Why Good People Sometimes Do Bad Things: Motivated Reasoning and Unethical Behavior

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

TIN BEHAVIOR

CAL PERSONALITY
Bersoff / MOTIVATED
AND SOCIAL
REASONING
PSYCHOLOGY
AND UNETHI-
BULLE-

Why Good People Sometimes Do Bad Things:

Motivated Reasoning and Unethical Behavior


David M. Bersoff

Diogenes Project

The hypothesis that unethical behavior is promoted when people 1980, 1986; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986)
are able to develop and maintain a biased characterization of on social behavior. The maintenance of self-esteem or
an unethical action as being morally acceptable was tested in an self-worth has been characterized as being among the
experiment in which 120 par ticipants were overpaid for taking strongest and most persistent of human goals (Hales,
par t in a study. The variable of interest—whether the par tici- 1985). Research has even shown that it is important to
pants pointed out the overpayment—was examined in a between- individuals to live up to their internal standards of right
participants design under three sets of contrasting manipulations and wrong in the absence of external sanctioning agents
designed to affect differentially participants’ abilities to convince (Cialdini, Finch, & De Nicholas, 1990; Hales, 1985; Reis,
themselves that keeping the overpayment was acceptable. Logistic 1981). If an insult or potential insult to one’s self-image
regressions revealed a decrease in unethical behavior when cannot be avoided, then a whole series of ego-defensive
participants’ abilities to construct neutralizations for keeping processes are brought to bear. These include various forms
the overpayment were impeded. A follow-up study indicated that of motivated reasoning (Kunda, 1990), self-handicapping
these results were unlikely to have been a consequence of changes (Jones & Berglas, 1978), and verbal strategies such as
in the ethicality of keeping the money as a function of the specific accounts (Scott & Lyman, 1968). The ultimate goal of
experimental manipulations used. The influence of moral atti-
these defensive processes is to sustain a phenomenal
tudes and gender on moral behavior was also examined. Moral
experience of the self as being moral, competent, good,
attitude measures were not predictive, but males were more likely stable, and capable of choice and control (Steele, 1988).
to act unethically. Given the well-documented importance of self-esteem
protection as a social motive, it is difficult to explain how
people freely come to decide to adopt an unethical
U nethical behavior is not a rare event, nor is it preva-

lent among only certain segments of society. In a random


course of action, thereby compromising their image of
themselves as fair, moral, and honest, without experienc-
sample of 401 people drawn from a large metropolitan ing significant amounts of dissonance, anxiety, and
community in the southwest, 25% of the respondents stress. There are, nodoubt, instances in which a person’s
reported having evaded taxes in the past, a fifth had tendency to act in accordance with moral dictates, al-
stolen something worth more than $20, and more than though an important factor underlying social behavior,
half had taken an item worth less than $20 at sometime
in their lives (Grasmick &Scott, 1982). Research suggests Author’s Note: This research was done under the auspices of the

that one in three employees steals at work (Hefter, 1986), Diogenes Project (our lamps are lit and the search is on) and was

whereas the average base rate of employee theft in the funded by both the Yale University and the University of Pennsylvania

retail sector is an even higher 42% (Werner, Jones, & departments of psychology. Study 1 is based on my doctoral disserta-
tion. I would like to thank the members of my thesis advisor y committee
Steffy, 1989). In a study by a security firm, 500 shoppers at Yale—Joan Miller, Mahzarin Banaji, William McGuire, Robert Abel-

were chosen at random and followed. One in 12 was son, and Assaad Azzi—for their general guidance and specific sugges-

observed shoplifting (Farrell & Ferrara, 1985). tions. My gratitude also extends to Allison Marks for her inspired

In apparent contradiction to the preceding data, a portrayal of a laconic disbursements clerk. Correspondence concern-

major theme in social psychology has been the influence


ing this article should be addressed to David M. Bersoff, Director,

Diogenes Project, 25 Dillon Road, Woodbridge, CT 06525, e-mail:

of self-image management processes (e.g., Steele, 1988; dbersoff@yankelovich.com.

Tetlock & Manstead, 1985; Tedeschi, 1981) and self- PSPB, Vol. 25 No. 1, Januar y 1999 28-39

esteem needs (Greenwald & Breckler, 1985; Schlenker, © 1999 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

28

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 29

is overpowered by other motivators (Rest, 1983). An and behavior, such as embarrassment (Abelson, 1983).
expectation that moral considerations must always be For example, work in the area of helping behavior
given priority in social situations would fail to take into (Schwartz, 1977) suggests that a high personal cost asso-
account psychological realities (Williams, 1981). ciated with engaging in a particular helping behavior
However, desperation or the furthering of some deep may lead people to form distorted judgments regarding
personal belief seem unlikely motivators in situations the reality of the need or their personal responsibility to
involving the failure to pay parking tickets, cheating on help in a given situation. This defensive denial
income taxes, the pilfering of office supplies, and other (Schwartz, 1977) can be such that the conditions for
such minor moral indiscretions. In fact, research on prosocial moral norm activation are not met, which in
employee theft does not support the theory that workers turn, can lead to a decision not to help another, even
steal, in general, because they need the money (Murphy, among people who possess the prerequisite values and
1993). It is such relatively small, nonduress-driven social attitudes (Tyler, Orwin, & Schurer, 1982).
breaches that should be most affected by self-presenta- Although they are used to explain different kinds of
tion and self-esteem influences, and yet, these are the behavior, neutralization and defensive denial can be
types of immoral behaviors that are the most common. seen as related forms of directional, motivated reasoning
For example, three out of four shoplifters can afford to (Kunda, 1990). In a sense, both allow individuals to feel
buy the merchandise they have taken, and many are even committed to certain prosocial norms and values and, at
caught carrying enough money to pay for the lifted items the same time, free them to perform acts contrary to
(Francis, 1979). Surely, one’s self-esteem and moral in- those norms and values. Indeed, if the decision to refrain
tegrity must be worth more than some small item from from a particular unethical action is conceptualized as
a store that could be more easily bought than stolen. involving personal cost, that is, the denial to oneself of
In attempting to explain deviant behavior among the potential benefit this act might bring, then the mo-
juvenile delinquents, Sykes and Matza (1957) asserted tivating influence underlying neutralization-like think-
that most delinquents possess conventional values and ing may be seen as similar to that underlying defensive
are only able to commit delinquent acts by subscribing denial—self-interest.
to certain rationalizations that define such acts as situ- To be sure, the notion of motivated cognition is not
ationally appropriate. These rationalizations, known as new, but it seems to have been used mainly to explain
techniques of neutralization, precede deviant behavior, nonmoral phenomena such as self-serving causal attri-
making delinquency possible by neutralizing potential butions, attitude change, lack of compliance with health-
disapproval from both internal and external sources. promoting behavior, and the failure to help others,
Thus, social controls that serve to inhibit deviant behav- usually in situations involving personal cost (Kunda,
ior are rendered inoperative, and the individual is free 1990). One consistent exception is cognitive dissonance
to engage in delinquent behavior without serious self- theory, which is often studied in the context of getting
esteem damage (Sykes & Matza, 1957). subjects to lie to another person or to act contrary to
My contention is that neutralization-like thinking is their previously stated moral convictions with potentially
not unique to delinquent populations. But whereas neu- harmful consequences (see, e.g., Festinger & Carlsmith,
tralization, as originally conceived, is presented as a 1959; Goethals, Cooper & Naficy, 1979). Dissonance,
trait-like, generalized learned response pattern (Akers, however, occurs after a person has committed to a behav-
1977; Sykes & Matza, 1957), I see such thinking as being ior, usually as a result of subtle but effective social pres-
highly vulnerable to situational influences and easily sure on the part of the experimenter. Because of its post
manipulated. In essence, what I want to argue is that a hoc etiology, cognitive dissonance is not useful in ac-
redefinition or distorted construal of an unethical action counting for how people initially decide to perform
as being morally acceptable often precedes and fosters actions contrary to their own values. In fact, fear of
decisions to act in an unethical manner among people dissonance, that is, of feeling hypocritical, should be
generally. another compelling reason to avoid ethical indiscretions.
Such erroneous characterizations of actions place In general, very little work has been done on the
their performance in accord with people’s prosocial direct cognitive precursors to unethical action, especially
motivations. As a result, self-esteem protection processes among nondelinquent, noncriminal populations in-
are less likely to be activated in opposition to the action volved in spontaneous, unambiguously unethical behav-
being considered. In addition, these distortions allow ior. What people are telling themselves just before com-
people to act contrary to the dictates of their values or mitting a social breach that enables them to freely
attitudes without experiencing cognitive dissonance or behave in a way that could potentially compromise their
any of the other discomforts that have been associated self-image and contradict their ethical beliefs is largely
with perceived inconsistencies between one’s attitudes an unanswered question. For the most part, researchers

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


30 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

have examined the relationship between moral judg- criticizing someone else for using neutralizations to jus-
ment and behavior by looking for a correlation between tify a completely unrelated action outside the moral
attitudes, measured in regard to hypothetical scenarios, domain. This manipulation is somewhat analogous to
and specific behaviors performed prior or subsequent to attitude inoculation (McGuire, 1964). By inducing the
the attitude measurement session (see Blasi, 1980, for a would-be neutralizers to judge and criticize arguments
review). Such data do not address the causal question of similar to those they might have been tempted to use
how people who know the moral rules come to break themselves, they should have had a more difficult time
them in some situations. Even work that has looked at using versions of these same neutralizations to justify
unethical behavior experimentally, such as that involving their own action. In a sense, participants were inoculated
deindividuation (e.g., Zimbardo, 1970) or obedience or vaccinated against neutralization-based reasoning by
(e.g., Milgram, 1974), does not speak directly to the their prior exposure to such reasoning. I further ex-
issues I am raising because of its reliance on external pected that combinations of these manipulations would
pressure and tacit experimenter approval to get subjects have an additive effect. Specifically, I hypothesized that
to act immorally. Subjects are directly asked or even one neutralization-impeding manipulation would lead
ordered by the experimenter, for example, to administer to less unethical behavior than none, and two such
shock to another. manipulations combined in a single condition would
Representing a departure from the way unethical reduce the number of participants failing to point out
behavior has been examined in past research, an experi- the overpayment even further than one alone.
ment was performed in which participants were “acci-
dentally” overpaid for their participation in a study. The STUDY 1
dependent variable was whether the participants sponta-
neously pointed out the overpayment. Specifically, my Method

hypothesis was that participants’ behavior in this situ- PARTICIPANTS


ation would be affected by manipulating the environ-
ment surrounding the overpayment in such a way that Participants were solicited via posters advertising the
participants would have either an easier or harder time opportunity to take part in psychology experiments for
constructing and accepting biased, morally acceptable a fee of $5 per hour. From among those who volunteered
characterizations of keeping the money. to participate, only people in college or graduate school
Generally, the strategy was to make potential neutrali- who were younger than 30 years of age were recruited.
zations more or less viable or accessible to participants. A total of 120 participants comprised the final sample.
In particular, I expected participants’ ability to neutral- Twenty participants (10 men and 10 women) were ran-
ize keeping the overpayment to be differentially im- domly assigned to each of six conditions.
peded under three sets of contrasting manipulations. PROCEDURES
The first manipulation involved asking subjects explicitly
if they had received the correct payment as opposed to Participants were brought into the laboratory under
just setting the money before them to take without the pretense of being needed to pilot test a piece of
question. This should have made denial of responsibility educational computer software designed to help school
neutralizations (Sykes & Matza, 1957), based on the children learn geographical information. The software
premise that they were passive recipients of the money used was a simple game that could be set to present
and thus really did nothing wrong, less available to the either the name of a European country or its capital to
participants. The second manipulation entailed leading players who would then be asked to supply the other
subjects to believe that the overpayment was from an piece of information.
individual graduate student whom they had met person- Condition 1: Baseline. In this condition, participants

ally as opposed to from a big, faceless company. Explicitly were led to believe that the study was being sponsored
drawing a link between an action and its harmful conse- by NORDCOM INC., a (fictitious) European electronics
quences to a specific individual should have undermined company.1 After completing the consent form, partici-
denial of harm/victim neutralizations (Sykes & Matza, pants were handed a one-page information sheet con-
1957) based on the premise that the action was victimless taining a short geographical description of Norway, the
or harmless and therefore acceptable. An empirical jus- home of NORDCOM. The sheet also explained NORD-
tification for using these two particular manipulations is COM’s interest in the data being gathered and served as
presented in Study 2. The last manipulation designed to an introduction to the study. As a further introduction,
inhibit keeping the overpayment involved getting par- participants were told that the purpose of the study was
ticipants to criticize someone else for using neutraliza- to look at aspects of learning and memory as they relate
tions to justify a similar unethical action as opposed to to the recall of geographical information and to pilot test

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 31

a software package being developed for use in the disbursements office, participants were told to hand the
schools to help children learn geography. receipt to the person in the office and that she would
After having the rules of the computer quiz game give them $6.25. Thus, every participant heard the cor-
explained to them, participants were instructed to go rect amount due to them twice, once at the end of the
through all 35 countries once and to make an effort to final quiz round and once right as they were leaving to
memorize any of the capitals they missed. On complet- get paid.
ing this first round, participants’ number correct and When participants entered the disbursements office,
overall score were recorded, and they were instructed the door closed behind them and they found a young
immediately to go through the countries again. After Caucasian woman about 30 years old (a confederate)
Round 2’s scores were recorded, participants were given seated behind a desk facing away from the door and
a short questionnaire to fill out. The questionnaire was toward a computer terminal. A second desk was placed
presented to participants with the explanation that a perpendicular to the desk with the computer and be-
distraction task was required to see if their Round 2 tween the door and the confederate. As participants
versus Round 1 increase in the number of capitals cor- approached this second desk, the woman turned toward
rect (all participants got more items correct the second them. The confederate was polite and replied to ques-
time) was a long-term memory gain or a short-term tions but was not warm and did not initiate any conver-
memory gain. In this condition, the distraction task was sation. When the participant handed her the receipt, the
a short vignette about a man and woman who worked in confederate reached into a drawer and pulled out an
the same office. The woman had a crush on this man envelope full of money. Counting aloud as she spread
who obviously had no interest in her. Participants were the money out on the table surface, the confederate
asked to evaluate five rationalizations the woman used to placed a $5 bill, three singles, and a quarter, one at a
justify her belief in the man’s interest in her despite the time, before the participant. (The use of a five and three
evidence to the contrary. singles along with the confederate’s counting the money
After they completed the questionnaire, participants out should have made it obvious to even the most inat-
were told that they needed to go through the game one tentive participants that they were not receiving $6.25.)
more time but that there would be two changes. First, Without making eye contact, she placed the envelope
instead of being given the countries and asked for the back into the drawer and immediately returned to her
capitals, they would be given the capitals and asked for typing at the computer. Participants who pointed out the
the countries. This, they were told, was to see if geo- $2 error were allowed to keep the extra money under the
graphic information was stored in memory in both di- cover story that NORDCOM had just increased their
rections. Such extraneous details were included to fur- participant fees.
ther the illusion that the participants were taking part in Condition 2: Antineutralization vaccine. This condition
a real, multicondition cognitive psychology experiment. was run using the same procedure as the baseline condi-
The second difference was more central to the true tion, with the following difference. The distraction task
experiment. Participants were told that they were guar- given to participants between Rounds 2 and 3 of the
anteed $5 for their participation in the study but that computer game did not involve evaluating a woman’s
they could earn bonus money in an amount determined rationalizations for why an inattentive man might ac-
by their score in this final round. This procedure made tually harbor an interest in her. Instead, participants
the subsequent overpayment more plausible to partici- were presented with a vignette about a woman working
pants. Because theoretically everyone was earning differ- for a computer company who, against company rules,
ing amounts of money, the possibility of confusion on took a small amount (less than $5 worth) of office
the part of the person handing out the participant fees supplies home for her personal use. Participants were
was enhanced. then asked to evaluate, one at a time, each of the
After completing the final round, all participants,
following five rationalizations the woman could use to
regardless of their score, were told that they had earned justify this behavior.
a total of $6.25. They were also told that NORDCOM ran
many experiments on campus and that the company had 1. She simply could not resist the temptation to take a few
a small office in the building right behind the laboratory things when it was so easy.
where they disbursed participant fees for all of their 2. Other employees of the company also secretly took
studies. Participants were then handed an official receipt supplies home for their personal use from time to time.
with $6.25 sloppily written on it to look somewhat like 3. The company that Carol worked for made so much
money last year that the cost of the materials that she
$8.25 and a sheet telling them where to get both the wasthinking oftakingwouldamountto aninfinitesimal
results of and a full debriefing regarding the experiment. percentage of their profits and an even smaller propor-
As they were leaving the laboratory on their way to the tion of their budgeted operating expenses.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


32 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

4. Carol’s company was guilty of bribing public officials to ment, of course, would have nothing to feel upset about
get government contracts and then overcharging for regardless of whether they were debriefed. More impor-
their work by fraudulently inflating their reported costs
by as much as 200%. tant, participants who kept the overpayment also should
5. She needed them but did not want to buy them. not experience much if any discomfort if there was no
debriefing because it is assumed that they will have
In particular, participants were asked if Carol’s rationali- conceptualized their act in terms with which they feel
zation would really justify her action and to indicate on comfortable. But, if those who kept the money were fully
a set of rating scales (a) how unethical it would be for debriefed, it would be much harder for them to view
Carol to take the supplies given the justification she their action in a benign manner. They would likely feel
offered; (b) how taking the office supplies home, given that they had, in a sense, flunked some kind of moral
her justification, would reflect on Carol’s moral charac- test. Thus, the debriefing was delayed so as not to involve
ter; and (c) how tempted would they be themselves to an embarrassing, face-to-face interaction and was made
take office supplies home given the circumstances in optional. As it turned out, only nine participants re-
Carol’s justification. quested a debriefing. Two of the nine had kept the
overpayment.
Condition 3: Humanization of victim. This condition was

run exactly like the baseline condition with the following Results

differences. Instead of being sponsored by NORDCOM


INC., the study was portrayed as being the experi- TEST OF HYPOTHESIS

menter’s dissertation research. This entailed several The central hypothesis of the study was that the per-
modifications to the stimulus materials. The NORD- centage of participants keeping the overpayment would
COM company logo was removed from everything par- be influenced by altering the social environment in such
ticipants saw. The initial information sheet was also a way that participants would have either an easier or
modified. Although it still contained a geographical harder time generating and accepting neutralizations
description of Norway, the last paragraph was altered to justifying the target behavior. In this regard, it was pre-
indicate that the study the participant was taking part in dicted that the baseline condition (Condition 1), having
was being financed entirely by the experimenter’s per- no antineutralization manipulations, would lead to the
sonal funds. The final change involved the cover story most unethical behavior followed by those conditions
used to explain to participants why the experimenter was (Conditions 2, 3, and 4) with a single manipulation. The
not giving out the money himself. Participants were told conditions (Conditions 5 and 6) with two manipulations
that the study was not being run in the experimenter’s were predicted to generate the least number of partici-
usual lab and that his friend and office mate was helping pants failing to point out the overpayment. In testing
him by handing out his money for him. these predictions, the condition data were aggregated,
Condition 4: Ask. This condition was run exactly like
as indicated above, according to their number of anti-
the baseline with only one difference. After the confed- neutralization manipulations.
erate counted out the $8.25 for the participants, she These data were analyzed using a logistic regression
looked up and asked them, “Is that right?” procedure to model the effects of participant gender
and number of antineutralization manipulations on re-
Condition 5: Humanization of victim and ask. This con-
turning the participant fee overpayment. Logistic regres-
dition combined the humanize and ask manipulations. sion is the appropriate regression method to use when
Condition 6: Antineutralization vaccine and ask. This an outcome variable is binary or dichotomous. It differs
condition combined the vaccine and ask manipulations.2 from linear regression in the parametric model it em-
Contrary to the usual practice, participants were not ploys and in its underlying assumptions, both of which
automatically debriefed on completion of the experi- are more suitable for the nonnormal distribution pat-
ment. Instead, a short memo was given to participants. tern of dichotomous data. Aside from these differences,
The memo invited them to call a day or so later if they the methods employed in an analysis using logistic re-
were interested in having a full and detailed explanation gression follow the same general principles used in lin-
of the experiment. ear regression (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989).
This procedure was adopted as a workable compro- Analyses indicated that females (M = 68.3%) were
mise between two opposing ethical considerations. On more likely to point out the overpayment than were
one hand, participants certainly have the right to be fully males (M = 45.0%), p < .01, and that both the single (M =
informed regarding all experimental procedures; but on 51.7%) and paired antineutralization manipulations
the other, participation in the study would have the least (M = 82.5%) led to more ethical behavior on the part of
potential to cause stress and discomfort if there was no participants than none (M = 20.0%, p < .01 (see Table 1).
debriefing. Participants who pointed out the overpay- The gender by number of manipulations interaction

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 33

TABLE 1: Logistic Regression of Gender and Antineutralization agent’s moral character—overall M = –.98 on a scale
Manipulations on Returning the Overpayment ranging from +4 (extremely well) to –4 (extremely poorly).
Odds The overall means, when interpreted in reference to the
Variables β β/ SE Ratio original scales participants used in recording their atti-
tudes, lead to a general characterization of stealing office
Gender
Male –1.22*** –2.80 .29 supplies as being between a little and moderately unethi-
(Female = 0) cal and as reflecting a little poorly on one’s moral char-
Conditions acter. Given the minor amount of harm involved and the
Vaccine 1.28* 1.72 3.60 daily exposure people have in the media to acts of much
Humanize 1.50** 2.01 4.48
Ask 1.94*** 2.58 7.00
greater consequence, this seems like a reasonable char-
Single manipulations aggregated 1.57*** 2.45 4.81 acterization. The point of critical importance is that
Humanize/ask 3.00*** 3.62 20.11 people have not become so jaded that little indiscretions
Vaccine/ask 3.37*** 3.84 29.00 are now considered acceptable or morally neutral.
Dual manipulations aggregated 3.16*** 4.32 23.77 In terms of potential mediators of unethical behavior,
(Baseline = 0)
Constant –.89 –1.50 .41
the two questions of interest on the vaccine question-
naire were “Would Carol be justified in taking a small
NOTE: The odds ratio indicates the likelihood of returning the over- amount (less than $5 worth) of these supplies home for
payment in comparison to the base group. For example, in this analysis, personal use?” and “How tempted would you, yourself,
males were .29 times (or a little less than a third) as likely to return the
money as were females. be to take office supplies home for personal use?” Per-
*p < .1. **p < .05. ***p < .01. haps differing attitudes toward what justifies small acts
of theft or the temptation to indulge in such behavior
may have differentiated those who kept the money from
term did not yield a significant coefficient nor did it those who tried to return the overpayment. Justification
significantly enhance the fit of the model. A follow-up scores (overall M = .98) were calculated by giving partici-
regression using the single manipulation conditions as pants one point for each of the five rationalizations they
the comparison group further indicated that two ma- felt justified Carol’s taking the supplies. Temptation
nipulations were significantly more effective in reducing scores—overall M = 2.56 on a scale ranging from 0 (not
unethical behavior than one alone (β = 1.59, p < .01). at all) to 5 ( I would definitely take the supplies)—were

Analyses examining the effectiveness of the individual calculated by taking the mean of participants’ answers to
conditions were also done. The percentage of partici- the temptation question across each of the five rationali-
pants who returned the overpayment by condition is zations. A logistic regression examining the relationship
presented in Table 2. A logistic regression revealed that, between both of these variables and returning the over-
of the five experimental conditions, only the anti- payment revealed that, whereas justification scores were
neutralization vaccine failed to reduce reliably keeping not significantly related to the outcome measure, self-
the overpayment relative to the baseline condition, al- reported temptation was a reliable predictor such that
though it did manifest a trend in that direction (see participants with higher temptation scores were less
Table 1). Follow-up logistic regressions further indicated likely to return the money (β = –.70, p < .05). It should
that no single manipulation alone was significantly more be noted, however, that the temptation effect did not
effective than any other single manipulation. In addi- eliminate but rather coexisted with a significant vaccine
tion, neither of the two manipulation combinations was versus vaccine/ask condition effect (β = –2.57, p < .01).
found to be significantly more effective than the other.
Discussion

CONTROL ANALYSES
The results obtained demonstrate the effectiveness of
Several control analyses also were done. The re- the experimental manipulations in reducing unethical
sponses of the 40 participants (20 men and 20 women) behavior. Specifically, significant increases in returning
who filled out the antineutralization vaccine question- the participant fee overpayment were found as the num-
naire involving a woman stealing office supplies were ber of antineutralization manipulations increased from
analyzed to get some indication of participants’ attitudes none to one and again from one to two. Furthermore,
toward an act similar in magnitude to keeping a $2 this manipulation effect was not mediated by any of the
overpayment. In response to each of the five rationaliza- individual difference variables measured.
tions presented, participants saw the act of taking the Examination of the data for the six individual condi-
office supplies as both unethical—overall M = –1.43 on tions speaks to the integrity of the experimental design
a scale ranging from –4 (extremely unethical) to +4 (ex- and results. The high incidence of failing to point out
tremely ethical)—and as reflecting negatively on the the overpayment across both genders in the baseline

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


34 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 2: Percentage of Participants Who Returned the Overpayment by Condition

Condition

Baseline Vaccine Humanize Ask Humanize/Ask Vaccine/Ask


Total Sample (N = 120) 20 45 50 60 80 85

Males only (n = 60) 10 30 30 40 80 80


Females only (n = 60) 30 60 70 80 80 90

condition indicates that the substance and focus of the it seems to be, but these two conceptions are actually
experiment was not obvious to participants, who most radically different. Neutralization presumes a motivated
likely would not have taken the money if they suspected distortion or a biased analysis of a situation. For example,
the true nature of the experiment. Similarly, the uni- in this experiment, it was assumed that underlying and
formly low incidence of unethical behavior in the two influencing participants’ judgments that keeping the
dual manipulation conditions indicates that participants money was permissible was the wish or hope that the act
were not acting mindlessly. If participants were not aware did not amount to stealing. If the act was not stealing,
of both the overpayment and that keeping the money then participants could keep the money without guilt or
was a questionable or unacceptable act, then the percent- self-recrimination.
age of participants failing to point out the extra money The utilitarian decision conception does not involve
would have been uniformly high across all conditions. motivated cognition or the attendant bias and distortion.
Although the results of this study are fully compatible It implies that people plainly recognize the immorality
with and suggestive of a motivated cognition conception inherent in the actions that they are considering but,
of unethical behavior, the mechanism itself can only be under certain circumstances, are willing to accept that
inferred from the data because participants’ thoughts price to achieve a gain. The only judgment-influencing
and judgments at the time of their deciding whether to motivation they are experiencing, if any, is to be accurate
keep or return the money were not directly measured. in their assessments.
Thus, an alternative mechanism may have mediated the The key to adjudicating between these two competing
influence of the experimental manipulations on the explanations for the Study 1 results is to remove partici-
participants’ behavior. In particular, an important, po- pants’ motivation to see the act of keeping the overpay-
tential alternative hypothesis is that the same situational ment as ethical. Under the neutralization model, with-
elements that made the act harder to neutralize also out some incentive, participants should no longer
made the act objectively more unethical. The pattern of engage in a biased analysis of the situation. As a result,
results, then, may simply have been a function of a they should see the act of keeping the money as equally
progressively greater amount of unethicality in the act of unethical across all six conditions. But under a utilitarian
keeping the money going from the baseline through the decision model, removing the monetary motivation
single manipulation and finally to the dual-manipulation should not change participants’ perceptions of the ethi-
conditions. Participants stole more in the baseline than cality of keeping the overpayment because their percep-
in the other conditions according to this line of reason- tions were not assumed to be originally biased by the
ing, because it was less unethical to keep the money in potential gain. As a consequence, participants should
that context and the less unethical an action is the more continue to see the act of keeping the money as differ-
people are likely to perform it, all else being equal. entially unethical across the conditions in a pattern
This explanation implies that unethical behavior similar to that reflected in the behavioral outcome data.
stems from a utilitarian-type decision procedure. Specifi- Specifically, those conditions that led to the most partici-
cally, the implication is that people know full well that pants keeping the money in Study 1 should also be the
what they are contemplating is wrong to some Degree X, ones in which participants evaluate the act as the least
but they are willing to accept this insult to their self-image unethical.
in return for potential Gain Y, at least up to a point. This There are, of course, some important psychological
point is where Gain Y is no longer seen as adequately differences between acting within and commenting on
compensating for the self-esteem damage caused by a moral situation (Saltzstein, 1994) aside from the ab-
knowingly performing an act with an X level of immorality. sence of the impetus for motivated cognition. Thus, it
The neutralization and utilitarian decision concep- would be overly stringent to demand a correspondence
tions of unethical behavior may appear to be similar in in the absolute levels of keeping the overpayment in
that they both predict that a person is more likely to Study 1 and approval of such behavior on the part of an
commit an act, all else being the same, the less unethical equal number of participants simply reporting their

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 35

moral perceptions. But if some sort of unbiased moral out the overpayment); and (d) how easily they could ration-
interpretation of the act of keeping the money was alize to themselves failing to point out an overpayment
driving the results of Study 1, it is not unreasonable to in a situation like this—ratings could range from 0 (I
expect measures of moral judgment to manifest a data could not rationalize keeping the overpayment) to +5 (ex-

pattern or shape approximating the pattern of those tremely). Those participants who indicated that they could

Study 1 results. The standard method for measuring rationalize failing to point out the overpayment (i.e.,
moral attitudes and perceptions is getting people to participants who gave a nonzero response to the pre-
react to vignettes. A follow-up experiment based on this vious question) were asked to supply the two rationaliza-
logic was performed and is reported below. tions that they would be most likely to use.
Results
STUDY 2
This study was a vignette version of the first study. As in Study 1, the condition data were aggregated
Because participants in this study were not in a position according to their number of antineutralization manipu-
to gain monetarily from their judgments, their responses lations. The participants’ dichotomous judgments of
were assumed to represent an unbiased, normative whether the person in the vignette was justified in failing
evaluation of the act of keeping the overpayment within to point out the overpayment were analyzed using a
the context of each of the six conditions used in the logistic regression procedure. This analysis did not pro-
previous study. duce any significant results. Participants considered
keeping the overpayment to be equally unjustified across
Method
all three antineutralization manipulation conditions
Ten males and 10 females were randomly assigned to (M = 80.0%).
one of the six conditions used in Study 1. Participants Consistent with the justification data, one-way ANO-
who were randomized into conditions involving the anti- VAs performed on participants’ ethicality and moral
neutralization vaccine were first given the stealing office character ratings also failed to produce any significant
supplies version of the rationalization questionnaire to results. In general, participants rated keeping the money
fill out while the remaining participants first completed as between a little and moderately unethical (M = –1.72)
the baseline office crush version of the questionnaire. and as reflecting a little poorly on one’s moral character
After filling out the rationalization questionnaire, par- (M = –0.98). In addition, the temptation and the ease of
ticipants were given a veridical, written description of rationalization data were also nonsignificant. Partici-
what Study 1 participants in the condition correspond- pants, across conditions, saw keeping the money as be-
ing to the one they were randomized into saw and tween somewhat and moderately tempting (M = 2.42) and
experienced. The description was prefaced with a para- as between somewhat and moderately easy to rationalize
graph that stated that the events to be described actually (M = 2.38).
happened at the participant’s university last year. Partici- In terms of the specific rationalizations that partici-
pants were also told that the identity and gender of the pants claimed that they would be most likely to use, 60%
person involved would be concealed, although they were of the responses revolved around two issues. The first
told that person who disbursed the subject fees was (M = 36%) was that the loss was at least partially the
female. In all cases, the account ended with this phrase: victim’s own fault (denial of responsibility), for example
The subject “reached down, picked up all $8.25, and “The woman counting out the money was careless,” “The
walked out of the office.” experimenter should have written the receipt more
Participants were then asked a series of six questions. clearly,” and “It is the company’s responsibility to make
First they were asked to answer yes or no as to whether the sure that things like this don’t happen.” The second issue
person in the description was justified in failing to point (M = 24%) was that the $2 loss itself did not represent a
out the $2 overpayment. Next, participants were in- great deal of harm, especially for a large corporation
structed to indicate on a set of rating scales (a) how (denial of harm). Albeit in a somewhat post hoc fashion,
unethical it was to fail to point out the $2 overpayment— these data thus serve as an empirical justification for the
ratings could range from –4 (extremely unethical) to +4 ask and humanize antineutralization manipulations
(extremely ethical) with a neutral midpoint; (b) how failing used in Study 1, because these were exactly the types of
to point out the $2 overpayment reflected on the per- rationalizations those manipulations were designed to
son’s moral character—ratings could range from +4 (ex- counter.
tremely well) to –4 (extremely poorly) with a neutral midpoint;
Discussion
(c) how tempted they themselves would be not to point
out the overpayment in this situation—ratings could The Study 2 evaluation data support a neutralization-
range from 0 (not at all) to +5 (I would definitely not point based approach over a utilitarian decision approach to

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


36 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

explaining the unethical behavior of the participants in founded to assume that people, at certain times, do not
Study 1. Specifically, the results indicate that normative weigh potential costs and benefits when making deci-
perceptions of the act of keeping the $2 overpayment sions about engaging in unethical or criminal behavior
most likely did not mediate participants’ behavior in (Cornish & Clarke, 1986). Utilitarian-based reasoning
Study 1. The presence or absence of antineutralization probably underlies, in part, the deterrence effect of
manipulations does not appear to have had any influ- alarms and other antitheft measures that counterbal-
ence on the perceived justifiability of keeping the money. ance a potential gain with an increased likelihood of
In addition, there were no significant differences in being caught. But it appears that such reasoning plays
participants’ perceptions of how unethical or image less of a role in decisions to commit everyday peccadil-
damaging keeping the money was as a function of the loes such as the one operationalized in Study 1, in which
Study 1 manipulations. Finally, there were absolutely no the chance of detection is low and the potential gain
discernable gender differences in this study, whereas does not seem to be worth the potential self-image
Study 1 yielded a clear main effect for gender. These damage.
data, however, should not be construed as implying that
the unethicality of an act as a rule is independent of the GENERAL DISCUSSION
context in which it occurs. Rather, these results are In Study 1, manipulations designed to affect the ac-
presented solely to support the claim that the specific cessibility of neutralizing cognitions without systemati-
contextual manipulations used here did not signifi- cally altering the experimental environment in terms of
cantly affect the perceived ethicality of keeping the $2 (a) the presence of external pressure (e.g., obedience or
overpayment. conformity demands); (b) the risk or penalty of being
The contexts created by the various conditions used caught; (c) the amount of money involved; or (d) the
in Study 1 appear to have merely made potential neu- characteristics of the participants (e.g., their need or
tralizations more or less available. This underscores the desire for money) were found to have a direct and
motivated aspect of neutralization-like thinking. The marked impact on the occurrence of unethical behavior.
Study 1 participants, having a $2 gain hanging in the The mechanism underlying their influence on behavior,
balance, were motivated to search for potential justifica- I argue, is that the manipulations disrupted participants’
tions and not motivated to question or critically evaluate neutralization-oriented motivated cognitions, thus mak-
any argument they could concoct for why keeping the ing it harder for participants to deceive themselves that
overpayment was acceptable behavior (Bach, 1981; the act of keeping the overpayment was ethically accept-
Kunda, 1990). Thus, the more neutralizations made able. Study 2 lent further support to this contention by
available to them, the more likely they were to keep the demonstrating that the Study 1 decreases in taking the
money. The Study 2 participants had nothing to gain as overpayment were unlikely to have been mediated by
a result of their evaluations of the act. As a consequence, objective increases in the magnitude of the moral breach
they were not motivated to seek neutralizations. This is as a function of the contexts created by the particular
reflected in the fact that they saw failing to return the manipulations used.
overpayment as equally unjustified across all condi- In both between- and within-participant comparisons,
tions regardless of the relative availability of potential antistealing attitudes were not found to be reliable pre-
neutralizations. dictors of actual behavior. In Study 1, among those
Although the ease of rationalization question data participants whose attitudes toward small acts of theft
also did not reflect the Study 1 pattern of results, it is still from a large company were measured, no significant
possible that some form of neutralization or motivated relationship between the attitudes expressed and sub-
cognition was mediating participants’ behavior in the sequent behavior was found. In Study 2, moral evalu-
previous study. Study 2 participants may simply have ations of the specific act examined in Study 1 did not
been unaware of how the situational factors depicted reflect the pattern of behavioral results obtained in that
would have influenced their ability to rationalize had study. Viewed together, these findings along with pre-
they been in the actual situation (Nisbett & Bellows, vious work showing moral attitude-behavior discontinu-
1977; Wilson & Nisbett, 1978). Alternatively, participants ity (see, e.g., Hartshorne & May, 1928; Thoma & Rest,
may have interpreted the rationalization question, given 1986; see also Blasi, 1980, for a general review), suggest
the context created by the previous questions, as asking that measurements of moral attitudes may not be as
another version of “How wrong or bad do you think the important to predicting behavior as is information re-
act is?” garding how those attitudes are applied to specific situ-
Despite the indications that a utilitarian decision- ations by agents acting within those situations.
making approach is not the best perspective from which Some researchers working in the area of attitudes and
to understand the results of Study 1, it would be un- behavior might disagree with this position, arguing that

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 37

attitude-behavior discrepancies such as those found here this regard (Ward & Beck, 1990). Research also indicates
are simply due to measurement problems. In particular, that men are bigger risk takers and more impulsive than
it could be asserted that the correspondence between are women (MacDonald, 1988; Zuckerman, 1979). Ten-
the context in which moral behavior and the context in dencies such as these, to deny or underestimate the
which moral attitudes were measured in both Studies 1 possible negative consequences of their actions and to
and 2 was not perfect (although Study 2 was somewhat act without careful thought, may make men more likely
better in this regard) (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977) and that to engage in unethical behavior. Alternatively, some re-
this imperfect correspondence is what led to the insig- search has indicated that women may possess a greater
nificant attitude-behavior relationships found. Although empathy and sensitivity to the welfare and feelings of
this may be true at some level, it is also true that paper- others (Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983). These predisposi-
and-pencil attitude measures in general are incapable of tions may make women less likely to engage in unethical
engendering all of the complex psychological reactions behavior.
attendant with actually being in a situation. As a conse- The precedent for gender differences in unethical
quence, moral attitude surveys and vignette studies can behavior aside, it is also possible that several aspects of
only supply limited insight into moral behavior. It is this study may have exaggerated or even given rise to the
unfortunate that such studies have traditionally been the gender effect found. In particular, the person making
sine qua non of morality research. the overpayment was female across all conditions. Per-
This conclusion, I would note, differs from the more haps people are less likely to steal from someone of their
extreme position that moral attitudes do not influence own sex. In addition, the protagonist in the antineutrali-
behavior. Research has demonstrated that moral values zation vaccine task was female. This may have made at
are related to behavior via their influence on intentions least the conditions involving the antineutralization vac-
(Harrison, 1995; Pomazal & Jaccard, 1976). But to the cine more immediate to and thus effective with females.
extent that motivated cognition leads an agent to erro- Despite the high levels of unethical behavior found in
neously perceive that a particular moral attitude—for Study 1 among participants of both genders, I would
example, stealing is wrong—is irrelevant to a given situ- suggest that self-serving moral evaluations should not be
ation, that attitude simply will not be in a position to viewed as an inevitable consequence of any conflict be-
influence intention and ultimately behavior (Zanna tween an individual’s selfish interest and the dictates of
& Fazio, 1982). It is likely, however, that some other his or her moral principles. The neutralizations that
attitude—for example, it is okay to take something that serve as the basis of these biased evaluations will facilitate
is not yours if no one is harmed—will be activated in such unethical behavior only to the extent the neutralizer
situations and will, as a consequence, influence the believes, in good faith, that his or her justificatory rea-
agent’s behavior. soning is valid. Even people motivated to reach a particu-
Men pointed out the overpayment significantly less lar conclusion still manifest an illusion of objectivity,
frequently than did women. The implication of these attempting to construct a justification for their actions
data is that women act unethically less frequently than that would persuade a dispassionate observer (Kunda,
do men.3 This is not the first study to yield sex differences 1990). The flaw inherent in neutralizations is not that
in cheating and dishonesty behavior (see, e.g., Eisen, they appeal to principles without merit but rather that
1972; Ward, 1986). Such findings are not easily inter- they overextend the use of a valid justification into a
preted. The majority of moral reasoning research has context or situation that is not recognized by society at
tended to find weak or nonexistent gender effects (see large as a valid application of such a justification (Sykes
reviews by Thomas, 1986; Walker, 1984). Consistent with & Matza, 1957). Neutralizations exploit the implicitly
this trend, Study 2 also failed to uncover any gender sanctioned flexibility in the normative rules and customs
differences in moral perceptions. This null finding is of a society (Williams, 1951). However, the less moral
even more striking given that participants were asked to ambiguity there is surrounding a situation, the less lati-
evaluate the very act that yielded a significant gender- tude an agent has in negotiating reality in such a way as
based behavioral difference in Study 1. Such evidence to provide justification for an unethical action.
would seem to contra-indicate explanations of the pre- In addition, the Study 1 finding of a 17.5% rate of
sent gender findings based on differences in moral atti- failure to return the money in the dual-manipulation
tudes or judgment. conditions versus 80% in the baseline condition indi-
In the past, sex differences in unethical behavior have cates that the neutralization process and the resultant
been explained using sex-role socialization theory unethical behavior are susceptible to moderately subtle
(Ward, 1986). This theory suggests that women are more situational influences and are fairly easily disrupted. This
honest because they are socialized to obey rules and is important to note because a common response to
norms, whereas men’s socialization is less stringent in unethical behavior, at least among employees, is for

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


38 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

management to increase random monitoring and Cornish, D. B., & Clarke, R. V. (Eds.). (1986). The reasoning criminal:
Rational choice perspectives on offending. New York: Springer-Verlag.
searches (Crossen, 1993). The problem is that such Crossen, B. R. (1993). Managing employee unethical behavior without
approaches to theft reduction entail many thorny ethi- invading individual privacy. Journal of Business and Psychology, 8(2),
cal, legal, and morale issues. In contrast, the types of 227-243.
Eisen, M. (1972). Characteristic self-esteem, sex, and resistance to
manipulations used in this study can reduce unethical temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(1), 68-72.
behavior without the problems associated with coercive Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and
approaches. related capacities. Psychological Bulletin , 94, 100-131.
The motivated cognition view of unethical behavior Farrell, K. L., & Ferrara, J. A. (1985). Shoplifting. New York: Praeger.
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of
developed and tested here does not deny people’s sense forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58,
of themselves as being moral, honest, and as possessing 203-210.
integrity. Nor does it deny the motivation people feel to Francis, D. B. (1979). Shoplifting: The crime ever ybody pays for. New York:
protect their self-image and to act in a prosocial manner. Elsevier/Nelson Books.
Goethals, G. R., Cooper, J., & Naficy, A. (1979). Role of foreseen,
It does, however, suggest that at least in situations involv- foreseeable, and unforeseen behavioral consequences in the
ing relatively minor acts of social deviance, people com- arousal of cognitive dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 1179-1185.
monly distort the moral implications of their desired
Grasmick, H. G., & Scott, W. J. (1982). Tax evasion and mechanisms of
behavioral response and end up acting contrary to these social control: A comparison with grand and petty theft. Journal of
motivations. On the positive side, gaining a fuller under- Economic Psychology, 2, 213-230.

standing of the cognitions that facilitate unethical behav- Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and
consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management
ior may suggest effective and generally applicable strate- theory. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 21-62).
gies for reducing the acts of theft, cheating, and lying in New York: Springer-Verlag.
which people too often indulge. Greenwald, A. G., & Breckler, S. J. (1985). To whom is the self-pre-
sented? In B. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 126-45).
New York: McGraw-Hill.
NOTES Hales, S. (1985). The inadvertent rediscovery of self in social psychol-
ogy. Journal for the Theor y of Social Behavior, 15(3), 237-282.
1. Using a foreign instead of a domestic company may have served
Harrison, D. A. (1995). Volunteer motivation and attendance deci-
not only to dehumanize the potential victim of the $2 theft but also to
sions: Competitive theory testing in multiple samples from a home-
create an in-group/out-group situation in the minds of the partici-
less shelter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(3), 371-385.
pants. If this did occur, it would only serve to strengthen the manipu-
lation because out-group members are easier to depersonalize and thus Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character:
Vol. I. Studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan.
are more likely to be perceived as being less worthy of ethical treatment
(Tyler & Lind, 1990). Hefter, R. (1986). The crippling crime. Security World, 23, 36-38.
2. The antineutralization vaccine and the humanization of victim Hosmer, D.W. Jr., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New
manipulations were not combined because the vaccine questionnaire York: John Wiley.
involved stealing from a company, whereas the humanization manipu- Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self
lation entailed making an individual and not a corporation the poten- through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and
tial theft victim. Altering the vaccine to reflect this change in victim the role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
would have substantially compromised the cross-condition compara- tin , 4(2), 200-206.
bility of the vaccine manipulation. Keeping the vaccine the same would Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological
have rendered it less similar to the specific situation the participant was Bulletin, 108(3), 480-498.
facing and, as a consequence, potentially less effective. This too would MacDonald, K. B. (1988). Social and personality development: An evolution-
have compromised cross-condition comparisons. ar y synthesis. New York: Plenum.
3. Visual inspection of the data would seem to indicate that gender McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion. In L. Berk-
interacted with number of manipulations, but statistical analyses clearly owitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 192-
indicated gender to be a simple main effect. The small gender differ- 229). New York: Academic Press.
ences in the baseline and dual manipulation conditions were due most Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York:
likely to a floor and a ceiling effect respectively. Harper & Row.
Murphy, K. R. (1993). Honesty in the workplace . Pacific Grove, California:
Brooks/Cole.
REFERENCES Nisbett, R. E., & Bellows, N. (1977). Verbal reports about causal influ-
Abelson, R. P. (1983). Whatever became of consistency theory? Person- ences on social judgments: Private access versus public theories.
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9 , 37-54. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (9), 613-624.

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theo- Pomazal, R. J., & Jaccard, J. J. (1976). An informational approach to
retical analysis and review of the empirical research. Psychological altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(3),
Bulletin, 84, 888-918. 317-326.
Akers, R. I. (1977). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach. Belmont, Reis, H. J. (1981). Self-presentation and distributive justice. In J. T.
CA: Wadsworth. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression management theor y and social psychological
Bach, K. (1981). An analysis of self-deception. Philosophy and Pheno- research (pp. 269-291). New York: Academic Press.

menological Research, 41, 351-370. Rest, J. R. (1983). Morality. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Manual of child psychology
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical (4th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 556-629). New York: John Wiley.
review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1-45. Saltzstein, H. (1994). The relation between moral judgment and be-
Cialdini, R. B., Finch, J. F., & De Nicholas, M. E. (1990). Strategic havior: A social-cognitive and decision-making analysis. Human
self-presentation: The indirect route. In M. J. Cody & M. L. Development , 37, 299-312.
McLaughlin (Eds.), The psychology of tactical communication (pp. 194- Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, social
206). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. identity, and interpersonal relations. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015


Bersoff / MOTIVATED REASONING AND UNETHICAL BEHAVIOR 39

Schlenker, B. R. (1986). Self-identification: Toward an integration of Walker, L. J. (1984). Sex differences in the development of moral
the public and private self. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and reasoning: A critical review. Child Development, 55, 677-691.
private self (pp. 21-62). New York: Springer-Verlag. Ward, D. A. (1986) Self-esteem and dishonest behavior revisited. Jour-
Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berk- nal of Social Psychology, 126(6), 709-713.
owitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10). New Ward, D. A., & Beck, W. L. (1990). Gender and dishonesty. Journal of
York: Academic Press. Social Psychology, 130(3), 333-339.
Scott, M. B., & Lyman, S. M. (1968). Accounts. American Sociological Werner, S. H., Jones, J. W., & Steffy, B. D. (1989). The relation between
Review, 33, 46-62.
intelligence, honesty, and theft admissions. Educational and Psycho-
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the logical Measurement , 49, 921-927.
integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental Williams, B. (1981). Moral luck. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261-302). New York: Academic Press.
Press.
Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization. Ameri-
can Sociological Review, 41(5), 664-670.
Williams, R. Jr. (1951). American society. New York: Knopf.
Tedeschi, J. T. (Ed.). (1981). Impression management and social psychologi- Wilson, T. D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1978). The accuracy of verbal reports
cal research. New York: Academic Press.
about the effects of stimuli on evaluations and behavior. Social
Psychology, 41(2), 118-131.
Tetlock, P. E., & Manstead, A.S.R. (1985). Impression management
versus intrapsychic explanations in social psychology: A useful Zanna, M. P., & Fazio, R. H. (1982). The attitude-behavior relation:
dichotomy? Psychological Review, 92, 59-77. Moving toward a third generation of research. In M. P. Zanna, E. T.
Thomas, S. J., & Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral judgment, behavior, decision- Higgins, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Consistency in social behavior: The
Ontario symposium (Vol. 2, pp. 283-301). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
making, and attitudes. In J. R. Rest (Ed.), Moral development: Ad-
vances in theor y and research (pp. 133-175). New York: Praeger.
Erlbaum.
Thomas, S. J. (1986). Estimating gender differences in comprehension Zimbardo, P. G. (1970). The human choice: Individuation, impulse
and preference of moral issues. Developmental Review , 6, 165-180. and chaos. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska symposium
on motivation, 1969 (Vol. 16). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1990). Intrinsic versus community-based
justice models: When does group membership matter? Journal of Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of
Social Issues, 46 (1), 83-94. arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tyler, T. R., Orwin, R., & Schurer, L. (1982). Defensive denial and high
cost prosocial behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 3(4), Recieved December 8, 1996
267-281. Revision Accepted January 21, 1998

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 8, 2015

You might also like