Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

REGULAR ARTICLE

Altered Social Hedonic Processing in Eating Disorders

Kate Tchanturia, PhD1* ABSTRACT


Objective: Exploring social anhedonia
than people without ED. Recovered AN
had an intermediate profile in between
Helen Davies, PhD1 in eating disorders (ED) was the main AN and HC. Clinical severity and alexithy-
Amy Harrison, PhD1 objective of this study. Social anhedonia mia were strongly associated with social
John R.E. Fox, PhD2 relates to a reduced feeling of pleasure anhedonia scores.
Janet Treasure, MD, PhD1 from social stimulation. Researching the
Discussion: Difficulty identifying and
exact nature of problems in interpersonal
Ulrike Schmidt, MD, PhD1 relationships is an important topic for
expressing feelings could be related to
this diminished drive for social pleasure.
clinicians and researchers in the field.
Tackling these interpersonal maintaining
Method: The revised social anhedonia factors in treatment is likely to be a
scale was completed by 148 participants: worthwhile endeavor. V C 2012 by Wiley

anorexia nervosa (AN) n 5 72; bulimia Periodicals, Inc.


nervosa (BN) n 5 19; recovered AN n 5
14; healthy controls (HC) n 5 43. Partici- Keywords: social anhedonia; anorexia;
pants also completed mood related meas- bulimia; recovered
ures and the Toronto Alexythimia scale.
(Int J Eat Disord 2012; 00:000–000)
Results: People with AN and BN had
higher self-reported social anhedonia

Introduction Anhedonia can be defined as a loss of interest in


activities in most or all activities and is associated
It is recognized that anorexia nervosa (AN) is a dis- with low motivation to engage in pleasurable
order which has core emotional disturbances1–5 events, therefore invoking the involvement of neu-
and it has been hypothesized that the development ral pathways related to seeking out and obtaining
and maintenance of eating disorders (EDs), like rewards.11 Anhedonia is part of the clinical presen-
AN, may be associated with a dysregulated reward tation of depression, which is the most commonly
system.6 For example, people with AN, relative to comorbid disorder in people with EDs12,13 with
healthy controls (HC), demonstrate low levels of 59% of people with EDs experiencing one or more
physiological responding to positive (reward) stim- episode of major depressive disorder.14 Socially,
uli and poor learning from past experiences, as well people with EDs, particularly AN, are observed to
as generally low levels of physiological responses be shy, have limited social networks, and self-
when skin conductance measures are combined report poorer quality and quantity of relationships
with a behavioral task of decision making.7 We fur- with their family, friends, and colleagues.15–17
ther understand that people with EDs show Therefore, it is possible that people with EDs may
reduced pleasure for a range of stimuli, such as be more likely than HCs to have high levels of
from food,8 amusing stimuli,9 and a reduced desire social anhedonia, which relates to a reduced feeling
for sexual activity.10 of pleasure from social stimulation and a reduced
drive to seek out social interaction. However, de-
Accepted 1 May 2012
spite the presence of interpersonal difficulties pro-
Supported by NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental posed to maintain the disorder,1 few studies have
Health at South London; Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust; Institute explored whether individuals with EDs find social
of Psychiatry, King’s College London; Swiss Anorexia Foundation.
interaction less pleasurable than the healthy popu-
*Correspondence to: Dr. Kate Tchanturia, PO59, King’s College
London, Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Psychological lation.
Medicine, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Evidence exists, however, that highlights the
E-mail: kate.tchanturia@kcl.ac.uk
1 potential effects of starvation on the reward sys-
Section of Eating Disorders, King’s College London, Institute of
Psychiatry, Psychological Medicine, London, United Kingdom tem individuals would usually gain from social
2
Division of Clinical Psychology, University of Manchester, contact. Keys et al.18 observed participants whose
Manchester, United Kingdom
diet was deliberately halved over a period of 24
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/eat.22032 weeks and found these individuals showed
VC 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. increased social withdrawal, reduced libido, and

International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012 1


TCHANTURIA ET AL.

they sought out social contact less frequently, In summary, limited empirical studies are avail-
resulting in higher rates of social isolation within able to help us to understand the construct of
the group. It is possible that the increased rates social anhedonia in people with AN and BN. This
of depression reported in these individuals dur- line of enquiry could be useful, as research in EDs
ing the semistarvation phase might have contrib- has highlighted the importance of interpersonal
uted to their reduced ability to derive pleasure and emotional processing skills on treatment out-
from social contact. Given that symptoms of comes in AN26,27 and the negative impact of sus-
depression may persist despite successful recov- tained social impairment on illness prognosis.28,29
ery from the ED,19 it is possible that untreated Exploration of the presence of social anhedonia
social anhedonic symptoms persist even after the could help us to understand more about the
disorder has remitted, perhaps limiting the indi- nature of the observed interpersonal difficulties in
vidual’s quality of life and accounting for the people EDs.
persistently small social networks observed de- Whereas social anhedonia refers to the dimin-
spite recovery.16 As higher rates of residual symp- ished capacity to experience pleasure from others,
toms at discharge predict relapse,20 exploring alexithymia refers to an inability to identify and/or
social anhedonia further in people with EDs communicate feelings. The relationship between
might offer opportunities to improve quality of social anhedonia and alexithymia is stronger than
life and increase the likelihood of successful that of physical anhedonia.30 It is well recognized
that people with EDs demonstrate high levels of
long-term recovery from the illness.
alexithymia.31,32 We were therefore interested to
There are three established ways to explore social know if alexithymia relates to social anhedonia in
anhedonia: interview-based instruments, labora- people with EDs based on the premise that if peo-
tory-based experiments, and self-report question- ple are unable to recognize and label their inner
naires.21 Interview-based methods are absent in emotions, this will pertain to anhedonia in a social
the literature on EDs. There are a limited number context as well.
of studies which have used an experimental design
This study aimed to explore social anhedonia in
in EDs, with each study employing a different para-
people with EDs generally, and AN in particular,
digm. For example, using the startle-reflex para-
using a well-established and widely used self-
digm, Friederich et al.22 report that people with
report instrument specifically designed to measure
EDs show aversive responses to appetitive stimuli.
this concept. It was predicted that those with a cur-
Studies exploring facial expressions using either rent ED (AN, BN), as well as individuals recovered
observed9 or unobserved (electromyography)23 from AN, would report significantly higher levels of
techniques have shown that people with AN dem- social anhedonia than HCs. It was also predicted
onstrate reduced facial expression compared to that alexithymia would be positively related to
controls in response to film clips or pleasant pic- social anhedonia, such that higher levels of alexi-
tures. Watson et al.24 found that women with AN thymia would be associated with higher levels of
find female faces less rewarding and avoid looking social anhedonia.
at their faces, spending less time looking into the
eye region than controls. The authors of this study
argued that this altered social reward processing in
AN may directly contribute to impaired social func-
Method
tioning in this illness.
In addition to these experimental studies, one Participants
study was identified in the literature which This study received approval from the ethical com-
employed a self-report measure. Davis and Wood- mittee (ref. 08/H0606/58) of the Institute of Psychiatry
side reported that individuals with AN demonstrate and South London and Maudsley (SLaM) NHS Foun-
significantly higher physical anhedonia scores in dation Trust. All participants gave informed consent
comparison to bulimia nervosa (BN) patients. prior to their inclusion in the study. Four groups of
Within this study, physical anhedonia was defined participants were recruited; one with a Diagnostic and
as the degree to which individuals are rewarded by Statistical Manual (DSM-IV)33 diagnosis of AN, who
physical sensations, such as touching, feeling, were recruited from inpatient and outpatient ED serv-
movement, and eating. One study which has ices across the SLaM Trust; a second group with a cur-
explored social anhedonia in people with BN in rent diagnosis of BN, who were recruited from a spe-
comparison to those with AN found that there was cialist outpatient department within SLaM’s ED serv-
no difference between the illness subtypes.25 ices; a third group of individuals who had recovered

2 International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012


ANHEDONIA IN EATING DISORDERS

from AN, recruited from the research volunteer data- nal 42-item measure and therefore when the final
base held within the section of EDs at the Institute of score for each subscale is calculated, it is multiplied
Psychiatry and a comparative sample of HCs with no by 2. Internal consistency for the current study
personal or family history of psychiatric illness who using Cronbach alpha coefficient was .87 for the total
were recruited via advertisements in the local commu- score.
nity. The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20). This is the
Exclusion criteria for all groups were nonfluent English most widely used measure of alexithymia.41 It consists of
or a history of head injury. Inclusion age was 18–55, as 20 items loading onto three subscales: difficulty identify-
the ED department from where recruitment took place ing feelings and distinguishing them from bodily sensa-
treats people within this age range. Participants were tions, difficulty describing feelings to others, and an
considered recovered if they reported restored, regular externally oriented style of thinking. A score of \51 indi-
menstruation for at least the previous year, did not report cates nonalexithymia, =61 indicates alexithymia, and 52–
clinically significant scores on the Eating Disorders Ex- 60 shows possible alexithymia. Bagby et al. found the
amination Questionnaire (EDE-Q)34 and if their body TAS-20 has good internal consistency for the total score
mass index (BMI) had been 18.5 or above for at least the (a 5 .81) and acceptable internal consistency for the sub-
previous year. Questions from the Structured Clinical scale scores.
Interview for DSM-IV disorders (SCID) Extended Module
H35 were used to ascertain a previous episode of AN Statistical Analysis
based on DSM-IV29 criteria. SPSS version 15 was used to analyze the data. The data
were inspected using histograms and Kolmogorov-Smir-
nov tests to assess assumptions of normal distribution.
Measures and Procedure
The main outcome measure, the social Anhedonia total
Demographic and Clinical Information. Participants score, was normally distributed, and so a one-way
self-reported their age, ethnicity, and years of education. ANOVA was applied to analyze between group differen-
In addition, duration of illness in the clinical groups was ces. Alpha was set at p \ .05 unless Bonferroni’s correc-
obtained from their clinical notes. At the beginning of the tion for multiple comparisons was applied as indicated
assessment, the research version of the SCID35 was below. Cohen’s d42 (mean1 2 mean2/pooled standard
administered by a trained researcher. This highlighted deviation) was calculated to provide effect sizes for nor-
current and past Axis I psychiatric comorbidity in mally distributed data, with an effect size of 42 Correla-
patients and was used as a screening tool for HCs. The tions were used to assess relationships between variables.
following self-report measures were completed by all Partial correlation was used to assess the relationship
participants. between two variables, while controlling for the potential
The Revised Social Anhedonia Scale (RSAS). This is a confounding effect of depression and anxiety, measured
40-item true/false inventory used to assess social anhe- by the DASS.
donia.36 It has high internal consistency and test–retest
reliability37 and has been widely used21,38 in studies of
individuals with schizophrenia and at-risk populations
with alpha coefficients above .80. Internal consistency for Results
the current study was .83. Pelizza and Ferrari39 recom-
mend a cut-off of =12 to suggest the presence of signifi- Clinical and Demographic Data
cant, functionally impaired social anhedonia. In total, 148 participants took part in the study;
Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q). This 91 had an ED (AN 5 72 and BN 5 19). There were
36-item self-report questionnaire is a measure of psycho- 14 participants in the recovered AN group and 43
pathological and behavioral indicators of disordered eat- HCs.
ing.34 It is derived from and scored in the same way as Those with AN and BN both reported comor-
the eating disorder examination interview schedule bid anxiety problems, assessed using the SCID.
(EDE). The EDE-Q provides a global score and has four In the AN group, 22.2% (n 5 16) had panic dis-
subscales measuring dietary restraint, eating concern, order, 16.7% (n 5 12) had agoraphobia, 30.6%
weight concern, and shape concern. Subscale and global (n 5 22) had social phobia, 20.8% (n 5 15) had
scores range from 0 to 6, with higher scores representing obsessive compulsive disorder, and 50% (n 5 36)
greater pathology. In the current study, the overall Cron- had symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder. In
bach alpha coefficient was .93. the BN group, 6.3% (n 5 2) had panic disorder,
Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS). This is a 6.3% (n 5 2) had agoraphobia, 31.3% (n 5 6)
21-item measure with 7 items in each of the three had social phobia, 6.3% (n 5 2) had obsessive
subscales.40 The DASS 21 is a short form of the origi- compulsive disorder, and 31.3% (n 5 6) had gen-

International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012 3


TCHANTURIA ET AL.

TABLE 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics of the healthy control, eating disorder, and recovered AN groups
HC (N 5 43) AN (N 5 72) BN (N 5 19) Rec AN (N 5 14) Test Statistics

Age 26.5 (8.8) 25.7 (7.5) 31.0 (11.7) 25.2 (8.7) F(3,145) 5 2.06, p 5 .108
BMI 21.6 (1.7) 14.5 (1.8) 21.0 (2.1) 21.1 (1.9) F(3,143) 5 149.67, p 5 .001
Duration of illness N/A 9.6 (7.0) 11.1 (9.4) 3.8 (2.6) F(2,98) 5 4.76, p 5 .012
DASS total 5.8 (4.6) 38.3 (14.9) 36.2 (14.1) 15.2 (10.7) F(3,144) 5 72.45, p 5 .001
DASS depression 1.2 (1.1) 13.9 (5.7) 13.5 (6.6) 4.1 (4.3) F(3,144) 5 72.71, p 5 .001
DASS anxiety 1.3 (1.5) 10.7 (5.9) 9.3 (3.9) 3.5 (3.6) F(3,144) 5 43.94, p 5 .001
EDEQ-global .7 (.7) 4.0 (1.4) 3.8 (1.1) 1.8 (.9) F(3,140) 5 77.51, p 5 .001
TAS total 37.9 (8.2) 60.4 (13.7) 56.8 (13.4) 46.3 (13.1) F(3,127) 5 30.59, p 5 .001
HC 5 healthy control group; AN 5 anorexia nervosa group; BN 5 bulimia nervosa group; Rec AN 5 recovered AN group; test statistics are ANOVAs and
descriptive statistics are means followed by standard deviations in parentheses.N/A 5 not applicable. BMI 5 body mass index (weight/height2). Where let-
ters in superscript differ, group means are significantly different, same letters indicate non-significant differences between the groups.

TABLE 2. Scores for the social anhedonia scale for the healthy control (HC), eating disorder (AN, BN), and recovered
AN (Rec AN) groups
HC (N 5 44) AN (N 5 72) BN (N 5 19) Rec AN (N 5 14)

Social anhedonia scale 5.5 (4.7)a 16.4 (8.6)b*; d 5 1.28 17.2 (8.3)b*; d 5 1.52 9.5 (6.7)a; d 5 .48
HC 5 healthy control group; AN 5 anorexia nervosa group; BN 5 bulimia nervosa group; Rec AN 5 recovered AN group. Analysis is based on a one-
way ANOVA and descriptive statistics are means followed by standard deviations in parentheses. Where letters in superscript differ, group means are signif-
icantly different, same letters indicate nonsignificant differences between the groups. d 5 Cohen’s D effect size for each group compared with HCs, with
an effect size of \.2 defined as small, \.5 defined as medium, and \.8 defined as large (Cohen 1988). * 5 significantly different to healthy controls at p
\ .001.

eralized anxiety disorder symptoms. Of those HCs, with large effect sizes. There was a small
currently ill with BN, 43.8% (n 5 8) reported a sized, albeit, nonsignificant difference between
past history of AN. There were low rates of drug, those who had recovered from AN and HCs (t 5
alcohol, and medication abuse in the currently 1.518, df 5 55, p 5 .14) and the recovered group
unwell groups. No BN participants endorsed dif- reported significantly less social anhedonia
ficulties with any of these substances. A small than those currently ill with AN (t 5 2.801, df 5 84,
proportion of the AN group (4.2%, n 5 3) p 5 \.006). There was no significant difference in
reported current alcohol abuse and 23.1% (n 5 self-reported social anhedonia between those with
3) of those in the recovered group reported a AN and BN (t 5 2.376, df 5 89, p 5 .71). Within the
past history of alcohol abuse. AN group, 70.83% (n 5 51) scored above the sug-
Those in the AN group had an average duration gested cut off for the RSAS of =12. Of those with
of illness of 9.69 years (SD 5 7.09) and those with BN, 47.5% (n 5 14) scored above the cut off;
BN had had the disorder for 11.15 (9.47). The recov- 14.29% (n 5 2) of the recovered participants scored
ered group reported suffering from the illness for a above the cut off, and 11.63% (n 5 5) scored above
fewer number of years (mean 5 3.90, SD 5 2.62). the cut off. The Figure 1 illustrates the significantly
Table 1, below, provides clinical and demo- higher social anhedonia reported by those cur-
graphic information for the four participant groups. rently unwell with an ED in comparison to HCs
and the intermediate profile of the recovered
The groups were similar in terms of age. Partici-
group.
pants with AN and BN were significantly more
depressed, anxious, and stressed, with higher levels
Correlations Between Clinical Characteristics
of ED symptomatology than HCs and those who
and Social Anhedonia
had recovered. Those with AN and BN and individ-
uals recovered from AN had significantly higher Table 3 below provides correlation coefficients
levels of alexithymia than HCs. between clinical and demographic characteristics
and self-reported social anhedonia for the entire
sample and the AN group.
Data for the Revised Social Anhedonia Scale
For the entire sample, there was a significant
Table 2, below, provides means, standard devia- negative correlation between social anhedonia and
tions, and effect sizes for the clinical, recovered, BMI, such that lower levels of social anhedonia
and HC groups for the RSAS. were related to higher BMI. There was a significant
Participants with AN and BN both reported sig- positive correlation between social anhedonia and
nificantly higher levels of social anhedonia than ED symptoms, measured using the EDE-Q, such

4 International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012


ANHEDONIA IN EATING DISORDERS

that higher levels of social anhedonia were related was found that only alexithymia and duration of ill-
to higher levels of ED symptoms. There was a sig- ness maintained a significant correlation with the
nificant positive correlation between social anhe- social anhedonia scale within these partial correla-
donia and depression, anxiety and stress, measured tions.
using the DASS, such that higher levels of social Given the significant correlation between social
anhedonia were related to higher levels of self- anhedonia and depression, measured using the
reported depression, anxiety, and stress. There was DASS, those with EDs were split around the ED
a significant positive correlation between social mean for depression into two groups: those with an
anhedonia and self-reported alexithymia, meas- ED who scored equal to, or above the mean for
ured using the TAS-20, such that higher levels of depression and those with an ED who scored below
social anhedonia were related to higher levels of the mean for depression. A t test was subsequently
alexithymia. When only the AN group was consid- carried out to compare self-reported social anhedo-
ered, the relationships remained the same for social nia in these groups. Those with an ED who scored
anhedonia and the DASS, EDEQ and the TAS. The above the mean (n 5 51) had significantly higher
relationship between BMI and social anhedonia social anhedonia scores (mean 5 19.61, SD 5 8.43)
was no longer significant. There was a significant than those with an ED (n 5 39) who scored below
positive relationship between social anhedonia and the mean for depression (mean 5 13.03, SD 5
age, as well as social anhedonia and duration of ill- 7.01), (t 5 3.943, df 5 88, p 5 .01).
ness, such that higher levels of social anhedonia Those in the ED group scoring above the mean
were related to AN patients being older and having for depression were most likely to have a comorbid
a longer duration of illness. major depressive episode and this group scored
As shown in Table 3, a number of partial correla- significantly higher for self-reported social anhedo-
tions were carried out, where depression (DASS- nia than Pelizza and Ferrari’s39 sample of 65 adults
Depression) was controlled for in the analysis. It with clinical depression (mean 5 13.07, SD 5 5.66),
(t 5 5.251, df 5 51, p 5 .001).
A linear regression was used to explore the signif-
FIGURE 1 Total social anhedonia scores for the anorexia icant predictors of social anhedonia within the ED
nervosa, bulimia nervosa, recovered and healthy control
groups. group. The overall model was significant (F(5,75) 5
9.013, p 5 .001) and of the predictors entered
using the enter method (BMI, age, years of illness,
depression measured using the DASS and EDE-Q
global score), years of illness (t 5 2.125, B 5 .296,
p 5 .037), and depression measured using the
DASS (t 5 3.737, B 5 .423, p 5 .001) were both
significant predictors of social anhedonia, explain-
ing 32.2 and 58.9% of the variance in social anhe-
donia, respectively.

Discussion
The aim of this study was to explore social anhedo-
nia in people with current EDs, people with a past

TABLE 3. Pearson correlations for the total sample and the AN group only for the social anhedonia scale and
demographic and clinical variables
Age Duration of Illness BMI EDE-Q DASS TAS

Social anhedonia scale total sample n 5 148 .14 N/A N/A .53a .63a .54a
Toronto alexithymia scale total sample n 5 148 2.11 N/A N/A .67a .70a —
Social anhedonia scale AN group only n 5 72 .25a .38a 2.05 .38a .48a .40a
Toronto alexithymia scale AN group only n 5 72 .49 .05 2.001 .42a .37a —
Social anhedonia scale AN group only with depression partialled out .24 .33a .07 .15 N/A .31b
a
Correlation is significant at the .01 level (two-tailed).
b
Correlation significant at .05 level (two-tailed). N/A 5 not applicable; AN 5 anorexia nervosa group; TAS 5 Toronto Alexithymia Scale; EDEQ 5 eating
disorders examination questionnaire; DASS 5 depression anxiety and stress scale, 21 item version. N/A 5 not appropriate.

International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012 5


TCHANTURIA ET AL.

history of AN, and a comparison group of HCs. As social anhedonia were related to a longer duration
expected, people with current AN or BN had higher of illness in those with AN.
self-reported social anhedonia than people without The data collected in this study do not support
EDs. The small sample of people recovered from Davis and Woodside’s findings that individuals with
AN had a profile that fell in between those who BN are less socially anhedonic than those with AN.
were currently ill and HCs. It is possible, therefore, However, our results should be interpreted with
that heightened social anhedonia could be present caution given the small sample size of the BN group.
to a higher degree in people with a history of EDs The data demonstrate that people with a current
and that this factor is exacerbated in the ill state, ED diagnosis report finding it difficult to experi-
due to the effects of starvation as highlighted by ence pleasure from their interactions with other
the Minnesota Starvation Studies.18 In relation to people. This is consistent with previous research
this, social anhedonia has been reported as being a suggesting that people with AN, in particular, have
promising indicator of vulnerability to developing diminished capacity to experience pleasure or
schizophrenia.21 However, it is also possible that reward from normally pleasurable or rewarding
higher levels of social anhedonia are a scar of the stimuli. The concept of high levels of social anhe-
illness and longitudinal studies would be required donia in people with EDs is relevant to the discus-
to explore this in more detail. sion around recent suggestions that EDs (AN in
The significant positive correlation between particular) may share a number of common fea-
social anhedonia and depression was large (both tures with other disorders that are characterized by
for the AN only group and for the whole sample), social and interpersonal dysfunction, such as autis-
suggesting that there is significant overlap between tic spectrum disorders (ASD) and psychosis.43,44
these two constructs and that the same causal fac- Indeed, a similar number of people in the AN group
tor may be implicated in both depression and (70.83%) scored above the cut off for social anhedo-
social anhedonia. Those with EDs who reported the nia to those with schizophrenia (65%) and the
highest rates of depression had greater social anhe- number of people with BN scoring above the cut
donia than those reporting fewer symptoms of off (47.5%) was similar to those with depression
depression, although both those with EDs scoring (45%).39 It would be interesting to explore in future
above and below the mean for depression scored studies whether self-reported social anhedonia is
above the cut off of 12 suggested for the scale.39 It related to objective measures of social cognition
is possible that the anhedonic component of and social reward. For example, there is research to
depression is expressed in EDs in a social form, suggest that people with AN avoid looking at the
particularly because of the interpersonal difficulties eyes and faces of others,24 as do people with ASD45
experienced by this group.15–17 It may also be that and psychosis.46 This has important implications
high levels of social anhedonia may predispose for social interaction, as the eyes are a rich source
someone to depression, as they may not get psy- of social information. It would be interesting to see
chological benefit from activities that normally whether social anhedonia mediates the relation-
promote positive mood (e.g., being with others). It ship between reduced eye contact and actual level
of social functioning in the future.
was of note that the regression showed that years
of illness and depression both explained a large
amount of the variance in social anhedonia for Limitations
those with EDs and this suggests that the develop- Only one self-report measure for social anhedo-
ment of clinical interventions for social anhedonia nia was used for this study. For future studies, addi-
in people with EDs might work best if they target tional interviews and experimental (performance-
those with a chronic illness and work with the indi- based task) designs might contribute to improved
vidual to help improve symptoms of depression in validity and reliability of findings. It would have
both a general and social context. also improved the reliability of the findings to have
When associations between social anhedonia had a larger sample of recovered participants with
and clinical and demographic variables were both AN and BN. Additionally, the sample was self-
explored, strong significant correlations were found selecting which may have biased the data and the
between self-reported depression, anxiety, ED exclusion criteria applied to the HC group may
symptoms, BMI, and alexithymia. Therefore, diffi- have resulted in these individuals being less repre-
culties identifying and expressing feelings are sentative of a community sample.
related to diminished pleasurable experience of It would be important to know if a factor such
social relationships. Furthermore, higher levels of as social anxiety contributes to social anhedonia.

6 International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012


ANHEDONIA IN EATING DISORDERS

It is possible that if you are anxious around people 3. Fox JR, Power MJ. Eating disorders and multi-level models of
you may not get much enjoyment from them. emotion: An integrated model. Clin Psychol Psychother
2009;16:240–267.
Therefore, including a measure of social anxiety 4. Kyriacou O, Easter A, Tchanturia K. Comparing views of patients,
would be helpful in future studies. Furthermore, it parents and clinicians on emotions in anorexia: A qualitative
was beyond the scope of this design to explore study. J Health Psychol 2009;14:843–854.
potential causal links between depression, social 5. Castro L, Davies H, Hale L, Surguladze S, Tchanturia K. Facial
anhedonia, and EDs. Causality could be investi- affect recognition in anorexia nervosa: Is obsessionality a miss-
ing piece of the puzzle? Aus N Zealand J Psychiatry 2010;
gated in future studies. 12:1118–1125.
6. Kaye W. Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia. Physiol Behav
Clinical Implications 2008;94:121–135.
The findings reported here support anecdotal 7. Tchanturia K, Liao T, Uher R, Lawrence N, Treasure J, Campbell
I. An investigation of decision making in people with anorexia
clinical reports of high levels of social anhedonia
nervosa using Iowa Gambling task and skin conductance meas-
in patients with EDs. Indeed, this was the first urements. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:635–641.
study to actively explore self-reported social 8. Davis C, Woodside DB. Sensitivity to the rewarding effects of
anhedonia in an ED population. Given the large food and exercise in eating disorders. Comp Psychiatry
effect sizes in patients groups it might be worth- 2002;43:189–194.
9. Davies H, Schmidt U, Stahl D, Tchanturia K. Evoked facial emo-
while addressing and targeting social anhedonia
tional expression and emotional experience in people with an-
in psychological interventions. Furthermore, a orexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 2010;44:531–539.
lack of pleasure derived from interacting with 10. Pinheiro AP, Raney TJ, Thornton LM, Fichter MM, Berrettini
others may contribute to the interpersonal diffi- WH, Goldman D, et al. Sexual functioning in women with eat-
culties that are often cited as maintaining factors ing disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2010;43:123–129.
for EDs.1,47 Tackling this important potential 11. Der Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia
and other reward related deficit. Trends Neurosci 2012;
maintaining factor in treatment is likely to be a
35:67–68.
worthwhile endeavor. This might involve psycho- 12. Herzog DB, Keller MB, Sacks NR, Yeh CJ, Lavori PW. Psychiatric-
education and increasing awareness about social comorbidity in treatment-seeking anorexics and bulimics. J Am
anhedonia, helping patients to develop strategies Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1992;31:810–818.
regarding change in this domain, and therapist- 13. Kaye WH, Bulik CM, Plotnicov K, Thornton L, Devlin B,
Fichter MM, et al. The genetics of anorexia collaborative
led experiential exercises to model and help rein-
study: Methods and sample description. Int J Eat Disord
force normal social interaction. All of these may 2008;41:289–300.
be beneficial in facilitating the long process of 14. Mischoulon D, Eddy KT, Keshaviah A, Dinescu D, Ross SL, Kass
recovery. The results of this study were in fact AE, et al. Depression and eating disorders: Treatment and
discussed with a group of our service users and course. J Affective Disord 2011;130:470–477.
15. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Doll HA, Welch, SL. Risk factors for ano-
their feedback was that it might be very useful
rexia nervosa: Three integrated case-control comparisons. Arch
to think together with clinicians about social sit- Gen Psychiatry 1999;56:468–476.
uations, not involving food, which could help 16. Tiller JM, Sloane G, Schmidt U, Troop N, Power M, Treasure J.
their reintegration back into the community. Social support in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia
nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1997;21:31–38.
17. Hartmann A, Zeeck A, Barrett MS. Interpersonal problems in
eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2010;43:619–627.
18. Keys A, Brozek J, Henschel A, Mickelsen O, Taylor HL.The Biol-
Conclusion ogy of Human Starvation, Vol. 2. Minneapolis, MH: University
of Minnesota Press, 1950.
Social anhedonia is part of the multiple problems
19. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Venkatraman VK, Fudge J, May C,
present in EDs. Specifically, addressing this in treat- Mazurkewicz L, et al. Altered reward processing in women
ment interventions could improve functional out- recovered from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 2007;
comes in these disorders. 164:1842–1849.
20. McFarlane T, Olmsted MP, Trottier K. Timing and prediction of
relapse in a transdiagnostic eating disorder sample. Int J Eat
Disord 2008;41:587–593.
21. Horan W, Kring A, Blanchard J. Anhedonia in schizophrenia: A
References
review of assessment strategies. Schiz Bull 2006;32:259–273.
1. Schmidt U, Treasure J. Anorexia nervosa: Valued and visible. A 22. Friederich F, Kumari V, Uher R, Riga M, Schmidt U, Campbell I,
cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implica- et al. Differential motivational responses to food and pleasura-
tions for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol 2006;45: ble cues in anorexia and bulimia: Startle paradigm. Psych Med
343–366. 2006;36:1327–1335.
2. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Enhanced cognitive behavior 23. Soussignan R, Schaal B, Rigaud D, Royet JP, Jiang T. Hedonic
therapy for eating disorders (‘‘CBT-E’’): An overview. In: Fair- reactivity to visual and olfactory cues: Rapid facial electromyo-
burn CG, editor. Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disor- graphic reactions are altered in anorexia nervosa. Biol Psychol
ders. New York: Guildford Press, 2008. 2011;86:265–272.

International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012 7


TCHANTURIA ET AL.

24. Watson KK, Werling DM, Zucker N, Platt ML. Altered social 36. Eckblad ML, Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Mishlove M. The Revised
reward and attention in anorexia nervosa. Front Psychol Social Anhedonia Scale. Unpublished test (copies available
2010;36:1–8. from T.R. Kwapil, Department of Psychology, University of North
25. Eiber R, Berlin I, de Brettes B, Foulon C, Guelfi JD. Hedonic Carolina at Greensboro, P.O. Box 26170, Greensboro, NC
response to sucrose solutions and the fear of weight gain 27402–6170), 1982.
in patients with eating disorders. Psychiatry Res 2002;113: 37. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Roulin ML. Scales for physical and
173–180. social anhedonia. J Abnormal Psychol 1976;85:374–382.
26. Goodwin RD, Fitzgibbon ML. Social anxiety as a barrier to treat- 38. Leung W, Counture M, Blanchard J, Lin S, Llerena K. Is social
ment for eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2002;32:103–106. anhedonia related to emotional responsivity and expressivity?
27. Speranza M, Loas G, Wallier J, Corcos M. Predictive value of A laboratory study in women. Schizophrenia Res 2010;124:
alexithymia in patients with eating disorders: A 3-year prospec- 66–73.
tive study. J Psychosomatic Res 2007;63:365–371. 39. Pelizza L, Ferrarri A. Anhedonia in schizophrenia and major
28. Keel PK, Dorer DJ, Franko D, Jackson S, Herzog DB. Post-remis- depression: State or trait? Ann Gen Psychiatry 2009;8:22.
sion predictors of relapse in women with eating disorders. Am J 40. Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the Depression Anxiety
Psychiatry 2005;162:2263–2268. Stress Scales, 2nd ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation, 1995.
29. Rastam M, Gillberg C, Wentz E. Outcome of teenage-onset ano- 41. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alex-
rexia nervosa in a Swedish community-based sample. Eur Child ithymia Scale I: Item selection and cross-validation of the factor
Adolesc Psychiatry 2003;12:i78–i90. structure. J Psychosomatic Res 1994;38:23–32.
30. Prince J, Berenbaum H. Alexithymia and hedonic capacity. 42. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences,
J Res Pers 1993;27:15–22. 2nd ed. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum, 1988.
31. Schmidt U, Jiwany A, Treasure J. A controlled study of alexithy- 43. Zucker N, Losh M, Bulik CM, LaBar KS, Piven J, Pelphrey KA. An-
mia in eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry 1993;34:54– orexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorders: Guided investi-
58. gation of social cognitive endophenotypes. Psych Bull 2007;6:
32. Berthoz S, Perdereau F, Godart N, Corcos M, Haviland MG. Ob- 976–1006.
server and self rated alexithymia in eating disorder patients: 44. Oldershaw A, Treasure J, Hambrook D, Tchanturia K, Schmidt
Levels and correspondence among three measures. J Psychoso- U. Is anorexia nervosa a version of autism spectrum disorders?
matic Res 2007;60:341–345. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2011;19:462–474.
33. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical 45. Kliemann D, Dziobek I, Hatri A, Steimke R, Heekeren HR. Atypi-
Manual of Mental Disorders,4th ed. Washington DC: APA, 2000. cal reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spec-
34. Fairburn CG, Beglin, SJ. Assessment of eating disorders: Inter- trum disorders. J Neurosci 2010;30:12281–12287.
view or self-report questionnaire? Int J Eat Disord 1994;16: 46. Morris RW, Weickert CS, Loughland CM. Emotional face proc-
363–370. essing in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2009;22:
35. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clini- 140–146.
cal Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, 47. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive behavior therapy
Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP). New York: Biometrics for eating disorders: A ‘‘transdiagnostic’’ theory and treatment.
Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 2002. Behav Res Ther 2003;41:509–528.

8 International Journal of Eating Disorders 00:0 000–000 2012

You might also like