Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 55 (2013) 921–925

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The role of Dependency and Self-Criticism in the relationship between


anger and depression
Rudy Abi-Habib a,⇑, Patrick Luyten a,b
a
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, P.O. Box 3720, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
b
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street London, WC1E 6BT, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study explored the role of Dependency and Self-Criticism, two personality dimensions conferring
Received 18 March 2013 vulnerability for depression, in explaining relationships among anger, anger regulation strategies, and
Received in revised form 9 July 2013 severity of depression in a sample of adults (n = 253) using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). Depen-
Accepted 18 July 2013
dency was associated with high levels of trait anger, the turning of anger towards the self, and low levels
Available online 12 August 2013
of anger directed towards others. Self-Criticism was associated with high levels of both state and trait
anger, low anger control, and high levels of anger towards the self and others. Moreover, turning anger
Keywords:
towards the self mediated the relationship among both Dependency and Self-Criticism and severity of
Personality
Depression
depression. Problems with anger control also mediated the relationship between Self-Criticism and
Anger severity of depression. These findings suggest that Self-Criticism may be closely related to angry/hostile
Dependency features in depression, while dependency may be more closely associated with the turning of anger
Self-Criticism towards the self. Although this study was cross-sectional and self-reports of anger might be biased by
impression management, results suggest that these personality factors should be taken into account in
both research on and treatment of anger in depression.
Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction the turning of anger against the self, 24% contradict this hypothesis,
and 32% yielded inconsistent results. One explanation for these
Anger is an important symptom in many psychological disorders conflicting findings is that depression is a heterogeneous condition
such as bipolar disorder (e.g. Perlis et al., 2004), obsessive compul- with respect to etiology and pathogenesis (Parker, 2000). This study
sive disorder (e.g. Painuly, Grover, Gupta, & Mattoo, 2011), chronic sets out to investigate the role of anger and anger regulation strat-
pain disorder (e.g. Bruehl, Liu, Burns, Chont, & Jamison, 2012), PTSD egies in particular in the relationship between the personality
(e.g. McHugh, Forbes, Bates, Hopwood, & Creamer, 2012), eating dimensions of Dependency and Self-Criticism and severity of
disorders (e.g. Giovanni et al., 2011), personality disorders (e.g. Fava depression in a normal community sample.
& Rosenbaum, 1998), anxiety (e.g. Painuly, Grover, Mattoo, & Gupta, More than three decades of research has provided considerable
2011) and depression. Studies suggest that anger and hostility are evidence that these two personality dimensions confer vulnerabil-
present in about 30–40% of patients with major depressive disorder ity to depression (Blatt, 2004; Luyten & Blatt, 2011, in press).
(Baeg, Wang, Chee, Kim, & Kim, 2011; Fava & Rosenbaum, 1998, Dependency refers to a cognitive-affective personality style that
1999; Giovanni et al., 2011; Koh, Kim, Kim, & Park, 2005; Newman, is characterized by strong needs to be loved and taken care of, in
Fuqua, Gray, & Simpson, 2006; Newman, Gray, & Fuqua, 1999; combination with exaggerated fears for loss and abandonment.
Rude, Chrisman, Denmark, & Maestas, 2012; Vliegen & Luyten, Self-Criticism implies a strong emphasis on control, self-definition,
2008). Yet, there is still relatively little known about the role of an- and autonomy, in combination with fears of disapproval and loss of
ger in depression. A review by Fisher and Greenberg (1996) of more autonomy and control. Central in these formulations is that Depen-
than 50 studies that have investigated the role of the turning of an- dency and Self-Criticism show important differences in anger
ger towards the self in depression found that about 44% of these expression and regulation (Blatt, 2004; Clark & Beck, 1999).
studies support the hypothesis that depression is associated with Although highly dependent individuals are described as very sensi-
tive to frustration, and particularly to interpersonal rejection, they
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sci-
would typically inhibit their anger, especially towards significant
ences, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, P.O. Box 3720, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. others, for fear of losing their love and support (‘‘rage threatens
Tel.: +32 4 88 95 36 03. the very hand that feeds’’). One way dependent individuals may
E-mail address: Roudy.AbiHabib@ppw.kuleuven.be (R. Abi-Habib). control anger is by turning their anger towards the self (Blatt,

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.466
922 R. Abi-Habib, P. Luyten / Personality and Individual Differences 55 (2013) 921–925

2008). Self-critical individuals, by contrast, are described as com- 2.2. Materials


petitive, hostile, and ambivalent towards others. They would have
difficulty controlling anger and would typically express their anger As part of a larger assessment battery, participants completed
towards others, often leading to conflict in interpersonal relation- the following questionnaires:
ships (Constantino & et al., 2008; Luyten, Fonagy, Lemma, & Target, The Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ; Blatt, D’Afflitti, &
2012). At the same time, however, because of their high personal Quinlan, 1976) was used to assess Dependency and Self-Criticism.
standards, they would also turn anger towards the self in the form The DEQ is a well-validated measure of these personality dimen-
of harsh Self-Criticism (Blatt, 2008; Luyten & Blatt, 2012). sions that contains 66 items measuring Dependency, Self-Criti-
Yet, despite the centrality of descriptions concerning problems cism, and Efficacy. Participants have to indicate on a 7-point
with anger and anger regulation in theoretical formulations of Likert-type scale how much they agree with each item going from
Dependency and Self-Criticism, this topic has attracted little re- 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The Dutch version of the
search attention. One exception is a longitudinal study by Koest- DEQ (Luyten, Corveleyn, & Blatt, 1997) has similar psychometric
ner, Zuroff, and Powers (1991) reporting that Self-Criticism at qualities as the original DEQ. In this study, only the Dependency
age 12 predicted inhibition of aggressive impulses at age 31. Sauro and Self-Criticism scales were used and local factor weights were
et al. (2001) reported, in line with theoretical expectations, that used to score the DEQ.
Dependency was not related to anger directed towards others, The State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI; Spielberger,
and was positively related to inward-directed anger in a sample 1996) has been developed to assess levels of State and Trait anger
of 67 college females. Dependency was not related to state and and three broad ways that people use to regulate anger, i.e. turning
trait anger in this study. Vliegen and Luyten (2008), finally, in a anger towards the self (‘‘Anger-In’’), directing anger towards others
sample of 55 postpartum depressed mothers, found that Depen- (‘‘Anger-Out’’) and not expressing anger by controlling angry feel-
dency was associated with the turning of anger against the self ings (‘‘Anger-Control’’). The STAXI consists of 44 items, each scored
but was not correlated with state and trait anger. Self-Criticism on a 4-point Likert type scale, measuring state anger, trait anger,
was associated with state and trait anger, the turning of anger to- anger directed towards the self (anger-in, e.g. ‘‘I tend to harbor
wards the self and others and was negatively related to anger grudges’’), the expression of anger towards other people or objects
control. (anger-out, e.g. ‘‘I say nasty things’’), and the control of anger (an-
Given that none of these studies directly investigated the role of ger-control, e.g. ‘‘I control my temper’’). Translation of the STAXI
anger and anger regulation in the relationship among these per- state and trait anger scales was based on the Dutch version of
sonality dimensions and severity of depression, this study aims the State-Trait Anger Scale (STAS; Van der Ploeg, Defares, & Spiel-
to further investigate the role of anger and anger regulation in berger, 1982). The anger-in, anger-out and anger-control scales
the relationship between Dependency and Self-Criticism and were translated using the commission approach consisting of one
severity of depression in a sample of community adults (n = 253). of the authors and two research assistants (Hambleton, 1994). Esti-
Because Dependency has been associated with increased interper- mates of internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) in the present
sonal sensitivity and Self-Criticism with competitiveness and hos- study were .83 for state anger, .86 for trait anger, .65 for anger-
tility, both Dependency and Self-Criticism were expected to be in, .64 for anger-out, and .86 for anger-control.
positively related to state and trait anger (the intensity of anger Severity of depression was measured with the Beck Depression
as an emotional state and angry feelings over time, respectively). Inventory (BDI; Beck & Steer, 1993). The BDI measures 21 symp-
Second, in line with theoretical formulations, we expected that toms of depression (scores 0–63). Psychometric characteristics of
Dependency would be positively related to anger control (i.e. con- the Dutch version of the BDI are similar to the original version
trolling anger by preventing its expression) and the turning of an- (Schotte, Maes, Cluydts, De Doncker, & Cosyns, 1997). In this study,
ger towards the self (i.e. suppressing angry feelings by being angry the estimate of internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was .79.
at oneself), and negatively to the expression of anger towards oth-
ers (i.e. expressing anger towards others or objects in the environ- 2.3. Procedures
ment). Self-Criticism was expected to be positively related to the
expression of anger towards others, or negatively with anger con- The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Uni-
trol, and positively with the turning of anger towards the self. Fi- versity of Leuven (Belgium). Participants were recruited in person
nally, we tested a mediation model depicted in Fig. 1, in which and at their homes by undergraduate students as part of a course
anger regulation styles partially mediate the relationships between on research methodology. After informed consent, participants re-
both personality dimensions and severity of depression. ceived a booklet containing questionnaires including the DEQ,
BDI, and STAXI, and were instructed to complete and return the
booklet in a closed, pre-addressed and stamped envelope within
2. Methods four weeks. If they had not returned the completed question-
naires by that time, they were contacted up to three times by
2.1. Participants the undergraduate students in the following two weeks. All par-
ticipants returned completed questionnaires within this 6-week
Participants were 253 community adults with a mean age of period.
32.21 years (SD = 5.40) (58.33% female). All participants had Bel-
gian nationality. They were recruited in person, in their homes, 2.4. Statistical analysis
by students attending a course of methodology at a large Belgian
university. Each student had to find one participant who met the Zero-order correlations were calculated to explore the relation-
following inclusion criteria: (a) age between 18 and 65 years, (b) ships among the study variables. Next Structural Equation Model-
Dutch literacy, and (c) willing to participate in a questionnaire ing (SEM) was used. SEM is an advanced multivariate form of
study concerning depression. There were no gender differences regression analysis that allows studying complex relationships
in anger and anger regulation strategies and in the relationships among variables. We investigated the theoretical model presented
between Dependency, Self-Criticism, and anger. Therefore, data in Fig. 1 following state of the art recommendations for the testing
for men and women were combined. Table 1 presents descriptive of mediation (Fairchild & MacKinnon, 2009; Hayes, 2009; Hoyle &
data for the sample. Smith, 1994) using a two-step strategy. In a first step, we tested a
R. Abi-Habib, P. Luyten / Personality and Individual Differences 55 (2013) 921–925 923

Fig. 1. Theoretical model of the relationships between Dependency, Self-Criticism, anger regulation strategies and severity of depression. Negative paths coefficients and
positive path coefficients are represented by – and + respectively DE P = Dependency, SC = Self-Criticism; Anger-in = Anger directed towards the self, Anger-out = the
expression of anger towards other people or objects, Anger-con = the control of anger.

Table 1
Means, standard deviation and zero-order correlations among study variables.

1 Dep – .140* .119 .117 .070 .207** .060 .045 0.418 0.965
2 SC – .455** .319** .220** .492** .393** .177** 0.615 0.898
3 Anger-in – .150* .037 .366** .337** .151* 15.395 3.567
4 Anger-out – .443** .167** .595** .204** 15.252 3.070
5 Anger-Control – .245** .537** .174** 23.584 4.559
6 Severity of Depression – .334** .353** 5.490 4.863
7 Trait Anger – .250** 16.6206 4.513
8 State Anger – 10.6877 1.871

12 345678 M SD.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

base model with all the paths included in Fig. 1. Non-significant state anger or to anger control. Concerning the regulation of anger,
paths were deleted, which yielded the final base model. In a second Dependency was, as expected, positively related to the turning of
step, we then tested a model in which the anger regulation strate- anger toward the self, and was not related to anger directed to-
gies as assessed with the STAXI fully mediated the relationships wards others. Self-Criticism was, as expected, positively related
between Dependency, Self-Criticism and severity of depression, to state and trait anger, anger-in and anger-out, and was negatively
i.e., by dropping the direct paths from Dependency and Self-Criti- related with anger control.
cism to severity of depression. If this more parsimonious model
did not provide a significantly worse fit than the model with the 3.2. Structural Equation Modeling
direct effects, this indicates full mediation; else anger regulation
strategies are only partial mediators in the association between The base model did not provide a good fit to the data;
these personality dimensions and severity of depression. The fol- v2 = 52.403 p < .001; df = 3; v2/df = 17.468, CFI = .96,
lowing fit indices and cut-off values were used: v2/df 6 3, RMSEA = .256. In this model, three paths were not significant,
CFI > .90 indicating acceptable fit and >.95 indicating good fit (Hu namely from DEP to Anger-out and Anger-control and from An-
& Bentler, 1999; Kline, 1998) and RMSEA < 0.08 (acceptable fit) ger-out to BDI. In subsequent analyses, we therefore dropped these
and <0.06 (good fit) (Byrne, 1998; Hu & Bentler, 1999). To compare paths. AMOS furthermore suggested to add two error covariances,
the fit of the nested models (e.g., with and without direct effects), namely between the errors of Anger-in, Anger-control and Anger-
we used v2-difference tests. AMOS (IBM, SPSS AMOS 20) was used out and Anger-control. This led to the final base model which
to test all SEM models (Fig. 2). had an excellent fit: v2 = 2.427, p = .658; df = 4; v2/df = .607,
CFI = 1.000; RMSEA = .000.
3. Results The model with only the indirect paths also provided a good fit
to the data, v2 = 7.253; p = .202; df = 5; v2/df = 1.451, CFI = .992;
3.1. Zero-order correlations RMSEA = .042. However, this model showed a significantly worse
fit compared to the model including the direct paths (Dv2 = 4.83,
As Table 1 indicates, Dependency was, as expected, positively df = 1, p = 0.02), implying that the model with both the direct
related to trait anger but, contrary to expectations, not related to and indirect paths explained the data better.
924 R. Abi-Habib, P. Luyten / Personality and Individual Differences 55 (2013) 921–925

Fig. 2. Final Model Standardized values are given for the path coefficients. For ease of presentation errors and error coefficients are not represented. ⁄⁄⁄p < .001. DE
P = Dependency, SC = Self-Criticism; Anger-in = Anger directed towards the self, Anger-out = the expression of anger towards other people or objects, Anger-con = the control
of anger.

4. Discussion and conclusions Analyses using Structural Equation Modeling shed further light
on these dynamics. First, both Self-Criticism and Dependency were
This study sought to further investigate the role of anger and associated with increased levels of directing anger towards the self,
anger regulation in the relationships between the personality which in turn were associated with higher levels of severity of
dimensions of Dependency, Self-Criticism, two personality dimen- depression. Second, Self-Criticism, but not Dependency, was asso-
sions that confer vulnerability to depression (Blatt, 2008; Luyten & ciated with problems with anger control, which in turn was related
Blatt, 2012) and severity of depression in a community sample. to increased severity of depression. These findings seems to sug-
Zero order correlations showed that Dependency was not related gest that the more self-critical individuals have the sense that they
to state anger, but was positively associated with trait anger. These are losing control of their anger, the more depressed they become,
findings are congruent with theoretical assumptions suggesting perhaps because this triggers further self-criticism. Third, Self-
that individuals with high levels of Dependency show elevated lev- Criticism was also associated with higher levels of directing anger
els of trait anger, which probably reflects their low frustration tol- towards others, but this association did not explain increased lev-
erance (Blatt, 2004). In contrast to a study by Sauro et al. (2001), els of severity of depression associated with Self-Criticism. It may
Dependency was not associated with the inhibition of anger gener- be that the observed association between high levels of Self-Criti-
ally and the inhibition of anger towards others specifically. Yet, cism and increased anger towards others plays an important role in
Dependency was, congruent with theoretical descriptions and ear- explaining vulnerability for depression associated with Self-Criti-
lier studies (Blatt, 2008; Vliegen & Luyten, 2008), associated with cism more generally, but not in explaining increased levels of
directing anger towards the self. Findings that Dependency was severity of depression as such. Indeed, congruent with interper-
not associated with state anger nor with directing anger towards sonal approaches of depression, the turning of anger towards oth-
others and with anger control may perhaps be explained by studies ers has been shown to lead to vicious interpersonal cycles
suggesting that Dependency is positively associated with impres- characterized by increased feelings of frustration and anger in (sig-
sion management in that dependent individuals often seem to nificant) others, finally resulting in social exclusion, rejection, iso-
underreport feelings of anger (e.g., Ewart, Jorgensen, & Kolodner, lation and ultimately loneliness and depression (Luyten et al.,
1998; Santor & Zuroff, 1997). Hence, while they do control anger 2012). This self-fulfilling prophecy has indeed been associated
(as is also suggested by the positive association with the turning with increased vulnerability for depression. Fichman, Koestner,
of anger towards the self), when directly asked, they may overtly and Zuroff (1996), for instance, found that Self-Criticism was re-
deny feelings of anger out of fear that even admitting anger to- lated to having low perceived competences and poor social func-
wards others will lead to rejection and abandonment. These spec- tioning in a sample of 77 adolescents. This may add to the
ulations await further research. difficulties self-critical individuals have to engage in relationships,
Self-Criticism was strongly related to both state and trait anger, distancing them even more from others, leading to increased risk
compatible with theoretical assumptions according to which self- for depression.
critical individuals have high levels of hostility (Fava & Rosenbaum, Taken together, findings of this study suggest that Dependency is
1998, 1999; Gilbert, Gilbert, & Irons, 2004). Moreover, Self- most closely related to depression associated with inhibited anger,
Criticism was also related to anger directed towards the self and and perhaps also with problems acknowledging anger. It may be
others and problems with the control of anger. Future studies that these individuals are not or only partially aware of underlying
should investigate whether the combination of high levels of feelings of anger, but further research using more implicit or behav-
directing anger towards others and low control of anger reflects ioral measures of anger is needed to substantiate this assumption.
problems with anger management (e.g., failure to control anger) Nevertheless, results for Dependency closely resemble evolutionary
or simply that self-critical individuals do not find it important to accounts of depression that have linked certain types of depression
control their anger towards others. to arrested anger (Gilbert et al., 2004). Self-Criticism, in contrast,
R. Abi-Habib, P. Luyten / Personality and Individual Differences 55 (2013) 921–925 925

appears to be associated with hostile/irritable issues in depression. Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. P. (1996). Freud scientifically reappraised. Testing the
theories and therapy. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
This latter finding might explain the increased risk for suicide asso-
Gilbert, P., Gilbert, J., & Irons, C. (2004). Life events, entrapments and arrested anger
ciated with this personality dimension, which has also been referred in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 79(1–3), 149–160.
to as the ‘‘destructiveness of self-criticism’’ (Blatt, 1995). A first lim- Giovanni, A. D., Carla, G., Enrica, M., Federico, A., Maria, Z., & Secondo, F. (2011).
itation of this study is the cross-sectional design, which limits the Eating disorders and major depression: Role of anger and personality.
Depression Research and Treatment, 2011, 194732.
ability to draw causal conclusions. It also does not allow testing po- Hambleton, R. K. (1994). Guidelines for adapting educational and psychological
tential reciprocal relations between personality dimensions and an- tests: A progress report. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 10,
ger regulation strategies. Further, estimates of internal consistency 229–244.
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the
for the anger-in and anger-out were relatively low which may have new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420 [article].
influenced results. As the results of this study suggest that personal- Hoyle, R. H., & Smith, G. T. (1994). Formulating clinical research hypotheses as
ity, and Dependency in particular, might be associated with impres- structural equation models. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(3),
429–440 [article].
sion management, future studies should use other methods to assess Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
anger and anger regulation (e.g., observation, implicit measures). analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
We also did not consider the subdimensions of dependency. Recent Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55 [article].
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York
work has distinguished between different subdimensions of Depen- (N.Y.): Guilford.
dency such as destructive overdependence, dysfunctional detach- Koestner, R., Zuroff, D. C., & Powers, T. A. (1991). Family origins of adolescent self-
ment and healthy mature dependency (Bornstein, 1992). Future criticism and its continuity into adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
100(2), 191–197.
studies should investigate the relation between these subtypes of
Koh, K. B., Kim, D. K., Kim, S. Y., & Park, J. K. (2005). The relation between anger
dependency and anger regulation strategies. Finally, this study expression, depression, and somatic symptoms in depressive disorders and
was conducted in a community sample, and hence replication in Somatoform disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(4), 485–491.
clinical samples is needed to investigate whether these findings Luyten, P., & Blatt, S. J. (2011). Integrating theory-driven and empirically-derived
models of personality development and psychopathology: A proposal for DSM
may be generalized to clinical samples. Despite these limitations, V. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 52–68.
overall, this study suggests considerable variability in the relation- Luyten, P., & Blatt, S. J. (2012). Psychodynamic treatment of depression. Psychiatric
ships among the personality dimensions of Dependency and Self- Clinics of North America, 35(1), 111–129.
Luyten, P., & Blatt, S. J. (2013). Relatedness and self-definition in normal and
Criticism and severity of depression that is influenced by anger reg- disrupted personality development: Retrospect and prospect. American
ulation strategies. Therefore, both in research and treatment, careful Psychologist 68(3), 2013, 172–183.
attention to personality issues linked with Dependency and Self- Luyten, P., Corveleyn, J., & Blatt, S. J. (1997). Dutch version of the depressive
experiences questionnaire. Leuven, Belgium: University of Leuven.
Criticism may both further our insight into the nature of the rela- Luyten, P., Fonagy, P., Lemma, A., & Target, M. (2012). Depression. In A. Bateman & P.
tionship between anger and depression and improve the treatment Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of Mentalizing in mental health practice (pp. 385–417).
of depression. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
McHugh, T., Forbes, D., Bates, G., Hopwood, M., & Creamer, M. (2012). Anger in
PTSD: Is there a need for a concept of PTSD-related posttraumatic anger?
Clinical Psychology Review, 32(2), 93–104.
References Newman, J. L., Fuqua, D. R., Gray, E. A., & Simpson, D. B. (2006). Gender differences in
the relationship of anger and depression in a clinical sample. Journal of
Baeg, S., Wang, S. K., Chee, I. S., Kim, S. Y., & Kim, J. L. (2011). Anger in elderly Counseling and Development, 84(2), 157–162.
patients with depressive disorders. Psychiatry Investigation, 8(3), 186–193. Newman, J. L., Gray, E. A., & Fuqua, D. R. (1999). Sex differences in the relationship of
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Beck depression inventory manual. San Antonio, TX: anger and depression: An empirical study. Journal of Counseling and
The Psychological Corporation. Development, 77(2), 198–203.
Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism – Implications for the Painuly, N. P., Grover, S., Gupta, N., & Mattoo, S. K. (2011a). Prevalence of anger
treatment of depression. American Psychologist, 50(12), 1003–1020. attacks in depressive and anxiety disorders: Implications for their construct?
Blatt, S. J. (2004). Experiences of depression: Theoretical, research and clinical Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 65(2), 165–174.
perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Painuly, N. P., Grover, S., Mattoo, S. K., & Gupta, N. (2011b). Anger attacks in
Blatt, S. J. (2008). Polarities of experience: Relatedness and self-definition in personality obsessive compulsive disorder. Industrial Psychiatry Journal, 20(2), 115–119.
development, psychopathology, and the therapeutic process. Washington, DC: Parker, G. (2000). Classifying depression: Should paradigms lost be regained? The
American Psychological Association Press. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(8), 1195–1203.
Blatt, S. J., D’Afflitti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976). Experiences of depression in Perlis, R. H., Smoller, J. W., Fava, M., Rosenbaum, J. F., Nierenberg, A. A., & Sachs,
normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(4), 383–389. G. S. (2004). The prevalence and clinical correlates of anger attacks during
Bornstein, R. F. (1992). The dependent personality: Developmental, social, and depressive episodes in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 79(1–3),
clinical perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 3–23. 291–295.
Bruehl, S., Liu, X., Burns, J. W., Chont, M., & Jamison, R. N. (2012). Associations between Rude, S. S., Chrisman, J. G., Denmark, A. B., & Maestas, K. L. (2012). Expression of
daily chronic pain intensity, daily anger expression, and trait anger expressiveness: direct anger and hostility predict depression symptoms in formerly depressed
An ecological momentary assessment study. Pain, 153(12), 2352–2358. women. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science-Revue Canadienne Des Sciences
Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Du Comportement, 44(3), 200–209.
Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah: Erlbaum. Santor, D. A., & Zuroff, D. C. (1997). Interpersonal responses to threats to status and
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and interpersonal relatedness: Effects of dependency and self-criticism. British
depression. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 521–541.
Constantino, M. J. et al. (2008). Interpersonal styles of chronically depressed Sauro, M. D., Jorgensen, R. S., Larson, C. A., Frankowski, J. J., Ewart, C. K., & White, J.
outpatients: Profiles and therapeutic change. Psychotherapy, 45(4), 491–506. (2001). Sociotropic cognition moderates stress-induced cardiovascular
Ewart, C. K., Jorgensen, R. S., & Kolodner, K. B. (1998). Sociotropic cognition responsiveness in college women. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 24(5),
moderates blood pressure response to interpersonal stress in high-risk 423–439.
adolescent girls. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the Schotte, C. K., Maes, M., Cluydts, R., De Doncker, D., & Cosyns, P. (1997). Construct
International Organization of Psychophysiology, 28(2), 131–142. validity of the Beck Depression Inventory in a depressive population. Journal of
Fairchild, A. J., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2009). A general model for testing mediation and Affective Disorders, 46(2), 115–125.
moderation effects. Prevention Science, 10(2), 87–99 [article]. Spielberger, C. D. (1996). State-Trait anger expression inventory. professional manual.
Fava, M., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1998). Anger attacks in depression. Depression and Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.
Anxiety, 8(1), 59–63. Van der Ploeg, H. M., Defares, P. B., & Spielberger, C. D. (1982). Handleiding bij de
Fava, M., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1999). Anger attacks in patients with depression. The Zelf-Analyse Vragenlijst ZAV [Manual of the Dutch State-Trait Anger Scale]. Lisse,
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60(Suppl. 15), 21–24. The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Fichman, L., Koestner, R., & Zuroff, D. C. (1996). Dependency, self-criticism, and Vliegen, N., & Luyten, P. (2008). The role of dependency and self-criticism in the
perceptions of inferiority at summer camp: I’m even worse than you think. relationship between postpartum depression and anger. Personality and
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25(1), 113–126. Individual Differences, 45(1), 34–40.

You might also like