Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

PSYCHOPATHY

THEORY, MEASUREMENT, AND TREATMENT

ANH VIEN
ANTHONY R. BEECH
University of Birmingham

The purpose of this article is to link empirical literature to the theoretical


background of the concept of psychopathy and the impact that this has had on the
development of treatment and intervention procedures for psychopathic offenders.
This article begins with a discussion of the different theories of psychopathy, which
leads into considerations of different developmental pathways of psychopathy in
the individual. The discussion will then lead on to the psychometrics and mea-
surement tools used to assess psychopathy in the individual. The measurement sec-
tion will primarily be focused on Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (PCL-R),
as this is the most frequently used and validated measure of psychopathy. The rela-
tionship between psychopathy and different types of crime is also discussed. The
final section of the article considers the treatment and interventions that are avail-
able to psychopathic offenders and the implications this has for future research.

Key words: psychopathy; theory; measurement; the Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (PCL-R); treatment

INTRODUCTION exactly what it is. The only aspect that has been
agreed on is that psychopathy is an integration
The most obvious expression of psychopathy of motivational dispositions, developmental
is criminal violation of society’s rules (Hare, factors, individual differences, and mental
1999). Hare (1995) estimated that 15% to 20% of health (Sloan, 1997). Although the evolution of
all prisoners are psychopaths and that these psychopathy as a formal clinical disorder began
individuals account for a large proportion of more than a century ago, it is only recently that
serious crimes involving violence in relation to scientifically sound psychometric procedures
treatment and the implications of this for future for its assessment have become available
research. Researchers theorizing about psy- making it possible to measure psychopathy as a
chopathy have attempted to provide a compre- construct (Hare, 1996). The construct of psy-
hensive and integrated general definition of the chopathy is proving to be particularly useful in
concept; however, there is little agreement of the Criminal Justice System, where it has

AUTHORS’ NOTE: Correspondence concerning this article should be directed to Contact: Anh Vien, University of Birmingham, School
of Psychology, Egdbaston Park Road, Egdbaston, UK; e-mail: axv396@bham.ac.uk
TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 7, No. 3, July 2006 155-174
DOI: 10.1177/1524838006288929
© 2006 Sage Publications
155

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


156 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

Cleckley in the The Researchers


KEY POINTS OF THE RESEARCH REVIEW Mask of Sanity (1941) was theorizing about
the first to look exten- psychopathy have
• The current trend in psychopathy research is the
investigation of neurological underpinnings influ- sively at the personality attempted to provide a
encing the behavior of psychopaths. This has been traits of the psychopath. comprehensive and
developed further as technology has advanced. Cleckley considered psy- integrated
Behaviors that psychopaths engage in are now chopathy as concealed general definition
attributed to specific cortical dysfunctions within psychosis and is only pri- of the concept;
the brain.
• The profile of a psychopath is entrenched with
marily revealed through however, there
deficits in moral reasoning, fear and anxiety pro- the disintegrating effects is little agreement of
cessing, and selective attention that are defined of strong emotion, as exactly what it is. The
either by neurological dysfunction and/or a well as inherent disjoint- only aspect that has
process of conditioned learning. ing of the relationship been agreed on is that
• The etiology of psychopathy is unclear, however,
between word and deed in psychopathy is an
a dimensional model seems to be fit best, in light
of the evidence provided here. Although there are which he terms “seman- integration
many academics and clinicians alike, who would tic dementia.” Cleckley’s of motivational
like to see psychopathy constructed as a categorical book has now become dispositions,
taxonomy of personality disorder, evidence the backbone of what we developmental
would suggest a dimensional model to be more view as psychopathic factors, individual
appropriate. Even though a categorical taxonomy
app-roach to psychopathy would allow for exten-
personality traits and, in differences, and
sive research into a homogenous sample, facili- turn, has lead to numer- mental health.
tated by the development of sound measures of ous research studies into
psychopathy, such as the Psychopathy the construct of psychopa-
Checklist–Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991) the notion thy and the psychopathic personality. Cleckley
of high and low scores would seem to suggest
proposed 16 core personality traits in psy-
dimensionality within the prototypical PCL-R
psychopath. Furthermore, a dimensional approach chopaths (see Table 1). Hare (1980, 1991, 2003)
to personality disorders has been rigorously has extended Cleckley’s ideas to develop the
argued in recent times, and this does seem to be Hare Psychopathy Checklist and the revised
the direction that will be adopted in the forthcom- version (PCL-R; see Hare, 1980, 1991, 2003) as
ing DSM-V. a standardized and clinical measure of psy-
• Intervention and treatment of high-risk offend-
ers, such as psychopaths, is high on the agenda
chopathy. The PCL-R will be discussed in more
in the United Kingdom and hence, the concep- detail in the measurement section of this article.
tion of dangerous and severe personality disor- Table 1 shows Cleckley’s and Hare’s conceptu-
der (DSPD) programs across the country. As yet alization of psychopathic traits.
there is no agreed systematic way of addressing
this issue; however, progress is being made on
all fronts, especially with the recent publication THEORIES OF PSYCHOPATHY
of the Wong and Hare (2005) Psychopathy
Treatment Program. However, all such The psychopath is not a person that can be
programs and interventions are still in their readily recognized by physical symptoms that
infancy; and until service evaluations are con- are seen within other types of mental illnesses
ducted on these programs and interventions, the or disorders. For example, the psychopath does
effectiveness of these types of interventions still
not experience hallucinations or even hear
remain questionable.
voices that tell him or her to do things and
behave in certain ways unlike someone who
important implications for sentencing, diver- is diagnosed with schizophrenia. Although
sion, placement, treatment options, and assess- psychopaths cannot be readily recognized by
ment for risk and recidivism (Edens, Desforges, any distinctive clinical symptoms, there are
Fernandez, & Palac, 2004; Hervé, Mitchell, distinctive personality characteristics that
Cooper, Spidel, & Hare, 2004; Hildebrand, de make psychopathy uniquely different from
Ruiter, & Nijman, 2004; Simourd & Hoge, 2000). other personality disorders. The notion of

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 157

innate antisocial predispositions has been criminality was measured at age 24 years. The
explored in a recent twin study conducted by findings of the study demonstrated that on all
Blonigen, Carlson, Kruegar, and Patrick (2003) measures of arousal, future criminals showed
that provided evidence of a significant genetic lower levels of arousal in the experimental sit-
influence in psychopathy, in that intraclass cor- uation than did future noncriminals. Therefore,
relations1 and model fitting analyses of the this does seem to provide some evidence that
data reveal substantial evidence of a large low arousal is associated with future criminal-
genetic contribution in monozygotic twins ity; however, criminality does not equate to
(identical) and relatively little relationship in psychopathy (Babiak, 1995; Hare, 1999). Blair,
dizygotic (nonidentical) twins that suggest a Jones, Clark, and Smith (1997) investigated the
genetic rather than an environmental contribu- psychophysiological responsiveness of psycho-
tion (Blonigen et al., 2003). We now examine a pathic individuals to distress cues and to
number of theories that have been suggested a threatening stimuli. Electrodermal responses
theoretical basis for the phenomenon. and skin conductance responses were recorded
during exposure to the stimuli. The results
Arousal Theory demonstrated that psychopaths, relative to
controls, had reduced electrodermal and skin
Low-arousal theory (Hare, 1970) suggests conductance responses to the threatening and
that psychopaths have a pathologically low neutral stimuli. Therefore, the psychopath’s
level of autonomic and cortical arousal, and low arousal and low physiological responsive-
hyperactivity when compared to nonpsycho- ness to threatening stimuli can be attributed to
pathic individuals. Consequently, the psy- high aversion.
chopath will be in a chronic state of stimulation Low arousal in psychopaths can be due to a
and sensation seeking and thus, explaining why number of things; Lorber (2004) conducted a
psychopaths do not become autonomically meta-analysis of 95 studies investigating the
aroused to stimuli that would otherwise be relations of heart rate and electrodermal activ-
stressful, exciting, or frightening to nonpsy- ity (EDA) with aggression, psychopathy, and
chopaths. This results in the psychopath need- conduct problems. He concluded that there was
ing a greater variety and intensity of sensory a low resting EDA and low-task EDA associ-
input to increase his or her arousal level to the ated with psychopathy and conduct problems.
optimum. The theory assumes that arousal Therefore, psychopaths’ low arousal could be a
level and sensory intake are dynamically related result of low EDA. Another explanation of the
in such a way that an optimal level of arousal is psychopaths’ low arousal could be an unnatu-
maintained, in other words, a common level rally high aversion threshold, whereby to reach
presumed to be functionally desirable for all the threshold, extreme levels of violence or
individuals. However, when that level of aggression would have to be obtained to have
arousal falls below the optimum, stimulation an aversive effect on psychopaths. For example,
and/or sensation-seeking behavior and sensory in a study conducted by Patrick (1994) height-
intake increases dramatically to raise the arousal ened aversion threshold in psychopaths was
level to the desired optimum. When the arousal demonstrated in an abnormal potentiation of
level rises above the optimum, the stimulation the startle reflex, which would normally occur
and/or sensation-seeking behavior and sensory during the processing of aversive stimuli.
intake attenuates and the level of arousal drops Patrick concluded that core emotional devia-
to the required optimal level. tion in psychopathy might be due to deficits in
Raine, Venables, and Williams (1990) conducted fear response that is defined by their failure to
a study looking at the relationship between exper- exhibit the normal defensive actions that are
imental measures of arousal and criminality at primed by aversive cues.
two different age intervals. Arousal was mea- Low arousal makes conditioning less likely
sured at age 15 years, using electrodermal, to occur (Eysenck, 1996/1998), and theres
cardiovascular, and cortical responses and some evidence to indirectly support low

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


158 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

conditionability due to low arousal in a study inferior frontal gyrus. Further analysis
reported by Raine et al. (1990) where it was revealed that psychopaths failed to show
found that identified low arousal in childhood appropriate neural differentiation between
was related to later levels of adult criminality. abstract and concrete stimuli in the right ante-
Flor, Birbaumer, Hermann, Ziegler, and Patrick rior temporal gyrus and the surrounding cor-
(2002) also provided evidence to support the tex. Kiehl et al. (2004) concluded that semantic
theory that psychopaths’ have low condition- processing of abstract material in psychopathy
ability compared to nonpsychopathic controls. is associated with abnormalities in the right
Flor et al. (2002) used a foul odor as the uncon- hemisphere. However, Blair (2003) argued that
ditioned stimulus (US) and neutral faces as the the lifestyle of the psychopath may exacerbate
conditioned stimulus (CD) to compare the con- neurobiological impairments, if any, rather
ditionability of psychopaths and nonpsy- than the impairments being defining at birth.
chopaths. As predicted, psychopaths’ failed Lifestyle habits, such as substance misuse
to exhibit a conditioned response; although would be more detrimental to neurological
the unconditioned response was comparable development than an innate impairment in the
between the two groups. Further analysis long term. We now examine some other ideas
of the data indicated that psychopaths have a that have neurobiological basis.
deficit in association formation, and it was
concluded that this deficit could be related to Amygdala and other cortical dysfunction.
deficient interaction between the limbic and Dysfunction in emotional processing seems to
cortical systems of the brain (see below). point toward a neural basis for the develop-
ment of psychopathy. Herpertz and Sass (2000)
Neurobiological Theory provided some evidence of a close association
between the difficulties that psychopaths have
One of the basic principles of neurobiology in emotional processing and poor prefrontal
and/or neuropsychology is the notion that cer- functioning. There is now, to a certain extent,
tain functions are, to some degree, localized agreement that amygdala dysfunction is the
within certain areas of the cerebral hemisphere, underlying neural structure responsible for
whereas others are lateralized to one hemi- the development of psychopathic tendencies
sphere of the brain. This principle of the local- (Blair, Morris, Frith, Perrett, & Dolan, 1999;
ization of functions has stemmed from the clas- Patrick, 1994). This is because of the association
sical studies of patients with localized brain of the amygdala with aversive conditioning
lesions who perform poorly on selected tasks and instrumental learning (LeDoux, 1998), as
and/or tests (Raine, 1993). There have been well as fearful and sad facial expression
strong associations and a belief that psycho- responses (Blair et al., 1999). Kiehl et al. (2001)
pathic individuals are biologically different used fMRI to examine neural responses in
from the norm, in the sense that their brains are criminal psychopaths during an affective word
structurally different. memory task. The results provided evidence
Kiehl et al. (2004) used functional magnetic that criminal psychopaths demonstrated sig-
resonance imaging (fMRI) to clarify and nificantly less affect-related activity in the
characterize the neural architecture involved in amygdala, when compared to criminal
the lexico-semantic processing in criminal psy- nonpsychopaths and noncriminal controls.
chopaths compared with a matched group of Müller et al. (2003) also presented similar
controls. Analysis of the data for both groups findings in their study, which again used fMRI
indicated that processing of word stimuli, to examine neural responses. Here they used
compared with the resting baseline condition, positive and negative pictures from the
was associated with neural activation in the International Affective Picture System (IAPS;
bilateral fusiform gyrus, anterior cingulated, Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1999) to examine
left middle temporal gyrus, right posterior the influence of emotional processing in
superior temporal gyrus, and the left and right psychopaths. Like the Kiehl et al. (2001) study,

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 159

Müller et al. found that there was increased psychopaths; Bernstein, Newman, Wallace, and
activation of the amygdala to negative pictures. Luh (2000) suggested a deficit in left hemispheric
Results from both studies provide evidence of activation; and Laakso et al. (2001) suggested
a strong association of neural activity in the impairments and lesions in the posterior hip-
amygdala and psychopathy; however, further pocampus in the development of psychopathic
research is needed to establish why psy- personality and behavioral tendencies.
chopaths have amygdala dysfunction and the However, this evidence is as yet inconclusive
roots of this dysfunction. because of small sample sizes and lack of repli-
Séguin (2004) reviewed the role of the cation studies to validate these data.
orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in relation to antiso- In a study conducted by Hiatt, Lorenz,
cial behavior. It was concluded that although and Newman (2003) the assumption was tested
OFC dysfunction seems to be associated with that psychopaths have abnormal language later-
variations in antisocial problems, they are alization and abnormal processing asymmetries
rarely associated with physical violence. in other domains. The study utilized the dichotic
Furthermore, Blair (2004) suggests that OFC listening task (Bryden & MacRae, 1988) to assess
dysfunction is involved in the modulation of the language and emotional lateralization in
reactive aggression, which is physical violence criminal psychopaths. Hiatt et al. (2003) pro-
elicited in response to frustration and/or threat. vided evidence that psychopaths demonstrate
Blair went on to distinguish reactive aggression a normal right ear advantage for word targets
from goal-directed, instrumental aggression, but showed reduced left ear advantage for
which the individual would engage in just to emotional targets. They concluded that psy-
gratify the self. Psychopaths tend to demon- chopaths’ abnormal processing asymmetries
strate marked levels of instrumental aggression are related to poor interhemispheric integration
(Blair, 2004; Martens, 2003), which is modulated (Raine et al., 2003).
by the amygdala (Blair, 2004). Therefore, OFC Using structural magnetic resonance imag-
dysfunction may not be evident in psychopaths. ing (sMRI), Raine et al. (2003) assessed whether
However, psychopaths seem to show marked psychopaths, and individuals with antisocial
levels of instrumental aggression and reactive personality disorder (APD) show any struc-
aggression, therefore both may play a role tural and/or functional impairments in the
(Blair, 2004). corpus callosum. A sample of 15 men with high
Even though the evidence for amygdala psychopathy scores and APD was compared to
dysfunction in psychopathy outweighs the a group of matched controls. They observed
evidence for OFC dysfunction, there is still that psychopaths showed an increase in cal-
dispute as to whether the neurobiological basis losal white matter and an increase in callosal
of psychopathy lies within the amygdala or the length when compared to controls. Raine et al.
OFC. As yet, the evidence is inconclusive, and concluded that corpus callosum abnormalities
the neurobiological basis of psychopathy still in psychopaths might be a reflection of neu-
remains unclear (Blair, 2003). However, what rodevelopmental processes involving early
can be concluded is that neurological dysfunc- axonal pruning or increased white myelina-
tion in psychopathy is not confined to any one tion, which results in abnormal or poor inter-
particular area, and the deficit in neurological hemispheric integration.
dysfunction may be quite widespread.
Social Learning Theory
Interhemispheric deficits. Although the amyg-
dala and OFC are the main focus of attention Although there is strong evidence linking
for the neurological cause of psychopathy, there psychopathy with neural and cerebral dys-
is now evidence to suggest poor interhemi- functions, there has been speculation that a
spheric integration and lateralization deficits. certain element of learning is involved in the
For example, LaPièrre, Braun, and Hodgins development of psychopathic personality and
(1995) suggested a ventral frontal deficit in tendencies. Lykken (1957) used passive-avoidance

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


160 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

TABLE 1: Cleckley’s Core Traits of Psychopathic Personality and Items From the PCL-R

Cleckley’s Core Traits Hare’s PCL-R Items

1. Superficial charm and good intelligence 1. Glibness/superficial charma


2. An absence of delusions & other signs of irrational thinking 2. Grandiose sense of self-wortha
3. An absence of “nervousness” or psychopathic manifestations 3. Need for stimulation and/or proneness to boredom
4. Unreliability 4. Pathological lyinga
5. Untruthfulness and insincerity 5. Conning and/or manipulative
6. A lack of remorse or shame for their behavior 6. Lack of remorse or guilta
7. Inadequately motivated antisocial behavior 7. Shallow affect
8. Poor judgment and failure to learn from previous experiences 8. Callous and/or lack of empathya
9. Pathologic egocentricity and incapacity for love 9. Parasitic lifestyle
10. General poverty in any major affective reactions or emotions 10. Poor behavioral controlsa
11. A specific loss of insight 11. Promiscuous sexual behavior
12. A general unresponsiveness to interpersonal relationships 12. Early behavioral problems
13. Fantastic and uninviting behavior with or without alcohol 13. Lack of realistic and long-term goalsa
14. Suicide is rarely carried out because of love of the self 14. Impulsivitya
15. Sex life will be impersonal, trivial, and poorly integrated 15. Irresponsibilitya
16. A failure to follow any kind of life plan 16. Failure to accept responsibility for own actionsa
17. Many short-term marital relationships
18. Juvenile delinquencya
19. Revocation of conditional release
20. Criminal versatility

a. These items are those included in the Psychopathy Checklist–Screening Version plus an additional item of adult antisocial behavior.

learning tasks to demonstrate psychopaths’ thus, they concluded that passive-avoidance


poor avoidance learning. The tasks required learning deficits may be a function of criminality
the respondents to learn a “mental maze”; and in general rather than a function of psychopathy.
at specific points the respondent had to choose Eysenck (1996/1998) proposed that poor avoid-
a response from a possibility of four. The cor- ance learning leading to antisocial behavior and
rect response from the four led to progression punishment is a result of poor conditionability
in the maze; however, one of the four responses and a failure of social training.
led to an electric shock. The main observa- According to Eysenck (1996/1998), con-
tion of the study was the extent to which science is a conditioned response (CR), which
control respondents learned to passively avoid is developed through socialization to control
the electric shocks, whereas psychopaths an individual’s behavior. Eysenck gave two
made significantly more responses resulting in possible explanations as to why some individ-
punishment, thus providing evidence of poor uals do not develop a conscience. First, a
avoidance learning in psychopaths. permissive society results in conditioning
However, Arnett, Howland, Smith, and experiences for conscience to either be missing
Newman (1993) provided evidence to suggest or inadequate and, hence, leading to antisocial
that poor avoidance learning was not confined behavior. Second, Eysenck suggested that the
to criminal psychopaths when compared to wrong experiences are reinforced; therefore,
criminal nonpsychopaths. They assessed the the CR to this reinforcement is also wrong.
autonomic response to rewards and punishment
in incarcerated psychopaths and incarcerated Moral-Reasoning Deficits
nonpsychopathic controls during a passive-
avoidance learning task. These results provide The idea that psychopaths have moral-
some evidence that psychopaths were relatively reasoning deficits stems back to Prichard’s
unresponsive to punishment compared to con- (1835) concept of “moral insanity,” whereby
trols. However, there were no significant group psychopaths’ have a diseased moral faculty.
differences in passive-avoidance learning; and Jurkovic and Prentice (1977) found some

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 161

evidence to support the idea that psychopaths’ suggesting that psychopaths’ do not have a
have less mature moral reasoning than other ToM deficit. Richell et al. (2003) conducted a
groups of delinquent and normal youths. study using reading the “mind in the eyes”
However, in a later study conducted by ToM test, which required the respondents to
Trevathan and Walker (1989) no significant dif- identify mental states of people depicted by
ference was found between the reasoning levels photographs of the eye region only. The results
of psychopaths and nonpsychopathic controls. of this study again provide evidence that
Therefore, the evidence to support the psychopaths do not have a generalized impair-
psychopath’s diseased moral faculty is incon- ment of ToM. Hence, the moral deficiency does
clusive. Rather than a diseased moral faculty, not stem from the inability to distinguish
Blair (1995) suggested that the psychopath’s between what is moral or immoral; psy-
fail to make moral and/or conventional chopaths are able to distinguish between the
distinctions because of their lack of a violence two verbally but are unable to put these words
inhibition mechanism. Blair tested this into action.
assumption and concluded that psychopaths Glannon (1997) suggested that the psy-
judged all transgressions as moral and not con- chopath’s inability to act on moral reasoning
ventional. However, Blair acknowledged that might be because of an emotional deficiency,
this overjudgment of transgressions as being which adversely affects their ability to deliber-
moral may be due to impression management ate and choose according to societal norms.
on the part of the psychopath, whereby he or One emotional deficit is a lack of empathy,
she wants to show that treatment has been which is the emotional reaction shown toward
effective in changing his or her moral disposi- another’s distress. This lack of empathy can be
tions. Blair, Jones, Clark, and Smith (1995) repli- linked to arousal theory; however, there is also
cated the Blair (1995) study and found similar the possibility that psychopaths have a deficit
results. Although psychopaths judged all trans- making mental representations of others,
gressions as moral, they did not justify their which leads to their moral-reasoning deficit.
responses with reference to welfare of others. However, in the Blair et al. (1996) study they
Furthermore, psychopaths were scoring the found that not only can psychopaths’ mental-
moral transgressions as being more serious ize compared to controls, they actually use
than the conventional transgressions. However, more mental state justifications than controls.
judging the moral transgressions as more seri- Blair, Sellers, et al. (1995) investigated emo-
ous would have been expected given that the tional deficiency in terms of a deficit in
sample were all incarcerated for actions similar emotional attribution, rather than failure to
to the moral transgressions (e.g., theft or vio- mentalize. Twenty-five psychopaths and 25
lence) and not for committing conventional nonpsychopathic controls were presented with
transgressions (e.g., talking in class). short vignettes or stories depicting happiness,
Researchers has often tried to draw parallels sadness, embarrassment, and guilt. The
of psychopaths’ moral-reasoning deficit to the respondents were then asked to attribute emo-
concept of the theory of mind (ToM) in children tion to the story protagonist. Psychopaths, and
(Leslie, 1987), where children understand other nonpsychopathic controls, did not differ in the
people’s behavior not according to reality attribution of happiness, sadness, or embar-
but according to what they believe as reality rassment but differed in the attribution of guilt.
and their feelings toward this belief. Blair et al. However, while the controls attributed guilt
(1996) used a number of ToM tasks developed correctly, psychopaths attributed either happi-
for adults, which required the respondents ness or indifference to vignettes depicting
to read 24 stories and then either interpret guilt. This suggests that psychopath’s deficient
the behavior of the characters or predict what moral-reasoning abilities may stem from the
would happen next. No significant difference inability to experience guilt and anxiety, rather
was found in psychopaths’ task performance than a deficit in making mental representa-
compared to nonpsychopathic controls, thus tions. We now examine research that has

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


162 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

looked at fear and anxiety in psychopaths in initial evaluation of the stimulus. There was also
more detail. startle inhibition in the psychopathic offender
sample when they were shown victim scenes that
Fear and Anxiety Deficits included mutilated figures or attacks on others.
In addition to a deficit in stimulus evaluation,
Patrick, Bradley, and Lang (1993) tested this there was also a reported high-aversion threshold
under the assumption that psychopaths have a among the psychopathic sample compared to the
fear and anxiety deficit, which is manifested nonpsychopathic sample; however, this may be a
through high aversion. They concluded that by-product of the deficit in stimulus evaluation.
nonpsychopaths displayed larger eye blinks if
startled, whereas psychopaths display smaller Selective Attention Deficits
eye blinks under the same condition. However,
in a study conducted by Hale, Goldstein, It has been demonstrated that high aversion
Abramowitz, Calamari, and Kosson (2004) the in psychopaths is facilitated by a weakness in
heightened aversion threshold in psychopaths the initial evaluation of the target stimulus
was not related to a reduced capacity for expe- (Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 2000).
riencing anxiety. Hale et al. (2004) tested the This was further demonstrated in a study con-
assumption that there would be a negative rela- ducted by Verona, Patrick, Curtin, Bradley, and
tionship between psychopath’s affective inter- Lang (2004) investigating the physiological
personal style and anxiety. However, results reactions to emotional sounds, either pleasant
provided no evidence for this even when trait or unpleasant, in prisoners. Those who scored
anxiety was controlled. Therefore, Hale et al. high on the psychopathy measure showed
concluded that although negative affectivity is diminished skin conductance responses to
evident in psychopaths, this differed from anx- pleasant and unpleasant sounds. Findings
iety, thus suggesting that anxiety may not be a indicate an abnormal reactivity to emotionally
confounding factor of psychopathy. positive and negative stimuli in psychopathic
Furthermore, the slides used in the Patrick individuals, which suggests that there is a
et al. (1993) study may not necessarily be fear deficit in the action-mobilization component
inducing, and thus reducing the likelihood of of their emotional responses. This would sug-
the respondent experiencing anxiety. Although gest that psychopaths’ have an inability to
psychopath’s high aversion cannot be attrib- differentiate between positive and negative
uted to a deficit in experiencing anxiety, there is stimuli and this selective attention deficit is
still plenty of evidence to support high aversion manifested in their high aversion, as measured
in psychopaths. Patrick (1994) reported that by low skin conductance and normal heart rate.
psychopaths do display smaller increases in Kosson (1998) assessed 129 right-handed
blink magnitude while viewing slides depicting male inmates (psychopathic and nonpsycho-
threats compared to nonpsychopathic controls. pathic controls) on a divided visual field task
These group differences can be interpreted as to assess whether psychopaths had divided
reflecting reduced activity in defense and with- attention deficits and whether these deficits
drawal mechanisms in psychopaths. could be attributed to specific hemispheric
In a more recent study, Bradley and Lang locations. The participants were given two
(2000) extended previous work on abnormal star- lateralized stimuli per trial under conditions
tle-reflex modulation on a sample of male pris- promoting attention either relatively focused
oners—psychopathic and nonpsychopathic. on one task or equally divided attention
Respondents were shown pictures from three between two tasks. The psychopathic group
distinct categories: pleasant (erotic and thrilling), misclassified more secondary targets and
unpleasant (victim or direct threat), and neutral marginally more primary targets than the
pictures. The results of the study provided nonpsychopathic control group. Psychopaths
evidence of a deficit in reflex modulation at ear- also tend to overrespond to distracters on
lier intervals, which suggests a deficit in their both tasks, replicating a reduced breadth of

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 163

attention but not inattention. Kosson con- Harris, Rice, and Lalumière (2001) examined
cluded that psychopaths are characterized by a the interrelationships and the independent contri-
specific divided attention deficit that lies butions of three major constructs associated with
within the left hemisphere. male criminal violence. The constructs examined
More recently, Hiatt, Schmitt, and Newman in this study were neurodevelopmental insults,
(2004) investigated the selective attention of psy- antisocial parenting, and psychopathy. Results
chopathic offenders using three Stroop indicated that neurodevelopmental insults and
paradigms, the standard word-word (CW) psychopathy were not interrelated but were inde-
Stroop, a picture-word (PW) Stroop, and a color- pendently and directly related to criminal vio-
word Stroop where the word and color was par- lence. Antisocial parenting had no direct relation-
tially separated (separated CW). The results ship with criminal violence however; it was
demonstrated that psychopathic offenders, related to neurodevelopmental insults and psy-
because of overselective attention, displayed a chopathy. Hence, Harris et al. concluded that
reduced Stroop interference on the separated criminal violence has at least two developmental
CW and PW tasks relative to nonpsychopathic pathways, one originating early in life involving
offenders. Furthermore, this reduced interfer- neurodevelopmental insults and one involving
ence was accompanied by normal facilitation. psychopathy, with antisocial parenting facilitat-
However, psychopathic offenders in the stan- ing the development of both.
dard CW Stroop displayed normal Stroop inter- Yochelson and Samenow (1976) presented a
ference. Hiatt et al. concluded that circumscribed different explanation for violent behaviors
attentional deficit in psychopathy that hinders exhibited by psychopaths, which also diverges
the use of unattended information is due to non- from the view that psychopaths have attenu-
integration of deliberately attended information ated fear and anxiety. They alleged that psy-
and is not compatible with the current goal- chopaths are able to experience or feel fear
directed behavior. and/or anxiety and that they experience fear
and worries frequently. However, they are
extremely cautious about revealing their fears
DEVELOPMENTAL PATHWAYS because it represents a weakness and vulnera-
Although the development of psychopathy bility in their character; and thus to cope with
in the individual can be based on neurological this affective state, psychopaths may use cog-
underpinnings and theoretical explanations, nitive strategies to alleviate the fear or to con-
there seems to be a number of different devel- vert the fear into anger, which then manifests
opmental pathways. Saltaris (2002) attributed itself into violent behavior toward others.
the development of psychopathy in adulthood Therefore, it would seem that psychopathy is
to aggressive and antisocial behavior exhibited an evolved life strategy (Szasz, 1960).
in childhood. Saltaris acknowledges that not all Although this can account for the constant
children with conduct problems will develop deviant and antisocial behavior of the psy-
psychopathy in adulthood; however, the chopath, and his or her grandiose sense of self-
link between the two must not be ignored, and worth, Yochelson and Samenow’s (1976) con-
she concluded that conduct problems coupled ceptualizations are largely untested, and the
with temperamental dispositions and an actual extent in which such individuals resem-
inability to form attachment bonds with ble the PCL-R identified psychopath is still
early caregivers are involved in the manifesta- unclear. However, it does explain to a certain
tion of psychopathic traits in adulthood. This is extent why psychopaths display such little fear
due to the aversive interpersonal style they and anxiety during clinical trials involving
engage during childhood and adolescence, aversive stimuli; however, actual empirical
which ultimately contributes to negative inter- work is needed before this standpoint can be
actions with family members and others in further emphasized.
their immediate environment and continues to Moffit (1993) suggested a theory of life-course-
deepen throughout the wider environment. persistent antisocial behavior as a developmental

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


164 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

pathway from childhood neuropsychological MEASUREMENT OF PSYCHOPATHY


impairments and adolescence behavioral prob-
lems to adult psychopathology. Although this is Even though psychopathy has yet to be rec-
generally more related to aspects of APD, there ognized as a specific personality disorder in
are distinct parallels with psychopathy. Moffit either of the diagnostic manuals, there are a
suggested that during a life course of an antisocial number of available psychometric scales that
individual there would be changing manifesta- measure the construct of psychopathy, Such as
tions of antisocial behavior. For example, an the Psychopathy Personality Inventory (PPI;
individual may exhibit behaviors of biting and Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), Blackburn and
hitting at age 4 years, shoplifting and truancy by Fawcett’s Antisocial Personality Questionnaire
age 10 years, selling drugs and stealing cars at (APQ; 1999), and Hare’s PCL-R (1991, 2003).
age 16 years, robbery and rape at age 22 years, The PPI (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996) is a
and fraud and child abuse at age 30 years. self-report measure of psychopathy that con-
The underlying disposition of the individual tains 163 items, split into eight subscales
is the same; however, the expression of behavior measuring specific personality traits associated
changes because of new social opportunities with psychopathy; however, it does not
that arise at different points in the individual’s measure the antisocial behaviors related to
life. psychopathy. Furthermore, as a self-report
Although Moffit’s (1993) life-course theory measure of psychopathy, the scores must be
does incorporate neurological and social factors viewed with caution, as pathological lying is
into the developmental pathway to psychopa- one of the main personality characteristics of
thy, this pathway is not dissimilar to persistent psychopathy; and hence the reliability of PPI
adult criminal behavior with the absence scores comes into question.
of psychopathology. Therefore, the use of life- Blackburn and Fawcett’s APQ (1999) was
course theory as an explanation to the adapted developed from Blackburn’s Special
development of psychopathy is insufficient. Hospitals Assessment of Personality and
Furthermore, the theory emphasizes the antiso- Socialization scale (SHAPS; 1982). The APQ is a
cial behavioral element of psychopathy with, 150-item scale with eight subscales that mea-
what seems to be, a complete disregard for sures psychopathy, as one of many dimensions
the core personality characteristics engrained of personality, hence there is a lot of comorbid-
in psychopathy. However, this may be because ity with other types of personality disorders.
of the fact that the life-course theory was pri- The APQ measures aspects of emotional dys-
marily developed as an explanation of how function, impulse control, deviant beliefs about
some childhood and adolescent antisocial self and others, and interpersonal problem
behavior develop into persistent adult antiso- behaviors (Blackburn & Fawcett, 1999).
cial behavior, whereas others would not. The PCL-R (Hare, 1991, 2003) is currently the
Hence, although it does provide some explana- most widely used and validated measure of
tion as to the developmental pathway of psy- psychopathy. At times when interview infor-
chopathy, its conception was not specifically for mation is unattainable, such as research using
this purpose and thus, its use as an explanation archival information, clinical assessments of
is limited. actively psychotic patients or even individuals
It can be concluded that the developmental who refuse to either be interviewed or cooper-
pathways of psychopathy in the individual ate with the clinician, it is suggested that
cannot be attributed to any one theory or ratings can be made solely from file informa-
explanation. One must take into account all the tion2 (Wong, 1988). PCL-R items are rated
differing, and sometimes even opposing, argu- based on a person’s lifetime functioning as evi-
ments to understand in more depth the devel- dent in the assessment of the interview and file
opment of psychopathy in the individual, information (Hare, 2003), The maximum total
which would then lead us to a better under- score on the PCL-R is 40, and the diagnostic
standing of the etiology of psychopathy. cutoff for psychopathy on the PCL-R is more

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 165

than 30 (Hare, 1991, 2003). Hare (1991) first Factor Structure of Psychopathy
suggested a cutoff score of more than 30 in the as Measured by the PCL-R
first edition of the PCL-R manual because it
was about one standard deviation above the When the PCL-R was first devised a two-
mean of the pooled samples of male offenders factor model of the measure was proposed
and patients that had taken part in his valida- (Harpur, Hare, & Hakstain, 1989). Factor 1
tion studies of the PCL-R and provided the best measures and identifies core personality char-
diagnostic efficiency with respect to the global acteristics of psychopathy, and Factor 2 mea-
clinical assessments of psychopathy (Hare, sures antisocial behaviors as assessed by APD.
2003). However, this cutoff score is based on a Hall, Benning, and Patrick (2004) suggested the
North American sample and when applied to PCL-R would be better accounted for in a
samples from other countries, such as the three-factor model. They validated this three-
United Kingdom, the prevalence of psychopa- factor model on a sample of 310 incarcerated
thy seems to decrease when the cutoff score of offenders. They concluded that the affective
30 is used. Hence, in the United Kingdom is factor was correlated with low social closeness
usually reported as above 25 (Cooke & Michie, and violent offending and the behavioral factor
1999). was associated with negative emotionality,
At present, there are two further psychopa- disinhibition, reactive aggression, and poor
thy checklists derived from the PCL-R, a 12- adaptability. The interpersonal factor was
item screening version (PCL: SV; Hart, Cox, & related to social dominance, low stress reactiv-
Hare, 1995; see Table 1 for the PCL: SV items) ity, and higher adaptive functioning. Like Hall
and the Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-YV; et al. (2004), Cooke and Michie (2001) also sug-
Forth, Kosson, & Hare, 2003) for use with gested a three-factor model after a factor analy-
adolescent offenders. There is also a nonclinical sis of a set of 13 items selected from the PCL-R.
measure, the Psychopathy-Scan version Cooke and Michie concluded from their analy-
(P-SCAN; Hare & Hervé, 1999) that should sis that the PCL-R measures a superordinate
only be used as tool to generate hypotheses construct, which is underpinned by three cor-
about people of interest, in terms of psychopa- related factors, in which they term interper-
thy and risk management for antisocial, crimi- sonal, affective, and behavioral/lifestyle factors.
nal, and violent behavior. The PCL: SV is con- However, Hare (2003) argued that the first two
ceptually and empirically related to the PCL-R; factors are just a simple split of the original
however, the two instruments serve different Factor 1 items, and the third factor simply con-
purposes, and the PCL: SV is not a replacement sists of the same items on the original Factor 2.
for the PCL-R (Hare, 2003). The PCL: SV However, Hare (2003) stated that Cooke and
should only be used as a screening tool for psy- Michie (2001) dropped items from their analy-
chopathic traits in offenders and forensic psy- sis; therefore he questions the validity of the
chiatric patients, those who score high on the three-factor model. Recently, Hare modified
PCL: SV should be further assessed using the the two-factor model into a two-by-four hierar-
PCL-R to obtain a more complete and reliable chical model or the “two-factor, four-facet”
assessment of psychopathy (Hart et al., 1995). model of psychopathy. Factor 1 and 2 are now
Forth et al. (2003) developed the PCL: YV, considered by Hare to be two higher order con-
specifically for assessing the prevalence of psy- structs that can be split further into two sub-
chopathy within young and adolescent factors or facets. Factor 1 is split into the two
offender populations, those ages 12 to 18 years. facets of interpersonal (four items) and affec-
Preliminary data does suggest that the PCL-YV tive (four items), and Factor 2 is split into the
has the same validity and reliability as the lifestyle (five items) and antisocial facets (five
PCL-R; however, there are still some ethical items). The two-by-four hierarchical model
issues surrounding the labeling of adolescents does account for discrepancies among offend-
as psychopathic that still needs to be resolved ers with high (above 30) and low (below 30)
and to date are still in general debate. PCL-R total scores.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


166 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

Different factor models can be derived from psychopaths is greater than 80% during a 5-year
various combinations of PCL-R items, and close period, there-fore, it would be reasonable to con-
inspection of the patterns that factor and facet clude that psychopaths are at high levels of risk
scores generate might help facilitate the inter- for future violence and violent recidivism.
pretation and implications of an individual’s However, the converse is not necessarily true
PCL-R protocol (Hare, 2003). Table 2 provides because not all offenders that are of high risk of
different factor solutions for the PCL-R, repro- recidivism score highly on the PCL-R. Other risk
duced with permission from Bishopp and Hare. factors also need to be accounted for, such as
expressed homicidal intent, sexual sadism, and
so on. Although psychopathy is not the only risk
Risk Assessment
factor to consider, it does play a central role in
The principle behind the construction of the any assessment of risk (Hart, 1998).
PCL-R was so that there could be a systematic In a retrospective study conducted by
and statistically valid way of measuring Hare and McPherson (1984), it was concluded
psychopathy as a clinical construct. Although that evidence of psychopathy in offenders
the PCL-R was not constructed as a risk- increases the likelihood of violent and-
assessment tool, it does seem to have an ability aggressive behavior. The data set also provided
to predict risk and recidivism quite accurately evidence of a strong relationship between
and is routinely used as part of risk- psychopathy and violence prior the age range
assessment, for example in the Violence Risk of 30 to 35 years; this is further supported in a
Assessment Guide (Quinsey, Rice, & Harris, later study, whereby Harpur and Hare (1994)
1995), The link between psychopathy and reported that psychopathy scores, in particular
offending seems to lie within the levels of risk Factor 2 scores, decline
of recidivism for violent sexual and violent with age. One possible
nonsexual reoffending and the personality . . . the rate of
explanation for this obser-
characteristics of psychopathy. We now exam- violent recidivism
vation is long spells of
ine the relationship between psychopathy and for psychopaths is
incarceration whereby they
violent and sexual offending in more detail. greater than 80%
cannot engage in certain
during a 5-year
antisocial behaviors that is
period, therefore, it
Prediction of Violent Offending measured by Factor 2 on
would be reasonable
the PCL-R, for example,
to conclude that
Psychopathy as Psychopathy as mea- Item 9 (Parasitic Lifestyle)
psychopaths are at
measured by the sured by the PCL-R and Item 19 (Revocation
high levels of risk for
PCL-R, suggests that suggests that individuals of Conditional Release).
future violence and
individuals with high with high (over 30) PCL-R However, evidence for
violent recidivism.
(over 30) PCL-R scores will commit more the decline of Factor 2
scores will commit violent and/or serious scores with the increase of
more violent and/or offenses compared to low age is inconclusive as there is not yet enough
serious offenses (below 30) PCL-R scores data to determine whether this relationship
compared to low (Brown & Forth, 1997; holds true across the life span (Hare &
(below 30) PCL-R Hare, 2002; Harris, Skilling, McPherson, 1984) or whether incarceration
scores. & Rice, 2001; Hart, 1998; was the main reason for a decline in antisocial
Kosson, Kelly, & White, behaviors. Hare and Jutai (1983) reported from
1997). Not only are they their longitudinal study that psychopaths may,
more likely to commit serious crimes, but they in fact, continue to engage in extensive criminal
are also at higher levels of risk to recidivism and antisocial behaviors and activities long
(Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996; Serin, Mailloux, after other criminals have shown decline; how-
& Malcolm, 2001; Steadman et al., 2000). ever, the extent to which these activities and/or
Hemphill, Templemann, Wong, and Hare (1998) behaviors include violent and aggressive acts
reported that the rate of violent recidivism for is still unclear, and further investigation is still

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 167

TABLE 2: A Comparison of the Different Factor Solutions of the PCL-R

2-Factor
Solution 3-Factor Solution 4-Factor Solution

PCL-R Items 1 2 1 2 3 1 2 3 4

1. Glibness and/or superficial charm   


2. Grandiose sense of self-worth   
3. Need for stimulation and/or proneness to boredom   
4. Pathological Lying   
5. Conning and/or manipulative   
6. Lack of remorse or guilt   
7. Shallow affect   
8. Callous and/or lack of empathy   
9. Parasitic lifestyle   
10. Poor behavioral controls  E E E 
11. Promiscuous sexual behavior X X E E E E E E E
12. Early behavioral problems  E E E 
13. Lack of realistic and long-term goals   
14. Impulsivity   
15. Irresponsibility   
16. Failure to accept responsibility for own actions   
17. Many short-term marital relationships X X E E E E E E E
18. Juvenile delinquency  E E E 
19. Revocation of conditional release  E E E 
20. Criminal versatility X X E E E 

SOURCE: Reproduced with permission from Bishopp and Hare (n.d).


NOTE: PCL-R = Psychopathy Checklist–Revised; E = Exclusions; X = Misfits.

warranted before any firm conclusions can be example, Rice and Harris (1997) reported that
made. sexual recidivism, as opposed to general violent
recidivism, was strongly predicted by a combi-
Prediction of Sexual Offending nation of a high PCL-R total score and deviant
sexual arousal (as defined by a phallometric
Porter et al. (2000) investigated the contribu- preference for deviant sexual stimuli, such as
tion of psychopathy to the heterogeneity of pictures of children, rape cues, or even nonsex-
sexual violence. The results showed that sex ual violent cues), 70% of those with such a pro-
offenders tend to have elevated Factor 1 scores, file recidivated sexually compared to 40% in all
and nonsexual offenders had higher scores on other groups. Similarly, Hildebrand, de Ruiter,
Factor 2. Therefore, it could be argued that and de Vogel (2004) examined the roles of the
sex offending is related to the interpersonal PCL-R and sexual deviancy scores in predicting
aspects of psychopathy, whereas nonsexual recidivism in a sample of convicted rapists in a
offences are more related to antisocial behav- Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. Survival
ioral aspects. It was also noted that rapists and analysis provided considerable evidence that
mixed rapists and/or molesters scored higher on sex offenders with high PCL-R scores (PCL-R
the PCL-R compared to offenders who victimize total score about 26 in the case of this sample)
only children. Furthermore, offenders who sexu- who also had high sexual deviancy scores are
ally victimize adults and children were substantially at a greater risk of committing a
between 2 to 10 times more likely to be high new sexual offense. Hildebrand et al. (2004) also
PCL-R scorers. reported that Factor 2 produced significant pre-
Other research suggests that there is a higher dictive validity for risk of sexual and violent
risk of offending and recidivating when deviant nonsexual reoffending, and Factor 1 signifi-
sexual arousal and psychopathy are present cantly predicted risk of sexual and general reof-
in the individual (Kosson et al., 1997). For fending as per previous research.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


168 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

Psychopathy and offenders. This leads to the alternative of


White-Collar Crime prisons and forensic hospitals to become increas-
ingly populated by psychopathic offenders,
Not all psychopaths Not all psychopaths are whose risk for offending and violence and their
are violent and violent and incar-cerated “lack of treatability” deems them too high a risk
incarcerated criminals, some are to warrant release into the community (Tucker,
criminals, some untrustworthy and 1999). There has been an increasing trend in the
are untrustworthy unreliable employees or United States to civilly commit offenders deemed
and unreliable unscrupulous and preda- as high risk under sexually violent predator (SVP)
employees or tory businessmen, corrupt legislations, whereby offenders will be indefi-
unscrupulous politicians, or even unethi- nitely detained even on release and/or comple-
and predatorybusi- cal or immoral profession- tion of their prison sentences (Tucker, 1999).
nessmen, als, who victimize their
corrupt politicians, or clients, patients and the
even unethical or
Previous Approaches to Treatment
general public (Hare,
immoral profession- 2003). However, little It is believed that traditional treatment
als, who victimize empirical research is avail- programs have a low success rate in treating
their clients, patients, able for this group of psychopaths; however, this does not mean that
and the general “white-collar” psychopaths all treatment would fail and be unsuccessful
public. (Hare, 2003); however, there (Wong, 2000). A number of studies do provide
are indications that the per- empirical evidence as to what can constitute
sonality structure and inappropriate treatment methods to use with
propensity for unethical psychopaths. For example, Rice et al. (1992)
behavior are probably much the same as the crim- reported that psychopaths who had taken part
inal psychopath (Babiak, 1995; Hare, 2003). in a patient-run, unstructured therapeutic com-
Babiak (1995) used the context of an organi- munity program had violently recidivated, on
zation going through chaotic change as the release, at a much higher rate than did psy-
backdrop to an example of a successful indus- chopaths who had not taken part in the
trial psychopath. In such circumstances, these program. Hare, Clark, Grann, and Thornton
psychopaths use their manipulation skills to (2000) also reported that a variety of short-term,
effectively manage the discrepant views of sup- nonspecific treatment programs, such as anger
porters and detractors to enhance their move- management and development of social and
ment up the career ladder. Although there is educational skills, had detrimental effects on
plenty of speculation of the similarities of per- recidivism rates for offenders with high Factor 1
sonality characteristics between the industrial scores. Therefore, it would seem that patient-
psychopath and the incarcerated offending psy- run treatment programs as well as nonspecific,
chopath, empirical research in this area remains short-term programs are inappropriate for
minimal, and further research is needed before psychopaths, and it would inadvertently help
any strong conclusions can be made. psychopaths to enhance and develop better
ways of manipulating, deceiving, and using
people rather than help them change their vio-
TREATMENT APPROACHES
lent-prone behaviors (Wong & Hare, 2005).
Psychopaths present the most serious prob- Psychopaths’ disruptive behavior during
lems for the Criminal Justice System, not only treatment has been attributed to the personality
are they responsible for many types of serious characteristics of glibness and/or superficial
crimes, they are also viewed to be “untreatable” charm and grandiose sense of self-worth
or disruptive in treatment (Loving, 2002; Rice, (Hobson, Shine, & Roberts, 2000). However,
Harris, & Cormier, 1992). However, Salekin Skeem, Monahan, and Mulvey (2002) concluded
(2002) argued that this pessimistic view under- from their study that patients with psycho-
mines the motivation to search for and develop pathic tendencies appeared just as likely to
effective treatment programs for psychopathic benefit from treatment as nonpsychopathic

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 169

patients, in terms of becoming less violent. cerated, and a suitable treatment program
They added that unfavorable treatment effects should focus on changing behavior rather than
on psychopaths could be eradicated if the fre- trying to change their core personality charac-
quency or dosage of treatment is increased for teristics. Wong and Hare proposed a set of
these individuals. guidelines for the effective treatment of violent
Patients who received seven or more ses- criminal psychopaths. They stressed that what
sions of treatment during a 10-week period is being proposed should only be used as a
were approximately 3 times less likely to be set of guidelines as to what methods are most
violent during a subsequent 10-week period, effective and not used as a comprehensive
when compared to those who received six or treatment program; however, for convenience,
fewer sessions (Skeem et al., 2002). This Wong and Hare reported these guidelines in
remained the case even after controlling for their Psychopathy Treatment Program (PTP).
other variables that were associated with treat- Here they noted that the primary objective of
ment attendance, such as substance abuse, the PTP is to reduce the frequency and the
race, employment status, and so on. This study severity of institutional and postrelease violent
yields important implications for the future behavior in psychopaths by convincing them
planning of treatment programs for psycho- that capitalizing their strengths and abilities in
pathic offenders; however, poor controls and a prosocial ways is more beneficial to them then
number of methodological flaws in the study their antisocial ways (Wong & Hare).
means that this optimistic view must be taken The PTP is more a strategy of behavioral self-
with caution. It should also be noted that treat- management rather than a cure for psychopathy;
ment effect had decreased or in some cases participants of the PTP should be assisted in
dissipated completely within a few months of developing deeper insights into their lifelong
treatment completion for those with high psy- psychopathology and to accept the fact that they
chopathy scores (Wong & Hare, 2005). will require long-term and continuing self-man-
Mulloy (1998) conducted a follow-up study of agement for most aspects of their lives to keep
psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders them from recidivating rather than any quick-fix
who were treated on a cognitive-behavioral treatment program. However, this is the current
treatment program (CBT). The results show that strategy that is adopted by many treatment
the overall completion rate was actually quite programs for nonpsychopathic offenders and
high for psychopaths and nonpsychopaths. The hence, not unique. However, Wong and Hare
sample was followed up 5 years posttreatment, (2005) do acknowledge that there are other fac-
and preliminary results indicate little difference tors to consider when treating psychopaths, such
in the recidivism rates for both groups. There- as the offender’s motivation and readiness for
fore, the data on treatment effectiveness with treatment, if neither is present than any type of
psychopaths are inconclusive; however, there is treatment will ultimately fail.
a clear indication as to what treatment programs In summary the PTP guidelines are similar
are unsuccessful with psychopaths, and the to many other treatment program guidelines
advice would be to steer clear of these types of that are currently available for nonpsycho-
treatment programs when planning treatment pathic offenders, However, it does document
work with psychopaths. Furthermore, with little ways in which to approach treatment with psy-
consensus to the underlying etiology of psy- chopaths by using, what can be termed as, the
chopathy, development of appropriate treatment “what not to do” approach. This has been done
programs seems less and less likely. by justifying using certain techniques with
psychopaths to bypass the obstacle of core per-
New Approaches sonality characteristics rather than head-on
to Treatment challenges. Furthermore, there is a strong theo-
retical link between the PTP and the PCL-R
Wong and Hare (2005) suggested that vio- prototypical psychopath; and hence based on
lence and antisocial behavior are the primary this, the PTP may well be successful for this
reasons for psychopaths to offend and be incar- particular group. Nonetheless, the PTP is still

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


170 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

TABLE 3: Pharmological Treatments for the Management of SSRIs or selective serotonin reuptake
Aggression and Impulsivity
inhibitors, such as Prozac, are agents that inhibit
Drug Associated Action of the Drugs: the reuptake of serotonin as part of a much
Lithium Associated with a decrease in impulsive
more widespread effect on neurotransmitters.
aggression Although SSRIs are not directly used as treat-
Selective Associated with a decrease in impulsive ment for aggression and impulsivity in psy-
serotonin aggression and overt hostility and chopaths, they have been found to be useful
reuptake antisocial behavior in the treatment of major depression, anxiety dis-
inhibitors
orders, pain disorders, and premature ejaculation
Divalproex Associated with decreases in irritability
sodium and overt aggression
(Adi et al., 2002).
Valproic acid Associated with a decrease in explosiveness
and mood lability CONCLUSIONS
Phenytoin Associated with a decrease in aggression
Even though there are numerous theories and
a mass of empirical research supporting the dif-
ferent theories and the development of a reliable
in its infancy, and evaluative studies on effec- and valid way of measuring the concept, the eti-
tiveness need to be conducted before any ology of psychopathy is still as yet not fully under-
firm conclusions can be made, although this is stood. Although it can be concluded that the
definitely a positive step forward at this etiology of psychopathy is an interplay between
moment in time. neurobiological, psychological, social and envi-
ronmental factors (Hare, 2003), there is still dis-
Pharmacological Approaches agreement as to what the underlying cause or
developmental pathway
Although there is some for each factor is (Hare,
The only agreement A gap in the
agreement to the neuro- Hart, & Harpur, 1991;
so far is some kind of literature is the
logical basis of psycho- Sloan, 1997). The only
amygdala and pharmological
pathy, pharmacological agreement so far is some
orbitofrontal cortex treatment of
treatment of psychopathy kind of amygdala and
dysfunction. Moral psychopaths, here
has had little attention orbitofrontal cortex dys-
reasoning deficits little research has
in terms of empirical function. Moral reasoning
and heightened been carried out in
research. The general deficits and heightened
aversion thresholds this area, and what
trend has been identifica- aversion thresholds do
does help put is available is mostly
tion of medication that is help put into context why
into context why based on literature
used to treat behavioral psychopaths are responsi-
psychopaths are written about
symptoms apparent in ble for a large majority of
responsible for a psychiatric disorders
psychiatric conditions, serious sexual and violent
large majority of and then transferred
such as schizophrenia, crimes. It would seem that
serious sexual and to psychopaths.
that are similar to behav- the pessimistic view of
violent crimes.
iors psychopaths engage in, treatment effectiveness on
such as impulsivity and psychopaths has been
aggression (Minzenberg & Siever, 2006). There fuelled by the unsuccessful out-comes of treat-
are a number of known drugs that seem to ment programs inappropriately used. It appears
demonstrate effectiveness in controlling that when an appropriately designed treatment
behavioral symptoms such as aggression and program is implemented, effectiveness of treat-
impulsivity in respondents with psychiatric ment can be demonstrated. A gap in the literature
disorders, and the move now would be to see is the pharmacological treatment of psychopaths,
whether the same applies to psychopathy. here little research has been carried out in this
Please refer to Table 3 for the different types of area, and what is available is mostly based on lit-
drug treatments available for aggression and erature written about psychiatric disorders and
impulsivity. then transferred to psychopaths.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 171

IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE PRACTICE, POLICY, AND RESEARCH


• The use of pharmacological treatment and drug • Detailed analysis of cognitions. Although neurologi-
therapy can also be another avenue to explore. cal deficits and personality characteristics of psy-
Although there are many drugs that are used to chopaths are somewhat understood and endlessly
control behavioral manifestations of symptoms in researched. The underlying cognitions psychopaths’
conditions, such as psychosis, the actual effective- hold remain unclear. Even though certain offense-
ness of these drugs to control behavior in psy- related schemas can be attributed to psychopaths,
chopaths has rarely been explored. Further explo- such as rape myths, there has been little empirical
ration in this area may complement the cognitive work in this area, and as yet a systematic way of
interventions that are currently available. assessing offense-related schemas in psychopaths.
• The development of effective intervention and • The emergence of the “white-collar” psychopath is
treatment programs for psychopaths will render another area where empirical research is limited.
changes in the sentencing phase for psychopathic At present, there is a huge amount of speculation to
offenders, whereby the current trend in the Criminal the similarity of personality structure between
Justice System, in the United Kingdom and in North violent criminal psychopaths and white-collar psy-
America is to give indeterminate sentences to psycho- chopaths; however, there is no empirical research
pathic offenders. This state of affairs may change either investigating or providing evidence for
when effective treatment and interventions are in this similarity. These assumptions must be empiri-
place. cally tested before any firm conclusions can be made.

NOTES Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to


morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1-29.
1. Intraclass correlations is a measure of the degree of rela-
tionships between numerous interconnected variables (Howell, Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopa-
1999), in case of Blonigen, Carlson, Kruegar, and Patrick (2003) thy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 5-7.
study, the variables are the total score and sub-scale scores on the Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in
Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI). the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and
2. Scoring the PCL-R solely on file information runs the risk
Cognition, 55, 198-208.
of over estimates in scores as well as difficulty scoring interper-
sonal and affective items without direct observation of the indi- Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1995). Is the
vidual’s interactional style and demeanor (Hare, 2003). psychopath “morally insane”? Personality and
Individual Differences, 19, 741-752.
Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1997). The
REFERENCES psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to
Adi, Y., Ashcroft, D., Browne, K., Beech, A., Fry-Smith, A., distress cues? Psychophysiology, 34, 192-198.
& Hyde, C. (2002). Clinical effectiveness and cost- Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan,
consequences of selective serotonin reuptake R. J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expres-
inhibitors in the treatment of sex offenders. Health sions of sadness and anger. Brain, 122, 883-893.
Technology Assessment, 6, 1-66. Blair, R. J. R., Sellers, C., Strickland, I., Clark, F., Williams,
Arnett, P. A., Howland, E. W., Smith, S. S., & Newman, A. O., Smith, M., et al. (1995). Emotion attributions in
J. P. (1993). Autonomic responsivity during passive- the psychopath. Personality and Individual Differences,
avoidance in incarcerated psychopaths. Personality and 19, 431-437.
Individual Differences, 14, 173-184. Blair, R. J. R., Sellers, C., Strickland, I., Clark, F., Williams,
Babiak, P. (1995). When psychopaths go to work: A case A. O., Smith, M., et al. (1996). Theory of mind in the
study of an industrial psychopath. Applied Psychology: psychopath. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 7, 741-752.
An International Review, 44, 171-188. Blonigen, D. M., Carlson, S. R., Kruegar, R. F., & Patrick,
Bernstein, A., Newman, J. P., Wallace, J. F., & Luh, K. E. C. J. (2003). A twin study report of self-reported psy-
(2000). Left-hemisphere activation and deficient chopathic personality traits. Personality and Individual
response modulation in psychopaths. Psychological Differences, 35, 179-197.
Science, 11, 414-418. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). The psychopath as
Bishopp, D., & Hare, R. D. (n.d.). A multidimensional scal- observer: Emotion and attention in picture processing.
ing analysis of Hare’s PCL-R. Unpublished manuscript. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 373-385.
Blackburn, R. (1982). The special hospitals assessment of per- Brown, S. L., & Forth, A. E. (1997). Psychopathy and
sonality and socialization. Unpublished manuscript, sexual assault: Static risk factors, emotional precur-
available from Park Lane Hospital, Liverpool, UK. sors, and rapist subtypes. Journal of Consulting and
Blackburn, R., & Fawcett, D. (1999). The Antisocial Clinical Psychology, 65, 848-857.
Personality Questionnaire: An inventory for assessing Bryden, M. P., & MacRae, L. (1988). Dichotic laterality
deviant traits in offender populations. European Journal effects obtained with emotional words. Neuropsychiatry,
of Psychological Assessment, 15, 14-24. Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 171-176.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


172 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity. St Louis, MO: Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psycho-
C. V. Mosby. pathy and the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personal-
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (1999). Psychopathy across cul- ity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100,
tures: North America and Scotland compared. Journal 391-398.
of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 58-68. Hare, R. D., & Hervé, H. (1999). Hare P-SCAN: Research
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of version. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
psychopathy: Towards a hierarchical model. Psychological Hare, R. D., & Jutai, J. W. (1983). Criminal history of the
Assessment, 13, 171-188. male psychopath: Some preliminary data. In K. T. Van
Edens, J. F., Desforges, D. M., Fernandez, K., & Palac, Dusen & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Perspective studies of
C. A. (2004). Effects of psychopathy and violence risk crime and delinquency (pp. 225-236). Boston: Kluwer-
testimony on mock juror perceptions of dangerous- Nijhoff.
ness in a capital murder trial. Psychology, Crime and Hare, R. D., & McPherson, L. M. (1984). Violent and
Law, 10, 393-412. aggressive behavior by criminal psychopaths.
Eysenck, H. J. (1998). Personality and crime. In T. Millon, International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 7, 35-50.
E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1994). Assessment of psy-
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal and violent behavior chopathy as a function of age. Journal of Abnormal
(pp. 40-49). London: Guilford. (Original work pub- Psychology, 103, 604-609.
lished in Psychology, Crime and Law, 2, 1996, 143-152). Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstain, A. R. (1989). Two-
Flor, H., Birbaumer, N., Hermann, C., Ziegler, S., & factor conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct valid-
Patrick, C. J. (2002). Aversive Pavlovian conditioning ity and assessment implications. Psychological Assessment:
in psychopaths: Peripheral and central correlates. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 6-17.
Psychophysiology, 39, 505-518. Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Lalumière, M. (2001). Criminal
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). The violence: The role of psychopathy, neurodevelopmen-
Psychopathy Checklist: Youth version. Toronto, Canada: tal insults, and antisocial parenting. Criminal Justice
Multi-Health Systems. and Behavior, 28, 402-426.
Glannon, W. (1997). Psychopathy and responsibility. Harris, G. T., Skilling, T. A., & Rice, M. E. (2001). The con-
Journal of Applied Philosophy, 14, 263-275. struct of psychopathy. Crime and Justice: A Review of
Hale, L. R., Goldstein, D. S., Abramowitz, C. S., Calamari, Research, 28, 197-264.
J. E., & Kosson, D. S. (2004). Psychopathy is related Hart, S. D. (1998). The role of psychopathy in assessing
to negative affectivity but not to anxiety sensitivity. risk for violence: Conceptual and methodological
Behavior Research and Therapy, 42, 697-710. issues. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 121-137.
Hall, J. R., Benning, S. D., & Patrick, C. J. (2004). Criterion- Hart, S. D., Cox, D. N., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Manual for the
related validity of the three-factor model of psychopa- Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV).
thy personality: Behavior and adaptive functioning. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Assessment, 11, 4-16. Hemphill, J. F., Templemann, R., Wong, S., & Hare, R. D.
Hare, R. D. (1970). Psychopathy: Theory and research. New (1998). Psychopathy and crime: Recidivism and behav-
York: John Wiley. ior. In D. J. Cooke, A. E. Forth, & R. D. Hare (Eds.),
Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of Psychopathy: Theory, research, and implications for society
psychopathy in criminal populations. Personality and (pp. 375-399). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Individual Differences, 1, 111-119. Herpertz, S. C., & Sass, H. (2000). Emotional deficiency
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. and psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18,
Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. 567-580.
Hare, R. D. (1995). Psychopaths: New trends in research. Hervé, H. F., Mitchell, D., Cooper, B. S., Spidel, A., & Hare,
Retrieved February 26, 1999, from www.mental R. D. (2004). Psychopathy and unlawful confinement:
health.com/mag1/p5hpe01.html An examination of perpetrator and event characteris-
Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose tics. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science—Revue
time has come. Criminal Justice & Behavior, 23, 25-54. Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement, 36, 137-145.
Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world Hiatt, K. D., Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2003).
of the psychopaths among us. New York: Guilford. Assessment of emotion and language processing in
Hare, R. D. (2002). Psychopathy and risk for recidivism psychopathic offenders: Results from a dichotic
and violence. In N. Gray, J. Laing, & L. Noaks (Eds.), listening task. Personality and Individual Differences, 32,
Criminal justice, mental health, and the politics of risk 1255-1268.
(pp. 27-47). London: Cavendish. Hiatt, K. D., Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (2004). Stroop
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised tasks reveal abnormal selective attention among psy-
(2nd ed.). Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. chopathic offenders. Neuropsychology, 18, 50-59.
Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Hildebrand, M., de Ruiter, C., & de Vogel, V. (2004).
Psychopathy and the predictive validity of the PCL-R: Psychopathy and sexual deviance in treated rapists:
An international perspective. Behavioral Sciences and Association with sexual and nonsexual recidivism.
the Law, 18, 623-645. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 16, 1-24.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


Vien, Beech / PSYCHOPATHY 173

Hildebrand, M., de Ruiter, C., & Nijman, H. (2004). PCL-R Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic
psychopathy predicts disruptive behavior among male personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 55, 6-10.
offenders in a Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. Martens, W. H. J. (2003). Constructive functions of aggres-
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19, 13-29. sion in psychopaths. Retrieved July 19, 2004, from
Hobson, J., Shine, J., & Roberts, R. (2000). How do psy- www.priory.com/psych/socio.html
chopaths behave in a prison therapeutic community? Minzenberg, M. J., & Siever, L. J. (2006). Neurochemistry
Psychology, Crime and Law, 6, 139-154. and pharmacology of psychopathy and related disor-
Howell, D. C. (1999). Fundamental statistics for the behav- ders. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy
ioral sciences (4th ed.). London: Wadsworth. (pp. 251-277). London: Guilford.
Jurkovic, G. J., & Prentice, P. M. (1977). Relation of moral Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-
and cognitive development to dimensions of juvenile persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxon-
delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 414-420. omy. Psychological Review, 100, 674-701.
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Müller, J. L., Sommer, M., Wagner, V., Lange, K., Taschler,
Forster, B. B., Brink, J., et al. (2001). Limbic abnormali- H., Röder, C. H., et al. (2003). Abnormalities in emotion
ties in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as processing within cortical and subcortical regions in
revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. criminal psychopaths: Evidence from a functional
Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677-684. magnetic resonance imaging study using pictures with
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Hare, emotional content. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 152-162.
R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2004). Temporal lobe abnormalities Mulloy, R. (1998, October). A follow-up of treated psycho-
in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as pathic and non-psychopathic sex offenders. Paper pre-
revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. sented at the annual meeting of the Association of the
Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging, 130, 297-312. Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Vancouver, Canada.
Kosson, D. S. (1998). Divided visual attention in psycho- Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: Startling
pathic and non-psychopathic offenders. Personality and new sights. Psychophysiology, 31, 319-330.
Individual Differences, 24, 373-391. Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion
Kosson, D. S., Kelly, J. C., & White, J. W. (1997). in the criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation.
Psychopathy-related traits predict self-reported sexual Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82-92.
aggression among college men. Journal of Interpersonal Porter, S., Fairweather, D., Drugge, J., Hervé, H., Birt, A., &
Violence, 12, 241-254. Boer, D. P. (2000). Profiles of psychopathy in incarcerated
Laakso, M. P., Vaurio, O., Koivisto, E., Savolainen, L., sexual offenders. Criminal Justice & Behavior, 27, 216-233.
Eronen, M., Aronen, H. J., et al. (2001). Psychopathy Prichard, J. C. (1835). A treatise on insanity and other disorders
and the posterior hippocampus. Behavioral Brain affecting the mind. London: Sherwood, Gilbert & Piper.
Research, 118, 187-193. Quinsey, V. L., Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Actuarial
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). prediction of sexual recidivism. Journal of Interpersonal
International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Violence, 10, 85-105.
manual and affective ratings. Gainesville: University of Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime: Criminal
Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology. behavior as a clinical disorder. London: Academic Press.
LaPiérre, D., Braun, C. M. J., & Hodgins, S. (1995). Ventral Raine, A., Lencz, T., Taylor, K., Hellige, J. B., Bihrle, S.,
frontal deficits in psychopathy: Neuropsychological Lacasse, L., et al. (2003). Corpus callosum abnormali-
test findings. Neuropsychologia, 33, 139-151. ties in psychopathic antisocial individuals. Archives of
LeDoux, J. E. (1998). The emotional brain: The mysterious General Psychiatry, 60, 1134-1142.
underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Weidenfeld & Raine, A., Venables, P., & Williams, M. (1990).
Nicholson. Relationships between central and autonomic
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense & representation: The origins measures of arousal at age 15 years, and criminality
of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412-426. at age 24 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47,
Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, 1003-1007.
P. J. (2000). The psychopath as observer: Emotion and Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1997). Cross-validation and
attention in picture processing. Journal of Abnormal extension of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide for
Psychology, 109, 373-385. child molesters and rapists. Law and Human Behavior,
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development 21, 231-241.
and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., & Cormier, C. A. (1992). An eval-
psychopathic personality traits in non-criminal popu- uation of a maximum-security therapeutic-community
lations. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 488-524. for psychopaths and other mentally disordered
Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 399-412.
psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G. V., Newman, C., Leonard,
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531-552. A., Baron-Cohen, S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Theory of
Loving, J. L. (2002). Treatment planning with the mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals
Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. International Journal of read the “language of the eyes”? Neuropsychologia, 41,
Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 46, 281-293. 523-526.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015


174 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / July 2006

Salekin, R. T. (2002). Psychopathy and therapeutic pes- Wong, S., & Hare, R. D. (2005). The guidelines for a psy-
simism: Clinical lore or clinical reality? Clinical chopathy treatment program. Toronto, Canada: Multi-
Psychology Review, 22, 79-112. Health Systems.
Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1996). A review Yochelson, S., & Samenow, S. E. (1976). The criminal per-
and meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and sonality: Volume 1. A profile for change. New York: Jason
Psychopathy Checklist–Revised: Predictive validity of Aronson.
dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
3, 203-215.
Saltaris, C. (2002). Psychopathy in juvenile offenders: Can SUGGESTED READINGS
temperament and attachment be considered as robust
developmental precursors? Clinical Psychology Review, Babiak, P. (1995). When psychopaths go to work: A case
22, 729-752. study of an industrial psychopath. Applied Psychology:
Séguin, J. R. (2004). Neurocognitive elements of antisocial An International Review, 44, 171-188.
behavior: Relevance of an orbitofrontal cortex account. Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity. St Louis, MO: C. V.
Brain and Cognition, 55, 185-197. Mosby.
Serin, R. C., Mailloux, D. L., & Malcolm, P. B. (2001). Cooke, D. J., Hart, S. D., & Michie, C. (2004). Cross-
Psychopathy, deviant sexual arousal, and recidivism national differences in the assessment of psychopathy:
among sexual offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Do they reflect variations in raters’ perceptions of
Violence, 16, 234-246. symptoms? Psychological Assessment, 16, 335-339.
Simourd, D. J., & Hoge, R. D. (2000). Criminal psy- Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world
chopaths: A risk-and-need perspective. Criminal Justice of the psychopaths among us. New York: Guilford.
& Behavior, 27, 256-272. Patrick, C. J. (Ed.). (2006). Handbook of psychopathy.
Skeem, J. L., Monahan, J., & Mulvey, E. P. (2002). London: Guilford.
Psychopathy, treatment involvement, and subsequent
violence among civil psychiatric patients. Law and
Human behavior, 26, 577-603.
Sloan, T. (1997). Theories of personality: Ideology and Anh Vien is currently a student in the
beyond. In D. Fox & I. Prilleltensky (Eds.), Critical psy- masters in forensic psychology practice
chology: An introduction (pp. 87-103). London: Sage.
course at the University of Birmingham.
Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Monhanahan, J., Appelbaum, P. S.,
Robbins, P. C., Mulvey, E. P., et al. (2000). A classification
Her main research interests are in psy-
tree approach to the development of actuarial violence chopathy and sexual offending. She is also
risk assessment tools. Law and Human Behavior, 2, currently a special constable working as a
83-100. voluntary police officer for West Midlands
Szasz, T. S. (1960). The myth of mental illness. American Constabulary in the United Kingdom. She is involved in
Psychologist, 15, 5-12. all aspects of everyday policing work ranging from keeping
Trevathan, S. D., & Walker, L. J. (1989). Hypothetical ver- public order, routine, and investigative police work.
sus real-life moral reasoning among psychopathic and
delinquent youth. Development and Psychopathology, 1, Anthony R. Beech is a professor of crim-
91-103.
inological psychology at the University of
Tucker, W. (1999). The “mad” vs. “bad” revisited:
Managing predatory behavior. Psychiatric Quarterly,
Birmingham in the United Kingdom, and a
70, 221-230. fellow of the British Psychological Society.
Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., Bradley, M. M., & During the past 10 years he has been the
Lang, P. J. (2004). Psychopathy and physiological lead researcher of the Sex Offender
response to emotionally evocative sounds. Journal of Treatment Evaluation Project (STEP) team. He has been
Abnormal Psychology, 113, 99-108. involved in treatment evaluation and the development of
Wong, S. (1988). Is Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist reli- systems to look at treatment need and treatment change in
able without the interview? Psychological Reports, 62, sex offenders. He has written widely on these topics and
931-934. other related subjects.
Wong, S. (2000). Psychopathic offenders. In R. Hodgins &
R. Müller-Isberner (Eds.), Violence, crime and mentally
disordered offenders (pp. 86-112) New York: John Wiley.

Downloaded from tva.sagepub.com at GEORGIAN COURT UNIV on March 10, 2015

You might also like