Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

The Accuracy of Substituted Judgments in Patients with

Terminal Diagnoses
Daniel P. Sulmasy, OFM, MD, PhD; Peter B. Terry, MD, MA; Carol S. Weisman, PhD;
Deborah J. Miller, RN, PhD; Rebecca Y. Stallings, MHS; Margaret A. Vettese, RN, PhD;
and Karen B. Haller, RN, PhD

Background: Patients' loved ones often make end-of-life Ann Intern Med. 1998;128:621-629.
treatment decisions, but the accuracy of their substituted
From Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, D.C.;
judgments and the factors associated w i t h accuracy are and the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, Mary-
poorly understood. land. For current author addresses, see end of text.
Objective: To assess the accuracy of judgments made by
surrogate decision makers; ascertain the beliefs, practices,
and clinical and sociodemographic factors associated w i t h
W hen patients face end-of-life decisions and
are unable to speak for themselves, loved
ones are often asked to make substituted judg-
accuracy of surrogates' decisions; assess the preferences of
patients for life-sustaining treatments; and compare dif- ments. Ethicists have argued that this is an impor-
ferences in accuracy across diagnoses. tant way of respecting patient autonomy (1, 2), but
preliminary studies indicate that substituted judg-
Design: Cross-sectional paired interviews.
ments may not be very accurate (3-11). This con-
Setting: Outpatient practices of three university hospitals. cern has led some observers to question the useful-
Patients: 250 patients w i t h terminal diagnoses of conges- ness of asking for substituted judgments (8, 12).
tive heart failure, AIDS, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, lung If the concept of substituted judgment is to re-
cancer, and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (50 pa- main clinically useful, health care professionals need
tient-surrogate pairs in each group) and 50 general med- a better sense of the circumstances under which
ical patients and their surrogates.
substituted judgments made by surrogates are accu-
Measurements: The accuracy of surrogate predictions rate or inaccurate. Such data may alert clinicians to
was measured by using scales based on 10 potential treat- instances in which patients are at risk for having
ments in each of three hypothetical clinical scenarios. their wishes misrepresented and may aid in the
Results: Preferences varied according t o mode of treat- design of education campaigns to improve the ac-
ment and scenario. On average, surrogates made correct curacy of substituted judgments.
predictions in 66% of instances. Accuracy was better for Little is known about the factors associated with
the permanent coma scenario than for the scenarios of accurate substituted judgments, especially among
severe dementia or coma w i t h a small chance of recovery
patients with terminal diagnoses (8, 11). In a pilot
{P < 0.001). In a binary logit model, the accuracy of substi-
tuted judgments was positively associated w i t h the patient
study of 50 general medical patients (13), we found
having spoken w i t h the surrogate about end-of-life issues that accuracy was positively associated with discus-
(odds ratio [OR], 1.9 [95% CI, 1.6 t o 2.3]), the patient sions between patients and surrogates and nega-
having private insurance (OR, 1.4 [CI, 1.1 t o 1.7]), the tively associated with religiosity. Building on this
surrogate's level of education (OR, 1.5 [CI, 1.2 to 1.9]), and experience, we studied 250 terminally ill patients
the patient's level of education (OR, 1.7 [CI, 1.4 t o 2.2]). and their legal surrogates and a comparison group
Accuracy was negatively associated w i t h the patient's be- of 50 general medical patients and their surrogates.
lief that he or she would live longer than 10 years (OR, 0.6 Our objectives were to 1) assess the accuracy of the
[CI, 0.5 t o 0.7]), surrogate experience w i t h life-sustaining substituted judgments made by the surrogates; 2)
treatment (OR, 0.4 [CI, 0.3 t o 0.5]), surrogate participation
ascertain the beliefs, practices, and clinical and so-
in religious services (OR, 0.67 [CI, 0.50 t o 0.91]), and a
diagnosis of heart failure (OR, 0.6 [CI, 0.5 t o 0.8]). Age,
ciodemographic factors associated with accuracy; 3)
ethnicity, marital status, religion, and advance directives assess the preferences of patients for life-sustaining
were not associated w i t h accuracy. treatments; and 4) compare differences in accuracy
across diagnoses.
Conclusions: The accuracy of substituted judgments is
associated w i t h multiple clinically apparent patient and
surrogate factors. This information can help clinicians Methods
identify conditions under which substituted judgments are
likely t o be accurate or inaccurate and can help target Patients
populations for education designed t o improve the accu- Between November 1993 and February 1996, pa-
racy of surrogate decision making.
tients were recruited from the outpatient practices
of Johns Hopkins Hospital and the Hopkins Bay-
This paper is also available at http://www.acponline.org. view Medical Center, Baltimore, Maryland, and
© 1998 American College of Physicians 621
Georgetown University Medical Center, Washing- or after the patient interview. Patients and surro-
ton, D.C. Eligible patients were older than 17 years gates were asked not to speak with each other about
of age, spoke English, and had one of the following the interviews until both interviews were completed.
conditions known to be associated with a less than Informed consent was obtained from all partici-
50% chance of surviving for 2 years: New York pants. The study was approved by the institutional
Heart Association class III or IV congestive heart review boards of the Johns Hopkins Medical Insti-
failure with no possibility of transplantation (14), tutions and Georgetown University Medical Center.
advanced HIV infection according to the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention case definition Interview Schedule
for AIDS (before the use of protease inhibitors) Structured interviews consisted of closed-ended
(15), amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with respiratory questions about sociodemographic factors, previous
compromise (16), unresectable non-small-cell lung discussions about end-of-life decisions, advance di-
cancer (17), or oxygen-dependent chronic obstruc- rectives, and past experiences. Patients were asked
tive pulmonary disease with dyspnea at rest (18, 19). about their treatment preferences, and surrogates
Treating physicians were asked to exclude patients were asked to predict patient preferences. Sociode-
whom they thought they were likely to live longer mographic information included age, sex, ethnic
than 2 years and patients who were cognitively in- group, exposure category for HIV infection, level of
capable of participating. The comparison group education, socioeconomic factors, insurance status,
comprised general medical patients who were older and religious beliefs and practices.
than 64 years of age, spoke English, and were ex- We elicited preferences for various medical in-
pected to live longer than 2 years. terventions in the setting of three hypothetical clin-
To have a power of 0.80 to detect a 15% differ- ical scenarios that would render patients unable to
ence between patient preferences and surrogate pre- make decisions for themselves: permanent coma,
dictions at a = 0.05, we needed 263 patient-surrogate coma with a small chance of recovery (the "small
pairs. Rounding up to 300 gave a power of 0.75 to chance" scenario), and severe dementia (Table 1).
detect a 25% difference in agreement between di- For each scenario, the patient was asked to report
agnostic groupings at a = 0.05. his or her preferences for intensive care, intubation,
Surrogates were chosen according to Maryland's cardiopulmonary resuscitation, feeding tube place-
legal hierarchy of surrogates for incapacitated pa- ment, nasotracheal suction, surgery, hemodialysis,
tients: durable power of attorney, guardian, spouse, esophagogastroduodenoscopy, phlebotomy, and chest
adult child, parent, sibling, other relative, or friend roentgenography. All scenarios and interventions
(20). For uniformity, surrogates of patients recruited were described briefly and simply to ensure under-
at Georgetown University Medical Center were also standing. For example, the feeding tube question
chosen according to the Maryland hierarchy. was worded, "Would you want to be fed by a feed-
Attending physicians were contacted 2 weeks be- ing tube (that is, a tube put through your nose or
fore scheduled patient visits. Patients were initially through the skin over your stomach)?"
informed of the general nature of the study and Requests for a limited trial of an intervention
asked to supply the phone number of the surrogate were interpreted as "yes" responses. Patients and
who was highest in Maryland's legal hierarchy. At surrogates were urged to give their best judgment.
the time of the appointment, details of the study "Unsure" responses were recoded as "yes" re-
were described and informed consent was obtained. sponses because they would be clinically interpreted
Surrogates were interviewed within 48 hours before that way. The interview schedules had been previ-
ously developed and tested for reliability (13) and
were based on the Medical Directive of Emanuel
Table 1 . Clinical Scenarios
and Emanuel (21). Slight modifications were made
Please imagine that each situation happens to you in addition to your for our protocol; the revised instruments showed
present medical condition. high internal consistency, with Kuder-Richardson
Scenario 1: Imagine that you are permanently unconscious and, in the
opinion of your physicians, have no hope of waking up and can feel statistics of 0.97 for the permanent coma scenario,
no pain. 0.94 for the small chance scenario, and 0.93 for the
Scenario 2: Imagine that you are in a coma and can feel no pain. The
physicians think that with aggressive treatment: severe dementia scenario. The complete set of in-
You have a high chance of dying (4 in 5). struments is available from the authors on request.
You have a small chance of coming out of the coma but not being able
to speak or understand others (1 in 5).
You have a very small chance of coming out of the coma and being Surrogate Accuracy in Matching Patient
able to speak and understand others (less than 1 in 100). Preferences Scale Scores
Scenario 3: Imagine that you are not in a coma, but you develop a brain
disease (such as Alzheimer disease) that cannot be cured and renders We constructed a Surrogate Accuracy in Match-
you unable to recognize people or to speak. Assume that you can
feel pain. ing Patient Preferences Scale (SAMPPS) for each of
the three scenarios (SAMPPS-1 for permanent
622 15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8
coma, SAMPPS-2 for small chance, and SAMPPS-3 the SAMPPS-1 model to predict the SAMPPS-2
for severe dementia). This scale assigns a score of 1 and SAMPPS-3 scores.
when the surrogate's prediction correctly matches
the patient's preference for each scenario-treatment
pair. Mismatches are assigned a score of 0. The sum
Results
of the correct answers constitutes the SAMPPS
score. For each of the three scenarios, a perfect score
Participants
is 10 and a complete mismatch has a score of 0.
Interviews of both patient and surrogate were
Statistical Analysis completed for 75% of pairs that met study inclusion
Categorical variables were analyzed by using the criteria. Of interviews that were not completed,
chi-square test or the Fisher exact test. Two-tailed t- 51% were not completed because of patient refusal;
tests were used for paired continuous variables. 25% were not completed because of patient refusal
Analysis of variance was used to compare continu- to allow surrogate contact; 15% were not completed
ous variables across multiple groups. Differences be- because of surrogate refusal; and 9% were not com-
tween surrogate and patient demographic character- pleted for other reasons, chiefly our inability to
istics were examined by using the McNemar test for arrange for a surrogate interview within 48 hours of
paired comparisons. The McNemar test was also the patient interview. Compared with participants,
used to assess any pattern in mismatches between nonparticipants were older (68 and 60 years of age;
patient and surrogate preferences. Differences in P< 0.001), more likely to be female (65% and
patient preferences for treatment according to mo- 39%; P < 0.001), more likely to receive Medicare
dality or scenario were assessed by using the Coch- (24% and 10%; P < 0.001), and more likely to be in
ran Q test. the chronic obstructive pulmonary disease or gen-
Although K scores have often been reported in eral medicine groups (P < 0.001). Participants and
similar studies, we do not report them because of nonparticipants did not differ significantly by ethnic
concern about the appropriateness of K as a test of group. Patients and surrogates were interviewed on
surrogate accuracy (11, 22), and the "paradox of the same day in all but 31 cases.
kappa" (23). We presented elsewhere an alternative Surrogates were younger, better educated, more
to K for measuring surrogate accuracy (24). likely to be married, and more likely to be female
than patients (Table 2). They were likely to be
Factors Associated with the Accuracy of spouses (57%) or adult children (22%). The surro-
Substituted Judgments gate that we interviewed, chosen according to Mary-
A skewed distribution was found for SAMPPS land's legal hierarchy, was the person the patient
scores. The Friedman two-way analysis of variance would have chosen (the patient's preferred surro-
was used to compare SAMPPS scores across scenar- gate) in 90% of cases. Patients with AIDS were the
ios, and the Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of vari- youngest, the most likely to be of minority ethnicity,
ance was used to compare SAMPPS-1 scores across the most likely to have not graduated from high
diagnoses. school, and the most likely to lack private insurance
A binary logit model (25) was constructed using (Table 3).
the 10 binary items (correct/incorrect) per patient-
surrogate pair of the SAMPPS-1 score. Indepen- Substituted Judgments
dent variables were screened for association with Surrogate accuracy did not differ significantly ac-
SAMPPS-1 score by using the Spearman rank cor- cording to whether patients were interviewed on the
relation and Kruskal-Wallis tests, as appropriate. same day (SAMPPS-1 score, 7.4) or on different
Variables that did not contribute significantly to the days (SAMPPS-1 score, 7.0) (P > 0.2). Overall, sur-
multivariate model were eliminated in a stepwise rogates correctly predicted the wishes of patients for
manner. When diagnostic categories were stratified particular treatments in specific scenarios in 66% of
by age (>65 years or <65 years), the log odds instances. Surrogates were better at predicting pa-
differed significantly by age group for patients with tients' wishes for more invasive procedures. For ex-
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, but no such interac- ample, in the permanent coma scenario, surrogates
tion was found between education and insurance. were correct about ventilator care in 84% of in-
Model diagnostics to identify influential observa- stances and about cardiopulmonary resuscitation in
tions were examined (26), revealing 9 outliers that 79% of instances compared with 57% of instances
were subsequently removed from the final model. for phlebotomy and 62% of instances for chest
Thus, the final model reports on only 291 patient- roentgenography (P < 0.001).
surrogate pairs. The model was validated by using As shown in Figure 1, accuracy scores did not
the independent variables found to be significant in differ significantly according to patient diagnosis,
15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8 623
Table 2. Characteristics of 300 Study Patients and thought that the patient would want the treatment,
300 Surrogates*
and only 36% of these patients did want it.
Characteristic Patient Surrogate P Value
Mistaken Substituted Judgments
Mean age (range), y 60(22-84) 55(18-83) <0.001t
Women, % 39.0 73.3 <0.001* For most modes of treatment, when the surro-
White persons, % 72.7 72.3 NS* gates were wrong, they were no more likely (by the
Marital status, %
Married 58.7 75.3 McNemar test) to state that the patient would want
Widowed, separated, or divorced 26.7 12.0 treatment when the patient did not want it than
Never married or living with
significant other 14.7 12.7 <0.001* they were to state that the patient would not want
Education, % treatment when the patient did want it. The only
Less than high school 22.0 14.7
High school graduate or some exceptions across all three scenarios were the less
college 50.3 53.7 invasive procedures of nasotracheal suctioning,
College graduate or more than
college 27.7 31.7 <0.05* phlebotomy, and chest roentgenography. In the per-
Religion, % manent coma scenario, for example, the McNemar
Protestant 56.7 58.7
Catholic 27.0 28.3 statistic was significant only for suctioning (P =
Jewish, other, or none 16.3 13.3 NS* 0.02), phlebotomy (P < 0.001), and chest roentgen-
Type of insurance, %
Private insurance, health ography (P < 0.001). For these treatments, the sur-
maintenance organization, rogate's tendency was to state that the patient would
or other 74.3
Medicare only 9.7 want treatment when the patient did not want it.
Medicaid only 16.0

* NS = not significant, Factors Associated with Accuracy


t Paired f-test.
* McNemar test. To sort out the weight and independence of fac-
tors associated with the accuracy of substituted
judgments, we developed a binary logit model by
but they were significantly better for the permanent using the responses for the permanent coma sce-
coma scenario (mean SAMPPS-1 score, 7.4) than nario as the dependent variable (Table 4). The sur-
for the small chance scenario (mean SAMPPS-2 rogates of 16 patients with amyotrophic lateral scle-
score, 6.2) and the severe dementia scenario (mean rosis who were older than 64 years of age did very
SAMPPS-3 score, 6.3) (P < 0.001 by Friedman two- well, with an average SAMPPS-1 score of 9.5 out of
way analysis of variance). 10 correct, whereas the surrogates of younger pa-
Surrogates generally chose for patients what they tients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and those
would choose for themselves (in 89% of instances of patients with congestive heart failure were only
for the permanent coma scenario, 65% of instances about half as accurate as the surrogates of patients
for the small chance scenario, and 72% of instances in the general medicine reference group. Accurate
for the dementia scenario), and this preference was substituted judgments were associated with patients
usually the same as the patient's preference. For who had private insurance, patients and surrogates
example, of the 51 surrogates who would choose who had high school diplomas, patients who had
ventilator support for themselves in the small spoken in detail about their wishes to surrogates,
chance scenario, 75% thought that the patient patients who expected to die in 10 years or less,
would want the treatment, and 61% of these pa- surrogates who never attended religious services,
tients did want it. Of the 249 surrogates who would and surrogates who had never had personal experi-
not want ventilator support themselves, only 37% ence with a ventilator. Factors that were not signif-

Table 3. Character istics and Diagnoses of 300 Patierits

Characteristic Diagnosis P Value


New York Heart Association AIDS Amyotrophic Unresectable Chronic Obstructive General Internal
Class III or IV Heart Failure and Lateral Sclerosis Non-Small-Cell Pulimonary Disease Medicine
No Possibility of Transplantation Lung Cancer

Mean age, y 64 37 55 63 67 73 <0.001*


Women, % 30 36 32 38 52 46 0.18t
Ethnic minority, % 36 62 8 18 12 28 <0.001t
High school graduate, % 84 68 96 70 74 76 0.01t
Protestant, % 58 66 70 40 46 60 0.02t
Private insurance, % 90 24 90 90 74 78 <0.001t

* Analysis of variance,
t Chi-square test.

624 15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8


Practices and Beliefs
Seventy-five percent of patients reported that
they had thought about advance directives, and 63%
reported having spoken to someone about end-of-
life issues. Only 21% of patients who had spoken to
someone about end-of-life issues reported that they
had spoken to a physician or nurse, and only 44%
had spoken with their legal surrogate. For patients
who had spoken about their wishes, hospitalization
was the cue that prompted this conversation only
4% of the time. Events in the lives of family or
friends (47%) were more commonly cited as rea-
Figure 1. Accuracy of substituted judgments according to diagno-
sis and scenario. Mean Surrogate Accuracy in Matching Patient Prefer- sons for such conversations than events in the pa-
ences Scale scores are given for three hypothetical scenarios. Responses of tients' own lives (36%).
50 surrogates of 50 patients with congestive heart failure (CHF), AIDS,
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), unresectable non-small-cell lung cancer Of patients who had not spoken with anyone
(CA), and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) were scored. Scores about end-of-life issues, 65% intended to do so.
of 50 general internal medicine (GIM) patients are also shown. Scores did
not differ significantly by diagnosis. Overall accuracy was higher for the Major barriers included not wanting to upset loved
permanent coma scenario than for the small chance and severe dementia ones (41%), not believing that they were ill enough
scenarios (P = 0.001 by Friedman two-way analysis of variance).
(32%), and uncertainty (30%).
Thirty-three percent of patients had living wills,
and 31% had durable powers of attorney for health
icant in univariate analysis or that became insignif- care. Patients in the general internal medicine
icant when other factors were adjusted for included group were less likely than patients with terminal
patient and surrogate sex, ethnicity, and religious diagnoses to have living wills (18%; P = 0.02) or
denominations; relationship of the surrogate to the durable powers of attorney for health care (16%;
patient; whether the patient had an advance direc- P = 0.01). Of patients who had living wills or dura-
tive; and the surrogates' degree of satisfaction with ble powers of attorney for health care, 13% and
their understanding of the patients' wishes. 15%, respectively, reported having learned about
To assess the validity of the final model, which these documents from a health care professional.
was developed using the SAMPPS-1 responses (per- These patients also learned about these documents
manent coma scenario) as the outcome variable, we from lawyers (19% and 22%, respectively), family
substituted SAMPPS-2 (small chance) and SAMPPS-3 and friends (29% and 19%), the media (31% and
(severe dementia) as the outcome variables. The 22%), and other sources (28% and 32%).
global fit of the model was significant for these two
scenarios, with chi-square values of 118.4 (P <
Table 4. Factors Associated with the >Vccuracy of
0.001) and 101.9 (P < 0.001), respectively, for - 2 Substituted Judgments in Peirmanent Coma
log likelihood. All independent variables from the Scenario: Binary Logit Model *
permanent coma model except for the surrogate's
personal experience with ventilator treatment and Factor Odds Ratio (95% CI)
type of patient insurance remained significant in at
Surrogate has experience with life-sustaining
least one of the two other models. treatmentst 0.41 (0.30-0.55)*
To discern whether surrogates who attended re- Surrogate does not go to church or temple 1.48(1.12-1.96)*
Patient has spoken about end-of-life care in
ligious services were "imposing their values" on pa- detail with surrogate§ 1.89(1.56-2.28)*
tients, we explored surrogates' preferences for Patient thinks that he or she will live for more
than 10 years 0.62(0.51-0.75)*
themselves according to their church- or temple- Patient has private insurance 1.35(1.06-1.73)*
going behavior. Although numbers were small, sur- Patient has high school diploma 1.73(1.38-2.19)*
Surrogate has high school diploma 1.50(1.16-1.92)*
rogates who attended religious services were not Patient diagnosis
significantly more likely to want ventilator treatment ALS and >65 years of age 5.40(2.14-13.60)*
ALS and <65 years of age 0.51 (0.36-0.71)*
for themselves than were surrogates who never at- New York Heart Association class III or IV
tended religious services (13% and 6% for the per- heart failure and no possibility of
transplantation 0.60(0.45-0.81)*
manent coma scenario; P = 0.18). However, surro- AIDS 1.07(0.77-1.48)
gates who attended religious services were significantly Unresectable non-small-cell lung cancer 1.22(0.89-1.67)
Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease 1.16(0.85-1.59)
more likely than nonattenders to think that the General internal medicine (reference group) 1.00
patients would want ventilator treatment in the per-
* ALS = amyotrophic lateral sclerosis,
manent coma (17% and 2%; P = 0.007), small t Cardiopulmonary resuscitation, intubation, or feeding t iibe treatment.
chance (46% and 30%, P = 0.04), and severe de- *P<0.01.
§ Surrogate and patient have spoken about preferences fc>r specific life-sustaining treat-
mentia (39% and 21%; P = 0.02) scenarios. ments.

15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8 625


terminal diagnoses judged their own life expectancy
to be less than 2 years. Twenty-six percent of pa-
tients thought that they would live longer than 10
years.
Preferences
Patient preferences varied according to treatment
and scenario. As shown in Figure 2, 13% of patients
would choose ventilator treatment if they were in a
permanent coma. However, three times as many
patients (39%) would choose such treatment if they
were told that they had even a less than 1% chance
of recovery to their present medical state, and 33%
would choose ventilator treatment if they were de-
mented rather than comatose.
Across all scenarios, more patients would choose
less invasive procedures (such as chest roentgenog-
raphy and phlebotomy) than more invasive proce-
dures (such as cardiopulmonary resuscitation and
ventilator support) (P < 0.001). For example, in the
permanent coma scenario, 20% of patients would
want cardiopulmonary resuscitation and 13% would
want ventilator support, whereas more than 30%
would want chest roentgenography and phlebotomy.
Surrogates were also less likely to prefer ventila-
tor support for themselves in the setting of perma-
nent coma (12%) than in the small chance (17%) or
the severe dementia (20%) scenarios (P = 0.003).
Across all three scenarios, surrogates were less
likely to choose ventilator support for themselves if
they were white or never attended religious services.

Discussion

Substituted judgment is an imperfect method for


assessing the true wishes of patients who lack deci-
sion-making capacity (3-11, 13). One way of grap-
pling with this imperfection would be to ascertain
the conditions under which substituted judgments
are likely to be inaccurate so that clinicians can
learn when to raise their index of suspicion that
Figure 2. Percentage of patients who would choose each inter-
vention according to scenario and intervention. Percentages are the patients may be at risk for erroneous substituted
proportion of patients (n = 300) who would choose the following interven- judgments. In addition, health care professionals
tions in three hypothetical scenarios: intensive care (ICU), ventilator treat-
ment (Vent), cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR), feeding tube placement might identify potentially remediable factors associ-
(Feed), surgery to fix a correctable problem (Surg), hemodialysis (Dial), ated with poor substituted judgments and design
esophagogastroduodenoscopy (EGD), nasotracheal suction (Suet), phlebot-
omy (Phlb), or chest roentgenography (CXR). targeted interventions to improve the process. Our
study takes initial steps toward these goals.

Overall, 27% of 249 patients with terminal diag- Substituted Judgments


noses thought that they would be cured. This belief Our results suggest that the substituted judg-
was especially common among patients with unre- ments made for patients with terminal diagnoses are
sectable non-small-cell lung cancer (54%) and not better than those made for general medical
AIDS (32%) compared with patients with chronic patients. These findings show the need for more
obstructive pulmonary disease (12%), amyotrophic careful interpretation of substituted judgments for
lateral sclerosis (16%), and congestive heart failure terminally ill patients and for improvements in the
(22%) (P < 0.001). Only 10% of the patients with process.
626 15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8
In addition to our pilot study (13), two other The surrogates of older patients with amyotro-
studies have attempted to elicit factors associated phic lateral sclerosis were remarkably accurate. Few
with the accuracy of substituted judgments. The patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis choose
study by Suhl and colleagues (8) was limited by ventilator support (29), and older patients with
small numbers (50 participants) and a lack of multi- amyotrophic lateral sclerosis report intense psycho-
variate analysis. The study by Layde and coworkers logical distress (30). Loved ones of older patients
(11) examined preferences only for cardiopulmo- with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis may be especially
nary resuscitation, explored a limited number of adept at reading these signals.
independent variables, sampled only hospitalized It is interesting that, when they were wrong, sur-
patients, and was unusual in that 74% of patients rogates were no more likely to say that a patient
wanted cardiopulmonary resuscitation. would want treatment when the patient actually did
Our study corroborates the results of previous not than they were to say that the patient would not
studies showing that substituted judgments are more want treatment when the patient actually did. This
accurate in the setting of an explicit discussion be- suggests that surrogate errors are more random
tween patient and surrogate about patient prefer- than systematic and may be more amenable to ed-
ences (8, 11, 13). In addition, we found associations ucational intervention.
with many other factors. For example, more accu-
rate substituted judgments are associated with better- Patient Beliefs, Practices, and Preferences
educated patients and surrogates. As other studies have shown (31, 32), although
As we reported elsewhere (13), the accuracy of most patients had thought about end-of-life issues,
substituted judgments is inversely related to the sur- relatively few had actually prepared advance direc-
rogate's church or temple attendance, independent tives or had spoken to their surrogates about their
of religious denomination. This counterintuitive wishes. Health care professionals were infrequently
finding does not seem to reflect an imposition of the source of patient knowledge about living wills or
the surrogates' wishes for themselves on the pa- durable powers of attorney.
tients. Surrogates who attend religious services are Terminally ill patients, especially those with un-
actually more likely to report that they do not want resectable non-small-cell lung cancer and AIDS,
the treatment for themselves but would choose it made estimates of their own survival prognoses that
for the patient. Surrogates who attend religious ser- far exceeded those in the published literature. De-
vices may be more likely to feel a sense of duty to spite this optimism, most patients (as other re-
choose treatment for their loved ones even if they searchers have found [30, 31]) preferred to forgo
would not choose it for themselves; may be moti- life-sustaining treatments. Our study provides a
vated by religiously mediated feelings of compas- more nuanced understanding of this phenomenon
sion, altruism, or guilt; or may be unable to "let by showing that patients with terminal diagnoses are
go." We did not ask questions that would allow us much more likely to forgo invasive procedures than
to explore these hypotheses. to forgo less invasive procedures.
Optimistic patient estimates of their own prog- The clinical scenario also influenced preferences.
noses for survival were associated with increased Almost three times as many terminally ill patients
preferences for treatments and inaccuracy in substi- would opt to continue ventilator support if they
tuted judgments. It is understandable that surro- were told that they had a less than 1% chance of
gates might err in their estimates of patient wishes recovery than if they were told that they would
if they have different views of the patient's life never recover from a coma. This finding raises ques-
expectancy. tions about the importance attached to suggesting
Surrogates of patients with congestive heart fail- any hope of survival and about how prognostic es-
ure who are not candidates for transplantation timates are framed (33).
made more inaccurate substituted judgments com-
pared with the reference group of general medical Limitations
patients. Predicting mortality is generally difficult Our study sampled only five terminal conditions
(27), but predictions may be even more difficult for and may not represent all patients with terminal
congestive heart failure than for other diagnostic conditions. We cannot be absolutely sure that "con-
categories because death in congestive heart failure taminating" discussions did not take place between
is usually sudden (28). Perhaps this greater prog- patients and surrogates, but our data show that
nostic uncertainty makes it more difficult for pa- accuracy did not differ significantly if patients and
tients to make choices or for surrogates to predict surrogates were interviewed on different days. Fi-
those choices. Alternatively, cardiologists may be nally, all scenario studies are somewhat artificial,
more reluctant to discuss end-of-life issues because and it is not known how substituted judgments
of this uncertainty or for other reasons. might change in actual clinical settings. However,
15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8 627
this matter can be studied only hypothetically be- Dr. Terry: Johns Hopkins University, Division of Pulmonary
Care, 5501 Bayview Circle, Asthma and Allergy Center Building,
cause one cannot know the minds of persons who Baltimore, MD 21224.
lack decision-making capacity. Dr. Weisman: Department of Health Management and Policy,
School of Public Health, University of Michigan, 109 South Ob-
servatory, Room M3138, Ann Arbor, MI 48109.
Conclusions and Implications Dr. Miller: Center for Nursing Research, Johns Hopkins School
of Nursing, 1830 East Monument Street, Room 233, Baltimore,
Reliance on surrogate decisions seems inescap- MD 21205.
able. Living wills are not universally used, and their Ms. Stallings: Johns Hopkins School of Hygiene and Public
specific provisions still require interpretation. If cli- Health, International Health/Human Nutrition, 615 North Wolfe
Street, Room 2041, Baltimore, MD 21205.
nicians remain committed to the belief that patient Dr. Vettese: Johns Hopkins School of Nursing, 1830 East Mon-
autonomy is not completely abrogated when pa- ument Street, Room 417, Baltimore, MD 21205.
tients lose their decision-making capacity, there re- Dr. Haller: Department of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University
School of Medicine, 1830 East Monument Street, Room 9061,
mains little choice but to continue to ask those who Baltimore, MD 21205.
know the patient best what they think the patient
would want. The information gathered in our study
suggests several testable strategies for grappling
with this imperfect process. References
The accuracy of substituted judgments is associ-
ated with multiple clinically apparent patient and 1. Substantive and procedural principles of decision making for incapacitated
patients. In: United States. President's Commission for the Study of Ethical
surrogate factors. By determining these factors, our Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research. Making Health
Care Decisions: A Report on the Ethical and Legal Implications of Informed
study helps clinicians know when to raise their index Consent in the Patient-Practitioner Relationship. Washington, DC: US Gov Pr
of suspicion that a patient's loved one may not be Office; 1982:177-82.
2. Buchanan A, Brock DB. Deciding for Others: The Ethics of Surrogate Deci-
representing the patient's true preferences. Under sion Making. New York: Cambridge Univ Pr; 1990.
these circumstances, the clinician may wish to take 3. Tomlinson T, Howe K, Notman M, Rossmiller D. An empirical study of
proxy consent for elderly persons. Gerontologist. 1990;30:54-64.
additional steps, such as probing more deeply or 4. Zweibel NR, Cassel CK. Treatment choices at the end of life: a comparison
of decisions by older patients and their physician-selected proxies. Gerontol-
involving other relatives. ogist. 1989;29:615-21.
Several testable strategies for intervention may 5. Uhlmann RF, Pearlman RA, Cain KC. Physicians' and spouses' predictions
of elderly patients' resuscitation preferences. J Gerontol. 1988;43:M 115-21.
help surrogates make more accurate substituted 6. Ouslander JG, Tymchuk AJ, Rahbar B. Health care decisions among elderly
judgments. For instance, health care professionals long-term care residents and their potential proxies. Arch Intern Med. 1989;
149:1367-72.
might actively urge patients to talk to their loved 7. Libbus MK. Russell C. Congruence of decisions between patients and their
potential surrogates about life-sustaining therapies. Image J Nurs Sch. 1995;
ones about their wishes for end-of-life care. Substi- 27:135-40.
tuted judgments may be improved if clinicians have 8. Suhl J, Simons P, Reedy Tf Garrick T. Myth of substituted judgment.
Surrogate decision making regarding life support is unreliable. Arch Intern
more frank discussions with terminally ill patients Med. 1994;154:90-6.
about their prognoses. These interventions might 9. Hare J, Pratt Cf Nelson C. Agreement between patients and their self-
selected surrogates on difficult medical decisions. Arch Intern Med. 1992;152:
take place in physicians' offices, in community set- 1049-54.
10. Seckler AB, Meier DEf Mulvihill M, Paris BE. Substituted judgment: how
tings, or in support groups for patients with terminal accurate are proxy predictions? Ann Intern Med. 1991;115:92-8.
diagnoses. Because surrogate attendance at religious 11. Layde PM, Beam CA, Broste SK, Connors AF Jr, Desbiens N, Lynn J, et
al. Surrogates' predictions of seriously ill patients' resuscitation preferences.
services is associated with inaccurate substituted Arch Fam Med. 1995;4:518-23.
judgments, community education efforts designed to 12. Baergen R. Revising the substituted judgment standard. J Clin Ethics. 1995;
6:30-8.
improve the quality of surrogate decision making 13. Sulmasy DP, Haller K, Terry PB. More talk, less paper: predicting the
accuracy of substituted judgments. Am J Med. 1994;96:432-8.
should perhaps target churches and temples in par- 14. Madsen BK, Hansen JF, Stokholm KH, Brons J. Husum D, Mortensen
ticular (34). Further study is needed to ascertain LS. Chronic congestive heart failure. Description and survival of 190 consec-
utive patients with a diagnosis of chronic congestive heart failure based on
whether any of the aforementioned strategies will clinical signs and symptoms. Eur Heart J. 1994;15:303-10.
be effective in improving substituted judgments. 15. Jacobson LP, Kirby AJ, Polk S, Phair JP, Besley DR, Saah AJ, et al.
Changes in survival after acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS): 1984-
1991. Am J Epidemiol. 1993;138:952-64.
Acknowledgments: The authors thank Ruth Faden and Edmund 16. Gay PC, Westbrook PR, Daube JR, Litchy WJ, Windebank AJ, Iverson
Pellegrino for their advice during the design and analysis of this R. Effects of alterations in pulmonary function and sleep variables on survival
project and Stephanie Poe and Andrea Kalfoglou for interview- in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Mayo Clin Proc. 1991;66:686-
94.
ing patients.
17. Rossing TH, Rossing RG. Survival in lung cancer. An analysis of the effects
of age, sex, resectability, and histopathologic type. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1992;
Grant Support: By the National Institute for Nursing Research 126:771-7.
(NIH-RO1NR03045-01A1). Dr. Sulmasy is supported by a Soros 18. Burrows B, Earle RH. Prediction of survival in patients with chronic airway
Faculty Scholar Award of the Project on Death in America. obstruction. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1969;99:865-71.
19. Dubois P, Jamart J, Machiels J, Smeets F, Lulling J. Prognosis of severely
hypoxemic patients receiving long-term oxygen therapy. Chest. 1994; 105:
Requests for Reprints: Daniel P. Sulmasy, OFM, MD, PhD, Cen- 469-74.
ter for Clinical Bioethics, Room 236, Building D, Georgetown 20. Maryland Annotated Code. Health-General, 5-605 C.
University Medical Center, Washington, DC 20007; e-mail 21. Emanuel LL, Emanuel EJ. The medical directive. A new comprehensive
sulmasyd@gunet.georgetown.edu. advance care document. JAMA. 1989;261:3288-93.
22. Coughlin SS, Trock B, Criqui M H , Pickle LW, Browner D, Tefft MC. The
logistic modeling of sensitivity, specificity, and predictive value of a diagnostic
Current Author Addresses: Dr. Sulmasy: Center for Clinical Bio- test. J Clin Epidemiol. 1992;45:1-7.
ethics, Room 236, Building D, Georgetown University Medical 23. Feinstein AR, Cichetti DV. High agreement but low kappa: I. The problems
Center, Washington, DC 20007. of two paradoxes. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990;43:543-9.

628 15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8


24. Sulmasy DP, Terry PB, Weisman CS, Miller DJ, Stallings R. Vettese MA, 30. McDonald ER, Wiedenfeld SA, Hillel A, Carpenter CL, Walter RA.
et al. End-of-life substituted judgments: sensitivity, specificity, and predictive Survival in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. The role of psychological factors.
value [Abstract]. J Gen Intern Med. 1997; 12 (Suppl 1):105. Arch Neurol. 1994;51:17-23.
25. Breslow NE, Day NE. Fitting models to grouped data. In: Davis W, Breslow 31. Emanuel LL, Barry MJ, Stoeckle JD, Ettelson LM, Emanuel EJ. Advance
NE. Statistical Methods in Cancer Research, v 2. Lyon: International Agency directives for medical care—a case for greater use. N Engl J Med. 1991;324:
for Research on Cancer; 1987:120-76. 889-95.
26. Pregibon D. Logistic regression diagnostics. Annals of Statistics. 1981;9:705-24. 32. Gamble ER, MacDonald PJ, Lichstein PR. Knowledge, attitudes, and be-
27. Lynn J, Harrell F Jr, Cohn F, Wagner D, Connors AF Jr. Prognoses of havior of elderly persons regarding living wills. Arch Intern Med. 1991; 151:
seriously ill patients on the days before death: implications for patient care 277-80.
and public policy. New Horiz. 199;5:56-61. 33. Rybash JM, Roodin PA. The framing heuristic influences judgments about
28. Lynn J. Caring at the end of our lives [Editorial]. N Engl J Med. 1996;335: older and younger adults' decision to refuse medical treatment. Applied Cog-
201-2. nitive Psychology. 1989;3:171-80.
29. Moss AH, Casey P, Stocking CB, Roos RP, Brooks BR, Siegler M. Home 34. Voorhees CC, Stillman FA, Swank RT, Heagerty PJ, Levine DM, Becker
ventilation for amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients: outcomes, costs, and D. Heart, body, and soul: impact of church-based smoking cessation inter-
patient, family, and physician attitudes. Neurology. 1993;43:438-43. ventions on readiness to quit. Prev Med. 1996;25:277-85.

15 April 1998 • Annals of Internal Medicine • Volume 128 • Number 8 629

You might also like