Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Behavioral Neuroscience © 2009 American Psychological Association

2009, Vol. 123, No. 3, 624 – 630 0735-7044/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015455

A Behavioral Analysis of the Role of CA3 and CA1 Subcortical Efferents


During Classical Fear Conditioning

Michael R. Hunsaker, Giang T. Tran, and Raymond P. Kesner


University of Utah

It has been proposed that the hippocampus and subcortical structures interact during the processing of
fear and anxiety-related information. It has been demonstrated that the subcortical efferents from CA3
and CA1 can be selectively disrupted without concomitant disruption to the afferents. The present
experiment was designed to evaluate the role of CA3 efferents via the fimbria and the CA1 efferents via
the dorsal fornix for encoding and consolidation/retrieval of classical fear conditioning. The present data
suggest that the subcortical projections from CA3 and CA1 are differentially involved in the processing
of classical fear conditioning, with CA3 subcortical efferents supporting acquisition of both cued and
contextual fear but only supporting retention of contextual fear and CA1 subcortical efferents supporting
the encoding and retrieval of both cued and contextual fear. These data further suggest that all
hippocampal efferents are not homogeneous, and that the hippocampus and subcortex interact to process
conditioned fear.

Keywords: dorsal fornix, fimbria, hippocampus, septum, classical fear conditioning

It has been proposed that septum and hippocampus interact to CA3 subcortical efferents project via the fimbria to the lateral
modulate anxiety and fear (Gray & McNaughton, 2000; Sheehan, septum, medial septum, and diagonal band of Broca. CA1 subcor-
Chambers, & Russell, 2004). The effects of lesions within the tical efferents project via the dorsal fornix to the lateral septum,
hippocampal-septal system are mixed—with some reports empha- medial septum, and diagonal band of Broca, as well as to the
sizing an interaction between the hippocampus and septal nuclei, mammillary bodies and other structures along the Papez circuit
and others suggesting that the hippocampus and septal nuclei act (Raisman, Cowan, & Powell, 1966; Swanson & Cowan, 1977;
independently (Bannerman, Matthews, Deacon, & Rawlins, 2001; Wyss, Swanson, & Cowan, 1980). More specifically, CA3 projects
Conejo, Lopez, Cantora, Gonzalez-Pardo, Lopez, & Begega, 2005; to the septofimbrial nucleus, throughout the rostral lateral septum,
Gray & McNaughton, 1983; Maren & Fanselow, 1997; Phillips & the dorsal portion of the medial septum, as well as the dorsal-most
LeDoux, 1995; Sparks & LeDoux, 1995; Thomas, Yadin, & portions of the vertical limb of the diagonal band of Broca. CA1
Strickland, 1991; Vouimba, Garcia, & Jaffard, 1999). In addition,
projects to the septofimbrial nucleus, the ventral portions of the
in studies of the direct interaction between the hippocampus and
medial septum, and the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of
septum, full fimbria/fornix lesions are usually employed, which pre-
Broca. There are only sparse projections to the ventral-most por-
clude analysis of the independent contributions of afferent and effer-
ent pathways (cf. Maren & Fanselow, 1997; Phillips & LeDoux, tion of the caudal lateral septum. These differential targets of CA3
1995). Despite these difficulties, it has been clearly demonstrated and CA1 subcortical efferents suggest that there may be functional
that the medial and lateral septum process conditioned fear heterogeneity between these two sets of efferents (cf. Hunsaker,
(Calandreau, Jaffard, & Desmendt, 2007), but they do so in qual- Tran, & Kesner, 2008; Sheehan et al., 2004).
itatively different ways from the contributions of the dorsal or To investigate the roles of these two subcortical efferent path-
ventral hippocampus (Bannerman et al., 2001; Hunsaker & ways for the encoding and retrieval of classical fear conditioning,
Kesner, 2008). To date, the role of the hippocampal-septal effer- we have developed a transection paradigm whereby we selectively
ents via the fimbria and dorsal fornix has not been specifically disrupt hippocampal efferents to the septum while leaving the
evaluated for fear-related information processing. afferents from the septum intact. In this experiment, one group of
animals had CA3 subcortical efferents in the fimbria transected,
but afferents left intact. A second group of animals had CA1
Michael R. Hunsaker, Giang T. Tran, and Raymond P. Kesner, Depart- subcortical efferents in the dorsal fornix similarly transected (cf.
ment of Psychology, University of Utah. Hunsaker, Allan, & Kesner, 2007; Hunsaker, Rogers, & Kesner,
Michael R. Hunsaker is now affiliated with Program in Neuroscience, 2007; Hunsaker et al., 2008). This experiment was designed to
University of California, Davis. better characterize the interaction between hippocampal efferent
This research was supported by NSF Grant: IBN-0135273 and NIH
pathways and their septal targets during tests of learning and
Grant: R01MH065314 awarded to R. P. K. We thank Adam Bevan and
Arik Hone for collecting pilot data.
memory. After transecting these pathways, animals were run on a
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Raymond P. classical fear-conditioning paradigm to evaluate the effects of the
Kesner, Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 South 1530 East, transections for the encoding and consolidation/retrieval of tone-
Room 502, Salt Lake City, UT 84112. E-mail: rpkesner@behsci.utah.edu cued and contextual fear.
624
DORSAL FORNIX AND FIMBRIA IN FEAR CONDITIONING 625

Materials and Method the blade was protracted such that potentials in the lateral septum
from CA3 stimulation were attenuated but the potentials in CA3
Subjects from lateral septal stimulation were unchanged. It was then veri-
Sixteen male Long–Evans rats, approximately 4 months of age fied that stimulation of the lateral septum was able to evoke
and weighing 280 to 350 g at the start of the experiment served as responses in CA3. This was then performed on the other hemi-
subjects. The colony room was maintained on a 12-hr light/dark sphere, resulting in a bilateral transection of the fimbria. The
cycle. All rats had ad libitum access to water and maintained at location of the partial fimbria transaction was chosen such that
90% to 95% of their free feeding weight. All experimental proto- CA3, the lateral septum, thalamic nuclei, and the dorsal fornix
cols conformed to Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee remained undisturbed.
(IACUC) and Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal After surgery, animals were placed in their cages on a heating
Care (AAALAC) regulations. Before participating in the present pad for 1 to 2 hours to recover and given sweetened, crushed food,
experiment, all animals had participated in a study involving a as well as given acetaminophen in their water (Children’s Tylenol;
modified Hebb Williams maze (Hunsaker et al., 2008). 50 ml/100 ml water) for 3 days after surgery as an analgesic.
Animals were given 2 weeks to recover from surgery before
starting any experimentation.
Surgery
Male Long–Evans rats received a transection of CA3 subcortical
Behavioral Apparatus and Procedure
efferents in the fimbria (n ⫽ 5), a transection of CA1 subcortical
efferents in the dorsal fornix (n ⫽ 5), or a control surgery (n ⫽ 6; Experimental apparatus. Two chambers were used during 3
fimbria control n ⫽ 3; dorsal fornix control n ⫽ 3). The transection days of testing. The first chamber was used for encoding and for
protocols used for these same animals have been detailed else- the contextual fear retention test. One chamber (28 ⫻ 21 ⫻ 22 cm;
where (Hunsaker et al., 2007a,b, 2008). Briefly, animals were Coulbourn Instruments; Allentown, PA) consisted of two transpar-
anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine and xylazine (65 mg/kg; ent Plexiglas walls (rear wall and front door) and two aluminum
10 mg/kg) and for dorsal fornix transections twisted bipolar stim- sidewalls. The chamber floor was made up of 18 steel rods
ulating/recording electrodes were lowered into the medial septum connected to a shock delivery apparatus used to deliver the shock.
(AP ⫹ 0.7, ML 0.1, DV ⬃4 –5—all at a slight angle away from the The chamber was surrounded by five small dolls and there were
midline (⬃5 degrees) to avoid rupturing any vasculature along posters on the walls to provide a clear context. Before and after
bregma) and CA1 (AP ⫺3.0, ML 1, DV ⬃1.5–2). A ground screw conditioning, the chamber was cleaned with an unscented cleaning
was secured in the skull above the rostral cortex in lowered to solution to remove any olfactory cues an animal may have left
come in contact with dura. After dura was carefully punctured to during conditioning.
allow easy penetration of electrodes, potentials were alternately The second observation chamber was used during tests of
evoked in the medial septum from CA1 and in CA1 from the auditory-cued fear retention. This chamber (32 cm2) was con-
medial septum. The placements of the electrodes were adjusted to structed entirely of transparent Plexiglas. No cues surrounded this
maximize this signal in both directions, and then remained un- chamber, only a green curtain and the table upon which the
touched throughout the remainder of the surgical procedure. A chamber was set. The floor of the chamber was covered with a
retractable wire knife was lowered stereotactically until it was single paper towel before placing the rat inside. Before and after
situated adjacent to the dorsal fornix (AP ⫺2.3, ML 0.5, DV ⬃2), conditioning, the chamber was cleaned with the same unscented
after which the blade was protracted such that it was between CA1 cleaning solution used in the other chamber to remove olfactory
and the dorsal fornix in the alveus. The knife was slowly raised cues.
until the medial septal responses evoked by CA1 stimulation were
attenuated. It was then verified that stimulation of the medial
Behavioral methods
septum was able to evoke responses in CA1. This was then
performed on the other hemisphere, resulting in a bilateral tran- Encoding—Day 1. Rats were placed in the conditioning cham-
section of the dorsal fornix. The choice of the location of the dorsal ber for 2 minutes before the first auditory stimulus, after which
fornix transection was such that there was never damage to CA1 or they received 10 auditory-shock pairings separated by 74 seconds.
to the fimbria, also such that the transection was as rostral as An auditory stimulus (10-s duration, 2 kHz, 85-dB) was presented
possible without risking disruption of fibers from CA3 to the through a speaker to initiate each trial. An electric foot shock (2-s
fimbria. Control surgery involved everything except the actual duration, 0.75 mA) was presented coterminal with the auditory
transection (i.e., the knife was lowered into place, protracted, and stimulus. A 74-s intertrial interval (ITI) separated each successive
then immediately retracted again). For partial fimbria transections tone ⫹ shock pairing. After 10 tone ⫹ shock pairings and subse-
electrodes were lowered into the lateral septum (AP ⫹ 0.6, ML quent ITIs, rats remained in the chamber for an additional 2
0.6, DV ⬃3– 4) and CA3 (AP ⫺3.0, ML 3.0, DV ⬃3.0). After dura minutes. A freezing response was measured by an observer who
was carefully punctured to allow easy penetration of electrodes, scored freezing behavior every 8 seconds during the preexposure
potentials were alternately evoked in the lateral septum from CA3 period and ITI (the 2 seconds after the shock were discarded and
and in CA3 from the lateral septum. The placements of the freezing during the subsequent 72 seconds was recorded) and
electrodes were adjusted to maximize this signal in both directions, every 4 seconds during the tone stimulus; resulting in two auditory
and then remained untouched throughout the remainder of the observations and nine ITI observations for each trial. The first nine
surgical procedure. The retractable wire knife was lowered to be trials of acquisition were blocked into 3-trial blocks for analysis of
just ventral to the fimbria (AP 2.3, ML 3.2, DV ⬃4), after which acquisition. Tone and contextual (ITI) freezing were also blocked
626 HUNSAKER, TRAN, AND KESNER

into a single 10-trial block for further characterization of any Results


specific tone or contextual fear acquisition deficits. It is understood
that the contextual measure is not completely dissociable from Histology
tone-related freezing, but previous research has shown that sepa- The results of the partial fimbria and dorsal fornix transections
rating tone and contextual freezing as we have here is a fair are depicted in Figure 1. More thorough anatomical, neurophysi-
approximation of separate processes (Hunsaker & Kesner, 2008). ological, and neurochemical analyses of the lesions of these same
Contextually fear retention test—Day 2 or 3. Each rat was animals have been reported elsewhere (Hunsaker et al., 2007,
tested for retrieval of contextually cued fear either 24 or 48 hours 2008). In short, for animals with dorsal fornix transections, the
after acquisition (half on Day 2 and half on Day 3, counterbalanced responses in the medial septum from CA1 stimulation were re-
with auditory-cued fear retention tests). The rat was placed in the duced by the transection, whereas the CA1 responses from medial
encoding chamber for 8 minutes in the absence of the auditory cue septal stimulation were unchanged. For animals with partial fim-
and shock for eight minutes (blocked for analysis into eight, 1-min bria transections, the responses in the lateral septum from CA3
trials). Freezing behavior was measured every 8 seconds. Because stimulation were reduced by the transection, whereas the CA3
of extinction in the control group, only the first 4 minutes of testing responses from the lateral septum were unchanged. Anatomical
were blocked and used for statistical analysis. tract tracing experiments using biotynilated dextran amine previ-
Auditory-cued fear retention test—Day 2 or 3. Each rat was ously reported by Hunsaker et al. (2008) revealed that the dorsal
tested for retrieval of auditory-cued fear either 24 or 48 hours after fornix transection disrupted ⬃50% of CA1 subcortical efferents,
acquisition (half on Day 2 and half on Day 3, counterbalanced with without significantly disrupting projections from the medial sep-
contextual fear retention tests). The rat was placed in the encoding tum into the hippocampus. It should be noted that since only 50%
chamber for 8 minutes in the continuous presence of the auditory of these efferents were transected, the dorsal fornix transection was
cue for 8 minutes (blocked for analysis into eight, 1-min trials). not complete. Partial fimbria transections disrupted ⬃63% of CA3
Freezing behavior was measured in 8-s intervals. Because of subcortical efferents, without significantly disrupting projections
extinction in control animals, only the first 4 minutes of testing from the septum into the hippocampus. Again, it must be empha-
were blocked and used for statistical analysis. sized that this manipulation was incomplete. In the case of either
transection, there was no disruption in acetylcholinesterase stain-
Histology ing in the hippocampus after the transection, additionally suggest-
ing that there was no large-scale disruption to the projections from
At the conclusion of testing, each rat was deeply anesthetized the medial septum into the hippocampus (Figure 1A–C). Detailed
with an injection of sodium pentobarbital (70 mg/kg i.p.), perfused anatomical, neurophysiological, and histochemical data that dem-
intracardially with PBS and 10% (wt/vol) formalin, and the brain onstrate the specificity and efficacy of these manipulations have
was removed from the skull and stored at 4 °C in a solution of 30% been reported previously (cf. Hunsaker et al., 2007a,b, 2008). The
sucrose in 10% formalin (wt/vol) for 72 hours before further reader is referred to those reports since all the animals in the
processing. The brain was frozen and 40 ␮m sections collected present study were those used by Hunsaker et al. (2008).
through the medial septum and hippocampus for Nissl and acetyl-
cholinesterase staining.
Behavior

Dependent Measures and Statistical Analysis Figure 2A shows the overall acquisition of classical fear condi-
tioning. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with groups (fim-
Freezing scores during encoding and retrieval tests were trans- bria transection, dorsal fornix transection, control) as the between
formed to percentages and were blocked for statistical analysis. A factor and blocks of 3-trials as the within repeated factor were
two-way repeated measures ANOVA with groups as the between
factor and blocks of three trials as the within factor was employed
for testing group differences during the encoding of delay fear
conditioning (e.g., the first nine trials of acquisition were blocked
into three, 3-trial blocks). The last trial was excluded arbitrarily to
keep all blocks the same size and to keep the number of blocks at
three, as has been presented previously (Hunsaker & Kesner,
2008). One-way ANOVA with groups as the between factor were
used to analyze encoding (single 10-trial acquisition block during
acquisition) and retention of contextual and auditory-cued fear
(single 4-min block during the tests) between groups. All main Figure 1. Histology. (A) Acetylcholinesterase (AChE) stained section
effects were considered significant at p ⬍ .05. Fisher’s least from a partial fimbria transection. (B) AChE stained section from a control
animal. (C) AChE stained section from a dorsal fornix transection. There
significant difference (LSD) post hoc paired comparisons tests
were no differences in AChE reactivity between control animals and
were performed upon all significant effects. Overall acquisition of
animals receiving either of the transections. In both (A) and (C) the
fear conditioning for each group is presented in three blocks of transections are signaled by arrows. (D) Diagram of a partial fimbria
trials, plotted as means ⫾SE. Contextual and auditory-cued fear transection after (A). (E) Diagram of a dorsal fornix transection. In both
encoding were collapsed into a single 10-trial block for analysis. (D) and (E) the largest (gray) and smallest (black) lesions are depicted. The
Retention tests are presented as mean percent freezing ⫾SE for reader is referred to Hunsaker et al. (2008) for more detailed neurophysi-
single 4-min blocks. ological, anatomical, and histochemical analyses of these lesions.
DORSAL FORNIX AND FIMBRIA IN FEAR CONDITIONING 627

Figure 2. Encoding and Consolidation/Retrieval of delay fear conditioning. (A) Both the partial fimbria
transection group and the dorsal fornix transection groups showed overall acquisition deficits relative to control
animals. (B) The partial fimbria transection and dorsal fornix transected animals showed deficits for the encoding
of tone and contextual fear during acquisition. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. (C) The partial fimbria transection group
showed no differences from controls for retention of tone-cued fear, whereas the dorsal fornix transected animals
showed large deficits. Both the partial fimbria and dorsal fornix transected animals showed deficits relative to
controls for the retention of contextual fear. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

performed on the data as plotted. There was a significant group Fisher’s LSD post hoc paired comparison revealed that both the
effect, F(1, 15) ⫽ 12.00, p ⬍ .002, an effect for blocks, F(2, 30) ⫽ fimbria transection and dorsal fornix transection group froze less
6.55, p ⬍ .006, and a group ⫻ block interaction, F(2, 30) ⫽ 7.87, than the control group ( ps ⬍ 0.01). The fimbria transection and
p ⬍ .001. A Fisher’s LSD post hoc paired comparisons test dorsal fornix transection groups did not differ ( p ⬎ .1). For context
revealed that both the fimbria transection and dorsal fornix tran- acquisition, there was an effect for groups, F(2, 15) ⫽ 96.17, p ⬍
section groups showed an overall acquisition deficit relative to .001. A Fisher’s LSD post hoc paired comparisons test revealed
control animals (all ps ⬍ 0.01). Fimbria transected animals and that the dorsal fornix transection groups froze less than the control
dorsal fornix transected animals did not differ ( p ⬎ .1). Despite the group ( p ⬍ .01). The dorsal fornix transection group froze less
deficit in acquisition, the transection groups did show some acqui- than the fimbria transection group ( p ⬍ .05). The fimbria transec-
sition and were not at floor levels. It also should be noted that no tion group also froze less than the control group ( p ⬍ .05). This
animals appeared either hyperactive or showed freezing behaviors suggests the fimbria transection group showed a lesser impairment
during the 2-min baseline period before acquisition, though quan- relative to the dorsal fornix transection group. Additionally,
titative data were not collected. though the dorsal fornix group showed impairments in acquisition
To evaluate whether it was a specific deficit in tone or contex- relative to the other groups, they were not at baseline freezing
tual fear encoding, the acquisition data were separated into tone levels. Their learning was impaired relative to the other groups, but
encoding and contextual encoding (Figure 2B), and the separated not absent.
data were collapsed into a single 10-trial block for analysis. One- One-way ANOVA with group as the between factor were used
way ANOVA with group as the between factor were used to to evaluate the tone and contextual fear retention data. The tone
analyze tone and contextual fear encoding. For tone acquisition, and contextual fear data were blocked into single 4-min blocks for
there was an effect for group, F(2, 15) ⫽ 45.98, p ⬍ .001. A analysis (Figure 2C). For the retention of tone-cued fear, there was
628 HUNSAKER, TRAN, AND KESNER

an effect for groups, F(2, 15) ⫽ 64.97, p ⬍ .001. A Fisher’s LSD septal lesions resulting in deficits for classical fear conditioning
post hoc paired comparisons test revealed that the dorsal fornix (Bannerman et al., 2001; Maren & Fanselow, 1997; Sparks &
transection group froze less than both the fimbria transection group LeDoux, 1995; cf. Calandreau et al., 2007 for an inactivation
and controls ( ps ⬍ 0.01). Fimbria transected animals and control experiment), but the present data do not match these previous
animals froze similarly ( p ⬎ .1). For the retention of contextual findings. It has also been demonstrated that fornix lesions result in
fear, there was an effect for groups, F(2, 15) ⫽ 96.25, p ⬍ .001. deficits for fear conditioning (Phillips & LeDoux, 1995). The
A Fisher’s LSD post hoc paired comparisons test revealed that present deficit pattern does not mimic those seen after dorsal and
fimbria transected animals froze less than control animals ( p ⬍ ventral CA1 and CA3 excitotoxic lesions (Hunsaker & Kesner,
.01), but froze more than dorsal fornix transected animals ( p ⬍ 2008), nor do they mimic the effects of full dorsal or ventral
.01). Dorsal fornix transected animals froze less then control hippocampal lesions (Bannerman et al., 2001). These data suggest
animals, as well ( p ⬍ .01). This pattern of results suggests that the that the current transections disrupted an interaction between the
fimbria transection group showed a lesser impairment relative to hippocampus and the subcortex, and did not result in specific
the dorsal fornix transection group. septal or hippocampal deficits per se. Further lesion and inactiva-
tion studies are necessary to better characterize the hippocampal-
Discussion subcortical interactions disrupted by these selective transections.
In previous reports (Hunsaker et al., 2007a, 2008), two animals
The present data suggest that although the CA3 and CA1 effer- received biotinylated dextran amine (BDA; an anterograde tracer)
ents into the lateral and medial septum (and CA1 efferents to the infusions 7 days postunilateral partial fimbria transection and two
subcortex) both participate in the acquisition of conditioned fear, animals had BDA injections 7 days postdorsal fornix transection.
they do so in a slightly different manner. This is reflected in the The results suggested that the CA3 and CA1 subcortical efferents
fact that animals without CA3 efferents to the lateral septum have project to separate populations within the septal nuclei (the study
some consolidation/retrieval of contextual fear and efficient con- did not investigate other subcortical structures aside from the
solidation/retrieval auditory-cued fear, whereas animals without septal nuclei). After infusions of BDA into CA3, there was stain-
CA1 efferents appear to have little to no memory whatsoever of ing present in the septofimbrial nucleus, throughout the rostral
the conditioned fear. Although the animals without CA1 subcor- lateral septum, the dorsal portion of the medial septum, as well as
tical efferents (the dorsal fornix transection group) as well as the the dorsal-most portions of the vertical limb of the diagonal band
animals without CA3 subcortical efferents (the partial fimbria of Broca. After infusions of BDA into CA1, there was staining in
transection group) showed deficits for acquisition, they were not at the septofimbrial nucleus, the ventral portions of the medial sep-
floor and did appear to acquire some cued fear, just less efficiently tum, and the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of Broca. There
than the control group. This is reflected in the fact that the animals was only sparse staining in the ventral-most portion of the caudal
froze more during conditioning than they did during the 2-min lateral septum. The differential targets of CA3 and CA1 subcorti-
baseline condition and the first trial of conditioning. cal efferents suggest that there may be functional heterogeneity
Although counterintuitive, it has been demonstrated that encod- between these two sets of efferents (cf. Hunsaker et al., 2008;
ing (or acquisition) and consolidation/retrieval processes are par- Sheehan et al., 2004). Additionally, during this anatomical tract
tially dissociable (cf. Aggleton & Saunders, 1997; Vann & tracing experiment, it was demonstrated that the transections of the
Aggelton, 2004). In fact, recent studies have shown similar phe- fimbria disrupted roughly 63% of CA3 efferent fibers the dorsal
nomena in primates and rodents with lesions to the fornix (Aggle- fornix transections disrupted slightly more than 50% of the CA1
ton & Saunders, 1997; Buckley, Eilson, & Gaffan, 2008; Eacott & efferent fibers.
Gaffan, 2005; Mitchell, Browning, Wilson, Baxter, & Gaffan, Unlike previous reports by Hunsaker et al. (2007a,b, 2008) that
2008) and in studies with lesions along the Papez circuit (targets of posited the subcortical outputs of the hippocampus were primarily
the projections in the fornix; cf. Vann & Aggleton, 2004). In fact, modulatory on the hippocampus via a feed forward inhibition
the same fimbria transection animals that participated in the system, the present data suggest that information processing within
present experiment previously caused acquisition or encoding def- the medial and lateral septum may be directly affected by efferents
icits on a modified Hebb-Willliams maze, without concomitant from the hippocampus during the conditioning of auditory-cued
deficits for retrieval (i.e., they had difficulty learning the task, but and contextual fear (cf. Gray & McNaughton, 2000; Sheehan et al.,
remembered what they had learned as well as the control group; 2004). Any differences between the effects of transecting CA1 or
Hunsaker et al., 2008). That is not to say that encoding and CA3 subcortical efferents may be attributed to the differences in
retrieval are completely dissociable, but it means that there can be the specific targets of the projections. It has previously been
intact retrieval after impaired acquisition, and vice versa (cf. demonstrated that there are dissociations between the medial and
Hunsaker et al., 2008). lateral septum for auditory-cued and contextual fear processing
It has been proposed that the hippocampus and subcortical (Calandreau et al., 2007). The present data suggest that the CA3
structures form a neural circuit that modulates anxiety in the efferents (primarily) to the lateral septum are involved for the
animal (Gray & McNaughton, 2000). It has also been determined acquisition of contextual fear, involved in consolidation/retrieval
that there are unique patterns of behavioral and cognitive deficits of contextual fear, but only involved for the acquisition of
after selective lesions at different locations within the septal- auditory-cued fear (i.e., the transection left auditory-cued fear
hippocampal circuit (Gray & McNaughton, 1983). The model consolidation/retrieval intact). Also of note is that the retrieval
proposed by Gray and McNaughton (2000) posits dynamic, real- deficits were never as severe as those displayed by the dorsal
time interactions between the hippocampus and septum during fornix transection group. The present data also suggest that the
some learning and memory paradigms. There are reports of medial CA1 efferents (primarily) to the medial septum (and structures
DORSAL FORNIX AND FIMBRIA IN FEAR CONDITIONING 629

along the Papez circuit) are involved during acquisition and (po- hippocampal subcortical efferents and their role in learning and
tentially) for the consolidation/retrieval of auditory-cued and con- memory processing should be reevaluated.
textual fear, though the present data are unclear on the nature of the
retrieval deficits, though these data are congruent with previous References
findings (Phillips & LeDoux, 1995). The present data do not lend Aggleton, J. P., & Saunders, R. C. (1997). The relationships between
themselves to a specific analysis of encoding as fully separable temporal lobe and diencephalic structures implicated in anterograde
from consolidation or retrieval processing as well as previous amnesia. Memory, 5, 49 –71.
studies (i.e., there needs to be some sort of measurable consolida- Bannerman, D. M., Matthews, P., Deacon, R. M., & Rawlins, J. N. (2001).
tion/retrieval to determine whether some learning had taken Medial septal lesions mimic effects of both selective dorsal and ventral
place—such as that seen in the animals with partial fimbria tran- hippocampal lesions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 1033–1041.
sections; cf. Hunsaker & Kesner, 2008; Hunsaker et al., 2008). Buckley, M. J., Eilson, C. R., & Gaffan, D. (2008). Fornix transection
An alternative explanation for the deficits seen after dorsal impairs visuospatial memory acquisition more then retrieval. Behavioral
fornix transactions may be that subcortical efferents from the Neuroscience, 122, 44 –53.
Calandreau, L., Jaffard, R., & Desmedt, A. (2007). Dissociated roles for the
subiculum were also disrupted by the dorsal fornix transactions—
lateral and medial septum in elemental and contextual fear conditioning.
not just those from CA1. These subcortical efferents from the Learning and Memory, 14, 422– 429.
subiculum also have been shown to project not only to the medial Conejo, N. M., Lopez, M., Cantora, R., Gonzalez-Pardo, H., Lopez, L.,
septum, but also to structures along the Papez circuit (Gray & Begega, A., et al. (2005). Effects of pavlovian fear conditioning on
McNaughton, 2000; Swanson & Cowan, 1977; Vann & Aggleton, septohippocampal metabolism in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 373, 94 –
2004), which may also have been involved. 98.
The present data add to the understanding of the neural circuitry Eacott, M. J., & Gaffan, D. (2005). The roles of perirhinal cortex, postrhi-
underlying affective and fear related learning, as well as provide nal cortex, and the fornix for objects, contexts, and events in the rat.
further insight into the subcortical projections of CA1. What the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, B, 58, 202–217.
present data suggest is that the accepted view of the hippocampus Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (1983). Comparison between the behav-
ioural effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: A review. Neuroscience
supporting contextual and the amygdala mediating both contextual
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 7, 119 –188.
and elemental (e.g., auditory) fear conditioning is at best incom- Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety:
plete (cf. Quinn, Wied, Ma, Tinsley, & Fanselow, 2008). These An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford,
findings also suggest that using fear conditioning as a test of UK: Oxford University Press.
amygdala function may be confounded by the presence of Hunsaker, M. R., Allan, K. D., & Kesner, R. P. (2007a). The role of CA3
hippocampal-septal (and hippocampal-subcortical) interactions efferents via the fimbria for the acquisition of a delay non-match to place
(cf. Conejo et al., 2005). Additionally, these data suggest that task. Hippocampus, 17, 494 –502.
hippocampal and septal interactions are involved during fear- Hunsaker, M. R., & Kesner, R. P. (2008). Dissociations across the dorsal-
related learning and not just during anxiety-related processing ventral axis of CA3and CA1 for encoding and retrieval of contextual and
(Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Of particular interest is the finding auditory-cued fear. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89, 61– 69.
Hunsaker, M. R., Rogers, J. L., & Kesner, R. P. (2007b). Behavioral
that disrupting subcortical efferents from either CA1 or CA3 was
characterization of a transection of dorsal CA3 subcortical efferents:
sufficient to disrupt acquisition of classically conditioned fear. Comparison with scopolamine and physostigmine infusions into dorsal
Retrieval of conditioned contextual fear was dependent upon sub- CA3. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 88, 127–136.
cortical projections from CA3 (the data do not allow any mean- Hunsaker, M. R., Tran, G. T., & Kesner, R. P. (2008). Dissociating
ingful interpretations for CA1 subcortical efferents other than that subcortical hippocampal efferents for encoding and consolidation/
they are critical for acquisition). The present study reveals a role retrieval of spatial information: CA1 from CA3. Hippocampus, 18,
for subcortical structures in both hippocampus dependent and 699 –709.
(presumably) hippocampus independent processing of conditioned Maren, S., & Fanselow, M. S. (1997). Electrolytic lesions of the fimbria/
fear. fornix, dorsal hippocampus, or entorhinal cortex produce anterograde
In the case of the present experiment, it appears that the hip- deficits in contextual fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiology of Learning
and Memory, 67, 142–149.
pocampus and subcortical structures interact to efficiently process
Mitchell, A. S., Browning, P. G., Wilson, C. R., Baxter, M. G., & Gaffan,
the auditory and contextual cues during the acquisition and re- D. (2008). Dissociable roles for cortical and subcortical structures in
trieval of classical fear conditioning. This suggests that the medial memory retrieval and acquisition. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 8387–
septum, in addition to sending cholinergic and GABAergic pro- 8396.
jection fibers into the hippocampus may potentially play an im- Phillips, R. G., & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Lesions of the fornix but no the
portant role in the formation of a representation of the tone and entorhinal or perirhinal cortex interfere with contextual fear condition-
context that are associated with an aversive shock. The lateral ing. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5308 –5315.
septum, it appears, also may have a role in the formation and recall Quinn, J. J., Wied, H. M., Ma, Q. D., Tinsley, M. R., & Fanselow, M. S.
of these representations. Additional subcortical effects of the tran- (2008). Dorsal hippocampus involvement in delay fear conditioning
sections may potentially contribute to deficits seen after the dorsal depends upon the strength of the tone-footshock association. Hippocam-
pus, 18, 640 – 654.
fornix transections. The medial septum has strong, reciprocal
Raisman, G., Cowan, M. W., & Powell, T. P. S. (1966). An experimental
connectivity with the entorhinal cortex and the amygdala, and any analysis of the efferent projection of the hippocampus. Brain, 89, 83–
alteration in the outputs from the septum to these areas may also 108.
potentially underlie the present effects (Gray & McNaughton, Sheehan, T. P., Chambers, R. A., & Russell, D. S. (2006). Regulation of
2000; Sheehan et al., 2004; Swanson & Cowan, 1977; Vann & affect by the lateral septum: Implications for neuropsychiatry. Brain
Aggleton, 2004). Overall, these data suggest that the role of the Research Reviews, 46, 71–117.
630 HUNSAKER, TRAN, AND KESNER

Sparks, P. D., & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Septal lesions potentiate freezing Vouimba, R. M., Garcia, R., & Jaffard, R. (1999). Pretraining tetanic
behavior to contextual but not phasic conditioned stimuli in rats. Behav- fimbrial stimulation impairs the expression but not the acquisition of
ioral Neuroscience, 109, 184 –188. contextual fear conditioning in mice. Neuroscience, 93, 869 – 876.
Swanson, L. W., & Cowan, M. W. (1977). An autoradiographic study of Wyss, X., Swanson, L. W., & Cowan, M. W. (1980). The organization of
the organization of the efferent connections of the hippocampal forma- the fimbria, dorsal fornix, and ventral hippocampal commissure in the
tion in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 172, 49 – 84. rat. Anatomy and Embryology, 138, 303–316.
Thomas, E., Yadin, E., & Strickland, C. E. (1991). Septal unit activity
during classical conditioning: A regional comparison. Brain Research,
547, 302–308. Received February 22, 2008
Vann, S. D., & Aggleton, J. P. (2004). The mammillary bodies: Two Revision received January 8, 2009
memory systems in one? Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 5, 35– 44. Accepted January 18, 2009 䡲

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to APA Journals at Reviewers@apa.org.
Please note the following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

You might also like