Inteligencia en Plantas

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

A REpORTER

AT LARgE

THE iNTELLigENT pLANT


Scientists debate a new way of understanding flora.

BY MiCHAEL pOLLAN

I n 1973, a book claiming that plants


were sentient beings that feel emo-
tions, prefer classical music to rock and
shrimp were dropped into boiling water,
an experiment that Backster wrote up
for the International Journal of Parapsy-
and Elizabeth Van Volkenburgh, an
American plant biologist—argued that
the sophisticated behaviors observed in
roll, and can respond to the unspoken chology, in 1968. plants cannot at present be completely
thoughts of humans hundreds of miles In the ensuing years, several legiti- explained by familiar genetic and bio-
away landed on the New York Times mate plant scientists tried to reproduce chemical mechanisms. Plants are able to
best-seller list for nonfiction. “The Secret the “Backster effect” without success. sense and optimally respond to so many
Life of Plants,” by Peter Tompkins and Much of the science in “The Secret Life environmental variables—light, water,
Christopher Bird, presented a beguiling of Plants” has been discredited. But the gravity, temperature, soil structure, nutri-
mashup of legitimate plant science, quack book had made its mark on the culture. ents, toxins, microbes, herbivores, chem-
experiments, and mystical nature wor- Americans began talking to their plants ical signals from other plants—that there
ship that captured the public imagination and playing Mozart for them, and no may exist some brainlike information-
at a time when New Age thinking was doubt many still do. This might seem processing system to integrate the data
seeping into the mainstream. The most harmless enough; there will probably al- and coördinate a plant’s behavioral re-
memorable passages described the exper- ways be a strain of romanticism running sponse. The authors pointed out that
iments of a former C.I.A. polygraph ex- through our thinking about plants. (Lu- electrical and chemical signalling systems
pert named Cleve Backster, who, in ther Burbank and George Washington have been identified in plants which are
1966, on a whim, hooked up a galva- Carver both reputedly talked to, and lis- homologous to those found in the ner-
nometer to the leaf of a dracaena, a tened to, the plants they did such bril- vous systems of animals. They also noted
houseplant that he kept in his office. To liant work with.) But in the view of that neurotransmitters such as serotonin,
his astonishment, Backster found that many plant scientists “The Secret Life of dopamine, and glutamate have been
simply by imagining the dracaena being Plants” has done lasting damage to their found in plants, though their role re-
set on fire he could make it rouse the nee- field. According to Daniel Chamovitz, mains unclear.
dle of the polygraph machine, registering an Israeli biologist who is the author of Hence the need for plant neurobiol-
a surge of electrical activity suggesting the recent book “What a Plant Knows,” ogy, a new field “aimed at understanding
that the plant felt stress. “Could the plant Tompkins and Bird “stymied important how plants perceive their circumstances
have been reading his mind?” the authors research on plant behavior as scientists and respond to environmental input in
ask. “Backster felt like running into the became wary of any studies that hinted at an integrated fashion.” The article argued
street and shouting to the world, ‘Plants parallels between animal senses and plant that plants exhibit intelligence, defined
can think!’ ” senses.” Others contend that “The Secret by the authors as “an intrinsic ability to
Backster and his collaborators went Life of Plants” led to “self-censorship” process information from both abiotic
on to hook up polygraph machines to among researchers seeking to explore the and biotic stimuli that allows optimal de-
dozens of plants, including lettuces, on- “possible homologies between neurobi- cisions about future activities in a given
ions, oranges, and bananas. He claimed ology and phytobiology”; that is, the pos- environment.” Shortly before the article’s
that plants reacted to the thoughts sibility that plants are much more intelli- publication, the Society for Plant Neuro-
(good or ill) of humans in close proxim- gent and much more like us than most biology held its first meeting, in Flor-
ity and, in the case of humans familiar to people think—capable of cognition, ence, in 2005. A new scientific journal,
them, over a great distance. In one ex- communication, information processing, with the less tendentious title Plant Sig-
periment designed to test plant mem- computation, learning, and memory. naling & Behavior, appeared the follow-
ory, Backster found that a plant that had The quotation about self-censorship ing year.
witnessed the murder (by stomping) of appeared in a controversial 2006 article in
another plant could pick out the killer
from a lineup of six suspects, registering
a surge of electrical activity when the
Trends in Plant Science proposing a new
field of inquiry that the authors, per-
haps somewhat recklessly, elected to call
D epending on whom you talk to in
the plant sciences today, the field
of plant neurobiology represents either a
murderer was brought before it. Back- “plant neurobiology.” The six authors— radical new paradigm in our under-
ster’s plants also displayed a strong aver- among them Eric D. Brenner, an Amer- standing of life or a slide back down into
sion to interspecies violence. Some had ican plant molecular biologist; Stefano the murky scientific waters last stirred
a stressful response when an egg was Mancuso, an Italian plant physiologist; up by “The Secret Life of Plants.” Its
cracked in their presence, or when live František Baluška, a Slovak cell biologist; proponents believe that we must stop
92 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
PhotograPh by grant Cornett

Plants have electrical and chemical signalling systems, may possess memory, and exhibit brainy behavior in the absence of brains.
ConstruCtion by stephen Doyle THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013 93
regarding plants as passive objects—the a new paradigm,” he wrote in a recent “inert material” with which they may do
mute, immobile furniture of our world— e-mail. Slayman has referred to the Alpi as they please. The aliens proceed ruth-
and begin to treat them as protagonists letter as “the last serious confrontation lessly to exploit us. (Mancuso subse-
in their own dramas, highly skilled in the between the scientific community and quently wrote to say that the story he re-
ways of contending in nature. They the nuthouse on these issues.” Scientists counted was actually a mangled recol-
would challenge contemporary biology’s seldom use such language when talking lection of an early “Star Trek” episode
reductive focus on cells and genes and about their colleagues to a journalist, but called “Wink of an Eye.”)
return our attention to the organism this issue generates strong feelings, per- In Mancuso’s view, our “fetishization”
and its behavior in the environment. haps because it smudges the sharp line of neurons, as well as our tendency to
It is only human arrogance, and the separating the animal kingdom from the equate behavior with mobility, keeps us
fact that the lives of plants unfold in plant kingdom. The controversy is less from appreciating what plants can do.
what amounts to a much slower dimen- about the remarkable discoveries of re- For instance, since plants can’t run away
sion of time, that keep us from appreci- cent plant science than about how to in- and frequently get eaten, it serves them
ating their intelligence and consequent terpret and name them: whether behav- well not to have any irreplaceable organs.
success. Plants dominate every terrestrial iors observed in plants which look very “A plant has a modular design, so it can
environment, composing ninety-nine much like learning, memory, decision- lose up to ninety per cent of its body
per cent of the biomass on earth. By making, and intelligence deserve to be without being killed,” he said. “There’s
comparison, humans and all the other called by those terms or whether those nothing like that in the animal world. It
animals are, in the words of one plant words should be reserved exclusively for creates a resilience.”
neurobiologist, “just traces.” creatures with brains. Indeed, many of the most impressive
Many plant scientists have pushed capabilities of plants can be traced to
back hard against the nascent field, be-
ginning with a tart, dismissive letter
in response to the Brenner manifesto,
N o one I spoke to in the loose, inter-
disciplinary group of scientists
working on plant intelligence claims that
their unique existential predicament as
beings rooted to the ground and there-
fore unable to pick up and move when
signed by thirty-six prominent plant sci- plants have telekinetic powers or feel they need something or when conditions
entists (Alpi et al., in the literature) and emotions. Nor does anyone believe that turn unfavorable. The “sessile life style,”
published in Trends in Plant Science. “We we will locate a walnut-shaped organ as plant biologists term it, calls for an ex-
begin by stating simply that there is no somewhere in plants which processes tensive and nuanced understanding of
evidence for structures such as neurons, sensory data and directs plant behavior. one’s immediate environment, since the
synapses or a brain in plants,” the authors More likely, in the scientists’ view, intel- plant has to find everything it needs, and
wrote. No such claim had actually been ligence in plants resembles that exhibited has to defend itself, while remaining
made—the manifesto had spoken only of in insect colonies, where it is thought to fixed in place. A highly developed sen-
“homologous” structures—but the use of be an emergent property of a great many sory apparatus is required to locate food
the word “neurobiology” in the absence mindless individuals organized in a net- and identify threats. Plants have evolved
of actual neurons was apparently more work. Much of the research on plant in- between fifteen and twenty distinct
than many scientists could bear. telligence has been inspired by the new senses, including analogues of our five:
“Yes, plants have both short- and science of networks, distributed comput- smell and taste (they sense and respond
long-term electrical signalling, and they ing, and swarm behavior, which has to chemicals in the air or on their bodies);
use some neurotransmitter-like chemi- demonstrated some of the ways in which sight (they react differently to various
cals as chemical signals,” Lincoln Taiz, remarkably brainy behavior can emerge wavelengths of light as well as to
an emeritus professor of plant physiol- in the absence of actual brains. shadow); touch (a vine or a root “knows”
ogy at U.C. Santa Cruz and one of the “If you are a plant, having a brain is when it encounters a solid object); and, it
signers of the Alpi letter, told me. “But not an advantage,” Stefano Mancuso has been discovered, sound. In a recent
the mechanisms are quite different from points out. Mancuso is perhaps the field’s experiment, Heidi Appel, a chemical
those of true nervous systems.” Taiz says most impassioned spokesman for the ecologist at the University of Missouri,
that the writings of the plant neurobiol- plant point of view. A slight, bearded Ca- found that, when she played a recording
ogists suffer from “over-interpretation labrian in his late forties, he comes across of a caterpillar chomping a leaf for a plant
of data, teleology, anthropomorphizing, more like a humanities professor than like that hadn’t been touched, the sound
philosophizing, and wild speculations.” a scientist. When I visited him earlier this primed the plant’s genetic machinery to
He is confident that eventually the plant year at the International Laboratory of produce defense chemicals. Another ex-
behaviors we can’t yet account for will be Plant Neurobiology, at the University of periment, done in Mancuso’s lab and not
explained by the action of chemical or Florence, he told me that his conviction yet published, found that plant roots
electrical pathways, without recourse to that humans grossly underestimate plants would seek out a buried pipe through
“animism.” Clifford Slayman, a profes- has its origins in a science-fiction story he which water was flowing even if the ex-
sor of cellular and molecular physiology remembers reading as a teen-ager. A race terior of the pipe was dry, which sug-
at Yale, who also signed the Alpi letter of aliens living in a radically sped-up di- gested that plants somehow “hear” the
(and who helped discredit Tompkins mension of time arrive on Earth and, un- sound of flowing water.
and Bird), was even more blunt. “ ‘Plant able to detect any movement in humans, The sensory capabilities of plant roots
intelligence’ is a foolish distraction, not come to the logical conclusion that we are fascinated Charles Darwin, who in his
94 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
later years became increasingly passion- “Plants perceive competitors and grow insects they emit volatile chemicals that
ate about plants; he and his son Francis away from them,” Rick Karban, a plant signal other leaves to mount a defense.
performed scores of ingenious experi- ecologist at U.C. Davis, explained, when Sometimes this warning signal contains
ments on plants. Many involved the root, I asked him for an example of plant information about the identity of the in-
or radicle, of young plants, which the decision-making. “They are more leery sect, gleaned from the taste of its saliva.
Darwins demonstrated could sense light, of actual vegetation than they are of in- Depending on the plant and the attacker,
moisture, gravity, pressure, and several animate objects, and they respond to po- the defense might involve altering the
other environmental qualities, and then tential competitors before actually being leaf ’s flavor or texture, or producing tox-
determine the optimal trajectory for the shaded by them.” These are sophisticated ins or other compounds that render the
root’s growth. The last sentence of Dar- behaviors, but, like most plant behaviors, plant’s flesh less digestible to herbivores.
win’s 1880 book, “The Power of Move- to an animal they’re either invisible or When antelopes browse acacia trees, the
ment in Plants,” has assumed scriptural really, really slow. leaves produce tannins that make them
authority for some plant neurobiologists: The sessile life style also helps account unappetizing and difficult to digest. When
“It is hardly an exaggeration to say that for plants’ extraordinary gift for biochem- food is scarce and acacias are over-
the tip of the radicle . . . having the power istry, which far exceeds that of animals browsed, it has been reported, the trees
of directing the movements of the ad- and, arguably, of human chemists. (Many produce sufficient amounts of toxin to
joining parts, acts like the brain of one of drugs, from aspirin to opiates, derive kill the animals.
the lower animals; the brain being seated from compounds designed by plants.) Perhaps the cleverest instance of
within the anterior end of the body, re- Unable to run away, plants deploy a com- plant signalling involves two insect spe-
ceiving impressions from the sense or- plex molecular vocabulary to signal dis- cies, the first in the role of pest and the
gans and directing the several move- tress, deter or poison enemies, and re- second as its exterminator. Several spe-
ments.” Darwin was asking us to think of cruit animals to perform various services cies, including corn and lima beans,
the plant as a kind of upside-down ani- for them. A recent study in Science found emit a chemical distress call when at-
mal, with its main sensory organs and that the caffeine produced by many plants tacked by caterpillars. Parasitic wasps
“brain” on the bottom, underground, and may function not only as a defense chem- some distance away lock in on that
its sexual organs on top. ical, as had previously been thought, but scent, follow it to the afflicted plant, and
Scientists have since found that the in some cases as a psychoactive drug in proceed to slowly destroy the caterpil-
tips of plant roots, in addition to sensing their nectar. The caffeine encourages lars. Scientists call these insects “plant
gravity, moisture, light, pressure, and bees to remember a particular plant and bodyguards.”
hardness, can also sense volume, nitro- return to it, making them more faithful
gen, phosphorus, salt, various toxins, mi-
crobes, and chemical signals from neigh-
boring plants. Roots about to encounter
and effective pollinators.
One of the most productive areas of
plant research in recent years has been
P lants speak in a chemical vocabulary
we can’t directly perceive or compre-
hend. The first important discoveries in
an impenetrable obstacle or a toxic sub- plant signalling. Since the early nineteen- plant communication were made in the
stance change course before they make eighties, it has been known that when a lab in the nineteen-eighties, by isolating
contact with it. Roots can tell whether plant’s leaves are infected or chewed by plants and their chemical emissions in
nearby roots are self or other and, if other,
kin or stranger. Normally, plants com-
pete for root space with strangers, but,
when researchers put four closely related
Great Lakes sea-rocket plants (Cakile
edentula) in the same pot, the plants re-
strained their usual competitive behav-
iors and shared resources.
Somehow, a plant gathers and inte-
grates all this information about its envi-
ronment, and then “decides”—some sci-
entists deploy the quotation marks,
indicating metaphor at work; others drop
them—in precisely what direction to
deploy its roots or its leaves. Once the
definition of “behavior” expands to in-
clude such things as a shift in the trajec-
tory of a root, a reallocation of resources,
or the emission of a powerful chemical,
plants begin to look like much more ac-
tive agents, responding to environmental
cues in ways more subtle or adaptive
than the word “instinct” would suggest.
Plexiglas chambers, but Rick Karban, the the lab, a gas chromatograph-mass spec- that the name had been changed after a
U.C. Davis ecologist, and others have set trometer would yield a list of the com- lively internal debate; she felt that jetti-
themselves the messier task of studying pounds collected—more than a hundred soning “neurobiology” was probably for
how plants exchange chemical signals in all. Blande offered to let me put my the best. “I was told by someone at the
outdoors, in a natural setting. Recently, I nose in one of the bags; the air was pow- National Science Foundation that the
visited Karban’s study plot at the Univer- erfully aromatic, with a scent closer to af- N.S.F. would never fund anything with
sity of California’s Sagehen Creek Field tershave than to perfume. Gazing across the words ‘plant neurobiology’ in it. He
Station, a few miles outside Truckee. On the meadow of sagebrush, I found it said, and I quote, ‘ “Neuro” belongs to
a sun-flooded hillside high in the Sierras, difficult to imagine the invisible chemical animals.’ ” (An N.S.F. spokesperson said
he introduced me to the ninety-nine chatter, including the calls of distress, that, while the society is not eligible for
sagebrush plants—low, slow-growing going on all around—or that these mo- funding by the foundation’s neurobiology
gray-green shrubs marked with plastic tionless plants were engaged in any kind program, “the N.S.F. does not have a
flags—that he and his colleagues have of “behavior” at all. boycott of any sort against the society.”)
kept under close surveillance for more Research on plant communication Two of the society’s co-founders, Stefano
than a decade. may someday benefit farmers and their Mancuso and František Baluška, argued
Karban, a fifty-nine-year-old former crops. Plant-distress chemicals could be strenuously against the name change, and
New Yorker, is slender, with a thatch of used to prime plant defenses, reducing continue to use the term “plant neurobi-
white curls barely contained by a floppy the need for pesticides. Jack Schultz, a ology” in their own work and in the
hat. He has shown that when sagebrush chemical ecologist at the University of names of their labs.
leaves are clipped in the spring—simulat- Missouri, who did some of the pioneer- The meeting consisted of three days of
ing an insect attack that triggers the re- ing work on plant signalling in the early PowerPoint presentations delivered in a
lease of volatile chemicals—both the nineteen-eighties, is helping to develop large, modern lecture hall at the Univer-
clipped plant and its unclipped neighbors a mechanical “nose” that, attached to a sity of British Columbia before a hundred
suffer significantly less insect damage tractor and driven through a field, could or so scientists. Most of the papers were
over the season. Karban believes that the help farmers identify plants under insect highly technical presentations on plant
plant is alerting all its leaves to the pres- attack, allowing them to spray pesticides signalling—the kind of incremental sci-
ence of a pest, but its neighbors pick up only when and where they are needed. ence that takes place comfortably within
the signal, too, and gird themselves Karban told me that, in the nineteen- the confines of an established scientific
against attack. “We think the sagebrush eighties, people working on plant com- paradigm, which plant signalling has be-
are basically eavesdropping on one an- munication faced some of the same out- come. But a handful of speakers presented
other,” Karban said. He found that the rage that scientists working on plant work very much within the new paradigm
more closely related the plants the more intelligence (a term he cautiously accepts) of plant intelligence, and they elicited
likely they are to respond to the chemical do today. “This stuff has been enor- strong reactions.
signal, suggesting that plants may display mously contentious,” he says, referring to The most controversial presentation
a form of kin recognition. Helping out the early days of research into plant com- was “Animal-Like Learning in Mimosa
your relatives is a good way to improve munication, work that is now generally Pudica,” an unpublished paper by Mon-
the odds that your genes will survive. accepted. “It took me years to get some ica Gagliano, a thirty-seven-year-old an-
The field work and data collection that of these papers published. People would imal ecologist at the University of West-
go into making these discoveries are literally be screaming at one another at ern Australia who was working in
painstaking in the extreme. At the bottom scientific meetings.” He added, “Plant Mancuso’s lab in Florence. Gagliano,
of a meadow raked by the slanted light of scientists in general are incredibly conser- who is tall, with long brown hair parted
late summer, two collaborators from vative. We all think we want to hear in the middle, based her experiment on a
Japan, Kaori Shiojiri and Satomi Ishizaki, novel ideas, but we don’t, not really.” set of protocols commonly used to test
worked in the shade of a small pine, learning in animals. She focussed on an
squatting over branches of sagebrush that
Karban had tagged and cut. Using click-
ers, they counted every trident-shaped leaf
I first met Karban at a scientific meet-
ing in Vancouver last July, when he
presented a paper titled “Plant Commu-
elementary type of learning called “habit-
uation,” in which an experimental subject
is taught to ignore an irrelevant stimulus.
on every branch, and then counted and re- nication and Kin Recognition in Sage- “Habituation enables an organism to
corded every instance of leaf damage, one brush.” The meeting would have been the focus on the important information,
column for insect bites, another for dis- sixth gathering of the Society for Plant while filtering out the rubbish,” Gagliano
ease. At the top of the meadow, another Neurobiology, if not for the fact that, explained to the audience of plant scien-
collaborator, James Blande, a chemical under pressure from certain quarters of tists. How long does it take the animal to
ecologist from England, tied plastic bags the scientific establishment, the group’s recognize that a stimulus is “rubbish,”
around sagebrush stems and inflated the name had been changed four years earlier and then how long will it remember what
bags with filtered air. After waiting twenty to the less provocative Society for Plant it has learned? Gagliano’s experimental
minutes for the leaves to emit their vola- Signaling and Behavior. The plant biol- question was bracing: Could the same
tiles, he pumped the air through a metal ogist Elizabeth Van Volkenburgh, of the thing be done with a plant?
cylinder containing an absorbent material University of Washington, who was one Mimosa pudica, also called the “sensi-
that collected the chemical emissions. At of the founders of the society, told me tive plant,” is that rare plant species with
96 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
sKETCHBOOK BY ROz CHAsT

THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013 97


“Why did we buy such huge furniture?”

• •

a behavior so speedy and visible that an- respond.” Gagliano reported that she plenty of time to rebuild their energy
imals can observe it; the Venus flytrap is retested her plants after a week and reserves.
another. When the fernlike leaves of the found that they continued to disregard On my way out of the lecture hall, I
mimosa are touched, they instantly fold the drop stimulus, indicating that they bumped into Fred Sack, a prominent
up, presumably to frighten insects. The “remembered” what they had learned. botanist at the University of British Co-
mimosa also collapses its leaves when Even after twenty-eight days, the lesson lumbia. I asked him what he thought
the plant is dropped or jostled. Gagliano had not been forgotten. She reminded of Gagliano’s presentation. “Bullshit,” he
potted fifty-six mimosa plants and her colleagues that, in similar experi- replied. He explained that the word
rigged a system to drop them from a ments with bees, the insects forgot what “learning” implied a brain and should be
height of fifteen centimetres every five they had learned after just forty-eight reserved for animals: “Animals can ex-
seconds. Each “training session” in- hours. Gagliano concluded by suggest- hibit learning, but plants evolve adapta-
volved sixty drops. She reported that ing that “brains and neurons are a so- tions.” He was making a distinction be-
some of the mimosas started to reopen phisticated solution but not a necessary tween behavioral changes that occur
their leaves after just four, five, or six requirement for learning,” and that within the lifetime of an organism and
drops, as if they had concluded that the there is “some unifying mechanism those which arise across generations. At
stimulus could be safely ignored. “By the across living systems that can process in- lunch, I sat with a Russian scientist, who
end, they were completely open,” Ga- formation and learn.” was equally dismissive. “It’s not learning,”
gliano said to the audience. “They A lively exchange followed. Some- he said. “So there’s nothing to discuss.”
couldn’t care less anymore.” one objected that dropping a plant was Later that afternoon, Gagliano seemed
Was it just fatigue? Apparently not: not a relevant trigger, since that doesn’t both stung by some of the reactions to
when the plants were shaken, they again happen in nature. Gagliano pointed out her presentation and defiant. Adapta-
closed up. “ ‘Oh, this is something that electric shock, an equally artificial tion is far too slow a process to explain
new,’ ” Gagliano said, imagining these trigger, is often used in animal-learning the behavior she had observed, she told
events from the plants’ point of view. experiments. Another scientist sug- me. “How can they be adapted to some-
“You see, you want to be attuned to gested that perhaps her plants were not thing they have never experienced in
something new coming in. Then we habituated, just tuckered out. She ar- their real world?” She noted that some
went back to the drops, and they didn’t gued that twenty-eight days would be of her plants learned faster than others,
98 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
evidence that “this is not an innate or Mancuso is the poet-philosopher of biology; his lab also receives grants from
programmed response.” Many of the the movement, determined to win for the European Union.
scientists in her audience were just get- plants the recognition they deserve and, Early in our conversation, I asked
ting used to the ideas of plant “behavior” perhaps, bring humans down a peg in Mancuso for his definition of “intelli-
and “memory” (terms that even Fred the process. His somewhat grandly gence.” Spending so much time with the
Sack said he was willing to accept); named International Laboratory of plant neurobiologists, I could feel my
using words like “learning” and “intelli- Plant Neurobiology, a few miles outside grasp on the word getting less sure. It
gence” in plants struck them, in Sack’s Florence, occupies a modest suite of turns out that I am not alone: philoso-
words, as “inappropriate” and “just labs and offices in a low-slung modern phers and psychologists have been argu-
weird.” When I described the experi- building. Here a handful of collabora- ing over the definition of intelligence for
ment to Lincoln Taiz, he suggested the tors and graduate students work on the at least a century, and whatever consen-
words “habituation” or “desensitization” experiments Mancuso devises to test sus there may once have been has been
would be more appropriate than “learn- the intelligence of plants. Giving a tour rapidly slipping away. Most definitions of
ing.” Gagliano said that her mimosa of the labs, he showed me maize plants, intelligence fall into one of two categories.
paper had been rejected by ten journals: grown under lights, that were being The first is worded so that intelligence re-
“None of the reviewers had problems taught to ignore shadows; a poplar sap- quires a brain; the definition refers to in-
with the data.” Instead, they balked at ling hooked up to a galvanometer to trinsic mental qualities such as reason,
the language she used to describe the measure its response to air pollution; judgment, and abstract thought. The sec-
data. But she didn’t want to change it. and a chamber in which a PTR-TOF ma- ond category, less brain-bound and meta-
“Unless we use the same language to de- chine—an advanced kind of mass spec- physical, stresses behavior, defining intel-
scribe the same behavior”—exhibited by trometer—continuously read all the ligence as the ability to respond in optimal
plants and animals—“we can’t compare volatiles emitted by a succession of ways to the challenges presented by one’s
it,” she said. plants, from poplars and tobacco plants environment and circumstances. Not sur-
Rick Karban consoled Gagliano after to peppers and olive trees. “We are prisingly, the plant neurobiologists jump
her talk. “I went through the same thing, making a dictionary of each species’ en- into this second camp.
just getting totally hammered,” he told tire chemical vocabulary,” he explained. “I define it very simply,” Mancuso
her. “But you’re doing good work. The He estimates that a plant has three said. “Intelligence is the ability to solve
system is just not ready.” When I asked thousand chemicals in its vocabulary, problems.” In place of a brain, “what I am
him what he thought of Gagliano’s while, he said with a smile, “the average looking for is a distributed sort of intelli-
paper, he said, “I don’t know if she’s got student has only seven hundred words.” gence, as we see in the swarming of
everything nailed down, but it’s a very Mancuso is fiercely devoted to plants— birds.” In a flock, each bird has only to
cool idea that deserves to get out there a scientist needs to “love” his subject in follow a few simple rules, such as main-
and be discussed. I hope she doesn’t get order to do it justice, he says. He is also taining a prescribed distance from its
discouraged.” gentle and unassuming, even when what neighbor, yet the collective effect of a
he is saying is outrageous. In the corner of great many birds executing a simple algo-

S cientists are often uncomfortable


talking about the role of metaphor
and imagination in their work, yet sci-
his office sits a forlorn Ficus benjamina, or
weeping fig, and on the walls are photo-
rithm is a complex and supremely well-
coördinated behavior. Mancuso’s hy-
pothesis is that something similar is at
entific progress often depends on both. work in plants, with their thousands of
“Metaphors help stimulate the investi- root tips playing the role of the individ-
gative imagination of good scientists,” ual birds—gathering and assessing data
the British plant scientist Anthony from the environment and responding in
Trewavas wrote in a spirited response to local but coördinated ways that benefit
the Alpi letter denouncing plant neuro- the entire organism.
biology. “Plant neurobiology” is obvi- “Neurons perhaps are overrated,”
ously a metaphor—plants don’t possess Mancuso said. “They’re really just excit-
the type of excitable, communicative able cells.” Plants have their own excit-
cells we call neurons. Yet the introduc- graphs of Mancuso in an astronaut’s able cells, many of them in a region just
tion of the term has raised a series of jumpsuit floating in the cabin of a zero- behind the root tip. Here Mancuso and
questions and inspired a set of experi- gravity aircraft; he has collaborated with his frequent collaborator, František
ments that promise to deepen our un- the European Space Agency, which has Baluška, have detected unusually high
derstanding not only of plants but po- supported his research on plant behavior levels of electrical activity and oxygen
tentially also of brains. If there are other in micro- and hyper-gravity. (One of his consumption. They’ve hypothesized in
ways of processing information, other experiments was carried on board the last a series of papers that this so-called
kinds of cells and cell networks that can flight of the space shuttle Endeavor, in “transition zone” may be the locus of the
somehow give rise to intelligent behav- May of 2011.) A decade ago, Mancuso “root brain” first proposed by Darwin.
ior, then we may be more inclined to persuaded a Florentine bank foundation The idea remains unproved and contro-
ask, with Mancuso, “What’s so special to underwrite much of his research and versial. “What’s going on there is not
about neurons?” help launch the Society for Plant Neuro- well understood,” Lincoln Taiz told me,
THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013 99
“but there is no evidence it is a com- with that slippery word “intelligence.” so-called “epigenetic” effect can persist
mand center.” Particularly when there is no dominant and sometimes be passed down to
How plants do what they do without a definition (and when measurements of offspring. More recently, scientists have
brain—what Anthony Trewavas has intelligence, such as I.Q., have been found that life events such as trauma or
called their “mindless mastery”—raises shown to be culturally biased), it is pos- starvation produce epigenetic changes in
questions about how our brains do what sible to define intelligence in a way that animal brains (coding for high levels of
they do. When I asked Mancuso about either reinforces the boundary between cortisol, for example) that are long-last-
the function and location of memory in animals and plants (say, one that entails ing and can also be passed down to
plants, he speculated about the possible abstract thought) or undermines it. Plant offspring, a form of memory much like
role of calcium channels and other mech- neurobiologists have chosen to define in- that observed in plants.
anisms, but then he reminded me that telligence democratically, as an ability to While talking with Mancuso, I
mystery still surrounds where and how our solve problems or, more precisely, to re- kept thinking about words like “will,”
memories are stored: “It could be the same spond adaptively to circumstances, in- “choice,” and “intention,” which he seemed
kind of machinery, and figuring it out in cluding ones unforeseen in the genome. to attribute to plants rather casually, al-
plants may help us figure it out in hu- “I agree that humans are special,” most as if they were acting consciously.
mans.” Mancuso says. “We are the first species At one point, he told me about the dod-
The hypothesis that intelligent be- able to argue about what intelligence is. der vine, Cuscuta europaea, a parasitic
havior in plants may be an emergent But it’s the quantity, not the quality” of white vine that winds itself around the
property of cells exchanging signals in a intelligence that sets us apart. We exist stalk of another plant and sucks nourish-
network might sound far-fetched, yet the on a continuum with the acacia, the rad- ment from it. A dodder vine will “choose”
way that intelligence emerges from a ish, and the bacterium. “Intelligence is a among several potential hosts, assessing,
network of neurons may not be very property of life,” he says. I asked him why by scent, which offers the best potential
different. Most neuroscientists would he thinks people have an easier time nourishment. Having selected a target,
agree that, while brains considered as a granting intelligence to computers than the vine then performs a kind of cost-
whole function as centralized command to plants. (Fred Sack told me that he can benefit calculation before deciding ex-
centers for most animals, within the abide the term “artificial intelligence,” actly how many coils it should invest—
brain there doesn’t appear to be any com- because the intelligence in this case is the more nutrients in the victim, the
mand post; rather, one finds a leaderless modified by the word “artificial,” but not more coils it deploys. I asked Mancuso
network. That sense we get when we “plant intelligence.” He offered no argu- whether he was being literal or meta-
think about what might govern a plant— ment, except to say, “I’m in the majority phorical in attributing intention to
that there is no there there, no wizard be- in saying it’s a little weird.”) Mancuso plants.
hind the curtain pulling the levers—may thinks we’re willing to accept artificial in- “Here, I’ll show you something,”
apply equally well to our brains. telligence because computers are our cre- he said. “Then you tell me if plants
ations, and so reflect our own intelligence have intention.” He swivelled his com-

I n Martin Amis’s 1995 novel, “The


Information,” we meet a character
who aspires to write “The History of In-
back at us. They are also our dependents,
unlike plants: “If we were to vanish to-
morrow, the plants would be fine, but if
puter monitor around and clicked open
a video.
Time-lapse photography is perhaps
creasing Humiliation,” a treatise chroni- the plants vanished . . .” Our dependence the best tool we have to bridge the
cling the gradual dethronement of hu- on plants breeds a contempt for them, chasm between the time scale at which
mankind from its position at the center Mancuso believes. In his somewhat plants live and our own. This example
of the universe, beginning with Coperni- topsy-turvy view, plants “remind us of was of a young bean plant, shot in the
cus. “Every century we get smaller,” Amis our weakness.” lab over two days, one frame every ten
writes. Next came Darwin, who brought “Memory” may be an even thornier minutes. A metal pole on a dolly stands
the humbling news that we are the prod- word to apply across kingdoms, perhaps a couple of feet away. The bean plant is
uct of the same natural laws that created because we know so little about how it “looking” for something to climb. Each
animals. In the last century, the formerly works. We tend to think of memories as spring, I witness the same process in my
sharp lines separating humans from ani- immaterial, but in animal brains some garden, in real time. I always assumed
mals—our monopolies on language, rea- forms of memory involve the laying that the bean plants simply grow this
son, toolmaking, culture, even self-con- down of new connections in a network of way or that, until they eventually bump
sciousness—have been blurred, one after neurons. Yet there are ways to store in- into something suitable to climb. But
another, as science has granted these ca- formation biologically that don’t require Mancuso’s video seems to show that this
pabilities to other animals. neurons. Immune cells “remember” their bean plant “knows” exactly where the
Mancuso and his colleagues are writ- experience of pathogens, and call on that metal pole is long before it makes con-
ing the next chapter in “The History of memory in subsequent encounters. In tact with it. Mancuso speculates that the
Increasing Humiliation.” Their project plants, it has long been known that expe- plant could be employing a form of
entails breaking down the walls between riences such as stress can alter the molec- echolocation. There is some evidence
the kingdoms of plants and animals, and ular wrapping around the chromosomes; that plants make low clicking sounds as
it is proceeding not only experiment by this, in turn, determines which genes will their cells elongate; it’s possible that they
experiment but also word by word. Start be silenced and which expressed. This can sense the reflection of those sound
100 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
waves bouncing off the metal pole. that plants don’t possess it. But if we What’s more, when plants are injured or
The bean plant wastes no time or en- define the term simply as the state of stressed, they produce a chemical—eth-
ergy “looking”—that is, growing—any- being awake and aware of one’s environ- ylene—that works as an anesthetic on
where but in the direction of the pole. ment—“online,” as the neuroscientists animals. When I learned this startling
And it is striving (there is no other word say—then plants may qualify as con- fact from Baluška in Vancouver, I asked
for it) to get there: reaching, stretching, scious beings, at least according to Man- him, gingerly, if he meant to suggest that
throwing itself over and over like a fly cuso and Baluška. “The bean knows ex- plants could feel pain. Baluška, who has
rod, extending itself a few more inches actly what is in the environment around a gruff mien and a large bullet-shaped
with every cast, as it attempts to wrap its it,” Mancuso said. “We don’t know how. head, raised one eyebrow and shot me a
curling tip around the pole. As soon as But this is one of the features of con- look that I took to mean he deemed my
contact is made, the plant appears to sciousness: You know your position in question impertinent or absurd. But ap-
relax; its clenched leaves begin to flutter the world. A stone does not.” parently not.
mildly. All this may be nothing more In support of their contention that “If plants are conscious, then, yes,
than an illusion of time-lapse photogra- plants are conscious of their environ- they should feel pain,” he said. “If you
phy. Yet to watch the video is to feel, ment, Mancuso and Baluška point out don’t feel pain, you ignore danger and
momentarily, like one of the aliens in that plants can be rendered unconscious you don’t survive. Pain is adaptive.” I
Mancuso’s formative science-fiction by the same anesthetics that put ani- must have shown some alarm. “That’s a
story, shown a window onto a dimen- mals out: drugs can induce in plants an scary idea,” he acknowledged with a
sion of time in which these formerly unresponsive state resembling sleep. shrug. “We live in a world where we
inert beings come astonishingly to life, (A snoozing Venus flytrap won’t no- must eat other organisms.”
seemingly conscious individuals with tice an insect crossing its threshold.) Unprepared to consider the ethical
intentions.
In October, I loaded the bean video
onto my laptop and drove down to Santa
Cruz to play it for Lincoln Taiz. He
began by questioning its value as scientific
data: “Maybe he has ten other videos
where the bean didn’t do that. You can’t
take one interesting variation and gener-
alize from it.” The bean’s behavior was,
in other words, an anecdote, not a phe-
nomenon. Taiz also pointed out that the
bean in the video was leaning toward the
pole in the first frame. Mancuso then
sent me another video with two perfectly
upright bean plants that exhibited very
similar behavior. Taiz was now intrigued.
“If he sees that effect consistently, it
would be exciting,” he said—but it would
not necessarily be evidence of plant in-
tention. “If the phenomenon is real, it
would be classified as a tropism,” such as
the mechanism that causes plants to
bend toward light. In this case, the stim-
ulus remains unknown, but tropisms “do
not require one to postulate either inten-
tionality or ‘brainlike’ conceptualiza-
tion,” Taiz said. “The burden of proof for
the latter interpretation would clearly be
on Stefano.”

P erhaps the most troublesome and


troubling word of all in thinking
about plants is “consciousness.” If con-
sciousness is defined as inward awareness
of oneself experiencing reality—“the
feeling of what happens,” in the words of
the neuroscientist Antonio Damasio—
then we can (probably) safely conclude “This is where we practice our warning shots.”
implications of plant intelligence, I could Their attempt to broaden these defini- just one of nature’s ways of getting com-
feel my resistance to the whole idea tions is made easier by the fact that the plex jobs done, for dealing intelligently
stiffen. Descartes, who believed that only meanings of so many of these terms are up with the challenges presented by the en-
humans possessed self-consciousness, for grabs. At the same time, since these vironment. But they are not the only
was unable to credit the idea that other words were originally created to describe way: “Yes, I would argue that intelligent
animals could suffer from pain. So he animal attributes, we shouldn’t be sur- behavior is a property of life.”
dismissed their screams and howls as prised at the awkward fit with plants. It
mere reflexes, as meaningless physiolog-
ical noise. Could it be remotely possible
that we are now making the same mis-
seems likely that, if the plant neurobiolo-
gists were willing to add the prefix “plant-
specific” to intelligence and learning and
T o define certain words in such a way
as to bring plants and animals be-
neath the same semantic umbrella—
take with plants? That the perfume of memory and consciousness (as Mancuso whether of intelligence or intention or
jasmine or basil, or the scent of freshly and Baluška are prepared to do in the case learning—is a philosophical choice with
mowed grass, so sweet to us, is (as the of pain), then at least some of this “scien- important consequences for how we see
ecologist Jack Schultz likes to say) the tific controversy” might evaporate. ourselves in nature. Since “ The Origin of
chemical equivalent of a scream? Or have Indeed, I found more consensus on Species,” we have understood, at least in-
we, merely by posing such a question, the underlying science than I expected. tellectually, the continuities among life’s
fallen back into the muddied waters of Even Clifford Slayman, the Yale biolo- kingdoms—that we are all cut from the
“The Secret Life of Plants”? gist who signed the 2007 letter dismiss- same fabric of nature. Yet our big brains,
Lincoln Taiz has little patience for the ing plant neurobiology, is willing to ac- and perhaps our experience of inward-
notion of plant pain, questioning what, knowledge that, although he doesn’t ness, allow us to feel that we must be fun-
in the absence of a brain, would be doing think plants possess intelligence, he does damentally different—suspended above
the feeling. He puts it succinctly: “No believe they are capable of “intelligent nature and other species as if by some
brain, no pain.” Mancuso is more cir- behavior,” in the same way that bees and metaphysical “skyhook,” to borrow a
cumspect. We can never determine with ants are. In an e-mail exchange, Slayman phrase from the philosopher Daniel Den-
certainty whether plants feel pain or made a point of underlining this distinc- nett. Plant neurobiologists are intent on
whether their perception of injury is tion: “We do not know what constitutes taking away our skyhook, completing
sufficiently like that of animals to be intelligence, only what we can observe the revolution that Darwin started but
called by the same word. (He and and judge as intelligent behavior.” He which remains—psychologically, at
Baluška are careful to write of “plant- defined “intelligent behavior” as “the least—incomplete.
specific pain perception.”) “We just don’t ability to adapt to changing circum- “What we learned from Darwin is
know, so we must be silent.” stances” and noted that it “must always that competence precedes comprehen-
Mancuso believes that, because plants be measured relative to a particular envi- sion,” Dennett said when I called to talk
are sensitive and intelligent beings, we ronment.” Humans may or may not be to him about plant neurobiology. Upon
are obliged to treat them with some de- intrinsically more intelligent than cats, he a foundation of the simplest compe-
gree of respect. That means protecting wrote, but when a cat is confronted with tences—such as the on-off switch in a
their habitats from destruction and avoid- a mouse its behavior is likely to be de- computer, or the electrical and chemical
ing practices such as genetic manipula- monstrably more intelligent. signalling of a cell—can be built higher
tion, growing plants in monocultures, Slayman went on to acknowledge that and higher competences until you wind
and training them in bonsai. But it does “intelligent behavior could perfectly well up with something that looks very much
not prevent us from eating them. “Plants develop without such a nerve center or like intelligence. “The idea that there is
evolved to be eaten—it is part of their headquarters or director or brain— a bright line, with real comprehension
evolutionary strategy,” he said. He cited whatever you want to call it. Instead of and real minds on the far side of the
their modular structure and lack of irre- ‘brain,’ think ‘network.’ It seems to be chasm, and animals or plants on the
placeable organs in support of this view. that many higher organisms are inter- other—that’s an archaic myth.” To say
The central issue dividing the plant nally networked in such a way that local that higher competences such as intelli-
neurobiologists from their critics would changes,” such as the way that roots re- gence, learning, and memory “mean
appear to be this: Do capabilities such as spond to a water gradient, “cause very nothing in the absence of brains” is, in
intelligence, pain perception, learning, local responses which benefit the entire Dennett’s view, “cerebrocentric.”
and memory require the existence of a organism.” Seen that way, he added, the All species face the same existential
brain, as the critics contend, or can they be outlook of Mancuso and Trewavas is challenges—obtaining food, defending
detached from their neurobiological “pretty much in line with my under- themselves, reproducing—but under
moorings? The question is as much phil- standing of biochemical/ biological net- wildly varying circumstances, and so they
osophical as it is scientific, since the an- works.” He pointed out that while it is an have evolved wildly different tools in
swer depends on how these terms get understandable human prejudice to favor order to survive. Brains come in handy
defined. The proponents of plant intelli- the “nerve center” model, we also have a for creatures that move around a lot; but
gence argue that the traditional definitions second, autonomic nervous system gov- they’re a disadvantage for ones that are
of these terms are anthropocentric—a erning our digestive processes, which rooted in place. Impressive as it is to us,
clever reply to the charges of anthropo- “operates most of the time without in- self-consciousness is just another tool for
morphism frequently thrown at them. structions from higher up.” Brains are living, good for some jobs, unhelpful for
102 THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013
GREETINGS, FRIENDS!
BY IAN FRAZIER

L ook earthward, angels! Here we are


With eyes affixed upon the star,
Winter moon, or Higher Power,
Mamie Gummer, Jakob Dylan,
Zosia Mamet, Allie Williams,
Cordell Broadus, J. Bongiovi,
Rising Dow, or Freedom Tower— Bria Murphy, Rainey Qualley,
With hearts uplifted, anyway, Danis Banks, Julie Pacino,
Waltzing through the holiday. And the Smiths: Trey, Jaden, Willow.
We have hired a party planner, The young folk laugh amongst themselves;
Opened the ancestral manor, Let’s move on now and find the elves
So c’mon over! All invited! Who circulate with trays of snacks,
Here, chez nous, the luau’s sited. And gifts in massive Santa packs,
Pig in the ground and beer on ice, Dispensing them to Victor Cruz,
We do intend to celebrice, Gov. Jerry Brown and Katherine Boo,
Or celebreeze, or celebrate— Bey and Jay (a.k.a. Hova),
Get geezer-funky, at any rate— And Irina Prokhorova,
Cut a rug, freak out the children, Terry Jones, Neil Patrick Harris,
Chuck the tsuris, loose the burden, Hoda Kotb, the Yogi Berras,
Ditch the iPhones, scotch our reason, Austan Goolsbee, Lorin Stein,
And sing praises of the season. And the Naomis, Wolf and Klein.
A silver ball’s fish-eye mirror
The guest list rocks! Let’s say “Hi-dee!” Shows Lester Holt coming nearer.
To tattoo artist Kat Von D, Hi, Les! What’s new? Meet our pal Pink!
Andy Samberg, Adam Sandler, Let’s step onto our indoor rink
Chelsea Clinton, Chelsea Handler, And slide around with Ron K. Unz,
Helen Vendler, Steven Weinberg, C. K. Williams, the St. Louis nuns,
Mayor Bloomberg, Gayfryd Steinberg, Rosie Perez, Wendy Davis,
Marie D’Origny, James Earl Jones, Warren Buffett, Darrelle Revis,
Joyce Carol Oates, the Mellowtones, Bill McKibben, Tori Spelling,
Justice Roberts, Robert Silvers, Jason Wu, and Diane Snelling.
Jimmy Fallon, Robin Quivers, We’ll hug John Boehner, just for grins,
Aretha Franklin, Ban Ki-moon, And the surprise as he begins
Kate Upton and Cindy Lou Who. To hug us back without thinking
Hot punch smokes upon the burner (The lesson here is ego-shrinking),
For the Herzogs (Lena, Werner), Then all yell yee-haw hip-HOORAY
Julian Bond and Julian Barnes, For brave Malala Yousafzai!
Daniel Day-Lewis and Kim Carnes,
Judith Shulevitz, Mary Karr,
Heidi Julavits, wry Bill Maher,
Chris Rock, Joe Buck, Jean Strouse, Sam Nunn,
D ear friends, this year was just so-so.
Things might not look too good, we know.
Near the sun a comet crumbles;
And S. Epatha Merkerson. Heaven’s vast-y aspect humbles
Sandra Oh grabs Darren Criss, All our works and our persuasions,
Bestows on him a quick air kiss; Tiny gains and huge evasions.
Freeman Dyson, Morgan Freeman, But our good cheer is resolute!
Amber Tamblyn, Judith Thurman, The kids are great! The baby’s cute!
J. Lo’s new love, Casper Smart, Hope keeps going, growing on us;
That crazy, wild Garfunkel (Art), Faith pays off with time, we promise!
Mayor-elect Bill de and family While one more yearly revel flies,
All mug for selfies, hammily. Calendar-page-like, ’cross the skies,
roman muradov

Here’s an insight we just had: We shall be anticipatory


New generations make us glad! Of new gladness, even glory;
So hurrah for Maia Keillor, And blessings send: Good will to all,
Rooney Mara (née part Steeler), Till Christmas starts again next fall.

THE NEW
THE NEW YORKER,
YORKER, DECEMBER
DECEMBER 23
23 && 30,
30, 2013
2013 103
103
others. That humans would rate this will too, but that is a separate issue.” Some scientists working on plant intel-
particular adaptation so highly is not sur- I asked Mancuso if he thought that a ligence have questioned whether the
prising, since it has been the shining des- plant decides in the same way we might “animal-centric” emphasis, along with
tination of our long evolutionary journey, choose at a deli between a Reuben or lox the obsession with the term “neurobiol-
along with the epiphenomenon of self- and bagels. ogy,” has been a mistake and possibly an
consciousness that we call “free will.” “Yes, in the same way,” Mancuso insult to the plants. “I have no interest in
In addition to being a plant physiolo- wrote back, though he indicated that he making plants into little animals,” one
gist, Lincoln Taiz writes about the his- had no idea what a Reuben was. “Just put scientist wrote during the dustup over
tory of science. “Starting with Darwin’s ammonium nitrate in the place of Reu- what to call the society. “Plants are
grandfather, Erasmus,” he told me, “there ben sandwich (whatever it is) and phos- unique,” another wrote. “There is no rea-
has been a strain of teleology in the study phate instead of salmon, and the roots son to . . . call them demi-animals.”
of plant biology”—a habit of ascribing will make a decision.” But isn’t the root When I met Mancuso for dinner
purpose or intention to the behavior of responding simply to the net flow of cer- during the conference in Vancouver, he
plants. I asked Taiz about the question of tain chemicals? “I’m afraid our brain sounded very much like a plant scientist
“choice,” or decision-making, in plants, makes decisions in the same exact way.” getting over a case of “brain envy”—what
as when they must decide between two Taiz had suggested was motivating the
conflicting environmental signals—
water and gravity, for example.
“Does the plant decide in the same

W hy would a plant care about
Mozart?” the late ethnobotanist
Tim Plowman would reply when asked
plant neurologists. If we could begin to
understand plants on their own terms,
he said, “it would be like being in contact
way that we choose at a deli between a about the wonders catalogued in “The with an alien culture. But we could have
Reuben sandwich or lox and bagel?” Secret Life of Plants.” “And even if it did, all the advantages of that contact with-
Taiz asked. “No, the plant response is why should that impress us? They can eat out any of the problems—because it
based entirely on the net flow of auxin light, isn’t that enough?” doesn’t want to destroy us!” How do
and other chemical signals. The verb One way to exalt plants is by demon- plants do all the amazing things they do
‘decide’ is inappropriate in a plant con- strating their animal-like capabilities. without brains? Without locomotion?
text. It implies free will. Of course, one But another way is to focus on all the By focussing on the otherness of plants
could argue that humans lack free things plants can do that we cannot. rather than on their likeness, Mancuso
suggested, we stand to learn valuable
things and develop important new tech-
nologies. This was to be the theme of his
presentation to the conference, the fol-
lowing morning, on what he called “bio-
inspiration.” How might the example of
plant intelligence help us design better
computers, or robots, or networks?
Mancuso was about to begin a collab-
oration with a prominent computer sci-
entist to design a plant-based computer,
modelled on the distributed computing
performed by thousands of roots process-
ing a vast number of environmental vari-
ables. His collaborator, Andrew Ad-
amatzky, the director of the International
Center of Unconventional Computing,
at the University of the West of England,
has worked extensively with slime molds,
harnessing their maze-navigating and
computational abilities. (Adamatzky’s slime
molds, which are a kind of amoeba, grow
in the direction of multiple food sources
simultaneously, usually oat flakes, in the
process computing and remembering the
shortest distance between any two of
them; he has used these organisms to
model transportation networks.) In an
e-mail, Adamatzky said that, as a substrate
for biological computing, plants offered
both advantages and disadvantages over
“The forecast for today is grumpy.” slime molds. “Plants are more robust,” he
wrote, and “can keep their shape for a
very long time,” although they are slower-
growing and lack the flexibility of slime
molds. But because plants are already
“analog electrical computers,” trafficking
in electrical inputs and outputs, he is
hopeful that he and Mancuso will be able
to harness them for computational tasks.
Mancuso was also working with Bar-
bara Mazzolai, a biologist-turned-en-
gineer at the Italian Institute of Technol-
ogy, in Genoa, to design what he called
a “plantoid”: a robot designed on plant
principles. “If you look at the history of
robots, they are always based on ani-
mals—they are humanoids or insectoids.
If you want something swimming, you
look at a fish. But what about imitating
plants instead? What would that allow
you to do? Explore the soil!” With a
grant from the European Union’s Future
and Emerging Technologies program,
their team is developing a “robotic root” “There’s so much evidence, we should put some aside for a different case.”
that, using plastics that can elongate and
then harden, will be able to slowly pene- • •
trate the soil, sense conditions, and alter
its trajectory accordingly. “If you want to
explore other planets, the best thing is to hubs, some with as many as forty-seven curred to me that plants do have a secret
send plantoids.” connections. The diagram of the forest life, and it is even stranger and more
The most bracing part of Mancuso’s network resembled an airline route map. wonderful than the one described by
talk on bioinspiration came when he dis- The pattern of nutrient traffic showed Tompkins and Bird. When most of us
cussed underground plant networks. Cit- how “mother trees” were using the net- think of plants, to the extent that we
ing the research of Suzanne Simard, a work to nourish shaded seedlings, in- think about plants at all, we think of
forest ecologist at the University of Brit- cluding their offspring—which the trees them as old—holdovers from a simpler,
ish Columbia, and her colleagues, Man- can apparently recognize as kin—until prehuman evolutionary past. But for
cuso showed a slide depicting how trees they’re tall enough to reach the light. Mancuso plants hold the key to a future
in a forest organize themselves into far- And, in a striking example of interspecies that will be organized around systems
flung networks, using the underground coöperation, Simard found that fir trees and technologies that are networked, de-
web of mycorrhizal fungi which connects were using the fungal web to trade nutri- centralized, modular, reiterated, redun-
their roots to exchange information and ents with paper-bark birch trees over dant—and green, able to nourish them-
even goods. This “wood-wide web,” as the course of the season. The evergreen selves on light. “Plants are the great
the title of one paper put it, allows scores species will tide over the deciduous one symbol of modernity.” Or should be:
of trees in a forest to convey warnings of when it has sugars to spare, and then their brainlessness turns out to be their
insect attacks, and also to deliver carbon, call in the debt later in the season. For strength, and perhaps the most valuable
nitrogen, and water to trees in need. the forest community, the value of this inspiration we can take from them.
When I reached Simard by phone, coöperative underground economy ap- At dinner in Vancouver, Mancuso
she described how she and her colleagues pears to be better over-all health, more said, “Since you visited me in Florence, I
track the flow of nutrients and chemical total photosynthesis, and greater resil- came across this sentence of Karl Marx,
signals through this invisible under- ience in the face of disturbance. and I became obsessed with it: ‘Every-
ground network. They injected fir trees In his talk, Mancuso juxtaposed a slide thing that is solid melts into air.’ When-
with radioactive carbon isotopes, then of the nodes and links in one of these sub- ever we build anything, it is inspired by
followed the spread of the isotopes terranean forest networks with a diagram the architecture of our bodies. So it will
through the forest community using a of the Internet, and suggested that in have a solid structure and a center, but
variety of sensing methods, including a some respects the former was superior. that is inherently fragile. This is the
Geiger counter. Within a few days, stores “Plants are able to create scalable net- meaning of that sentence—‘Everything
of radioactive carbon had been routed works of self-maintaining, self-operating, solid melts into air.’ So that’s the ques-
from tree to tree. Every tree in a plot and self-repairing units,” he said. “Plants.” tion: Can we now imagine something
thirty metres square was connected to the As I listened to Mancuso limn the completely different, something inspired
network; the oldest trees functioned as marvels unfolding beneath our feet, it oc- instead by plants?” 
THE NEW YORKER, DECEMBER 23 & 30, 2013 105

You might also like