Speech Perception in Infants PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Speech Perception in Infants

Peter D. Eimas; Einar R. Siqueland; Peter Jusczyk; James Vigorito

Science, New Series, Vol. 171, No. 3968. (Jan. 22, 1971), pp. 303-306.

Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819710122%293%3A171%3A3968%3C303%3ASPII%3E2.0.CO%3B2-V

Science is currently published by American Association for the Advancement of Science.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/aaas.html.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic
journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers,
and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take
advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

http://www.jstor.org
Mon Feb 18 09:29:12 2008
excreted by ten hyperkinetic children References and Wotes 9. G . Brooker, L. J. Thomas, M. hf. Appleman,
Biochernisfrj~7. 4157 (1968).
off medication was similar to that ex- 1. W. E. Bunney, Jr., G. F. Borge, D. L. 10. E. W. Sutherlznd. G. A. Robison, R. W.
Murphy, F . K. Goodwin, paper presented at Butcher, Circ~rlatiorl 37. 179 (1968).
creted by an age-matched group of the annual meeting of the American Psychi- 11. R. C . Haynes, S. B. Koritz, F. G. Pcron,
normal controls. In addition, the mean atric Assoc., Bal Harbour, Fla., 1969; W. E. J . Hiol. Chenr. 234, 1421 (1959).
Bunncy, Jr., D. L. Murphy, F. K. Goodwin, 12. R. V. Fnrese, L. G. Linarelli, W. H. Glins-
values for both the hyperactive patients G. F. Borgc, Lancet 1970-1, 1022 (1970). mann, B. R. Ditzion, M. I. Paul, G. L. Pauk,
and control children were in the same 2. H. Crarner and W. Kuhlo. Acta Nerrrol. Endocriizolo~y 85. 867 (1969).
Psychiaf. Belg. 67, 658 (1967). 13. P. S. Schonhofer, I. F. Skidmore, M. I. Paul,
range as our adult normal controls. 3. M. I. Paul, B. R. Ditzion, G. L. Pauk, D. S. B. R. Ditzion, G. L. Pauk. G. Kiishna, B. B.
After prolonged physical activity (foot- Janowsky, Amer. J. Ps)'clziat. 126, 1493 Brodic, unpublished data.
(1970); M. I. Paul. B. R. Ditzion, D. S. 14. R. Strom-Olsen and H. W. Weil-Malherbe,
ball), there was no significant differ Janonsky, Lancet 1970-1, 88 (1970). J . Ment. Sci. 104, 696 (1958); A. Bergsman.
ence between the pre- and postexercise 4. Y. fI. Abdullah and K. Hamadah, I.arrcet Acta Psycl?irrt. Nerrrol. Scanrl. Srdppl. 33,
1970-1, 378 (1970). 5133 (1959); N. Shinfuku, 0. Michio, K.
levels of urinary cyclic AMP in seven 5. M. I. Paul, H. Cramer, F. K. Goodwin, ibid., Masao, Yotlngo Acta filerl. 5, 109 (1961);
p. 996; Arch. Gen. Psj>chiaf.,in press. R. B. Sloane, W. Hughes, hf. L. Haust, Can.
normal subjects. In contrast to our Psychiat. Ass. J . 11, 6 (1966).
6. M. I. Paul, B. R. Ditzion, G. L. Pauk.
findings, one study suggested that ex- Phar~ttacology 3, 148 (1970). 15. J. J . Schildkraut, E. K. Gordon, J . Durell,
J . Psyclziat. Re.r. 3, 213 (1965).
ercise may elevate urinary cyclic AMP 7. G. L. Gessa, J. Forn, A. Taglian~onte, G. 16. W. E. Bunney, Jr., and J . M. Davis, Arch.
Krishna, in Role of Cyclic A M P in Ser[ronnl
levels ( 17). Robison et al. also recently Function, E. Costa and P. Greengard, Eds.
Cen. Psycltiat. 13, 483 (1965).
(Raven, New York, in press). 17. D. Eccleston, R. Loose, I. A. P~illar,R. F.
reported normal levels of cerebral spinal Sugden, Lancet 1970-11, 612 (1970).
8. \V. E. Bunney and D. A. Hamburg. Arch. 18. G. R. Robison et al., ibid., p. 1028.
fluid cyclic AMP in manic patients; Gen. Psyclziat. 9, 280 (1963); A. Beige], D.
however, they did not study patients at Murphy, W. E. Bunncy, unpublished data. 23 July 1970; revised 14 October 1970 m
the time of the switch into mania and
thtrs would have missed a transient
marked peak at that time (IS). In our
studies, cyclic AMP excretion is inde- Speech Perception in Infants
pendent of age and sex ( 3 ).We have re-
viewed the known factors influencing Abstract. Discrimination o j synthetic speech .so~~nds was st~idiecl in 1- and 4-
cyclic AMP excretion ( 3 , 5), but the month-old infants. T h e speech so~trzdsvaried along alr acou~ticdiinension pre-
relative proportion of cyclic AMP com- viously sholtvz to cue phonemic distinctions arnong tlze voiced and voiceless stop
ing from extrarenal sources remains to consonants in adults. Dircrimii~ahility was measured by an itzcrecise itz cotzdi-
be determined. Thus, at this time, one tiorzed response rate to a second speech ~ o u n dalter habituation to the first speech
cannot state whether the cyclic AMP ,sound. Recovery from habituation was greater for a given acoustic difference
response is mediated centrally or is a ~ztherz the tn-o stimuli wete from different adult phonemic categories than when
reflection of peripheral metabolism. De- they were from the same category. T h e discontinuity in discrimination at tlze
creases in the excretion of urinary cy- region o/ the adult phonemic bourzdary was taken as evidence for cafegorical
clic AMP have been demonstrated in perception.
patients with pseudohypoparathyroidism.
Thus, changes in calcium metaboliqm In this study of speech perception, investigation of this nature (2) revealed
may be associated with alterations in cy- it was found that 1- and 4-month-old that the perception of this cue was very
clic AMP metabolism ( 3 ) .Although the infants were able to discriminate the nearly categorical in the sense that
changes in urinary cyclic AMP may acoustic cue urderlying the adult pho- listeners could discriminate continuous
be secondary to catecholamine or cal- nemic distinction between the voi ed variations in the relative onset of the
cium changes, our evidence documents and voiceless stop consonants / b / and first formant very little better than they
an alteration in an important process / p / . Moreover, and morc important, could identify the sound patterns abso-
that acconlpanies and, in at least one there was a tendency in these subjects lutely. That is, listeners could readily
instance, preceded gross behavioral toward categorical perception: discrim- discriminate between the voiced and
changes. This further suggests the im- ination of the same physical difference voiceless stop consonants, just as they
portance of biochemical changes in the was reliably better across the adult would differentially label them, but they
manic-depressive illness. It is of inter- phonemic boundary than within the were virtually unable to hear intra-
est to consider the possibility that the adult phonemic category. phonemic ditt'erences, despite the f a 3
concept of cyclic AMP as a trigger Earlier research using synthetic that the acoustic variation was the same
mechanism for metabolic processes may speech sounds with adult subjects un- in both conditions. The most measur-
be relevant to the switch process from covered a sufficient cue for the per- able indication of this catcgorical per-
depression to mania. ceived distinction in English between ception was the occurrence of a high
MICHAELI. PAUL the voiced and voiceless forms of the peak of discriminability at the boundary
Neuropsychiatric Institute, Centt7r stop consonants, /b-p/, /d-t/, and /g- between the voiced and voiceless stops,
for the Health Sciences, University k / , occurring in absolute initial position and a nearly chance level of discrim-
o f California, Los Angeles 90024 ( 1 ) . The cue, which is illustrated in inability among stimuli that represented
HINKICH C R A M E H the spectrograms displayed in Fig. 1, is acoustic variations of the same pho-
Laboratory o f Chernical Phart~~ncology, the onset of the first formant relative to neme. Such categorical perception is
National Heart and Lung Institute, the second and third formants. It is pos- not found with nonspeech sounds that
Betl~esda,hfaryland 20014 sible to construct a series of stimuli that vary continuously along physical con-
WILLIAME. BUNNEY, JR. vary continuously in the relative onset tinua such as frequency or intensity.
Laboratory of Clinical Science, time of the first formant, and to investi- Typically, listeners are able to discrim-
National Institute of Mental Realtlz, gate listeners' ability to identify and inate many more stimuli than they are
Betkesda, Maryland 20014 discriminate these sound patterns. An able to identify absolutely, and the dis-
22 JANUARY 1971 303
the special processing to which sounds Not all languages studied make use
1 of speech are subjected and thus t o be of the three modal positions. English,
for example, uses only two locations, a
short lag in voicing and a relatively long
lag in voicing. Prevoicing o r long voic-
ing lead, found in Thai, for example,
11 nearly so, it may be thought t o be is omitted. Of interest, however, is the
t 10rnsec
reasonably close to the biological- basis fact that all languages use the middle

- '1
-,,- 7-
>
-O
d

F-3
'" of speech and hence of special interest location, short voicing lag, which, given
to students of language development. certain other necessary articulatory
Though the distinctions made along the events, corresponds to the English
voicing dimension are not phonetically voiced stop / b / , and one o r both of
the same in all languages, it has becn the remaining modal values. The acous-
found in the cross-language research of tic consequences for two modes of pro-

i100
. rnsec
Lisker and Abramson (5) that the duction are shown in Fig. 1; these cor-
usages are not arbitrary, but rather respond to short and long voicing lags,
very much constrained. I n studies of / b / and / p / , respectively.
Fig. 1. Spectrograms of synthetic speech
showing two conditions of voice onset time the production of the voicing distinc- Given the strong evidence for uni-
(VOT): slight voicing lag in the upper tion in 11 diverse languages, these in- versal-and presumably biologically de-
figure and long voicing lag in the lower vestigators found that, with only minor termined-modes of production for the
figure. The symbols F-l* F-2$ and F-3 rep- exceptions, the various tokens fell at voicing distinction, we should suppose
resent the first three formants, that is, the
relatively intense bands of energy in the three values along a single continuum. that there might exist complementary
speech spectrum, [courtesy of L. ~ i The
~ continuum,
k ~ ~called voice onset time processes of perception ( 6 ) . Hence, if
and A. S. Abramson] ( V O T ) , is defined as the time between we are to find evidence marking the
the release burst and the onset of beginnings of speech perception in a
laryngeal pulsing o r voicing. H a d the linguistic mode, it would appear rea-
criminability functions d o not normally location of the phonetic distinctions sonable to initiate our search with in-
show the same high peaks and low been arbitrary, then different languages vestigations of speech sounds differing
troughs found in the case of the voic- might well have divided the V O T con- along the voicing continuum. What was
ing distinction ( 3 ) . The strong and un- tinuum in many different ways, con- done experimentally, in essence, was to
usual tendency for the stop consonants strained only by the necessity to space compare the discriminability of two
to be perceived in a categorical manner the different modal values of V O T suffi- synthetic spcech sounds separated by a
has been assumed to be the result of ciently far apart as to avoid confusion. fixed difference in V O T under two con-
ditions: in the first condition the two
stimuli to be discriminated lay on op-
75 - 20 D
I
- 20 S L
I
-
0
I
I

I
posite sides of the adult phonemic
boundary, whereas in the second con-
I I I
I

I dition the two stimuli were from the


I I
I

t I I
same phonemic category.
I
The experimental methodology was a
60
-
8

ur I
- I
I

- I
I modification of the reinforcement pro-
2
e

I I

I I

I I
cedure developed by Siqueland ( 7 ) .

liL
9
".
l
I I
I After obtaining a baseline rate of high-
I
F
t
I
amplitude, nonnutritive sucking f o r
.Y-
- I - I
- I
I
I
I
I each infant, the presentation and inten-
U I
2l
"
I I
I
sity of an auditory stimulus was made
Y-
g 4 5
O
L

2 30- I
I
-
!\-
I
I
I
I
I
I
contingent
high-amplitude
uponsucking.
the i nfant's
The nipple
rate on
which the child sucked was connected
of

!\
$e

e I
I
0 I
I B to a positive pressure transducer that
e
I I
provided polygraphic recordings of all
w
m
I I I
I responses and a digital record of cri-
= 15 - I
I
-
I

I
I
-
terional high-amplitude sucking re-
I
I
I
I
II sponses. Criterional responses activated
I
I I a power supply that increased the in-
I I
tensity of the auditory feedback. A
:
I I
I I I I I I I I I I - 1 1 1 1 1 , I l l I I I I I I
I
I I I I I

B 5 4 3 2 1 1 2 3 59 3 2 1 1 2 3 8 5 4 3 2 1 1 2 1 4 - sucking rate per set-


ond maintained the stimulus at maxi-
Tirile ( m ~ n )
munl intensity, about 75 db (13 db
Fig. 2. Mean number of sucking responses for the 4-month-old infants, as a function over the background intensity o f
of time and experimental condition. The dashed line indicates the occurrence of the 62 d b ) .
stimulus shift, or in the case of the control group the time at which the shift would
have occurred. The letter B stands for the baseline rate. Time is measured with The presentation of an auditory stim-
reference to the moment of stimulus shift and indicates the 5 minutes prior to and lllus in this manner results in
the 4 minutes after shift. an increase in the rate of sucking com-
304 SCIENCE, VOL. 171
pared with the baseline rate. With con- stimulation was made. The control
tinued presentation of the initial stim- group served to counter any argument
ulus, a decrement in the response rate that the increment in response rate as-
occurs, presumably as a consequence of sociated with a change in stimulation
the lessening of the reinforcing prop- was artifactual in that the infants
erties of the initial stimulus. When it tended to respond in a cyclical manner.
was apparent that attenuation of the Eight infants from each age level were
reinforcing properties of the initial stim- randomly assigned to conditions 20D
ulus had occurred, as indicated by a and 20S, and ten infants from each age
decrement in the conditioned sucking level were assigned to the control con-
rate of at least 20 percent for two con- dition.
secutive minutes compared with the im- Figure 2 shows the minute-by-rnin-
mediately preceding minute, a second ute response rates for the 4-month-old
auditory stimulus was presented with- subjects for each of the training condi-
out interruption and again contingent tions separately. The results for the
upon sucking. The second stimulus was younger infants show very nearly the
maintained for 4 minutes after which identical overall pattern of results seen
the experiment was terminated. Control with the older infants. In all conditions
Fig. 3. The mean change in response rate
subjects were treated in a similar man- as a function of experimental treatments, at both age levels, there were reliable
ner, except that after the initial de- shown separately for the 1- and 4-month- conditioning effects: the response rate
crease in response rate, that is, after old infants. (See text for details.) in the third minute prior to shift was
habituation, no change was made in significantly greater than the baseline
the auditory stimulus. Either an increase rate of responding ( P < .01). As was
in response rate associated with a The main experiment was begun expected from the nature of the pro-
change in stimulation or a decrease of after several preliminary studies estab- cedure, there were also reliable habitu-
smaller magnitude than that shown by lished that both age groups were re- ation effects for all subjects. The mean
the control subjects is taken as infer- sponsive to synthetic speech sounds as response rate for the final 2 minutes
ential evidence that the infants per- measured by la reliable increase in the prior to shift was significantly lower
ceived the two stimuli as different. rate of sucking with the response-con- than the response rate for the third min-
The stimuli were synthetic speech tingent presentation of the first stimulus ute before shift ( P < .01). As is ap-
sounds prepared by means of a paraliel ( P < .01) . Furthermore, these studies parent from inspection of Fig. 1, the
resonance synthesizer at the Haskins showed that stimuli separated by dif- recovery data for the 4-month-old in-
Laboratories by Lisker and Abramson. ferences in VOT of 100, 60, and 20 fants were differentiated by the 'nature
There were three variations of the bi- msec were discriminable when the stim- of the slhift. When the mean response
labial voiced stop / b / and three varia- uli were from different adult phonemic rate during the 2 minutes after shift
tions of its voiceless counterpart / p / . categories; that is, there was reliable was compared with the response rate
The variations between all stimuli were recovery of the rate of sucking with a for the 2 minutes prior to shift, condi-
in VOT, which for the English stops change in stimulation after habituation tion 20D showed a significant incre-
/ b / and / p / can be realized acoustically (P < .05). The finding that a VOT dif- ment ( P < .05), whereas condition 20s
by varying the onset of the first formant ference of 20 msec was discriminable showed a nonsignificant decrement in
relative to the second and third form- permitted within-phonemic-category dis- responding ( P > .05). In the control
ants and by having the second and criminations of VOT with relatively condition, there was a fairly substantial
third formants excited by a noise source realistic variations of both phonemes. decrement in responding during the first
during the interval when the first form- In the main experiment, there were 2 minutes of what corresponded to the
ant is not present. Identification func- three variations in VOT differences at shift period in the experimental condi-
tions from adult listeners ( 8 ) have in- each of two age levels. In the first con- tions. However, the effect failed to
dicated that when the onset of the first dition, 20D, the difference in VOT be- reach the .05 level of significance, but
formant leads or follows the onset of tween the two stimuli to be discrim- there was a reliable decrement when the
the second and third formants by less inated was 20 msec and the two stimuli mean response rate for the entire 4
than 25 msec perception is almost in- were from different adult phonemic minutes after shift was compared with
variably / b / . When voicing follows the categories. The two stimuli used in con- the initial 2 minutes of habituation
release burst by more than 25 msec the dition 20D had VOT values of +20 and ( P < .02). The shift data for the
perception is / p / . Actually the sounds +40 msec. I n the second condition, younger infants were quite similar. The
are perceived as /ba/ or /pa/, since the 20S, the VOT difference was again 20 only appreciable difference was that in
patterns contain three steady-state form- msec, but now the two stimuli were condition 20s #there was a nonsignifi-
ants appropriate for a vowel of the type from the same phonemic category. In cant increment in the response rate
/ a / . The six stimuli had VOT values of this condition the stimuli had VOT during the first 2 minutes of shift.
-20, 0, +20, +40, +60, and +SO values of -20 and 0 msec or $-60 and In Fig. 3 the recovery data are sum-
msec. The negative sign indicates that +80 msec. The third condition, 0, was marized for both age groups. The mean
voicing occurs before the release burst. a control condition in which each sub- change in response rate (that is, the
The subjects were 1- and 4-month-old ject was randomly assigned one of the mean response rate for the initial 2
infants, and within each age level half six stimuli and treated in the same man- minutes of shift minus the mean re-
of the subjects were males and half ner as the experimental subjects, except sponse rate during the final 2 minutes
were females. that after habituation no change in before shift) is displayed as a function
22 JANUARY 1971
of experimental treatments and age. 8. L. Lisker and A. S. Abramson, Proc. Int. facilities of the Haskins Laboratories. We also
Congr. Phone!. Sci. 6th (1970), p. 563. thank Drs. A. M. Liberman, I. G. Mattingly,
Analyses of these data revealed that the 9. ,Supported by grants H D 03386 and H D 04146 A. S. Abramson, and L. Lisker for their critical
magnitude of recovery for the 20D con- from the National Institute of Child Health comments. Portions of this study were p r e
and Human Development. P.J. and J.V. were sented before the Eastern Psychological As-
dition was reliably greater than that for supported by the NSF Undergraduate Partici- sociation, Atlantic City (April 1970).
the 20s condition ( P < .01). In addi- pation Program (GY 5872). We thank Dr. F.
S. Cooper for generously making available the 14 September 1970 a
tion, the 20D condition showed a great-
er rate of responding than did the con-
trol condition ( P < -01), while the dif-
ference between the 20s and control Randomization and the Draft Lottery
conditions failed to attain the .05 level
of significance. Abstract. Fifty "random permutations" were prepared for use by the Selecti~e
In summary, the results strongly indi- Service System as a basis for a two-stage randomization that preceded the lottery
cate that infants as young as 1 month drawing on I July 1970. This report identifies the perm~ttations used. It also
of age are not only responsive to speech gives the orders in which calendar dates and numbers were put into and drawn
sounds and able to make fine discrim- from t w o drums and the correlations between them.
inations but are also perceiving speech
sounds along the voicing continuum in The Selective Service System asked dars" and 25 "random permutations"
a manner approximating categorical the National Bureau of Standards of the numbers 1 to 365 for use in
perception, the manner in which adults (NBS) to prepare 25 "random calen- connection with preparations for the
perceive these same sounds. Another draft lottery conducted on 1 July 1970,
way of stating this effect is that infants which determined the order in which
are able to sort acoustic variations of 'Fable 1. Coefficients of correlation f r ) and men boll3 in 1951 would be called for
adult phonemes into categories with their significance levels (P) for pairs of induction into military service (. I .) .
relatively limited exposure to speech, permutations used in and produced by the
draft lottery procedures. I, Calendar dates
In order that all details of the prep-
as well as with virtually no experience and ranks in the order loaded into capsules; arations be reproducible, the permuta-
in producing these same sounds and permutations 53 and 43, respectively. 11, tions were drawn from published tables
Calendar dates (ranks) in the order of entry
certainly with little, if any, differential into drum. comouted from ~ermutation 51 Ltables and in (2), pp. 152-2291.
reinforcement for this form of behav- (43) and permuiation 46. 111,~~alendar dates Table S contains 38 permutations of
ior. The implication of these findings is (ranks) in the order drawn from the drum; 1 to 500; by omitting
permutation shown in Fig. 2 (Fig. 3 ) .
that the rneans by which the catego- numbers greater than 365. 38 uermu-
u

rical perception of speech, that is, per- Calendar dates Ranks tations of the numbers 1 to 365 were
Pair
ception i n a linguistic mode, is ac- r PY r P" formed. Table 9 contains 20 permuta-
complished may well be part of the tions of the numbers 1 to 1000; by
biological makeup of the organism and, considering numbers from 1 to 365 and
moreover, that these means must be those from 501 to 865, 40 permutations
operative at an unexpectedly early age. * Probability that correlation between two ran- of the numbers 1 to 365 were formed.
dom pertnutations exceeds (in magnitude) the
PETERD. EIMAS observed value. An assortment of tests for randomness

PETERJUSCZYK
JAMES VIGORITO 1. NOVEYRER 3 n 30 rlnVEMNER 11/30 334
Departmerzt o f Psychology, 2. AUG'IST 1 9 IR AIJGUST 8/18 230
Browrz University, 3. APRIL R 8 4PYIL 4/R 98
Providence, Rhode Island 02912 4, J'JhlF 1'1 I Y .liJhiE 6/!q 165
- .-
5 OCTORER 2 2 2 2 OCTOIJER 1@/22 295
References and Notes he JAtliJARY 9 R JANUAHY 118 8
7. DECEMPER 1 2 1 2 OECEMRER 12/ 12 3Y6
1. A. hl. Libcrman. P. C. Delattre, F. S. Cooper, Au sT 27 2 7 PIJSIJST 8/27 239
.
I
Language and Speech 1, 153 (1958); A. M.
Liberrnan, F. Ingernann, L. Lisker, P. C . Delat- 9 EPT E 3ER 2 1 2n s E P T E ! ~ ~ E ? 9/28 263
tre. F. S. Cooper, I. Acorcst. Soc. Amer. 31, I9 , S E p T E M R E II 8 9 SEPTEI.'RE[I 9/9 251
1490 (1959). It should be emphasized that the 31 r)ECEMOFR 12/3 1 365
cues underlying the voicing distinction as dis- 1 '
cussed in the present report apply only to 12 , OC T 0 R E R 1 3 13 !>cTo~~FIR l0/13 2 86
sound segments in absolute initial position. 13. FE~RUARY 2 2 FERRlJARY 2/ 2 33
2. A. M. Liberman, K. S . Harris, H. S. Hoffman,
H. Lane, I . E x p . Psychol. 61, 370 (1961). 14. MAY 15 14 M A Y 5/15 135
3. P. D. Eimas, Language and Speech 6 , 206 15 . O C T rli R E R 2 (1 20 ~cToHFR 1f l / 2 f l 29 3
(1963); G. A. Miller, Psychol. Rev. 63, 81
(1956); R. S . Woodworth and H. Schlosberg,
Erperimenfol Psychology (Holt, New York, 17
'P 1 6
A
OH
9 1.. 1
4 ncToRFR
16 A P R I L
10/4
4/16
277
106
1954). 1 , 3
hll3VFMRER 3 ~IOVEMRER 11/3 3 I7 7
4. A. hf. Liberman, F. S. Cooper, D. P. Shank-
ueiler. 1%. Studdert-Kennedy, Psychol. Rev. 74,
431 (1967); M. Studdert-Kennedy, A. M. Liber-
]
7 11 ,
* ' '' ' 3
A
')
1)
€ ?.
G I)5 T 2
*?n
23
J ~ N E
AUGUST
6/20
R/?3
171
2 35
man, K. S. Harris, F. S. Cooper, ibid. 77,
234 (1970); M. Studdert-Kennedy and D.
Shankweiler, I . Acoust. Soc. Amer., in press. 22r
' MAY 3
0 CTn E R 9
3
9
YAY
oCTOREQ
5/3
1019
123
282
5. L. Lisker and A. S. Abramson, Word 20, 23 , rlA Y 2 2 NAY 5/ 2 122
6.
384 (1964).
P. Lieberman, Li81guislic Inquiry 9, 307 (1970).
' J'.JNE 1 1 JUNE b/ 1 152
7. E. R. Sicjueland, address presented before the Fig. 1. P a r t of a random calendar ("calendar number 53") prepared by NBS, that
29th Inte~national Congress of Psychology,
London, England (August 1969); - and was used f o r loading dates into capsules, showing the redundant f o r m a t used f o r
printing. [Derived f r o m pages 204-205 of Moses and Oakford ( 2 ) l
C. A. DeLucia, Science 165, 1144 (1969).
306 SCIENCE, VOL. 171
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 1 of 1 -

You have printed the following article:


Speech Perception in Infants
Peter D. Eimas; Einar R. Siqueland; Peter Jusczyk; James Vigorito
Science, New Series, Vol. 171, No. 3968. (Jan. 22, 1971), pp. 303-306.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819710122%293%3A171%3A3968%3C303%3ASPII%3E2.0.CO%3B2-V

This article references the following linked citations. If you are trying to access articles from an
off-campus location, you may be required to first logon via your library web site to access JSTOR. Please
visit your library's website or contact a librarian to learn about options for remote access to JSTOR.

References and Notes

7
Visual Reinforcement of Nonnutritive Sucking in Human Infants
Einar R. Siqueland; Clement A. DeLucia
Science, New Series, Vol. 165, No. 3898. (Sep. 12, 1969), pp. 1144-1146.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819690912%293%3A165%3A3898%3C1144%3AVRONSI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-L

NOTE: The reference numbering from the original has been maintained in this citation list.

You might also like