Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 29

Vol. 74, No. 11 Whole No.

498, 1960
Psychological Monographs: General and Applied

THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN


BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS1
GEORGE SPERLING2
Harvard University

H o w much can be seen in a single brief The statement that more is seen than can
exposure ? This is an important prob- be remembered implies two things. First,
lem because our normal mode of seeing it implies a memory limit, that is, a limit
greatly resembles a sequence of brief ex- on the (memory) report. Such a limit on
posures. Erdmann and Dodge (1898) the number of items which can be given
showed that in reading, for example, the in the report following any brief stimulation
eye assimilates information only in the brief has, in fact, been generally observed; it is
pauses between its quick saccadic move- called the span of attention, apprehension,
ments. The problem of what can be seen or immediate-memory (cf. Miller, 1956b).
in one brief exposure, however, remains Second, to see more than is remembered
unsolved. The difficulty is that the simple implies that more information is available
expedient of instructing the observer of during, and perhaps for a short time after,
a single brief exposure to report what he the stimulus than can be reported. The
has just seen is inadequate. When complex considerations about available information
stimuli consisting of a number of letters are are quite similar, whether the information is
tachistoscopically presented, observers enig- available for an hour (as it is in a book
matically insist that they have seen more that is borrowed for an hour), or whether
than they can remember afterwards, that the information is available for only a frac-
is, report afterwards.3 The apparently tion of a second (as in a stimulus which
simple question: "What did you see ?" re- is exposed for only a fraction of a second).
quires the observer to report both what he In either case it is quite probable that for a
remembers and what he has forgotten. limited period of time more information
will be available than can be reported. It
1 This paper is a condensation of a doctoral is also true that initially, in both examples,
thesis (Sperling, 1959). For further details, the information is available to vision.
especially on methodology, and for individual data,
the reader is referred to the original thesis. It is a In order to circumvent the memory limita-
pleasure to acknowledge my gratitude to George A. tion in determining the information that
Miller and Roger N. Shepard whose support made
this researcli possible and to E. B. Newman, J. becomes available following a brief ex-
Schwartzbaum and S. S. Stevens for their many posure, it is obvious that the observer must
helpful suggestions. Thanks are also due to Jerome not be required to give a report which
S. Brunei' for the use of his laboratory and his exceeds his memory span. If the number of
tachistoscope during his absence in the summer
of 1957. This research was carried out under letters in the stimulus exceeds his memory
Contract AF 33(038)-14343 between Harvard Uni- span, then he cannot give a whole report
versity and the Operational Applications Labo- of all the letters. Therefore, the observer
ratory, Air Force Cambridge Research Center, Air must be required to give only a partial re-
Research Development Command.
port of the stimulus contents. Partial re-
2 Now at Bell Telephone Laboratories, Murray porting of available information is, of
Hill, New Jersey.
3 Some representative examples are: Bridgin course, just what is required by ordinary
(1933), Cattell (1883), Chapman (1930), Dallen- schoolroom examinations and by other
bach (1920), Erdmann and Dodge (1898), Glanville methods of sampling available information.
and Dallenbach (1929), Kulpe (1904), Schumann
(1922), Wagner (1918), Whipple (1914), Wil- An examiner can determine, even in a
cocks (1925), Woodworth (1938). short test, approximately how much the
GEORGE SPERLING

student knows. The length of the test is estimates have not been made of the total
not so important as that the student not be information available to the observer fol-
told the test questions too far in advance. lowing a brief exposure of a complex
Similarly, an observer may be "tested" on stimulus. Furthermore, instructions have
what he has seen in a brief exposure of a generally not been coded in such a way as
complex visual stimulus. Such a test re- to make it possible to control the precise
quires only a partial report. The specific
time at which they were presented. Con-
instruction which indicates which part of
sequently, the temporal course of available
the stimulus is to be reported is then given
only after termination of the stimulus. On information could not have been quantita-
each trial the instruction, which calls for tively studied. In the absence of precise
a specified part of the stimulus, is randomly data, experimenters have all too frequently
chosen from a set of possible instructions assumed that the time for wdiich informa-
which cover the whole stimulus. I>y re- tion is available to the observer corresponds
peating the interrogation (sampling) pro- exactly to the physical stimulus duration.
cedure many times, many different random Wundt (1899) understood this problem
samples can be obtained of an observer's and convincingly argued that, for extremely
performance on each of the various parts short stimulus durations, the assumption
of the stimulus. The data obtained thereby that stimulus duration corresponded to
make feasible the estimate of the total in- the duration for which stimulus informa-
formation that was available to the observer tion was available was blatantly false, but
from which to draw his report on the aver- he made no measurements of available
age trial.
information.
The time at which the instruction is given
determines the time at which available in- The following experiments were con-
formation is sampled. By suitable coding, ducted to study quantitatively the informa-
the instruction may be given at any time: tion that becomes available to an observer
before, during, or after the stimulus presen- following a brief exposure. Lettered stimuli
tation. Not only the available information were chosen because these contain a rela-
immediately following the termination of tively large amount of information per item
the stimulus, but a continuous function and because these are the kind of stimuli
relating the amount of information available that have been used by most previous in-
to the time of instruction may be obtained vestigators. The first two experiments are
by such a procedure. essentially control experiments; they at-
Many studies have been conducted in tempt to confirm that immediate-memory
which observers were required to give for letters is independent of the parameters
partial reports, that is, to report only on of stimulation, that it is an individual
one aspect or one location of the stimulus. characteristic. In the third experiment the
In prior experiments, however, the instruc- number of letters available immediately
tions were often not randomly chosen, and after the extinction of the stimulus is de-
the set of possible instructions did not termined by means of the sampling (partial
systematically cover the stimulus. The no- report) procedure described above. The
tions of testing or sampling were not ap- fourth experiment explores decay of avail-
plied.' .It is not surprising, therefore, (hat able information with time. The fifth
experiment examines some exposure param-
eters. Tn the sixth experiment a technique
'' The experiments referred to are (cf. Sperling,
1959) : Kiilpc (1904), Wilcocks (1925), Chapman which fails to demonstrate a large amount
(1932), Long, Henneman, and Reid (1953), Long of available information is investigated.
and Lee (1953a), Long and Lee (1953b), Long, The seventh experiment deals with the role:
Reid, and Garvey (1954), Lawrence and Coles of the historically important variable : order
(1954), Adams (1955), Lawrence and Laherge
(1956), liroadbent (1957a). of report.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS

General Method illustrates a SO-mscc. light impulse on a linear


intensity scale. The exposure time of 50 msec.
Apparatus. The experiments utilized a Gerbrands was used in all experiments unless exposure time
tachistoscope." This is a two-field, mirror tachisto- was itself a parameter. Preliminary experiments
scope (Dodge, 1907b), with a mechanical timer. indicated that, with the presentations used, ex-
Viewing is binocular, at a distance of about 24 posure duration was an unimportant parameter.
inches. Throughout the experiment, a dimly il- Fifty msec, was sufficiently short so that eye
luminated fixation field was always present. movements during the exposure were rare, and
The light source in the Gerbrands tachistoscope it could conveniently be set with accuracy.
is a 4-watt fluorescent (daylight) bulb. Two such Stimulus materials. The stimuli used in this
lamps operated in parallel light each field. The experiment were lettered 5x8 cards viewed at a
operation of the lamps is controlled by the micro- distance of 22 inches. The lettering was done
switches, the steady-state light output of the lamp with a Leroy No. 5 pen, producing capital letters
being directly proportional to the current. How- about 0.45 inch high. Only the 21 consonants
ever, the phosphors used in coating the lamp con- were used, to minimize the possibility of Ss inter-
tinue to emit light for some time after the cessation preting the arrays as words. In a few sets of cards
of the current. This afterglow in the lamp follows the letter Y was also omitted. In all, over 500
an exponential decay function consisting of two different stimulus cards were used.
parts: the first, a blue component, which accounts There was very little learning of the stimulus
for about 40% of the energy, decays with a time materials either by the .Ss or by the E. The only
constant which is a small fraction of a millisecond; learning that was readily apparent was on several
the decay constant of the second, yellow, component stimuli that had especially striking letter com-
was about IS msec, in the lamp tested. Fig. 1 binations. Except for the stimuli used for train-
ing, no .S" ever was required to report the same
part of any stimulus more than two or three times,
and never in the same session.
Figure 2 illustrates some typical arrays of
letters. These arrays may be divided into several
categories: (a) stimuli with 3, 4, 5, 6, or 7 letters
normally spaced on a single line; (b) stimuli with
six letters closely spaced on a single line (6-
massed) ; (c) stimuli having two rows of letters
with three letters in each row (3/3), or two rows
ID
_l
<

o K LB
RNF
< Y NX
Ul
z
-J
1
>- XMR J
1- XVNKH
</z>
z P NK P
UJ

TOR
LQDKKJ SR N
FZR

50 MILLISECONDS PER DIVISION


Fig. 1. A SO-millisecond light flash, such as was 7 1 VF
used in most of the experiments. (Redrawn from ZYVVFF XL53
a photograph of an oscilloscope trace) B 4 W7

5 Ralph Gerbrands Company, 96 Ronald Road, Fig. 2. Typical stimulus materials. Col. 1 : 3, 5,
Arlington 74, Massachusetts. 6, 6-masscd. Col. 2: 3/3, 4/4, 3/3/3, 4/4/4 L&N.
GEORGE SPERLING

of four letters each (4/4) ; (d) stimuli having tones. This was about three to four stimuli per
three rows of letters with three letters in each minute.
row (3/3/3). The stimulus information, calculated Each of the first four and last two sessions
in bits, for some of the more complex stimuli is began with and/or ended with a simple task:
26.4 bits (6-lettcrs, 6-massed, 3/3), 35.1 bits (4/4), the reporting of all the letters in stimuli of 3,
and 39.5 bits (3/3/3). 4, 5, and 6 letters. This procedure was under-
tn addition to stimuli that contained only letters, taken in addition to the usual runs with these
some stimuli that contained both letters and num- stimuli to determine if there were appreciable
bers were used. These had eight (4/4 CAN, learning effects in these tasks during the course
35.7 bits) and twelve symbols (4/4/4 L&N, 53.6 of the experiment and if there was an accuracy
bits), respectively, four in each row. Each row had decrement (fatigue) within individual sessions.
two letters and two numbers—the positions being Very little improvement was noted after the second
randomly chosen. The .9 was always given a sample session. This observation agrees with previous
stimulus before L&N stimuli were used and told reports (Whipple, 1914). There was little differ-
of the constraint above. He was also told that ence between the beginning and end of sessions.
O when it occurred was the number "zero" and was Scoring and tabulation of results. Every report
not considered a letter. Calculated with these con- of all ,9s was scored both for total number of
straints, the information in each row of Tour letters in the report which agreed with letters in
letters and numbers (17.9 bits) on such a card the stimulus and for the number of letters reported
is nearly equal to the information in a row of four in their correct positions. Since none of the pro-
randomly chosen consonants (17.6 bits), even cedures of the experiments had an effect on cither
though there are different kinds of alternatives in of these scores independently of the other, only
each case. the second of these, letters in the correct position,
is tabulated in the results. This score, which takes
Subjects. The nature of the experiments made position into account, is less subject to guessing
it more economical to use small numbers of trained error," and in some cases it is more readily inter-
,9s rather than several large groups of untrained ,9s. preted than a score which does not take position
Four of the five 5s in the experiment were ob- into account. As the maximum correction for
tained through the student employment service. guessing would be about 0.4 letter for the 4/4/4
The fifth 5 (RNS) was a member of the faculty (12-letter) material—and considerably less for all
who was interested in the research. Twelve ses- other materials—no such correction is made in the
sions were regularly scheduled for each S, three treatment of the data. In general, data were not
times weekly. tabulated more accurately than 0.1 letter.
Instructions and trial procedures. S was in- Data from the first and second sessions were
structed to look at the fixation cross until it was not used if they fell below an 5"s average per-
clearly in focus; then he pressed a button which formance on these tasks in subsequent sessions.
initiated the presentation after a 0.5-sec. delay. This occurred for reports of five and of eight
This procedure constituted an approximate be- (4/4) letters for some ,9s. A similar criterion
havioral criterion of the degree of dark adaptation applied in later sessions for tasks that were
prior to the exposure, namely, the ability to focus initiated later. In this case, the results of the
on the dimly illuminated fixation cross. first "training" session(s) are not incorporated in
Responses were recorded on a specially prepared the total tabulation if they lie more than 0.5
response grid. A response grid appropriate to each letter from ,9's average in subsequent sessions.
stimulus was supplied. The response grid was placed
on the table immediately below the tachistoscope,
the room illumination being sufficient to write by. Experiment. 1 : Immediate-Memory
The .9s were instructed to fill in all the required
squares on the response grid and to guess when
When an S is required to give a complete
they were not certain. The .9s were not permitted (whole) report of all the letters on a briefly
to fill in consecutive X's, but were required to guess exposed stimulus, he will generally not re-
"different letters." After a response, .9 slid the
paper forward under a cover which covered his last
response, leaving the next part of the response 8 If there are a large number of letters in the
grid fully in view. stimulus, the probability that these same letters
will appear somewhere on the response grid, irre-
Scries of 5 to 20 trials were grouped together spective of position, becomes very high whether or
without a change in conditions. Whenever con- not .9 has much information about the stimulus.
ditions or stimulus types were changed, .9 was In the limit, the correspondence approaches 100%
given two or three sample presentations with the provided only that the relative frequency of each
new conditions or stimuli. Within a sequence of letter in the response matches its relative fre-
trials, S set his own rate of responding. The -9s quency of occurrence in the stimulus pack. If the
(except ND) preferred rapid rates. In some response is scored for both letter and position, then
conditions, the limiting rate was set by the E's the percent guessing correction is independent of
limitations in changing stimuli and instruction changes in stimulus size.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS

port all the letters correctly. The average massed, and 3/3-letters (see Fig. 2). When
number of letters which he does report the data for all ^s are pooled, the scores
correctly is usually called his immediate- for each of these three types of materials
memory span or span of apprehension for are practically the same: the range is 4.1-4.3
that particular stimulus material under the letters. The same constancy holds for
stated observation conditions. An expres- stimuli containing eight symbols. The aver-
sion such as immediate-memory span age number of letters correctly reported for
(Miller, 1956a) implies that the number each of the two different kinds of eight
of items reported by S remains invariant letter stimuli, 4/4, 4/4 L&N, is nearly the
with changes in stimulating conditions. The same: 4.4, 4.3, respectively.
present experiment seeks to determine to Most .S*s felt that stimuli containing both
what extent the span of immediate-memory letters and numbers were more difficult
is independent of the number and spatial than those containing letters only. Never-
arrangement of letters, and of letters and theless, only NJ showed an objective deficit
numbers on stimulus cards. If this inde- for the mixed material.
pendence is demonstrated, then the quali-
In conclusion, the average number of
fication "for that particular stimulus
material" may be dropped from the term correct letters contained in an S"s whole
immediate-memory span when it is used report of the stimulus is approximately
in these experiments.
Procedure. Ss were instructed to write all the AVERAGE
letters in the stimulus, guessing when they were ALL SUBJECTS
not certain. All 12 types of stimulus materials
were used. At least IS trials were conducted with
each kind of stimulus with each S. Each 6" was
given at least SO trials with the 3/3 (6-letter)
stimuli which had yielded the highest memory
span in preliminary experiments. The final run
made with any kind of stimulus was always a test lilt I i i i
of immediate-memory. This procedure insured that £>2
J?s were tested for memory when they were RNS / '
maximally experienced with a stimulus. Eio
>, /
Results. The lower curves in Fig. 3 r>
represent the average number of letters <c e'••• / '
IoE s^
correctly reported by each S for each ma- ; / * * ~ ^ -
terial.7 The most striking result is that
,-j-i i i iii i.„
immediate-memory is constant for each S, 1 4 5 6 B 9 10 12
being nearly independent of the kind of
stimulus used. The immediate-memory span
for individual vS*s ranges from approxi-
mately 3.8 for JC to approximately 5.2 for
NJ with an average immediate-memory
span for all ^s of about 4.3 letters. (The
upper curves are discussed later.)
The constancy which is characteristic of 3 4 5 6 8 9 10 12 3 4 5 6 8 9 10 12 ~
individual immediate-memory curves of NUMBER OF LETTERS IN STIMULUS
Fie. 3. "Channel capacity curves." Immediate-
Fig. 3 also appears in the average curve for memory and letters available (output information)
all Ss. For example, three kinds of stimuli as functions of the number of stimulus letters
were used that had six letters each: six (input information). Lower curves = immediate-
letters normally spaced on one line, 6- memory ( E x p . 1) ; upper curves = letters available
immediately after termination of the stimulus;
diagonal lines = maximum possible score (i.e.,
7 See Sperling (1959) f o r tables giving the input = output). Code: X = letters on one line;
numerical values of all points appearing in this and + = 6-massed; o = 3/3, 4/4, 5 / 5 ; A = 3 / 3 / 3 ;
in all other figures. • = 4/4 L&N, 4/4/4 L&N.
6 GEORGE SPERLING

equal to the smaller of (a) the number of exposure. The main result is that exposure
letters in the stimulus or (b) a numerical duration, even over a wide range, is not an
constant—the span of immediate-memory— important parameter in determining the
which is different for each .S". The use of number of letters an S can recall correctly.
the term immediate-memory span is there- Both individually and as a group, S's show
fore justified within the range of materials no systematic changes in die number of
studied. This limit on the number of letters letters correctly reported as the exposure
that can be correctly reported is an individ- duration was varied from 0.015 to 0.500 sec.
ual characteristic, but it is relatively similar The invariance of the number of letters
for each of the five .S"s of the study. reported as a function of exposure dura-
tions up to about 0.25 sec. for the kind of
Experiment 2: Exposure Duration presentation used (dark pre- and post-
exposure fields) has long been known
The results of Experiment 1 showed that, (Schumann, 1904).
regardless of material, .9s could not report
more than an average of about 4.5 items Experiment 3: Partial Report
per stimulus exposure. Jn order to deter-
mine whether this limitation was a peculiar Experiments 1 and 2 have demonstrated
characteristic of the short exposure dura- the span of immediate-memory as an in-
tion (0.05 sec), it was necessary to vary variant characteristic of each .9. In Experi-
the exposure duration. ment 3 the principles of testing in a
Procedure. As in the previous experiment, .S's perceptual situation that were advanced in
were instructed to report all the letters in the the introduction are applied in order to
stimulus. The stimuli were six letter cards (3/3). determine whether S has more information
NJ, who was able to report more thanfivecorrect
letters in Experiment 1, was given 4/4 stimuli in available than he can indicate in his limited
order to make a possible increment in his accuracy immediate-memory report.
of responding detectable. The Ss were given 10 The S is presented with the stimulus as
trials in each of the four conditions, .015-, .050-, before, but he is required only to make a
.150- (.200-), .500-scc. exposure duration, in the
order above, hi a later session, additional trials partial report. The length of this report
were conducted at .015-sec. exposure as a control is four letters or less, so as to lie within
for Experiment 5. These trials are averaged with .V's immediate-memory span. The instruc-
the above data. tion that indicates which row of the stimulus
Results and discussion. Figure A illus- is to be reported is coded in the form of a
trates the number of letters correctly re- tone. The instruction tone is given after the
ported as a function of the duration of visual presentation. The .9 docs not know
until he hears the tone which row is called
for. This is therefore a procedure which
samples the information that 5" has avail-
able after the termination of the visual
stimulus.
Procedure. Initially, stimulus materials having
only two lines were used, that is, 3/3 and 4/4. The
.9 was told that a tone would be sounded, that this
tone would come approximately simultaneously
with the exposure, and that it would be either a
high tone (2500 cps) or a low tone (250 cps).
I f it were a high tone, he was to write only the
upper row of the stimulus; if a low tone, only
the lower row. He was then shown a sample card
of 3/3 letters and given several high and low
tones. It was suggested that he keep his eyes
.015 .05 fixated on the fixation point and be equally pre-
EXPOSURE DURATION (SEC) pared for either tone. \t would not be possible to
Fig. 4. Letters correctly reported as a function outguess the E who would be using a random
of exposure duration. sequence of tones.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS

The tone duration was approximately O.S sec. Results. The development of the final,
The onset of the tone was controlled through stable form of the behavior is relatively
the same microswitch that controlled the off-go of
the light, with the completion of a connection from rapid for 5s giving partial reports. The
an audio-oscillator to the speaker. Intensity of the average for all 5s after 30 trials (first
tone was adjusted so that the high (louder) tone session) with the 3/3 stimuli was 4.5; on
was "loud but not uncomfortable." the second day the average of 30 more
In each of the first two sessions, each 5* re- trials was S.l. On the third day 5s aver-
ceived 30 training trials with each of the materials aged 5.6 out of a possible six letters. Most
3/3, 4/4. In subsequent sessions 5s were given
series of 10 or more "test" trials. Later, a third, of the improvement was due to just one 5 :
middle (650 cps) tone was introduced to cor- N D who improved from 2.9 to 5.8 letters
respond to the middle row of the 3/3/3 and 4/4/4 available. I n the 3 / 3 / 3 stimulus training,
stimuli. The instructions and procedure were es- all 5s reached their final value after the
sentially the same as before.
initial 40 trials on the first day of training.
In any given session, each tone might not occur
with equal frequency for each type of stimulus. The considerable experience 5s had ac-
Over several sessions, usually two, this unequal quired with the partial reporting procedure
frequency was balanced out so that an 5* had an at this time may account for the quick
exactly equal number of high, medium, and low stabilization. N J , whose score was 7.7
tones for each material. If an 5 "misinterpreted" letters available on the first 20 trials, was
the tone and wrote the wrong row, he was asked
to write what he could remember of the correct given almost 150 additional trials in an
row. Only those letters which corresponded to the unsuccessful attempt to raise this initial
row indicated by the tone were considered. score.
Treatment of the Data. In the experiments I n Fig. 3 the number of letters available
considered in this section, S is never required to
report the whole stimulus but only one line of a as a function of the number of letters in
possible two or three lines. The simplest treatment the stimulus are graphed as the upper
is to plot the percentage of letters correct. This, curves. For all stimuli and for all 5s, the
in fact, will be done for all later comparisons. available information calculated from the
The present problem is to find a reasonable meas- partial report is greater than that contained
ure to enable comparison between the partial in the immediate-memory report. Moreover,
report and the immediate-memory data for the same
stimuli. The measure, percent correct, does not from the divergence of the two curves it
describe the results of the immediate-memory seems certain that, if still more complex
experiments parsimoniously. In Experiment 1 it stimuli were available, the amount of avail-
was shown that 5s report a constant number of able information would continue to increase.
letters, rather than a constant percentage of
letters in the stimulus. The measure, number of The estimate above is only a lower bound
letters correct, is inappropriate to the partial re- on the number of letters that 5s have avail-
port data because the number of letters which 5" able for report after the termination of the
reports is limited by the E to at most three or stimulus. A n upper bound cannot be ob-
four. The most reasonable procedure is to treat
tained from experiments utilizing partial
the partial report as a random sample of the
letters which the 5 has available. Each partial reports, since it may always be argued that,
report represents a typical sample of the number with slightly changed conditions, an im-
of letters 5 has available for report. For ex- proved performance might result. Even the
ample, if an 5 is correct about 90% of the time lower-bound measurement of the average
when he is reporting three out of nine letters, then
he is said to have 90% of the nine letters—about available information, however, is twice
eight letters—available for partial report at the as great as the immediate-memory span.
time the instruction tone is given, The immediate-memory span for the 4 / 4 / 4
(12-letters and numbers) stimuli ranges
In order to calculate the number of available
letters, the average number of letters correct in from 3.9 to 4.7 symbols for the 5s, with
the partial report is multiplied by the number of an average of 4.3. Immediately after an
equiprobable (nonoverlapping), partial reports. If exposure of the 4 / 4 / 4 stimulus material,
there are two tones and two rows, multiplication the number of letters available to the 5s
is by 2.0; if three, by 3.0. As before, only the
ranged from 8.1 ( N D ) to 11.0 ( R O R ) ,
number of correct letters in the correct position is
considered. with an average of 9.1 letters available.
8 GEORGE SPERLING

This number of letters may be transformed randomly. The advantages of this procedure are
into the bits of information represented by (a) optimal performance is most likely in each
delay condition, if .S" is prepared for that precise
so many letters. For the 4 / 4 / 4 (12-letters condition (cf. Klemmer, 1957), (b) minimizing
and numbers) material, the average number delay changes makes possible a higher rate of
of bits available, then, is 40.6, with a range stimulus presentations. On the other hand, a
from 36.2 to 49.1 (out of a possible 53.6 random sequence of instruction tone delays would
make it more likely that 5 was "doing the same
bits). These figures are considerably higher thing" in each of the different delay conditions.
than the usual estimates. For example, in
The sequence in which the different delay con-
a recent review article Quastler (1956) ditions followed each other was chosen either as
writes: " A l l experimental studies agree that given above (ascending series of delay con-
that man can . . . assimilate less than 25 ditions) or in the reverse order (descending
bits per glance" (p. 32). The data obtained series). Within a session, a descending series
always followed an ascending series and vice versa,
in Experiment 3 not only exceed this maxi- irrespective of the stimulus materials used. At
mum, but they contain no evidence that the least two ascending and two descending series of
information that became available to the delay conditions were run with each 5" and with
i's following the exposure represented a each material after the initial training (Experi-
ment 3) with that material. This number of trials
limit of " m a n " rather than a maximum insures that for each S there are at least 20 trials
determined by the limited information con- at each delay of the indicator tone.
tained in the stimuli which were used.
Results and discussion. The development
of the typical behavior is illustrated by the
Experiment 4 : Decay of Available S, ROR, in Figs. 5a, b, c. Figure 5a shows
Information ROR's performance in a single session, the
first posttraining session. The upper and
Part 1 : Development of Strategies of lower curves represent the ascending" and
Observing descending series of tone instruction delays,
I t was established in Experiment 3 that respectively. The arrows indicate the order.
more information is available to the .Js Although each point is based upon only
immediately after termination of the five trials, the curves are remarkably similar
stimulus than they could report. I t remains and regular. Clearly, most of the letters
to determine the fate of this surplus in- in excess of ROR's memory span are for-
formation, that is, the "forgetting curve." gotten within about 0.25 sec. The rapid for-
The partial report technique makes possible getting of these letters justifies calling this a
the sampling of the available information at short-term memory and accounts for the
the time the instruction signal is given. By fact that it may easily be overlooked under
delaying the instruction, therefore, decay of less than optimal conditions. I n the follow-
the available information as a function of ing session (Fig. 5b) the descending series
time will be reflected as a corresponding
decrease in the accuracy of the report.
Procedure. The principal modification from the
preceding experiment is that the signal tone, which
indicates to the S which row is to be reported, is
given at various other times than merely "zero
delay" following the stimulus off-go. The follow-
ing times of indicator tone onset relative to the
stimulus were explored: 0.0S sec. before stimulus
onset (-0.10 sec), ±0.0-, +0.1S-, +0.30-, +0.S0-, .150 .30 .50 -.10 O .150 .30
+1.0-sec. delays after stimulus off-go. The stimuli DELAY OF INSTRUCTION TONE (SEC.)
used were 3/3, 4/4. Fjg. 5. Partial report of eight (4/4) letters,
The 5s were given five or more consecutive three consecutive sessions. Arrows indicate the
trials in each of the above conditions. These trials sequence in which conditions followed within a
were always preceded by at least two samples in session. The light flash is shown on same time
order to familiarize S with the exact time of scale at lower left of each figure. Bar at right
onset. The particular delay of the instruction indicates immediate-memory for this material. One
tone on any trial was thus fixed rather than chosen subject (ROR).
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS

was given first. Here orderly behavior dis- is highly similar to that of Fig. 5a. A run
integrates. I n the third session (Fig. Sc) with 3/3 stimuli was interposed between
two modifications were introduced: (a) the the ascending and descending series shown
number of trials in each delay condition was in Fig. 5c. Since the guessing procedures
increased to eight and (b) a new delay con- were easily sufficient for a nearly perfect
dition was given—namely, a signal tone com- score with 3/3 materials, when the descend-
ing 0.05 sec. before the stimulus onset. The ing series of delay conditions was run, R O R
curves are again regular, but they are ob- continued guessing. While guessing was
viously different for the ascending and de- advantageous at the long delays, at the short
scending series. For the session indicated in
delays it was a decided disadvantage.
Fig. 5c, an analysis of the errors by position
shows that in the ascending series the errors Figure 6 illustrates the performances of
are evenly split between the top and bottom RNS, a sophisticated observer. R N S de-
rows of the stimulus; in the descending scribed the two strategies (equal attention,
series, the top row is favored 3 : 1 . guessing) to E. I n accordance with the
ROR's performance is analyzable in instructions to do as well as he could, R N S
terms of two kinds of observing behavior said that he switched from the first to the
(strategies) which the situation suggests. second strategy at delays longer than 0.15
l i e may follow the instruction, given by E sec. Thus in the three short delay con-
prior to training, that he pay equal attention ditions, R N S divided his errors almost
to each row. I n this case, errors are evenly evenly (19:21) between the favored (top)
distributed between rows. Or, he may try and the unfavored rows; in the two longer
to anticipate the signal by guessing which delays, errors were split 4:26. The dip in
instruction tone will be presented. I n this the curve indicates that R N S did not switch
case, ^ is differentially prepared to report strategies quite as soon as he could have,
one row. I f the signal and S's guess coin- for optimal performance. Such a dip is
cide, S reports accurately; if not, poorly.
Such a guessing procedure would lead to
the variability observed in Fig. 5b. On the
other hand, S may prefer always to antici-
pate the same row—in the case of R O R
( l u g . 5c, descending series), the top row. l±J
This would again allow reliable scores, pro- _J
CD
vided only that there are an equal number <
of instruction signals calling for the top and
bottom rows. Concomitantly, a differential
accuracy of report for the two rows is I
observed. (ROR's preference for the top in
a:
row is again prominent in Experiment 7, UJ
Figs. 10, 11.)
Equal attention responding is initially uj 2-
reinstated on the third day. The obvious
change in procedure which is responsible
is the introduction of a tone 0.05 sec. before
the stimulus onset (—0.10 sec. "after" its .10 0 .15 .30 .50
termination). This signal is sufficiently in
DELAY OF INSTRUCTIONS (SEC.)
advance of the stimulus so that perfect
responding is possible by looking at only Fig. 6. Partial report of eight (4/4) letters,
the row indicated by the signal tone. ROR last of three sessions. Arrows indicate the se-
scores 100%, both in this condition and in quence in which conditions followed within session.
The light flash is shown on same time scale at
the succeeding zero delay. The whole lower left. Bar at right indicates immediate-
(ascending series) decay curve of Fig. 5c memory for this material. ( R N S )
10 GEORGE SPERLING

characteristically seen in experiments of this ferential attention to a constant small part


kind. of the stimulus is less likely to be rein-
The other ^s exhibit similar curves.8 forced, the larger the stimulus. The use of
These are not presented, as the main fea- three tones instead of two diminishes the
tures have already been demonstrated by probability of guessing the correct tone.
ROR and RNS. In summary, the method 2. Training with instruction tones that
of delaying the instruction tone is a feasible begin slightly before the onset of the
one for determining" the decay of the short- stimulus. Jt is not necessary for S to guess
term memory contents, but experience with in this situation since he can succeed by
the difficult, long" delays causes a consider- depending upon the instruction tone alone.
able increase in the variability of 5"s' per- This situation not only makes equal attention
formance which is carried over even to the likely to occur, but differentially reinforces
short delay conditions. This has been at- it when it does occur.
tributed to ..Vs' change from an equal atten-
When delays of longer length are tested,
tion to a guessing strategy in observing the
priority should be given to an ascending
stimulus.0
series of delays so that S will, at the be-
ginning of a particular delay sequence, have
Part 2: Pinal Level of Performance a high probability of entering with the de-
The analysis of the preceding experiment sired observing behavior. This probability
has indicated that two distinct kinds of might be nearly 1.0 by interposing a series
observing behavior develop when the in- of trials on which the instruction is given
struction to report is delayed. The accuracy in advance of, or immediately upon, termi-
of report resulting from the first of these nation of the stimulus and requiring that .S"
behaviors (equal attention) is correlated perform perfectly on this task before he
with the delay of the instruction tone; it is can continue to the particular delay being
associated with the .9s initially giving equal tested.10 This tedious procedure was tried;
attention to all parts of the stimulus. The but, as it did not have an appreciable effect
accuracy of the other kind of report (guess- upon the results, it was discontinued. The
ing) is uncorrected with the delay of the problem is that J?s learn to switch between
instruction ; it is characterized by Ss' differ- the two modes of behavior in a small num-
ential preparedness for some part of the ber of trials.
stimulus (guessing). Equal attention ob- 3. The Jl may be able to gain verbal
serving is selected for further study here. control over Ss' modes of responding. In-
The preceding experiment suggests three itially, however, even i" cannot control his
modifications that would tend to make equal own behavior exactly. This suggests a
attention observing more likely to occur, limit to what E can do. For example, fre-
with a corresponding exclusion of guessing. quently >.9s reported that, although they had
1. The use of stimuli with a larger number tried to be equally prepared for each row,
of letters, that is, 3/3/3 and 4/4/4. Dif- after some tones they realized that they
had been selectively prepared for a par-
8 Sec Sperling (1959) for tables containing indi- ticular row. This comment was made both
vidual averages of all trials for each S at each when the tone and the row coincided and
delay of instruction tone : 3/3, 4/4 (more frequently) when they differed.
9 Increase in variability (with consequent decre-
ment in accuracy and/or speed) is not unusual
after difficult conditions. For example, Cohen and 10 Jt takes, on the average, a very large number
Thomas (1957) in a clinically oriented study have of trials for S , ~ to get 10 consecutive perfect trials
reported an exactly analogous "hysteresis" phe- even if he has 6 or 7 of 9 letters available or
nomenon in a study of discriminative reaction time. knows with 2/3 probability what the tone will be.
Hysteresis refers to the fact that, when the diffi- Success in this task within a reasonable time limit
culty of an experimental task is changed, the cor- demands a level of excellence reached only with
responding change in accuracy of response lags "equal attention" observing, as judged by the
behind the change in task. other criteria.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 11

Some verbal control is, of course, pos- ordinates are linearly related by the equa-
sible. An instruction that was well under- tion :
stood was: percent correct
100 X
You will see letters illuminated by aflashthat
quickly fades out. This is a visual test of your no. letters in stimulus — letters available
ability to read letters under these conditions, not
a test of your memory. You will hear a tone Each point is based on all the test trials in
during the flash or while it is fading which will the delay condition. The points at zero
indicate which letters you are to attempt to read. delay of instructions for NJ and JC also
Do not read the card until you hear the tone, include the training trials, as these Ss
[etc.].
showed no subsequent improvement.
The instruction was changed at the midway The data indicate that, for all kS's, the
point in the experiment. The 5 was no period of about one sec. is a critical one
longer to do as well as he could by any for the presentation of the instruction to
means, but was limited to the procedure report. If J5"s receive the instruction 0.05
described above. Part 2 of this experiment, sec. before the exposure, then they give
utilizing 9- and 12-letter stimuli, was carried accurate reports: 9 1 % and 82% of the
out with the three modifications suggested letters given in the report are correct for
above. the 9- and 12-letter materials, respectively.
These partial reports may be interpreted to
Results. The results for 3/3/3 and 4/4/4 indicate that the S's have, on the average,
letters and numbers are shown for each 8.2 of 9 and 9.8 of 12 letters available.
individual ^ in Figs. 7 and 8. The two However, if the instruction is delayed until
one sec. after the exposure, then the ac-
curacy of the report drops 32% (to 69%)
1
1 for the 9-letter stimuli, and 44% (to 38%)
AVERAGE A
-< 4 SUBJECTS
for the 12-letter stimuli. This substantial
- !\ decline in accuracy brings the number of
1 N \
1
- 1 o^. ^SK*° 1
1
1 J
• ,1 , ,
-.10 0 .15 .30*.50
!00 12 100
Io-
10 UJ
75 <K
A mo
I N °--o- o
50 •_

-.10 0 .15 .30 u -.10 0 .15 .30 .50


100 12r

i \

~I0 0 .15 .30 10 0 .15 .30 .50 -.10 0 .15 .30 .50 1.0 " " -.10 0 .15
DELAY OF INSTRUCTION TONE (SEC.) DELAY OF INSTRUCTION TONE (SEC.)
Fig. 7. Decay of available information: nine Fjg. 8. Decay of available i n f o r m a t i o n : twelve
(3/3/3) letters. Light flash is shown on same ( 4 / 4 / 4 ) letters and numbers. L i g h t flash is shown
time scale at lower left. Bar at right indicates on same time scale at lower left. B a r at right
immediate-memory for this material. indicates immediate-memory for this material.
12 GEORGE SPERLING

letters available very near to the number of and eight- (4/4) letter stimuli arc taken
letters that 5s give in immediate-memory from the two ascending delay series with
(whole) reports. each material that yielded monotonic re-
The decay curves are similar and regular sults. Figure 9 clearly highlights the sig-
for each 5 and for the average of all 5s. nificance of a precisely controlled coded
Although individual differences are readily instruction, given within a second of the
apparent, they are small relative to the stimulus off-go, for the comparison of
effects of the delay of the instruction. For partial and immediate-memory reports. One
example, when an instruction was given second after termination of the stimulus,
with zero delay after the termination of the accuracy of ROR's partial reports is no
the stimulus, the least accurate reports by longer very different from the accuracy of
any 5s are given by ND, who has 8.1 letters his whole reports.
available immediately after the termination
of the stimulus. With a one-sec. delay of Experiment 5: Some Exposure Parameters
instructions, the most accurate reports were
given by }C, who has only 5.1 letters avail- In Experiment 3 it was shown that the
able at this time. number of letters reported correctly is al-
most independent of the exposure duration
Tn Fig. 3, in which whole reports and over a range from 15 to 500 msec. It is
partial reports were compared, only that well known, however, that the relation be-
particular partial report was considered in tween the accuracy of report and the ex-
which the instruction tone followed the posure duration depends upon the pre- and
stimulus with zero delay. It is evident from post-exposure fields (Wundt, 1899).
this experiment that the zero delay instruc- In a technique developed in Helmholtz's
tion is unique only in that it is the earliest laboratory (Baxt, 1871) the informational
possible "after" instruction, but not because (stimulus) field is followed, after a variable
of any functional difference. delay, by a noninf ormational, homogeneous,
In Fig. 9, therefore, the 0.15-, 0.50-, and bright post-exposure field. Using this
1.00-sec. instruction delays are also plotted method, Baxt showed that the number of
for one 5, ROR. Data for the six- (3/3) reportable letters was a nearly linear func-
tion of the delay of the bright post-exposure
field.11
Other combinations of pre- and post-
exposure fields have also been tried (Dodge,
1907a). The usual tachistoscopic presenta-
tion utilizes gray pre- and post-exposure
fields (Woodworth, 1938). Baxt's pro-
cedure, however, is the most disadvan-
tageous for the observer. A similar
procedure was therefore selected, in order

1 x This important method was described by


Ladd (1899) and James (1890) in their textbooks,
but it is no longer well known. Consequently it
has been "rediscovered," most recently, by Lindslcy
and Emmons (19S8). Baxt (1871) intended that
the bright second field would interfere with the
lingering image of (he first (informational) field.
3 4 5 6 8 9 10 12 Unfortunately, the effect depends in a complex
NUMBER OF LETTERS IN STIMULUS way upon the intensity of the two fields. Derived
Fig. 9. Immediate-memory and available infor- time values must be used with caution. In some
mation. The parameter is the time at which avail- cases the Baxt technique may actually result in
able information is sampled (delay of instruction). no loss of legibility, the second field producing
Heavy line indicates immediate-memory for the a negative "afterimage" instead of merely inter-
same materials. One subject (ROR). fering with the positive image (cf. Footnote IS).
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 13
to study whole and partial reports in a TABLE 1
vastly different visual presentation from A Comparison of Response Accuracy with
that of the previous experiments. Two Different Post-Exposure Fields
Procedure. 1. 6"s were instructed to write all
the letters of the stimulus; 3/3 stimuli were used.
After several sample presentations of a stimulus Subject Exposure (sec.) Normal Baxt
card followed by a light post-exposure field, i"s
were given a random sequence of normal (pre-
and post-exposure fields dark) and Baxt (pre- RNS (0.015) 3.9 2.5
exposure dark, post-exposure field light) trials. (0.050) 4.0 2.2
The Baxt trials do not correspond exactly to the
presentation that Baxt used. In this experiment, ROR (0.015) 4.8 3.5
the post-exposure field is the same intensity as (0.050) 4.4 2.8
the stimulus (informational) field, whereas Baxt
usually used more intense post-exposure fields; ND (0.015) 3.8 1.9
also, the stimulus field always remains on until (0.050) 3.7 2.3
the onset of the post-exposure field, whereas Baxt
used a fixed five-msec. duration for the stimulus NJ (0.015) 5.1 2.4
field. The post-exposure field itself remains on (0.050) 5.4 3.4
for about one sec. The pre-exposure field is
always dark, as in all the previous experiments. JC (0.015) 4.1 2.7
Two exposure durations, 0.01S and 0.050 sec, were (0.050) 3.8 3.4
tested.
Mean (0.015) 4.3 2.6
2. Three .9s were tested with the Baxt presenta- (0.050) 4.3 2.8
tion of a 3/3/3 stimulus at an exposure duration
of 0.015 sec. The partial report procedure was Note.—Number of letters correctly reported. Whole report
used to determine the effects of the post-exposure of six (3/3) letter stimuli. Normal = pre- and poat-exposure
field on the number of letters available. fields dark; Baxt = pre-expoKure field dark, post-exposure
field white.
3. The same three Ss were run as their own
controls. The procedure was exactly the same as
in Paragraph 2 above except that the post-exposure as accurate as his reports in control pre-
field was normal (dark). sentations. R O R seemed to show improve-
Results. The complete results are given ment on successive Baxt trials. JC, another
in Tables 1 and 2. I n all tests, the Baxt S who seemed to show improvement, was
procedure reduces the response accuracy of given additional Baxt trials on which he
all Ss to about one-half of their normal continued to improve slowly. Unfortunately,
score. This finding' confirms the earlier it was unfeasible to determine the asymp-
studies. However, a linear relation between totic performance of these two 5s. Whether
exposure duration and the number of letters the difference in performance between
reported was not observed. The failure to ROR, and R N S and N D is attributable to
find a linear relation may be due to the some overt response, such as squinting or
previously mentioned differences between blinking, was not determined.
the presentations. Table 2 also enables the comparison of
0.015- and 0.050-sec. exposures of 3 / 3 / 3
For R N S and N D , the number of letters
stimuli. A decrease in exposure duration
available is nearly the same (about two)
has only a slight effect on the number of
in the partial report of 9-letter stimuli as
letters available. This suggests, as in the
the whole report of 6-letter stimuli. The
immediate-memory experiments, that the
fact that in both procedures the number of
duration of a tachistoscopic exposure is
letters given by 5"s is the same suggests
not as important a determinant of the num-
that a Baxt presentation reduces the num-
ber of letters available as the fields which
ber, or the length of time that letters are
follow the exposure.
available, and that it does not directly affect
the immediate-memory span.
Experiment 6: Letters and Numbers
ROR's partial reports of Baxt presenta-
tions are considerably more accurate than I n Experiment 3 partial reports were
those of the other 5"s, although they are not found to be uniformly more accurate than
14 GEORGE SPERLING

TABLE 2
A Comparison of Response Accuracy with Two Different Post-Exposure Fields

Delay of Instruction (sec.): 00 0. IS

Exposure Duration (sec): 0.015 0.05 0.015 0.05

Subject

RNS (N) 8.0 8.7 5.4 6.6


(B) 2.0 2.2

ROR (N) 8.6 8.9 8.3 8.5


(B) 6.3 5.4

ND (N) 7.3 7.0 5.8 6.4


(B) 2.2 1.7

Mean (N) 8.0 8.2 6.5 7.2


(B) 3.5 3.1

Note.—Number of letters available (fraction of letters correct in partial report X number of letters in stimulus). Stimuli: nine
(3/3/3) letters. (N) = normal (pre- and post-exposurefieldsdark), (B) = Baxt (pre-exposure field dark, post-exposure field
white).

whole reports. I n one case, stimuli of eight 4. Bottom only—Write only (he bottom row
letters were used and only one row of four in the following card(s). (4 trials)
5. Instruction tone—Write either letters or num-
letters was reported. Designating the letters bers as indicated by tone. Tone onset 0.05 sec.
to be reported by their location is only one before stimulus onset. (16 trials) ROR was also
of a number of possible ways. I n the follow- given additional trials at longer delay times.
ing experiment, a quite similar set of stimuli Results. The results are illustrated in
is used; each stimulus has two letters and Table 3. For purposes of comparison, the
two numbers in each of the two rows. The number of correct letters is multiplied by
partial report again consists of only four two when an instruction was used which
symbols, but these are designated either as required 5" to report only four of the eight
letters or as numbers rather than by row. symbols of the stimulus. This includes
i n addition, a number of controls which instructions given well in advance of the
arc also relevant to .Experiment 3 are con- stimulus. A l l measures, then, have 8.0 as
ducted. the top score and are thus equivalent within
Procedure. I. Training: The .9s were given a scale factor to percent correct measures.
practice trials with the instruction: "Write clown The range is 0-8 instead of 0-100. Scores
only the numbers if you hear a short pip (tone which are based on partial reports are
0.05-scc. duration) and only the letters if you therefore directly comparable to the partial
hear the long tone (0.50-sec. duration)." The report scores (letters available) obtained in
tones were then given with zero delay following
the stimulus off-go. The stimuli were 4/4 L&N. Experiments 3, 4, and S.
If. In the following session, tests were con- When stimuli consist of letters and num-
ducted with five different instructions: bers, but 5s report only the letters or only
1. Letters only—Instructions given well in ad- the numbers, then the SV partial reports
vance of stimulus to write only the letters in the arc only negligibly more accurate than their
following card(s). (8 trials)
whole reports of the same stimuli. The
2. Numbers only—Write only the numbers in
the following card(s). (8 trials) average number of letters available (cal-
3. Top only—Write only the top row in the culated from the partial report) is just 0.2
following card(s). (4 trials) letter above the immediate-memory span
THE INFORMATION' AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS IS

TABLE 3
Comparison of Five Procedures

Letters Numbers Average Instr. tone Immediate- One row


Subject only only L&N -0.10 memory only

RNS 5.0 4.5 4.8 4.3 4.6 7.3


ROR 6.5 6.5 6.5 6.3 4.5 7.3
ND 3.5 3.8 3.6 4.1 4.1 7.5
NJ 4.0 5.0 4.5 4.6 4.3 —
JC 3.3 4.0 3.6 3.4 4.1 8.0
Mean 4.5 4.8 4.6 4.5 4.3 —
Note.—Average letters and/or numbers available (fraction of letters-
-numbers—correct in partial report X number of symbols
ia stimulus). Stimuli: eight (4/4) lettcis and numbers,

for the same material. For practical pur- series, although checks with other 5s con-
poses, the partial report score is the same firmed this conclusion. Table 4 indicates
score that 5s would obtain if they wrote that two extra symbols are available to
all the letters and numbers they could (that ROR for report only when the tone is given
is, gave a whole report) but were scored before the stimulus, but not if it is given
only for letters or only for numbers, in- immediately after. It should be noted that
dependently by the experimenter. The par- the information in the instruction tone
tial report of letters only (or of numbers comes only after it has been on for 0.05 sec.
only) does not improve even when the in- A t this time it either continues or is termi-
struction is given long in advance verbally nated. The actual "instruction" is thus
instead of immediately before the exposure given 0.05 sec. after the tone onset. ROR
by a coded signal tone. therefore requires that the instruction be
The estimate of the number of available given within 0.05 sec. of the stimulus
letters and numbers which is obtained from termination if any benefit of the partial
the partial report of letters (or numbers) report procedure is to be retained.
only is also the same as the estimate that Whether the 5s would have shown im-
would be obtained if, on each trial, 5s wrote provement with a large amount of addi-
only one row—either the top or the bottom tional training in the partial report of letters
—according to their whim. Reporting only or numbers cannot be stated. Table 3 shows
one row of four letters and numbers is a that, when 5s are required in advance to
task at which the 5 s succeed with over 90% report only one row, this task is trivial. The
accuracy, liven if they are scored for the substantial advantage of partial reports of
whole stimulus, by arbitrarily reporting rows (report by position) over partial rc-
only one row they would still achieve a
score of almost 50% correct or almost four
letters available. This is why no delay TABLE 4
series were conducted. I f 5s had ignored Partial Reports of Letters or Numbers
the instruction to write only the letters (or
numbers) and had written only a single
Prior Delay of Instr. Tone Immedi-
row on each trial, they would have shown Subj. Verbal (sec.) ate
less than a 0.5 letter decrement, no matter Instr. -0.10 0.0 +0.25 Memory
what the delay of the instruction.
Only ROR showed a substantial improve- ROR 6.5 6.3 4.7 4.4 4.5
ment when reporting only the numbers (or
letters). He was the only 5 with whom it Note.—Average of symbols available (fraction of letters—
made sense to conduct a systematic delay numbers—correct in partial report X number of symbols in
stimulus). Stimuli: eight (4/4) letters and numbers.
16 GEORGE SPERLING

ports of numbers or letters (report by Procedure. The .S"s were instructed to write the
category) when the instruction is given row indicated by the tone (high, low) first, then
to write the other row. They were to try to get
verbally long in advance of the exposure as many total letters correct as possible, it being
is retained even when the instruction is of no importance in which particular row the
coded and given shortly after the exposure. correct letters might be. The instruction tone was
The failure in Experiment 6 to detect a given with 0.0-, .30- (or .50-), and 1.0-sec. delay
after the termination of the stimulus.
substantial difference in accuracy between
Controls. In addition to the trials with a high
partial reports of only letters (or only or low tone, two sets of 8 (or 10) trials were
numbers) and whole reports clearly illus- given with a neutral, middle tone. The instruction
trates that partial reports by position are was : "Write all the letters in any order you wish,
more effective for studying the capacity of but do not begin writing until you hear the tone."
The tone was sounded with 0.0-sec. delay follow-
short-term information storage than partial ing termination of the stimulus and also with
reports by category. 1.0-sec. delay. It bears repeating here that 5s
were not permitted just to mark X's but were
required to guess various letters.
Experiment 7: Order af Report
Results. Controls: The instruction which
Interpretations of the effects of instruc- required ^s to wait for 1.0 sec. before be-
tions upon the report following a single ginning to write their answer was ignored
brief visual exposure have often been con- by the ^s, since it was almost physically
cerned with cither the perceptual sensitizing impossible to begin writing sooner. Con-
effects of an instruction given before the sequently the two different controls—trials
exposure or with the importance of for- on which 5" was required to "wait" for 1.0
getting between the exposure and a post- sec. and trials on which 5* could begin his
exposure instruction to report. The decay report immediately—are grouped together.
curves of Experiment 4 include both of These data, which are almost exactly the
these effects. Previous studies, however, same as the memory span data (Experi-
have usually assumed the order in which ment 1), are presented on the far right in
the various parts (aspects, dimensions, etc.) Fig. 10.
of the stimulus are reported to be the sig- The Ss' responses on the control trials
nificant correlate of post-exposure for- are analyzed in terms of the correlation
getting. The possibility that information between the location of letters on the
might be well retained even though not stimulus and the accuracy of the report of
immediately reported has been mentioned these letters in the response. The symbols
(Broadbent, 1958), but experimental in- T and B above I-M in Fig. 10 represent the
vestigations of such an effect by an inde- percentage of the letters of the top and of
pendent variation of the order of report the bottom rows that 5s report correctly.
by Wilcocks (1925), Lawrence and Laberge The middle point is the average percentage
(1956), and Broadbent (1957a) have ap- of the letters of the top and bottom rows
parently shown otherwise. Broadbent that were correctly reported by 5"s. The
(1957a) has also shown a case in which middle point is therefore also the average
independent variation of the order of report percentage correct of all the letters that
did not reduce overall response accuracy. were reported. Figure 10 shows that all -S"s
In the present experiment, order of re- report the top row of the stimulus more
port is introduced as a purely "nuisance" accurately than the bottom row, if they are
variable for the S. The S is instructed to not instructed with regard to the order in
get as many letters correct as possible, but which they must report the rows.
the E randomly manipulates the order in The average accuracy with which 5"s
which they are to be reported. The experi- report the top and the bottom rows, when
ment is a survey of how ..9s adapt to this instructions to report one or the other of
kind of interference with the normal order these rows are given with various delays
of their report. after the exposure, is also illustrated in
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS n
100 100r
T
1
• ' s TOP ] -
1
75- 1
.1ST ST
• -
- 2™Ss^"^3 1 _ 1
AVERAGE N o 1
1
-
""^BOT
ROR
1

T1
75 1
1
"J-" TOP \ 1
1 -
1
50 4
~S^ /° i -
25 ii
!"^bot/ B-
1 VV N°
0 I , 1.0 I-M
DELAY OF INSTRUCTION TONE (SEC)
Fig. 10. Accuracy of the first (second) row reported and of the top (bottom) row as a function of
the delay of the instruction to report one row first. Light flash is indicated at lower left; immediate-
memory (I-M) at right.

Fig. 10. The accuracy of reports of the top tions increases. On the whole, however, the
row decreases slightly as the delay of the overall accuracy of report decreases slightly
instructions increases. In other respects, with the delay of the instruction to report
however, the data show no systematic one row first. The experimental interfer-
changes in accuracy with changes in the ence with the normal order of report does
delay of instructions. The data clearly indi- not change the overall number of letters
cate that the top row is generally reported reported correctly by any ^ by more than
more accurately than the bottom row al- about 0.5 letter.
though the instruction to report each row is In this task, unlike the preceding ones,
given with equal frequency. individual differences are more striking
The same data may also be analyzed with than the similarities. The pooled data are
regard to the accuracy of the row that must highly untypical of three of the five 5s.
be reported first and the row that is re- Figure 11 was devised as a two-dimensional,
ported last. All ^s except ROR are more graphical analysis of variance to compress
accurate when they report the first row the details of Fig. 10 into one figure.12
(the row called for by the instruction tone) Each coordinate represents the accuracy of
than the second row. For most 6"s, there-
fore, the order of report is correlated with 12 Figure 11 is based upon a suggestion by E. B.
the accuracy of report. Newman. A statistical analysis of variance was
There is a slight tendency for the accu- not attempted since it would have had to be carried
out separately on each S. There was not enough
racy of report of the row which is reported data to make this worthwhile, and Fig. 11 serves
first to decrease as the delay of the instruc- the same purpose.
IX GEORGE SPERLING

100 indifferent to whether it is called for first


or last. Other 5s lie between these ex-
5 90=-R0R r \
tremes, each 5 maintaining approximately
o — RNS f \.
IX. BO— ND , >> the same relative accuracy for the top and
n. the first rows throughout the various delay
o
i- to
conditions. Each 5, therefore, operates
l 60 ~NJ 1 |»WRNS
* AVE ^*o within a characteristic, limited area of the
.i
<t
H
<) hf) graph. N D is an exception. A t zero delay
»- a c of the instruction tone, both position and
o 40 SEC. DELAY order account heavily for the correct letters
>- 30 o 0 reported by N D . A t 0.5-sec. delay, N D
7- • .30
UJ • .50 ignored the order (preferring to concen-
o 20
cr • 1.0 trate on the top r o w ) , and at 1.0-sec. delay
hi
a. 10 she lost her position (top row) preference
i i l l i i i t as well. I n these three conditions, the total
v 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 number of letters correctly reported by
PER CENT OF TOTAL - FIRST ROW
N D remained approximately the same,
[*'jg. 11. Graphical analysis of position on within 0.5 letter of the control condition.
stimulus vs. order of report as contributors to
response accuracy. Each point represents the At 1.0-sec. delay neither position nor the
average of all trials of an S at a particular delay order contribute to N D ' s accuracy of report.
of instruction. The order in which the points arc A l l 5s operate in the upper right quadrant
connected corresponds to the magnitude of the
delays. Upper left: position preference in control of Fig. 11. This illustrates the finding that
(immediate-memory) report, no 5 consistently reported the bottom row-
more accurately than the top, nor the last
row better than the row first called for by
one row of the report relative to the whole the instruction tone. I t docs not, of course,
report. Thus the ordinate represents the indicate that 5s could not report the bottom
number of letters that an individual o re- row or the last row more accurately under
ports correctly in the top row of the other conditions. While 5s normally behave
stimulus (independently of order) divided quite consistently, the data of N D show that
by the total number of letters (both rows) they may try a number of different pro-
that he reports correctly. Similarly, the cedures. The instructions given the 5s prior
abscissa represents the percentage of the to the experiment were not restrictive. No
total correct letters reported by 5 that are specific procedure for making a report was
contained in his report of the first row. suggested to the 5s, because the purpose
Since each coordinate is relative to 5's own of the experiment was to find out how 5s
accuracy, no point of the graph is inacces- respond when they are not given detailed
sible to 5 provided that, if necessary, he is instructions. W i t h suitable instructions,
willing to sacrifice some accuracy. Since training, and reinforcements, 5s could
the interference with 5's order of report probably be induced to make most of the
in this experiment had only slight effects possible kinds of reports that can be dia-
on the overall accuracy of 5's report, this gramed by Fig. 11. This remains an
method of presenting the data is justifiable. empirical problem.
From Fig. 11 it is immediately evident The results obtained in this experiment
that, for example, 50% of the correct support the conclusions that both a position
letters that JC reports are from the top preference and the order of report ordi-
row and, by implication, 50% are from the narily correlate with the accuracy of re-
bottom row of the stimulus. More than sponse, but that probably neither are
70% of the correct letters that JC reports necessary conditions for response accuracy.
are in the first row reported by him. R O R Some 5s can relinquish the position prefer-
represents the converse, preferring to re- ence and a favorable order of report with
port the top row accurately, remaining no appreciable decrement in accuracy. This
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE LIS! BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 19
finding is in opposition to Lawrence and racy of the report. In other words, if order
Laberge's (1956) contention that accuracy of report is effective in determining the
is accounted for by the order of report. accuracy of report, then this effect must be
Accuracy and order are often correlated, a function of instructions given prior to
but if a favorable order of report is not any report at all. For some 5s, no effect of
necessary for accuracy, then it cannot be order of report upon response accuracy was
the cause of accuracy. observed.
When 5* is given a signal indicating which The two findings, that partial reports are
row is to be reported first (Experiment 7), uniformly more accurate than whole reports
the accuracy of report of the row indicated and that order of report may be uncorrelated
by the signal (the first row reported) may with accuracy, contradict Lawrence and
be compared to the accuracy of the partial Laberge's conclusion that "partial" reports
report (Experiment 3). The overt pro- are essentially similar to "first" reports. In
cedure on each trial is quite similar in fact, the second finding (that in some cir-
Experiments 3 and 7. The only difference cumstances order of report and accuracy
is that in the order of report experiment, of report are not correlated) provides a
after the 5s have finished writing the row direct counterexample to their conclusion.
indicated by the signal, they must also write Their different results may be in part due
down the other row. In the partial report to the vastly different stimuli which they
procedure they do not have to write the used. Lawrence and Laberge's entire
second row. The partial report and the stimuli each contained less information than
order of report experiments also share a two randomly chosen letters.
common dependent variable: the accuracy
of report of the row indicated by the in- Discussion
struction signal. In all seven experiments, 5s were re-
In view of the similarity in procedure, quired to report the letters of briefly ex-
it is surprising that the accuracy of this posed lettered stimuli. Two kinds of reports
common datum should be so different in were explored: partial reports, which re-
the two experiments. For example, when quired the 5s to report only a specified
5s give only the partial reports (the in- part of the stimulus, and whole reports,
struction signal being given immediately which required the 5s to report all the
after termination of the stimulus), then letters of the stimulus. Experiment 3
they report 90% of the letters correctly in demonstrated that the accuracy of partial
one row of 4/4 stimuli. When they are reports was consistently greater than the
required to write the other row also, then accuracy of whole reports. Another im-
they report only 69% of the first four portant difference between partial and whole
letters correctly. Every 5", individually, reports is the correlation of accuracy with
gives a more accurate partial report (Ex- the delay of the instruction to report. This
periment 3) than a report of the first row— was shown in Experiment 4 in which the
of two rows to be reported (Experiment 7). time delay of the instruction signal, which
The consistent superiority of the partial re- indicated the row of the stimulus to be
port over the first half of a whole report reported, was varied. The accuracy of the
prevails even when the instruction to report partial report was found to be a sharply
is delayed for 0.5 (or 0.3) sec. In all cases decreasing function of the time at which
where data are available, each 5 reports a the instruction was given. If the instruc-
row of four letters more accurately when tion signal was delayed for one sec. after
he does not have to write another row of the exposure, the accuracy of the partial
four letters afterwards. That what 5s must report was no longer very different from
write later should affect the accuracy of that of the whole report. In Experiment 7
what they write first must be explained— it was shown that the accuracy of the whole
if we disregard teleological explanation—by report does not change as the time of the
the effect of prior instructions on the accu- signal to report is varied—over the same
20 GEORGE SPERLING

range of time—up to one sec. after the able at the time a tone is heard which
exposure. follows the termination of the stimulus by
The two kinds of report can also be 150 msec. In other words, the subjective
considered in terms of the information (in image or sensation induced by the light
these experiments, letters) which they indi- flash outlasts the physical stimulus at least
cate the .S" has available for report. In the until the tone is heard. The stimulus in-
whole report, the S reports all the informa- formation is thus "stored" for a fraction
tion that he can. When he gives a partial of a second as a persisting image of the
report, the 5" may have additional available objective stimulus. As the visual image
information that is not required for the fades, its legibility (information content)
report. A calculation of the information decreases, and consequently the accuracy of
available to the ^s for their partial reports reports based upon it decreases.
indicates that between two and three times There is other evidence, besides such
more information is available for partial re- phenomenological accounts, that suggests
ports than for the whole reports. This that information is available in the form
discrepancy between the two kinds of report of an image for a short time after extinc-
is short-lived. Information in excess of tion of the physical stimulus. In the first
that indicated by the whole report was place, it is inconceivable that the observers
available to the -Ss for only a fraction of a should stop seeing the stimulus at exactly
second following the exposure. At the end the moment the light is turned off. The
of this time, the accuracy of partial reports rise and fall of sensation may be rapid, but
is no longer very different from that of they are not instantaneous. The question is
whole reports. not whether the observer continues to see
The whole report has already been ex- the stimulus after the illumination is turned
tensively studied by psychologists. The on, but for how long he continues to see
maximum number of items an individual the stimulus. A number of different kinds
can give in such a report is called his span of psychophysical measurements of the rise
of immediate-memory; whole reports are and fall of sensation have been attempted.
usually called immediate-memory reports. These estimates of the persistence of the
Experiments 1 and 2 extend the well-known visual sensation vary from a minimum of
conclusions that the span of immediate- 0.05 sec. (Wundt, 1899) to almost one
memory is an individual characteristic and sec. (McDougall, 1904). The most repre-
that it is constant over a wide range of sentative estimates are in the neighborhood
stimuli and exposure conditions. Although, of 1/6 sec. (cf. Pieron, 1934), a figure that
in immediate-memory experiments, items is in good agreement with the results.13
are conventionally presented sequentially,
Experiments 1 and 2 illustrate that this is 13 Measurements of the persistence of sensation
not necessary—that a simultaneous presen- have almost invariably used techniques which have
at most questionable validity. Wundt's method de-
tation may also give results characteristic pends upon masking, the effect of the persisting
of immediate-memory experiments. stimulus upon another stimulus. The masking
The main problems to be considered here power of a stimulus may be quite different from
its visibility. McDougall's measurements, as well
concern the partial-whole report discrep- as those cited by Pieron, depend upon motion of
ancy: (a) Why is the partial report more a stimulus across the retina. Such measurements
accurate than the whole report? (b) Why are undoubtedly influenced by the strong temporal
does the partial report retain this added and spatial interactions of the eye (Alpern,
accuracy only for a fraction of a second 1953). Schumann's ingenious application of the
method of Baxt to the determination of persistence
after the exposure ? is probably the only experiment that utilizes pat-
The answers proposed are a systematic tern stimulation. The other methods have not
elaboration of an observation that is readily been tried with pattern stimuli although there is,
made by most viewers of the actual tachis- a priori, no good reason why they have not been.
The possibility that the persistence of pattern
toscopic presentation. They report that information is quite different from persistence of
the stimulus field appears to be still read- "brightness" has not been investigated.
THF. INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 21

In Experiment 5 it was shown that the impossible, because of the relatively small
post-exposure field strongly influences the time between the stimulus exposure and the
accuracy of both the partial and the whole report.
report. This experiment indicates that the An analysis of errors reveals numerous
available information is sensitive to inter- cases of errors that may be classified as
ference by noninfarmational visual stimuli "misreading" (for example, confusions be-
which follow the exposure. The dependence tween E and F, B and R) and as "mis-
of available information upon noninforma- hearing" (for example, confusions between
tional visual stimulation is just the depend- B and D, D and T—Miller & Nicely, 1955).
ence that would be expected of a visual Still other confusions (for example, C
image. and G) are ambiguous. All of these types
Finally, there are subtle aspects of the of errors occurred whenever errors oc-
sequence of letters reported by an S which curred at all. The ubiquity of misreading
characterize the information that is avail- and mishearing errors, taken at face value,
able to him. In sequentially spoken letters, suggests that both visual and auditory
for example, there is a limit—two—on the storage of information are always involved
number of letters that can be adjacent to in both whole and partial reports. A non-
any given letter. Different limits apply to quantitative error analysis is therefore not
a two-dimensional visual display. If in- likely to shed much light on the question of
formation is stored in a form topologically visual imagery. The frequent mishearing
similar to the stimulus, this may be detected errors suggest that the storage of letters,
by noting the sequential dependencies that just prior to a written report, may share
limit successive responses to the stimulus. some of the characteristics of audition. Like
Probably the kinds of sequential respond- the preceding analysis of the constraints
ing that would most clearly distinguish upon successive responses, error analysis
visual from auditory information storage requires considerable research before it can
would be (a) the ability of the S to read be quantitatively applied to problems of
the rows of the visual stimulus backwards imagery.
as well as forwards, or to report the columns This then is the evidence—phenomeno-
or the diagonals, and (b) his inability to logical reports, the effects of the post-
do an equivalent task when presented with exposure fields, the known facts of the
the information sequentially. (All these persistence of sensation, and the detailed
procedures merely require the report of characteristics of the responses—that is
adjacent letters if the stimulus is two consistent with the hypothesis that informa-
dimensional.) Unfortunately, these particu- tion is initially stored as a visual image and
lar experiments were not conducted. that the ^s can effectively utilize this in-
The foregoing experiments offer some formation in their partial reports. In the
relevant evidence. In contrast to spoken present context, the term, visual image, is
letters and numbers, which are most accu- taken to mean that (a) the observer behaves
rately recalled if they occur at the beginning as though the physical stimulus were still
or end of a sequence (Pollack, 1952)," present when it is not (that is, after it has
no obvious gradients of accuracy were been removed) and that (b) his behavior
found in the foregoing experiments. The in the absence of the stimulus remains a
middle row actually tended to be slightly function of the same variables of visual
better reported than the other rows. There- stimulation as it is in its presence. The
fore, it is unlikely that the entire visual • units of a visual image so defined are al-
stimulus (12-letters and numbers) was ways those of an equivalent "objective
transformed into an auditory (sequential) image," the physical stimulus. It is as logi-
representation for storage. Such an entire cal or illogical to compute the information
transformation is also unlikely, though not contained in a visual image (as was done in
Experiments 3 and 4) as it is to compute
34 Summarized in Luce (1956). the information in a visual stimulus.
22 GEORGE SPERLING

"Visual image" and "persistence of sen- positive.35 Some authors distinguish the
sation" are terms suggested by the asyn- initial image from a positive afterimage
chrony between the time during which a (for example, McDougall, 1904) ; others
stimulus is present and the time during do not (for example, von Helmholtz, 1924-
which the observer behaves as though it 25). It is often implicit in such distinctions
were present. Although asynchrony is in- that the persistence of the initial image is
evitable for short exposure durations, there due to a continued excitatory process,
is, of course, no need to use the term "visual whereas afterimages arise from receptor
image" in a description of this situation. fatigue. If there is no repeated waxing and
One might, for example, refer simply to waning of sensation, but merely a single
rise and fall, one cannot distinguish two
an "information storage" with the charac-
phases in the primary image, one cor-
teristics that were experimentally observed.
responding to the "initial image" and the
This form of psychological isolationism other to an identical "positive afterimage"
does injustice to the vast amount of relevant of it.
researches.
Although it is difficult to prove that
Imagery that reputedly occurs long after visual information is stored in the initial
the original stimulation (memory images, image, there can be no gainsaying that an
eidctic images, etc.) is of interest as well afterimage may be a rich store of informa-
as imagery that occurs for only a few tenths tion. Positive or negative afterimages may
of a second following stimulation. Whether carry many fine details, including details
the term "imagery," as it has been used that were not visible at the time of stimula-
here to describe the immediate effects of tion (von Helmholtz, 1924-25). After-
brief stimulation, is an appropriate term for images generally last for at least several
the description of the lasting effects of seconds, and following high energy stimula-
stimulation is an empirical problem. It is tion they normally last for several minutes
hoped, however, that the principles and (Berry & Imus, 1935). The clarity of the
methods developed here will not be without details, of course, deteriorates with the pas-
relevance to these traditional problems. sage of time. Since afterimages appear to
move when the eye is moved, they usually
have been considered retinal phenomena.
Persistence of Vision and Afterimages
Taken together, these facts imply that there
Between the short persistence of vision is a considerable capacity for visual in-
and the remembrance of a long-passed formation storage in the retina. If the
event, there is an intermediate situation, illumination of the stimulus cards used in
the afterimage, which requires considera- the foregoing experiments had been suffi-
tion. In discussing afterimages, it will be ciently intense to blaze the letters upon the
useful to distinguish some phases of vision retina and thereby take maximum advantage
that normally follow an intense or pro- of its information storage capacity, there
longed stimulus. First, there is the "initial" would have been little doubt afterwards as
(or primary, or original) "image" (or sen- to the nature or location of most of the
sation, or impression, or perception, or available information. The stimulus presen-
tations actually used, however, rarely elicited
response). Any combination of a term from
reports of afterimages; .S's usually reported
the first and from the second of these
seeing simply a single brief flash. The
groups may be used. The initial image is problem is therefore to determine the pcr-
followed by a latent period during which
nothing is seen and which may in turn be
followed by a complex sequence of after- 3BRidwell (1897) and Sperling (1960) describe
images. Afterimages may be cither positive conditions for seeing a negative "after-image
or negative; almost any sequence is possi- without prior positive image." The method in-
volves a presentation quite similar to that of
ble, but the initial image is almost always Baxt (1871).
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 23

sistence of the image of the brief flash, or few tenths of a second after the exposure
equivalently, the duration of seeing (the than they can give in a complete report of
stimulus) or the persistence of vision (of what they have seen. It was suggested that
the stimulus), rather than the duration of the limit on the number of items in the
an afterimage. These terms are used to memory report is a very general one, the
suggest that the -S1 feels he is responding span of immediate-memory, which is rela-
directly to the stimulus rather than to after- tively independent of the nature of the
effects of stimulation. stimulus. Evidence was offered that infor-
Psychologists have often carelessly as- mation in excess of the immediate-memory
sumed that the absence of discernible after- span is available to the J? as a rapidly fading
images following a visual presentation was visual image of the stimulus. If more in-
sufficient to insure that the duration of sen- formation is available to him than he can
sation will correspond to the duration of the remember, the 5" must "choose" a part of
physical stimulus, that is, that there is no it to remember. In doing so, he has chosen
persistence of vision at all. Wundt (1899) the part to forget. In Experiments 3 and 4,
was one of the first to take vigorous excep- i's exercised only locational choices, that is,
tion to this naive view. Wundt's most com- portions of the stimulus were remembered
pelling example was drawn from VVeyer only on the basis of their location. Loca-
(1899). Weyer had found that two 40- tional choices are probably not the only
microsecond light flashes had to be sepa- effective choices that the 51 can make. Dur-
rated by 40 to 50 msec, in order for them ing the short time that information is avail-
to be seen as two distinct flashes; at smaller able to him, the S may process it in any
separations they were seen as a single flash. way in which he normally handles informa-
In the dark adapted eye, the minimum tion. Usually, what he does, or attempts
separable interval that consistently yielded to do, is determined by the instructions.
reports of "two flashes" was 80 to 100 The .S"'s (unobservable) response to the
msec. stimulus is probably the same whether the
instruction to make this response is given
Wundt argued that the two flashes could before the stimulus presentation or after
not be seen as distinct until the sensation it; the difference between the two cases lies
occasioned by the first flash had ceased. in the information that the response can
Thus, under optimum conditions, the mini- draw upon. If the stimulus contains more
mum duration of the sensation of a short information than the S's immediate-memory
flash was at least 40 msec, which, in this span, and if the post-exposure instruction is
case, was 1,000 times the stimulus duration. delayed until the 6* has little of this extra
W.aidt thought that, in order to determine information available, then a difference in
t'ne duration of a longer flash, one must the accuracy of the responses with prior-
merely add the 40 msec, of fade-out time to and post-exposure instructions will be ob-
the actual physical light duration. While served. If the stimulus does not contain
these details of Wundt's reasoning may be more information than can be coded for
questioned, his main point, based on the immediate-memory, or if the post-exposure
example of the short flash, is indisputable: instruction is given soon enough so that
one docs not directly control the time for the S can utilize the still available informa-
which information is visually available tion effectively, then only minor differences
simply by manipulating exposure duration. in the accuracy of responses with prior- and
The experiments reported here provide a post-exposure instructions will be observed.
direct proof of this assertion. If the stimulus is destitute of information
(for example, a single, mutilated, dimly
An Application of the Results to illuminated letter of the alphabet) then a
"Before and After" Experiments host of other factors which are normally
insignificant may become crucial. In this
The previous experiments showed that
case, the "stimulus" itself ma)' well be
more information is available to i's for a
24 GEORGE SPERLING

irrelevant (Goldiamond, 1957), and the short time difference, 0.1 sec, accounts for
effects of instructions given before or after the similar accuracy of responding in these
the exposure must be predicted on some two conditions.
other basis. Once his attention is directed to the ap-
There are some simple experiments in propriate row, the S still has to read the
which it is known a priori that the effects letters. This, too, takes time. Baxt's (1871)
of instructions given either before or after data indicate that the time required to read
the exposure will be exactly the same. This a letter is about 10 msec. Baxt's experiment,
degenerate situation can be illustrated by a with some modifications, was repeated by the
stimulus which is exposed for one micro- author, and similar results were obtained.16
second and with sufficient energy to be How is all this relevant to the order of
clearly visible. By suitable coding, the pre- report ? The order in which the letters are
exposure and post-exposure instructions finally reported can be an important variable
can be separated by only two microseconds. because of (a) purely temporal factors
The example serves to emphasize that what (letters that arc reported first will be more
is implicitly referred to by "before and accurately reported only because they are
after" is not the exposure but something reported sooner after the exposure, the
else: traditionally, the sensitization and/or actual order of reporting the letters per se
forgetting that presumably occur in con- being relatively unimportant) or (b) inter-
junction with the exposure. Thus, the action effects (the report of some letters is
theory that has been presented here merely detrimental to the report of the remaining
gives an explicit statement of assumptions letters, that is, letters reported later suffer
that have long been implicit. from proactive interference by the letters
reported earlier).
Unobscrvable Responses and That purely temporal factors cannot be
the Order of Report very important can be seen from the slope
of the curves describing available informa-
The subjective response to the high signal tion as a function of time. In the foregoing
tone is "looking up." Since eye movements experiments, the amount of available in-
cannot occur in time to change the retinal formation approached the immediate-
image with any of the presentations used memory span at about 0.5 to 1.0 sec. after
(Diefendorf & Dodge, 1908) a successful the exposure; further decrements in avail-
looking-up must be described in terms of a able information as a function of time are
shift in "attention." Nonetheless, such a slight. The report of the letters usually
shift in attention can be quantitatively does not begin until 1.0-1.5 sec. after the
studied by means of a stabilized retinal exposure. The passage of time during the
image (Pritchard, 1958) although Wundt actual time that letters are being reported,
(1912), who did not use this modern, tech- therefore, cannot account for appreciable
nically difficult technique, was able to give accuracy changes as a function of the order
many essential details. The reaction time of report.
for the attentional response, like the reaction The second possible effect of order of
time for more observable responses, is report—the interfering effect of the letters
greater than zero. Therefore, if the -S* is reported first upon unreported letters—
given an instruction before the presentation, cannot be so readily discounted. Proactive
he can prepare for, or sensitize himself to, interference would imply that partial re-
the correct row of the stimulus even though ports are more accurate than whole reports
there is not time enough for a useful eye by an amount dependent upon their relative
movement. The response to an instruction lengths. The results of Experiment 4 tend
which is given 0.05 sec. before the stimulus to support this view. At delays of the in-
is probably the same as the response to a
similar instruction that is given 0.1 sec.
16 Sperling, G. Unpublished experiments con-
later, immediately after the exposure. The ducted at the Bell Telephone Laboratories, 1958.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 25
struction signal greater than 0.3 sec, partial information can be rejected not only on
reports of three letters (from stimuli of prior grounds (see Apparatus) but also by
nine letters) indicate more available letters the empirical findings. For example, in
than do partial reports of four letters (from Fig. 9, the curve representing the number
stimuli of twelve letters). On the other of letters available 0.15 sec. after exposure
hand, in Experiment 7 one S, ROR, does is quite similar to the 0.0-sec. delay curve.
not show decreased accuracy as a function There is no visible energy emitted by the
of the length of his report. ROR is able to light source 0.15 sec. after its termination.
report eight letters as accurately when he In the present case, the answer to the
begins his report with three or four incor- problem of repeatability of the results is
rect letters as when he ends his report with made less speculative by three separate
three or four incorrect letters. Other 5s did investigations that have since been con-
not systematically attempt to report incor- ducted with similar techniques to those re-
rect letters first. Had they been required to ported here.
report incorrect letters first, they might well The experiments have been repeated by
have been able to do so. the author18 with a different tachistoscope,
The choice of what part of the stimulus timer, and a light source that has only a
to attend to or of which letters to read is negligible afterglow. All the main findings
the choice of what fraction of the stimulus were reproducible.
information to utilize. This choice can be Klemmer and Loftus (1958) confirmed
made successfully only while the informa- the existence of a short-term, high informa-
tion is still available. The 5s prefer to tion storage. They used a display consisting
report what they remember first, but this of four discrete line patterns, the 5 being
does not imply that they remember it be- required to report only one of these. The
cause they report it first. It is difficult to instruction was coded either as a signal
disentangle the many factors that determine light or verbally. Decay curves obtained
precisely what stimulus letters will appear when the instruction is delayed are similar
in the response, but important choices of to those reported above. A similar experi-
what information is to be recalled must ment has also been conducted by Aver-
occur while there is still something to bach,10 who used a television tachistoscope
choose from. Since the actual report begins to present stimuli containing up to 16 letters.
only when there are available but a few A pointer appeared above the letter to be
letters in excess of the immediate-memory reported. Initially, 5"s had about twelve
span, the order of report can at most play letters available for report, but the number
only the minor role of determining which decreased rapidly when the visual instruc-
of the few "excess" letters will be for- tion was delayed.
gotten.17 It is usually technically more difficult to
code instructions visually than acoustically.
The Questions of Generality Although the principle of sampling in order
and Repeatability to determine available information is com-
mon to both kinds of instructions, visually
To what extent arc the results obtained
coded instructions differ in some interesting-
limited to the particular conditions of the
ways from acoustically coded instructions.
foregoing experiments ? The possibility that
For example, the time taken to "interpret"
the actual physical fading of the light
—or even to find—a visual instruction may
source is important to the availability of
well depend on its location relative to the
fixation point. Moreover, there may be
17 There are many ways in which proactive
interference might occur. For example, if letters
are stored "sequentially" prior to report (cf. 18 Sperling, G. Unpublished experiments con-
Broadbent, 1957b), then the importance of order ducted at the Bell Telephone Laboratories, 19S8.
of report may lie in the agreement of the two 10 Averbach, E. Unpublished experiments con-
sequences : storage and report. ducted at the Bell Telephone Laboratories, 1959.
26 GEORGE SPERLING

spatial interactions between the visual "in- image of the stimulus. Evidence in support
struction" and the symbols to be reported. of this hypothesis of visual information
On the other hand, prior to training, the storage was found in introspective accounts,
task of interpreting a visual marker is in the type of dependence of the accuracy
easier for Ss than the equivalent task with of partial reports upon the visual stimula-
an acoustically coded instruction. Ulti- tion, and in an analysis of certain response
mately, such differences are probably only characteristics. These and related problems
of secondary importance since the two were explored in a series of seven experi-
kinds of experiments agree quite well. ments.
Three main findings emerge from the ex- An attempt was first made to show that
periments reported here: a large amount of the span of immediate-memory remains
information becomes available to observers relatively invariant under a wide range of
of a brief visual presentation, this informa- conditions. Five practiced observers were
tion decays rapidly, the final level is ap- shown stimuli consisting of arrays of sym-
proximately equal to the span of immediate- bols that varied in number, arrangement,
memory. Although the exact, quantitative and composition (letters alone, or letters
aspects of information that becomes avail- and numbers together). It was found (Ex-
able following a brief exposure unquestion- periments 1 and 2) that each observer was
ably depend upon the precise conditions of able to report only a limited number of
presentation, it seems fair to conclude that symbols (for example, letters) correctly.
the main results can be duplicated even For exposure durations from 15 to 500
under vastly different circumstances in msec, the average was slightly over four
different laboratories. letters; stimuli having four or fewer letters
were reported correctly nearly 100% of the
time.
Summary and Conclusions
In order to circumvent the immediate-
When stimuli consisting of a number of memory limit on the (whole) report of
items are shown briefly to an observer, what has been seen, observers were required
only a limited number of the items can be to report only a part—designated by loca-
correctly reported. This number defines tion—of stimuli exposed for 50 msec.
the so-called "span of immediate-memory." (partial report). The part to be reported,
The fact that observers commonly assert usually one out of three rows of letters,
that they can see more than they can report was small enough (three to four letters) to
suggests that memory sets a limit on a lie within the memory span. A tonal signal
process that is otherwise rich in available (high, middle, or low frequency) was used
information. In the present studies, a to indicate which of the rows was to be
sampling procedure (partial report) was reported. The ^ did not know which signal
used to circumvent the limitation imposed to expect, and the indicator signal was not
by immediate-memory and thereby to show given until after the visual stimulus had
that at the time of exposure, and for a been turned off. In this manner, the infor-
few tenths of a second thereafter, observers mation available to the J? was sampled im-
have two or three times as much informa- mediately after termination of the stimulus.
tion available as they can later report. The Each observer, for each material tested
availability of this information declines (6, 8, 9, 12 symbols), gave partial reports
rapidly, and within one second after the that were more accurate than whole reports
exposure the available information no for the same material. For example, follow-
longer exceeds the memory span. ing the exposure of stimuli consisting of 12
Short-term information storage has been symbols, 76% of the letters called for in the
tentatively identified with the persistence partial report were given correctly by the
of sensation that generally follows any brief, observers. This accuracy indicates that the
intense stimulation. In this case, the per- total information available from which an
sistence is that of a rapidly fading, visual observer can draw his partial report is
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 27

about 9.1 letters (76% of 12 letters). This than by location. The observer reported
number of randomly chosen letters is numbers only (or the letters only) from
equivalent to 40.6 bits of information, which stimuli consisting of both letters and num-
is considerably more information than pre- bers (Experiment 6). These partial reports
vious experimental estimates have suggested were no more accurate than (whole) re-
can become available in a brief exposure. ports of all the letters and numbers. The
Furthermore, it seems probable that the ability of observers to give highly accurate
40-bit information capacity observed in partial reports of letters designated by loca-
these experiments was limited by the small tion (Experiment 3), and their inability to
amount of information in the stimuli rather give partial reports of comparable accuracy
than by a capacity of the observers. when the symbols to be reported are desig-
In order to determine how the available nated as cither letters or numbers, clearly
information decreases with time, the instruc- indicates that all kinds of partial reports
tion signal, which indicated the row of the are not equally suitable for demonstrating
stimulus to be reported, was delayed by the ability of observers to retain large
various amounts, up to 1.0 sec. (Experi- amounts of information for short time
ment 4). The accuracy of the partial report periods.
was shown to be a sharply decreasing func- In the final study (Experiment 7), the
tion of the delay in the instruction signal. order of report was systematically varied.
Since, at a delay of 1.0 sec, the accuracy Observers were instructed to get as many
of the partial reports approached that of letters correct as possible, but the order in
the whole reports, it follows that the in- which they were to report the letters was
formation in excess of the immediate- not indicated until after the exposure. An
memory span is available for less than a instruction tone, following the exposure,
second. In contrast to the partial report, the indicated which of the two rows of letters
accuracy of the whole report is not a func- on the stimulus was to be reported first.
tion of the time at which the signal to This interference with the normal order of
report is given (Experiment 7). report reduced only slightly the total num-
The large amount of information in ex- ber of letters that were reported correctly.
cess of the immediate-memory span, and As might be expected, the first row—the
the short time during which this informa- row indicated by the instruction tone—was
tion is available, suggests that it may be reported more accurately than the second
stored as a persistence of the sensation re- row (order effect). There was, however,
sulting from the visual stimulus. In order a strong tendency for the top row to be
to explore further this possibility of visual reported more accurately than the bottom
information storage, some parameters of row (position effect). Although, as a group,
visual stimulation were studied. A decrease the observers showed both effects, some
of the exposure duration from 50 to IS failed to show either the order or the posi-
msec, did not substantially affect the accu- tion effect, or both. The fact that, for some
racy of partial reports (Experiment 5). On observers, order and position are not cor-
the other hand, the substitution of a white related with response accuracy suggests that
post-exposure field for the dark field order of report, and position, are not the
ordinarily used greatly reduced the accu- major causes of, nor the necessary con-
racy of both partial and whole reports. The ditions for, response accuracy. The high
ability of a homogeneous visual stimulus accuracy of partial report observed in the
to affect the available information is evi- experiments does not depend on the order
dence that the process depends on a per- of report or on the position of letters on
sisting visual image of the stimulus. the stimulus, but rather it is shown to
Whether other kinds of partial reports depend on the ability of the observer to read
give similar estimates of the amount of a visual image that persists for a fraction
available information was examined by ask- of a second after the stimulus has been
ing observers to report by category rather turned off.
28 GEORGE SPERLING

REFERENCES

Adams, J. S. The relative effectiveness of pre- Glanvillk, A. D., & Dallenbach, K. M. The
and post-stimulus setting as a function of stimu- range of attention. Amer. J. Psychol., 1929, 41,
lus uncertainty. Unpublished master's disserta- 207-236.
tion, Department of Psychology, University of Goldiamond, I. Operant analysis of perceptual
North Carolina, 1955. behavior. Paper read at Symposium on Experi-
Ai.pern, M. Metacontrast. /. Opt. Soc. Amer., mental Analysis of Behavior, APA Annual Con-
1953, 43, 648-657. vention, 1957.
Baxt, N. "Obcr die Zeit welche notig ist damit James, W. The principles of psychology. New
ein Gesichtseindruck zum Bewusstsein kommt York: Holt, 1890.
und iiber die Grosse (Extension) der bewussten Klemmer, E. T. Simple reaction time as a func-
Wahrnehmung bei einem Gesichtseindrucke von tion of time uncertainty. /. exp. Psychol., 1957,
gegebener Dauer. Pfliiger's Arch. ges. Physiol., 54, 195-200.
1871, 4, 325-336. Klemmer, E. T., & Loftus, J. P. Numerals,
Berry, W., & Imus, H. Quantitative aspects of nonsense forms, and information. USAF Cam-
the flight of colors. Amer. J. Psychol., 1935, hridge Research Center, Operational Applications
47, 449-457. Laboratory, Boiling Air Force Base, 1958. (Astia
Bidwell, S. On the negative after-images follow- Doc. No. AD110063)
ing brief retinal excitation. Proc. Roy. Soc. Kulpe, O. Versuche iiber Abstraktion. In, Bericht
Lond., 1897, 61, 268-271. iiber den erste Kongress fur experimentelle
Bridgin, R. L. A tachistoscopic study of the Psychologic. Leipzig: Barth, 1904. Pp. 56-68.
differentiation of perception. Psychol. Monogr., Ladd, G. T. Elements of physiological psychology:
1933, 44(1, Whole No. 197), 153-166. A treatise of the activities and nature of the
Broadbent, D. E. Immediate memory and simul- mind. New York: Scribner, 1889.
taneous stimuli. Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 1957, Lawrence, D. H., & Coles, G. R. Accuracy of
9, 1-11. (a) recognition with alternatives before and after
Broadbent, D. E. A mechanical model for human the stimulus. /. exp. Psychol., 1954, 47, 208-214.
attention and memory. Psychol. Rev., 1957, 64, Lawrence, D, H., & Laberge, D. L. Relationship
205-215. (b) between accuracy and order of reporting stimulus
Broadbent, D. E. Perception and communication. dimensions. /. exp. Psychol., 1956, 51, 12-18.
New York: Pergamon, 1958. Lindsley, D. B., & Emmons, W. H. Perception
Cattell, J. McK. t)ber die Tragheit dcr Netzhaut time and evoked potentials. Science, 1958, 127,
und des Sehcentrums. Phil. Stud., 1883, 3, 1061.
94-127. Long, E. R., Henneman, R. H., & Reid, L. S.
Chapman, D. W. The comparative effects of Theoretical considerations and exploratory in-
determinate and indeterminate aufgaben. Un- vestigation of "set" as response restriction: The
published doctor's dissertation, Harvard Uni- first of a series of reports on "set" as a deter-
versity, 1930. miner of perceptual responses. USAF WADC
Chapman, D. W. Relative effects of determinate tech. Rep., 1953, No. 53-311.
and indeterminate "Aufgaben." Amer. J. Psychol, Long, E. R., & Lee, W. A. The influence of
1932, 44, 163-174. specific stimulus cuing on location responses:
Cohen, L. D., & Thomas, D. R. Decision and The third of a series of reports on "set" as a
motor components of reaction time as a function determiner of perceptual responses. USAF
of anxiety level and task complexity. Amer. WADC tech. Rep., 1953, No. 53-314. (a)
Psychologist, 1957, 12, 420. (Abstract). Long, E. R., & Lee, W. A. The role of spatial
Dallenbach, K. M. Attributive vs. cognitive cuing as a response-limiter for location re-
sponses : The second of a series of reports on
clearness. /. exp. Psychol., 1920, 3, 183-230. "set" as a determiner of perceptual responses.
Diefendorf, A. R., & Dodge, R. An experimental USAF WADC tech. Rep., 1953, No. 53-312. (b)
study of the ocular reactions of the insane from
photographic records. Brain, 1908, 31, 451-489. Long, E. R., Reid, L. D., & Garvey, W. D. The
role of stimulus ambiguity and degree of re-
Dodge, R. An experimental study of visual fixa- sponse restriction in the recognition of distorted
tion. Psychol. Monogr., 1907, 8(4, Whole No. letter patterns: The fourth of a series of reports
35). (a) on "set" as a determiner of perceptual responses.
Dodge, R. An improved exposure apparatus. USAF WADC tech. Rep., 1954, No. 54-147.
Psychol. Bull., 1907, 4, 10-13. (b) Luce, D. R. A survey of the theory of selective
Erdmann, B., & Dodge, R. Psychologische Unter- information and some of its behavioral applica-
suchungen iiber das Lesen auf experimenteller tions. New York: Bureau of Applied Social
Grundlage. Halle : Niemeyer, 1898. Research, 1956.
THE INFORMATION AVAILABLE IN BRIEF VISUAL PRESENTATIONS 29

McDougall, W. The sensations excited by a Schumann, F. The Erkennungsurteil. Z. Psychol.,


single momentary stimulation of the eve. Brit. 1922, 88, 205-224.
J. Psychol, 1904, 1, 78-113. Sperling, G. Information available in a brief
Miller, G. A. Human memory and the storage of visual presentation. Unpublished doctor's disser-
information. IRE Trans. Information Theory, tation, Department of Psychology, Harvard Uni-
1956, IT-2, No. 3, 129-137. (a) versity, 1959.
Miller, G. A. The magic number seven, plus or Sperling, G. Afterimage without prior image.
minus two: Some limits on our capacity for Science, 1960, 131, 1613-1614.
processing information. Psychol. Rev., 1956, 63, von Helmholtz, H. Treatise on physiological
81-97. (b) optics. Vol. I I . The sensations of vision.
Miller, G, A., & Nicely, P. E. An analysis of (Transl. from 3rd German ed.) Rochester, New
perceptual confusions among some English con- York: Optical Society of America, 1924-25.
sonants. /. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 1955, 27, 338- Wagner, J. Experimented Beitrage zur Psy-
352. chologie des Lesens. Z. Psychol, 1918, 80, 1-75.
Pieron, H. L'evanouisscment de la sensation Weyer, E. M. The Zeitschwellcn gleichartiger und
lumineuse: Persistance indifferenciable et per- disparater Sinneseindrucke. Phil Stud., 1899,
sistance totale. Ann. psychoi, 1934, 35, 1-49. 15, 68-138.
Pollack, T. The assimilation of sequentially- Whipple, G. M. Manual of physical and mental
encoded information. Hum. Resources Res. Lab. tests. Vol. I. Simpler processes. Baltimore:
memo Rep., 1952, No. 25. Warwick & York, 1914.
Pritchard, R. M. Visual illusions viewed as Wilcocks, R. W. An examination of Kiilpe's ex-
stabilized retinal images. Quart. J. cxp. Psychol., periments on abstraction. Amer. J. Psychol,
1958, 10, 77-81. 1925, 36, 324-340.
Quastler, H. Studies of human channel capacity. Woodworth, R. S. Experimental psychology.
In H. Quastler, Three survey papers. Urbana, New York: Holt, 1938.
111.: Control Systems Laboratory, Univer. Illinois, Wundt, W. Zur Kritik tachistosckopischer Ver-
1956. Pp. 13-33. suche. Phil. Stud. 1899, 15, 287-317.
Schumann, F. Die Erkennung von Buchstaben Wundt, W. An introduction to psychology. Lon-
und Wortcn bei momentaner Beleuchtigung. In, don : Allen & Unwin, 1912.
Bericht iiber den erste Kongress fur experi-
mentelle Psychologie. Leipzig: Barlh, 1904. Pp.
34-40. (Ear
3arly publication received April 28, 1960.

You might also like