Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat.

41 (2010) 389e396

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Behavior Therapy and


Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep

Stability of dysfunctional attitudes and early maladaptive schemas: A 9-year


follow-up study of clinically depressed subjects
Catharina E.A. Wang*, Marianne Halvorsen, Martin Eisemann, Knut Waterloo
Department of Psychology, University of Tromsø, N-9037 Tromsø, Norway

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS) and the Young Schema Questionnaire (YSQ) have been suggested
Received 2 July 2009 as relatively stable vulnerability markers for depression and entrenched psychological disorders,
Received in revised form respectively. One-hundred-and-forty-nine clinically depressed (CDs), previously depressed (PDs) and
30 March 2010
never-depressed subjects (NDs) completed the DAS, the YSQ and the Beck Depression Inventory in the
Accepted 13 April 2010
index study and were followed-up nine years later. Results showed: (1) Elevated scores in CDs and PDs as
compared to NDs; (2) Some stability of depressive symptoms; (3) Significant moderate testeretest
Keywords:
correlations for DAS scales and YSQ scales Disconnection and Impaired Limits, also after controlling for
Cognitive vulnerability
Depression
depression severity; (4) A significant reduction in mean score for DAS total, Approval by Others and YSQ
Dysfunctional attitudes scales Disconnection and Impaired Limits; (5) Significant correlations between DAS and YSQ scales. Our
Early maladaptive schemas results support a state-trait vulnerability model of depression comprising stable differences in avail-
ability of dysfunctional attitudes and early maladaptive schemas, but also fluctuating differences in
accessibility of those schemas during the course of depression. The findings point to the significance of
the YSQ scales as vulnerability markers for depression in addition to the more established DAS scales.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction DAS scores have been found to fluctuate with the clinical state,
indicating that dysfunctional attitudes may rather be a state than
Since major depression is a highly prevalent, recurrent, and a trait characteristic. State-dependent effects of dysfunctional
often chronic disorder (Andrade et al., 2003; Kessler et al., 2003) it attitudes have been reported both in cross-sectional (Clark & Beck,
is imperative to increase our knowledge about vulnerability factors 1999; Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998; Power, Duggan, Lee, &
in order to prevent and treat depression. Murray, 1995) and in treatment studies (Beevers & Miller, 2004;
According to Beck’s (1967, 1987) cognitive theory of depression, Hamilton & Abramson, 1983; Zuroff, Blatt, Sanislow, Bondi, &
vulnerability may develop in individuals with a history of loss or Pilkonis, 1999). However, some studies have found DAS scores of
adversity in childhood contributing to the formation of negative remitted depressed patients still elevated after treatment as
self-schemas, containing dysfunctional attitudes. Dysfunctional compared to non-depressed controls (Beevers & Miller, 2004;
attitudes, frequently assessed with the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale Peselow, Robins, Block, Baraouche, & Five, 1990; Zuroff et al., 1999).
(DAS; Weissman & Beck, 1978), include core beliefs such as that Moreover, research has consistently shown that vulnerable as
one’s happiness depends on being perfect, being in control, or on compared to less vulnerable individuals, report more dysfunctional
other people’s approval (Beck, Hollon, Young, Bedrosian, & Budenz, attitudes during negative mood-states (Scher, Ingram, & Segal,
1985). Furthermore, the core beliefs are according to Beck’s theory 2005). Such findings have made several researchers to claim that
assumed to be relatively stable cognitive characteristics that confer it is a simplification of a more complex causality to explain elevated
vulnerability to stress which subsequently may lead to the onset of DAS scores solely with either state or trait factors. For example,
depression. Person and Miranda (1992), in line with Beck et al.’s theory have
Numerous studies have examined the stability of negative hypothesized that putative vulnerability factors are present in
schemas operationalized by means of the DAS. Fairly consistently, vulnerable individuals, but are more or less accessible depending
on mood-state. Accordingly, Zuroff et al. (1999) proposed a state-
trait vulnerability model comprising both stable differences in
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ47 97 43 42 60; fax: þ47 77 64 56 10. availability of cognitive-affective structures, and fluctuating
E-mail address: catharina.wang@uit.no (C.E.A. Wang). differences in accessibility of those structures dependent on

0005-7916/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.04.002
390 C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396

current levels of depressive symptoms. In an 18 months follow-up Abramson, Whitehouse, & Hogan, 2006; Segal, Gemar, &
study of depressed patients they found evidence of trait-like Williams, 1999, 2006; Zuroff, Igreja, & Mongrain, 1990). Compa-
properties of DAS scores, although mood-state dependent effects rably fewer studies have investigated EMSs as predictors of
were evident. depression severity (Harris & Curtin, 2002; Hoffart et al., 2005;
More recently, similar findings have been obtained in a treat- Schmidt, Joiner, Young, & Telch, 1995). More recently, however,
ment study by Beevers and Miller (2004) who assessed cognitive the authors of the present paper found that YSQ scales emerged as
bias, dysfunctional attitudes and depression severity in 121 significant predictors of concurrent depression severity and
subjects who were hospitalized for depression and followed-up depression severity and episodes of Major Depression nine years
during a 6-months outpatient treatment and one year after its later in a sample of 149 currently and previously depressed indi-
completion. Mean score change, testeretest correlation, and path viduals, and never-depressed controls (Halvorsen, Wang,
analysis both demonstrated relative stability over time, but also Eisemann, & Waterloo, 2009).
that fluctuations in mood-state influenced dysfunctional attitudes Previous work on the stability of dysfunctional attitudes has
and cognitive bias. Otto et al. (2007) as well, found in a three-year illustrated the importance of both examining changes in mean
prospective study of a large, community-based sample of 750 scores, i.e., absolute stability and examining relative stability by
women, an association between DAS scores and subsyndromal using correlational analyses (Santor, Bagby, & Joffe, 1997). Zuroff
levels of depressive symptoms indicating mood-state effects on et al. (1999), when investigating the stability of DAS scores, argued
dysfunctional attitudes. However, they also found dysfunctional that “It is possible for a group to show large, significant changes in
attitudes as predictive of future episodes of depression in women, mean scores on a trait in the presence of perfect stability in the
but only when a history of depression was ignored. participants’ relative standing on the trait” (p. 77). However,
Young (1990, 1999) has elaborated on the schema concept to research on the stability of schemas has primarily been carried out
reflect themes of adverse relational experiences in childhood. with cross-sectional and treatment designs. Accordingly, little
Young (1990) hypothesized that connectedness, autonomy, knowledge exists concerning the stability of both schemas and
worthiness, reasonable expectations and realistic limits, are five depressive symptoms in a natural course of major depression. The
primary objectives that the child has to fulfill in order to pursue present study attempts to replicate and extend previous findings by
a healthy development. When caregivers make it difficult for the including the YSQ as a second measure of schemas in addition to the
child to achieve one or more of these five objectives, Young DAS in a sample of clinically depressed (CDs), previously depressed
proposed that Early Maladaptive Schemas (EMSs) will develop. (PDs) and never-depressed subjects (NDs), who were followed-up
Young (1990) originally identified sixteen schemas, which are after nine years without any treatment intervention.
grouped into five domains reflecting the childhood objectives Four aims were addressed: (1) to compare groups of CDs, PDs,
mentioned above: Disconnection, Impaired Autonomy, Undesir- and NDs on dysfunctional attitudes and EMSs, (2) to investigate the
ability, Restricted Self-Expression, and Impaired Limits. In this way, conceptual overlap between dysfunctional attitudes and EMSs in
Young assumed the EMS domains to represent predisposing factors a cross-sectional design, and (3) to examine the relative and (4)
for the development and maintenance of clinical symptom states, absolute stability of dysfunctional attitudes, EMSs, and depressive
and to explain various types of interpersonal and personality symptoms after nine years.
related problems. To assess the EMSs, Young (1990) developed the
Young Schema Questionnaire (YSQ; Young & Brown, 1990).
Beck (1967, 1987) and Young (1990) both suggested that the most 2. Method
important etiological pathways of developing dysfunctional atti-
tudes and EMSs are related to adverse experiences during childhood. 2.1. Participants and procedure
Young also, influenced by the work of Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991)
on attachment theory, emphasized that EMSs refer to the deepest The analysis is based on a sample of 149 subjects taking part in
level of cognitive structures representing the self in relation to other a study on depression and cognitive vulnerability in the years
persons and the environment. Accordingly, EMSs are suggested to be 1997e1999, i.e., the index study (T1) (Wang, Brennen, & Holte,
more persistently activated compared to dysfunctional attitudes, 2005). In the years 2006e2007 (T2) totally 115 participants from
which require the presence of certain stressors or conditions the index study were followed-up. Among the 34 participants who
(Schmidt & Joiner, 2004). Because Young (1999) explicitly empha- were not part of the follow-up study, one had died, 15 were
sized the highly stable and enduring themes of the EMSs, the issue of untraceable and 18 were not willing to take part due to various
stability is probably even more crucial to Young’s theory. Few studies reasons. The mean period of time from the T1 to T2 was nine years
have investigated the long-term stability of EMSs. However, Riso (M ¼ 8.94, Mdn ¼ 9.00, SD ¼ .99).
et al. (2006) examined 55 depressed outpatients over a 2.5e5- The 149 participants in the index study (T1) comprised both
years interval and found moderate to good levels of relative stability undergraduate students at the University of Tromsø and patients
also after controlling for severity of depression at both assessments. consulting their general practitioner, also in Tromsø, Norway. They
Further, they found that the mean-scores of the majority of the EMSs were diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
did not drop significantly in spite of a significant decrease in of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association
depression severity at follow-up. (APA), 1994), using “The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV,
The clinical relevance of demonstrating the relative stability of Axis I disorders” (SCID-CV) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams,
dysfunctional attitudes and EMSs is essential due to the assumption 1997). Based on the information given in the clinical interview,
raised by Beck and Young that dysfunctional attitudes and EMSs the participants were grouped as a) being clinically depressed (CDs;
may predict the development and maintenance of clinical N ¼ 61), b) having experienced a depressive episode during the past
symptom states including depression. Accordingly, to be regarded five years and having fully recovered for at least 8 weeks or longer
as clinically meaningful predictors of vulnerability, also some (PDs; N ¼ 42) or c) having never been clinically depressed (NDs;
relative stability is warranted. Several studies have found N ¼ 46). Subjects with ongoing manic, hypo-manic or psychotic
dysfunctional attitudes as predictive of depression severity, first- symptoms were excluded. None of the participants was treated as
onset, relapse and recurrence of major depression (e.g., Alloy et al., inpatient at the time of assessment. For further description of the
2006; Hankin, Abramson, Miller, & Haeffel, 2004; Iacoviello, Alloy, sampling and group-classification reliability, see Wang et al. (2005).
C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396 391

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are 1986). A full description, including psychometric properties is
presented in Table 1. provided by Chioqueta and Stiles (2004), Dobson and Breiter (1983)
In the index study (T1), the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Form A, and Oliver and Baumgart (1985). In the present study, the Cron-
DAS; Weisman & Beck, 1978), the Young Schema Questionnaire bach’s alphas for the total scores at the two different assessments
(YSQ; Young & Brown, 1990), and the BDI e First Edition (BDI-I; Beck, were .94 and .92, respectively. The Cronbach’s alphas for Perfor-
Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) were administered and completed by all mance Evaluations and Approval by Others were .91 and .82 at T1,
149 participants on the same occasion as the diagnostic interview. At and .92 and .78 at T2, respectively.
the follow-up assessment (T2) the DAS and the YSQ were completed
on separate occasions as the participants took part in a larger study 2.2.2. The Young Schema Questionnaire
over two days on depression and vulnerability (Halvorsen et al., The Young Schema Questionnaire (YSQ; Young & Brown, 1990) is
2009). Accordingly, the DAS was administered first and completed a 205-item self-report inventory to be rated along a six-point Likert
by all 115 participants, while the YSQ was completed by 82 partici- scale ranging from “completely untrue of me” to “describes me
pants some days later. The Beck Depression Inventory e Second perfectly” designed to assess the presence of Early Maladaptive
Edition (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) was administered Schemas (EMS) that may relate to vulnerability to clinical symptom
separately in relation to both the DAS and the YSQ. states and personality related problems. The 205 items are grouped
Among the 115 participants who completed the DAS, there were within 16 subscales which in turn are subsumed into five domain
47 CDs, 39 PDs, and 29 NDs from the index study. Of the 82 scales: 1) Disconnection: Emotional Deprivation, Abandonment/
participants who completed the YSQ, there were 35 CDs, 27 PDs, Instability, Mistrust/Abuse, Social Isolation/Alienation; 2) Impaired
and 20 NDs. The T2 DAS and YSQ sample had a mean age of 37.55 Autonomy: Dependence/Incompetence, Vulnerability to Harm/
years (SD ¼ 9.49 years) and 38.12 years (SD ¼ 9.62 years), respec- Illness, Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self, Subjugation; 3) Undesir-
tively and was predominantly comprised of females (n ¼ 97; 84%; ability: Defectiveness/Shame, Social undesirability, Failure to Ach-
n ¼ 70; 85%). There were no significant differences on any of the ieve; 4) Restricted Self-Expression: Emotional Inhibition, Self-
demographic variables between the original T1-sample of 149 Sacrifice, Unrelenting Standards; 5) Impaired Limits: Entitlement/
participants and the two T2-samples of 115 participants and 82 Grandiosity and Insufficient Self-Control. In the present study,
participants, respectively. Slightly fewer never-depressed partici- scores for individual EMSs were obtained by summing items for
pants from T1 took part in the follow-up study at T2. each subscale and dividing by the number of items in the scale. The
Also at T2, all participants were re-diagnosed and a history of YSQ has been subjected to psychometric evaluation, and the
major depressive episodes between T1 and T2 was taken according majority of the EMSs have been confirmed in student and clinical
to the criteria of the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). Of the totally 86 CDs samples (Lee, Taylor, & Dunn, 1999; Schmidt et al., 1995; Hoffart
and PDs entering the study at T1, 61 (71%) had experienced et al., 2005). Factor-analytic studies on clinical samples have
a recurrent major depressive episode. Twenty-six (30%) subjects provided good support for the scales with the exception of ‘Social
had experienced two or more depressive episodes and seven (8%) Undesirability’ (Hoffart et al., 2005; Lee et al., 1999). Cronbach’s
were chronically depressed. Three individuals (10%), who had alphas for the various subscales at the two different assessments
never experienced a major depressive episode at T1, had a first- varied from .85 to .95.
onset episode between the two points of time (Halvorsen et al.,
2009). For further description of the group-classification reli- 2.2.3. The Beck Depression Inventory e First Edition and Second
ability, see Halvorsen et al. (2009). Edition
The study was approved by the Regional Medical Research The Beck Depression Inventory e First Edition (BDI-I; Beck et al.,
Ethics Committee. All participants gave written informed consent 1979) and The Beck Depression Inventory e Second Edition (BDI-II;
before entering the study. Beck et al., 1996) are versions of a 21-item self-report inventory
designed to assess the presence and severity of depressive symp-
2.2. Measures toms. Both are rated on a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from
0 to 3, reflecting the severity of each item. These scales were
2.2.1. The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Form A) included in the study to assess depression severity when answering
The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Form A) (DAS; Weissman & the DAS and the YSQ at T1 and T2, respectively. Beck and Steer
Beck, 1978) is a 40-item self-report inventory to be rated along (1987) classified BDI-I scores as follows: 0e9 normal range;
a seven-point Likert scale ranging from “totally agree” to “totally 10e18 mild-moderate; 19e29 moderate-severe; and 30e63 severe.
disagree”, designed to assess the presence of dysfunctional atti- The BDI-II scores are classified somewhat different: 0e13 minimal;
tudes that may relate to cognitive vulnerability to depression 14e19 mild; 20e28 moderate; and 29e63 severe (Beck et al., 1996).
(Oliver & Baumgart, 1985). Scores on the DAS can range from 40 to A full description of the two inventories including psychometric
280, with higher scores indicating more dysfunctional attitudes. properties can be found in Beck, Steer, and Garbin (1988), and in
Two major factors, Performance Evaluations and Approval by Steer, Ball, Ranieri, and Beck (1999). In the present study, the
Others, have been found to account for a large proportion of the Cronbach’s alpha for the BDI-I total score was .92, and for the BDI-II
variance in the total DAS score (Cane, Olinger, Gotlib, & Kuiper, total score .94 and .95, respectively.

Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics at T1 for the three groups of subjects (N ¼ 149).

Variable CD (n ¼ 61) PD (n ¼ 42) ND (n ¼ 46) Significance test and p value

M SD M SD M SD
Gender (f/m) 52/9 35/7 35/11 c2 (2) ¼ 1.57, ns
Age 30.89 10.33 27.00 8.25 26.93 9.45 F (2,146) ¼ 3.06, p ¼ .05
Single/recurrent episode 24/37 18/24 c2 (1) ¼ .13, ns
Antidepressant 9 1 c2 (1) ¼ 4.34, p ¼ .037
Note: T1 ¼ Time 1; CD ¼ clinically depressed; PD ¼ previously depressed; ND ¼ never depressed.
392 C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396

3. Results Furthermore, post hoc ANOVAs showed that the groups differed
significantly on all YSQ scales (see Table 2). The effect size (partial eta
3.1. Response patterns of DAS, YSQ and BDI-I squared) in the ANOVAs for all the scales was between .048 for Enti-
tlement and .414 for Disconnection. When applying a Bonferroni
To address the first aim of the study, i.e., to compare groups of adjusted alpha level of .001, CDs remained significantly different from
CDs, PDs, and NDs on the DAS and YSQ domains and subscales, PDs on the YSQ domains Disconnection, Impaired Autonomy, Unde-
Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), with age as cova- sirability and the subscales Abandonment/Instability, Social Isolation/
riate, were calculated followed by univariate post hoc analyses when Alienation, Dependence/Incompetence, Subjugation, Vulnerability to
an overall difference between groups occurred (Bonferroni adjust- Harm/Illness, and Social Undesirability. CDs remained significantly
ment: P < .006 e .05/3 comparisons/3 scales for the DAS scales and different from NDs on all the scales with the exception of Entitlement/
P < .001 e .05/3 comparisons/21 scales for the YSQ scales). Because Grandiosity. PDs remained significantly different from NDs on the
the YSQ scales of the three groups were not normally distributed and YSQ domain Disconnection, and the subscales Abandonment/Insta-
the groups had heterogeneous variances, these data were trans- bility, Mistrust/Abuse, and Social Isolation/Alienation.
formed. Multivariate analyses were conducted with the transformed To test for differences between CDs, PDs and NDs with respect to
and untransformed variables. The comparisons showed equivalent depressive symptoms, an ANCOVA, with age as a covariate, was con-
results. Consequently, parametric tests with the untransformed YSQ ducted on the BDI-I, which indicated that the three groups differed
scales were used for group comparisons. significantly, with CDs and PDs showing elevated scores compared to
As concerns DAS, a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), NDs, (F(2,145) ¼ 78.33, p < .001, partial eta squared ¼ .519) (Table 2).
with age as covariate, indicated significant group differences on the Because obtained group differences on the DAS and the YSQ might
DAS total score (F(2,145) ¼ 19.72, p < .001, partial eta be related to differences in depression severity, bivariate correlations
squared ¼ .214) (Table 2). Moreover, a highly significant main effect were calculated between the BDI-I and the DAS scales, and the BDI-I
for group (ND, PD, and CD) was found in the MANCOVAs, with age and the YSQ scales, respectively. While no significant correlations
as covariate, both for the DAS subscales Approval by Others and were found for the ND-group, some significant positive correlations
Performance Evaluation (F(2,145) ¼ 11.56, p < .001, partial eta were found for the PD-group (r between .33 and .47, p < .05). For the
squared ¼ .138) and for the YSQ scales (F(2,145) ¼ 3.70, p < .001, CD-group, the majority of the correlations were significant (r between
partial eta squared ¼ .313). Post hoc ANOVAs indicated that the .26 and .62, p < .05). Accordingly, depression severity may partly
three groups differed significantly on the DAS scales (see Table 2). account for group differences on the DAS and YSQ scales.
The effect size (partial eta squared) in the ANOVAs was .184 for
Performance Evaluation and .211 for Approval by Others. When 3.2. Conceptual overlap between DAS and EMSs at T1
applying a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of .006, PDs did not
remain significantly different from NDs on the DAS subscale To address the second aim of the study, i.e., to investigate
Performance Evaluation. conceptual overlap between the DAS and the YSQ scales, zero-order

Table 2
Means and standard deviations for the BDI-I, the DAS and the YSQ at T1 for the total sample and for three sub-groups (N ¼ 149).

Variable Total (n ¼ 149) CD (n ¼ 61) PD (n ¼ 42) ND (n ¼ 46) Post hoc univariate ANOVA
controlling for age

M SD M SD M SD M SD F (2,146) Contrasts
BDI-I 8.58 8.61 15.70 8.50 6.40 4.05 1.13 1.68 78.33** CD > PD > ND

DAS total 117.03 35.07 133.20 38.17 116.62 30.44 95.98 21.27 19.72** CD > PD > ND
DAS approval 36.22 10.81 39.75 10.59 38.19 10.60 29.74 8.32 16.33** CD, PD > ND
DAS performance 40.12 16.44 48.03 18.33 38.95 13.71 30.70 9.63 19.40** CD > PD > ND

Disconnection 1.93 .81 2.45 .86 1.92 .54 1.26 .26 51.19** CD > PD > ND
Emotional deprivation .96 .97 2.45 1.06 1.98 .85 1.29 .38 25.52** CD > PD > ND
Abandonment/instability 2.01 .92 2.55 1.00 1.99 .71 1.31 .32 39.09** CD > PD > ND
Mistrust/abuse 1.85 .83 2.28 .95 1.85 .67 1.26 .26 30.57** CD > PD > ND
Social isolation/alienation 1.92 1.03 2.52 1.22 1.88 .56 1.17 .33 36.34** CD > PD > ND

Impaired autonomy 1.72 .67 2.15 .77 1.63 .34 1.23 .21 38.92** CD > PD > ND
Dependence/incompetence 1.56 .73 1.95 .95 1.44 .35 1.15 .16 22.23** CD > PD, ND
Vulnerability to harm/illness 1.80 .70 2.20 .81 1.75 .47 1.31 .28 30.13** CD > PD > ND
Enmeshment/undeveloped self 1.52 .74 1.90 .95 1.42 .45 1.11 .20 18.90** CD > PD, ND
Subjugation 2.00 1.00 2.57 1.20 1.89 .61 1.34 .42 25.17** CD > PD > ND

Undesirability 1.84 .86 2.35 1.00 1.77 .56 1.23 .25 34.31** CD > PD > ND
Defectiveness/shame 1.75 .85 2.20 1.06 1.68 .48 1.21 .29 25.26** CD > PD > ND
Social undesirability 2.00 1.01 2.56 1.20 1.89 .69 1.36 .39 28.03** CD > PD > ND
Failure to achieve 1.78 .97 2.30 1.16 1.74 .70 1.12 .17 26.30** CD > PD > ND

Restricted self-expression 2.37 .78 2.76 .89 2.36 .60 1.86 .37 24.14** CD > PD > ND
Emotional inhibition 2.02 .98 2.49 1.15 2.04 .75 1.37 .40 22.79** CD > PD > ND
Self-sacrifice 2.83 .89 3.22 .98 2.82 .72 2.35 .64 14.91** CD > PD > ND
Unrelenting standards 2.25 .88 2.56 1.00 2.23 .77 1.86 .61 11.09** CD > PD, ND

Impaired limits 1.95 .63 2.12 .70 2.03 .58 1.66 .46 11.81** CD, PD > ND
Entitlement/grandiosity 1.86 .67 1.87 .65 2.04 .76 1.69 .57 3.67* PD > ND
Insufficient self-control 2.04 0 .76 2.38 .88 2.02 .59 1.63 .48 20.06** CD > PD > ND

Note: T1 ¼ Time 1; CD ¼ clinically depressed; PD ¼ previously depressed; ND ¼ never depressed; BDI ¼ Beck Depression Inventory; DAS ¼ Dysfunctional Attitude Scale;
YSQ ¼ Young Schema Questionnaire. Welch’s F. *p < .05; **p < .001. Post hoc tests were generated from Bonferroni at the level of p < .05 (Two-tailed).
C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396 393

and partial correlations controlling for depression severity were Furthermore, YSQ domain scales showed significant testeretest
calculated (Table 3). As shown, significant moderately to highly correlations for all the domain scales (r between .48 and .72,
positive correlations were found between a majority of the DAS and p < .001). When controlling for depression severity, both at T1 and
the YSQ scales (r between .29 and .74, p < .001). However, when T2, correlations remained significant at .001-level only for the
controlling for depression severity the correlations decreased (r domain scales Disconnection and Impaired Limits (r between .50
between .26 and .59, p < .001). The strongest correlation was found and .59, p < .001). Moreover, the YSQ subscales that showed
between the DAS total score and the YSQ domain scale Undesir- significant moderate testeretest correlations also after controlling
ability whereas the weakest correlation was found between the for depression severity, both at T1 and T2, were: Mistrust/Abuse,
DAS total score and the YSQ domain scale Impaired Limits. In Social Isolation/Alienation, Social Undesirability, Entitlement/
general, the correlations between the Performance Evaluation and Grandiosity, and Insufficient Self-control (r between .50 and .60,
the YSQ subscales were stronger than the correlations between the p < .001). Accordingly, while some domains and subscales were
Approval by Others and the YSQ subscales. When using the trans- strongly affected by depression severity, others were only margin-
formed rather than the YSQ scale’s raw scores, the above analyses ally so. When excluding currently depressed participants (N ¼ 13) at
revealed a similar pattern. T2 and re-running the analyses, the results revealed a similar
pattern, i.e., the domain and subscales that showed to be most
3.3. Relative stability of DAS, EMSs and BDI between T1 and T2 affected by depressive symptoms (when controlling for BDI), were
also most affected by excluding currently depressed subjects at T2.
To address the third aim of the study, i.e., the examination of the When using the transformed rather than the YSQ scale’s raw scores,
relative stability of the DAS and the YSQ scales from T1 to T2, zero- the above analyses revealed a similar pattern.
order and partial correlations controlling for depression severity at To explore the relative stability of the BDI, from T1 to T2,
both time points were calculated. Although similar to the BDI-I, the bivariate correlations were calculated for the DAS and the YSQ
BDI-II for instance replaces previous items relating to changes in sample using the converted BDI-I to BDI-II total score at T1 and BDI-
body image, somatic preoccupation, and work difficulty with other II total score at T2. As shown in Table 4, significant moderate
depressive symptoms such as worthlessness, concentration diffi- testeretest correlations were found for the DAS sample and for the
culties and loss of energy (Beck et al., 1996). Because of differences YSQ sample. However, when excluding participants who were
in psychometric characteristics of these two versions of the BDI, we currently depressed at T2, the testeretest correlations were
used the scoring adjustment recommended in the BDI-II manual reduced for the DAS sample (r ¼ .35, p < .01) and the YSQ sample
(Beck et al., 1996, pp. 32e34). That is, when controlling for (r ¼ .28, p < .05).
depression severity at T1, we used a converted BDI-I to BDI-II total Additionally, when using the original BDI-I total score at T1
score. Testeretest correlations for scores of the DAS and the YSQ rather that the converted score, the above analyses revealed
scales are presented in Table 4. a similar pattern.
As shown, DAS total and the two subscales, showed significant
testeretest correlations also after controlling for depression
severity both at T1 and T2 (r between .52 and .63, p < .001). 3.4. Absolute stability of DAS, EMSs and BDI between T1 and T2
Approval by Others was least affected by depression severity. When
excluding currently depressed participants (N ¼ 19) at T2 and re- To address the fourth aim of the study, i.e., to examine the
running the analyses, the results revealed a similar pattern. absolute stability of the DAS and YSQ scales, from T1 to T2, t-tests

Table 3
Correlations between the YSQ and the DAS at T1 (N ¼ 149).

YSQ DAS

DAS total (control BDI-I) DAS approval (control BDI-I) DAS performance (control BDI-I)
Disconnection .69*** .51*** .59*** .41*** .68*** .49***
Emotional deprivation .53*** .34*** .49*** .32*** .51*** .30***
Abandonment/instability .66*** .49*** .61*** .47*** .63*** .43***
Mistrust/abuse .63*** .43*** .46*** .25** .63*** .44***
Social isolation/alienation .58*** .37*** .47*** .26** .60*** .39***

Impaired autonomy .68*** .48*** .60*** .43*** .66*** .44***


Dependence/incompetence .65*** .45*** .55*** .36*** .63*** .41***
Vulnerability to harm/illness .58*** .34*** .50*** .29*** .55*** .31***
Enmeshment/undeveloped self .36*** .13 ns .38*** .20* .37*** .13 ns
Subjugation .68*** .50*** .58*** .41*** .65*** .45***

Undesirability .74*** .57*** .61*** .43*** .73*** .56***


Defectiveness/shame .74*** .59*** .58*** .41*** .74*** .58***
Social undesirability .65*** .44*** .57*** .38*** .63*** .40***
Failure to achieve .64*** .43*** .52*** .30*** .64*** .43***

Restricted self-expression .71*** .57*** .62*** .48*** .69*** .54***


Emotional inhibition .60*** .40*** .48*** .27** .60*** .40***
Self-sacrifice .53*** .38*** .50*** .36*** .50*** .33***
Unrelenting standards .68*** .57*** .61*** .50*** .66*** .55***

Impaired limits .49*** .38*** .44*** .34*** .45*** .33***


Entitlement/grandiosity .29*** .29*** .27** .26*** .26** .26**
Insufficient self-control .56*** .39*** .50*** .35*** .52*** .34***

Note: T1 ¼ Time 1; BDI ¼ Beck Depression Inventory; DAS ¼ Dysfunctional Attitude Scale; YSQ ¼ Young Schema Questionnaire.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 (Two-tailed).
394 C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396

Table 4 (t(68) ¼ 2.02, p ¼ .047), and Vulnerability to Harm/Illness (t


Means, standard deviations and correlations between T1 and T2, for the BDI, the DAS (68) ¼ 2.38, p ¼ .020). However, when using a Bonferroni adjusted
(N ¼ 115) and the YSQ (N ¼ 82).
alpha level of .002, no significant differences in mean scores of the
Not control M-T1/ SD-T1/ scales remained. Subsample analyses revealed that only PDs’ mean
BDI/(Control M-T2 SD-T2 scores were significantly reduced in the same manner as for the total
BDI  2)
sample, in addition to a significant reduction in a few more
BDI e DASa .52*** 9.7/8.5 9.4/9.8
subscales. This pattern remained after excluding currently
BDI e YSQa .50*** 10.0/8.3 9.5/10.0
depressed participants at T2 and using a Bonferroni adjusted alpha
DAS total .71*** (.61***) 116.1/106.3 33.9/37.1 level of .002. When using the transformed rather than the YSQ
DAS approval .67*** (.63***) 36.5/34.1 10.7/9.8
DAS performance .66*** (.52***) 39.5/39.2 15.9/14.6
scale’s raw scores, the above analyses revealed a similar pattern.
Finally, we examined in a similar way, mean-level stability of
Disconnection .72*** (.50***) 1.9/1.8 .8/.8
scores on the BDI for the follow-up samples, as BDI-II were
Emotional deprivation .66*** (.45***) 1.9/2.0 .9/1.0
Abandonment/instability .56*** (.34**) 2.0/1.9 1.0/.8 administrated in relation to both DAS and YSQ at T2 (Table 4). We
Mistrust/abuse .73*** (.60***) 1.9/1.7 .8/.7 used the converted BDI-I to BDI-II total score when comparing BDI
Social isolation/alienation .67*** (.52***) 1.9/1.8 .9/1.0 at T1 and T2. The mean scores on the BDI-II, administrated both
Impaired autonomy .48*** (.21 ns) 1.7/1.6 .6/.6 with the DAS and YSQ, respectively, were not significantly different
Dependence/incompetence .27** (.04 ns) 1.5/1.4 .5/.6 at retest for the total sample. However, when excluding currently
Vulnerability to harm/illness .56*** (.47***) 1.8/1.7 .7/.7 depressed participants at T2, mean scores on BDI were significantly
Enmeshment/undeveloped self .20 ns (.16 ns) 1.6/1.5 .8/.6
reduced at T2 in relation to both re-assessments (t(95) ¼ 3.15,
Subjugation .54*** (.18 ns) 2.0/2.0 .9/1.0
p ¼ .002; t(68) ¼ 2.83, p ¼ .006), respectively. Subsample analyses
Undesirability .60*** (.27*) 1.8/1.8 .8/.8 revealed: (1) a significant reduction in mean-scores for CDs at both
Defectiveness/shame .64*** (.30**) 1.7/1.7 .8/.9
re-assessments, (2) a significant elevation of mean scores at the
Social undesirability .67*** (.50***) 2.0/1.9 .9/1.0
Failure to achieve .42*** (.22*) 1.6/1.7 .9/.9 DAS assessment at T2 for NDs, and finally (3) no significant change
in mean scores for PDs at T2. These results persisted when
Restricted self-expression .59*** (.34**) 2.4/2.3 .7/.7
Emotional inhibition .54*** (.35**) 2.0/1.8 .9/.8
excluding currently depressed participants at T2.
Self-sacrifice .57*** (.45***) 2.8/2.9 .9/.9
Unrelenting standards .57*** (.41***) 2.2/2.3 .9/.9 4. Discussion
Impaired limits .58*** (.59***) 2.0/1.8 .6/.7
Entitlement/grandiosity .53*** (.56***) 1.9/1.8 .7/.7 The present study sought to increase our knowledge about
Insufficient self-control .54*** (.51***) 2.0/1.9 .7/.8 cognitive characteristics that may predispose to depression by
Note: T1 ¼ Time 1; T2 ¼ Time 2; BDI ¼ Beck Depression Inventory; examining the relative and absolute stability and conceptual
DAS ¼ Dysfunctional Attitude Scale; YSQ ¼ Young Schema Questionnaire. overlap between dysfunctional attitudes and early maladaptive
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 (Two-tailed). schemas (EMSs).
a
Converted BDI-I to BDI-II total score.
First, to begin with the stability of depressive symptoms,
a significant relative and absolute stability was found over the time
span of nine years, but the testeretest correlation decreased in
for paired samples were used to examine mean-level stability of addition to a significant reduction in mean-level scores, when
scores of the DAS (N ¼ 115) and the YSQ scales (N ¼ 82) for the excluding currently depressed participants at T2. This finding was
follow-up samples (Table 4). T-tests were first performed on the not surprising, given the fact that subjects diagnosed with a major
total sample and next on each subsample of CDs, PDs, and NDs (i.e., depression usually report high scores on the BDI. Subsample
the subsamples reflecting diagnostic status at T1). In order to analyses indicated however, stable and elevated level of depressive
correct for multiple testing, a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level was symptoms in PDs. Since several researchers have pointed out that
used (P ¼ .017 e .05/3 scales for the DAS and P ¼ .002 e .05/21 scales a negative mood-state may increase accessibility of putative
for the YSQ scales). vulnerability factors in vulnerable individuals (Person & Miranda,
In relation to the DAS scales, mean scores on DAS total 1992; Zuroff et al., 1999), this finding is important. Accordingly,
(t(114) ¼ 3.85, p < .001) and Approval by Others (t(114) ¼ 2.99, our results indicate that vulnerable individuals (i.e., PDs and CDs),
p ¼ .003) were significantly reduced at retest but not Performance although going in and out of major depressive episodes, may
Evaluation. Also, when excluding participants who were currently display some stability of depressive symptoms.
depressed (N ¼ 19) at T2 and re-running the analyses, the results With regard to the stability of schemas characterizing individ-
were equivalent. Subsample analyses revealed that only CDs’ mean uals vulnerable to depression, we found significant relative stability
scores were significantly reduced in the same manner as for the of dysfunctional attitudes and several EMSs, also after controlling
total sample. Furthermore, when also excluding currently for depression severity at both assessments, indicating that an
depressed participants at T2, PDs showed the same pattern of mean elevation of these schemas are not merely a consequence of
score reduction as CDs. increased levels of depressive symptoms, but may also constitute
Concerning the YSQ scales, mean scores for the vast majority of traits. In general, the testeretest correlations of the DAS, and
the domains and subscales were not significantly different at retest especially the DAS Approval by Others, were moderate significant
for the total sample with the exception of a significant reduction on and little influenced by depression severity. Analyses on absolute
the following subscales: Abandonment/Instability (t(81) ¼ 2.02, stability, however, revealed the opposite pattern, i.e., Approval by
p ¼ .047), Mistrust/Abuse (t(81) ¼ 2.70, p ¼ .009), and Emotional Others, in addition to the DAS total, showed a significant reduction
Inhibition (t(81) ¼ 2.40, p ¼ .019) at T2. Also, when excluding in mean score from T1 to T2, whereas this was not the case for
participants who were currently depressed (N ¼ 13) at T2 and re- Performance Evaluation.
running the analyses the results were equivalent in addition to Furthermore, testeretest correlations of the YSQ scales indi-
a significant reduction in mean scores on the domain scales of cated moderate significant relative stability for the domain scales
Disconnection (t(68) ¼ 2.46, p ¼ .016) and Impaired Limits (t Disconnection and Impaired Limits, also after controlling for
(68) ¼ 2.03, p ¼ .046), and the subscales of Social Isolation/Alienation depression severity at both assessments. Analyses on absolute
C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396 395

stability, however, showed the opposite pattern. That is, only three research is needed to investigate the specificity of the EMSs in
subscales showed a significant reduction in mean scores, of which depression by including a control group with psychological disor-
two from the domain scale of Disconnection. After excluding ders different from major depression. Finally, explorative analyses
currently depressed at T2, a significant reduction in mean score was showed now differences in demographic variables between those
also evident for the domain scale Impaired Limits. Accordingly, the who were followed-up compared to those who did not participate
DAS Approval by Others, and the YSQ domain scales Disconnection at T2. However, as parameter estimates can be inflated by list-wise
and Impaired Limits, were the most stable scales on relative deletion, the withdrawal of participants from T1 to T2 may still be
stability, but least stable on absolute stability. considered as a limitation of the study.
Regarding the other three YSQ domains, i.e., Impaired In conclusion, the results from the present study support a state-
Autonomy, Undesirability, and Restricted Self-Expression, the trait vulnerability model of depression comprising stable differ-
testeretest correlations were moderately significant, but dropped ences in availability of dysfunctional attitudes and EMSs, but also
considerable when controlling for depression severity. While the fluctuating differences in accessibility of those schemas during the
Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self subscale showed no stability at all, course of depression. Furthermore, the findings point to the
the Dependence/Incompetence and Subjugation subscales seemed significance of the YSQ scales as vulnerability markers for depres-
to be especially mood sensitive. Previous research has reported that sion in addition to the more established DAS scales.
themes of dependence, incompetence and subjugation may be
considered as cognitive characteristics, which increase vulnera-
Acknowledgements
bility to depression (Beck, 1983; Blatt, 1974; Dunkley, Sanislow,
Grilo, & McGlashan, 2004). However, our findings indicate that
The authors would like to thank the participants and the
these schemas may be highly state dependent and more charac-
research assistants who contributed in the data collection and
teristic of the depressive state rather than being a predisposing
Professor Mick Power for his valuable comments on the statistical
vulnerability factor.
analyses and on an early draft of the paper. The project has been
As concerns conceptual overlap between the DAS and the YSQ
financially supported by the Norwegian Research Council, the
scales, cross-sectional analyses revealed significant positive corre-
Norwegian Foundation for Health and Rehabilitation, the Norwe-
lations, especially between the DAS total and the YSQ domain
gian Council for Mental Health, and the Psychiatric Research Centre
Undesirability. Also, our findings indicate that core beliefs
of Northern Norway.
embedded in the subscale of DAS Performance Evaluation were
more strongly related to the EMSs as compared to core beliefs
comprised in the subscale of DAS Approval by Others. This is References
especially true for the EMSs included in the YSQ subscales of
Defectiveness/Shame and Unrelenting standards, where the Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality
development. American Psychologist, 46, 331e341.
correlation to Performance Evaluation remained strong also after Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Panzarella, C., &
controlling for depression severity. Generally, these scales reflect Rose, D. T. (2006). Prospective incidence of first onsets and recurrence of
themes related to having a negative self-concept, being highly depression in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 145e156.
perfectionistic or self-critical, and/or sensitive to performance American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
evaluation, which have shown to be cognitive characteristics pre- disorders (4th ed.).. Washington, DC.
disposing to depression (Zuroff, Mongrain, & Santor, 2004). American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., text rev.).. Washington, DC.
The weakest correlations between the DAS and YSQ scales were Andrade, L., Caraveo-Anduaga, J. J., Berglund, P., Bijl, R. V., De Graaf, R.,
obtained for the two YSQ subscales Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self Vollebergh, W., et al. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive episodes:
and Entitlement/Grandiosity. While the first one was highly mood- results from the International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE)
surveys. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 12, 3e21.
dependent, i.e., showing no significant correlation when controlling
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New
for depression severity, the subscale of Entitlement/Grandiosity was York: Harper & Row.
the only scale that did not show a significant difference between CDs Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: new perspectives. In
P. J. Clayton, & Barrett. (Eds.), Treatment of depression: Old controversies and new
and NDs, indicating no mood-state effect. According to Young,
approaches (pp. 265e290). New York: Raven.
Klosko, and Weishaar’s theory (2003), the Entitlement/Grandiosity Beck, A. T. (1987). Cognitive models of depression. Journal of Cognitive Psycho-
reflects overcompensation for the EMSs of Emotional Deprivation therapy: An International Quarterly, 1, 5e37.
and may be regarded as the counterpart of a depressive reaction. Beck, A. T., Hollon, S. D., Young, J. E., Bedrosian, R. C., & Budenz, D. (1985). Treatment
of depression with cognitive therapy and amitriptyline. Archives of General
This is an interestingly point of view given the fact that PDs scored Psychiatry, 42, 142e148.
significantly higher on this schema compared to NDs. A possible Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.
interpretation of this finding to be tested in further studies may New York: Guilford.
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory manual. New York: The
be that in PDs, the Entitlement/Grandiosity schema is activated as Psychological Corporation/Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.
self-defence against negative core beliefs and other maladaptive Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory (2nd
schemas which may be more present when currently depressed. ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck
Taken together, our findings indicate that the construct of EMSs, as Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology
reflected in the YSQ, has unique conceptual bearings and may be Review, 8, 77e100.
fruitful in the understanding of depression-prone cognitive char- Beevers, C. G., & Miller, I. W. (2004). Depression-related negative cognition: mood-
state and trait dependent properties. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28,
acteristics in future studies. 293e307.
Limitations of this study should be mentioned. Since our sample Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective
comprised of mainly female and mildly depressed subjects, future depression. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 29, 107e157.
Cane, D. B., Olinger, L. J., Gotlib, I. H., & Kuiper, N. A. (1986). Factor structure of the
studies are necessary to investigate in clinical samples the gener-
Dysfunctional Attitude Scale in a student population. Journal of Clinical
alizability of our findings to both more severely depressed and male Psychology, 42, 307e309.
individuals. Furthermore, due to the fact that almost all of the YSQ Chioqueta, A. P., & Stiles, T. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the Norwegian
domains and subscales were elevated in the depression-prone version of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Form A). Cognitive Behaviour
Therapy, 33, 83e86.
individuals, it is elusive whether any of the EMSs is representing Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy
a unique vulnerability factor to depression. Accordingly, further of depression. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
396 C.E.A. Wang et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 389e396

Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: reliability Power, M. J., Duggan, C. F., Lee, A. S., & Murray, R. M. (1995). Dysfunctional attitudes
and validity of three measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 107e109. in depressed and recovered depressed patients and their first-degree relatives.
Dunkley, D. M., Sanislow, C. A., Grilo, C. M., & McGlashan, T. (2004). Validity of DAS Psychological Medicine, 25, 87e93.
perfectionism and need for approval in relation to the five-factor model of Riso, L. P., Froman, S. E., Raouf, M., Gable, P., Maddux, R. E., Turini-Santorelli, N., et al.
personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1391e1400. (2006). The long-term stability of early maladaptive schemas. Cognitive Therapy
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). The Structured and Research, 30, 515e529.
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I Disorders e Clinician Version (SCID-CV). Santor, D. A., Bagby, R. M., & Joffe, R. T. (1997). Evaluating stability and change in personality
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1354e1362.
Halvorsen, M., Wang, C. E., Eisemann, M., & Waterloo, K. (24 July 2009). Dysfunc- Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnera-
tional attitudes and early maladaptive schemas as predictors of depression: a 9- bility: empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diathesis in
year follow-up study. Cognitive Therapy and Research. doi:10.1007/s10608-009- unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 487e510.
9259-5. [Epub ahead of print]. Schmidt, N. B., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2004). Global maladaptive schemas, negative life
Hamilton, W. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (1983). Cognitive patterns and major depressive events and psychological distress. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
disorder: a longitudinal study in a hospital setting. Journal of Abnormal Assessment, 26, 65e72.
Psychology, 92, 173e184. Schmidt, N. B., Joiner, T. E., Young, J. E., & Telch, M. J. (1995). The schema questionnaire:
Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Miller, N., & Haeffel, G. J. (2004). Cognitive vulnera- investigation of psychometric properties and the hierarchical structure of
bility-stress theories of depression: examining affective specificity in the a measure of maladaptive schemas. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 295e301.
prediction of depression versus anxiety in three prospective studies. Cognitive Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S. (1999). Differential cognitive response to
Therapy and Research, 28, 309e345. a mood challenge following successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy
Harris, A. E., & Curtin, L. (2002). Parental perceptions, early maladaptive schemas, for unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 3e10.
and depressive symptoms in young adults. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, Segal, Z. V., Kennedy, S., Gemar, M., Hood, K., Pedersen, R., & Buis, T. (2006).
405e416. Cognitive reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive
Hoffart, A., Sexton, H., Hedley, L. M., Wang, C. E., Holthe, H., Haugum, J. A., et al. relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 749e755.
(2005). The structure of maladaptive schemas: a confirmatory factor analysis Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Dimensions of the Beck
and a psychometric evaluation of factor-derived scales. Cognitive Therapy and Depression Inventory-II in clinically depressed outpatients. Journal of Clinical
Research, 29, 627e644. Psychology, 55, 117e128.
Iacoviello, B. M., Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., & Hogan, M. E. Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2005). Mechanisms of recurrent depression:
(2006). The course of depression in individuals at high and low cognitive risk a cognitive battle model and some preliminary results. Clinical Psychology and
for depression: a prospective study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, 61e69. Psychotherapy, 12, 427e442.
Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of the
New York: Guilford Press. Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale: a preliminary investigation. In Paper presented at
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K. R., et al. the annual meeting of the Education Research Association, Toronto, Canada.
(2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the Young, J. E. (1990). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders. Sarasota, FL: Profes-
National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of American Medical sional Resource Press.
Association, 289, 3095e3105. Young, J. E. (1999). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused
Lee, C. W., Taylor, G., & Dunn, J. (1999). Factor structure of the schema questionnaire approach (rev. ed.). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press.
in a large clinical sample. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 441e451. Young, J. E., & Brown, G. (1990). Young schema questionnaire. New York: Cognitive
Oliver, J. M., & Baumgart, E. P. (1985). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: psycho- Therapy Centre of New York.
metric properties and relation to depression in an unselected adult population. Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner’s
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 161e167. guide. New York: The Guilford Press.
Otto, M. W., Teachman, B. A., Cohen, L. S., Soares, C. N., Vitonis, A. F., & Harlow, B. L. Zuroff, D. C., Blatt, S. J., Sanislow, C. A., III, Bondi, C. M., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1999).
(2007). Dysfunctional attitudes and episodes of major depression: predictive Vulnerability to depression: reexamining state dependence and relative
validity and temporal stability in never-depressed, depressed, and recovered stability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 76e89.
women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 475e483. Zuroff, D. C., Igreja, I., & Mongrain, M. (1990). Dysfunctional attitudes, dependency,
Person, J. B., & Miranda, J. (1992). Cognitive theories of vulnerability to depression: and selfcriticism as predictors of depressive mood states: a 12-month longi-
reconciling negative evidence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 458e502. tudinal study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 315e326.
Peselow, E. D., Robins, C. R., Block, P., Barouche, F., & Five, R. R. (1990). Dysfunctional Zuroff, D. C., Mongrain, M., & Santor, D. A. (2004). Conceptualizing and measuring
attitudes in depressed patients before and after clinical treatment and in personality vulnerability to depression: comment on Coyne and Whiffen
normal control subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 439e444. (1995). Psychological Bulletin, 130, 489e511.

You might also like