tions: An identity-based perspective. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 21: 201–246.
van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. 2009. Leveling Samantha A. Conroy (Samantha.conroy@colostate.edu) is
up and down: The experiences of benign and mali- an assistant professor of management in the College of
cious envy. Emotion, 9: 419–429. Business at Colorado State University. She received her
Vignoles, V. L., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Golledge, J., & PhD from the University of Arkansas. Her research in-
Scabini, E. 2006. Beyond self-esteem: Influence of terests include work-related identity, emotions, and pay
multiple motives on identity construction. Journal of systems.
Personality and Social Psychology, 90: 308–333. William J. Becker (beckerwj@vt.edu) is an associate pro-
Wegge, J., van Dick, R., Fisher, G. K., West, M. A., & fessor of management in the Pamplin College of Business at
Dawson, J. F. 2006. A test of basic assumptions of af- Virginia Tech. He received his PhD from the University of
fective events theory (AET) in call centre work. British Arizona. His research interests include emotion, turnover,
Journal of Management, 17: 237–254. organizational neuroscience, and leadership.
Weick, K. E., Sutcliffe, K. M., & Obstfeld, D. 2005. Orga- Jochen I. Menges (jochen.menges@whu.edu) holds the
nizing and the process of sensemaking. Organization Chair of Leadership and HRM at WHU – Otto Beisheim
Science, 16: 409–421. School of Management. He received his PhD in manage-
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. 1996. Affective events ment from the University of St. Gallen, Switzerland, and is
theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, cau- a lecturer at the University of Cambridge. His research fo-
ses and consequences of affective experiences at work. cuses on leadership, emotions in organizations, and work
In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in motivation.
organizational behavior, vol. 18: 1–74. Greenwich,
CT: JAI Press.

You might also like