Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Formation of the Indo-European branches [2]

Formation of the Indo-European branches in cluster, which reflects a mixture of two earlier distinct populations: approximately 50%
Eastern European Hunter-Gatherer (EHG) and 50% Caucasian Hunter-Gatherer (CHG, also
the light of the Archaeogenetic Revolution known as ‘Iranian-associated’).
Before attempting to build on this foundation, it is important to recognize its limits, what it
John T. Koch does not mean. It does not mean that everyone on the Pontic–Caspian steppe ~5000 BP spoke
Proto-Indo-European, or conversely that all speakers of PIE ~5000 BP lived on the Pontic–
University of Wales Centre for Advanced Welsh and Celtic Studies Caspian steppe. It does not mean that all users of Yamnaya culture ~5000 BP spoke PIE, or
Draft [19-iii-2019] of paper read at the conference ‘Genes, Isotopes and Artefacts. How should we conversely that all speakers of PIE ~5000 BP used Yamnaya culture. It does not mean that all
interpret the movement of people throughout Bronze Age Europe?’ Austrian Academy of Sciences, individuals carrying the Steppe component at high levels ~5000 BP spoke PIE, or conversely
Vienna, 13–14 December 2018. that all speakers of PIE ~5000 BP carried the Steppe component, either at high levels or
necessarily at all. How closely or loosely these categories coincide should become clearer, even
Er cof am Eric P. Hamp | 16 xi 1920 – 17 ii 2019 precisely quantifiable, as more evidence comes in.

Introduction Preliminaries 1: A tree model of the Indo-European macro-family

Using the historical-comparative method, linguists can recover many details of unattested The Indo-European sub-families or branches are usually reckoned as ten: (in order of
languages. With enough of the right kind of data, it is even possible to reconstruct detailed attestation) Anatolian, Indo-Iranian, Greek, Italic, Celtic, Germanic, Armenian, Tocharian, Balto-
lexicons and grammatical descriptions. Even so, it is often difficult to assign an absolute date, Slavic, Albanian.6, 7 It should be mentioned also that there are several fragmentarily attested
geographical location, and cultural context to some of the most fully reconstructed prehistoric ancient Indo-European languages (such as Phrygian, Thracian, and Lusitanian) that cannot be
languages. The common ancestor of the attested Indo-European languages is such a case, and certainly affiliated with any of the ten branches. It is possible that there were other IE branches
the question of its homeland has been keenly disputed since the 19th century, through the that died out completely unattested.
20th, and into the 21st. In recent years, with the availability of ancient DNA data, the situation At the stage when each of the ten branches was first attested in writing, they were all fully
has suddenly improved, adding to the evidence base genetic relationships between prehistoric separate languages. In other words, in a hypothetical encounter between speakers of any two
populations and groups in the historical period speaking attested languages. of them, there would have been little mutual intelligibility. At the horizon of written evidence,
This essay works from recently published archaeogenetic evidence, drawing attention to what the separation of each of these from Proto-Indo-European (and from any post-PIE intermediate
it might imply for some longstanding issues in historical linguistics. Seven working hypotheses ancestor) lay deep in the past.
are presented concerning prehistoric languages in western Eurasia. These hypotheses aim to It is common for the ten Indo-European branches to be represented with a tree model.
situate speech communities in time and space, and to identify archaeological cultures and Sometimes all ten will be shown branching from a single point from the end of the Proto-Indo-
genetic populations associated with them. Hypotheses 1–6 deal with particular nodes and European trunk.8 But that is surely not what happened. More scientific trees are constructed as
splits on the tree model of the Indo-European macro-family, the seventh with the prehistoric a series of two-way splits. So, for example, the Anatolian branch comes off first, as universally
ancestor of the non-Indo-European language Basque. agreed, with the rest of Indo-European, still a single unified language, on the other side of that
As essential background, it is noted, but will not be recapitulated at length, that aDNA data split. Most, but not all, Indo-Europeanists think that Tocharian branched off next. This again
published in two milestone papers in 20151, 2 was argued to support elements of the steppe would involve all the rest of Indo-European as a still unified proto-language on the other side
or kurgan hypothesis of the Proto-Indo-European homeland. This hypothesis was formulated of the split, and so on until all ten branches were separate and there was no residual Late PIE
by Gimbutas in the mid 20th century,3, 4 then advanced by Mallory5–7 and Anthony8–10. The commonality.
implications of this strong, but not closely precise, archaeogenetic support can be summarized If archaeogenetics has now brought increasing confidence concerning the homeland of PIE,
as follows. it should do so likewise for combining linguistics, archaeology, and genetics to locate the
The common ancestor of most of the attested Indo-European languages was spoken on the reconstructed languages occupying the gap between PIE and the attested Indo-European
grasslands of what is now Ukraine and South Russia about 5000 years ago. This language’s languages. We might expect this task to be easier, as it deals with times closer to the horizon
territory then expanded by mass migration. There is a significant correlation between where we have written records in their archaeological contexts, as well as now increasingly
speakers of this proto-language and the Yamnaya material culture and a genetic ‘Steppe’ information about the genetic connections of the associated human remains. Even prior to
[3] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [4]

the availability of aDNA evidence, some adherents of the steppe hypothesis had elaborated Schleicher’s 1861 tree model (Fig. 1) shows several basic features have persisted for over 150
identifications of several specific post-Proto-Indo-European prehistoric languages with Copper years.22 He recognized Balto-Slavic (slawolitauisch) and Indo-Iranian (arisch), which remain
and Bronze Age cultures.5, 8, 86 However, there remain unresolved questions about how PIE standard features, as well as Italo-Celtic (italokeltisch), which is a common—probably the
split up and diversified, as well as the most meaningful way to conceptualize these processes. most common—view today.95 An Italo-Celtic node (representing an undifferentiated speech
So, the question is not just which family tree (Stammbaum) but whether any tree model will community) is a feature of the Ringe et al. tree. They also include a Greco-Armenian node,
provide an accurate enough roster of Indo-European linguistic groupings in later prehistory. which has been proposed elsewhere,12, 16–18,29 but are more ambivalent about such a proto-
language, finding the evidence for it to be ‘disappointingly meager’.
For several reasons, the tree of Ringe et al. 2002 is used here. First of all, their study aims to
do exactly what is of interest presently, “... to recover the first-order subgrouping of the Indo- With Ringe et al.’s tree, no details relevant presently are lost by following Mallory’s 2013
European family ...”11, rather than, say, work out the time depth of PIE12–18, 98, 99 or merely to simplification.23 However, for reasons explained below, it is their less problematical tree,
illustrate the subclassifications of numerous attested IE languages8. Secondly, Ringe et al. use omitting Germanic, that is simplified here (Fig. 2). In this tree Proto-Indo-European first
a robust base of phonological, morphological, and lexical evidence, which is rigorously filtered divides, as expected, into the Anatolian branch on one side and the rest of Indo-European on
for known and conceivable pitfalls. Thirdly, they seriously consider an alternative model for the other. Next, that ‘rest of Indo-European’ divides, becoming Tocharian on one side and a
the formation of the IE branches, namely one of a shallowly diversified dialect continuum new, smaller rest of Indo-European on the other. Then, another split with Albanian on one side
formed by rapid expansion into new territory followed by the crystallization of branches from and yet a newer and smaller rest of Indo-European on the other. Then Italo-Celtic splits off,
neighbouring dialects within emerging sociocultural areas.19, 20 Such a model is favoured by
myself in earlier work.21 Ringe et al. conclude that Garrett’s proposal for the IE branches is
compatible with their Stammbaum. In other words, the basic groupings and their structure Proto-Indo-European PIE 1
would remain the same whether they had arisen by groups of speakers arriving in new lands
and abruptly losing contact with their homeland or the crystallization of branches from PIE 2
mutually intelligible dialects in contact.

Tocharian PIE 3 Anatolian


Figure 1. August Schleicher’s 1861 tree model
of the Indo-European language family. deutsch.
German (Germanic)

litauisch. PIE 4
Lithuanian (Bal�c)
Albanian
slawisch. Figure 2. Simplified version of
Slavic Ringe et al. 2002, Fig. 8, ‘One of
the best trees with Germanic
vic omitted’, to which is added labels
Celtic
-Sla Italic
to for the nodes PIE 1–6 as explained PIE 5
Bal keltisch.
h. in this essay. The bracketed
isc Cel�c
tau position of Germanic in the tree
oli h.
s law isc
italisch. model is that indicated in the [Germanic] PIE 6
elt Italic passage quoted from Ringe et al.
h. l ok Greek
utsc ita
Ce
l�c 2002. Armenian
de h. lo- albanesisch.
wo tisc Ita
sla okel Albanian
ital
indogermanische eco griechisch.
gra Greek
ursprache. ariogra
e arisch.
Proto-Indo- italoke co-
ltisch. Indo-Itr
European anian eranisch.
Iranian
Indic
indisch. Slavic Iranian
Indic Baltic
[5] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [6]

then Greco-Armenian. The last residual common ancestor of more than one branch is Balto-
been maintained by prolonged intense contact, in which speakers habitually used their own
Slavic+Indo-Iranian, which will be of special interest here. Amongst the evidence supporting
dialect—rather than switching or using a lingua franca—in speaking to speakers of another
such a commonality are two shared innovations in the consonant system, known as the satǝm
dialect. For historical linguistics, an important implication of the concept of dialect as used
and RUKI innovations. (satǝm is the Avestan word for ‘100’, contrasting with Latin centum, both
here is that borrowings between dialects will often not be detectable linguistically because
from PIE *k̂m̥tóm.) In the first change, the PIE consonant series *k *g *gh and *kw *gw *gwh
cognate sounds will be substituted in the borrowing dialect. (This, incidentally, has been
merged as *k *g *gh. PIE palatovelars*k̂, *ĝ, and *ĝh become assibilated (s-like sounds) in the
one chief argument supporting the Anatolian Neolithic hypothesis of Indo-European origins,
satǝm languages. In the second change, PIE *s became *š (something like English seat versus
despite shared Indo-European vocabulary for a later [Copper Age] material culture.96–100)
sheet), following the sounds *r, *w, *k, *g, *gh, or *y.86
Related languages, on the other hand, will refer to genetically related varieties of speech with
In historical linguistics various labels are attached to the recurring ‘rest of Indo-European’
relatively low, even virtually no, mutual intelligibility. Such cognate speech forms will usually
unities. These include ‘Late Indo-European’, ‘Middle Indo-European’, ‘Nuclear Indo-European
have been separate from one another longer with less regular contact. Communication will
(NIE)’, and ‘Surviving Indo-European (SIE)’ (meaning after the now-extinct Anatolian and
require a speaker from one language to learn a second language, or the use of a shared lingua
Tocharian branches split off). The usage has been inconsistent and confusing. Purely for
franca. Borrowings between separate related languages will more often be detectable as such
present purposes, the following terms will be used for the ancestral core of the family and
by linguists because they are less likely to involve assimilative substitution of cognate sounds.
successive residues of this core:
In referring to reconstructed prehistoric languages the prefixes Pre- and Proto- will be used for
• the earliest Proto-Indo-European (ancestor of all the branches including Anatolian) =
concepts along the same lines as dialect and language. So, for example, Pre-Indo-Iranian could
PIE 1;
be thought of as a dialect within a still intact larger linguistic unity and speech community,
• the residual core after Anatolian branches off = PIE 2; Proto-Indo-Iranian/Balto-Slavic in this case. Proto-Indo-Iranian and Proto-Balto-Slavic would
belong to a later stage, no longer a single speech community with a relatively high degree of
• the residual core after Tocharian branches off = PIE 3;
mutual intelligibility. Loanwords between Proto-Indo-Iranian and Proto-Balto-Slavic would
• the residual core after Albanian branches off = PIE 4; more often be detectable as loanwords, than between Pre-Indo-Iranian and Pre-Balto-Slavic,
which were still part of a coherent dialect continuum.
• the residual core after Italo-Celtic branches off = PIE 5;
Another important point in the distinctions between dialects versus related languages and pre-
• the residual core after Greco-Armenian branches off (bearing in mind that the existence
versus proto-languages is that it is more common for an adult to successfully change dialect
of such a branch is uncertain for the reason explained above) = PIE 6, which can also be
than to learn a second language and use it like a native speaker. Accordingly, in the languages
called Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian.
of the world, mixed dialects are common, mixed languages (as distinct from a language with
¶Note. As will be important in connection with Working Hypothesis 4 below, PIE 6 originally numerous loanwords) are rare. So, as proposed below, at the stage Pre-Germanic/Balto-Slavic/
included Pre-Germanic also. But at an early prehistoric stage, Pre-Germanic separated from Indo-Iranian, it was possible for the western part of that continuum to shift speech areas,
Pre-Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian and reoriented towards Italo-Celtic. becoming reoriented towards Pre-Italo-Celtic. We do not need to think of this process as
speakers at the western end of Pre-Germanic/Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian changing language, but
rather contacts intensifying with mutually intelligible dialects in the West—permitting shared
innovations—and declining with those in the East.
Preliminaries 2: some terminology and concepts
The term dialects refers here to genetically related forms of speech (i.e. having a common Working hypothesis 1: PIE 1 and Anatolian
ancestral form of speech, nothing to do with genetics per se) retaining a high degree of mutual
The homeland of PIE 1—ancestral to all Indo-European, including the Anatolian branch—was
intelligibility. This relationship may be the case when the communities speaking the dialects
more probably south of, or possibly in, the Caucasus than on the Pontic–Caspian steppe. The
have lost contact with each other more-or-less abruptly and fully, but at a relatively short time
speakers of PIE 1 were probably not closely associated genetically with the ‘Steppe component’,
previously, say eight generations or fewer, so that linguistic entropy has not set in sufficiently
that is, ~50 EHG and ~50% CHG.
to block mutual intelligibility. On the other hand, the related speech forms may be distinct
for far longer than eight generations and still be dialects rather than separate languages In its unrevised form, the steppe hypothesis is that the parent language of all Indo-European,
in the senses used here, for example the Greek dialects or the largely mutually intelligible including the Anatolian branch, what is called here PIE 1, came from the Pontic–Caspian
national ‘languages’ of Norway, Sweden, and Denmark. In these cases, mutual intelligibility has steppe. Thus far, the archaeogenetic evidence—including that published in the two seminal
[7] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [8]

papers of 20151, 2—has supported the Pontic–Caspian steppe as the homeland of PIE 2 (Proto- to learn Hittite as a second language and pass it on to their children as a first language, that
Indo-European after Anatolian branched off) rather than PIE 1. Therefore, on this basic matter, is, assuming that Damgaard et al. are right in first place that their five individuals were Hittite
the new evidence has not confirmed the steppe hypothesis. speakers.
For purely linguistic reasons Anatolian has always been a vulnerable point in the hypothesis. Slight though this evidence may be and provisional any conclusions drawn from it, we are
Not only is it universally recognized as the first branch to separate—and therefore the most at present looking at falsification of the steppe hypothesis as formulated by Lazaridis rather
relevant for dating and locating PIE 1—but also the reason why it is universally agreed to be than support for it. And though that evidence could be said to support a model of infiltration
the first branch is that it is so different and so much more archaic than the other branches. For by small, culturally influential groups with Steppe ancestry, it could just as well—and more
example, it is the only branch to retain the laryngeal sounds directly; their former presence economically—be used to bolster the case that the PIE 1 homeland was not the Pontic–Caspian
must be reconstructed to explain features of the other branches, but they have not survived steppe and was not spoken by groups with Steppe or EHG ancestry.
in any of them, even those attested in the Bronze Age, i.e. Mycenaean Greek and Old Indic.
The alternative proposal made by Reich (before Damgaard et al. 2018 was available) appears
Anatolian is also the only branch lacking a feminine gender, which is also usually understood
viable, as we await further evidence to fill in gaps and reinforce (or not) preliminary findings:
to be an archaism retained from PIE 1 rather than a subsequent innovation. Tense and aspect
in the verbal system of Anatolian is also significantly simpler and can be seen as reflecting a Ancient DNA available from this time in Anatolia shows no evidence of Steppe ancestry
state of affairs preceding changes common in all the languages descended from PIE 2. In other similar to that in the Yamnaya (although the evidence here is circumstantial as no ancient
words, there is a big gap between PIE 1 (reflected in Anatolian) and PIE 2. It is hard to imagine DNA from the Hittites themselves has yet been published). This suggests to me that the
that there could be less than 1000 years between the first split and the second or that both PIE most likely location of the population that first spoke an Indo-European language was south
1 and PIE 2 could be assigned to a single archaeological culture. of the Caucasus Mountains, perhaps in present-day Iran or Armenia, because ancient DNA
from people who lived there matches what we would expect for a source population both
This same point is now made more concretely by Kroonen et al. 2018. They present personal
for the Yamnaya and for ancient Anatolians [i.e. CHG]. If this scenario is right the population
names recorded ~2500/2400 BC, relating to a country somewhere in Anatolia called Armi.
sent one branch up into the steppe—mixing with steppe hunter-gatherers in a one-to-one
These names appear to be in an early Anatolian, leading to the conclusion: “... since the
ratio to become the Yamnaya … —and another to Anatolia to found the ancestors of people
onomastic evidence from Armi is contemporaneous with the Yamnaya culture (3000–2400
there who spoke languages such as Hittite.27
BCE), a scenario in which the Anatolian Indo-European language was linguistically derived
from [Proto-]Indo-European speakers originating in this culture can be rejected”.24 These Armi Incidentally, on the strength forthcoming genetic evidence mentioned in Kroonen et al. 2018,24
names are provisionally accepted here as including archaic Anatolian at the proposed date, the dynamic and influential Maykop culture of the north-western foothills of the Caucasus
though the matter requires further study. ~3700–3000 BC does not look immediately promising as a context for PIE 1, as EHG ancestry
has been found in samples from this culture, unlike the five Hittite individuals of Damgaard et
In a lucid overview of ‘The evolutionary history of human populations in Europe’ published on
al.26
the internet on 4 May 2018, Iosif Lazaridis writes:
If this pattern continues as more samples are sequenced—that is, no Steppe or EHG ancestry
In the next few years this lingering mystery will be solved: either Anatolian speakers will be
in the genomes of probable Anatolian speakers—more researchers will wish to reopen the
shown to possess steppe-related ancestry absent in earlier Anatolians (largely proving the
question of how Indo-European is defined. When Hittite was first deciphered over a hundred
steppe PIE hypothesis), or they will not (largely falsifying it, and pointing to a Near Eastern
years ago,28 its differentness from Latin, Greek, and Sanskrit upset philologists. One reaction
PIE homeland).25
was to model Hittite (and then the Anatolian branch, as the subfamily became known) as a
As if to emphasize the pace of discovery, an answer—at least the beginnings of one—came in a sister of PIE rather than a daughter.34, 35 This Indo-Hittite theory, which nowadays could more
few days rather than a few years with the appearance of the preprint of Damgaard et al. 2018, accurately be called Indo-Anatolian, lost ground in the later 20th century, as fewer linguists
publishing evidence of full genomes of 74 ancient individuals, including five deemed probably were deeply invested in the painstaking 19th-century Greco-Aryan reconstruction of Proto-
to be speakers of Hittite. These five had no Steppe component, nor any EHG ancestry.26 Indo-European. Nonetheless, Indo-Hittite was retained by Hamp,29 and most linguists see
Anatolian as clearly the first branch to separate and uniquely affording insights into an earlier
Without further confirmation, it would be premature to see the matter decisively settled at
stage of the proto-language. For many, ‘Indo-Anatolian’ versus ‘Early Indo-European’ is an
this point. There are only five individuals. The Hittite Empire of the Late Bronze Age was a
arbitrary choice of terminology. The archaeogenetic evidence may now prove decisive in
complex, stratified society—a fairly brittle structure that terminally shattered as part of the
favouring a shift, if it continues to indicate that the common ancestor of Anatolian and the rest
widespread upheavals ~1200 BC. The empire contained influential groups with exceptional
of the family had been spoken in a different region and by a different population from those
political power, specialist expertise, and mercantile wealth. In such a society, individuals who
associated with the separate evolution of the residual core following Anatolian’s branching off
were not descended from Old Hittite-speaking founders might have seen it as advantageous
[9] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 10 ]

(i.e. PIE 2). If PIE 1 becomes henceforth ‘Indo-Anatolian’, or the like, the hypothesis that the innovations distinguishing the languages within the family had occurred—it can be hard to
homeland of Proto-Indo-European was the Pontic–Caspian steppe will possibly be saved by a distinguish such inter-dialect borrowings from shared inheritances. With a language family
terminological sleight of hand, moving the goal posts. as vast and complex as Indo-European, both types of separation—abrupt loss of contact and
prolonged mutual influence within a dialect continuum—probably occurred in various facets
of its history. That means we won’t be comparing like with like. All else being equal, the
Working hypothesis 2: PIE 2, Afanasievo, and Tocharian daughter language formed by a clean break might look more archaic than a related language
which formed at the same date but stayed in contact with other members of the family. The
The homeland of PIE 2—following the branching off of Anatolian, but before the branching off
latter was in position to share innovations as borrowings. Because the Indo-European that
of Tocharian—was the Pontic–Caspian steppe. There was a general close association between
became Tocharian was geographically widely separated from the rest of the family, with
speakers of PIE 2 and users of the Yamnaya material culture and a genetic population with the
3000+ kilometres and, as explained below, a non-Indo-European language or languages were
Steppe component (~50% EHG : ~50% CHG).
originally in that gap, it is likely that it would be a case of abrupt separation, at least relatively
This hypothesis would be supported by affirmative answers to the three questions below. so. The Indo-European languages of Europe would not be. So, while accepting that the
linguistic evidence supports a model of Tocharian as the second branch to separate from Indo-
1. Are the populations associated with the Yamnaya and Afanasievo archaeological
European, caution is advisable.
cultures the same (or very closely related) genetically?
In the case Anatolian, the barrier of the Caucasus Mountains with its non-Indo-European
2. Is the link between the Tocharian languages and Afanasievo culture secure?
languages might similarly have caused a clean break with the rest of the family. But for
3. Was Tocharian the second branch to separate from Proto-Indo-European? Anatolian, its differentness from the rest of Indo-European is more clear-cut and the written
evidence is much earlier. It is not credible that all the innovations that distinguish the rest of
Current evidence favours all three, but not with equal confidence. The strongest yes is for
Indo-European from Anatolian spread by inter-dialect borrowing.
question 1. The six Afanasievo individuals sequenced as part of Allentoft et al. 2015 were
virtually indistinguishable from the Yamnaya samples, showing ‘Steppe ancestry’.1 This result At present, the available evidence implies an intriguing story in which Pre-Tocharian became
is now confirmed in 20 of 23 Afanasievo individuals in Narasimhan et al. 2018, as well as detached from PIE 2 by a long-distance migration across the central Eurasian steppe of
subsequent sampling of Yamnaya individuals.30 Kazakhstan ~3300–3000 BC when Yamnaya migrants founded Afanasievo in the Altai. Other
recent aDNA findings provide a possible explanation for why so much intermediate grazing
Turning to question 2, the Afanasievo culture of the Siberian Altai ~3300–2500 BC and the
land was bypassed. The clearest evidence for early horse domestication comes from the
attested Tocharian language in the Tarim Basin ~500–1000 AD are separated by three millennia
pre-agricultural Botai–Tersek culture of the North Kazakh steppe ~3700–3000 BC.11, 36, 37 The
in time and over 1000 kilometres in space. On the other hand, there is no viable alternative
aDNA evidence published by Damgaard et al. 2018 finds that Botai and Yamnaya/Afanasievo
scenario for a how a centum language became established—and seemingly stranded—on the
individuals differed greatly genetically. The Botai people did not have Steppe ancestry or either
far side of a vast area of Central, South-west, and South Asia, dominated by satəm Indo-Iranian
of its main subcomponents.26 As Botai–Tersek was also wholly unlike Yamnaya/Afanasievo as a
languages from the time the earliest of them was attested (as the closely similar Mitanni Indic
culture,11 it is unlikely that the groups spoke related languages.
and Vedic Sanskrit).8, 24, 31, 32, 33 The publication of a high-coverage genome of typical Yamnaya/
Afanasievo type, dating to ~2900 BC from Karagash in central Kazakhstan,26 bridges the Another finding of Damgaard et al. 2018 was that Botai horses were not the ancestors of
geographical gap between the main Afanasievo territory and the culture’s suspected Yamnaya other tested ancient populations of domestic horses.26 In other words, this implies a second
source. centre of domestication. Therefore, it remains possible, on the basis of the genetic evidence,
that a parallel domestication had taken place on the Pontic–Caspian steppe at approximately
For question 3, the answer yes is provisional—probably the best, but not the only possibility.
the same time, a possibility that further evidence will either confirm or rule out. However, at
That Tocharian is the second branch is not the universal view. Its separation was plotted
present it may be pointed out that the wild horse is a powerful and aggressive animal. Taming
further along the tree according to Hamp,29, 31 for example. As explained above, a tree model
an adult wild stallion is extremely difficult. Therefore, there are reasons of logical economy to
is more unambiguously suitable when speech communities split cleanly by migration followed
suppose that the horse was not domesticated independently by two groups living near each
by abrupt loss of contact across a geographic barrier, as was at least sometimes the case in
other (Yamnaya and Botai–Tersek) at about the same time, but easier to suppose that the
the linguistic prehistory of Oceania.11 An alternative scenario in which the rapid expansion of
complex skillset arose amongst one group and then passed between the central and western
a language forms a dialect continuum, followed by ongoing contact between neighbouring
steppe. In either case, the Yamnaya pastoralists of the western steppe may have found the
dialects with a high degree of mutual intelligibility, will complicate the picture.19, 20 An
specialized herders and hunters of horses on the central steppe difficult to displace or live
innovation could occur in the lexicon, sound system, or morphology of one dialect then spread
beside, so migrated well beyond them to unexploited pastures further east.
to others. If this occurs at a relatively early stage of linguistic prehistory—before most of the
[ 11 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 12 ]

Working hypothesis 3: The Beaker expansion and the genetic and linguistic heterogeneity of probably spoke Indo-European. The CWC groups east of the Rhine who came into contact with
the Beaker People Beaker People also had high levels of Steppe ancestry and therefore probably also spoke Indo-
European. The eastern connections of CWC and western genesis of the Beaker Phenomenon
The earliest Beaker package arose amongst speakers of a non-Indo-European language by
means that it is likely that there was a dialect difference.
the Tagus estuary in present-day central Portugal ~2800 BC. Beaker material was adopted by
speakers of Indo-European as it spread east and north from its place of origin.

14
C dates imply that the region around modern Lisbon was the home of the earliest Beaker
package ~2800 BC, including the eponymous ceramic vessel, archery equipment, daggers,
Working hypothesis 4: PIE 6, Corded Ware cultures, Germanic/Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian, and
and continued or renewed interest in megalith-related funerary practices.38–42, 94 The aDNA
Alteuropäisch
data published by Olalde et al. 2018 shows that individuals associated with Beaker material
were genetically heterogeneous. Most of the sequenced individuals associated with Beaker ~2800–2550 BC the region of Corded Ware cultures (CWC) in northern Europe—bounded
material found in Germany, the Netherlands, France, Switzerland, and Britain had high levels approximately by the Rhine in the west and the Volga in the east—was the territory of an
of the Steppe component. However, of the 32 Beaker-associated individuals from the Iberian Indo-European dialect continuum ancestral to the Indo-Iranian, Balto-Slavic, and Germanic
Peninsula, only 8 showed the Steppe component at all. None of the 5 Beaker individuals from branches. The separation of the Pre-Germanic dialect from Pre-Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian, and
Portugal had any. Most Iberian Beaker men did not have R1b Y chromosomes of any of the its reorientation towards Pre-Italo-Celtic, was the result of Beaker influence in the western CWC
subclades found in Indo-European-speaking areas. The Iberian Beaker People who lacked the area that began ~2550 BC.
Steppe component were of an ancestry closely related to Iberian Neolithic and pre-Beaker
One important finding of Ringe et al. 2002 (a version of whose tree model is Fig. 2 here) is the
Copper Age individuals sequenced in the same study.43, 44
difficulty encountered in seeking the place of Germanic within the first-order subgroupings of
After spreading eastward across Spain, the Beaker package spread rapidly ~2600 BC along the Indo-European. They offer the following plausible explanation, which takes on new meaning in
Atlantic façade and along the North-west Mediterranean to the western Alps. ~2550 BC the light of archaeogenetic evidence.
Beaker Phenomenon entered West-central Europe, spreading into areas where Corded Ware
This split distribution of character states leads naturally to the hypothesis that Germanic
cultures (CWC) were already established, within an area that can be defined as bounded by
was originally a near sister of Balto-Slavic and Indo-Iranian (possibly before the satem
the Rhine in the west, the Upper Danube in the south and Jutland and the western Baltic in
sound changes spread through that dialect continuum, if that is what happened); that
the north.40, 42 The Beaker Phenomenon reached Britain and Ireland beginning ~2450 BC41, 42, 45
at that very early date it lost contact with its more easterly sisters and came into closer
The Olalde et al. study found a 90% population replacement in Britain at this time, from British
contact with the languages to the west; and that contact episode led to extensive
Neolithic individuals lacking the Steppe component to Beaker associated individuals showing
vocabulary borrowing at a period before the occurrence in any of the languages of any
high levels of it.43 The four individuals sequenced by Cassidy et al. 2016 are consistent with a
distinctive sound changes that would have rendered the borrowing detectable. (p. 111)11
similar dramatic shift in Ireland:
Now that we know both CWC People and the Beaker groups in West-central Europe had high
1) a Rathlin Island cist burial with three Early Bronze Age men (2026–1534 cal BC)
levels of Steppe ancestry, but had followed different histories after their ancestors left the
showing a significant Steppe component, R1b Y chromosome; like modern Irish
Pontic–Caspian steppe, we can contextualize this realignment of dialects. As purely a matter
(Scottish, Welsh) population, the Rathlin men had genes for haemochromatosis, blue
of geographic correspondence, an early Indo-European dialect bloc giving rise to Germanic,
eyes, tall stature, lactase persistence;
Balto-Slavic, and Indo-Iranian strongly suggests the geographic distribution of CWC, especially
2) the Ballynahatty Neolithic woman (3343–3020 cal BC) showed no Steppe component, once we take into account the case for placing the origins of Indo-Iranian in Eastern Europe in
mostly Anatolian Neolithic ancestry (differing from the Neolithic admixture of the Hypothesis 5 below. The R1a Y chromosome also lines up suggestively with this subset of the
Rathlin men) and WHG admixture; overall she was less similar to the modern Irish Indo-European branches.
gene pool and more similar to the Sardinian.46
It has many times been pointed out that the geographic distribution of the Beaker
The Olalde et al. study’s British Beaker People were found to be virtually indistinguishable Phenomenon corresponds approximately, but strikingly, with that of the Ancient Celtic
genetically from its Dutch Beaker People.43 languages. Within the CWC area, the dialect shift that Ringe at al. envision for Pre-Germanic
on purely linguistic evidence has an exact analogue in archaeology. ~2550 BC the Beaker
The above evidence suggests that the first Beaker People along the Lower Tagus spoke a non-
phenomenon entered the CWC area from the west and was henceforth interacted and partly
Indo-European language inherited from earlier inhabitants of the Iberian Peninsula.47 As it
fused with CWC in West-central Europe, in that zone extending as far east as the Middle
then spread north and east, the package was taken up by groups with Steppe ancestry who
Danube and Jutland. These ‘Beakerized’ regions henceforth had reduced contact with non-
[ 13 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 14 ]

Beakerized CWC to the east, and entered a cultural sphere with non-CWC areas in the West Working hypothesis 5: Eastern CWC, Sintashta, Andronovo, and the attested Indo-Iranian
with Beaker culture, including the Iberian Peninsula, northern Italy, parts of France, and Britain languages
and Ireland. Linguistically, these developments may be plausibly interpreted as intensification
After Pre-Germanic reoriented towards Italo-Celtic, in the context of the Beaker phenomenon in
of contacts with Pre-Italo-Celtic and reduction of contacts with Pre-Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian.
Central Europe ~2550–2200 BC, the satəm and RUKI linguistic innovations spread through the
When the Beaker phenomenon reached the CWC area ~2550 BC, the linguistic result of the
remainder of the Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian continuum. The dialect(s) at the eastern end of CWC
ensuing cultural interaction was to detach Pre-Germanic in the west from the Balto-Slavic/Indo-
developed towards Indo-Iranian. The Abashevo culture between the Don and southern Urals
Iranian bloc to the east, bringing it into the Italo-Celtic sphere dominant amongst the Indo-
(~2500–1900 BC) is a likely candidate for the Pre-Indo-Iranian homeland. The Sintashta culture,
Europeanized Beaker groups.
east of the southern Urals ~2100–1800 BC, can be identified as a key centre from which an early
There is another linguistic phenomenon with a geographic distribution corresponding stage of Indo-Iranian spread via the Andronovo horizon of central Asia ~2000–1200 BC to South
strikingly with CWC and necessarily assigned to a time depth pre-dating the Late Bronze Age and South-west Asia by 1500 BC. That Indo-Iranian came as a reflux from north-eastern Europe
and emergence of the post-PIE 2 Indo-European branches as separate languages. This is the (rather than a direct migration from Yamnaya on the Pontic–Caspian steppe) is shown by the
phenomenon of the so-called ‘Old European’ or alteuropäisch river names. The linguistic European Middle Neolithic (EMN) ancestry present in Sintashta individuals and carried forward
earmarks of this early layer of place-names include IE word roots having to do with water and to Andronovo and South Asian populations.
most often surviving in Baltic languages and the merger of earlier ŏ and ăas ă, as occurred in
The hypothesis that the Sintashta culture was the homeland of Indo-Iranian (or specifically
Germanic, Balto-Slavic, and Indo-Iranian. Kitson describes the core geographic distribution of
Proto-Indic) developed and gained considerable acceptance on the basis of archaeological
these names as follows.48–53
and philological evidence alone, before supporting aDNA data became available.8, 56 The key
The contribution of river-names to this argument [about the PIE homeland] is that in archaeogenetic detail is that the signature for most Sintashta individuals has ~68% Steppe
Europe south of the Baltic and north of the Alps and Carpathians, between roughly ancestry, ~24% EMN, ~8% West Siberian HG. This profile is thus distinct from Yamnaya/
the Rhine in the west and perhaps the Don in the east, all ancient river-names are Afanasievo, which lacks EMN.1, 26 In this light, the Sintashta population cannot be explained as a
etymologically alteuropäisch. At least so say the hydronymists, and river-names in the area result of a primary direct migration by Yamnaya groups on the Pontic–Caspian steppe ~3300–
have been so intensively studied, and attempts to overturn the assertion have been so 2400 BC, as is the case with the primary migration giving rise to the Afanasievo population.
conspicuously unsuccessful, that I think we must take it as established.54 (p. 101)
This model finds further confirmation in archaeological evidence for the origins of the
Although Kitson is arguing here for a North-European homeland for PIE 1, the alteuropäisch Sintashta material culture, which indicates sources in eastern CWC, such as the Fatyanovo
evidence he cites suits PIE 6 (Germanic/Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian) linguistically as well as culture ~3200–2300 BC, Middle Dnieper culture ~2800–1800 BC, and most especially the
geographically. Abashevo culture between the Middle Don and southern Urals ~2500–1900 BC.56, 8, 59–61, 86, 92, 93
This culture is epitomized by Anthony, as ‘the easternmost of the Russian forest-zone cultures
The altoeuropäisch river names remain problematical and controversial for many linguists.
that were descended from Corded Ware ceramic traditions. The Abashevo culture played an
Mallory and Adams’s overviews of Indo-European studies do not include them as a meaningful
important role in the origin of Sintashta.’8 Abashevo is identified as the source of Sintashta
category.6, 7 Vennemann has argued that they are ‘Vasconic’, i.e. a prehistoric non-IE
metallurgical and ceramic traditions and stock-breeding economy, as well as the key detail of
language family ancestral to Basque.57 Kitson’s paper focuses on the alteuropäisch type in
the disc-shaped cheek pieces characteristic of the distinctive horse gear of Sintashta chariotry.
Britain,54, 55 where, like other parts of Europe west of the Rhine, they are found together with
Sintashta is widely credited with invention of the light-weight war chariot, with two spoked
unproblematically Celtic names. Kitson hovers somewhat, hinting at an interpretation of layers
wheels and tightly controlled two-horse teams. 56, 8, 59–61 The Abashevo people who moved
belonging to different Indo-European languages—at first one not on the branch leading to
eastward to found the Sintashta culture were attracted by abundant arsenic-rich copper ores
Celtic then followed by Celtic—but he appears to favour alteuropäisch evolving into Celtic
in Transuralia.59 That Abashevo was associated with an early stage of Indo-Iranian, or already
in Britain, rather than being replaced by it. This process would involve a reinterpretation of
specifically Proto-Indic, had been proposed on the basis of archaeological evidence together
obsolete and opaque names. One example is the recurrent British river-name Derwent, Welsh
with the linguistic evidence of ~100 Indo-Iranian loanwords in the Uralic languages and
Derwennydd. Its original form was PIE *Dreu̯ entiH2- ‘Running [river]’, attested widely on the
correspondences between Sintashta burial rites and Vedic religion.86 As I write now, there is no
Continent as Druentia, &c. (Pokorny *dreu- ‘to run’).58 Becoming opaque, the PIE river-name
Abashevo aDNA to confirm or contradict the expectation that its gene pool was the source of
was then reinterpreted as a meaningful Celtic *Deru-u̯ ent- ‘[river] with oaks’. This is a plausible
the genetic type found at Sintashta with Steppe + ~24% EMN ancestry.
enough explanation, and there is nothing in it requiring that the original river name was coined
in a language other than the PIE dialect that evolved into Brythonic Celtic. That genetic signature can be traced forward to sampled individuals of the Sintashta-derived
Andronovo horizon widely spread across Central Asia ~2000–1200 BC and, afterwards, to
genomes of probably Indic-speaking groups in Iron Age South Asia.26, 30 It is present in South
[ 15 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 16 ]
Centum PIE63
Asia today—at higher levels in the North of Pakistan and India and among Indic-speaking
and high-caste Hindu groups. A recently sequenced genome from the Harappan (Indus Valley p t k kw
Civilization) site of Rakhigarhi north-west of Delhi, dating ~2500 BC, shows no Steppe or EMN
(b) d g gw
ancestry, implying that this ancestry entered the northern Subcontinent later than that. The
Rakhigarhi man was of the ‘Ancestral South Indian’ type, more similar to the modern genetic bh dh gh gwh
profile common in southern India and amongst Dravidian speakers.111 Modern South Asian
mitochondrial DNA implies that the Bronze Age immigrants who introduced the Steppe+EMN
component were mostly men.62 Proto-Italo-Celtic (after Schrijver)78
p t k kw
Working hypothesis 6: Non-IE influence in the West and the separation of Celtic from Italo- (b) d g gw
Celtic
β δ γ γw
1. The Beaker phenomenon spread when a non-Indo-European culture and identity from
Atlantic Europe was adopted by speakers of Indo-European with Steppe ancestry
~2550 BC. Proto-Celtic63
2. Interaction between these two languages turned the Indo-European of Atlantic Europe _ t k kw
into Celtic.
b d g gw
3. That this interaction probably occurred in South-west Europe is consistent with
the historical location of the Aquitanian, Basque, and Iberian languages and also
aDNA from Iberia indicating the mixing of a powerful, mostly male instrusive group Michelena’s ‘sistema fonológico principal del vasco antiguo’ follows.
with Steppe ancestry and indigenous Iberians beginning ~2450 BC, rsulting in total
replacement of inmdigenous paternal ancestry with R1b-M269 by ~1900 BC.
Palaeo-Basque 74, 75
4. The older language(s) survived in regions that were not integrated into the Atlantic
fortes: _ t c ć k N L R
Bronze Age network.
lenes: b d s ś g n l r
¶NOTE. This hypothesis should not be construed as a narrowly ‘Out of Iberia’ theory of Celtic.
Aquitanian was north of Pyrenees. Iberian in ancient times and Basque from its earliest
attestation until today are found on both sides of the Pyrenees. The contact area envisioned is
A third parallel is shown in the table of Palaeo-Basque consonants as reconstructed by
Atlantic Europe in general and west of the CWC zone bounded approximately by the Rhine.
Mitxelena above. Opposed series of corresponding strong and weak consonants (fortes and
In separating from Proto-Indo-European, Proto-Celtic developed new features found also lenes) is also a characteristic of the Celtic languages, though it was not an inherited feature
in Palaeo-Basque that can be explained as the result of contact, including the four below. from PIE. Systems of fortis and lenis consonants are found in all medieval and modern Celtic
First, the weakening, followed in most phonetic environments by complete loss, of the PIE languages. This is the phonological basis of the mutations that are a pervasive characteristic
consonant*p in Proto-Celtic is now seen by several historical linguists as the result of contact of the grammars of the Celtic languages.79–81 Lenition is not regularly shown in written Ancient
with languages like the non-Indo-European Iberian and Palaeo-Basque/Aquitanian.63–69 That Celtic languages. However, in texts in the Roman alphabet, it is not uncommon to find
Iberian and Aquitanian were p-less is directly observable in the substantial remains of those etymological p, t, and k standing between vowels in Ancient Celtic forms written as b, d, and
ancient languages.70–72 Linguistic reconstruction applied to the modern Basque language also g, implying that phonetic lenition had already taken place. There is also a structural argument
indicates that Palaeo-Basque lacked *p.74–76 explaining the simplification of the Proto-Celtic consonant system from the Proto-Italo-Celtic
as the result of phonetic lenition: in other words, with the reflexes of PIE */bh dh gh gwh/
Secondly, when the series of stop consonants of Proto-Celtic is compared to that of that of
becoming Italo-Celtic / β δ γ γw/ in all positions, and the Proto-Celtic reflexes of PIE /(b) d
centum PIE and the reflex of this system in Proto-Italo-Celtic in the tables below, an overall
g/ then articulated as [β δ γ] in lenis position, the two series of consonant phonemes were no
reduction of 12 to 7 consonants is seen in the evolution from centum PIE or Proto-Italo-Celtic
longer distinctive enough to be kept separate, which resulted in reductions in the consonant
to Proto-Celtic.77
inventory at the beginning of Proto-Celtic. 95
[ 17 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 18 ]

A fourth detail in which the centum PIE consonant system and that reconstructed for Proto-
Italo-Celtic, on the one hand, differed from those of Palaeo-Basque/Aquitanian, Iberian, and
Proto-Celtic, on the other, is the phoneme */b/. This existed in PIE and in Proto-Italo-Celtic, but
was remarkably rare at those stages, occurring in extremely few words. For this reason, it is
printed in the tables above in parentheses. However, /b/ appears with great frequency in the
written evidence for Aquitanian and Iberian and is reconstructed as part of the basic consonant
system of Palaeo-Basque. */b/ likewise had high frequency in Proto-Celtic, as the high-
frequency centum PIE consonant phonemes */gw/ and */bh/ both became Proto-Celtic */b/.
All of the stop consonants eliminated between centum PIE and Proto-Italo-Celtic, on the one
hand, and Proto-Celtic, on the other, were absent also from Palaeo-Basque. Conversely, the
stop consonant */b/ that had been extremely rare in centum PIE and Proto-Italo-Celtic then
became extremely common in Proto-Celtic, as it was likewise in Palaeo-Basque. It follows that
an adult native speaker of a language with a consonant system like that of Palaeo-Basque
would find it easier to learn Proto-Celtic (or a least to master competently this central aspect
of it) than centum PIE or Proto-Italo-Celtic. This transformation of the consonant system is
consistent with a situation in which adult learners of this description were numerous and in a
position to influence Indo-European speech as passed on to the next generation.47, 77
The Steppe component has now been found widely in aDNA samples from Atlantic Europe
(Ireland, Britain, the Iberian Peninsula, and Western France).27, 43, 44, 46, 82–5, 112 It was not present
in the Neolithic or earlier populations of these areas.27, 44, 43, 46, 112 In the various sub-regions the
key transition period appears to be ~4500–4000 years ago.

Figure 3. Zones
of pre-Roman
Indo-European and
non-Indo-European
names in the
Iberian Peninsula:
the dashed green
line shows the
divide, pointing
out group names
with ‘Celt-’ and
the diagnostically
Celtic -briga ‘hill,
hillfort, major town’,
contrasting with
non-IE il(t)i. Some
IE (mostly Celtic) Figure 4. Map illustrating the mutually exclusive distributions of metalwork of the Atlantic Late Bronze Age and
outliers are shown evidence for ancient non-Indo-European languages around the western Mediterranean.
in green.
[ 19 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 20 ]

Samples from all sub-regions of the Iberian Peninsula now show a similar pattern in which the generations of men with Steppe DNA were exceptionally successful in producing offspring with
Steppe component appears at this period at relatively low levels and with a strong male bias indigenous Iberian women, who probably spoke an indigenous non-IE language or languages
(90%+ of the males with any Steppe ancestry have R1b-M269 Y chromosomes).27, 43, 44, 84, 85, 112 with a consonant system similar to that of Palaeo-Basque.
Although the Steppe component occurs in autosomal aDNA at somewhat higher percentages
On the other hand, the result of Olalde et al. for British Beaker People—which showed a 90%
in the North of the Peninsula, the pattern is found both in zones with evidence for Ancient
replacement of the British Neolithic population43—would not lead us to expect substratum
Celtic languages (that is the zone of place-names with Celtic brigā ‘hillfort’, Fig. 3) and where
influence on the language of the incoming group. However, the same study also detected
non-Indo-European Iberian and Basque languages predominated.47, 112 It is also the pattern in
a reduction of Steppe ancestry in samples of the Iron Age and Roman Period from South-
the modern gene pool of the Basque area, where R1b-M269 predominates amongst Basque
east Britain. One possible explanation is that this reflects a resurgence of surviving Neolithic
men today.
ancestry that had remained invisible in the remains available for the British Beaker Period and
As shown in Olalde et al. 2019, the changeover occurred ~2450–1900 BC, with no Steppe Bronze Age. That is what many archaeologists expect, as the high population-replacement
ancestry detected before that time. Within that interval, paternal ancestry of both types occur; figure was startling, seemingly inconsistent with the less complete break reflected in the
indigenous Y chromosomes and R1b-M269 are found in all regions of the Peninsula. Later material culture.90, 91 In other words, it may not be only in the Iberian Peninsula that there were
in the Bronze Age, there is only R1b-M269. That paper draws highlights the burial of a man favourable conditions in Atlantic Europe for indigenous languages to have had an impact on
and a woman ~2300–1900 BC in Tomb 4 of the important ritual site of Castillejo del Bonete, the incoming Indo-European.
which lies inland in South-central Spain. The 40–50 year-old man had Steppe ancestry; the
30–40 year-old woman had none, but a geneome resembling that of Copper Age Iberians.
Of course, nothing certain can be concluded about the first language of either individual. Working hypothesis 7: The origins of Basque
But this microcosmic vignette illustrates the genetic transformation that swept the Iberian
1. The prehistoric ancestor of the Basque language was already in SW Europe when Indo-
Peninsula at this time, and that very probably involved many individual pairings of men whose
European speakers with Steppe ancestry arrived in the Early Bronze Age (~4500–4000
first language was Indo-European with women speaking an indigenous non-Indo-European
BP).
language. In such cases, it is likely that one or both learned a second language as an adult.
Olalde et al. 2019 also find that Steppe ancestry continued to rise more gradually across all 2. That prehistoric non-IE language could go back to the First Farmers from Anatolia
regions after the Early Bronze Age, and they suggest that gene flow from Central Europe took (arriving (~7500 BP).
place during the Urnfield Late Bronze Age. This is not improbable, though Urnfield did not
3. However, Iberian aDNA shows Western Hunter-Gather (WHG) admixture—so a
reach the North or West of the Pninsula. Another possible factor is that individuals and groups
linguistic continuity from the Mesolithic or Palaeolithic cannot be immediately ruled
with more Steppe ancestry tended to enjoy social advantages and were consequently more
out.
successful reproductively over the course of the Bronze Age.
4. Comparative evidence for other pre-IE languages in Europe and Anatolia may help to
The situation is brodly analogous to parts of South Asia where R1a Y chromosomes, often
decide. In other words, was the language of the First Farmers related to Basque?
found together with Eastern Indo-European languages, are common also in some non-Indo-
European-(Dravidian‑)speaking areas.62 Similarly, the Old Indic from the Mitanni empire (in As formulated by Renfrew, the Anatolian Hypothesis of PIE origins envisioned Indo-European
present-day northern Syria and Iraq) survives only as the names of kings and gods and horse reaching Western Europe, brought by the First Farmers of Anatolian ancestry (arriving ~5500
and chariot terminology; the incoming warrior élite gave up their ancestral language for non- BC in Iberia), and their language then evolved into Celtic in situ there.96, 97, 100 By default, that
Indo-European Hurrian.86 It appears, therefore, that paternal Steppe ancestry was not enough would push the origins of Basque back before farming, to WHG. Now that archaeogenetic
in and of itself to determine the survival of an intrusive steppe language, i.e. Indo-European. evidence consistent with the steppe hypothesis is known and reveals in particular the arrival
A continued pattern of contacts with and influences from Indo-European-speaking areas of a large and dynamic group of newcomers related to other Indo-European speakers ~2500–
was also necessary.47 This pattern can be illustrated by comparing the survival of Palaeo- 2000 BC, the linguistic forebears of pre-IE Basque can be moved forward to the available
Basque/Aquitanian73 and Iberian languages87 in South-west Europe to the distribution of slot of the First Farmers. That is the least problematical solution. A key element of Renfrew’s
the metalwork that defines the Atlantic Bronze Age (~1300–900 BC)88, 89: these show a direct original reasoning remains compelling: if the First Farmers expanded across Europe from a
inverse relationship (Fig. 4).47 compact homeland in the Near East, it is likely that they introduced a single new language or
a single family of closely related languages. In that much, the new aDNA evidence has actually
The aDNA evidence from the Iberian Peninsula—specifically a widespread low level of the
confirmed Renfrew’s hypothesis in showing that the First Farmers in Europe were associated
Steppe component with a strong male bias27—is consistent with a scenario of substratum
with a specific genetic type.1, 2, 106, 107, 109
influence from the language of mothers. The pattern reflects a situation in which successive
[ 21 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 22 ]

Figure 5. Map showing developments before ~4500 BP: 1) primary outward migration from Yamnaya on
the Pontic–Caspian steppe, bringing with it the genetic steppe component and PIE 2 speech to found
CWC in northern Europe and Afanasievo in the Siberian Altai; 2) the Proto-Beaker package spreads
by sea from its origin in the Tagus estuary to Brittany and Mediterranean France. In Figures 5 and 6,
straight sans-serif type indicates archaeological cultures, italic sans-serif type indicates linguistic stages,
and straight serif type indicates genetic structure.

However, the Neolithic Iberians were not of pure Anatolian Neolithic ancestry, but also show Second, the building of megalithic monuments was a distinctive cultural feature that, to a
WHG admixture.44, 85 It could be possible to judge better whether or not a pre-Neolithic source certain extent at least, united Neolithic Atlantic Europe as a region and distinguished it from
in Western Europe is a remote possibility for the Basque language through further research in other parts of Neolithic Europe.40, 101 Where did this tradition come from? Were its origins pre-
three areas. Neolithic? Recent work by Schulz Paulsson supports the case that monumental earthen burials
began in North-west France ~5000 BC, then spread from the Paris Basin to Brittany (Carnac),
First, additional gauging of WHG admixture: as more genomes at higher resolution become
by ~4800 BC. The earliest megalith building arose there in the following centuries, then spread
available, we will gain a clearer idea of the degree of survival of pre-Neolithic population in
by sea. For Brittany this dating is early enough to allow that the ideology and practice might
South-west Europe. The structure as well as the quantity of WHG admixture is meaningful. Was
precede farming.102 Of course, these two points—WHG admixture and possible pre-Neolithic
it more often family groups of farmers who advanced into Mesolithic Europe or pioneering
origins for megalithism—are circumstantial and only suggestive rather than decisive regarding
single men? It is conceivable that mitochondrial DNA will reveal the reverse. Had there been a
possible WHG origins for the Basque language.
division of labour like that common amongst indigenous societies of eastern North America, in
which men hunted and fished while keeping crops was largely the domain of women, farming Linguistic evidence could be more decisive. In this last area, we might make progress with
knowhow might have spread with women who paired with hunters outside their kindred. a detailed comparison of Basque, the closely related Aquitanian, and typologically similar
Iberian, on the one hand, and traces of other non-IE languages, following back eastwards
[ 23 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 24 ]

Figure 6. Map showing developments after ~4500 BP: 1) reflux of from north-eastern Europe
bringing with it Proto-Indo-Iranian from PIE 6 and a genetic signature with ~68% steppe
component and ~24% MNE; this genetic signature and Indo-Iranian speech subsequently spread
from the Sintashta culture eastward to the Andronovo horizon and thence to South Asia; 2) the
reflux of the Bell Beaker Complex westward from west-central Europe, bringing with it a form of
Indo-European speech, probably Pre-Italo-Celtic.

along the route farming took from Anatolia—thus studying Palaeo-Sardinian, Etruscan and Conclusion
Rhaetian, Minoan and the pre-Greek words and names in the Aegean,103 Eteo-Cypriot,104 and
From about 500 BC written history achieves a much wider awareness of Eurasia and its
in Anatolia itself Hattic105 and Hurrian106, as well as the better known Semitic and Caucasian
peoples, opening a new window onto non-literate groups in the North. At this time the two
language families. Such a programme would certainly be a challenge and possibly fruitless.
most extensive branches are documented at opposite ends of the Indo-European world. In
There are known pitfalls and numerous widely disbelieved attempts to connect Basque with
the West, stretching from the Atlantic to Central Europe, was Celtic. Indo-Iranian extended far
several of these languages.75 It would require sustained effort to push past the superficial. But a
eastwards to the great mountain ranges in the middle of Asia—the Altai, Tian Shan, Pamirs,
rigorous project along these lines might uncover evidence for one non-IE language or family of
Hindu Kush, and Himalayas. As Celts and Iranian-speaking Scythians, the two branches met
languages spreading west with farming. Or, with the reverse outcome, such research could give
along the Danube. This cultural confrontation gave rise to the systematized Classical view of a
us a clearer idea whether non-IE languages in the West look fundamentally unlike those in and
northern barbaricum divided between Celts in the West and Scythians in the East.
nearer to Anatolia? Continuity or discontinuity? If the former case, that would be consistent
with deriving Basque from the language of the First Farmers. A clear case of the latter would be Archaeogenetic evidence published in 201826, 43 shows that the Botai–Tersek culture (~3700–
consistent with the hypothesis deriving Basque from a WHG language. 3100 BC on the North Kazakhstan steppe, credited with first domestication of the horse37,
110
) and the Proto-Beaker package (~2800 BC on the Tagus estuary in central Portugal) arose
amongst populations genetically unrelated to their contemporaries on the Pontic–Caspian
[ 25 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 26]

steppe. From this, it is inferred that the Botai–Tersek and Proto-Beaker People probably spoke Slavic/Indo-Iranian, then that was the scene of cultural fission and an emerging barrier. As
non-Indo-European languages. the passage quoted above from Ringe et al. 2002 implies, communication broke down to the
degree that the satəm and RUKI innovations were not communicated to western CWC. New
In Hypotheses 3 and 5 above, it is argued that the migrations that resulted in the vast extents
ideas and vocabulary came from the West, areas with Beaker culture and Pre-Italo-Celtic
of these two branches took place after ~2500 BC. They were not the immediate effects of the
speech. With this estrangement in the West, it is understandable the eastern groups with
primary gene flow from the Yamnaya Pontic–Caspian steppe ~3100–2700 BC, but secondary
CWC-derived cultures, such as Abashevo, turned their attention to the country beyond the
refluxes some centuries later, pushing out at either end of Corded Ware in northern Europe,
Urals with its abundant copper resources and tradition of intensive horse husbandry going
between the Rhine and Volga. In the West, an innovative version of the Beaker package
back to Botai–Tersek.
expanded, returning westward, including new movements into Britain and Ireland. These
issued from the Beaker–CWC fusion zone by the Lower Rhine. The genetic populations
associated with the Beaker Package were heterogeneous, and it is argued here that they were
Acknowledgements
linguistically heterogeneous likewise—beginning amongst probable non-Indo-Europeans in
Iberia, then adopted by speakers of Pre-Italo-Celtic PIE north and east of the Pyrenees, then This draft benefited from comments made by participants at the ‘Genes, Isotopes and Artefacts’
coming into contact with Pre-Germanic/Balto-Slavic/Indo-Iranian when they settled alongside conference held at the Austrian Academy of Sciences in December 2018, including Catherine Frieman,
Anthony Harding, Kristian Kristiansen, Sabine Reinhold, Natalia Shishlina, Martin Sikora, and Alan
western CWC groups, east of the Rhine. Apart from the ambiguous evidence of some probably Williams, also from Martin Richards who commented on a draft.
alteuropäisch names west of the CWC zone, most of the linguistic evidence associated with this
reflux is Celtic.
On the eastern end of the CWC, Abashevo can be seen as the main forebear of Sintashta on
the Transural steppe. Sintashta was itself the forerunner of the vastly extensive Andronovo
horizon of western Central Asia. That Sintashta’s founders represented a reflux of a group of References
Kunst, P. Kuznetsov, H. Meller, O. Mochalov, V.
Steppe background returning from northern Europe is indicated by a genetic profile ~68% 1 Allentoft, M. E., M. Sikora, K.-G. Sjögren, S. Moiseyev, N. Nicklisch, S. L. Pichler, R. Risch,
Steppe ancestry, ~24% EMN, ~8% West Siberian HG. Rasmussen, M. Rasmussen, J. Stenderup, P. B. M. A. Rojo Guerra, C. Roth, A. Szécsényi-
Damgaard, H. Schroeder, T. Ahlström, L. Vinner, Nagy, J. Wahl, M. Meyer, J. Krause, D. Brown,
The chief competitive advantages behind both the western and eastern reflux expansions
A.-S. Malaspinas, A. Margaryan, T. Higham, D. Anthony, A. Cooper, K. W. Alt, & D. Reich
were technologies of mobility. Whatever the appeal had been of the communal drinking of the D. Chivall, N. Lynnerup, L. Harvig, J. Baron, 2015 ‘Massive migration from the steppe
Beaker People, their ideal of warrior as bowman, and burial rites harking back to the Atlantic Ph. Della Casa, P. Dąbrowski, P. R. Duffy, A. V. was a source for Indo-European languages in
megalithic past, the Proto-Beaker homeland on the lower Tagus and the rapid expansion by sea Ebel, A. Epimakhov, K. Frei, M. Furmanek, T. Europe’, Nature 522, 207-11.
to Brittany and southern France imply an advanced skillset for the high seas. The ‘Maritime’ Gralak, A. Gromov, S. Gronkiewicz, G. Grupe, T. 3 Gimbutas, M. 1970 ‘Proto-Indo-European
Beaker culture arose from a deep background within interconnected Atlantic Megalithic Hajdu, R. Jarysz, V. Khartanovich, A. Khokhlov, Culture: The Kurgan Culture During the 5th to
cultures, which included those of Ireland and Britain. Advanced seafaring is something the V. Kiss, J. Kolář, A. Kriiska, I. Lasak, C. Longhi, the 3rd millennia BC’, Indo-European and Indo-
speakers of Proto-Indo-European on the Pontic–Caspian steppe had lacked, but needed to G. McGlynn, A. Merkevicius, I. Merkyte, M. Europeans, ed. G. Cardona, H. M. Koenigswald
take up new lands in Atlantic Europe. It is not an accident that it was with the Beaker People Metspalu, R. Mkrtchyan, V. Moiseyev, L. Paja, & A. Senn, 155–98. Philadelphia, University of
G. Pálfi, D. Pokutta, Ł. Pospieszny, T. D. Price, Pennsylvania Press.
that Steppe ancestry—probably together with Indo-European speech—first reached Britain
L. Saag, M. Sablin, N. Shishlina, V. Smrčka, 4 Gimbutas, M. 1981 ‘The Three Waves of the
and Ireland. Whatever else we make of the figure of 90% population replacement in Britain V. I. Soenov, V. Szeverényi, G. Tóth, S. V. Kurgan People into Old Europe, 4500–2500
between the Neolithic and Beaker Age,43 we must recognize a group capable of moving en Trifanova, L. Varul, M. Vicze, L. Yepiskoposyan, BC’, Anthropologie et archéologie: le cas
masse over rough seas within a limited time span. V. Zhitenev, L. Orlando, T. Sicheritz-Pontén, des premiers âges des Métaux. Actes du
S. Brunak, R. Nielsen, K. Kristiansen & E. Symposium de Sils-Maria, 25–30 septembre
The expansion in the east is analogous. But in this case the technological advance emerged
Willerslev 2015 ‘Population genomics of 1978, Archives Suisses Anthr. Générale 43,
for navigating the oceanically vast steppes. This is an advance that is not inferred but directly Bronze Age Eurasia’, Nature 522, 167–72. 2ed. R. Menk, A. Gallay, 113–37.
observable in the archaeological evidence for Sintashta as the epicentre of the horse and 2 Haak, W., I. Lazaridis, N. Patterson, N. 5 Mallory, J. P. 1989 In Search of the Indo-
chariot revolution. Rohland, S. Mallick, B. Llamas, G. Brandt, Europeans: Language, Archaeology and Myth.
S. Nordenfelt, E. Harney, K. Stewardson, London, Thames and Hudson.
In their symmetry, the Beaker expansion and that of Indo-Iranian chariot warriors were
Q. Fu, A. Mittnik, E. Bánffy, C. Economou, 6 Mallory, J. P., & D. Q. Adams (eds.) 1997
possibly linked, though indirectly, as cause and effect. If the proposal is correct that the zone M. Francken, S. Friederich, R. G. Pena, F. Encylopedia of Indo-European Culture.
of Beaker/CWC overlap in Central Europe was where Pre-Germanic separated from Pre-Balto- Hallgren, V. Khartanovich, A. Khokhlov, M. Chicago & London, Fitzroy Dearborn.
[ 27 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 28]

7 Mallory, J. P., & D. Q. Adams 2006 The Oxford 337, 957–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/ of Indo-European in Atlantic Europe, Celtic M. Hajdinjak, É. Harney, T. K. Harper, D.
Introduction to Proto-Indo-European and the science.1219669 Studies Publications 16, eds J. T. Koch & B. Keating, A. M. Lawson, M. Michel, M.
Proto-Indo-European World. Oxford, Oxford 16 Bouckaert, R., P. Lemey, M. Dunn, S.J. Cunliffe, 17–40, Oxford, Oxbow Book. Novak, J. Oppenheimer, N. Rai, K. Sirak, V.
University Press. Greenhill, A. V. Alekseyenko, A. J. Drummond, 24 Kroonen, G., G. Barjamovic, & M. Peyrot Slon, K. Stewardson, Z. Zhang, G. Akhatov,
8 Anthony, D. W. 2007 The Horse, the Wheel, R. D. Gray, M. A. Suchard & Q. D. Atkinson 2018 ‘Linguistic supplement to Damgaard A. N. Bagashev, B. Baitanayev, G. L. Bonora,
and Language: How Bronze-Age Riders from 2013 ‘Correction to: Mapping the Origins and et al. 2018: Early Indo-European languages, T. Chikisheva, A. Derevianko, E. Dmitry, K.
the Eurasian Steppes Shaped the Modern Expansion of the Indo-European Language Anatolian, Tocharian and Indo-Iranian’. Douka, N. Dubova, A. Epimakhov, S. Freilich,
World, Princeton, Princeton University Press. Family’, Science 342, 1446. 25 Lazaridis, I. 2018 ‘The evolutionary history D. Fuller, A. Goryachev, A. Gromov, B. Hanks,
9 Anthony, D. W., & D. Ringe 2015 ‘The 17 Heggarty, P. 2014 ‘Prehistory by Bayesian Phylo­ of human populations in Europe’, Current M. Judd, E. Kazizov, A. Khokhlov, E. Kitov,
Indo-European Homeland from Linguistic genetics? The State of the Art on Indo-European Opinion in Genetics & Development 53, 21– E. Kupriyanova, P. Kuznetsov, D. Luiselli, F.
and Archaeological Perspectives’, Annual Origins’, Antiquity 88, 566–77. http://antiquity. 27. Maksudov, C. Meiklejohn, D. Merrett, R.
Review of Linguistics 1, 199-219. https://doi. ac.uk/ant/088/ant0880566.htm. 26 Damgaard, P. de Barros, R. Martiniano, J. Micheli, O. Mochalov, Z. Muhammed, S.
org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124812 18 Chang, W., C. Cathcart, D. Hall, & A. Garrett 2015 Kamm, J. V. Moreno-Mayar, G. Kroonen, Mustafokulov, A. Nayak, R. M. Petrovna, D.
10 Anthony, D. W. & D. R. Brown 2017 ‘Molecular ‘Ancestry-Constrained Phylogenetic Analysis M. Peyrot, G. Barjamovic, S. Rasmussen, C. Pettener, R. Potts, D. Razhev, S. Sarno, K.
archaeology and Indo-European linguistics: Supports the Indo-European Steppe Hypothesis’, Zacho, N. Baimukhanov, V. Zaibert, V. Merz, A. Sikhymbaeva, S. M. Slepchenko, N. Stepanova,
Impressions from new data’, Usque as radices: Language 91/ 1, 194–244. Biddanda, I. Merz, V. Loman, V. Evdokimov, E. S. Svyatko, S. Vasilyev, M. Vidale1, D. Voyakin,
Indo-European studies in honour of Birgit 19 Garrett, A. 1999 ‘A new model of Indo- Usmanova, B. Hemphill, A. Seguin-Orlando, F. A. Yermolayeva, A. Zubova, V. S. Shinde, C.
Anette Olsen, Copenhagen Studies in Indo- European subgrouping and dispersal’, E. Yediay, I. Ullah, K.-G. Sjögren, K. H.Iversen, J. Lalueza-Fox, M. Meyer, D. Anthony, N. Boivin,
European 8, eds. B. S. S. Hansen, A. Hyllested, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Choin, C. de la Fuente, M. Ilardo, H. Schroeder, K. Thangaraj, D. J. Kennett1, M. Frachetti,
A. R. Jørgensen, G. Kroonen, J. H. Larsson, B. Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, V. Moiseyev, A. Gromov, A. Polyakov, S. Omura, R. Pinhasi, D. Reich 2018 ‘The Genomic
N. Whitehead, T. Olander, T. M. Søborg, 25–54. February 12–15, 1999, ed. S. S. Chang, L. Liaw, S. Y. Senyurt, H. Ahmad, C. McKenzie, A. Formation of South and Central Asia’, bioRxiv
Copenhagen, Museum Tusculanum Press. J. Ruppenhofer, 146–56. Berkeley, Berkeley Margaryan, A. Hameed, A. Samad, N. Gul, M. preprint first posted online Mar. 31, 2018; doi:
11 Ringe, D., T. Warnow, & A. Taylor 2002 ‘Indo- Linguistics Society. H. Khokhar, O. I. Goriunova, V. I. Bazaliiskii, http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/292581
European and Computational Cladistics’, 20 Garrett, A. 2006 ‘Convergence in the J. Novembre, A. W. Weber, L. Orlando, M. E. 31 Hamp, E. P. 1998 ‘Whose were the
Transactions of the Philological Society 100/1, Formation of Indo-European Subgroups: Allentoft, R.Nielsen, K. Kristiansen, M. Sikora, Tocharians?’ The Bronze Age and Early Iron
59–129. Phylogeny and Chronology’, Phylogenetic A. K. Outram, R. Durbin, E. Willerslev 2018 Age Peoples of Eastern Central Asia, ed. V. H.
12 Gray, R. D. & Q. Atkinson 2003 ‘Language- Methods and the Prehistory of Languages, ed. ‘The first horse herders and the impact of Mair, vol 1, 307–46. Journal of Indo-European
Tree Divergence Times Support the Anatolian P. Forster & C. Renfrew, McDonald Institute early Bronze Age steppe expansions into Asia’, Studies Monograph 26. Washington DC,
Theory of Indo-European Origin’, Nature Monographs, 139–51. Cambridge, McDonald Science 10.1126/science.aar7711 Institute of the Study of Man.
426, 435–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ Institute for Archaeological Research. 27 Reich, D. 2018 Who We Are and How We Got 32 Mallory, J. P. 2015. The problem of Tocharian
nature02029 21 Koch, J. T. 2013a ‘Out of the Flow and Ebb of There, Oxford, Oxford University Press. origins: An archaeological perspective, Sino-
13 Atkinson, Q. D. , G. Nicholls, D. Welch, & R. the European Bronze Age. Heroes, Tartessos, 28 Hrozný, B. 1915 ‘Die Lösung des hethitischen Platonic Papers 259. Philadelphia,: University
D. Gray 2005 ‘From Words to Dates: ‘Water and Celtic’, Celtic from the West 2. Rethinking Problems’, Mitteilungen der Deutschen Orient- of Pennsylvania. [www.sino-platonic.org]
into Wine, Mathemagic or Phylogenetic the Bronze Age and the Arrival of Indo- Gesellschaft 56, 17–50. 33 Mallory, J. P., & V. H. Mair 2000 The Tarim
Inference?’ Transactions of the Philological European in Atlantic Europe, Celtic Studies 29 Hamp, E. P. (with D. Q. Adams) 2013 The Mummies: Ancient China and the mystery of
Society 103, 193–219 http://dx.doi. Publications XVI, eds. J. T. Koch & B. Cunliffe, Expansion of the Indo-European Languages: the earliest peoples from the West. London,
org/10.1111/j.1467968X.2005.00151 101–146. Oxford, Oxbow Books. An Indo-Europeanist’s Evolving View, Sino- Thames & Hudson.
14 Pagel, M. & A. Meade 2006 ‘Estimating Rates 22 Schleicher, A. 1861/1862 Compendium Platonic Papers 239, Philadelphia, Department 34 Sturtevant, E. H. (with E. Adelaide Hahn)
of Lexical Replacement on Phylogenetic Trees der vergleichenden Grammatik der of East Asian Languages and Civilizations, 1951 Comparative Grammar of the Hittite
of Languages’, Phylogenetic methods and indogermanischen Sprachen. (Kurzer Abriss University of Pennsylvania. Language. Rev. ed. New Haven,Yale University
the prehistory of languages, ed. P. Forster & der indogermanischen Ursprache, des 30 Narasimhan, V. M. , N. Patterson, P. Press. First edition: 1933.
C. Renfrew, 173–82, Cambridge, McDonald Altindischen, Altiranischen, Altgriechischen, Moorjani, I. Lazaridis, M. Lipson, S. Mallick, 35 Oettinger, N. 2013–2014 ‘Die Indo-Hittite-
Institute for Archaeological Re­search Altitalischen, Altkeltischen, Altslawischen, N. Rohland, R. Bernardos, A. M. Kim, N. Hypothese aus heutiger Sicht’, Münchener
15 Bouckaert, R., P. Lemey, M. Dunn, S.J. Litauischen und Altdeutschen.) (2 vols.) Nakatsuka, I. Olalde, A. Coppa, J. P. Mallory, Studien zur Sprachwissenschaft 67(2): 149–76.
Greenhill, A. V. Alekseyenko, A. J. Drummond, Weimar, H. Boehlau; reprinted by Minerva V. Moiseyev, J. Monge, L. M. Olivier, N. 36 Anthony, D. W. & D. R. Brown 2003 ‘Eneolithic
R. D. Gray, M. A. Suchard & Q. D. Atkinson GmbH, Wissenschaftlicher Verlag. Adamski, N. Broomandkhoshbacht, F. horse rituals and riding in the steppe: New
2012 ‘Mapping the Origins and Expansion of 23 Mallory, J. P. 2013 ‘The Indo-Europeanization Candilio, O. Cheronet, B. J. Culleton, M. evidences’, prehistoric adaptation and the
the Indo-European Language Family’, Science of Atlantic Europe’, Celtic from the West 2: Ferry, D. Fernandes, B. Gamarra, D. Gaudio, horse, eds. M/ G. Levin, C. Renfrew, & K.
Rethinking the Bronze Age and the Arrival
[ 29 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 30]

Boyle, 55–65. Oxford, McDonald Institute for Uriarte, P. Lefranc, O. Lemercier, A. Lefebvre, 49 Krahe, H. 1964. Unsere ältesten Flußnamen, Press.
Archaeological Research. J. Lomba Maurandi, T. Majó, J. I. McKinley, K. Wiesbaden, Otto Harrassowitz. 60 Kohl, P. 2007 The Making of Bronze Age
37 Outram, A. K. et al. 2009 ‘The Earliest Horse McSweeney, B. G. Mende, A. Modi, G. Kulcsár, 50 Bichlmeier, H. 2011 ‘Einige grundsätzliche Eurasia. Cambridge, Cambridge University
Harnessing and Milking’, Science, 323: 1332– V. Kiss, A. Czene, R. Patay, A. Endrődi, K. Köhler, Überlegungen zum Verhältnis von Press.
1335, doi:10.1126/science.1168594 T. Hajdu, J. Cardoso, C. Liesau, M. Parker- Indogermanistik und voreinzelsprachlicher 61 Koryakova L. & A. Epimakhov 2007 The Urals
38 Case, H. 2007 ‘Beakers and the Beaker Pearson, P. Włodarczak, T. D. Price, P. Prieto, resp. alteuropäischer Namenkunde and Western Siberia in the Bronze and Iron
Culture’, Beyond Stonehenge: Essays on the P.-J. Rey, P. Ríos, R. Risch, M. A. Rojo Guerra, A. mit einigen Fallbeispielen (Moderne Ages. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Bronze Age in honour of Colin Burgess, eds. C. Schmitt, J. Serralongue, A. M. Silva, V. Smrcka, Indogermanistik vs. traditionelle 62 Silva, M., M. Oliveira, D. Vieira, A. Brandão,
Burgess, P. Topping, F. Lynch, 237–54. Oxford, L. Vergnaud, J. Zilhão, D. Caramelli, T. Higham, Namenkunde, Teil 1)’, Namenkundliche T. Rito, J. B. Pereira, R. M. Fraser, B. Hudson,
Oxbow Books. P. W. Stockhammer, V. Heyd, A. Sheridan, Informationen 95/96, 2009, 173–208. F. Gandini, C. Edwards, M. Pala, J. Koch, J. F.
39 Harrison, R. J. & V. Heyd 2007 ‘The K.-G. Sjögren, M. G. Thomas, R. Pinhasi, J. 51 Bichlmeier, H. 2012 ‘Anmerkungen Wilson, L. Pereira, M. B. Richards, P. Soares
Transformation of Europe in the Third Krause, W. Haak, I. Barnes, C. Lalueza-Fox, & zum terminologischen Problem der 2017 ‘A genetic chronology for the Indian
Millennium BC: the Example of ‘Le Petit- D. Reich 2017 ‘The Beaker Phenomenon and ‚alteuropäischen Hydronymie’ samt Subcontinent points to heavily sex-biased
Chasseur I + III’ (Sion, Valais, Switzerland)’, the Genomic Transformation of Northwest indogermanistischen Ergänzungen dispersals’, BMC Evolutionary Biology 17:88.
Prähistorische Zeitschrift, Band S, 129–214. Europe’, bioRxiv 135962. doi: https://doi. zum Namen der Elbe’, Beiträge zur 63 McCone, K. R. 1996 Towards a Relative
40 Cunliffe, B. 2010 ‘Celticization from the West: org/10.1101/135962. Namenforschung, Neue Folge 47/4, 365–95. Chronology of Ancient and Medieval Celtic
The Contribution of Archaeology’, Celtic 44 Valdiosera, C., T. Günther, J. C. Vera-Rodríguez, 52 Bichlmeier, H. 2013 ‘Analyse und Bewertung Sound Change. Maynooth Studies in Celtic
from the West. Alternative Perspectives I. Ureña, E. Iriarte, R, Rodríguez-Varela, L, der sprachwissenschaft­lichen Standards Linguistics I. Department of Old and Middle
from Archaeology, Genetics, Language and G. Simões, R. M. Martínez-Sánchez, E. M. aktueller Forschungen traditioneller Art Irish, St Patrick’s College, Maynooth.
Literature, Celtic Studies Publications 15, eds. Svensson, H. Malmström, L. Rodríguez, J.- zur ‚alteuropäischen Hydronymie’ aus der 64 Ballester, X. 2004 ‘Hablas indoeuropeas y
B. Cunliffe & J. T. Koch, 13–38. Oxford, Oxbow M.a Bermúdez de Castro, E. Carbonell, A. Perspektive der heutigen Indogermanistik’, anindoeuropeas en la Hispania prerromana’,
Books. Alday, J. A. Hernández Vera, A. Götherström, Namenkundliche Informationen Real Academia de Cultura Valenciana, sección
41 Fitzpatrick, A. P. 2013 ‘The Arrival of the Bell J.-M. Carretero, J. L. Arsuaga, C. I. Smith, M. 101/102, 397–437. (online unter: www. de estudios ibéricos. Estudios de lenguas y
Beaker Set in Britain and Ireland’, Celtic from Jakobsson 2018 ‘Four millennia of Iberian namenkundliche-informationen.de) epigrafía antiguas – ELEA 6, 107–38.
the West 2. Rethinking the Bronze Age and the biomolecular prehistory illustrate the impact 53 Schmid, W. P. 1998 ‘Das baltische Zentrum in 65 Ballester, X. 2012 Falas Indo-Europeias e
Arrival of Indo-European in Atlantic Europe, of prehistoric migrations at the far end of der alteuropaischen Hydronymie’, Baltistica Anindo-Europeias na Hispânia Pré-Romana.
Celtic Studies Publications XVI, eds. J.T. Koch & Eurasia’, Proceedings of the National Academy 33(2), 145–53. Lisboa, Apenas Livros.
B. Cunliffe, 41–70. Oxford, Oxbow Books. of Sciences Mar 2018, 115 (13) 3428–3433, 54 Kitson, P. R. 1996 ‘British and European 66 Jordán Cólera, C. 2005 Celtibérico, Zaragoza,
42 Cleary, K. & C. Gibson 2019 ‘Connectivity DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1717762115. River-Names’, Transactions of the Philological Ediciones del Departamento de Ciencias de la
in Atlantic Europe during the Bronze Age 45 O’Brien, W. 2004 Ross Island: Mining, Metal Society 94/2, 73–118. Antigüedad.
(2800–800 BC)’, Exploring Celtic Origins: New and Society in Early Ireland, Bronze Age 55 Nicolaisen, W. F. H. 2008 ‘On River-names 67 Koch, J. T. 2011 Tartessian 2: The Inscription
ways forward in archaeology, linguistics, Studies 6. Galway, National University of in the Scottish Landscape’, A Commodity of of Mesas do Castelinho, ro and the Verbal
and genetics, Celtic Studies Publications 22, Ireland. Good Names: Essays in Honour of Margaret Complex, Preliminaries to Historical Phonology.
eds. B. Cunliffe & J. T. Koch, 80–116. Oxford, 46 Cassidy, L. M., R. Martiniano, E. M. Murphy, Gelling, eds. O. J. Padel, D. Parsons, 233–8. Aberystwyth: University of Wales Centre for
Oxbow Books. M. D. Teasdale, J. Mallory, Barrie Hartwell, & Donnington, Tyas. Advanced Welsh and Celtic Studies.
43 Olalde, I., S. Brace, M. Allentoft, I. Armit, D. G. Bradley 2016 ‘Neolithic and Bronze Age 56 Kuz’mina, E. E. 2007 The Origin of the 68 Schrijver, P. 2015 ‘Pruners and Trainers of the
K. Kristiansen, N. Rohland, S. Mallick, migration to Ireland and establishment of the Indo-Iranians, ed. J. P. Mallory, Leiden Indo- Celtic Family Tree: the Rise and Development
T. Booth, A. Szécsényi-Nagy, E. Altena, insular Atlantic genome’, PNAS [Proc. National European Etymological Dictionary Series 3. of Celtic in the Light of Language Contact’,
M. Lipson, I. Lazaridis, N. Patterson, N. Academy of Sciences USA] 113/2, 368–73. Leiden/Boston, Brill. Proc. XIV International Congress of Celtic
Broomandkhoshbacht, Y. Diekmann, Z. 47 �Koch, J. T., with F. Fernández Palacios 2019 57 Vennemann, T. 1994 ‘Linguistic reconstruction Studies, Maynooth 2011, ed. L. Breatnach, R.
Faltyskova, D. Fernandes, M. Ferry, E. ‘A case of identity theft? Archaeogenetics, in the context of European prehistory’, Ó hUiginn, D. McManus, K. Simms, 191-219.
Harney, P. de Knijff, M. Michel, A. Mittnik, J. Beaker People, and Celtic origins’, Exploring Transactions of the Philological Society 92, Dublin, Dublin Institute for Advanced Studies.
Oppenheimer, K. Stewardson, A. Barclay, K. W. Celtic Origins: New ways forward in 215–84. 69 Vennemann, T. 2016 ‘Celtic as Vasconized Indo-
Alt, A. Avilés Fernández, E. Banffy, M. Bernabò- archaeology, linguistics, and genetics, Celtic 58 Pokorny, J. 2002 Indogermanisches European? Three Structural Arguments’, Celtic
Brea, D. Billoin, C. Blasco, C. Bonsall, L. Bonsall, Studies Publications 22, eds. B. Cunliffe & J. T. etymologisches Wörterbuch. 4th ed. 2 vols. from the West 3, eds. J. T. Koch, B. Cunliffe,
O. E. Craig, G. Cook, B. Cunliffe, A. Denaire, Koch, 38–79. Oxford, Oxbow Books. Tübingen, A. Francke. K. Cleary, C. Gibson, 503–32. Oxford, Oxbow
M. Ernée, M. Kuchařík, J. F. Farré, H. Fokkens, 48 Krahe, H. 1962. Die Struktur der 59 Cunliffe, B. 2015 By Desert, Steppe, & Ocean: Books.
M. Gazenbeek, R. Garrido Pena, M. Haber- alteuropäischen Hydronymie. Wiesbaden: The birth of Eurasia. Oxford, Oxford University 70 Untermann, J. (ed.) 1980 Monumenta
Steiner.
[ 31 ] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 32]

Linguarum Hispanicarum II. Die Inschriften Languages. Longman Linguistics Library. et rythmes de constitution des dépots Language: The Puzzle of Indo-European
in iberischer Schrift aus Südfrankreich. London, Longman. métalliques dans le contexte européen (XIIIe– Origins. London, Pimlico, 1998. First
Wiesbaden, Ludwig Reichert. 82 Silva, M., K. Dulias, G. Oteo-Garcia, VIIe av. J.-C.). Presses Universitaires de Rennes. published, London, Cape.
71 Untermann, J. (ed.) 1990 Monumenta F. Gandini, C. Edwards, M. Pala, P. 89 Gerloff, S. (with J. P. Northover) 2010 Atlantic 97 Renfrew, A. C. 1990 ‘Models of Change in
Linguarum hispanicarum III.: Die iberischen Soares, J. F. Wilson, & M. B. Richards Cauldrons and Buckets of the Late Bronze Language and Archaeology’, Transactions of
Inschriften aus Spanien. Wiesbaden, Ludwig 2019 ‘Once Upon a Time in the West: and Early Iron Ages in Western Europe: With the Philological Society 87, 103–78.
Reichert. The Archaeogenetics of Celtic Origins’, a Review of Comparable Vessels from Central 98 Renfrew, A. C. 1999 ‘Time Depth,
72 Gorrochategui Churruca, J. 1984 Estudio Exploring Celtic Origins: New ways forward Europe and Italy, Prähistorische Bronzefunde Convergence Theory, and Innovation in
sobre la onomástica indigena de Aquitania. in archaeology, linguistics, and genetics, II, 18. Stuttgart, Franz Steiner. Proto-Indo-European: “Old Europe” as a PIE
Bilbao, Servicio Editorial Universidad del País Celtic Studies Publications 22, eds. B. 90 Heyd, V. 2017 ‘Kossinna’s smile’, Antiquity 91, Linguistic Area’, Journal of Indo-European
Vasco. Cunliffe & J. T. Koch, 153–91. Oxford, Oxbow 356, 348–59. Studies 27, 257–93.
73 Gorrochategui Churruca, J. 2013 ‘Linguistisque Books. 91 Cunliffe, B. & J. T. Koch 2019 ‘A dialogue at 99 Renfrew, A. C. 2000 ‘10,000 or 5000 Years
et peuplement en Aquitania’, L’âge du Fer 83 Szecsenyi-Nagy, A., C. Roth... W. Haak, the crossroads’, Exploring Celtic Origins: New Ago?—Questions of Time Depth’, Time
en Aquitaine et sur ses marges. Mobilité & K. W. Alt 2017 ‘The maternal genetic ways forward in archaeology, linguistics, and Depth in Historical Linguistics, 2 vols, ed.
des hommes, diffusion des idées, circulation make-up of the Iberian Peninsula between genetics, Celtic Studies Publications 22, eds. C. Renfrew, A. McMahon, L. Trask, vol 2,
des biens dans l’espace européenà l’âge du the Neolithic and the Early Bronze Age’, B. Cunliffe & J. T. Koch, 192–206. Oxford, 413–39. Cambridge, McDonald Institute for
Fer. Actes du 35e Colloque international de Scientific Reports 7(1), 15644. Oxbow Books. Archaeological Research.
l’AFEAF (Bordeaux, 2–5 juin 2011), Aquitania 84 Haak, W., C. Rihuete-Herrada, C. Oliart, 92 Parpola, A. & C. Carpelan 2005 ‘The cultural 100 Renfrew, A. C. 2013 ‘Early Celtic in the West:
Supplément 30, ed. A. Colin, F. Verdin, 17–32. M.- I. Fregeiro Morador, V. Lull, R. Micó, G. counterparts to Proto-Indo-European, The Indo-European Context’, Celtic from the
74 Michelena, L. [Koldo Mitxelena] 1977 Fonética García Atiénzar, V. Barciela, M. Hernández, Proto-Uralic and Proto-Aryan: matching West 2: Rethinking the Bronze Age and the
histórica vasca, 2nd edn. (first published J. Jiménez Echevarría, D. C. Salazar-García, the dispersal and contact patterns in the Arrival of Indo-European in Atlantic Europe,
1961). Donostia–San Sebastián. R. Risch, J. Krause 2018 ‘The genetic linguistic and archaeological records’, The Celtic Studies Publications 16, ed. John T.
75 Trask, R. L. 1997 The History of Basque. history of El Argar and contemporaneous Indo-Aryan Controversy: Evidence and Koch & Barry Cunliffe, 207–217. Oxford,
London, Routledge. groups of the Southern Iberian Peninsula’, inference in Indian history, eds. E. F. Bryant & Oxbow Books.
76 Egurtzegi, A. K. 2013 ‘4. Phonetics and paper read at the European Association of L. L. Patton, 107–41. London, Routledge. 101 Cunliffe, B. ‘Setting the scene’, Exploring
Phonology’, Basque and Proto-Basque: Archaeologists conference, Barcelona 2018. 93 Lamberg-Karlovsky, C. C. 2005 ‘Archaeology Celtic Origins: New ways forward in
Language-Internal and Typological Approaches 85 Martiniano, R., L. M. Cassidy, R. Ó Maoldúin, and language: the case of the Bronze Age archaeology, linguistics, and genetics, Celtic
to Linguistic Reconstruction, Minority R. McLaughlin, N. M. Silva, L. Manco, D. Indo-Irianians’, The Indo-Aryan Controversy: Studies Publications 22, eds. B. Cunliffe & J.
Language Studies vol 5, ed. M. Martínez-Areta, Fidalgo, T.Pereira, M. J. Coelho, M. Serra, J. Evidence and inference in Indian history, eds. T. Koch, 1–17. Oxford, Oxbow Books.
119–72. Frankfurt, Peter Lang. Burger, R. Parreira, E. Moran, A. C. Valera, E. F. Bryant & L. L. Patton, 142–77. London, 102 Schulz-Paulsson, B. 2019 ‘Radiocarbon dates
77 Koch, J. T. 2016 ‘Phoenicians in the West and E. Porfirio, R. Boaventura, A. M. Silva, D. Routledge. and Bayesian modeling support maritime
Break-up of the Atlantic Bronze Age’, Celtic G. Bradley 2017 ‘The population genomics 94 Gibson, C. D. 2016 ‘Closed for Business of diffusion model for megaliths in Europe’,
from the West 3. Atlantic Europe in the Metal of archaeological transition in west Iberia: Cultural Change? Tracing the re-use and PNAS www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/
Ages. Questions of shared language, Celtic Investigation of ancient substructure using final blocking of the megalithic tombs during pnas.1813268116
Studies Publications XIX, eds. J. T. Koch, B. imputation and haplotype-based methods’, the Beaker Period’, Celtic from the West 103 Beekes, R. (with L. van Beek) 2010
Cunliffe, C. D. Gibson & K. Cleary, 431–76. PLoS Genet 13(7): e1006852. https://doi. 3: Atlantic Europe in the Metal Ages — Etymological Dictionary of Greek, 2 vols.
Oxford, Oxbow Books. org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1006852 questions of shared language, Celtic Studies Leiden Indo-European Etymological
78 Schrijver, P. 2012 ‘The Origin of Celtic: How, 86 Parpola, A. 2015 The Roots of Hinduism: Publications XIX, eds. Koch, Cunliffe, Cleary, Dictionary Series 10. Leiden and Boston, Brill.
When, Where?’ The Anders Ahlqvist Lecture The Early Aryans and The Indus Gibson, 83–110. Oxford, Oxbow Books. 104 Steele, P. M. 2013 A linguistic history of
12/6/2012 (handout), Helsinki. Civilization. Oxford, Oxford University 95 Schrijver, P. 2016 ‘Sound change: The Ancient Cyprus. Cambridge, Cambridge
79 Lewis, H., & H. Pedersen 1989 A Concise Press. Italo-Celtic linguistic unity, and the Italian University Press.
Comparative Celtic Grammar. 3rd edn. 87 Hoz, J. de 2011 Historia lingüística de homeland of Celtic’, Celtic from the West 105 Ivanov, V. V. 1985 ‘On the Relationship
Göttingen, Vandenhoeck and Ruprecht. First la Península Ibérica en la Antigüedad 3. Atlantic Europe in the Metal Ages. of Hattic to the Northwest Caucasian
published, 1937. II: El mundo ibérico prerromano y la Questions of shared language. Celtic Languages’, Drevnyaya Anatoliya – Ancient
80 Ball, M. J. & J. Fife, eds. 1993 The Celtic indoeuropeización. Madrid, Consejo Studies Publications XIX, eds J. T. Koch, B. Anatolia, eds. B. B. Piotrovskij, V. V. Ivanov, &
Languages. Routledge Language Family Superior de Investigaciones Científicas. Cunliffe, C. D. Gibson & K. Cleary, 489–502. V.G. Ardzinba, 25–59. Moscow, Nauka.
Descriptions. London, New York: Routledge. 88 Milcent, P.-Y. 2012 Le temps des élites en Oxford, Oxbow Books. 106 Wegner, I. 2000 Hurritisch, eine Einführung.
81 Russell, P. 1995 An Introduction to the Celtic Gaule atlantique: chronologie des mobiliers 96 Renfrew, A. C. 1987 Archaeology and Wiesbaden, Harassowitz.
[ 33] Koch | draft Formation of the Indo-European branches [ 34]
107 Mathieson, I., S. Alpaslan-Roodenberg, C. Current Biology 26, 270–5 http://dx.doi. V. S. Gonçalves, E. Guerra-Doce, A. M.
Posth, A. Szécsényi-Nagy4, N. Rohland, S. org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.019 Herrero-Corral, J. Juan-Cabanilles, D. López-
Mallick, I. Olalde, N. Broomandkhoshbacht, 110 Anthony, D. W., & D. R. Brown 2011 ‘The Reyes, S. B. McClure, Marta Merino Pérez,
F. Candilio, O. Cheronet7, D. Fernandes, Secondary Products Revolution, Horse- A. O. Foix, M. Sanz Borràs, A. C. Sousa, J. M.
M. Ferry, B. Gamarra, G. González Fortes, Riding, and Mounted Warfare’, J World Vidal Encinas, D. J. Kennett, M. B. Richards,
W. Haak, E. Harney, E. Jones, D. Keating, Prehist 24:131–160. DOI 10.1007/s10963- K. W. Alt, W. Haak, R. Pinhasi, Carles
B. Krause-Kyora, I. Kucukkalipci, M. 011-9051-9 Lalueza-Fox, D. Reich 2019 ‘The genomic
Michel, A. Mittnik, K. Nägele, M. Novak, J, 111 Friese, K. 2018 ‘4500-year-old DNA from history of the Iberian Peninsula over the
Oppenheimer, N. Patterson, S. Pfrengle, K. Rakhigarhi reveals evidence that will past 8000 years’, Science 363, 1230–1234.
Sirak, K. Stewardson, S. Vai, S. Alexandrov, K. unsettle Hindutva nationalists’, India Today
W. Alt, R. Andreescu, D. Antonović, A. Ash, N. 31 August, also online: www.indiatoday.
Atanassova, K. Bacvarov, M. Balázs Gusztáv, in/magazine/cover-story/story/20180910-
H. Bocherens, M. Bolus, A. Boroneanţ, rakhigarhi-dna-study-findings-indus-valley-
Y. Boyadzhiev, A. Budnik, J. Burmaz, S. civilisation-1327247-2018-08-31 (accessed
Chohadzhiev,N. J. Conard, R. Cottiaux, Maja February 2019)
Čuka, C. Cupillard, D. G. Drucker, N. Elenski, 112 Olalde, I., S. Mallick, N. Patterson, N.
M.l Francken, B. Galabova, G. Ganetsovski, B. Rohland, V. Villalba-Mouco, M. Silva, K.
Gély, T. Hajdu, V. Handzhyiska, K. Harvati25, Dulias, C. J. Edwards, F. Gandini, M. Pala, P.
T. Higham, S. Iliev, I. Janković, I. Karavanić, Soares, M. Ferrando-Bernal, N. Adamski,
D. J. Kennett, D. Komšo, A. Kozak, D. Labuda, N. Broomandkhoshbacht, O. Cheronet, B.
M. Lari, C. Lazar, M. Leppek, K. Leshtakov, J. Culleton, D. Fernandes, A. M. Lawson,
D. Lo Vetro5, Dž. Los, I. Lozanov, M. Malina, M. Mah, J. Oppenheimer, K. Stewardson,
F.Martini, K. McSweeney, H. Meller, Marko Z. Zhang, J. M. Jiménez Arenas, I. J. Toro
Menđušić, P. Mirea, V. Moiseyev, V. Petrova, Moyano, D. C. Salazar-García, P. Castanyer,
T. D. Price, A. Simalcsik, Luca Sineo, M. Šlaus, M. Santos, J. Tremoleda, M. Lozano, P.
V. Slavchev, P. Stanev, Andrej Starović, T. García Borja, J. Fernández-Eraso, J. A.
Szeniczey, S. Talamo, M. Teschler-Nicola, Mujika-Alustiza, C. Barroso, F. J. Bermúdez,
C. Thevenet, I. Valchev, F. Valentin, S. E. Viguera Mínguez, J. Burch, N. Coromina,
Vasilyev, F. Veljanovska, S. Venelinova, E. D. Vivó, A. Cebrià, J. M. Fullola, O. García-
Veselovskaya, B. Viola, C. Virag, J. Zaninović, Puchol, J. I. Morales, F. Xa. Oms, T. Majó,
S. Zäuner, P. W. Stockhammer, G. Catalano, R. J. M. Vergès, A. Díaz-Carvajal, I.Ollich-
Krauß, D. Caramelli, G. Zariŋa, B. Gaydarska, Castanyer, F. J. López-Cachero, A. M. Silva,
M. Lillie, A. G. Nikitin, I. Potekhina, A. C. Alonso-Fernández, G. Delibes de Castro,
Papathanasiou, D. Borić, C. Bonsall, J. Krause, J. Jiménez Echevarría, A. Moreno-Márquez,
R. Pinhasi, & D. Reich 2018 ‘The genomic G. P. Berlanga, P. Ramos-García, J. Ramos-
history of southeastern Europe’, Nature Muñoz, E. Vijande Vila, G. Aguilella Arzo,
doi:10.1038/nature25778 Á.l Esparza Arroyo, K. T. Lillios, J. Mack, J.
108 Skoglund, P., H. Malmström, M. Raghavan, Velasco-Vázquez, A. Waterman, L. Benítez de
J. Storå, P. Hall, E. Willerslev, M. T. Gilbert, Lugo Enrich, M. Benito Sánchez, B. Agustí,
A. Götherström, M. Jakobsson 2012 ‘Origins F. Codina, G. de Prado, A. Estalrrich, Á.
and genetic legacy of Neolithic farmers and Fernández Flores, C. Finlayson, G. Finlayson,
hunter-gatherers in Europe’, Science 336, S. Finlayson, F. Giles-Guzmán, A. Rosas, V.
466–9. Barciela González, G. García Atiénzar, M.
109 Omrak, A., T. Günther, C. Valdiosera, E. M. S. Hernández Pérez, A. Llanos, Y. Carrión
Svensson, H. Malmströmm, H. Kiesewetter, Marco, I. Collado Beneyto, D. López-Serrano,
W. Aylward, J. Storå, M. Jakobsson, & A, M. Sanz Tormo, A. C. Valera62, Concepción
Götherström 2016 ‘Genomic Evidence Blasco43, C. Liesau, P. Ríos, J. Daura, M. J.
Establishes Anatolia as the Source of de Pedro Michó, A. A. Diez-Castillo, R. Flores
the European Neolithic Gene Pool’, Fernández, J. Francès Farré, R. Garrido-Pena,

You might also like