Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES

Self-Regulation Underlies Temperament and


Personality: An Integrative Developmental Framework
Jaap J. A. Denissen,1 Marcel A. G. van Aken,2 Lars Penke,3 and Dustin Wood4
1
Tilburg University, 2Utrecht University, 3University of G€
ottingen, and 4Wake Forest University

ABSTRACT—In this article, we present an integrative per- other conceptions, see Zentner, 2012). Personality traits are
spective on temperament and personality development. defined as “relatively enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings,
Personality and temperament are conceptualized as regu- and behaviors that distinguish individuals from one another”
latory systems that start as physiological reactivity to (Roberts & Mroczek, 2008, p. 31). Unlike conceptualizations of
environmental features early in life, but are increasingly temperament, classic conceptualizations of personality typically
supplemented by regulation efforts oriented toward refer- do not include notions of reactivity and self-regulation (for an
ence values such as personal goals and social norms. exception, see Block, 2002). However, Denissen and colleagues
These reference values change during development as (Denissen, Penke, & Wood, 2013; Wood, Spain, & Denissen,
society expects increasingly mature behaviors, but it takes 2013) proposed a regulatory model of adult personality traits in
regulatory resources and incremental practice before which behaviors are interpreted as functional reactions to envi-
people can conform to these higher standards. Consistent ronmental features (see Fleeson, 2012, for a model with similar
with this view, a meta-analysis of mean-level development assumptions); that is, behaviors are meant to achieve certain
of personality traits in adolescence revealed a decrease in desired future states.
conscientiousness and openness during early adolescence. The core of our conception of personality traits is that behav-
Negative discrepancies between reference values and iors that are typically associated with traits (e.g., talking to
actual behavior are apparently responsible for decreases strangers for extraversion) are performed because they are stra-
in perceived maturity, but more direct evidence is needed tegic means to desired end states (see also McCabe & Fleeson,
to support this claim. 2012). Others have highlighted the importance of self-regulation
for understanding personality development. For example, Roth-
KEYWORDS—self-regulation; temperament; personality
bart (1981) articulated the role of primary and secondary regula-
tion, and described how temperament development can be
Temperament has been defined as “constitutionally based indi- understood as the maturation of regulatory systems (Posner &
vidual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, with ‘consti- Rothbart, 2000).
tutional’ referring to the relatively enduring biological makeup In the domain of lifespan personality development, our theory
of the individual, influenced over time by heredity, maturation, compares with social investment theory (SIT; Roberts, Wood, &
and experience” (Rothbart, 1986, p. 356; for an overview of Lodi-Smith, 2005). Like our theory, SIT acknowledges the life-
span malleability of personality and points to social roles as
driving forces in the maturation of personality. Within SIT, per-
Jaap J. A. Denissen, Developmental Psychology, Tilburg Univer-
sity; Marcel A. G. van Aken, Developmental Psychology, Utrecht sonality traits mature when individuals encounter new expecta-
University; Lars Penke, Biological Personality Psychology and tions for communal, responsible behavior in adulthood and alter
Psychological Assessment, University of G€ottingen; Dustin Wood, their behavior to correspond with these changing expectations.
Department of Psychology, Wake Forest University. Consistent with this, a study (Bleidorn, 2012) demonstrated that
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed the increase in conscientiousness during late adolescence is
to Jaap J. A. Denissen, Developmental Psychology, Tilburg Univer-
mediated by the desire to reach scholarly goals once students
sity, PO Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, Netherlands; e-mail:
jjadenissen@gmail.com. approach high school graduation (for cross-cultural evidence,
see Bleidorn et al., in press). Our own perspective expands SIT
© 2013 The Authors
Child Development Perspectives © 2013 The Society for Research in Child Development by detailing the regulatory processes that explain how individu-
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12050 als change their behavior to meet these expectations and how

Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260


256 Jaap J. A. Denissen et al.

these changes become habitual and sufficiently automatic to be Finally, individuals can try to suppress their primary reaction
regarded as part of their personality. to the situational feature. For example, they might experience
In this article, we describe five regulatory mechanisms that an emotion due to their attention to a discrepancy that is rele-
are used to achieve desired states and that provide the basis for vant for a certain desired end state, but refrain from behaviorally
individual differences in personality. Then we show how these expressing the emotion. For example, an extraverted individual
regulatory mechanisms can be a driving force of personality might feel bored while studying for an exam, but might try to
development. Finally, we describe how this account can be suppress the urge to talk to a neighboring student while studying
applied to understanding personality development in adoles- at the library.
cence by presenting the results of a new meta-analysis and We argue that many of the consistent patterns of affect,
outlining other questions that can be tested by research. (For a behavior, and cognition that define personality can be conceptu-
complete account of the theory, see Denissen et al., 2013.) alized as functionally motivated to achieve certain reference val-
ues (e.g., goals, social norms). Personality might thus be seen as
A TAXONOMY OF REGULATORY MECHANISMS a heterogeneous mix of mechanisms that constitute, in an addi-
tive or interactive fashion, alternative routes to similar develop-
Regulatory mechanisms are defined as reactions that are strate- mental and social outcomes (equifinality). For example,
gically performed to decrease discrepancies between a person’s agreeable people may be characterized by a reference value
current state and some referent standard (Carver & Scheier, (i.e., goal, desired end state) of promoting and conserving
2001; Denissen et al., 2013). We discuss five such mechanisms harmonious interactions with others. As such, they might avoid
(for their origin in the literature on emotion regulation, see Gross situations and people that are associated with high levels of
& Thompson, 2007). First, individuals can select or deselect sit- conflict, attenuate conflicts by appeasing behaviors when it
uations based on their eliciting features (e.g., leaving a stressful nevertheless arises, selectively ignore provocations by others,
situation). Selection is best understood as a movement toward or systematically appraise others’ behaviors as benign, suppress
away from the triggering set of environmental features. For overt expressions of anger when they arise, or any combination
example, people may select certain environments that fit their of these.
personality (Scarr, 1996), or they may learn or decide to avoid To summarize, we propose that the five self-regulation mecha-
situations that are not conducive to their goals. nisms constitute a basis for individual differences in personality.
Second, individuals might modify the situational features that Evidence that supports this claim is that personality is robustly
are associated with undesired outcomes. However, in contrast to associated with a systematic range of constructs related to self-
situation selection, the person modifies environmental features regulation. In fact, traits are related substantially to constructs
(e.g., trying to attenuate or accentuate certain environmental fea- such as goals (e.g., Roberts, O’Donnell, & Robins, 2004) and
tures, or trying to alter certain features of a social role) without situational expectancies (Ames, 2008; Wood, Harms, & Vazire,
moving to a different situation. For example, a conscientious 2010). For example, in a study (McCabe & Fleeson, 2012) that
individual could structure his office (e.g., by organizing files used a diary design to relate daily fluctuations in extraversion-
according to work projects) to achieve certain goals. related goals (e.g., “trying to have fun”) and extraverted behav-
Third, people can direct attention away from undesired features ior, the former predicted almost three fourths of the variance in
of the situation to increase its hedonic value (e.g., looking away the latter.
from an aversive stimulus) or facilitate more long-term goals. As
an example of the latter, in a famous experiment by Mischel and SELF-REGULATION AS DRIVING FORCE OF
his colleagues (Mischel, Ebbesen, & Raskoff Zeiss, 1972), PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT
children were more successful at suppressing the urge to eat a
marshmallow when they directed their attention away from it. A core tenet of our theoretical framework is that regulatory
Fourth, people can change their appraisals or representations mechanisms can be improved by practice, which optimizes and
of the situational feature in question. For example, if they are automates regulatory behaviors (Mauss, Bunge, & Gross, 2007).
facing a difficult project deadline, they can redefine stress as a In addition, people can draw on previous experiences in regula-
challenge. In the previously described experiment, this strategy tory situations. For example, many older adults apply lessons
suppressed the urge to eat a tempting marshmallow: Children learned from past experiences when confronted with problems
who modified their cognitive representation of the marshmal- (Aldwin, Sutton, Chiara, & Spiro, 1996). Such improved self-
low’s appetitive features were more successful at this than chil- regulation by practice can explain why mean levels of personal-
dren who did not. Similarly, a highly conscientious individual ity traits increase in a socially desirable direction over the
who is following a diet could try to attenuate the appetitive lifespan (the “maturity principle”; Roberts & Wood, 2006).
appeal of the marshmallow by cognitively reconstructing it as a To the extent that one’s personality is rooted in stable individ-
food that is undesirable because of its caloric value (Metcalfe & ual differences in the frequency and efficiency of self-regulation
Mischel, 1999). mechanisms, age-related changes in personality development

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260


Self-Regulation, Temperament, and Personality Development 257

should be driven (mediated) by shifts in the individual’s use of underdeveloped regulatory ability (this is also consistent with a
these mechanisms. In fact, preliminary evidence shows that the decrease in self-esteem during this period; Robins, Trzesniew-
association between age and affect is partially mediated by the ski, Tracy, Gosling, & Potter, 2002). After this initial phase is
self-perceived efficiency of self-regulation mechanisms (Kessler completed and a novel regulatory repertoire is in place, incre-
& Staudinger, 2009). Similarly, age-related decreases in nega- mental practice is expected to yield strengthening regulatory
tive emotions such as anger and anxiety are partially mediated ability until it reaches levels that are consistent with the corre-
by changes in the reappraisal strategy of accepting of negative sponding reference value. As soon as observers (including the
emotions nonjudgmentally (Shallcross, Ford, Floerke, & Mauss, self) detect these improvements, ratings of personality maturity
in press). should increase.
Ultimately, the most conclusive piece of evidence to support In a meta-analysis of studies that reported age differences in
that personality maturation depends on improved self-regulation personality traits in adolescence, we found 14 articles (with 20
comes from intervention studies. Specifically, interventions that samples) that reported age differences in mean levels of the Big
successfully boost self-regulation mechanisms should be associ- Five personality factors across adolescence, which we define as
ated with increases in maturity of personality. Targeted mecha- 10–20 years (details are available in the online Supporting
nisms might include the nonjudgmental acceptance of negative Information Appendix S1). Effect sizes were calculated as the
emotions (Baer, 2003), shifting attention away from threatening difference between two age groups (for cross-sectional studies)
cues (Hakamata et al., 2010), or the positive reappraisal of or time points (for longitudinal studies), standardized by the
threats (Hallion & Ruscio, 2011). Recently, an intervention to standard deviation of the first measurement. Figures 1 and 2
increase conscientiousness was developed based on expectancy- thus plot the direction of development (i.e., slope) at each of the
value theory (Magidson, Roberts, Collado-Rodriguez, & Lejuez, time points on the x-axis. Numbers below zero indicate a
in press). This intervention requires people to identify important decrease in mean levels, whereas numbers above zero indicate
life goals (e.g., “being a dedicated employee”) and plan specific increases in mean levels (the resulting projected mean levels
activities to fulfill these goals (e.g., “showing up at work on are depicted in Figure 3, as described later).
time,” “limiting leisurely Internet use at work”). According to Applied to conscientiousness, we found evidence for a linear
our perspective, the repeated practice of these activities will, mean-level shape of conscientiousness. That is, the effect size
over time, lead to their automatization and subsequent personal- plot (of slopes) indicates a decrease in early adolescence, fol-
ity change. lowed by an increase in late adolescence (see Figure 1). Open-
ness showed both a linear and quadratic trend, decreasing
EXPLAINING MEAN-LEVEL PERSONALITY (rather steeply) in early adolescence and increasing (relatively
DEVELOPMENT IN ADOLESCENCE less steeply) in later adolescence (see Figure 2). The other
three personality factors did not change.
To illustrate the usefulness of our theoretical model, we applied To facilitate interpretation, we saved the estimated values for
it to explain mean-level development of personality traits during both conscientiousness and openness to experience. Starting
adolescence. The shape of adolescents’ mean-level personality from age 10 as a reference value, we added the predicted slope
development has been suggested to be curvilinear, with decreas-
ing maturity in early puberty and increases thereafter (i.e.,
U-shape; Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011). However, the
only meta-analysis on mean-level personality development
(Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006) did not differentiate
between early and late adolescence, so the exact shape of mean-
level development required a closer look.
Applied to a transition like puberty, our model predicts a
U-shaped developmental pattern of levels in personality traits.
This prediction starts from the assumption that most important
social or demographic transitions (like puberty) are associated
with a shift in reference values (e.g., parents increasingly expect
responsible behaviors from their adolescent children). These ref-
erence values might not result in immediate corresponding
behavioral changes because individuals need time to become
aware of and internalize the new values and develop a behav-
ioral repertoire to accommodate them. During this initial phase,
Figure 1. Plot of effect sizes (standardized mean differences between age
perceived maturity might even decline because individuals are groups) for conscientiousness. Negative values indicate decreases following
now held to a higher standard that they cannot (yet) meet due to the age on the x-axis; positive values indicate increases.

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260


258 Jaap J. A. Denissen et al.

Supporting Information Appendix S1), we replicated the decrease


in extraversion and emotional stability. Only the U-shaped pat-
tern of agreeableness could not be replicated, which might be
due to differences in sample, instrument, and rating source.
Our proposed framework explains the curvilinear shape of
mean-level personality change during adolescence, with
decreases in early adolescence followed by increases in later
adolescence. Increasingly responsible and socially desirable ref-
erence values are key. In early life, adolescents do not yet have
the regulatory resources to live up to these demands, so they are
judged as less mature. For example, early adolescents might be
limited in their regulatory ability, perhaps because the neuronal
foundations of self-control are still developing (Steinberg, 2007).
Only later, when they have learned from experiences and accu-
Figure 2. Plot of effect sizes (standardized mean differences between age
mulated resources, do most of them gradually live up to social
groups) for openness to experience. Negative values indicate decreases fol- reference values, resulting in stabilization of and, eventually,
lowing the age on the x-axis; positive values indicate increases. increases in maturity.
An alternative explanation is that the ability to self-regulate is
not actually limited during adolescence, but reference values
0.00 temporarily shift toward values that society considers socially
-0.05 immature. With some leeway, a version of this argument is con-
-0.10 sistent with the theory of antisocial behavior (Moffitt, 1993),
which states that the maturity gap during adolescence temporar-
Predicted standardized mean

-0.15

-0.20 ily increases the attractiveness of deviant behavior. Applied to


-0.25 conscientiousness personality development, in the transition to middle school,
openness
-0.30 socially endorsed reference values might be attuned toward
-0.35 rebellious behaviors that run counter to “adult” social institu-
-0.40 tions such as school work and other intellectual endeavors. Such
-0.45 temporary counter norms (i.e., reference values that are incon-
-0.50
sistent with more adult notions of maturity) might lead to corre-
10.0 12.7 15.6 18.3 21.1 sponding decreases in conscientiousness and openness to
Age
experience.
Of course, this explanation is still consistent with the assump-
Figure 3. Estimated values for conscientiousness and openness to experi- tions of our regulatory framework—that explaining changes in
ence. Age 10 was used as reference value (set at zero). For every 2.77-year
increase along the x-axis (average time lag), the (cumulative) predicted traits requires understanding both changes in reference values
value based on the estimated slopes that are depicted in Figures 1 and 2 and changes in the regulatory resources and behavioral patterns
(dotted lines) is plotted. needed to implement them. Our results suggest that reference
values related to conscientiousness and openness begin to
value to this reference value. To the result of this computation, increase substantially in adolescence, perhaps due to the sal-
we added the slope for age 12.77, age 15.54, and age 18.31 ience of developmental norms that increasingly specify to
(i.e., multitudes of 2.77, the average time lag between studies). behave in a fashion that is more hardworking and encourage
For example, the predicted slope for openness for age 10 was exploring novel ideals during adolescence. The observed
.33, so we plotted this as the predicted absolute value for age decreases in openness and conscientiousness might reflect a
12.77. The results (depicted in Figure 3) clearly show the combination of shifting social and personal expectations toward
expected U-shaped development in mean level. more mature behavior and a temporary lack of regulatory ability
These plots are partly consistent with the findings (Soto et al., and resources to fulfill these new expectations. By comparison,
2011) that early puberty is a time of decreases in conscientious- the lack of corresponding effects for agreeableness might be due
ness and openness, followed by an increase. Decreases in extra- to the overriding concern for peer popularity during this period
version, emotional stability, and agreeableness (reported by Soto (e.g., Van der Linden, Scholte, Cillessen, te Nijenhuis, &
et al., 2011) were not significant in our meta-analysis when effect Segers, 2010). Testing these explanations will require more
sizes were aggregated within samples. However, when age differ- direct assessments of regulatory abilities as well as reference
ences were analyzed using multilevel modeling (see the online values such as personal goals and perceived norms.

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260


Self-Regulation, Temperament, and Personality Development 259

CONCLUSION Bleidorn, W., Klimstra, T. A., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Potter,
J., & Gosling, S. D. (in press). Personality maturation around the
We have described a portion of a theory that personality devel- world: A cross-cultural examination of social investment theory.
Psychological Science.
opment is driven by shifts in self-regulation systems that make
Block, J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing system: Toward an
use of mechanisms like the selection and modification in envi- integrative theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ronmental features, shifts of attention, the reappraisal of envi- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). On the self-regulation of behavior.
ronmental challenges, and the suppression of undesired New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
impulses. Our theory integrates conceptualizations of childhood Denissen, J. J. A., Penke, L., & Wood, D. (2013). Lifespan personality
temperament and adult personality, as well as adult emotional development is driven by momentary self-regulation dynamics.
aging. It points to promising ways to modify temperament Unpublished manuscript, Tilburg University.
through the incremental practice of self-regulation mechanisms. Fleeson, W. (2012). Rapid growth and opportunities for integration. In
K. Deaux & M. Snyder (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of personality
This model is generative: We were able to predict how patterns
and social psychology (pp. 33–63). New York, NY: Oxford Univer-
of personality change should look for traits related to the Big sity Press.
Five personality factors in early adolescence (ages 10–20). For Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual
every period of the lifespan, we expect that an assessment of ref- foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation
erence values and average regulatory capacity determine the (pp. 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford.
shape of mean-level development of different personality traits. Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Lei-
Specifically, this model predicts why personality traits change in benluft, E., . . . Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treat-
ment: A meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment
this manner during these ages (and at other ages): Personality
for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 982–990. doi:10.1016/j.bio-
changes should be initiated by changes in reference values that psych.2010.07.021
individuals slowly start meeting. Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of
Somewhat counterintuitively, when a reference value for the cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychologi-
trait begins to increase substantially, the self-perceived trait cal bulletin, 137, 940–958. doi:10.1037/a0024355
level will decrease if the behavioral level does not meet this Kessler, E. M., & Staudinger, U. M. (2009). Affective experience in
new standard. However, over time, increases in a reference adulthood and old age: The role of affective arousal and perceived
affect regulation. Psychology and Aging, 24, 349–362. doi:10.
value should ultimately yield increases in both self-perceived
1037/a0015352
and behavioral levels of the trait as people actively work to Magidson, J. F., Roberts, B. W., Collado-Rodriguez, A., & Lejuez, C. W.
adjust their behavior and develop the regulatory skills to match (in press). Theory-driven intervention for changing personality:
and accommodate this standard. When reference values shift Expectancy value theory, behavioral activation, and conscientious-
toward less socially desirable levels, or when individuals’ regu- ness. Developmental Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0030583
latory resources are eroded (e.g., in very old age or during peri- Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Automatic emotion reg-
ods of extensive stress), the corresponding trait should regress ulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1, 146–167.
toward less socially desirable levels. Although these possibili- doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.x
McCabe, K. O., & Fleeson, W. (2012). What is extraversion for? Inte-
ties are exciting, we need more evidence to demonstrate the
grating trait and motivational perspectives and identifying the pur-
extent to which individual differences and lifespan changes in pose of extraversion. Psychological Science, 23, 1498–1505. doi:10.
self-regulation capacity underlie the development of personality 1177/0956797612444904
traits. Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay
of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106,
REFERENCES 3–19. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972). Cognitive and
Aldwin, C. M., Sutton, K. J., Chiara, G., & Spiro, A. (1996). Age differ- attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Person-
ences in stress, coping, and appraisal: Findings from the normative ality and Social Psychology, 21, 204–218. doi:10.1037/h0032198
aging study. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sci- Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
ences and Social Sciences, 51B, P179–P188. doi:10.1093/geronb/ antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
51B.4.P179 Review, 100, 674–701. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674
Ames, D. R. (2008). Assertiveness expectancies: How hard people push Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of
depends on the consequences they predict. Journal of Personality self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427–
and Social Psychology, 95, 1541–1557. doi:10.1037/a0013334 441.
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. K. (2008). Personality trait change in
conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 31–35.
Practice, 10, 125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015 doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x
Bleidorn, W. (2012). Hitting the road to adulthood: Short-term personal- Roberts, B. W., O’Donnell, M., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Goal and per-
ity development during a major life transition. Personality and sonality trait development in emerging adulthood. Journal of Per-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1594–1608. doi:10.1177/ sonality and Social Psychology, 87, 541–550. doi:10.1037/
0146167212456707 0022-3514.87.4.541

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260


260 Jaap J. A. Denissen et al.

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. Journal of Person-
mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A ality and Social Psychology, 100, 330–348. doi:10.1037/
meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, a0021717
1–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1 Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives
Roberts, B. W., & Wood, D. (2006). Personality development in the con- from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in
text of the neo-socioanalytic model of personality. In D. K. Mroczek Psychological Science, 16, 55–59. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.
& T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of personality development (pp. 11– 00475.x
39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Van der Linden, D., Scholte, R. H. J., Cillessen, A. H. N., te Nijenhuis,
Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Lodi-Smith, J. (2005). Evaluating five fac- J., & Segers, E. (2010). Classroom ratings of likeability and popu-
tor theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait larity are related to the Big Five and the general factor of personal-
development. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 166–184. ity. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 669–672. doi:10.1016/j.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.08.002 jrp.2010.08.007
Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tracy, J. L., Gosling, S. D., & Pot- Wood, D., Harms, P., & Vazire, S. (2010). Perceiver effects as projective
ter, J. (2002). Global self-esteem across the life span. Psychology tests: What your perceptions of others say about you. Journal of
& Aging, 17, 423–434. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.3.423 Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 174–190. doi:10.1037/
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child a0019390
Development, 52, 569–578. doi:10.2307/1129176 Wood, D., Spain, S. M., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2013). Functional explana-
Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. tions of actions and traits. Unpublished manuscript, Wake Forest
Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365. doi:10.1037/0012-1649. University.
22.3.356 Zentner, M., & Shiner, R. L. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of temperament.
Scarr, S. (1996). How people make their own environments: Implications New York, NY: Guilford.
for parents and policy makers. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law,
2, 204–228. SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Shallcross, A. J., Ford, B. Q., Floerke, V. A., & Mauss, I. B. (in press).
Getting better with age: The relationship between age, acceptance,
Additional supporting information may be found in the online
and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
doi:10.1037/a0031180 version of this article at the publisher’s website:
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age Appendix S1. Description of Meta-Analytic Procedure.
differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 7, Number 4, 2013, Pages 255–260

You might also like