Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Review

Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis


Nancy D Chiaravalloti, John DeLuca

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a progressive disease of the CNS that is characterised by widespread lesions in the brain Lancet Neurol 2008; 7: 1139–51
and spinal cord. MS results in motor, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric symptoms, all of which can occur independently Neuropsychology and
of one another. The common cognitive symptoms include deficits in complex attention, efficiency of information Neuroscience Laboratory,
Kessler Medical Rehabilitation
processing, executive functioning, processing speed, and long-term memory. These deficits detrimentally affect many
Research and Education Center,
aspects of daily life, such as the ability to run a household, participate fully in society, and maintain employment— West Orange, NJ, USA
factors that can all affect the overall quality of life of the patient. The increased use of neuroimaging techniques in (N D Chiaravalloti PhD,
patients with MS has advanced our understanding of structural and functional changes in the brain that are J DeLuca PhD); Departments of
Physical Medicine and
characteristic of this disease, although much remains to be learned. Moreover, examination of efforts to treat the
Rehabilitation
cognitive deficits in MS is still in the early stages. (N D Chiaravalloti, J DeLuca) and
Neurology and Neurosciences
Introduction the course of the disease, with a ratio of 1∙3:1 (women to (J DeLuca), University of
Medicine and Dentistry of
Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a progressive disease of the men) in primary-progressive MS.11 New Jersey, New Jersey Medical
CNS and is characterised by the production of widespread In this paper, we review the current understanding of School, NJ, USA
lesions, or plaques, in the brain and spinal cord. These cognitive impairment in MS and describe the common Correspondence to:
lesions and plaques affect the myelin sheath, thus cognitive profile, the factors that contribute to cognitive John DeLuca, Kessler Medical
causing inhibition of axonal transmission. Inflammatory dysfunction, and the effect of such impairments on daily Rehabilitation Research and
Education Center, 1199 Pleasant
demyelination has traditionally been seen as the main life. In addition, we review cognitive and pharmacological
Valley Way, West Orange,
disease process in MS; however, axonal damage or loss is treatments for cognitive deficits, followed by contributions NJ 07052, USA
increasingly being documented to occur early in the from structural and functional imaging techniques to the jdeluca@kmrrec.org
disease and to result in permanent disability.1–3 Whether understanding of cognitive impairments in MS.
grey matter pathology is independent of that seen in
white matter, is a result of axonal injury in the white Cognitive deficits in MS
matter, or is simply similar to the pathology seen in the Since the 1980s, research has indicated that cognitive
white matter is debated.4 Because of the widespread impairment is a common concomitant of MS, with
development of the plaques, MS results in a broad range prevalence rates ranging from 43% to 70%12–14 at both the
of symptoms, which include motor, cognitive, and earlier and later stages of the disease.15,16 MS detrimentally
neuropsychiatric problems,5 and no two individuals with affects various aspects of cognitive functioning, including
MS have exactly the same symptom profile or disease attention,17,18 information processing efficiency,17,19,20
course.6 In addition, cognitive deficits can occur executive functioning,21–23 processing speed,24 and long-
independently of physical disability, which complicates term memory.5,25,26 Processing speed, and visual learning
their identification and recognition.7 This wide variability and memory seem to be most commonly affected in MS
in symptoms and disease course hampers understanding (in 51∙9% and 54∙3%, respectively; figure 1).13,14 Areas of
of the disease process and identification of effective cognition that are not usually affected are “simple”
treatments. Although the precise cause of MS is not yet attention (eg, repeating digits) and essential verbal skills
known, it is currently thought to be the result of (eg, word naming and comprehension).13 Although most
immunological, genetic, and viral factors.8 studies indicate that general intelligence remains intact
Four clinical courses of MS have been identified on the in patients with MS,27 other investigations have detected
basis of the rate of progression of the disease.9 Relapsing- slight but significant decrements.13 Overt dementia is
remitting MS is characterised by periods in which rare in MS,28 and the more common clinical presentation
symptoms are exacerbated and full recovery is noted is one of specific and subtle cognitive deficits that can
between attacks. About 80% of individuals with relapsing- vary substantially among patients.28
remitting MS later develop secondary-progressive MS;9
in this type of MS, the symptoms gradually worsen with Long-term memory
or without occasional relapses or minor remissions. Long-term memory refers to the ability to learn new
Progressive-relapsing MS is characterised by a progressive information and to recall that information at a later time
decline after onset of the disease, with some acute periods point.29 Long-term memory is one of the most consistently
of symptom relapse. There might or might not be impaired cognitive functions in MS and is seen in
recovery from these acute periods. Finally, primary- 40–65% of patients.25 Early work on memory impairment
progressive MS has a continuous and gradual worsening in MS suggested that difficulty in retrieval from long-
of the symptoms with no distinct exacerbation or term storage was the primary cause for the deficit in
remission of symptoms. Although epidemiological long-term memory.30–32 More recently, however, research
studies indicate that women are about twice as likely to has shown that the primary memory problem is in the
have MS than are men,10 this association is affected by initial learning of information.26,33,34 Patients with MS

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1139


Review

60 10

50
8
Percentage of patients impaired

40

Trials to criterion
6
30

20 4

10
2

0
cy

e)

e)

ed

n
ed

ed

ee
io

io 0
iat

iat
en

pe
pt

lay

lay

at
sp
ed

ed
Flu

gs

m
ce

y/
de

de

MS Healthy controls
m

or
er

sin
or
y(

y(
im

im

tf
lp

em

es
or

or

ep
y(

y(
tia

oc
em

em

gm

nc
or

or
pa

Pr
m

m
em

em

Co
os

kin

Figure 2: Comparison between the number of trials needed for patients with
al

al
su

or
rb

su
Vi

al

al

W
Ve

Vi

MS and healthy controls to reach criterion* on the open-trial selective


rb

su
Ve

Vi

reminding test
Cognitive domain
Patients with MS needed substantially more trials to reach criterion relative to the
healthy controls, which is indicative of deficits in verbal acquisition in MS. Adapted
Figure 1: Frequency of impairment in 291 patients with MS by cognitive domain from DeLuca J et al,26 with permission from Taylor and Francis. *Criterion was
Data from Benedict RH et al.14 correct recall of ten words in two consecutive trials.

require more repetitions of information to reach a higher-order cognition (eg, resistance to interference or
predetermined learning criterion, but once that executive functioning) when there is a speed of processing
information has been acquired, recall and recognition component to task performance.27
are at the same level as for healthy controls (figure 2).26,34 Deficits in working memory and speed of processing
This deficit in learning new information results in poor affect each other in patients with MS: as the demands on
decision-making abilities35 and seems to affect prospective working memory increase, both deficits in speed of
memory abilities.36 Several factors have been associated processing and working memory become more
with poor learning abilities in people with MS, including prominent.44,45 Impairments in working memory have
slow processing speed, susceptibility to interference (ie, been detected in the earliest stage of MS46 and across
difficulty disregarding irrelevant stimuli), executive disease courses.47 Results from recent large sample
dysfunction, and perceptual deficits. studies have shown that people with MS present with a
significantly higher occurrence of deficits in speed of
Efficiency of information processing processing than in working memory, particularly in
Information processing efficiency refers to the ability to patients who have a secondary-progressive course
maintain and manipulate information in the brain for a (figure 3).24 The paced auditory serial addition test
short time period (working memory)37 and to the speed (PASAT), a test of working memory with substantial
with which one can process that information (processing demands on processing speed, is a sensitive measure of
speed). A reduced speed of processing is the most cognitive dysfunction in MS and has a sensitivity and
common cognitive deficit in MS.24,38,39 Tests of processing specificity of 74% and 65%, respectively.48 The PASAT is
speed can be used to predict long-term cognitive decline, the cognitive component of the widely used multiple
with a pronounced decline in processing speed occurring sclerosis functional composite,49 which is a multi-
over several years of follow-up.39 Such deficits in dimensional quantitative outcome measure designed
processing speed are typically seen concurrently with specifically for use in patients with MS.
other cognitive deficits that are common in MS,24 such as Performance on tasks of attention is associated with
deficits in working memory and long-term memory;38,40,41 speed of processing and working memory. Typically,
in some studies the extent of memory impairment has basic attention tasks (eg, repeating digits) are unaffected
been positively correlated with deficits in processing in patients with MS,14 although a deficit has been noted
speed.24,40,42 One study has shown that processing speed in at least one study.50 Impairment in sustained attention
can predict performance on the everyday tasks component is more common51 and specific decrements in divided
of the executive function and performance test (discussed attention (ie, tasks in which patients are asked to attend
below).43 Therefore, clinicians who assess patients with to several tasks simultaneously, such as identifying
MS should be cautious when interpreting deficits in specific targets on a computer screen, while monitoring

1140 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

a second task) have been reported in patients with MS.51


80
Variability in the exact cognitive processes labelled Impairment in processing speed
Impairment in working memory
“attention” makes it difficult to draw conclusions about
the effect of MS on attention processes: for example, 70
some investigators label a task an “attention” task when it
might more accurately involve processing speed52,53 or 60
executive control.53 In addition, the involvement of fatigue

Percentage of patients impaired


is not often accounted for and this factor probably exerts
50
a considerable effect on the performance of tasks that
demand longer attention spans and that are typically
used to identify deficits in sustained or divided attention. 40
Finally, differences among studies in the description of
various disease courses could affect conclusions about 30
attention because the more complex forms of attention
(ie, sustained and divided) are more likely to be affected
by the progressive forms of MS. 20

Executive functions 10
Executive functioning refers to the cognitive abilities
needed for complex goal-directed behaviour and
0
adaptation to environmental changes or demands. This Secondary-progressive MS Relapsing-remitting MS
includes the ability to plan, anticipate outcomes, and
direct resources appropriately.54 Deficits in executive Figure 3: Percentage of patients with secondary-progressive MS or relapsing-
functions (ie, abstract and conceptual reasoning, fluency, remitting MS who have impairments in speed of processing and working
memory
planning, and organisation) occur in patients with MS, In this sample of 215 patients with MS, considerably more patients had
albeit less frequently than deficits in memory and impairments in speed of processing than in working memory. Adapted from
information processing efficiency.5,55,56 Drew and DeLuca J et al,24 with permission from Taylor and Francis.
co-workers57 noted that 17% of patients with MS had
difficulties across a range of executive abilities (ie, primary visual or other cognitive abnormalities can also
shifting, inhibition, and fluency). Fluency tests “evaluate occur.66 Primary visual problems can contribute to
the spontaneous production of words under restricted difficulties on higher-order cognitive tasks that have
search conditions”,58 such as words that begin with a visual demands.67
specific letter and words from a particular category. Henry
and Beatty59 noted that patients with MS had substantial Assessment of cognitive functioning
deficits in both phonemic and semantic fluency, and Given the high occurrence of cognitive dysfunction in
concluded that levels of fluency were sensitive measures patients with MS, adequate assessment and diagnosis of
of neurological impairment in MS. Similarly, perseverative these deficits is essential. Clinicians and researchers
errors (ie, errors that are repeated despite evidence that have used various tests of cognitive functioning to
they are incorrect) have been repeatedly seen in patients identify the deficits commonly seen in these patients. As
with MS.60–62 Measures of executive functioning are also the cognitive deficits in MS frequently fall within specific
particularly susceptible to the effects of depression in cognitive domains and can be subtle and vary considerably
patients with MS,61,63–65 and this should always be among patients,28 a carefully selected neuropsychological
considered in the interpretation of poor performance on test battery is essential. Various batteries have been
executive tasks in individuals with MS. recommended for use, although many have been
criticised for their emphasis on brevity rather than
Visual perceptual functions comprehensiveness and thus often fail to identify
Visual perceptual functions not only include the cognitive deficits accurately.68,69 However, other tests have
recognition of a visual stimulus, but also the ability to been deemed too comprehensive because areas that are
perceive the characteristics of that stimulus accurately. not typically affected by MS are assessed and these tests
Compared with studies on long-term memory and are thus inefficient in terms of cost and time.70
information processing efficiency, there has been little Consequently, an International Conference of MS Experts
work on visual perceptual processing. Up to about a resulted in the development of the minimal assessment
quarter of people with MS might have deficits in visual of cognitive function in MS test battery, which is now
perceptual functions.66 Difficulties in primary visual recommended for use with patients with MS. This battery
processing (eg, from optic neuritis) in MS can have a is composed of seven tests that assess word fluency,
detrimental effect on visual perceptual processing, visuospatial ability, verbal memory, visuospatial memory,
although perceptual deficits that are independent of processing speed, working memory, and executive

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1141


Review

function, and has been shown to be sensitive to the between depression and cognitive functioning.
cognitive profiles characteristic of patients with MS.14 In Depression affects many aspects of cognitive functioning
an effort to increase the availability of screening in MS, including working memory,87,88 processing speed,89
assessments, researchers have also started to investigate learning and memory functions,90 abstract reasoning,90
the validity of internet-based cognitive testing.71 and executive functioning.23,64 The amelioration of
depression could also lead to neuropsychological
Other factors that affect cognitive performance improvements in patients with MS.91
Disease-related factors Up to 60% of people with MS have depression,92
Cognitive impairment can be seen irrespective of the although efforts to explain its source, from factors such
duration of disease and is only mildly associated with as effects of drugs, physical disability, cognitive deficits,
physical disability.72,73 However, the course of the disease fatigue, and disease duration, have not been successful.92
does play a part in the pattern of cognitive dysfunction in However, depression in patients with MS is less common
MS. Progressive MS generally results in more severe in individuals with lesions that are predominantly in the
cognitive impairment than does relapse-remitting MS;74,75 spinal cord than in patients with brain lesions.93 Recent
however, in these data the course of the disease was studies have shown a relation between depression and
confounded with duration of disease. For example, about demyelination94–97 with demyelination in some brain
50% of individuals with untreated relapsing-remitting regions appearing to contribute to depression more than
MS will develop secondary-progressive MS within other areas. For example, Sabatini and co-workers98 have
10–15 years of onset.76 Thus, by definition, disease duration suggested that a disconnection between cortical and
and the extent of neurological disability (measured using subcortical areas that are important for limbic system
the expanded disability status scale) are greater in patients function might contribute to depression in MS. Similarly,
with a secondary-progressive course compared with Bakshi and co-workers94 have concluded that a
relapsing-remitting MS. In the few studies that have cortical–subcortical disconnection caused by lesions in
distinguished between patients with primary-progressive frontal and parietal white matter (independent of
MS and patients with secondary-progressive MS, neurological disability), as well as atrophy, might
significantly greater cognitive dysfunction was seen in contribute to depression in individuals with MS. Other
the secondary-progressive MS group.23,39,40,77,78 studies have shown a greater number of lesions in the
Few studies have examined the characteristics of temporal region in depressed versus non-depressed
cognitive decline in MS over time. Such studies have individuals with MS.95–97 In addition to differences in
methodological difficulties, such as a solely cross-sectional patterns of lesion location, immunological markers (such
design or short follow-up periods. In one study,79 cognition as increased T4+ helper/inducer cell counts)99 have also
was assessed in individuals with MS who were examined been associated with depression in MS.82
initially at enrollment and then after 4 and 10 years. As the
disease progressed, the percentage of patients with Fatigue
cognitive impairment tended to increase, although, even Fatigue is one of the most common symptoms in MS
after 10 years, most patients did not have cognitive and has been reported in over 90% of patients.100 Fatigue
impairment. Similar results were found in another 10-year has two components: physical and cognitive101 and,
follow-up study,80 in which no significant change in although difficulties in the accurate assessment of
performance on tests of memory and semantic retrieval cognitive fatigue is an obstacle to forming a complete
were seen over the study period. However, on tests of understanding of the construct, researchers still seek to
working memory and processing speed, 27–44% of better understand the relation between cognitive fatigue
patients showed a decline over time. and cognition in MS.101 No association has been found
between subjective reports of fatigue and cognitive
Depression dysfunction.101–103 However, efforts to measure fatigue
Research has shown a link between emotional symptoms objectively have been more successful; decrements in
and cognitive functions in many people with neurological performance over time in tasks that require sustained
disorders. Depression is common in MS and is thought mental effort have suggested that fatigue might affect
to interfere considerably with cognitive and non-cognitive performance.101 This pattern has also been noted in
activities.81 The relation between depression and cognitive cognitive tasks of working memory103 and visual
functioning is not clear:82 although results from several vigilance.101
studies have shown depression to be associated with
neuropsychological functioning in patients with MS,83,84 Impairment in oral motor functions
other investigations have not shown such a relation.26,85,86 Cognitive tasks that require a rapid spoken response
In a critical review of the literature, Arnett and might be detrimentally affected by impairments in
co-workers82 concluded that when studies are adequately rudimentary oral motor functions in people with MS.
powered and include a representative sample of people Decreased performance on tasks that require oral
with MS, a positive link has been consistently noted responses are partially caused by dysarthria in patients

1142 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

with MS.104 The assessment of rudimentary oral motor disability,116 severity and progression of disease,117–119
problems was recommended as part of the minimal disease duration,117 decreased cerebral integrity on MRI,120
assessment of cognitive function in MS and was further and decreased ability to do everyday activities.121 There is
investigated empirically by Arnett and co-workers.105 In also a lower QoL during periods of relapse compared
an effort to minimise the effect of the characteristic slow with periods of remission.116,122 Cognitive deficits are
motor response of patients with MS on performance of detrimental to the QoL and productivity of patients with
cognitive tests, researchers and clinicians have frequently MS108,113 and neuropsychological deficits have a large effect
used tasks that do not require motor function for writing on QoL in particular.113
or for manual manipulation. As a result, the primary The onset of MS typically occurs between the ages of
response modality in the study of a person with MS tends 20 and 40 years, when individuals are most active and
to be speech, which is susceptible to oral motor productive in many aspects of their lives,123 and frequently
impairments. Thus, caution is needed in the interpretation leads to the loss of gainful employment for many
of tasks that could be affected by rudimentary speech patients.106 Specifically, 40–80% of individuals with MS
problems to avoid over-diagnosis of problems in higher- are unemployed124–126 and cognitive impairment is a large
order cognitive processing. contributor to this high rate of unemployment.106,108 Beatty
and co-workers106 have reported five variables that account
The effect of cognitive impairment on daily for 49% of the variance in employment status in MS,
living three of which are measures of cognitive functioning.
Cognitive dysfunction is closely associated with functional Several investigations have noted that 50–80% of patients
status in MS.106,107 Rao and co-workers108 found that with MS are unemployed within 10 years of disease
individuals with MS who were cognitively impaired onset,124–128 and this number is frequently attributed to
participated in fewer social and vocational activities, were deficits in cognition:106,108 as physical disability and
less likely to be employed, had greater difficulties in doing demographic factors account for less than 14% of the
routine household tasks, and were more vulnerable to variance in employment status in patients with MS,128
psychiatric illness than individuals with a purely physical cognitive problems are thought to account for much of
disability. By use of an objective, structured, and the variance.129 Deficits in various domains of cognitive
standardised assessment battery designed by occupational impairment have been blamed for this difficulty in
therapists, the executive functions performance test,109 maintaining employment, particularly information
Kalmar and co-workers43 found that cognitive performance processing efficiency, memory performance,106 and
was correlated with objective assessment of activities executive dysfunction.45 Benedict and co-workers14 have
during everyday life, whereas subjective assessment of found that poor cognitive performance—particularly on
functional activity was correlated with emotional distress.43 measures of processing speed, verbal memory, and
Although performance on the executive functions executive functioning—predicted vocational status, even
performance test does not depend on speed (ie, patients after the effects of age, education, sex, depression, and
can take their time), a significant association between disease course were taken into account. Similarly, Rao
performance on this test and speed of processing was and co-workers108 noted that cognitive impairment was
seen. Performance on tests of processing speed is also important in determining the work status of individuals
associated with measures of everyday life that require with MS, even when groups were matched on several
speeded responses.110 Nagy and co-workers35 have reported measures of disease severity (the expanded disability
that patients with MS have a reduced ability to make status scale scores, disease course, and disease duration).
decisions that could affect functioning during everyday Thus, MS results in considerable disruptions to the lives,
life, which was mainly caused by deficits in new learning. lifestyles, employment status, and yearly earnings of
Similarly, Kessler and co-workers107 found that considerable affected individuals,128–130 which has a detrimental effect
variance in activities of daily living in MS could be on personal, occupational, and social functioning, thereby
accounted for by the results of ten memory tests, affecting overall QoL. Although physical disability is
independent of demographic or physical disability important for the performance of everyday activities, it
variables; functional impairment was primarily associated cannot account for the extent of difficulties that
with difficulties in new learning.107 Functional impairments individuals with MS encounter for many everyday
that are commonly seen in MS include difficulty in activities, particularly in those activities that require a
shopping independently, completing housework, washing substantial cognitive load.
clothes, ironing, completing home repairs, cooking,
driving, and using public transportation.111 Treatment of cognitive deficits
Considerable evidence indicates that quality of life Cognitive rehabilitation
(QoL) is decreased in individuals diagnosed with MS.112 There have been few studies on the treatment of cognitive
Furthermore, the extent to which an individual has a deficits so far,131 and several authors have highlighted the
poor QoL correlates with deficits in cognitive need for additional effective neuropsychological
functioning,113 depressive symptomatology,114,115 increased rehabilitation techniques in MS.132,133 The few cognitive

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1143


Review

Total scores on the Hopkins verbal learning test—revised 4


patients with MS. Techniques such as self-generated
learning146,147 and spaced learning148 can improve learning
3
and memory considerably in MS, whereas no benefit has
2 been seen with simple repetition of information.149 Non-
1 specific cognitive protocols have also been examined in
0 patients with MS and cognitive benefits have been noted
–1 in some studies137,150 but not in others.151
Baseline to immediate Baseline to long-term The conclusions that can be made from available
–2 follow-up follow-up
studies are limited by the methodological problems, such
–3
as baseline differences among groups,137 the use of a
–4 qualitative rather than quantitative research design,135
–5 and reliance on case studies.136 There is an important
Experimental group
–6 Control group clinical need for methodologically rigorous research into
cognitive rehabilitation in patients with MS: the ideal
Figure 4: Hopkins verbal learning test—revised change scores for the study would be an adequately powered placebo-controlled,
moderately impaired group randomised clinical trial.
Participants in the experimental group showed an increase in verbal learning
scores from baseline to immediate follow-up and from baseline to long-term
follow-up. Participants in the control group showed no meaningful change Pharmacological interventions
during the same assessment period. Adapted from Chiaravalloti ND et al,140 Two pharmacological approaches have been examined in
with permission from SAGE Publications. an effort to improve cognitive deficits in patients with
MS: disease-modifying therapies and pharmacological
rehabilitation programmes available for MS aim to treatments specifically targeted at improving cognition.
improve attentional deficits,134 communication skills,135 Disease-modifying therapies are drugs that alter the
and memory impairments.136–138 Although some studies disease course of MS. Although many studies have
have detected a benefit of cognitive rehabilitation for examined the efficacy of various disease-modifying
individuals with MS,137–140 other investigations have not therapies in MS, not all trials have included cognition as
shown any improvement.141,142 In a comprehensive an outcome variable and, when they do, cognition tends
evidence-based review of the available data on the use of to be a secondary outcome; it is therefore difficult to deter-
cognitive rehabilitation for patients with MS, O’Brien mine the effect of the drug on cognitive function from
and co-workers concluded that, although this research is these studies. In such studies, the cognitive status of
still in its infancy, there have been some well designed included participants is commonly comparable to that of
studies that can provide a foundation from which to healthy individuals.152 In one study, a significant beneficial
advance the field.143 effect of interferon beta-1a was shown for processing
A few studies have examined the efficacy of attention speed, and learning and memory.153 Interferon beta-1b
remediation protocols for the treatment of patients with has shown some mild benefit for cognitive function,154,155
MS. Although results from some studies have not shown whereas research on glatiramer acetate has shown no
benefits of the specific training programme studied,141,142 effect on cognition.152 However, glatiramer acetate has
other investigations have shown some objective been found to improve fatigue in patients with MS,
benefit.134,144 We know of only two studies on the cognitive which resulted in fewer absences from work.156,157
rehabilitation of executive functions in patients with MS: Researchers have also examined the benefit of
one that detected a significant treatment effect145 and pharmacological treatments specifically targeted at
another that found no treatment effect.141 Learning and improving cognition. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors
memory have received the most attention with regard to were originally developed to treat cognitive decline in
cognitive rehabilitation in MS. Building on earlier Alzheimer’s disease. The study of acetylcholinesterase
work,136,138 Chiaravalloti and co-workers140 did a double- inhibitors in individuals with MS dates back to 1988,
blind randomised placebo-controlled trial of the modified when Leo and co-workers158 noted an improvement in
“story memory technique” to improve learning and verbal memory with physostigmine in a double-blind,
memory in 28 individuals with mixed subtypes of MS placebo-controlled study; a similar effect was seen by
(figure 4). A significant effect of the treatment on list Unverzgagt and co-workers.159 However, both studies had
learning performance was seen, particularly among those methodological limitations: the former had a small
with a moderate-to-severe memory impairment. Although sample and the latter did not have a control group. In
less rigid in design, some studies have also shown some addition, the short half-life and considerable adverse
benefit of memory rehabilitation programmes in patients effects of physostigmine have tempered enthusiasm for
with MS;142 however, others have failed to document any the drug. Donepezil has received the most research
benefit.136,138 Although not part of structured treatment attention in efforts to improve cognition in MS and
protocols, researchers have also started to examine the improvements in memory dysfunction in MS have been
effect of specific memory-enhancing techniques in seen with this drug.160 However, methodological problems,

1144 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

such as no placebo-control group, have limited the associated with deficits in visual memory and working
conclusions that could be drawn from earlier studies.161,162 memory.179 Neocortical volume correlates with cognitive
A recent double-blind, placebo-controlled randomised performance across a wide range of neuropsychological
clinical trial with 69 individuals with MS showed that measures, and also distinguishes patients with MS with
donepezil improved objective memory performance on cognitive impairment from those without cognitive
list learning, as well as subjective ratings and clinician impairment.178 Thus, measures of central atrophy, such as
ratings of memory abilities.163,164 lesion load, seem to be more useful than conventional
Stimulant drugs have also been investigated in measures in determining the extent of cerebral change in
patients with MS. Amantadine hydrochloride is MS.178 Longitudinal imaging studies have shown a strong
commonly used to treat fatigue in MS and has shown correlation between changes in cognitive functioning and
modest benefit.165 However, studies of the efficacy of an increase in brain atrophy;180,181 moreover, progression of
amantadine for treating the cognitive deficits in MS brain atrophy early in the disease can predict cognitive
have not shown an effect.166 In addition, in a randomised impairment 5 years later.170
clinical trial of 38 patients with MS no effect of ginkgo New technologies can help to maximise our ability to
biloba on cognition was seen overall.167 However, a trend assess cerebral integrity in MS. Although some studies
was noted for improved performance on the Stroop test, that use diffusion-tensor MRI have found a correlation
which measures resistance from interference and between functional anisotropy and cognitive perfor-
processing speed. mance,182 other investigations have found no such
Although cognitive impairment is a main concomitant association.183 Measures derived from magnetisation
of MS, an effective treatment has not yet been identified; transfer ratio have also consistently been shown to be
the strongest evidence suggests that acetylcholinesterase associated with cognition, as documented with many
inhibitors and donezepil in particular are the most types of brain tissue, including cortical and subcortical
promising drugs. However, many of these studies have regions,184 normal-appearing brain tissue on conventional
had small samples and few studies have applied optimum imaging,185 and normal-appearing white matter.186
research strategies.160,163 Future research is therefore Magnetic resonance spectroscopy, which provides a
necessary to enable a recommendation for pharmacological measure of metabolic changes in the cerebral cortex and
therapy for cognitive deficits in patients with MS. white matter, is also a sensitive indicator of cognitive
functioning in MS, particularly in normal-appearing
Neuroimaging and cognition white matter.187,188 Multi-slice echo-planar spectroscopic
Structural imaging imaging provides global metabolic measures that can be
Structural neuroimaging has become a key element of used to distinguish between patients with MS with and
diagnosis and care in MS.168 Various techniques designed without cognitive impairment.189
to capture brain integrity—MRI being the most widely
used—have shown correlations with cognitive functioning. Functional imaging
Several measures can be obtained by use of MRI, Compared with structural imaging, the application of
including whole brain atrophy,169,170 cortical atrophy,171 and functional imaging to study brain function is a relatively
lesion volume.172 Studies have shown that patients with new area of research in MS. Initial studies using PET,
greater lesion burden have significantly more cognitive single photon emission computed tomography
dysfunction than those with less lesion burden.62,95,172,173 (SPECT),190–195 and perfusion MRI196 have shown a
Measures of brain atrophy are particularly sensitive in reduction in cerebral oxygen use and blood flow in
elucidating the relation between brain integrity and individuals with MS, and these changes correlate with
cognitive status.95,174–176 The width of the third ventricle has cognitive impairment. More recently, functional MRI
a strong association with cognitive status176 and subcortical (fMRI), which has many advantages over PET and SPECT,
atrophy is more highly correlated with cognition than has begun to be used in MS research. The increased use
whole brain atrophy or lesion load.174,177 The width of the of functional imaging techniques to study clinical
third ventricle correlates not only with cognitive problems in MS over the past 10 years has provided a
performance in MS,176,178 but also provides predictive wealth of new knowledge about brain–behaviour relations
validity for cognition.178 Atrophy in the thalamus is a in MS. However, this increasing body of new knowledge
strong predictor of cognitive dysfunction,178 although this must be met with the appropriate scientific scrutiny and
is not necessarily better than other MRI measures, such scepticism with regard to interpretation and application.
as width of third ventricle or whole brain atrophy. Grey Functional imaging studies that have investigated
matter and white matter atrophy have been associated cognitive processes in MS have focused on three main
with performance in particular cognitive skills, which areas: working memory, attention, and executive
indicates that atrophy in these distinct tissue types could functions. fMRI studies of working memory in MS have
result in specific functional deficits. Similarly, left frontal mainly used three verbal working memory cognitive
atrophy has been associated with performance on tests of tasks: the PASAT46 (or a modification of the PASAT
verbal memory, whereas right frontal atrophy has been referred to as the mPASAT197), the N-back test,198 and the

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1145


Review

Sternberg protocol.199 Studies that have used the PASAT greater activation in the left inferior parietal lobe than did
and mPASAT have shown that individuals with MS the control group. By use of PET, Santa Maria and
have bilateral frontal cortical activation,46,197,200,201 by co-workers206 showed that, compared with the control
contrast with the more focal left frontal activation seen group, patients with MS had significantly less glucose use
in healthy controls. However, patients with MS also in the right anterior cingulate gyrus and increased
show considerably greater activation throughout other activation in the left superior temporal gyrus on an
brain regions such as bilateral parietal lobes,197,201 auditory attention task. Despite methodological
bilateral temporal lobes,201 and the right cerebellum200 differences, both studies provide evidence for substantial
when compared with healthy controls. In fMRI studies differences in activation levels between patients with MS
that have compared individuals with MS with and and controls in brain regions that support these cognitive
without impaired working memory,46,197,201 all have shown processes.
differences in the pattern of cerebral activation between Although people with MS have difficulties on tasks of
the two groups, although the specific patterns of cerebral planning and problem solving (an area of executive
activation differed among studies. Several method- function),21,207,208 we know of only one study that used
ological differences, such as response modality (verbal fMRI to investigate planning abilities in participants with
vs motor), the use of a control task, and the speed of MS.209 In this study, similar levels of activation in the
stimulus presentation, might account for the variation bilateral frontal and parietal lobes were found in patients
in results. and controls in response to a modified version of the
Data from fMRI studies of the 2-back working memory Tower of London test.
task consistently suggest that, when performance is Overall, two main patterns of functional cerebral
similar among groups, the same cortical regions (frontal activity during cognitive functioning in MS can be
and parietal areas) are activated in participants with MS identified. First, the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent
and healthy controls.198,202,203 However, although some responses are frequently seen in patients with MS in
results have shown that the magnitude of the activation regions where no activation was found in healthy
is greater in the patients with MS compared with the controls. Second, the regions of cortical activation are
healthy controls,198,203 other investigations have shown largely similar between patients with MS and healthy
lower activation in frontal regions in those with MS.202 controls, but the level of activation can be increased in
By use of the Sternberg protocol, Hillary and co-workers199 patients with MS. Several explanations for these findings
found significantly greater activation in the right frontal have been suggested,210 such as cerebral reorganisation201,211
and right temporal lobes in individuals with MS than in and recruitment of other cortical regions as a
healthy controls, with no difference in left hemisphere compensatory mechanism to account for the effects of
activation. Participants with poorer performance motor and cognitive impairments related to MS.203,211 Task
showed increased activation in the right frontal and difficulty has also been suggested as an explanatory factor
right temporal lobes. A similar inverse relation was for these findings: the patterns of activation seen in MS
shown by Chiaravalloti and co-workers197 between might be caused by recruitment of additional cerebral
performance on the PASAT and the level of activation resources when the task demands reach a certain
of the right frontal and parietal regions. However, Li threshold,212 perhaps on the basis of the individual’s
and co-workers204 found a significant decrease in the experience of task difficulty. Neuronal integrity (eg,
number of voxels activated in the right and left cerebellar presence of brain lesions) might also affect the patterns
hemispheres in participants with MS compared with of activation seen in functional neuroimaging studies,
the healthy control group by use of the Sternberg either directly or indirectly. In fact, Audoin and
protocol. This finding suggested that the cognitive co-workers46 noted that the pattern of cerebral activation
processes of the cerebellum are also disrupted in MS, in patients with MS performing the PASAT correlated
possibly caused by a disruption in the pathways from significantly with severity of brain disease.
the cortex to the cerebellum.
Two studies have applied functional neuroimaging Conclusions
techniques to examine attention in MS, both of which Although the cognitive sequelae of MS have only been
showed significant differences in patterns of cortical recognised as an important symptom over the past few
activation during attention tasks between patients with decades, research has shown that they exert a large effect
MS and healthy controls.205,206 By use of a visual attention on many everyday activities, including social and
task during fMRI, Penner and co-workers205 showed that emotional function, the ability to do household tasks,
patients with MS who had mild cognitive impairment had maintenance of gainful employment, and overall QoL.
significantly greater activation in the right dorsolateral With the recognition of the importance of cognitive
prefrontal cortex, right superior temporal gyrus, right functioning in MS comes a responsibility on the part of
lateral cerebellum, left angular gyrus, and bilateral inferior both clinicians and researchers to seek to increase
parietal cortex than did the control group. Patients with understanding of these cognitive deficits, their effect on
MS who were severely impaired also had significantly other abilities (such as everyday life activities, employment

1146 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

Contributors
Search strategy and selection criteria The authors contributed equally to this Review.

References for this Review were identified through searches Conflicts of interest
We have no conflicts of interest.
of PubMed and PsycINFO by use of the search term “multiple
sclerosis” in combination with other appropriate targets, such Acknowledgments
NDC and JD are supported by grants from the National Institutes of
as “cognition”, “cognitive dissonance”, “comprehension”,
Health (R01 HD045798, STTR R42 NS050007), the National Institute on
“consciousness”, “intuition”, “mental processing”, “learning”, Disability and Rehabilitation Research (H133A070037), and the National
“intention”, “higher nervous activity”, “mental fatigue”, Multiple Sclerosis Society (RG 3330, PP1331).
“MRI”, “perception”, “thinking and volition”, “cognitive References
impairment”, “cognitive deficits”, “cognitive dysfunction”, 1 Trapp BD, Peterson J, Ransohoff RM, Rudick R, Mork S, Bo L. Axonal
transection in the lesions of multiple sclerosis. N Engl J Med 1998;
and “executive dysfunction”, up to September, 2008. Further 338: 278–85.
articles were identified from the reference sections of these 2 Trapp BD, Ransohoff RM, Fisher E, Rudick R. Neurodegeneration in
publications. Only papers published in English were reviewed. multiple sclerosis: relationship to neurological disability.
Neuroscientist; 1999; 5: 48–57.
Abstracts or unpublished material were excluded.
3 Chelune GJ, Stott H, Pinkston J. Multiple sclerosis. In: Morgan JE,
Ricker JH. Textbook of clinical neuropsychology. New York, USA:
Taylor and Francis, 2008: 599–615.
status, or social participation), the factors that might 4 Benedict RHB, Shucard JL, Zivadinov R, Shucard DW.
affect their presentation, and techniques for improvement. Neuropsychological impairment in systemic lupus erythematosus: a
comparison with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychol Rev 2008; 18: 149–66.
Both fatigue and depression have been identified as 5 Brassington JC, Marsh NV. Neuropsychological aspects of multiple
important contributors to cognitive performance in sclerosis. Neuropsychol Rev 1998; 8: 43–77.
patients with MS; therefore, when assessing cognition in 6 Gordon PA, Lewis MD, Wong D. Multiple sclerosis: strategies for
rehabilitation counselors. J Rehabil 1994; 60: 34–38.
research and clinical settings, these factors need to be
7 Cobble N. The rehabilitative management of patients with multiple
taken into account. sclerosis. J Neurol Rehab 1992; 6: 141–45.
In a broad sense, intact cognitive function is a result of 8 Rumrill PD, Kaleta DA, Battersby JC. Etiology, incidence, and
a complicated network of specific cognitive abilities that prevalence. In: Rumrill PD, Ed. Employment issues and multiple
sclerosis. New York, USA: Demos Vermande, 1996: 1–18.
function together in an optimum way. Thus, whether 9 Herndon RM. Multiple sclerosis: immunology, pathology and
doing research or treating individual patients with MS, pathophysiology. New York, USA: Demos Medical Publishing, 2003.
one must recognise that deficits in basic cognitive 10 Kraft GI. Multiple sclerosis. In: Stolov WC, Cloers MR, eds. Handbook
processes, such as processing speed and attention, will of severe disability. Washington, USA: United States Department of
Education and Rehabilitation Services Adminstration, 1981: 111–18.
detrimentally affect performance in other areas, such as 11 Cottrell DA, Kremenchutzky M, Rice GP, et al. The natural history of
executive control and learning and memory, and affect multiple sclerosis: a geographically based study. 5. The clinical
activities in everyday life. Similarly, motor deficits in features and natural history of primary progressive multiple sclerosis.
Brain 1999; 122: 625–39.
tasks that require a motor response or oral motor 12 Peyser JM, Rao SM, LaRocca NG, Kaplan E. Guidelines for
problems in tasks that require an oral response must also neuropsychological research in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1990;
be taken into account. 47: 94–97.
13 Rao S, Leo G, Bernardin L, Unverzagt F. Cognitive dysfunction in
The investigation of efforts to improve cognitive status multiple sclerosis: frequency, patterns, and predictions. Neurology
in MS is still in its infancy and future work will be 1991; 41: 685–91.
important to improve the lives of individuals with MS. 14 Benedict RH, Cookfair D, Gavett R, et al. Validity of the minimal
assessment of cognitive function in multiple sclerosis.
Pharmacological and behavioural studies to enhance J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12: 549–58.
cognitive function in MS are scant and there are few 15 Pelosi L, Geesken JM, Holly M, Hayward M, Blumhardt LD. Working
studies that have used appropriate methodological rigor. memory impairment in early multiple-sclerosis. Evidence from an
Therefore, treatments with proven effectiveness have not event-related potential study of patients with clinically isolated
myelopathy. Brain 1997; 120: 2039–58.
yet been identified. Future research that applies optimum 16 Piras MR, Magnano I, Canu ED, et al. Longitudinal study of
research strategies is necessary to enable recommend- cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: neuropsychological,
ations for either pharmacological therapy or behavioural neuroradiological, and neurophysiological findings.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74: 878–85.
interventions for cognitive deficits in MS. 17 Litvan I, Grafman J, Vendrell P, Martinez JM. Slowed information
The application of novel measurement criteria in processing in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1988; 45: 281–85.
available technologies and new technologies to 18 Beatty WW, Wilbanks SL, Blanco CR, Hames KA, Tivis R,
Paul RH. Memory disturbance in multiple sclerosis:
understand further the association between brain reconsideration of patterns of performance on the selective
structure and function—as well as their effect on reminding test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1996; 18: 56–62.
cognition—is an exciting opportunity to advance our 19 Diamond BJ, DeLuca J, Kim H, Kelley SM. The question of
disproportionate impairments in visual and auditory information
knowledge of the neurological effects of MS. Both processing in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1997;
researchers and clinicians need to keep up to date with 19: 34–42.
the rapidly changing technology, to apply this technology 20 Grafman J, Rao S, Bernardin L, Leo GJ. Automatic memory processes
in clinically meaningful ways, and to develop optimum in patients with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1991; 48: 1072–75.
21 Foong J, Rozewicz L, Quaghebeur G, et al. Executive function in
ways to understand the structure and function of the multiple sclersois. The role of frontal lobe pathology. Brain 1997;
brain in MS. 120: 15–26.

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1147


Review

22 Lazeron RH, Rombouts SA, Scheltens P, Polman CH, Barkhof F. An 47 Grigsby J, Ayarbe SD, Kravcisin N, Busenbark D. Working memory
fMRI sudy of planning-related brain activity in patients with impairment among persons with chronic progressive multiple
moderately advanced multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2004; 10: 549–55. sclerosis. J Neurol 1994; 241: 125–31.
23 Denney DR, Sworowski LA, Lynch SG. Cognitive impairment in 48 Rosti E, Hamalainen P, Koivisto K, Hokkanen L. PASAT in detecting
three subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsy 2005; cognitive impairment in relapsing-remitting MS. Appl Neuropsychol
20: 967–81. 2007; 14: 101–12.
24 DeLuca J, Chelune GJ, Tulsky DS, Lengenfelder J, Chiaravalloti ND. 49 Fischer JS, Rudick RA, Cutter GR, Reingold SC. The multiple
Is speed of processing or working memory the primary information sclerosis functional composite measure (MSFC): an integrated
processing deficit in multiple sclerosis? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004; approach to MS clinical outcome assessment. National MS Society
26: 550–62. clinical outcomes assessment task force. Mult Scler 1999; 5: 244–50.
25 Rao SM, Grafman J, DiGuilio D, et al. Memory dysfunction in 50 Beatty WW, Paul RH, Blanco CR, Hames KA, Wilbanks SL. Attention
multiple sclerosis: its relation to working memory, semantic encoding in multiple sclerosis: correlates of impairment on the WAIS-R digit
and implicit learning. Neuropsychology 1993; 7: 364–74. span test. Appl Neuropsychol 1995; 2: 139–44.
26 DeLuca J, Barbieri-Berger S, Johnson SK. The nature of memory 51 McCarthy M, Beaumont JG, Thompson R, Peacock S. Modality-
impairments in multiple sclerosis: acquisition versus retrieval. specific aspects of sustained and divided attentional performance in
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1994; 16: 183–89. multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2005; 20: 705–18.
27 Macniven JA, Davis C, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, 52 Olivares T, Nieto A, Sánchez MP, Wollmann T, Hernández MA,
Constantinescu CS. Stroop performance in multiple sclerosis: Barroso J. Pattern of neuropsychological impairment in the early
information processing, selective attention, or executive functioning. phase of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2005;
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14: 805–14. 11: 191–97.
28 Fischer JS. Cognitive impairments in multiple sclerosis. In: Cook SD, 53 Kujala P, Portin R, Revonsuo A, Ruutiainen J. Attention related
ed. Handbook of multiple sclerosis. New York, USA: Marcel Dekker, performance in two cognitively different subgroups of patients with
2001: 233–56. multiple sclerosis. J Neurol NeurosurgeryPsychiatry 1995; 59: 77–82.
29 Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological 54 Loring DW. INS dictionary of neuropsychology. New York, USA:
assessment (4th edn). New York, USA: Oxford Univeristy Press, 2004. Oxford University Press, 1999.
30 Caine ED, Bamford KA, Schiffer RB, Shoulson I, Levy S. A controlled 55 Bagert B, Camplair P, Bourdette D. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple
neuropsychological comparison of Huntington’s disease and multiple sclerosis: history, patho-physiology and management. CNS Drugs
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1986; 43: 249–54. 2002; 16: 445–55.
31 Rao SM. Neuropsychology of multiple sclerosis: a critical review. 56 Bobholz JA, Rao SM. Cognitive Dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: a
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1986; 8: 503–42. review of recent developments. Curr Opin Neurol 2003; 16: 283–88.
32 Rao SM, Leo GJ, St. Aubin-Faubert P. On the nature of memory 57 Drew M, Tippett LJ, Starkey NJ, Isler RB. Executive dysfunction and
disturbance in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989; cognitive impairment in a large community-based sample with
11: 699–712. multiple sclerosis form New Zealand: a descriptive study.
33 Thornton AE, Raz N, Tucke KA. Memory in multiple sclerosis: Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2008; 23: 1–19.
contextual encoding deficits. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8: 395–409. 58 Straus E, Sherman E, Spreen O. A compendium of
34 DeLuca J, Gaudino EA, Diamond BJ, Christodoulou C, Engel RA. neuropsychological test: administration norms and commentary. New
Acquisition and storage deficits in multiple sclerosis. York, USA: University Oxford Press, 2006.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1998; 20: 376–90. 59 Henry JD, Beatty WW. Verbal fluency deficits in multiple sclerosis.
35 Nagy H, Bencsik K, Rajda C, et al. The effects of reward and Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 1166–74.
punishment contingencies on decision-making in multiple sclerosis. 60 Rao SM, Hammeke TA, Speech TJ. Wisconsin card sorting test
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12: 559–65. performance in relapsing-remitting and chronic-progressive multiple
36 Renell PG, Jensen F, Henry JD. Prospective memory in multiple sclerosis. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55: 263–65.
sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 410–16. 61 Parmenter BA, Zivadinov R, Kerenyi L, et al. Validity of the
37 Baddeley A. Working memory. Science 1992; 255: 556–59. Wisconsin card sorting and delis-kaplan executive function system
38 Janculjak D, Mubrin A, Brinar V, Spilich G. Changes of attention (DKEFS) sorting tests in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
and memory in a group of patients with multiple sclerosis. 2007; 29: 215–23.
Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2002; 104: 221–27. 62 Arnett PA, Rao SM, Bernardin L, Grafman J, Yetkin FZ, Lobeck L.
39 Bergendal G, Fredrikson S, Almkvist O. Selective decline in Relationship between frontal lobe lesions and Wisconsin card sorting
information processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8 year test performance in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1994;
old longitudinal study. Europ Neurol 2007; 57: 193–202. 44: 420–25.
40 Gaudino EA, Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J, Diamond BJ. A 63 Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Randolph JJ. Depression in multiple
comparison of memory performance in relapsing-remitting, sclerosis: relationship to planning ability. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2001;
primary progressive and secondary progressive and secondary 7: 665–74.
progressive, multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol 64 Denney DR, Lynch SG, Parmenter BA, Horne N. Cognitive
Behavior Neurol 2001; 14: 32–44. impairment in relapsing and primary progressive multiple sclerosis:
41 Lengenfelder J, Chiaravalloti ND, Ricker JH, DeLuca J. Deciphering mostly a matter of speed. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10: 948–56.
components of impaired working memory in multiple sclerosis. 65 Channon S, Baker J, Robertson M. Working memory in clinical
Cognitive Behavior Neurol 2003; 16: 635–39. depression: an experimental study. Psychol Med 1993; 23: 87–91.
42 Archibald CJ , Fisk JD. Information processing efficiency in patients 66 Vleugels L, Lafosse C, van Nunen A, et al. Visuospatial impairment in
with multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000; 22: 686–701. multiple sclerosis patients diagnosed with with neuropsychological
43 Kalmar JH, Gaudino EA, Moore NB, Halper J, DeLuca J. The tasks. Mult Scler 2000; 6: 241–54.
relationship between cognitive deficits and everyday functional 67 Bruce JM, Bruce AS, Arnett PA. Mild visual acuity disturbances are
activities in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology 2008; 22: 442–49. associated with performance on tests of complex visual attention in
44 Lengenfelder J, Bryant D, Diamond BJ, Kalmar JH, Moore NB, MS. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 544–48.
DeLuca J. Processing speed interacts with working memory efficiency 68 Franklin GM, Heaton RK, Nelson LM, Filley CM, Seibert C.
in multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2006; 21: 229–38. Correlation of neuropsychological and MRI findings in chronic
45 Parmenter JL, Shucard JL, Schucard DW. Information processing progressive multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1988; 38: 1826–29.
deficits in multiple sclerosis: a matter of complexity. 69 Basso MR, Beason-Hazon S, Lynn J, Rammohan K, Bornstein RA.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 417–23. Screening for cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol
46 Audoin B, Au Duong MV, Ranjeva JP, et al. Magnetic resonance study 1996; 53: 980–84.
of the influence of tissue damage and cortical reorganization on 70 Peyser JM, Rao SM, LaRocca NG, Kaplan E. Guidelines for
PASAT performance at the earliest stage of multiple sclerosis. neuropsychological research in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1990;
Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 24: 216–28. 47: 94–97.

1148 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

71 Younes M, Hill J, Quinless J, Kilduff M, Peng B, Cook SDCD. 96 Feinstein A , Roy P, Lobaugh N, Feinstein K, O’Connor P, Black S.
Internet-based cognitive testing in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2007; Structural brain abnormalities in multiple sclerosis patients with
13: 1011–19. major depression. Neurology 2004; 62: 586–90.
72 Beatty WW, Goodkin DE, Hertsgaard D, Monson N. Clinical 97 Zorzon M, deMasi R, Nasuelli D, et al. Depression and anxiety in
demographic predictors of cognitive performance in multiple multiple sclerosis: a clinical and MRI study in 95 subjects. J Neurol
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1990; 45: 611–19. 2001; 248: 416–21.
73 Lynch SG, Parmenter BA, Denney DR. The association between 98 Sabatini U, Pozzilli C, Pantano P, et al. Involvement of the limbic
cognitive impairment and physical disability in multiple sclerosis. system in multiple sclerosis patients with depressive disorders.
Mult Scler 2005; 11: 469–76. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39: 970–75.
74 Beatty WW, Goodkin DE, Monson N, Beatty PA. Cognitive 99 Foley FW, Miller AH, Traugott U, et al. Psychoimmunological
disturbances in patients with relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. dysregulation in multiple sclerosis. Psychosomatics 1988; 29: 398–404.
Arch Neurol 1989; 46: 1113–19. 100 Schapiro R. The pathophysiology of MS-related fatigue: what is the
75 Rao SM, Hammeke TA, McQuillen MP, Khatri BO, Lloyd D. Memory role of wake promotion? Int J MS Care 2002; 6–8.
disturbance in chronic progressive multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 101 DeLuca J. Fatigue, cognition, and mental effort. In: DeLuca J, ed.
1984; 41: 625–31. Fatigue as a window to the brain. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press,
76 Weinshenker BG, Brass B, Rice GA, et al. The natural history of 2005: 37–57.
multiple sclerosis: a geographically based study. I. Clinical course and 102 Johnson SK, Lange G, DeLuca J, Korn LR, Natelson BH. The effects
disability. Brain; 112: 133–46. of fatigue on neuropsychological performance in patients with
77 Comi G, Filippi M, Martinelli V, et al. Brain NRI correlates of chronic fatigue syndrome, multiple sclerosis, and depression.
cognitive impairment in primary and secondary progressive multiple Appl Neuropsychol 1997; 4: 145–53.
sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 1995; 132: 222–27. 103 Krupp LB, Elkins LE. Fatigue and declines in cognitive functioning in
78 Huijbregts SC, Kalkers NF, de Sonneville LM, de Groot VRIE, multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2000; 55: 934–39.
Polman CH. Differences in cognitive impairment of relapsing 104 Smith MM, Arnett PA. Dysarthria predicts poorer performance on
remitting, secondary, and primary progressive MS. Neurology 2004; cognitive tasks requiring a speeded oral response in an MS
63: 335–39. population. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2007; 29: 804–12.
79 Amato MP, Ponziani G, Siracusa G, Scorbi S. Cognitive dysfunction 105 Arnett PA, Smith MM, Barwick FH, Benedict RH, Ahlstrom BP.
in early-onset multiple sclerosis: a reappraisal after 10 years. Oral motor slowing in multiple sclerosis: relationship to
Arch Neurol 2001; 58: 1602–06. neuropsychological tasks requiring an oral response.
80 Schwid SR, Goodman AD, Weinstein A, McDermott MP, J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14: 454–62.
Johnson KP. Cognitive function in relapsing multiple sclerosis: 106 Beatty WM, Blanco CR, Wilbanks SL, Paul RH, Hames KA.
minimal changes in a 10-year clinical trial. J Neurol Sci 2007; Demographic, clinical, and cognitive characteristics of multiple
255: 57–63. sclerosis patients who continue to work. J Neurol Rehab 1995;
81 Schwid SR. Management of cognitive impairment in multiple 9: 167–73.
sclerosis. In: Rudick RA, Cohen JA, eds. Multiple sclerosis 107 Kessler HR, Cohen RA, Lauer K, Kausch DF. The relationship
therapeutics (2nd edn). London, UK: Martin-Dunitz, 2003: 715–27. between disability and memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis.
82 Arnett PA, Barwick FH, Beeney JE. Depression in multiple Int J Neurosci 1992; 62: 17–34.
sclerosis: review and theoretical proposal. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 108 Rao SM, Leo GJ, Ellington L, Nauertz T, Bernardin L, Unverzagt F.
2008; 14: 691–724. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. II. Impact on
83 Arnett P. Longitudinal consistency of the relationship between employment and social functioning. Neurology 1991; 41: 692–96.
depression symptoms and cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis. 109 Baum C, Edwards DF. Cognitive performance in senile dementia of
CNS Spectr 2005; 10: 372–82. the Alzheimer’s type: the kitchen task assessment. Am J Occ Ther
84 Landro NI, Celius EG, Sletvoid H. Depressive symptoms account 1993; 47: 431–36.
for deficient information processing speed but not for impaired 110 Goverover Y , Genova H, Hillary FG, DeLuca J. The relationship
working memory in early phase multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci between neuropsychological measuers and the Timed Instrumental
2004; 217: 211–16. Activities of Daily Living task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2006;
85 Fischer JS. Using the Wechsler memory scale-revised to detect and 12: 1–9.
characterize memory deficits in multiple sclerosis. Clin Neuropsychol 111 Staples D, Lincoln NB. Intellectual impairment in multiple sclerosis
1988; 2: 149–72. and its relation to functional abilities. Rheumatology Rehabilitation
86 Good K, Clark CM, Oger J, Paty D, Klonoff H. Cognitive impairment 1979; 18: 153–60.
and depression in mild multiple sclerosis. J Nerv Ment Dis 1992; 112 Grima DT, Torrance GW, Francis G, Rice G, Rosner AJ, Lafortune L.
180: 730–32. Cost and health related quality of life consequences of multiple
87 Arnett PA, Higgonson CI, Voss WD, Bender WI, Wurst JM, sclerosis. Mult Scler 2000; 6: 91–98.
Tippin JM. Depression in multiple sclerosis: relationship to working 113 Cutajar R, Ferriani E, Scandellari C, et al. Cognitive function and
memory capacity. Neuropsychology 1999; 13: 546–56. quality of life in multiple sclerosis patients. J Neurovirol 2000;
88 Thornton AE, Raz N. Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: 6: S186–90.
a quantitative review. Neuropsychology 1997; 11: 357–66. 114 Wang JL, Reimer MA, Metz LM, Patten SB. Major depression
89 Arnett PA, Higgonson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depressed mood in and quality of life in individuals with multiple sclerosis.
multiple sclerosis: relationship to capacity-demanding memory and Int J Psychiatry Med 2000; 30: 309–17.
attentional functioning. Neuropsychology 1999; 13: 434–46. 115 Jonsson A, Dock J, Ravnborg MH. Quality of life as a measure of
90 Gilchrist AC, Creed FH. Depression, cognitive impairment and social rehabilitation outcome in patients with multiple sclerosis.
stress in multiple sclerosis. J Psychosom Res 1994; 38: 193–201. Acta Neurol Scand 1996; 93: 229–35.
91 Demaree HA, Gaudino E, DeLuca J. The relationship between 116 Henriksson F, Fredrikson S, Masterman T, Jönsson B. Costs, quality
depressive symptoms and cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. of life and disease severity in multiple sclerosis: a cross-sectional
Cognit Neuropsychiatry 2003; 8: 161–71. study in Sweden. Eur J Neurol 2001; 8: 27–35.
92 Minden SL, Schiffer RB. Depression and mood disorders in multiple 117 Pfennings L, Cohen L, Ader H, et al. Exploring differences between
sclerosis. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1991; 4: 62–77. subgroups of multiple sclerosis patients in health-related quality of
93 Rabins PV, Brooks BR, O’Donnell P, et al. Structural brain correlates life. J Neurol 1999; 246: 587–91.
of emotional disorder in multiple sclerosis. Brain 1986; 109: 585–87. 118 The Canadian Burden of Illness Study Group. Burden of illness of
94 Bakshi R, Czarnecki D, Shaikh ZA, et al. Brain MRI lesions and multiple sclerosis: part II: quality of life. Can J Neurol Sci 1998;
atrophy are related to depression in multiple sclerosis. Neuroreport 25: 31–38.
2000; 11: 1153–58. 119 Nortvedt MW, Riise T, Myhr KM, Nyland HI. Performance of the
95 Berg D, Supprian T, Thomae J, et al. Lesion pattern in patients with SF-36, SF-12, and RAND-36 summary scales in a multiple sclerosis
multiple sclerosis and depression. Mult Scler 2000; 6: 156–62. population. Med Care 2000; 38: 1022–28.

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1149


Review

120 Janardhan V, Bakshi R. Quality of life and its relationship to brain 147 Basso MR, Lowery N, Ghormley C, Combs D, Johnson J.
lesions and atrophy on magnetic resonance images in 60 patients Self-generated learning in people with multiple sclerosis.
with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 2000; 57: 1485–91. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12: 640–48.
121 Gulick EE. Correlates of quality of life among persons with multiple 148 Goverover Y, Hillary FG, Chiaravalloti ND, Arango JC, DeLuca J.
sclerosis. Nurs Res 1997; 46: 305. A functional application of the spacing effect to improve learning
122 Brunet DG, Hopman WM, Singer MA, Edgar CM, MacKenzie TA. and memory in persons with multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exper
Measurement of health-related quality of life in multiple sclerosis Neuropsychol (in press).
patients. Can J Neurol Sci 1996; 23: 99–103. 149 Chiaravalloti ND, Demaree H, Gaudino E, DeLuca J. Can the
123 Reingold SC. Research directions in multiple sclerosis. New York, repetition effect maximize learning in multiple sclerosis?
USA: National Multiple Sclerosis Society, 1995. Clin Rehabil 2003; 17: 58–68.
124 Mitchell JN. Multiple sclerosis and the prospects for employment. 150 Lincoln NB, Dent A, Harding J. Treatment of cognitive problems
J Soc Occup Med 1981; 31: 134–38. for people with multiple sclerosis. Int J Ther Rehab 2003; 10: 412–16.
125 Gronning M, Hannisdal E, Mellgren SV. Multivariate analysis of 151 Mendoza RJ, Pittenger DJ, Weinstein CS. Unit management
factors associated with unemployment in people with multiple of depression of patients with multiple sclerosis using
sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1990; 53: 388–90. cognitive remediation strategies: a preliminary study.
126 Kornblith AB, LaRocca NG, Baum HM. Employment in individuals Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2001; 15: 9–14.
with multiple sclerosis. Int J Rehabil Res 1986; 9: 155–65. 152 Weinstein A , Schwid SL, Schiffer RB, McDermott MP, Giang DW,
127 Bauer HJ. Problems with symptomatic therapy in multiple Goodman AD. Neuropsychologic status in multiple sclerosis after
sclerosis. Neurology 1978; 28: 8–20. treatment with glatiramer. Arch Neurol 1999; 56: 319–24.
128 LaRocca N, Kalb R, Schneinberg L, Kendall P. Factors associated 153 Fischer JS, Priore RL, Jacobs LD, Cookfair DL, Rudick RA, Herndon
with unemployment in patients with multiple sclerosis. RM. Neuropsychological effects of interferon beta-1a in relapsing
J Chronic Dis 1985; 38: 203–10. multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol 2000; 48: 6: 885–92.
129 Edgley K, Sullivan MJ, Dehoux E. A survey of multiple sclerosis, 154 Pliskin NH, Hamer DP, Goldstein DS, Towle VL, Reder AT,
part 2: determinants of employment status. Can J Rehab 1991; Noronha A. Improved delayed visual reproduction test performance
4: 127–32. in multiple sclerosis patients receiving interferon beta-1b. Neurology
1996; 47: 6: 1463–68.
130 Harvey C. Economic costs of multiple sclerosis: how much and who
pays? New York, USA: National Multiple Sclerosis Society, 1995. 155 Barak Y, Achiron A. Effect of interferon beta-1b on cognitive
functions in multiple sclerosis. Eur Neurol 2002; 47: 11–14.
131 LaRocca NG, Kalb RC. Efficacy of rehabilitation in multiple
sclerosis. J Neurol Rehab 1992; 6: 147–55. 156 Ziemssen THJ, Apfel R, Kern S. Effects of glatiramer acetate on
fatigue and days of absence from work in first-time treated relapsing-
132 Prosiegel M, Michael C. Neuropsychology and multiple sclerosis:
remitting multiple sclerosis. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2008; 6: 1–6.
diagnostic and rehabilitative approaches. J Neurol Sci 1993; 115: S51–4.
157 Galetta SL, Markowitz C, Lee AG. Immunomodulatory agents for
133 LaRocca NG. A rehabilitation perspective. In: Rao SM, Ed.
the treatment of relapsing multiple sclerosis: a systematic review.
Neurobehavioral aspects of multiple sclerosis. New York: Oxford
Arch Intern Med 2002; 162: 2161–69.
University Press; 1990: 215–29.
158 Leo GJ, Rao SM. Effects of intravenous physostigmine and
134 Plohmann AM, Kappos L, Ammann W, et al. Computer assisted
lecithin on memory in multiple sclerosis: report of a pilot study.
retraining of attentional impairments in patients with multiple
J Neurol Rehab 1988; 2: 123–29.
sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 64: 455–62.
159 Unverzgagt FW, Rao SM, Antuono PG. Oral physostigmine
135 Foley FW, Dince WM, LaRocca NG, et al. Psychoremediation of
in the treatment of memory loss in multiple sclerosis (MS).
communication skills for cognitively impaired persons with
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1991; 131: 74.
multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Rehab 1994; 8: 165–76.
160 Krupp LB, Christodoulou C, Melville P, et al. Donezepil improved
136 Allen DN, Longmore S, Goldstein G. Memory training and multiple
memory in multiple sclerosis in a randomized clinical trial.
sclerosis: a case study. Int J Rehab Health 1995; 1: 189–202.
Neurology 2004; 63: 1579–85.
137 Jonsson A, Korfitzen EM, Heltberg A, Ravnborg MH,
161 Krupp LB, Elkins LE, Scheffer S, Smiroldo J, Coyle PK. Donepezil
Byskov-Ottosen E. Effects of neuropsychological treatment in
for the treatment of memory impairments in multiple sclerosis.
patients with multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand 1993;
Neurology 1999; 52: A137.
88: 394–400.
162 Greene YM, Tariot PN, Wishart H, et al. A 12-week open trial of
138 Allen DD, Goldstein G, Heyman RA, Rondinelli T. Teaching
donepezil hydrochloride in patients with multiple sclerosis and
memory strategies to persons with multiple sclerosis.
associated cognitive impairments. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2000;
J Rehabil Res Dev 1998; 35: 405–10.
20: 350–56.
139 Rodgers D, Khoo K, MacEachen M, Oven M, Beatty WW.
163 Christodoulou C, Melville P, Scherl WF, MacAllister WS, Elkins LE,
Cognitive therapy for multiple sclerosis: a preliminary study.
Krupp LB. Effects of donepezil on memory and cognition in
Altern Ther Health Med 1996; 2: 70–74.
multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 2006; 245: 127–36.
140 Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J, Moore NB, Ricker JH. Treating learning
164 Christodoulou C, MacAllister WS, McLinskey NA, Krupp LB.
impairments improves memory performance in multiple sclerosis:
Treatment of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: is the use
a randomized clinical trial. Mult Scler 2005; 11: 58–68.
of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors a viable option? CNS Drugs 2008;
141 Lincoln NB, Dent A, Harding J, et al. Evaluation of cognitive 22: 87–97.
assessment and cognitive intervention for people with people with
165 Cohen RA, Fisher M. Amantadine treatment of fatigue associated
MS. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002; 72: 93–98.
with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1989; 46: 676–80.
142 Solari A, Motta A, Mendozzi L, et al. Computer-aided retraining of
166 Geisler MW, Sliwinski M, Coyle PK, Masur DM, Doscher C,
memory and attention in people with multiple sclerosis: a
Krupp LB. The effects of amantadine and pemoline on cognitive
randomized, double-blind controlled trial. J Neurol Sci 2004;
functioning in multiple sclerosis. 1996; 53: 185–88.
222: 99–104.
167 Lovera J, Bagert B, Smoot K, et al. Ginkgo biloba for the
143 O’Brien A, Chiaravalloti N, Goverover Y, DeLuca J. Evidenced based
improvement of cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis: a
cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: a review
randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Mult Scler 2007; 13: 376–85.
of the literature. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008; 89: 761–69.
168 Napoli SQ, Bakshi R. Magnetic resonance imaging in multiple
144 Plohmann A, Kappos L, Brunnschweiler H. Evaluation of
sclerosis. Rev Neurol Dis 2005; 2: 109–16.
a computer-based attention retraining program for patients with
MS. Schweiz Arch Neurol Psychiatr 1994; 145: 35–36. 169 Christodoulou C, Krupp LB, Liang Z, et al. Cognitive performance
and MR markers of cerebral injury in cognitively impaired MS
145 Birnboim S, Miller A. Cognitive rehabilitation for multiple sclerosis
patients. Neurology 2003; 60: 1793–98.
patients with executive dysfunction. J Cogn Rehab 2004; 22: 11–18.
170 Summers MM, Fisniku LK, Anderson VM, Miller DH, Cipolotti L,
146 Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Self-generation as a means of
Ron M. Cognitive impairment in relapsing-remitting multiple
maximizing learning in multiple sclerosis: an application of the
sclerosis can be predicted by imaging performed several years
generation effect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2002; 83: 1070–79.
earlier. Mult Scler 2008; 14: 197–204.

1150 www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008


Review

171 Amato MP, Bartolozzi ML, Zipoli V. Neocortical volume decrease in 192 Lycke J, Wikkelso C, Bergh AC, Jacobsson L, Andersen O. Regional
relapsing-remitting MS patients with mild cognitive impairment. cerebral blood flow in multiple sclerosis masured by single photon
Neurology 2004; 63: 89–93. emission tomography with technetium-99m
172 Rao SM, Leo GJ, Haughton VM, Aubin-Faubert PS, Bernardin L. hexamethylpropyleneamine oxime. Eur Neurol 1993; 33: 163–67.
Correlation of magnetic resonance imaging with 193 Paulesu E, Perani D, Fazio F, et al. Functional basis of memory
neuropsychological testing in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1989; impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage 1996; 4: 87–96.
39: 161–66. 194 Pozzilli C, Passafiume D, Bernardi S, et al. SPECT, MRI and
173 Swirsky-Sacchetti T, Mitchell DR, Seward J. Neuropsychological and cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg
structural brain lesions in multiple sclerosis: a regional analysis. Psychiatry 1991; 54: 110–15.
Neurology 1992; 42: 1291–95. 195 Roelcke U, Kappos L, Lechner-Scott J, et al. Reduced glucose
174 Bermel RA, Bakshi R, Tjoa C, Puli SR, Jacobs L. Bicaudate ratio as a metabolism in the frontal cortex and basal ganglia of multiple
magnetic resonance imaging marker of brain atrophy in multiple sclerosis patients with fatigue a 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 2002; 59: 275–80. emission tomography study. Neurology 1997; 48: 1566–71.
175 Sanfilipo MP, Benedict RHB, Weinstock-Guttman B, Bakshi R. 196 Inglese M, Adhya S, Johnson G, et al. Perfusion magnetic
Gray and white matter brain atrophy and neuropsychological resonance imaging correlates of neuropsychological impairment in
impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2006; 66: 685–92. multiple sclerosis. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2008; 28: 164–71.
176 Benedict RH , Weinstock-Guttman B, Fishman I, Sharma J, 197 Chiaravalloti ND, Hillary FG, DeLuca J, Ricker JH, Liu WC,
Tjoa CW, Bakshi R. Prediction of neuropsychological impairment in Kalnin AJ. Cerebral activation patterns during working memory
multiple sclerosis: comparison of conventional magnetic resonance performance in multiple sclerosis using fMRI.
imaging measures of atrophy and lesion burden. Arch Neurol 2004; J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2005; 27: 33–54.
61: 226–30. 198 Sweet LH, Rao SM, Primeau M, Mayer AR, Cohen RA. Functional
177 Benedict RHB, Carone DA, Bakshi R. Correlating brain atrophy magnetic resonance imaging of working memory among multiple
with cognitive dysfunction, mood disturbance and personality sclerosis patients. J Neuroimaging 2004; 14: 150–57.
disorder in multiple sclerosis. J Neuroimaging 2004; 14: 365–55. 199 Hillary FG, Chiaravalloti ND, Ricker JH, et al. An investigation of
178 Benedict RHB, Bruce JM, Dwyer MG, et al. Neocortical atrophy, working memory rehearsal in mulitple sclerosis using fMRI.
third ventricular width, and cognitive dysfunction in multiple J Clin Experimental Neuropsychol 2003; 25: 965–78.
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 2006; 63: 1301–06. 200 Audoin B, Ibarrola D, Ranjeva JP, et al. Compensatory cortical
179 Tekok-Kilic A, Benedict RH, Weinstock-Guttman B, et al. activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest
Independent contributions of cortical gray matter atrophy and stage of MS. Human Brain Mapping 2003; 20: 51–58.
ventricle enlargement for predicting neuropsychological 201 Mainero C, Caramia F, Pozzilli C, et al. fMRI evidence of brain
impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage 2007; 36: 1294–3000. reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple
180 Hohol MJ, Guttmann CR, Orav J. Serial neuropsychological sclerosis. Neuroimage 2004; 21: 858–67.
assessment and magnetic resonance imaging analysis in multiple 202 Cader S, Cifelli A, Abu-Omar Y, Palace J, Matthews PM. Reduced
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1997; 54: 1018–25. brain functional reserve and altered functional connectivity in
181 Pelletier J, Suchet L, Witjas T, et al. A longitudinal study of callosal patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain 2006; 129: 527–37.
atrophy and interhemispheric dysfunction in relapse-remitting 203 Wishart HA, Saykin AJ, McDonald BC, et al. Brain activation
multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol 2001; 58: 105–11. patterns associated with working memory in relapsing-remitting
182 Roca M, Torralva T, Meli F, et al. Cognitive deficits in multiple MS. Neurology 2004; 62: 234–38.
sclerosis correlate with changes in fronto-subcortical tracts. 204 Li Y, Chiaravalloti ND, Hillary FG, et al. Differential cerebellar
Mult Scler 2008; 14: 364–69. activation on magnetic resonance imaging during working
183 Rovaris M, Iannucci G, Falautano M. Cognitive dysfunction in memory performance in persons with multiple sclerosis.
patients with mildly disabling relapsing-remitting multiple Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85: 635–39.
sclerosis: an exploratory study with diffusion tensor. J Neurol Sci 205 Penner IK, Rausch M, Kappos L, Opwis K, Radu EW. Analysis of
2002; 195: 103–09. impairment related functional architecture in MS patients during
184 Rovaris M, Filippi M, Minicucci L. Cortical/subcortical disease performance of different attention tasks. J Neurol 2003; 250: 461–72.
burden and cognitive impairment in patients with multiple 206 Santa Maria MP, Benedict RH, Bakshi R, et al. Functional imaging
sclerosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2000; 21: 402–08. during covert auditory attention in multiple sclerosis. J Neruol Sci
185 Zivadinov R, Sepcic J, Nasuelli D. A longitudinal study of brain 2004; 218: 9–15.
atrophy and cognitive disturbances in the early phase of relapsing- 207 Arnett PA, Rao SM, Grafman J, et al. Executive functions in
remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; multiple sclerosis: an analysis of temporal ordering, semantic
70: 773–80. encoding, and planning abilities. Neuropsychology 1997; 11: 535–44.
186 Deloire M, Salort E, Bonnet M. Cognitive impairment as marker of 208 Stablum F, Meligrana L, Sgaramella T, Bortolon F, Toso V.
diffuse brain abnormalities in early relapsing remitting multiple Endogenous task shift processes in relapsing-remitting multiple
sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76: 519–26. sclerosis. Brain Cognitive Sci 2004; 56: 328–31.
187 Filippi M, Tortorella C, Rovaris M. Changes in the normal 209 Lazeron RH, Rombouts SA, Scheltens P, Polman CH, Barkhof F.
appearing brain tissue and cognitive impairment in patients with An fMRI study of planning-related brain activity in patients with
MS. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000; 21: 402–08. moderately advanced multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2004; 10: 549–55.
188 Staffen W, Zauner H, Mair A, et al. Magnetic resonance 210 Voelbel GT, Chiaravallloti ND, DeLuca J. Functional neuroimaging
spectroscopy of memory and frontal brain region in early multiple in multiple sclerosis. In: DeLuca J, Hillary FG, eds.
sclerosis. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 17: 357–63. Functional neuroimaging in clinical populations. New York, USA:
189 Mathiesen HK, Jonsson A, Tscherning T, et al. Correlation of global Guilford (in press).
N-Acetyl aspartate with cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. 211 Filippi M, Rocca MA. Cortical reorganisation in patients with MS.
Arch Neurol 2006; 63: 533–36. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75: 1087–89.
190 Blinkenberg M, Rune K, Jensen CV, et al. Coritcal cerebral 212 Hillary FG. Neuroimaging of working memory dysfunction and the
metabolism correlates with MRI lesion load and cognitive dilemma with brain reorganization hypotheses.
dysfunction in MS. Neurology 2000; 54: 558–64. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14: 526–34.
191 Brooks DJ, Leenders KL, Head G, Marshall J, Legg NJ, Jones JT.
Studies on regional cerebral oxygen utilisation and cognitive
function in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1984;
47: 1182–91.

www.thelancet.com/neurology Vol 7 December 2008 1151

You might also like