Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Application Of

Descriptive Linguistics
To Child Language
R o b e r t W. A l b r i g h t

Joy B u c k A l b r i g h t

Modern linguistic science has two discovery consists in finding the best
chief branches, one of descriptive and scheme in terms of which to describe
the other of historical linguistics. The the facts . . .'
study of child language, from a lin- One important adaptation is to sub-
guistic point of view, is considered stitute what might be called the
to be a special branch of historical 'equivalence of recurrence' for the lin-
linguistics (8). In applying linguistic guist's 'equivalence of repetition.' Not
methods to child language, however, having adult informants who can con-
it is important to be aware of the dif- veniently repeat forms for comparison
ferences between it and conventional as being the 'same' or 'different,' lin-
language. A child's language is not guists working with children often
used by a community of speakers, and depend upon the forms recurring at
it is changing rapidly, in contrast to different times in a sample of dis-
the slow rate of change in conven- course. Thus, with an adult speaker
tional languages. Consequently, the of English, it would not be hard to
collection and analysis of specimens contrast speech forms such as you,
of child language require a special her, and him in I see you, I see her and
adaptation of the techniques of de- I see him. But if a child says OErr ZE
scriptive linguistics. Some adapta- vI~ NO, he may mean take me up, no?,
tion of .techniques is necessary, of they eat up, no?, or final ,no may
course, m any linguistic study, and represent negation as it often does in
Wells (9) has made the useful sug- child syntax, don't take me up, or
gestion that 'In descriptive linguistics, they are not eating up. 1 If one at-
Robert W. Albright (Ph.D., Stanford tempts the 'equivalence of repetition'
University, 1953) is Chairman of the De- and instructs the child to 'Say that
partment of Speech and Drama at Arizona
State College. Dr. Robert W. and Mrs.
Joy Buck Albright (B.A., University of qn the following discussion, orthographic
Washington, 1940) have their residence in representation of conventional speech forms
Tempe, Arizona. This article is adapted will be in italics and those of child forms
from a paper presented at the 1956 conven- in small caps; phonetic transcription will
tion of the Speech Association of America be bracketed, and phonemic transcription
in Chicago. will be in boldface type.
Volume 1, No. 3 257 September, 1958

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of Kansas - Libraries, Watson User on 09/25/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
2S8 JOURNAL OF SPEECH AND HEARING RESEARCH

again,' the response will usually be though there is usually a residue of


something such as DEN? (again?) or speech characteristics, sound features,
TEY WHA ANEN? (say what again?). and so forth, which cannot be tran-
So one must usually wait until the scribed with confidence. For example,
speech forms recur in the discourse, it is a fairly common mistake in tran-
in order to contrast them in environ- scribing child English to 'mishear'
ments that are as identical as possible. the glottal stop [~] as one of the con-
The collecting of data by tape-re- ventional stops: [p, t, k]. By having
cording should be done informally in at least two investigators work to-
order to elict samples of speech that gether, errors of 'mishearing' various
are as representative as possible of a features of a child's language are re-
child's language system. For example, duced.
restrictive procedures such as merely The danger of mishearing sounds
naming objects or pictures may elicit has caused some linguists to suggest
utterances which are mere 'formulas' the use of special symbols in order to
with limited sound environments: avoid the associational influences of fa-
DAT'S A (that's a), ISA (it's a) or DERSA miliar letters whose sound-values one
(there's a). Objects and picture s are will tend to 'hear.' However, the gain
useful in stimulating free, running in rigor in using special, unfamiliar
speech, however, and in providing symbols does not make up for the in-
referents .for. the speech forms that convenience of using them and the
the child uses. Parents and older sib- later confusion they may cause an in-
lings are invaluable as 'translators' in vestigator who tries to recall exactly
the sampling situation and later when what sound-values were assigned to
the data are being transcribed and them. Besides, phoneticians have long
analyzed. used the same symbols to represent
After a sample of one to two hours dissimilar sounds in different lan-
of continuous discourse has been tape- guages. The sounds of [t] and [d],
recorded, the recording is transcribed for example, are made differently in
into a 'narrow' phonetic transcrip- French and English, yet the letters
tion. 2 For greater accuracy, it is prob- may be used to represent the apical
ably best tO have at least two persons stop consonants in both languages
transcribe independently and check without confusion. The point is to
with one another at intervals of 25 make sure that the symbol represents
or 50 utterances, depending upon the a sound in a particular language and
complexity of the data. Disagreements is not assigned the value that it has in
may be resolved by rechecking the some other language. In the final pres-
tape-recording and, if the point at is- entation of child language studies,
sue seems crucial, by resampling the however, one might very well use
child-informant. Older siblings and small capitals or other special letters
parents who have a good grasp of the to avoid confusing the child's lan-
child language being studied can be guage structure with that of a con-
helpful in resolving problems, al- ventional language.
2The manual found most useful by the 'Narrow' transcriptions include fea-
authors was that of Bloch and Trager (2). tures of pitch and stress which occur
For a concise summary of field methods in in a language sample. The authors
linguistic work, see Swadesh (7), and for
special symbols and diacritics, see The Prin- have attempted to establish 'juncture'
ciples of the IPA (6). (5), the boundary features of smaller

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of Kansas - Libraries, Watson User on 09/25/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
ALBRIGHT AND ALBRIGHT: CHILD LANGUAGE 259

units within a stretch of utterance be- tion of sounds also need to be de-
tween pauses, but have not been suc- scribed.
cessful. This failure is partly due to In the following discussion con-
the disagreement among linguists as ventional symbols of the International
to how such boundaries should be de- Phonetic Alphabet are used. The
fined and their features described, and numbers placed above letters indicate
partly to the difficulty of establishing successively higher pitch levels with
regular features of juncture points in '1' for the lowest, '3' the highest, and
samples of child language. '2' the average pitch level. Four de-
In analyzing a sample of discourse, grees of stress are represented by
a stretch of continuous speech be- [', ^, '] for heavy, medium, and light,
tween pauses is usually taken as the with very light stress unmarked.
utterance unit, unless smaller units can This approach helps to get at the
be established. Using the method of systematic structure underlying the
paired comparison, recurring utter- variability of forms in a child's lan-
ance units are compared with one an- guage. The functioning of a child's
other; speech sounds and larger forms sound system is usuat~ quite com-
that seem to contrast in the same en- plicated, as the following summary of
vironments are isolated. Thus, mini- the alternation of glides in the speech
mal sound-units and minimal mean- of a 26-months-old child suggests.
ing-units (tentative 'phonemes' and The sound [w] alternated with [1]
'morphemes') are distinguished. It is initially in utterances, [1] was re-
possible that the units of child lan- placed by zero [0] 3 following [u]
guage should be classified in terms and [~], and [1] was replaced by
that would fit child speech forms [ o] following [ e] and It]. The sound
more closely. Perhaps they could be [~] was an ahernant of [1] before back
set up as 'infantemes,' units of child vowels and after back consonants.
language having varying forms. Co- The sound [w] alternated with [r]
hen (4) has suggested calling each before [i] and in clusters of a stop
,t p
element of communication an elem- plus [r] in segments such as [3tw~mi]
corn,' the unit of gesture a 'gestcom,' corresponding to English train. The
of crying and groaning a 'gtmcom' sound [w] replaced [r] in combina-
and of speech a 'parlcom.' Whatever tionations of a vowel plus [r] before
unit is employed, it is important to [ai]. In sequences of [au] plus [r]
avoid the error of classifying the units followed by a vowel, It] was re-
of a child's language as if they were placed by [~]. Zero [o] was an alter-
the same as those of conventional lan- nant of [r] in clusters of a stop plus
guage. It], and of It] plus a stop: e.g.,
In analyzing units of utterance, en- [3m~itoufbnl] corresponding to mi-
vironmental features need to be care- crophone, and [3k~'od~l] correspond-
fully described. The immediate en- ing to recorder. Syllabic [9] was re-
vironments of preceding and follow- placed by [6] before a stop: e.g.,
ing sounds in many utterances may be
noted to provide information as to 3The concept of zero [~] is important to
the characteristic sequences of sounds. child language study, as the regular ab-
Other features such as the positional sence of a sound in its patterning in a sound
system may bc as much of a signal as the
occurrence of sounds in utterances, regular presence of a sound or other au-
intonational features and reduplica- dible feature.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of Kansas - Libraries, Watson User on 09/25/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
260 JOURNAL OF: SPEECH AND HEARING RESEARCH

[ 2di3brSp1] corresponding to I burp. When one considers how resistant


The sounds [o] and [tr] replaced most languages are to change, it is
[w] in clusters of a consonant plus difficult to understand how children
[w]. Otherwise [w] had no alternants manage to keep changing their lan-
and thus showed less alternation than guage systems as rapidly as they do.
[1] and [r]. Also [j] had no al- In this struggle between maintaining
ternants. This child's sound-system is what Jespersen called a 'little lan-
more completely discussed elsewhere guage' for communication with others
by Albright and Albright (1). and, at the same time, developing it
In a study of a one-year-old child through successive stages of increas-
with a less developed language system, ing complexity, lies what is probably
the phonetic transcription indicated the central problem of speech de-
a total of 41 sounds, 27 vowels and velopment in children. Careful de-
14 consonants. After making a ten- scriptive studies should help to clarify
tative analysis of the phonemes, or this development.
'infantemes,' there was a remainder
of seven consonants, five vowels, and
two diphthongs, a reduction from 41 Summary
to 14 sounds as units in a sound-sys-
tem. The following chart presents the Techniques of descriptive linguis-
general structure of the younger tics may be applied to the analysis
child's consonant-system: of samples of child language if such
techniques are adapted to particular
Labial Apical Velar Glottal problems. In the paired comparison of
Stop b d g ? speech forms that are the 'same' or
Fricative h 'different,' one must usually substitute
Nasal m n the 'equivalence of recurrence' for
the linguist's 'equivalence of repeti-
This sound system may be de- tion.'
scribed as one based mainly upon
stop-continuant and oral-nasal con- Tape-recordings of several hours of
trasts. The voiced-voiceless contrast a child's informal discourse probably
of conventional English did not seem furnish the most adequate samples for
to be established. There was a good attempting linguistic analysis. In tran-
deal of variation of [p] with [b] and scribing and developing a 'tentative'
or 'suggested' phonemic analysis, it
of [t] with [d] in the child's speech,
is best to have at least two trained in-
however, indicating that the voiced-
voiceless contrast was probably de- vestigators check one another's work.
veloping (3). Since there is considerable variation
The vowel-system was reduced to in the language systems of ehildren,
~, e, a, u, plus the central vowel ~, and it is important to avoid mistaking
the two compound vowels ci and variant forms for essential units. In
az. The general structure of the spite of the variation, however, and
vowel-system may be charted as fol- the fact that the child's language sys-
lows: tem is developing, it still may be
T l.l treated at a particular time as a self-
contained system-a language in its
i~ lit own right.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of Kansas - Libraries, Watson User on 09/25/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
ALBRIGHT AND ALBRIGHT: CHILD LANGUAGE 261

References of Theory and Research Problems, ed.


by C. E. Osgood and T. A. Seboek.
1. ALBRIGHT,R. W., and ALBRIGHT, J. B., Bloomington, Ind.: Ind. Univ. Pubs. in
The phonology of a two-year-old child. Anthrop. and Ling., 1954.
Word, 12, 1956, 382-390. 6. INTERNATIONAL PHONETIC ASSOCIATION.
2. BLOCH, B., and TRAGER, G. L., Outline Tbe Principles of IPA. London: IPA,
of Linguistic Analysis. Baltimore: Ling. 1949.
Soc. of Amer., 1942.
3. CHAO, Y. R., The non-uniqueness of 7. SWAD~H, M., A method for ]~honetic
phonemic solutions of phonetic sys- accuracy and speed. Amer. z~ntbrop.,
tems. In Readings in Linguistics, ed. 39, 1937, 728-732.
by M. Joos. Wash.: Amer. Counc. of 8. TRACER, G. L., The field of linguistics.
Learned Soc., 1957. Studies in Ling., 1, 1949, 1-8.
4. COHEN, M., Sur l'6tude du language en- 9. WELLS, R., Immediate constituents. In
fantin. Enfance, 5, 1952, 181-249. Readings in Linguistics, ed. by M. Joos.
5. GREENBER~, J. H., The linguistic ap- Wash.: Amer. Counc. of Learned Soc.,
proach. In Psycbolinguistics: a Survey 1957.

Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University of Kansas - Libraries, Watson User on 09/25/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like