Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage - Environment Fit PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 31

CHAPTER 12

Schools, Academic Motivation,


and Stage–Environment Fit
JACQUELYNNE S. ECCLES AND ROBERT W. ROESER

From the time individuals first enter school Hunt, 1975), as well as more general ideas
until they complete their formal schooling, person–process–context models of human
children and adolescents spend more time in development (e.g., Lerner, 2002; Sameroff,
schools than in any other place outside their 1983), they argued that schools need to change
homes. Exploring all of the possible ways in in developmentally appropriate ways if they
which educational institutions influence moti- are to provide the kind of social context that
vation and development during adolescence will continue to motivate students’ interest
is beyond the scope of a single chapter. In this and engagement as the students mature. To
chapter, we discuss the ways in which schools the extent that this does not happen, they pre-
influence adolescents’ social–emotional and dicted that students would disengage first psy-
behavioral development through organiza- chologically and then physically from school
tional, social, and instructional processes as they matured into and through adolescence.
ranging from those based in the immediate, This should be particularly true as the ado-
proximal relation between students and the lescents acquired more incentives and more
tasks they are asked to perform to the role that power to control their own behavior. We say
principals and the school boards play in setting more about both of these psychological per-
school-level and district-level policies, which spectives on the impact of classroom experi-
in turn influence the social organization of the ences later.
entire school community. In 1999, we (see Eccles & Roeser, 1999)
Understanding the impact of schools on proposed a framework for thinking about
adolescent development requires a conceptual school influences that conceptualized the
framework for thinking simultaneously about school context into a series of hierarchically
schools as contexts in which development takes ordered, interdependent levels of organiza-
place and about the changing developmental tion beginning at the most basic level of the
needs of students as they move through the classroom and then moving up in complex-
school system. In the late 1980s, Eccles ity to the school as an organizational sys-
and Midgley proposed a model of stage– tem embedded in a larger cultural system. In
environment fit to guide research on the impact adopting this heuristic, we assumed that (1)
of school transitions on adolescent development schools are systems characterized by multiple
(see Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Eccles et al., levels of analysis composed of various regula-
1993). They argued that individuals have tory processes (organizational, interpersonal,
changing emotional, cognitive, and social needs and instructional in nature); (2) these pro-
and personal goals as they mature. Drawing cesses are interrelated across levels of analy-
on ideas related to person–environment fit and sis; (3) such processes are usually dynamic in
self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2002; nature, sometimes being worked out each day
404

c12.indd Sec1:404 11/3/08 8:09:11 PM


An Ecological View of Schools and Their Impact on Development During Adolescence 405

between the various social actors (e.g., teach- of schools embedded in larger social systems,
ers and students); (4) these processes change we discussed such issues as school resources,
as children move through different school as well as the linkages of schools with parents
levels (elementary, middle, and high school); and with the labor market.
and (5) these processes regulate children’s We further assumed that in any given school
and adolescents’ cognitive, social–emotional, setting these multilevel processes are highly
and behavioral development. In this chapter, interdependent. Relations between different
we focus on the interface between various levels of organization in the school may be
theoretical frameworks that are consistent with complementary or contradictory and may
these tenets of school influences. We begin influence students either directly or indirectly.
with a summary of our multilevel description For instance, a principal may decide that all
of school contexts. of his or her teachers should use a particular
practice such as cooperative learning or small
AN ECOLOGICAL VIEW OF learning communities. However, the impact
SCHOOLS AND THEIR IMPACT of such a decision on the daily experiences of
ON DEVELOPMENT DURING students depends on how well this practice is
ADOLESCENCE actually implemented at the classroom level.
From the location of the school within macro- If done well, students should be seen working
regulatory systems characterized by national, successfully in groups on complex, authentic
state, and school district laws and educational problems. Such a well-implemented school
policies to the miniregulatory systems that policy is likely to produce gains in self-esteem,
involve the minute-to-minute interactions interethnic relationships, and achievement
between teachers and individual students, among students, especially those of low ability
schools are systems of complex, multilevel, or status (Connell & Klem, 2000; Connell,
regulatory processes. Eccles and Roeser (1999) 2003; Felner, Seitsinger, Brand, Burns, &
described these different levels of the school Bolton, 2007; National Research Council
environment in terms of their hierarchical [NRC], 2004; Slavin, 1990; Wigfield, Eccles,
ordering—moving from the student in a class- Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006). In
room to the school building itself, then to the contrast, if done poorly, classroom disorgani-
school district, and finally to the larger com- zation can result, leading to far less positive
munities in which school districts are located. outcomes at the student level. How such a
Within each of these levels, we discussed schoolwide instructional policy is implemented
those beliefs and practices that affect students’ depends on many factors, including the morale
experiences on a daily basis. At the classroom within the school, the relationships between
level, we focused attention on teacher beliefs the principal and the teachers, the teach-
and instructional practices, teacher–student ers’ understanding and endorsement of the
relationships, the nature and design of tasks new instructional practice, the way in which
and instruction, and the nature and structure the policy change was decided upon, the pro-
of classroom activities and groups. At the vision of adequate in-service training, the
level of the school building, we focused atten- provision of adequate supports for imple-
tion on organizational climate and such school- mentation of new strategies, and the students’
wide practices as academic tracking, school willingness to go along with the new practice.
start time, and the provision of extracurricular Recent debates about the No Child Left Behind
activities. At the level of the school district, we policy provide another example of the com-
focused on the between-school grade configu- plex ways in which a new policy—this time
rations that create particular school-transition a national-level policy—can affect the daily
experiences for students. Finally, at the level experiences of teachers and students in the

c12.indd 405 11/3/08 8:09:12 PM


406 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

classroom and in the school building (Darling- from educational psychologists. In this sec-
Hammond & Bransford, 2005; NRC, 2004). tion, we review some of what we know about
Eccles and Roeser (1999) also assumed teacher beliefs, classroom climate, the nature
that the processes associated with the different of the academic work itself, and experiences of
levels of the school environment interacting racial–ethnic discrimination.
dynamically with each other, rather than static Teacher Beliefs
resources or characteristics of the curriculum,
Teacher beliefs have received much attention
teachers, or school per se, influence adolescents’
in educational psychology. In this section, we
development. In addition, adolescents’ own
focus on two types of beliefs: Teachers’ gen-
constructions of meaning and interpretations of
eral sense of their own teaching efficacy and
events within the school environment are criti-
teachers’ expectations for specific students in
cal mediators between school characteristics
their class.
and students’ feelings, beliefs, and behavior.
Finally, in keeping with the stage–envi- Teachers’ General Sense of Efficacy
ronment perspective proposed by Eccles and When teachers hold high general expectations
Midgely (1989), Eccles and Roeser (1999) for student achievement and students per-
assumed that these different school-related pro- ceive these expectations, students learn more,
cesses change across the course of children’s experience a greater sense of self-worth and
and adolescents’ development as they progress competence as learners, feel more connected
through elementary, middle, and high school. to their teacher and their school, and resist
That is, not only are children and adolescents involvement in problem behaviors (Brophy,
developing, but so too is the whole nature of 2004; Lee & Smith, 2001; NRC, 2004; Roeser,
the schools that they attend. For example, the Eccles, & Sameroff, 1998; Rutter, 1983;
organizational, social, and instructional pro- Weinstein, 1989; Wigfield, Byrnes, & Eccles,
cesses in schools change as children move 2006). Alternatively, when teachers lack con-
from elementary to middle school. Eccles and fidence in their teaching efficacy, they can
Midgley (1989) argued that these changes are engage in behaviors that reinforce feelings of
often associated with declines in many adoles- incompetence and alienation in their students,
cents’ motivation and behavior. Understanding increasing the likelihood that their students
the interaction of different school features will develop learned helpless responses to
with the developmental needs of adolescents failure in the classroom (see Roeser & Eccles,
is critical to understanding the role of school- 2000). As we discuss in more detail later, the
ing in young people’s development. In the next prevalence of teachers with a low sense of
sections, we discuss those characteristics of personal teaching efficacy is higher in junior
each level of the school that are most likely high and middle schools than in elementary
to be important for understanding the impact schools. Low teacher efficacy rates are also
of schools on adolescent development. We higher in schools that serve high proportions
also discuss how school characteristics at each of ethnic minority and poor adolescents than
level may also influence group differences in in schools that serve more affluent and higher
adolescent development, paying particular achieving adolescents (Darling-Hammond,
attention to gender and ethnic group differ- 1997; Juvonen, Le, Kaganoff, Augustine, &
ences within the United States. Constant, 2004; Wigfield et al., 2006).

LEVEL 1: CLASSROOMS Differential Teacher Expectations


The most immediate educational environment Equally important are the differential expecta-
for the student is the classroom. This is also tions teachers often hold for various individuals
the level that has received the most attention within the same classroom and the differential

c12.indd 406 11/3/08 8:09:12 PM


Level 1: Classrooms 407

treatments that sometimes accompany these nature of commonly held stereotypes about the
expectations. Many researchers have shown intellectual strengths and weaknesses of differ-
that undermining teacher-expectancy effects ent social groups.
depend on how teachers structure activities Classroom Climate
differently, as well as interact differently with,
Classroom climate refers to the more general
high- and low-expectancy students and on
character of the classroom and teacher–
how the students perceive these differences
student relationships within the classroom.
(Brophy, 2004; Cooper, 1979; Weinstein, 1989;
In this section, we focus on the following
Wigfield et al., 2006). Much of the work on
aspects of classroom climate: Teacher–student
teacher expectancy effects has focused on the
relationships, classroom management, and
negative effects of differential treatment related
motivational climate.
to gender, race–ethnic group, and/or social
class (see Ferguson, 1998; Jussim, Eccles, & Teacher–Student Relationships
Madon, 1996; Valencia, 1991; Wigfield et al., The quality of teacher–student relationships
2006). Jussim et al. (1996) found that even is a key aspect of the classroom climate.
though these effects are typically quite small, Teachers who trust, care about, and are
young women, African-American adolescents, respectful of students, and who care specifi-
and students from poorer homes are more cally about students’ learning, provide the
subject to both the positive and negative social–emotional and intellectual scaffolding
effects of teacher expectancy effects than are that students need to approach, engage, and
other students. persist on academic learning tasks; to develop
Researchers such as Steele and Aronson positive, achievement-related self-percep-
(1995) have linked this form of differential tions, values, and a sense of school belong-
treatment, particularly for African-American ing; and more generally to experience a sense
students, to school disengagement and of well-being when in school (Deci & Ryan,
disidentification (the separation of one’s 2002; Goodenow, 1993; Midgley et al., 1989b;
self-esteem from school-related feedback). NRC, 2004; Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996;
Steele and Aronson argued that African- Wentzel, 2002; Wigfield et al., 2006). Feeling
American students become aware of the fact emotionally supported is one of the most
that teachers and other adults have negative important characteristics of developmental
stereotypes of African-Americans’ academic contexts like schools for fostering adolescents’
abilities. This awareness (labeled stereotype positive development—people and feelings of
threat by Steele and colleagues; see Aronson & belonging and support really matter. Declines
Steele, 2005; increases their anxieties, which in both adolescents’ perception of emotional
in turn lead them to disidentify with the support from their teachers and in the adoles-
school context to protect their self-esteem. cents’ sense of belonging in their classrooms
It is interesting that recent studies using the are quite common as adolescents move from
same theoretical notions and experimental elementary school into secondary schools
techniques have shown that Asian students (NRC, 2004; Roeser, Peck, & Nasir, 2006;
believe that teachers and adults expect them Wigfield et al., 2006). This shift is particu-
to perform very well and that this belief leads larly troublesome in our highly mobile soci-
Asian students to perform better on tests when ety in which teachers represent one of the last
their ethnic identity is made salient (Shih, stable sources of nonparental role models for
Pittinsky, & Ambady, 1999). Thus, the psycho- adolescents. In addition to teaching, teachers
logical processes associated with stereotype in mobile societies such as the United States
threat can either undermine or facilitate perfor- can provide guidance and assistance when
mance on standardized tests depending on the social–emotional or academic problems arise.

c12.indd 407 11/3/08 8:09:12 PM


408 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

This role is especially important for promoting they pass through the K–12 school years, their
developmental competence when conditions in school motivation should decline as they get
the family and neighborhood cannot or do not older (Eccles et al., 1993).
provide such supports (Eccles, Lord, & Roeser,
1996; NRC, 2004; Simmons & Blyth, 1987). Motivational Climate
Several teams of researchers have suggested
Classroom Management that teachers engage in a wide range of behav-
Work related to classroom management has iors that create a pervasive motivational climate
focused on two general issues: orderliness/ in the classroom. For example, Rosenholtz
predictability and control/autonomy. With and Simpson (1984) suggested a cluster of
regard to orderliness and predictability, the general teaching practices (e.g., individual-
evidence is quite clear: Student achievement and ized versus whole-group instruction, ability
conduct are enhanced when teachers establish grouping practices, and publicness of feed-
smoothly running and efficient procedures back) that should affect motivation because
for monitoring student progress, providing these practices make ability differences in
feedback, enforcing accountability for work classrooms especially salient to students. They
completion, and organizing group activi- assumed that these practices affect the motiva-
ties (e.g., Darling-Hammond & Bransford, tion of all students by increasing the salience
2005; Pintrich & Schunk, 1996; Roeser et al., of extrinsic motivators and ego-focused learn-
2008). Unfortunately, such conditions are ing goals, leading to greater incidence of social
often absent, particularly in highly stressed comparison behaviors and increased percep-
and underfunded schools with inexperienced tion of ability as an entity state rather than an
teachers (Darling-Hammond, 1997; Darling- incremental condition. All of these changes
Hammond & Bransford, 2005; NRC, 2004). reduce the quality of students’ motivation and
Research on autonomy versus control is learning. The magnitude of the negative con-
equally compelling. Many researchers believe sequences of these shifts, however, should
that classroom practices that support student be greatest for low-performing students: As
autonomy are critical for fostering intrinsic these students become more aware of their
motivation to learn and for supporting socio- relative low standing, they are likely to adopt
emotional development during childhood and a variety of ego-protective strategies that
adolescence (Deci & Ryan, 2002; Grolnick, unfortunately undermine learning and mastery
Gurland, Jacob, & Decourcey, 2002). Support (Covington & Dray, 2002; NRC, 2004).
for this hypothesis has been found in both lab- Researchers interested in goal theory have
oratory and field-based studies (Deci & Ryan, proposed a similar set of classroom charac-
2002; Grolnick et al., 2002; NRC, 2004). teristics (Maehr & Midgley, 1996; Midgley,
However, it is also critical that the teacher 2002; NRC, 2004; Pintrich, 2000; Pintrich &
supports student autonomy in a context of Schunk, 2003). Goal theorists propose two
adequate structure and orderliness (Wigfield major achievement goal systems: mastery-
et al., 2006). This issue is complicated by oriented goals and performance-oriented
the fact that the right balance between adult- goals. Students with mastery-oriented goals
guided structure and opportunities for student focus on learning the material and on their own
autonomy changes as the students mature: improvement over time. Students with perfor-
Older students desire more opportunities for mance-oriented goals focus on doing better
autonomy and less adult-controlled structure. than other students in their class. Goal theo-
To the extent that the students do not experi- rists further argue that a mastery orientation
ence these changes in the balance between sustains school engagement and achievement
structure and opportunities for autonomy as better than does a performance orientation

c12.indd 408 11/3/08 8:09:13 PM


Level 1: Classrooms 409

(see Maehr & Midgley, 1996; Midgley, 2002). rather than a theoretical or abstract perspec-
Evidence is quite strong for the first prediction tive, if it is taught using a hands-on approach
and more mixed for the second: The desire to rather than a book-learning approach, and if
do better than others often has positive rather the teacher avoids sexism in its many subtle
than negative consequences, whereas the forms. The reason given for these effects is
fear of failing (performance-avoidance goal the fit between the teaching style; the instruc-
orientation) undermines school performance tional focus; and females’ values, goals, moti-
(see Midgley, 2002). Finally, these theorists vational orientations, and learning styles.
suggest that the publicness of feedback, par- The few relevant studies support this hypoth-
ticularly social comparative feedback, and esis (Eccles, 1994; Hoffmann, 2002; Wigfield
a classroom focus on competition between et al., 2006). If such classroom practices are
students undermine mastery motivation and more prevalent in one subject area (e.g., physi-
increase performance motivation. The school- cal science or math) than another (e.g., biolog-
reform work of Midgley, Maehr, and their ical or social science), one would expect sex
colleagues has shown that school reform efforts differences in motivation to study these subject
to reduce these types of classroom practices, areas. In addition, however, math and physical
particularly those associated with performance science do not have to be taught in these ways;
feedback, social comparative grading systems, more girl-friendly instructional approaches can
and ego-focused, competitive motivational be used. When they are, girls as well as boys
strategies have positive consequences for ado- are more likely to continue taking courses in
lescents’ academic motivation (e.g., Maehr & these fields and to consider working in these
Midgley, 1996). Creating classroom climates fields when they become adults.
that reframe student role identities in terms of The girl-friendly classroom conclusion is a
cooperation, multiple intelligences, effort, and good example of person–environment fit. Many
improvement toward attaining standards rather investigators have suggested that students are
than in terms of competition and relative abil- maximally motivated to learn in situations that
ity has been an important approach of school fit well with their interests, current skill level,
reform movements whose aim is to achieve and psychological needs, so that the material
equity and excellence in learning outcomes is challenging, interesting, and meaningful
(e.g., Darling-Hammond, 1997; Maehr & (e.g., Ainley, Hidi, & Berndorff, 2002; Chen,
Midgley, 1996). Darst, & Pangrazi, 2001; Csikszentmihalyi,
The work on understanding group differ- Rathunde, & Whalen, 1993; Eccles et al.,
ences in achievement and achievement choices 1993; Hidi, 2001; Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000;
is another example of an attempt to identify a Köller, Baumert, & Schnabel, 2001; NRC,
broad set of classroom characteristics related to 2004; Renninger, Ewen, & Lasher, 2002).
motivation. The work on girls and math is one Variations on this theme include aptitude by
example of this approach. There are sex differ- treatment interactions and theories stressing
ences in adolescents’ preference for different cultural match or mismatch as one explanation
types of learning contexts that likely interact for group differences in school achievement
with subject area to produce sex differences and activity choices (e.g., Fordham & Ogbu,
in interest in different subject areas (Eccles, 1986; Ogbu, 1992; Okagaki, 2001; Suarez-
1994; Hoffmann, 2002; Wigfield et al., 2006). Orozco & Suarez-Orozco, 2001; Valencia,
Females appear to respond more positively to 1991). For example, Valencia (1991) concluded
math and science instruction if it is taught in that a mismatch of both the values of the school
a cooperative or individualized manner rather and the materials being taught contributed to
than a competitive manner, if it is taught from the poor performance and high dropout rates
an applied or person-centered perspective among Latino youth in the high school they

c12.indd 409 11/3/08 8:09:13 PM


410 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

studied. Deyhle and LeCompte (1999) made Unfortunately, American secondary schools
a similar argument in their discussion of the have problems providing each of these types of
poor performance of Native American youth educational experiences. Larson and colleagues
in traditional middle school contexts. The mis- have documented the fact that adolescents are
fit between the needs of young adolescents and bored most of the time that they are in sec-
the nature of junior high school environments ondary school classrooms (see Larson, 2000).
is another example of these person–environ- Culturally meaningful learning experiences
ment fit dynamics. are rare in many American secondary schools
(Fine, 1991; Garcia-Coll et al., 1996; Graham
The Nature of Academic Work & Taylor, 2002; Okagaki, 2001; Valencia,
Academic work is at the heart of the school 1991; Wigfield et al., 2006). The disconnec-
experience. Two aspects of academic tasks are tion of traditional curricula from the experi-
important: the content of the curriculum and ences of these groups can explain the alienation
the design of instruction. The nature of aca- of some group members from the educational
demic content has an important impact on stu- process, sometimes eventuating in school
dents’ attention, interest, and cognitive effort. dropout (Fine, 1991; Sheets & Hollins, 1999).
Long ago, Dewey (1902/1990) proposed that Appropriately designed tasks that adequately
academic work that is meaningful to the his- scaffold learning are also rare in many inner-
torical and developmental reality of students’ city and poor schools (Darling-Hammond, 1997).
experiences will promote sustained attention, In addition, from a developmental perspective,
high investment of cognitive and affective there is evidence that the nature of academic
resources in learning, and strong identification work too often does not change over time in
with educational goals and aims. In general, ways that are concurrent with the increasing
research supports this hypothesis: Content cognitive sophistication, diverse life experi-
that provides meaningful exploration is criti- ences, and identity needs of adolescents as they
cal given that boredom in school, low interest, move from the elementary into the secondary
and perceived irrelevance of the curriculum school years (Carnegie Council on Adolescent
are associated with poor attention, diminished Development, 1989; Juvonen et al., 2004; Lee &
achievement, disengagement, and alienation Smith, 1993, 2001).
from school (e.g., Finn, 1989, 2006; Jackson & For example, middle school students
Davis, 2000; NRC, 2004). Curricula that repre- report the highest rates of boredom when
sent the voices, images, and historical experi- doing schoolwork, especially passive work
ences of traditionally underrepresented groups (e.g., listening to lectures) and in particular
are also important (Valencia, 1991). Choosing classes such as social studies, math, and sci-
materials that provide an appropriate level of ence (Larson & Richards, 1989). There is also
challenge for a given class, designing learn- evidence that the content of the curriculum
ing activities that require diverse cognitive taught in schools does not broaden to incor-
operations (e.g., opinion, following routines, porate either important health or social issues
memory, comprehension), structuring lessons that become increasingly salient as adolescents
so that they build on each other in a system- move through puberty and deal with the iden-
atic fashion, using multiple representations of tity explorations associated with adolescence
a given problem, and explicitly teaching stu- (Carnegie Council, 1989; Juvonen et al., 2004)
dents strategies that assist in learning are but a It may be that declines in some adolescents’
few of the design features that scaffold learn- motivation during the transition to secondary
ing and promote effort investment, interest in school in part reflect academic work that lacks
learning, and achievement (Blumenfeld, 1992; challenge and meaning commensurate with
Deci & Ryan, 2002; Wigfield et al., 2006). adolescents’ cognitive and emotional needs

c12.indd 410 11/3/08 8:09:13 PM


Level 1: Summary 411

(Eccles & Midgley, 1989). Recent efforts at and anger. Interestingly, evidence is beginning
middle school reform support this hypothesis: to show that a strong positive ethnic identity has
Motivation is maintained when middle schools protective effects against the aversive effects of
and junior high schools introduce more chal- daily experiences of racial and ethnic discrimi-
lenging and meaningful academic work nation (Chavous et al., 2003; Harris-Britt et al.,
(Jackson & Davis, 2000). We discuss this in 2007; Sellers et al., 2003; Wong et al., 2003).
more detail later. Thus, educating for diversity and redress-
ing discrimination are among two of goals
Experiences of Racial–Ethnic secondary school educators can pursue in
Discrimination efforts to reduce achievement gaps. If young
Researchers interested in the relatively poor people from immigrant and ethnic minor-
academic performance of adolescents from ity backgrounds are afforded environments
some ethnic-racial groups have suggested that offer them social support, the develop-
another classroom-based experience as critical ment of life skills and transfer of cultural
for adolescent development, namely, experi- capital, and strategies for addressing the
ences of racial–ethnic discrimination (Brody twin challenges of racism and poverty, then
et al., 2006; Essed, 1990; Fordham & Ogbu, such challenges can become sources of motiva-
1986; Garcia Coll et al., 1996; Graham & tion and engagement that eventuate in the pur-
Taylor, 2002; Harris-Britt, Valrie, Kurtz- suit of a college education (Darling-Hammond,
Costes, & Rowley, 2007; Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997). Providing access to equal educational
1995; Sellers, Caldwell, Schmeelk-Cone, & opportunities also requires attention to the
Zimmerman, 2003; Taylor, Casten, Flickinger, language in which instruction is provided. For
Roberts, & Fulmore, 1994; Wong, Eccles, & many immigrant children, schools do not pro-
Sameroff, 2003). Two types of discrimination vide adequate linguistic supports to allow the
have been discussed: (1) anticipation of future children to master the material being taught in
discrimination in the labor market, which might English (Padilla & Gonzalez, 2001).
be seen as undermining the long-term benefits
of education (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986); and (2) LEVEL 1: SUMMARY
the impact of daily experiences of discrimina- The studies of classroom-level influences
tion on one’s mental health and academic moti- suggest that development is optimized when
vation (Essed, 1990; Sellers et al., 2003; Wong students are provided with challenging tasks in
et al., 2003). Both types have been shown to a mastery-oriented environment that also pro-
adversely affect the development of ethnic vides good emotional and cognitive support,
minority adolescents. For example, Wong et al. meaningful and culturally diverse material to
(2003) found that anticipated future discrimi- learn and master, and sufficient support for
nation leads to increases in African-American their own autonomy and initiative. Connell
youths’ motivation to do well in school, which and Wellborn (1991), as well as Deci and Ryan
in turn leads to increases in academic perfor- (2002), suggested that humans have three
mance. In this sample, anticipated future dis- basic needs: to feel competent, to feel socially
crimination appeared to motivate the youth to attached, and to have autonomous control in
do their very best so that they would be maxi- their lives. Further, they hypothesized that indi-
mally equipped to deal with future discrimina- viduals develop best in contexts that provide
tion. In contrast, daily experiences of racial opportunities for each of these needs to be met.
discrimination from their peers and teach- Clearly, the types of classroom characteristics
ers led to declines in school engagement and that emerge as important for both intellectual,
confidence in one’s academic competence motivational and socioemotional development
and grades, along with increases in depression would provide such opportunities.

c12.indd 411 11/3/08 8:09:14 PM


412 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

LEVEL 2: SCHOOL BUILDINGS achievement goals, so too do schools through


Schools are formal organizations and, as such, particular policies and practices. A school-level
have characteristics and features that are super- emphasis on different achievement goals cre-
ordinate to classroom characteristics. These ates a schoolwide psychological environment
aspects of the whole school environment that affects students’ academic beliefs, affects,
should impact on adolescents’ intellectual, and behaviors (e.g., Maehr & Midgley, 1996;
social-emotional, and behavioral development. Roeser et al., 1996). For example, schools’ use
Important school-level organizational features of public honor rolls and assemblies for the
include school climate and sense of commu- highest achieving students, class rankings on
nity (Connell, 2003; Connell & Klem, 2000; report cards, differential curricular offerings
NRC, 2004; Rutter & Maughan, 2002) and the for students of various ability levels, and so
relationships among the students themselves. on all emphasize relative ability, competition,
School organizational features also include and social comparison in the school and cre-
such schoolwide practices as curricular track- ate a school-level ability rather than mastery/
ing, start and stop times, and the availability of task focus. However, through the recognition
extracurricular activities. of academic effort and improvement, rewards
for different competencies that extend to all
General School Climate students, and through practices that emphasize
Researchers have become interested in the gen- learning and task mastery (block scheduling,
eral school climate or culture of the entire school. interdisciplinary curricular teams, cooperative
These researchers suggest that schools vary in learning), schools can promote a school-level
the climate and general expectations regarding focus on discovery, effort and improvement,
student potential, and that such variations affect and academic mastery.
the development of both teachers and students In studies of adolescents, Roeser et al. (1996)
in very fundamental ways (e.g., Bandura, found that students’ perceptions of the school
2006; Bryk, Lee, & Holland, 1993; Darling- mastery goal structure predicted their own per-
Hammond & Bransford, 2005; Jackson & sonal mastery goals, which in turn were posi-
Davis, 2000; NRC, 2004). For example, tively predictive of their academic self-efficacy
in their analysis of higher achievement in and positive affect in school. Students’ percep-
Catholic schools, Bryk et al. (1993) discussed tions of the school performance goal structure
how the culture within Catholic schools is fun- were positively associated with their personal
damentally different from the culture within performance goal orientations, which in turn
most public schools in ways that positively predicted their feelings of self-consciousness
affect the motivation of students, parents, and in school. What were interesting about this
teachers. This culture (school climate) values study were the correlations between indi-
academics, has high expectations that all stu- cators of the social climate (i.e., respectful
dents can learn, and affirms the belief that the and caring relationships between teachers and
business of school is learning. Similarly, Lee students) and the academic climate. Students
and Smith (2001) showed that between-school reporting a strong performance-goal structure
differences in teachers’ sense of their own in their school where much less likely to report
personal efficacy as well as their confidence that their teachers cared for them, whereas
in the general ability of the teachers at their those perceiving a task goal structure in the
school to teach all students accounted, in part, school were more likely to see their teachers
for between-school differences in adolescents’ as caring.
high school motivation and performance. Roeser, Eccles & Sameroff (1998) examined
Maehr, Midgley, and colleagues argued that the relation of perceived school goal structures
just as classroom practices give rise to certain to longitudinal change in adolescent students’

c12.indd 412 11/3/08 8:09:14 PM


Level 2: School Buildings 413

motivation to learn and well-being after con- goal structures, is associated with aspects
trolling for adolescents’ sex, race, parental of adolescents’ academic motivation, well-
education level, parental occupational prestige, being, achievement, and school conduct.
and income. Adolescent students’ perceptions They also suggest the importance of con-
of their school as performance oriented were sidering how certain academic cultures in
related to diminished feelings of academic schools may collude with perceptions of racial
competence and valuing of school, increased discrimination, and may undermine students’
feelings of emotional distress, and decreased perceptions of whether the school is a moral
grades over time, whereas perceived school place and whether or not teachers actually care
task goal structures were associated with for students (see Roeser et al., 2008).
increased valuing of school and diminished
emotional distress over time (Roeser et al., Academic Tracks and Curricular
1998). Using the same sample with person- Differentiation
centered techniques, they found that youth who In the middle and high school years, between-
were most engaged in school reported a clus- class tracking becomes both more widespread
ter of positive school perceptions, including a and more broadly linked to the sequencing of
mastery-oriented school climate and positive specific courses for students bound for different
teacher–student relationships. In contrast, those post secondary school trajectories (college
who were most disengaged reported more of preparation, general education, and vocational
an ability-oriented school (Roeser, Eccles, & education). As curriculum differentiation
Sameroff, 2000). In this study and others, ado- practices intensify in public schools during
lescents’ perceptions of a school ability goal secondary school, students of different ability
structure is found to be highly positively corre- levels get exposed to (often very) different
lated (r around 0.60) with perceptions of racial kinds of academic work, classmates, teachers,
discrimination in school among African- and and teaching methods (Eccles & Roeser, 1999;
Latin-American youth (Roeser & Peck, 2003; Oakes, 2005).
Roeser, 2004). A general consensus on the overall effects
Similarly, Kaplan and Maehr (1999) of curriculum differentiation as an educa-
reported that perceptions of a mastery goal tional practice remains elusive (Eccles &
structure at the school level were associated Roeser, 1999). Research suggests that stu-
with greater sense of well-being and less dents who are placed in high tracks evi-
misconduct than when students perceived an dence some educational benefits; whereas
emphasis on performance goals in the school. low tracks placements are associated with
Fiqueira-McDonough (1986) reported related negative achievement outcomes (see Fuligni,
findings in a study of two high schools that were Eccles, & Barber, 1995; Kao & Thompson,
similar in intake characteristics and achieve- 2003; Oakes, Gamoran, & Page, 1992). As
ment outcomes but differed in their academic just one example, Hallinan and Kubitschek
orientation and rates of delinquent behavior. (1999) found that assignment to high track
The high school characterized by a greater classes accelerated growth in school achieve-
emphasis on competition and high grades ment, whereas assignment to a lower level
(ability orientation) had higher delinquency or vocational track decelerated such growth.
rates, and the students’ grades were a major Studies have also demonstrated that lower
correlate of students’ involvement in delin- track students report being labeled “dumb”
quent behavior (low grades predicted increased by teachers and peers, feel less committed to
delinquent behavior). school, and feel less successful academically
Overall, these studies suggest that the (see Oakes et al., 1992). In our own work,
general school climate, especially its academic we have found that youth who were in lower

c12.indd 413 11/3/08 8:09:14 PM


414 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

track math, English or ESL (English as a adolescence, when course placement is linked
second language) courses saw themselves directly to the kinds of educational options that
as less scholastically competent, perceived are available to the student after high school.
school as less valuable, and felt less of a Poor children, among whom African-American,
sense of school belonging than students in Latino, and Native American children are
higher track math and English (Roeser, 2005; overrepresented, are more likely than their
Roeser et al., 1998). One factor that appears to wealthier and European- or Asian-American
explain some of these differential effects con- peers to be placed in low-ability classrooms
cerns teacher quality—those students who are and in vocational track courses during second-
placed in lower tracks during secondary school ary school (Oakes, 2005). Even in integrated
are often exposed to teachers with less qualifi- schools, minority students tend to receive poor
cations, experience less constructivist teaching access to teaching resources through tracking
practices, and are exposed to what amounts practices (Oakes et al., 1992; Noguera & Wing,
to watered-down curricula (e.g., Darling- 2006). Furthermore, there is some evidence
Hammond, 1997; Oakes, 2005). that students with limited English proficiency
In addition, ability grouping has an impact who are otherwise capable are placed in lower
on students’ peer groups: Between-classroom track classes (see Kao & Thompson, 2003).
ability grouping and curricular tracking increase Finally, careful assessment of these types of
the extent of contact among adolescents with track placements has shown that many of these
similar levels of achievement and engagement youth are incorrectly assigned to these classes
with school. For those doing poorly in school, and tracks (Dornbusch, 1994; Oakes, 2005).
tracking is likely to facilitate friendships Such misassignment has long-term conse-
among students who are similarly alienated quences for students’ ability to go to college
from school and are more likely to engage in once they complete secondary school.
risky or delinquent behaviors (Dryfoos, 1990). Finally, concerns have also been raised about
Dishion, McCord, and Poulin (1999) showed the marginalization and segregation of ESL
experimentally how such collecting of alien- students on middle and high school campuses
ated adolescents increases their involvement (Olsen, 1997; Valdez, 2001). ESL programs
in problem behaviors. This collecting of ado- are often housed on the periphery of regular
lescents with poor achievement or adjustment school campuses and often fail to provide real
histories also places additional discipline bur- opportunities for them to interact with native
dens on the teachers who teach these classes English speakers. Furthermore, similar to
(Oakes, 2005), making such classes unpopular the misassignment of African-American and
with the teachers as well as the students and Latino students to and lack of mobility out of
decreasing the likelihood that the teachers with low academic tracks, there is some evidence
the most experience will allow themselves to that ESL students often get reassigned to ESL
be assigned to these classes. programs following school transition events
Given this accumulating evidence on even though they may have graduated from
the potential costs of tracking, educational such programs into mainstream classes in their
scientists are now questioning the advisability previous schools (Valdez, 2001).
of between class tracking. Concerns have also
been raised about the ways in which students School Size
get placed in different classes and how difficult In 1964, Barker and Gump proposed that
it is for students to change tracks once initial smaller schools afford young people greater
placements have been made. These issues are opportunities for close relationships, make it
important both early in a child’s school career easier for students to be monitored by adults,
(e.g., Entwisle & Alexander, 1993) and later in and have a favorable roles-to-people ratio with

c12.indd 414 11/3/08 8:09:15 PM


Level 2: School Buildings 415

respect to school extracurricular activities that class and racial composition of the school.
allows for widespread student participation Unfortunately, minority and poor adolescents
in the life of the school. All of these factors are most likely to be concentrated in the most
enabled higher achievement, the theory went, overcrowded and largest secondary schools
by providing bonds between the student and (United Way, 2008).
the school (Barker & Gump, 1964). In recent
studies, support for the positive influence of Extracurricular and Out-of-School
small school size has grown. For example, Activities
Lee and Loeb (2000) found that elementary There is growing interest in the role of extra-
school size in an urban Chicago sample of curricular activities in adolescent development
264 (K–8) schools, 5000 teachers, and 23,000 (see chapter 7, vol. 2 of this Handbook). Some
students was correlated with both teacher people are interested because these activi-
beliefs and students’ achievement gains. In ties can fill time and thus decrease the time
the smaller schools (size < 400 students), available for adolescents to get in trouble. For
teachers took greater responsibility for foster- example, in communities where few structured
ing students’ learning and students showed opportunities for after-school activities exist
greater 1-year gains in their mathematics test (especially poor urban communities), adoles-
scores. Lee and Smith (1995) found a negative cents are most likely to be involved in high-
relation between school size and students’ risk behaviors such as substance use, crime,
self-reported school engagement (e.g., positive violence, and sexual activity during the period
attitudes towards classes, investing effort in between 2 and 8 PM. Providing structured activ-
school, feeling challenged) in the National ities either at school or within community orga-
Educational Longitudinal Study: 88 datasets nizations after school when many adolescents
of approximately 12,000 students in 830 high have no adults at home to supervise them is
schools. Similarly, Elder and Conger (2000) an important consideration in preventing ado-
reported that school size was associated with lescents from engaging in high-risk behaviors
adolescent developmental outcomes among (Carnegie Council, 1989; Eccles & Gootman,
high school students in rural Iowa during the 2001; Mahoney, Harris, & Eccles, 2006).
1990s. Across a variety of measures of aca- Others are interested in the potential benefits
demic and social functioning (e.g., grades, of such activities for adolescent development
problem behavior), results showed that ado- (Carnegie Corporation of New York, 1992;
lescents attending smaller schools, on average, Eccles & Gootman, 2001; Eccles & Templeton,
did better than the adolescents attending larger 2002; Mahoney et al., 2006; Mahoney,
schools after sociodemographic factors were Larson, & Eccles, 2005). There is a positive
controlled. link between adolescents’ extracurricular
In summarizing this work, Lee and Smith activities and both educational outcomes (e.g.,
(1997) proposed that the most effective K–8 high school completion, adult educational
elementary schools with respect to student attainment, occupation, and income) and posi-
achievement gains are those that enroll 400 tive youth development (better mental health
students or less, whereas the ideal 9–12 sec- and lower rates of involvement in delinquent
ondary school in this regard enrolls between activities), even after controlling for social
600 and 900 students. Students in elemen- class and ability (Barber, Eccles, & Stone,
tary/middle schools that are larger than 400, 2001; Eccles & Barber, 1999; Eccles, Barber,
and those in high schools smaller than 600 Stone, & Hunt, 2003; Mahoney & Cairns,
or larger than 2,100, learn less in reading and 1997; McNeal, 1995; Peck, Roeser, Zarrett &
mathematics. These findings regarding optimal Eccles, 2007). Participation in sports, in par-
size were consistent regardless of the social ticular, has been linked to lower likelihood of

c12.indd 415 11/3/08 8:09:15 PM


416 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

school dropout, higher rates of college atten- supportive adults and good mentoring, which,
dance, greater educational attainment by age in turn can lead to superior career guidance and
25, and higher occupational attainment at least encouragement; participation can increases
through the 20s, especially among low-achiev- one’s social networks and social capital; and
ing and blue-collar male athletes (Barber et finally participation can increase both soft
al., 2001; Eccles & Barber, 1999; Eccles & skills and other skills needed for success in
Templeton, 2002; McNeal, 1995). school and the transition to adulthood.
Participation in school-based extracurricular Investigators have been especially interested
activities has also been linked to increases on in the links among peer group formation,
such positive developmental outcomes as high identity formation, and activity involvement
school GPA, strong school engagement, and (Eccles & Barber, 1999). For example, Eckert
high educational aspirations (Eccles & Barber, (1989) explored the link between the peer
1999; Lamborn, Brown, Mounts, & Steinberg, group identity formation and both in- and
1992). Roeser and Peck (2003) found that out-of-school activity involvement. As one
among adolescents highly vulnerable to school moves into and through adolescence, individu-
disengagement, after-school activity involve- als become identified with particular groups
ment was associated with a twofold increase in of friends or crowds (see also Brown, 1990).
college attendance rates. In a follow-up study, Being a member of one of these crowds helps
specific kinds of activity involvements were structure both what one does with one’s time
found to underlie this association – those asso- and the kinds of values and norms to which
ciated with extracurricular activities at school, one is exposed. Over time, the coalescence of
with religious activity, and with volunteering one’s personal identity, one’s peer group, and
were particularly important for educational the kinds of activities one participates in as
resilience (Peck et al., 2008). Similarly, partici- a consequence of both one’s identity and one’s
pation in high school extracurricular activities peer group can shape the nature of one’s devel-
and out-of-school volunteer activities predicts opmental pathway into adulthood.
high levels of adult participation in the political This strong link between activity participa-
process and other types of volunteer activities, tion and peer group membership also provides
continued sport engagement, and better physical an explanation for the negative influences
and mental health (Youniss, McLellan, & Yates, of sports participation on drug and alcohol
1997; Youniss, Yates, & Su, 1997). In contrast use. Knowing what an adolescent is doing
to these positive associations, sports has also often tells us a lot about who the adolescent
been linked to increased rates of school devi- is with: It is very likely that participation in
ance and drug and alcohol use (e.g., Eccles & organized activity settings directly affects
Barber, 1999; Lamborn et al., 1992). adolescents’ peer groups precisely because
These results suggest that participation such participation structures a substantial
in organized extracurricular activities can amount of peer group interaction. One’s copar-
have both positive and negative effects. Why? ticipants become one’s peer crowd. And such
Summarizing research from several disciplines, peer crowds often develop an activity-based
Eccles and Templeton (2002) suggested the culture, providing adolescents with the oppor-
following possible mediating mechanisms: tunity to identify with a group having a shared
participation increases the association with sense of style and commitment. Involvement
academically oriented peers and exposure in a school organization or sports links an
to academic and prosocial values; participa- adolescent to a set of similar peers, provides
tion can lead to enhanced self-esteem and shared experiences and goals, and can rein-
generalization of a high sense of personal effi- force friendships between peers (see Mahoney
cacy; participation can increase exposure to et al., 2005). In turn, these experiences should

c12.indd 416 11/3/08 8:09:16 PM


Level 2: School Buildings 417

influence identity formation as well as other the day where violence is most likely to occur.
aspects of adolescent development. For example, in a recent study of students in five
What is important from a school-building high school settings in southeastern Michigan,
perspective is that schools differ in the extent Astor and colleagues (1999) found that most
to which they provide positive extracurricular violent events reported by students occurred in
activities for their students. Researchers who what the authors called the “undefined public
study the advantages of small schools often spaces” of the school—spaces such as parking
point to the fact that more students get to par- lots, bathrooms, particular hallways, and so on,
ticipate in extracurricular activities in small where no adults assumed supervisory jurisdic-
schools because there are fewer bodies to fill tion. These spaces were undefined in terms of
all of the available slots (Barker & Gump, adult monitoring of behavior in them, and thus
1964; Elder & Conger, 2000). Large schools were the frequent sites for fights, unwanted
have an overabundance of students to fill all sexual attention, and so forth.
of the available activity slots. The situation is Fagan and Wilkinson (1998) reviewed
even worse in poor, large secondary schools theory and evidence that suggest several dif-
that have had to cut extracurricular activities to ferent functional goals that violence can serve
stay within their budgets. Recently, federal and for youth, including the securing of high status
state initiatives have emerged to help increase among peers, acquisition of material goods,
the availability of after-school programs that dominance of others and retribution for insults
are housed in school buildings. Unfortunately, to the self, defiance of authority, and a form
most of this money is going to elementary of “rough justice” in situations in which there
school and middle school programs rather than is little legitimate adult authority. All of these
high schools (Eccles & Gootman, 2001). goals likely reflect responses to the frustra-
tion or anticipated frustration of basic needs
Unsupervised Spaces for autonomy and security in social situations
Another important physical dimension of characterized by a lack of adult supervision
school buildings to consider is the nonin- and an absence of opportunities for wholesome
structional space that adolescents move in learning, work, and recreation. In sum, under-
and through before school, after school, and standing how undefined school spaces affect
between classes. These spaces include the park- the motivation and well-being of students who
ing lots and the school grounds, the hallways are potential victims, as well as how particular
and the bathrooms, the sports fields (if there school spaces offer disenfranchised victimiz-
are any), and the cafeteria(s). One example of ers a venue to express themselves in violent
the importance of considering noninstructional ways, can enhance our overall understanding
aspects of the school in studies of school- of lives in school contexts.
ing and motivation comes from the work of
Astor and colleagues (19981999). Astor ’s School Start and Stop Times
(1998) interest is in students’ experiences of School start time is another tangible school-
school violence and their related feelings of anx- level characteristic that can influence stu-
iety or safety while in school. Clearly, concerns dents’ motivation, learning, and development.
about physical safety can undermine readiness Research conducted by Carskadon (1990,
and motivation to learn. These authors have 1997) has shown that as children progress
found that even though students may respond through puberty, they need more sleep and
affirmatively to a series of questions about how their natural sleep cycles shift to a desire to
safe they feel in school in general, they still go to sleep later in the evening and to wake
can show strong fears in particular areas of the up later in the morning. Unfortunately, sec-
school or school grounds at particular times of ondary schools typically begin earlier in the

c12.indd 417 11/3/08 8:09:16 PM


418 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

morning than primary schools, necessitating school climate, school size, curricular tracking
earlier rise times for adolescents (Carskadon, practices, the availability of extracurricular
1997). In concert with other changes, such activities, and the use of noninstructional
as the later hours at which adolescents go to spaces. There is very strong evidence that each
bed, the earlier school start times of middle of these schoolwide characteristics impacts
and high school create a “developmental mis- adolescent development. Often, between-
match” that can both promote daytime sleepi- school variations on these characteristics
ness and undermine adolescents’ ability to result from school district policies or financial
make it to school on time, alert, and ready to constraints that are beyond the control of the
learn. A study of 5th grade students in Israel, building’s principal and staff. Reform efforts,
for example, compared of two groups: those in however, have shown that changes can be cre-
a school that started at 7:10 AM (early risers) ated in each of these domains and that such
and those in a school that started at 8:00 AM changes can have a positive impact on the
(regular risers). Results showed that early ris- development of the adolescents attending
ers slept less, reported more daytime fatigue the reformed school.
and sleepiness, and reported greater atten-
tion and concentration difficulties in school LEVEL 3: SCHOOL DISTRICTS
compared to their later rising counterparts AND SECONDARY SCHOOL
(Epstein, Chillag, & Lavie, 1998). The impli- TRANSITIONS
cation is that the time that schools begin can School transitions are an excellent example
have a profound effect on mood, energy, atten- of how the multiple levels of schools interact
tion, and, therefore, motivation and learning. to affect adolescent development. All school
The time at which school ends also has districts must decide how they will group
implications for students’ motivation to learn the grade levels within the various school
and development. In communities where few buildings. One common arrangement is to
structured opportunities for after-school activi- group children in kindergarten through 6th
ties exist, especially impoverished communities, grade in elementary schools, young adoles-
young people are more likely to be involved in cents in grades 7–9 in junior high schools, and
high-risk behaviors such as substance use, crime, older adolescents in grades 10–12 in senior
violence, and sexual activity, and less likely high schools. Another common arrangement
to be engaged in productive or academically places the transitions after grades 5 and 8,
relevant activities during the period between creating elementary schools, middle schools,
2 and 8 PM. Providing structured activities and senior high schools. The third popular
either at school or within community organi- arrangement groups young people in grades
zations after school when many young people K–8 in one school and then grades 9–12 in
have no adults at home to supervise them is an a high school. In each of these arrangements,
important consideration in preventing students the students typically move to a new and often
from engaging in high risk behaviors (Eccles & larger building at each of the major transition
Gootman, 2000) and for keeping education- points. These moves typically also involve
ally vulnerable students on track academically increased bussing and exposure to a much
(Peck, Roeser, Zarrett, & Eccles, 2008). more diverse student body. In this section, we
discuss two of these transitions: the transi-
SUMMARY OF SCHOOL-LEVEL tion from elementary to middle or junior high
EFFECTS school and the transition from middle or junior
In this section, we reviewed the impact of high school to high school. Because most of
several features of the whole school on ado- the empirical work has focused on the junior
lescent development. These features included high–middle school transition, we emphasize

c12.indd 418 11/3/08 8:09:16 PM


Level 3: School Districts and Secondary School Transitions 419

this transition. Recent research, however, sug- or social-class differences in these declines,
gests quite similar developmentally inappropri- academic failure and dropout are especially
ate changes with the transition to high school. problematic among some ethnic groups and
among youth from communities and families
The Middle-Grades School Transition of low socioeconomic status. It is probable then
There is substantial evidence of declines in aca- that these groups are particularly likely to show
demic motivation and achievement across the these declines in academic motivation and self-
early-adolescence and high school years (Dweck, perception as they move into and through the
2002; Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Eccles et al., secondary school years.
1993; Finn, 2006; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Several explanations have been offered for
Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, these seemingly negative changes in academic
2002; Roeser, Eccles, & Freedman-Doan, motivation: Some point to the intrapsychic
1999). These declines often coincide with the upheaval associated with young adolescent
transition into either middle/junior high or high development (see Arnett, 1999). Others point
school. For example, there is a marked decline to the simultaneous occurrence of several life
in some early adolescents’ school grades as changes. For example, Simmons and Blyth
they move into junior high school (Simmons & (1987) attributed these declines, particularly
Blyth, 1987). Similar declines occur for such among females, to the coincidence of the
motivational constructs as interest in school junior high school transition with pubertal
(Wigfield et al., 2006), intrinsic motivation development. Still others point to the nature of
(Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001; Harter, the junior high school environment itself rather
1998; Harter, Whitesell, & Kowalski, 1992), than the transition per se.
self-concepts/self-perceptions and confidence Extending person–environment fit theory
in one’s intellectual abilities (Wigfield, Eccles, (see Hunt, 1975) into a developmental per-
MacIver, Reuman, & Midgley, 1991), mastery spective (stage–environment fit theory), Eccles
goal orientation (Anderman & Midgley, 1997), and Midgley (1989) proposed that these nega-
and a sense of belonging at school (Anderman, tive developmental changes result from the
1999). There are also increases in test anxiety fact that traditional junior high schools do not
(Wigfield et al., 2006), focus on self-evaluation provide developmentally appropriate educa-
and performance rather than task mastery tional environments for young adolescents.
(Anderman & Midgley, 1997), and both The authors suggested that different types of
truancy and school dropout (Rumberger, 1995; educational environments are needed for dif-
Rumberger & Thomas, 2000). Furthermore, ferent age groups to meet developmental needs
increasing evidence indicates that these and foster continued developmental growth.
declines predict subsequent school dropout and Exposure to the developmentally appropriate
high school failure (Connell, Halpern-Felsher, environment would facilitate both motivation
Clifford, Crichlow, & Usinger, 1995; Connell, and continued growth; in contrast, exposure to
Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Finn, 2006; Roeser & developmentally inappropriate environments,
Eccles, 1998; Roeser, Eccles, & Strobel, 1998). especially developmentally regressive envi-
Although these changes are not extreme for ronments, should create a particularly poor
most adolescents, there is sufficient evidence person–environment fit, which should lead to
of declines in various indicators of academic declines in motivation as well as detachment
motivation, behavior, and self-perception over from the goals of the institution. What is criti-
the early adolescent years to make one wonder cal to this argument is that the transition itself
what is happening (see Eccles & Midgley, is not the cause of the declines; instead, it is
1989; Ryan & Patrick, 2001). Further, although the nature of the school into which the students
few studies have gathered information on ethnic move. Within this framework, the right kinds

c12.indd 419 11/3/08 8:09:17 PM


420 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

of middle school reforms can be quite effective also shown that changing these aspects of the
at reducing these declines. middle school environment can be effective in
Two approaches have been used to study reducing the declines in school engagement
the middle school transition: one focused on often associated with this school transition
more global school-level characteristics such (Anderman, Maehr, & Midgley, 1999; Maehr &
as school size, degree of departmentalization, Midgley, 1996).
and extent of bureaucratization and the other
on more specific classroom and motivational Grade-Related Differences in
dynamics. The first type is best exemplified by Teacher Beliefs
the work of Simmons and Blyth (1987). They
Differences in all types of teacher beliefs have
pointed out that most junior high schools are
been shown in studies comparing elementary
substantially larger than elementary schools
and middle grades teachers. For example,
and that instruction is more likely to be orga-
junior high school teachers on average have
nized departmentally. As a result, junior high
lower confidence in their own teaching efficacy
school teachers typically teach several different
than do elementary school teachers (i.e., their
groups of students, making it very difficult for
ability to teach and influence all of the students
students to form a close relationship with any
in their classes; Feldlaufer, Midgley, & Eccles,
school-affiliated adult precisely at the point
1988; Midgley & Feldlaufer, 1987; Midgley,
in development when there is a great need for
Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1989a). An equally trou-
guidance and support from nonfamilial adults.
bling difference occurs for teachers’ views of
Such changes in student-teacher relationships
their roles in their students’ lives. For example,
are also likely to undermine the sense of com-
Roeser and colleagues found that with increas-
munity and trust between students and teach-
ing grade level, middle school (6th–8th grades)
ers, leading to a lowered sense of efficacy
teachers are less likely to endorse the notion
among the teachers, an increased reliance
that students’ mental health concerns are part
on authoritarian control practices by the teach-
of the teacher role (Roeser & Midgley, 1997;
ers, and an increased sense of alienation among
Roeser, Marachi & Gehlbach, 2000). Thus,
the students. Finally, such changes are likely to
at a time when adolescents need academic
decrease the probability that any particular stu-
and social–emotional guidance and support
dent’s difficulties will be noticed early enough
from both parents and nonparental adults (i.e.,
to get the student necessary help, thus increas-
during early adolescence), teachers appear
ing the likelihood that students on the edge
less likely to be able to provide such support
will be allowed to slip onto negative motiva-
given the number of students they teach, their
tional and performance trajectories, leading to
educational training, and the size of second-
increased school failure and dropout.
ary schools. This creates holes in the safety
The latter is best exemplified by the work
net available to adolescents at a time when they
of Eccles and Midgley and by the studies on
are in particularly acute need of adult support
middle school reform initiated by the Carnegie
and guidance (Simmons & Blyth, 1987). It is
Foundation after their report Turning Points
not surprising that the most at-risk youth often
(Carnegie Council, 1989; Jackson & Davis,
fall through these holes.
2000). These scholars have looked at several
specific aspects of the classroom and school
environment and have shown that negative Grade-Related Differences in
changes in these aspects of student’ experiences Authority Relationships
at school as they make the middle or junior high Despite the increasing maturity of students,
school transition are linked to the declines in junior high school teachers place a greater
school motivation and engagement. They have emphasis on teacher control and discipline

c12.indd 420 11/3/08 8:09:17 PM


Level 3: School Districts and Secondary School Transitions 421

and provide fewer opportunities for student (Alspaugh, 1998; Eccles & Midgley, 1989;
decision making, choice, and self-manage- Finger & Silverman, 1966; Harter, Whitesell &
ment than do elementary school teachers (e.g., Kowalski, 1992; Simmons & Blyth, 1987).
Feldlaufer et al., 1988; Midgley & Feldlaufer, Imagine what such a decline in grades might
1987). Both stage–environment fit theory do to young adolescents’ self-confidence and
(Eccles et al., 1993) and self-determination motivation. Although Simmons and Blyth
theory suggest that these practices will create (1987) did not look at this specific question,
a mismatch between young adolescents’ desires they did document the impact of this grade
for autonomy and control and their perceptions drop on subsequent school performance and
of the opportunities in their learning environ- dropout. Even after controlling for a youth’s
ments; this mismatch is predicted to lead to a performance prior to the school transition,
decline in the adolescents’ intrinsic motivation the magnitude of the grade drop following
and interest in school. Evidence supports this the transition into either junior high school
prediction (see Wigfield et al., 2006). or middle school was a major predictor of
leaving school early in both studies (see also
Grade-Related Differences in Finn, 2006; Roderick, 1993; Roderick &
Affective Relationships Camburn, 1999).
Junior high and middle school classrooms
are often characterized by a less personal and Grade-Related Differences in
positive teacher–student relationship than are Motivational Goal Context
elementary school classrooms (Feldlaufer Several of the changes just noted are linked
et al., 1988; Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, together in goal theory. Classroom practices
1988). Given the association of classroom related to grading practices, support for
climate and student motivation reviewed autonomy, and instructional organization affect
earlier, it should not be surprising that mov- the relative salience of mastery versus perfor-
ing into a less supportive classroom leads to mance goals that students adopt as they engage
a decline in these young adolescents’ interest in the learning tasks at school. Given changes
in the subject matter being taught in that class- associated with these practices, it is not sur-
room, particularly among the low achieving prising that both teachers and students think
students (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Anderman & that their school environment is becoming
Anderman, 1999; Midgley et al., 1988). increasingly focused on competition, relative
ability, and social comparison as the young
Grade-Related Differences in adolescents progress from elementary to mid-
Grading Practices dle or junior high school (Midgley, Anderman,
There is no stronger predictor of students’ & Hicks, 1995). Midgley et al. (1995) found
self-confidence and efficacy than the grades that both teachers and students indicated
they receive (Guay, Marsh, & Boivin, 2003). that performance-focused goals were more
If academic marks decline with the junior high prevalent and task-focused goals were less
or middle school transition, then adolescents’ prevalent in the middle school classrooms than
self-perceptions and academic motivation in the elementary school classrooms. In addi-
should also decline. In fact, junior high school tion, the elementary school teachers reported
teachers do use stricter and more social com- using task-focused instructional strategies
parison–based standards than do elementary more frequently than did the middle school
school teachers to assess student competency teachers. Finally, at both grade levels the
and to evaluate student performance, leading to extent to which teachers were task-focused
a drop in grades for many young adolescents as predicted the students’ and the teachers’ sense
they make the transition to junior high school of personal efficacy. It is thus no surprise that

c12.indd 421 11/3/08 8:09:17 PM


422 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

personal efficacy was lower among the middle Summary


school participants than among the elemen- Changes such as those just reviewed are likely
tary school participants. Extending this work, to have a negative effect on many children’s
Roeser et al. (2002) looked at how elemen- motivational orientation toward school at any
tary and middle school teachers motivational grade level. However, Eccles and Midgley
practices and perceptions of the learning (1989) argued that these types of school envi-
environment for teachers was related to their ronmental changes are particularly harmful at
perceptions of their own work environments early adolescence given what is known about
using both self- and principal reports. psychological development during this stage
Results showed that teachers who were more of life. Evidence from a variety of sources
performance-oriented based on self-reported suggests that early adolescent development
instructional practices also (1) believed there is characterized by increases in desire for
was an emphasis on performance goals for autonomy, peer orientation, self-focus and
students in the wider school environment; (2) self-consciousness, salience of identity issues,
worked in schools where their school principals concern over heterosexual relationships,
reported greater use of performance-oriented and capacity for abstract cognitive activity
practices and policies in the school as a whole; (see Brown, 1990; Eccles & Midgley, 1989;
and (3) believed there was competition among Simmons & Blyth, 1987; Wigfield, Byrnes, &
staff and inequitable treatment of teachers by Eccles, 2006). Simmons and Blyth (1987)
the administration (school performance goal argued that adolescents need safe, intellectu-
structure for teachers). Similarly, teachers at ally challenging environments to adapt to these
both levels who reported a greater mastery ori- shifts. In light of these needs, the environmen-
entation also (1) perceived a broader empha- tal changes often associated with transition to
sis on such goals for students in the wider junior high school are likely to be especially
school culture and (2) perceived an emphasis harmful in that they emphasize competition,
on innovation and improvement for teach- social comparison, a performance-goal orienta-
ers among the staff and administration. These tion rather than a mastery-goal orientation, and
results suggest that the changing nature of the self-assessment of ability at a time of height-
motivational climate for learning for students ened self-focus; they decrease decision mak-
as they progress through school is paralleled ing and choice at a time when the desire for
by a changing motivational climate for teach- control is growing; and they disrupt the oppor-
ing for teachers as well. tunity for a close relationship between students
Anderman et al. (1999) also extended this and teachers at a time when adolescents may
work by comparing two groups of young be in special need of close adult relationships
adolescents: a group who moved into a middle outside of the home. The nature of these envi-
school that emphasized task-focused instruc- ronmental changes, coupled with the normal
tional practices, and a group who moved into a course of individual development, is likely to
middle school that emphasized more traditional result in a developmental mismatch so that the
performance/ability-focused instructional prac- fit between the young adolescent and the class-
tices. Although these two groups of students room environment is particularly poor, increas-
did not differ in their motivational goals prior ing the risk of negative motivational outcomes,
to the school transition, they did after the especially for adolescents who are having
transition. As predicted, the adolescents who difficulty succeeding in school academically.
moved into the first type of middle school were
less likely to show an increase in their extrinsic The High School Transition
motivational and performance-oriented moti- Although there is less work on the transition
vational goals. to high school, the existing work suggests

c12.indd 422 11/3/08 8:09:18 PM


Level 3: School Districts and Secondary School Transitions 423

quite similar problems (Coleman & Hoffer, students moved into middle school than when
1987; Jencks & Brown, 1975; Roeser & they moved into high school. As one would
Gonzalez, 1997; Wehlage, Rutter, Smith, expect with the stage–environment fit theory,
Lesko, & Fernandez, 1989). For example, high they found that the motivational declines were
schools are typically even larger and more associated with the high school rather than the
bureaucratic than are junior high schools and middle school transition (see Midgley, 2002,
middle schools. Lee and Smith (2001) provided for relevant chapters). They concluded that
numerous examples of how the sense of com- middle school reform efforts have been effec-
munity among teachers and students is under- tive in changing the middle school environment
mined by the size and bureaucratic structure in ways that support rather than undermine the
of most high schools. There is little opportu- young adolescents’ school engagement and
nity for students and teachers to get to know motivation. Further, they concluded that reform
each other, and, likely as a consequence, there is now needed at the high school level. These
is distrust between them and little attachment reforms look very much like the reforms that
to a common set of goals and values. There is were advocated for the middle school years.
also little opportunity for the students to form Most large public high schools also organize
mentor-like relationships with nonfamilial instruction around curricular tracks that sort
adults, and little effort is made to make instruc- students into different groups. As a result,
tion relevant to the students. Such environments there is even greater diversity in the educa-
are likely to undermine further the motivation tional experiences of high school students than
and involvement of many students, especially of middle grades students; unfortunately, this
those not doing particularly well academi- diversity is often associated more with the
cally, those not enrolled in the favored classes, students’ social class and ethnic group than
and those who are alienated from the values of with differences in the students’ talents and
the adults in the high school (e.g., Roeser et al., interests (Lee & Smith, 2001).
1999). These hypotheses need to be tested. Consequently, curricular tracking has
The few available studies provide initial served to reinforce social stratification rather
support (see Lee & Smith, 2001; Roeser et al., than foster optimal education for all students,
1999). For example, Fine (1991) documented particularly in large schools (Dornbusch,
how secondary school practices cumulate 1994; Lee & Smith, 2001). Lee and Smith
to drive out students who are not doing very documented that average school achievement
well academically. Similarly, studies of ethnic levels do not benefit from this curricular track-
minority youth provide extensive evidence that ing. Quite the contrary—evidence comparing
alienating and noninclusive high school prac- Catholic high schools with public high schools
tices undermine the school engagement and suggests that average school achievement lev-
achievement of students of color (e.g., Darling- els are increased when all students are required
Hammond, 1997; Deyhle & LeCompte, 1999; to take the same challenging curriculum. This
Ferguson, 1998; Jackson & Davis, 2000; Lee & conclusion is true even after one has controlled
Smith, 1993; Suarez-Orozco & Suarez-Orozco, for student selectivity factors. A more thor-
1995; Taylor et al., 1994; Valencia, 1991). ough examination of how the organization and
Recent work by Midgley and colleagues pro- structure of our high schools influence cogni-
vides additional support. In a longitudinal tive, motivational, and achievement outcomes
study of adolescents from elementary school is needed.
to high school, they were able to look at the
impact of both the middle school and the high Summary
school transition. They found less evidence of In this section we summarized the evidence
negative changes in school experiences as the related to the impact of school transitions on

c12.indd 423 11/3/08 8:09:18 PM


424 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

development. As one would expect, given various researchers have commented on the
what we now know about the ecological nature “religious schools effect” of Catholic schools in
of the junior high school transition, many early terms of student achievement and educational
adolescents, particularly the low achievers and attainments, especially among adolescent
the highly anxious, experience great difficulty non-Catholics, those of lower socioeconomic
with this transition. In many ways, this tran- status, and African Americans and Latinos liv-
sition can be characterized as a developmen- ing in urban areas (Bryk et al., 1993; Coleman,
tally regressive shift in one’s school context. Hoffer, & Kilgore, 1982; Jeynes, 2002). In
Consistent with our stage–environment fit a meta-analysis of the effects of Catholic reli-
perspective, such a shift has negative con- gious school attendance and personal religious
sequences for many youths’ school engage- commitment on academic achievement and
ment and performance. Also consistent with school conduct, for instance, Jeynes (2002)
our stage–environment fit perspective, there found that, after accounting for socioeco-
are now an increasing number of intervention nomic status and gender, the effect sizes for
studies showing that the junior high school religious school attendance were between 0.20
transition does not have to yield negative con- and 0.25 of a standard deviation for both aca-
sequences for vulnerable youth. Educational demic achievement and school conduct. These
institutions for the middle grades can be effects were particularly evident for Black and
designed in a developmentally progressive Hispanic secondary school students. Although
manner; when they are, the majority of early some suggest these effects are due to Catholic
adolescents gain from this school transition. schools selecting superior students, others
Finally, emerging evidence on the senior high have suggested that this claim is overdrawn
school transition suggests that reforms are and that the effects of a Catholic school edu-
badly needed at this level. cation on achievement are quite robust (e.g.,
Bryk et al., 1993; Sander, 1995).
LEVEL 4: SCHOOLS AS Three core features of the culture of these
EMBEDDED ORGANIZATIONS IN schools have been examined as instrumental
THE LARGER COMMUNITY in the reduction of inequality that are relevant
The most distal aspect of school influence on here: a communal organization, a philosophy
adolescent development lies in the fact that of human dignity, and a restricted range of
schools are embedded in much larger social curricular offerings (Bryk et al., 1993). First,
systems. Characteristics of the communities Catholic secondary schools tend to be some-
and the nations in which schools are placed what smaller than public secondary schools
influence everything about what goes on in and have strong communal culture grounded
the school building itself. Discussing all of the in a rich array of rituals and activities outside
macro influences is beyond the scope of a of the classroom where teachers and students
single chapter. In this section we focus on two get to know one another beyond their school-
macro characteristics: private versus public related roles. This community environment
schools and school resources. provides a social basis for motivating school
learning—a set of caring relationships and
Public Versus Private Schools corresponding sense of community become
The question of whether public versus private faculty, staff and students become major
schools do a better job motivating adolescent stu- motivators of in-school behavior.
dents and reducing achievement gaps between In addition, these secondary schools
those from different social backgrounds is are characterized by a set of shared moral
long-standing. Because of their record with commitments and a spiritual ideology that
socially disadvantaged students in particular, emphasize the dignity of each individual and

c12.indd 424 11/3/08 8:09:18 PM


Level 4: Schools as Embedded Organizations in the Larger Community 425

a corresponding ethic of care. These shared in middle or junior high schools. Furthermore,
beliefs are grounded in a religious theology students in the K–8 schools reported higher
(Christian personalism) in which social justice self-concepts and greater locus of control,
and the desire to provide a humanistic educa- received higher grades, and did better on stan-
tion for all individuals are paramount, and in dardized achievement tests than those in the
which the dignity of the individual as having middle grades schools. These K–8 schools
moral worth is preeminent to a view in which were predominantly private religious schools
worth is accorded to individuals based on rela- (74%) and were smaller size. Both sector
tive social and academic statuses. Thus, the (religious) and size (small) were identified
school culture is one that bridges two worlds by Eccles et al. (1991) as factors that could
for individuals—a moral–spiritual one in explain why students in K–8 schools showed
which the dignity of all individuals is recog- greater student commitment and engagement
nized and acknowledge as primary; and a prag- than those who were in middle schools or
matic one in which individuals are prepared junior high schools during these grades.
for the demands of economic and civic life in
a capitalist democracy. It is our view that this School Resources
moral center and related humanistic approach School resources in terms of adequate mate-
to education that characterizes the culture of rials, a safe environment, and continuity of
Catholic schools affords young people a non- teaching staff are often considered impor-
status-based foundation of worth and a sense tant for adolescents’ learning and well-being.
of belonging and corresponding sense of dig- Early studies of school effects on adolescents’
nity that disrupts pervasive negative images in development and achievement were based
the wider culture that may afflict ethnic minor- on economic models in which the relation of
ity youth and undermine their perceptions of so-called tangible school inputs (e.g., school
themselves as successful students. resources or size) to student outputs (e.g.,
The final, related feature of Catholic second- achievement and attainments) was the focus.
ary schools is their “delimited technical core” Although the central question of how much
(Bryk et al., 1993, p. 297). Students in these school resources matter for raising achieve-
schools have many required classes and less ment and reducing inequality in student
electives. Generally, all students are exposed outcomes is still being debated, school dis-
to a common curriculum that the faculty trict–level variations in such school resources
expect them to learn. Although administrative are likely a major contributor to the continuing
sorting still occurs, there are less “tracks” and inequity in educational outcomes for several
less differentiation of curricula by such tracks. minority groups in the United States.
The message to students is that every student Evidence does show that tangible physical
is not only capable of, but is expected to, learn plant of the school can affect 31students’
the core curriculum. behavioral conduct in school. In their study of
Similarly, Eccles, Lord, and Midgley 12 London area secondary schools, Rutter and
(1991) found that student outcomes, as rated colleagues (1979) found that although the age
by both teachers and students, were better in of the school buildings was not significantly
those attending K–8 schools than those who related to achievement or behavioral outcomes
made a transition into a middle or junior high in students, the cleanliness and use of plants,
school during grade 6, 7, or 8. Students in pictures, and other decorations inside the
K–8 schools were less likely to be truant, vio- school buildings was a significant predictor of
lent, or use substances at school, and were more the level of behavioral misconduct students dis-
likely to say they felt prepared for and inter- played in the school (after accounting for their
ested in their classwork compared to students social background). The more inhospitable and

c12.indd 425 11/3/08 8:09:19 PM


426 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

cold the school was, the greater the miscon- SECONDARY SCHOOL
duct of students. This finding may reflect the REFORM EFFORTS
“broken windows” theory (Wilson & Kelling, We want to end our chapter with a discussion of
1982) of delinquency and crime in relation several promising efforts at secondary school
to school physical environments. The basic reforms. As noted earlier, in 1989 the Carnegie
thesis is that abandoned and dirty physical Corporation issued the report Turning Reports
spaces connote a message of a lack of own- calling for the reform of education for early
ership and monitoring, and therefore become adolescents. Based in part on notions linked to
seedbeds for criminal activity and violence. It stage environment fit as well as linked to the
may be harder to value school and feel good needs of early adolescent children, they sug-
about oneself as a learner in a broken-down, gested that the middle grades should have the
leaky school building that communicates a following characteristics:
serious lack of societal value for teachers and
students (Kozol, 2006). It also may be harder • Create small learning communities that will
for an adolescent to be intrinsically motivated allow close relationships to emerge between
in a school environment in which poor light- teachers and students.
ing, crowding, noise, and debris are features • Teach a core academic program to everyone
that are as common as technology, books, and that include opportunities for service.
adequate desks and chairs (e.g., Clark et al., • Ensure success for all by eliminating track-
2006; Evans, 2004). ing, using cooperative learning, providing
Unfortunately, about 37% of African- flexible scheduling and adequate resources
American youth and 32% of Latino youth, to meet the learning needs of all students.
compared to 5% of European-American and • Empower teachers and administrators to
22% of Asian youth, are enrolled in the 47 take control of and responsibility for their
largest city school districts in this country; in schools.
addition, African-American and Latino youth • Staff schools with teachers who are trained
attend some of the poorest school districts in to teach early adolescents.
this country. In turn, 28% of the youth enrolled • Foster health and fitness.
in city schools live in poverty, and 55% are eli- • Reengage families.
gible for free or reduced-cost lunch, suggesting • Connect schools with communities.
that class may be as important (or more impor-
tant) as race in the differences that emerge. Similar recommendations have been
Teachers in these schools report feeling less offered by several other scholars, including
safe than do teachers in other school districts, Connell and colleagues at the Institute for
dropout rates are highest, and achievement Research and Reform in Education (Connell,
levels at all grades are the lowest (Council of 2003), Roderick (1993), Juvonen et al. (2004),
the Great City Schools, 1992; United Way, Lehr, Johnson, Bremer, Cosio, Thompson
2008). Finally, schools that serve these popula- (2004), as well as the many professionals inter-
tions are less likely than schools serving more ested in the “Middle School Philosophy” (see
advantaged populations to offer either high- Felner et al., 1997; Jackson & Davis, 2000;
quality remedial services or advanced courses Lipsitz, Mizell, Jackson, & Austin, 1997;
and courses that facilitate the acquisition of MacIver & Plank, 1997; MacIver, Young, &
higher order thinking skills and active learning Washburn, 2002; Midgley & Edelin, 1998).
strategies. Even adolescents who are extremely An increasing number of scholars and student
motivated may find it difficult to perform well advocates have argued for a return to the K–8
under these educational circumstances (United format because it seems to create more devel-
Way, 2008). opmentally suitable environments for the early

c12.indd 426 11/3/08 8:09:19 PM


Secondary School Reform Efforts 427

adolescent years (e.g., Juvonen et al., 2004; • Organizational structures that support a
Simmons & Blyth, 1987). climate of intellectual development and
The importance of small schools, schools a caring community with shared educa-
within schools, or small learning communities tional goals
has been stressed in many reform proposals, • Staff who are trained experts at teaching
along with the need to provide rigorous, young adolescents
challenging, and high-quality instruction. • Ongoing professional development oppor-
Small learning communities are likely to be tunities for the staff
particularly important during this develop- • Democratic governance that involves both
mental period because they support the emer- the adults and the adolescents
gence of strong teacher–student relationships • Extensive involvement of parents and the
that will allow students some autonomy within community
a very tight support network. These character- • High levels of safety and practices that
istics should support stronger engagement and support good health
identification with the school institution. When
engagement is accompanied by high-quality Similar conclusions were reached by
instruction then academic failure should be Juvonen and colleagues (2004), Lehr et al.
preventable. Interestingly, there are calls for (2004), and the NRC(2004) in their reviews of
quite similar reforms at the high school level. well-studied intervention and reform efforts.
Not surprisingly, the Carnegie Corporation Juvonen et al. (2004) also argued that K–8
report stimulated a major reassessment of structures might be more successful at imple-
schooling for early adolescents throughout menting the types of classroom characteristics
the country. The results have been disappoint- and building-level opportunities most support-
ing. Many districts changed from a junior high ive of continued academic engagement and
school format to a middle school format based positive youth development.
on the fact that middle school philosophy Together, these recommendations fit very
includes many of the components outlined in nicely with the stage–environment fit perspec-
the Carnegie Corporation report. Unfortunately, tive we outlined earlier. They are consistent
many of these changes failed to produce truly with both the developmental needs of early
successful middle schools. Often, the new mid- adolescence and what we know about high-
dle schools looked a lot like the old junior high quality instruction. We would like to provide
schools except for the fact that they contained a brief description of four promising programs
grades 6–8 rather than grades 7–9 (Jackson & as examples of changes that can be made at var-
Davis, 200l; Juvonen et al., 2004). ious levels within the secondary school context
In 2000, Jackson and Davis (2000) summa- that could support these types of changes: the
rized the findings of these many middle school Coca-Cola Valued Youth Program (CCVYP,
reform efforts. They concluded that the follow- www.idra.org/CCVYP/default.htm#vyp), the
ing middle grade school characteristics support Teen Outreach Program (www.cornerstone
both learning and positive youth development: .to), Oyserman’s possible selves intervention
(Oyserman, Terry, & Bybee, 2002) and the
• A curriculum grounded in rigorous First-Things-First whole school reform pro-
academic standards and current knowledge gram (Institute for Research and Reform in
about how students learn best and is relevant Education [IRRE], 2004). We pick these par-
to the concerns of adolescents ticular programs because they relate directly
• Instructional methods designed to prepare the developmental needs during adolescence.
all students to achieve at the highest The CCVYP took unique advantage of ado-
standards lescents’ desire to make a difference in their

c12.indd 427 11/3/08 8:09:20 PM


428 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

community. It offers 7th- through 12th-grade Trained facilitators lead discussions of such
students considered to be at risk for dropping topics as values clarification, managing family
out of school an opportunity to tutor elemen- relationships, and handling close relationships.
tary school students who were also identified Participants are encouraged to discuss their
as being at risk. The tutors are provided with feelings and attitudes.
training and support by teacher coordinators. Several evaluation studies have been done
Such a program is unique in its attention to on TOP (Allen et al., 1994, 1997). The students
providing adolescent youth with a meaningful who performed more volunteer service were
and authentic opportunity to “matter” in their at lower risk for course failure while they
school community. By allowing them to tutor were involved in the program; they were
younger children, the program also provides also less likely to be suspended from school
academically challenged youths with an oppor- and to get pregnant. Also, implementation
tunity to feel good about their academic skills quality of the TOPS curriculum did not sig-
and their ability to help other children do well nificantly influence program outcomes (Allen,
in school. Finally, it provides an unobtrusive Philliber & Hoggson, 1990), suggesting that
and respectful means for the tutors’ teachers to it is the community service and possibly the
become both mentors and protectors. mentoring components that are most important
The Teen Outreach Program (TOP; Allen, program.
Kuperminc, Philliber, & Herre, 1994; Allen, The intervention work by Oyserman and
Philliber, Herrling, & Kuperminc, 1997), colleagues (Oyserman, Gant, & Ager, 1995)
a national volunteer service program, is is based on the importance of group and
designed to both help adolescents understand individual differences in possible selves for
and evaluate their future life options and develop students’ engagement in school. Oyserman
life skills and autonomy in a context featuring et al. (1995) found that African-American stu-
strong social ties to adult mentors. The three dents are more motivated to invest time and
program components are supervised community energy in mastering school learning materials
service, classroom-based discussions of service if they include academic success in their future
experiences, and classroom-based discussion possible selves and if these African-American
and activities related to social–developmental adolescents included academic success in
tasks of adolescence. Participants choose their their view of what it means to a successful
volunteer activities with the assistance of trained African American (Oyserman et al., 1995).
staff who helps match the individual’s interests Subsequently, Oyserman and colleagues have
and skills with community needs. TOP sites developed and tested school-based interven-
typically offer a minimum of 20 hours per year tions designed to increase the salience of
of volunteer service for each participant. In one academic achievement in both individuals’
evaluated program, participants averaged 45.8 possible selves and ethnic identity. For exam-
hours of volunteer service during their 9 months ple, using a randomized treatment interven-
of involvement. tion design, Oyserman et al. (2002) provided
The Teen Outreach Curriculum provides a a group of African-American adolescents
framework for classroom meetings that include with a series of experiences designed to help
structured discussions, group exercises, them expand both their views of themselves
role-playing exercises, guest speakers and in various future occupations and the means
informational presentations. These discussions of obtaining these various occupational goals.
are designed to help students prepare for, and These means included increased commitment
learn from, their service experiences by deal- to educational success. Those students who
ing with topics such as lack of self-confidence, were part of the treatment reported greater
social skills, assertiveness, and self-discipline. bonding with school and greater concern with

c12.indd 428 11/3/08 8:09:20 PM


Summary and Conclusions 429

doing well in school than the controls. They The program both reduces high school dropout
also evidenced better school attendance. and increases academic performance, as well
First Things First, created by the Institute as closing the gap in academic performance
for Research and Reform in Education, entails between White and Black students.
three basic strategies: the creation of small
learning communities, the creation of strong SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
connections between family and school, and We have outlined many ways in which schools
the provision of high-quality instruction. These affect the development of adolescents and
strategies were selected because they facilitate stressed the need to take both a systems-level
the following four experiences for students: and a developmental perspective on schools.
We began by pointing out how the multiple
1. “Continuity of care” and strong student– levels of school organization interact to shape
teacher relationships the day-to-day experiences of adolescents
2. “Flexible scheduling that allows for addi- and teachers. We also stressed the interface
tional instructional time and attention to of schools as complex changing institutions
individual learning needs” with the developmental trajectories of indi-
3. “High, clear and fair standards for academics viduals. To understand how schools influence
and conduct” development, one needs to understand change
4. Exposure to “enriched and diverse learning at both the individual and the institutional
opportunities” level. Stage–environment fit theory provides
an excellent example of the linking of these
To accomplish these goals, IRRE works two developmental trajectories. Imagine two
with districts to provide the following three trajectories: one at the school level and one
experiences for the teachers and staff: (1) at the individual level. Schools change in many
“equip, empower, and expect staff to implement ways over the grade levels. The nature of these
effective instructional practices”; (2) flexibility changes can be developmentally appropriate
to redirect resources to meet emerging needs; or inappropriate in terms of the extent to which
and (3) “ensuring collective responsibility.” they foster continued development toward
(All quotes are from pages 6 and 7 of IRRE, the transition into adulthood and maturity.
2004). All three of these features require Youth travel through this changing context
school districts to put together teams of teach- as they move from grade to grade and from
ers that work with the same students over school to school. Similarly, youths develop
time and across school years. These teams and change as they get older. They also have
are provided with common planning time and assumptions about their increasing maturity
with remedial curricular materials that can be and the privileges it ought to afford them.
used to help students succeed. The teams are Optimal development is most likely when these
also provided with resources for their own two trajectories of change are in synchrony
continued development as high-quality teach- with each other—that is, when the changes in
ers and mentors. All students are provided the context mesh well with, and perhaps even
with a family advocate who works with 15–20 slightly precede, the patterns of change occur-
students and their families over time to help ring at the individual level.
the students succeed. This reform has been We also discussed the many ways in which
implemented in many school districts across experiences at school are influenced by the
the country and has been carefully evaluated larger cultural and social milieu in which
in the Kansas City, Kansas, school district. The schools are nested. Culturally shared beliefs
results of this evaluation are quite positive for influence how we fund our schools, what and
both the middle and senior high school grades. how we teach, and how we design school policy

c12.indd 429 11/3/08 8:09:20 PM


430 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

at all levels. These policies, in turn, influence Astor, R. A., Meyer, H. A., Behre, W. J. (1999). Unowned places
and times: Maps and interviews about violence in high schools.
the types of connections that schools have with American Educational Research Journal, 36, 3–42.
families, communities, higher educational insti- Astor, R. A. (1998). Moral reasoning aboug school violence:
Informational assumptions about harm within school subcon-
tutions, the labor market, and the daily experi- texts. Educational Psychologist, 33, 207–221.
ences of youths in the schools they attend. On Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency.
Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.
some levels, our schools are succeeding very Barber, B. L., Eccles, J. S., & Stone, M. R. (2001). Whatever hap-
well in supporting both learning and positive pened to the Jock, the Brain, and the Princess? Young adult
pathways linked to adolescent activity involvement and social
youth development for many groups of people. identity. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16, 429–455.
At other levels, schools are not supporting Barker, R. G., & Gump, P. (1964). Big school, small school.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
optimal learning or preparation for adult devel-
Blumenfeld, P. C. (1992). Classroom learning and motivation:
opment for many young people. Adolescents Clarifying and expanding goal theory. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 84, 272–281.
of color, particularly African Americans,
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y-F., Murry, V. M., Simons. R. L., Ge, X.,
Latinos, and Native Americans, still perform Gibbons, F. X., et al. (2006). Perceived discrimination and
the adjustment of African American youths: A five-year lon-
less well than European Americans and some gitudinal analysis with contextual moderation effects. Child
groups of Asian Americans (for discussions, Development, 77(5), 1170–1189.
Brophy, J. E. (2004). Motivating students to learn (2nd ed.).
see, e.g., Jencks & Phillips, 1998; Steinberg, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992; Suarez-Orozco & Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer culture. In S. S. Feldman &
Suarez-Orozco, 1995; Valencia, 1991). G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent
(pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bryk, A. S., Lee, V. E., & Holland P. B. (1993). Catholic schools
REFERENCES and the common good. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest, learning, and Carnegie Corporation of New York. (1992). A matter of time: Risk
the psychological processes that mediate their relationship. and opportunity in the non school hours. New York: Author.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 545–561.
Carnegie Council on Adolescent Development (1989). Turning
Allen, J. P., Kuperminc, G., Philliber, S., & Herre, K. (1994).
points: Preparing American youth for the 21st century.
Programmatic prevention of adolescent problem behaviors:
Washington, DC: Author.
The role of autonomy, relatedness, and volunteer service in
the Teen Outreach Program. American Journal of Community Carskadon, M. A. (1990). Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in adoles-
Psychology, 22(5), 617–638. cents. Pediatrician, 17, 5–12.
Allen, J. P., Philliber, S., & Hoggson, N. (1990). School-based pre- Carskadon, M. A. (1997, April). Adolescent sleep: Can we recon-
vention of teen-age pregnancy and school dropout: Process eval- cile biological needs with societal demands? Lecture given at
uation of the national replication of the Teen Outreach Program. Stanford University, April 21, 1997.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(4), 505–524. Chavous, T.M., Bernat, D. H., Schmeelk-Cone, K., Caldwell, C. H.
Allen, J. P., Philliber, S., Herrling, S., & Kuperminc, G. P. (1997). Kohn-Wood, L., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2003). Racial identity
Preventing teen pregnancy and academic failure: Experimental and academic attainment among African American adolescents.
evaluation of a developmentally based approach. Child Child Development, 74(4), 1076–1090.
Development, 68(4), 729–742. Chen, A., Darst, P. W., & Pangrazi, R. P. (2001). An examina-
Alspaugh, J. W. (1998). Achievement loss associated with the tion of situational interest and its sources. British Journal of
transition to middle school and high school. The Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 383–400.
Educational Research, 92, 20–26. Clark, C. Martin, R., van Kempen, E., Alfred, T., Head, J., Davies,
Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (1997). Changes in achievement H.W., et al. (2006). Exposure–effect relations between aircraft
goal orientation, perceived academic competence, and grades and road traffic noise exposure at school and reading compre-
across the transition to middle level schools. Contemporary hension. American Journal of Epidemiology, 163, 27–37.
Educational Psychology, 22, 269–298. Coleman, J. S., & Hoffer, T. (1987). Public and private high schools:
Anderman, E. M., Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1999). Declining The impact of communities. New York: Basic Books.
motivation after the transition to middle school: Schools can Coleman, J. S., Hoffer, T., & Kilgore, S. (1982). Cognitive out-
make a difference. Journal of Research and Development in comes in public and private schools. Sociology of Education,
Education, 32, 131–147. 55(2/3), 65–76.
Anderman, L. H. (1999). Classroom goal orientation, school belong- Connell, J. P. (2003). Getting off the dime: First steps toward imple-
ing and social goals as predictors of students’ positive and nega- menting First Things First. Reported prepared for the U.S.
tive affect following the transition to middle school. Journal of Department of Education. Philadelphia: Institute for Research
Research and Development in Education, 32, 90–103. and Reform in Education.
Anderman, L. H., & Anderman, E. M. (1999). Social predic- Connell, J. P., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Clifford, E., Crichlow, W., &
tors of changes in students’ achievement goal orientations. Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there: Behavioral, psychologi-
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 21–37. cal, and contextual factors affecting whether African-American
Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. adolescents stay in high school. Journal of Adolescent Research,
American Psychologist, 54, 317–326. 10, 41–63.
Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Stereotypes and the fragility Connell, J. P., & Klem, A. M. (2000), You can get there from here:
of academic competence, motivation, and self-concept. In A. Using a theory of change approach to plan urban education
J.Elliot & C. S.Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and reform. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation,
motivation, (pp. 436–456). New York: Guilford Press. 11, 93–120.

c12.indd 430 11/3/08 8:09:21 PM


References 431

Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Educational risk of Educational Research, vol. 26 (pp.113–180). Washington
and resilience in African American youth: Context, self, and DC: American Educational Research Association Press.
action outcomes in school. Child Development, 65, 493–506. Eccles, J. S., Lord, S. E., & Roeser, R. W. (1996). Round holes,
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, autonomy, square pegs, rocky roads, and sore feet: The impact of stage/
and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self-system pro- environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools
cesses. In R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota sym- and families. In S. L. Toth & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Adolescence:
posia on child psychology, vol. 23 (pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Opportunities and challenges, vol. 7 (pp. 49–93). Rochester,
Lawrence Erlbaum. NY: University of Rochester Press.
Cooper, H. M. (1979). Pygmalion grows up: A model for teacher Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman,
expectation communication and performance influence. Review D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development during adoles-
of Educational Research 49(3), 389–410. cence: The impact of stage–environment fit on adolescents’
Council of the Great City Schools. (1992). National urban educa- experiences in schools and families. American Psychologist,
tion goals: Baseline indicators, 1990–91. Washington, DC: 48, 90–101.
Author. Eccles, J., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we doing to
Covington, M. V., & Dray, E. (2002). The developmental course of early adolescents? The impacts of educational contexts on early
achievement motivation: A need-based approach. In A. Wigfield adolescents. American Educational Journal, August, 521–542.
& J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation Eckert, P (1989). Jocks and burnouts: Social categories and identity
(pp. 33–56). San Diego: Academic Press. in the high school. New York: Teacher College Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S, (1993). Talented Elder, G. H., Jr., & Conger, R. D. (2000). Children of the land.
teenagers: The roots of success and failure. New York: Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Cambridge University Press. Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1993). Entry into school: The
Darling-Hammond, L. (1997). The right to learn: A blueprint for beginning school transition and educational stratification in the
creating schools that work. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. United States. Annual Review of Sociology, 19, 401–423.
Darling-Hammond, L., & Bransford, J. (Eds.). (2005). Preparing Epstein, R., Chillag, N., & Lavie, P. (1998). Starting times of school:
teachers for a changing world: What teachers should learn and Effects on daytime functioning of fifth-grade children in Israel.
be able to do. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Sleep, 3, 250–256.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Self-determination research: Essed, P. (1990). Everyday racism: Reports from women of two cul-
Reflections and future directions. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan tures. Claremont, CA: Hunter House.
(Eds.), Handbook of self-determination theory research (pp. Evans, G.W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty.
431–441). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. American Psychologist, 59, 77–92.
Deyhle, D., &LeCompte, M. (1999). Cultural differences in child Fagan, J., & Wilkinson, D. L. (1998). Guns, youth violence and
development: Navaho adolescents in middle schools. In R. H. social identity. In M.Tonry & M. H. Moore (Eds.), Youth vio-
Sheets & E. R. Hollins (Eds.), Racial and ethnic identity in lence (pp. 373–456). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
school practices: Aspects of human development (pp. 123–140).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Feldlaufer, H., Midgley, C., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). Student, teacher, and
observer perceptions of the classroom environment before
Dewey, J. (1990). The child and the curriculum. Chicago: University and after the transition to junior high school. Journal of Early
of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1902.) Adolescence, 8, 133–156.
Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interven- Felner, R. D., Jackson, A. W., Kasak, D., Mulhall, P., Brand, S., &
tions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Flowers, N. (1997). The impact of school reform for the middle
Psychologist, 54(9), 755–764. years: Longitudinal study of a network engaged in Turning
Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Off the track. Presidential address at the Points–based comprehensive school transformation. Phi Delta
biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence, Kappan,78, 528–532, 541–550.
San Diego, CA. Felner, R. D., Seitsinger, A. M. Brand, S., Burns, A, & Bolton, N.,
Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and preven- (2007). Creating small learning communities: Lessons from the
tion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. project on high performing learning communities about “what
Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability conceptions. In works” in creating productive, developmentally enhancing,
A.Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement learning contexts. Educational Psychologist, 42(4), 209–221.
motivation (pp. 57–88). San Diego: Academic Press. Ferguson, R. F. (1998). Teachers’ perceptions and expecta-
Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women’s educational and tions and the Black–White test score gap. In C. Jencks & M.
occupational choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, Phillips (Eds.), The Black–White test score gap (pp. 273–317).
585–609. Washington, DC: Brookings Institute Press.
Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, volunteer- Fine, M. (1991). Framing dropouts: Notes on the politics of an urban
ing, basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular public high school. Albany: State University of New York Press.
involvement matters? Journal of Adolescent Research, 14, 10–43. Finger, J. A., & Silverman, M. (1966). Changes in academic per-
Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., Stone, M., & Hunt, J. (2003). formance in the junior high school. Personnel and Guidance
Extracurricular activities and adolescent development. Journal Journal, 45, 157–164.
of Social Issues, 59, 865–889. Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Educational
Eccles, J. S., & Gootman, J. (2001). Community programs to promote Research, 59, 117–142.
youth development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Finn, J. D. (2006). The adult lives of at-risk students: The roles of
Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: attainment and engagement in high school. Report to National
Developmentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. Center of Educational Statistics, Washington DC: U.S.
In R. Ames & C.Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in educa- Department of Education (NCES 2006–328).
tion, vol. 3 (pp. 139–181). New York: Academic Press. Fiqueira-McDonough, J. (1986). School context, gender, and delin-
Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. (1999). School and community influ- quency. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15, 79–98.
ences on human development. In M. Bornstein & M. Lamb Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. U. (1986). Black students’ school success:
(Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (4th Coping with “the burden of ‘acting white.’”Urban Review, 18,
ed., pp. 503–554). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 176–206.
Eccles, J. S., & Templeton, J. (2002). Extracurricular and other Fredricks, J., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children’s competence and
after-school activities for youth. In W. S. Secada (Ed.), Review value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth

c12.indd 431 11/3/08 8:09:21 PM


432 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

trajectories in two male sex-typed domains. Developmental Jencks, C. L., & Phillips, M. (Eds.). (1998). The Black-White test
Psychology, 38, 519–533. score gap. Washington, DC: Brookings Institute Press.
Fuligni, A. J., Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1995). The long-term Jeynes, W. H. (2002). A meta-analysis of the effects of attending
effects of seventh-grade ability grouping in mathematics. religious schools and religiosity on Black and Hispanic aca-
Journal of Early Adolescence, 15(1), 58–89. demic achievement. Education and Urban Society, 35, 27–49.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in Jussim, L.Eccles, J. S., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception,
children’s academic engagement and performance. Journal of social stereotypes, and teacher expectations: Accuracy and the
Educational Psychology, 95, 148–162. quest for the powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. In L. Berkowitz
Garcia Coll, C. T., Crnic, K., Hamerty, G., Wasik, B. H., Jenkins, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp.
R., Vazquez Garcia, H., et al. (1996). An integrative model for 281–388). New York: Academic Press.
the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Juvonen, J., Le, V. N., Kaganoff, T., Augustine, C., & Constant,
Child Development, 20, 1891–1914. L. (2004). Focus on the wonder years: Challenges facing the
Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early adolescent American middle school. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Corporation.
students: Relationships to motivation and achievement. Journal Kao, G., & Thompson, J. S. (2003). Racial and ethnic stratification
of Early Adolescence, 13(1), 21–43. in educational achievement and attainment. Annual Review of
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Sociology, 29, 417–442.
Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from childhood Kaplan, A, & Maehr, M. (1999) Achievement goals and student
through late adolescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of well-being. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 330–358.
Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13. Köller, O., Baumert, J.& Schnabel. K. (2001). Does interest matter?
Graham, S., & Taylor, A. Z. (2002). Ethnicity, gender, and the devel- The relationship between academic interest and achievement in
opment of achievement values. In A.Wigfield & J. S. Eccles mathematics. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education,
(Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 123–146). 32, 448–470.
San Diego: Academic Press. Kozol, J. (2006). Shame of the nation. NYC: Three Rivers Press.
Grolnick, W. S., Gurland, S. T., Jacob, K. F., & Decourcey, W. Lamborn, S. D., Brown, B. B., Mounts, N. S., & Steinberg, L.
(2002). The development of self-determination in middle child- (1992). Putting school in perspective: The influence of family,
hood and adolescence. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), peers, extracurricular participation, and part-time work on aca-
Development of achievement motivation (pp. 147–171). San demic engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engage-
Diego: Academic Press. ment and achievement in American secondary schools (pp.
Guay, F., Marsh, H. W., & Boivin, M. (2003). Academic self-con- 153–181). New York: Teachers College Press.
cept and academic achievement: Developmental perspectives Larson, R. W. (2000). Toward a psychology of positive youth devel-
on their causal ordering. Journal of Educational Psychology, opment. American Psychologist, 55(1), 170–183.
95, 124–136.
Larson, R. & Richards, M. (Eds.). (1989). The changing life space
Hallinan, M. T., & Kubitschek, W. N. (1999). Curriculum differ- of early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18 (6),
entiation and high school achievement. Social Psychology of 501–626.
Education, 3, 41–62.
Lee, V. E. & Loeb, S, (2000). School size in Chicago elementary
Harris-Britt, A., Valrie, C.R., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Rowley, S.J. schools: Effects on teachers’ attitudes and students’ achieve-
(2007). Perceived racial discrimination and self-esteem in ment. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 3-31.
African American youth: Racial socialization as a protective
factor. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17(4), 669–682. Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1993). Effects of school restructuring
on the achievement and engagement of middle-grade students.
Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W. Sociology of Education, 66, 164–187.
Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1995). Effects of high school restruc-
child psychology, vol. 3 (5th ed., pp. 553–617). New York: John
turing and size on early gains in achievement and engagement.
Wiley & Sons.
Sociology of Education, 68(4), 241–270.
Harter, S., Whitesell, N. R., & Kowalski, P. (1992). Individual differ-
Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1997). High school size: Which works
ences in the effects of educational transitions on young adoles-
best, and for whom? Educational Evaluation and Policy
cents’ perceptions of competence and motivational orientation.
Analysis, 19(3), 205–227.
American Educational Research Journal, 29, 809–835.
Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (2001). Restructuring high schools for equity
Hidi, S. (2001). Interest, reading, and learning: Theoretical and practi-
and excellence: What works. New York: Teacher’s College Press.
cal considerations. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 191–209.
Lehr, C. A., Johnson, D. R., Bremer, C. D., Cosio, A., & Thompson,
Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. (2000). Motivating the academically
M. (2004). Essential tools: Increasing rates of school comple-
unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of
tion: Moving from policy and research to practice. Minneapolis.
Educational Research, 70, 151–180.
MN: ICI Pulications Office.
Hoffmann, L. (2002). Promoting girls’ interest and achievement in
Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development.
physics classes for beginners. Learning and Instruction, 12,
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
447–465.
Lipsitz, J., Mizell, M. H., Jackson, A. W., & Austin, L. M. (1997).
Hunt, D. E. (1975). Person–environment interaction: A challenge Speaking with one voice: A manifesto for middle-grades
found wanting before it was tried. Review of Educational reform. Phi Delta Kappan, 78, 533–540.
Research, 45, 209–230.
MacIver, D. J., & Plank, J. B. (1997). Improving urban schools:
Institute for Research and Reform in Education. (2004). First Things Developing the talents of students placed at risk. In J. L. Irvin (Ed.),
First. Internal working document. Philadelphia: Author. What current research says to the middle level practitioner (pp.
Jackson, A. W., & Davis, G. A. (2000). Turning Points 2000: 243–256). Columbus, OH: National Middle School Association.
Educating adolescents in the 21st century. New York: Teachers MacIver, D. J., Young, E. M., & Washburn, B. (2002). Instructional
College Press. practices and motivation during middle school (with special
Jacobs, J., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. attention to science). In A. Wigfield& J. S. Eccles (Eds.), The
(2002). Ontogeny of children’s self-beliefs: Gender and domain development of achievement motivation (pp. 333–351). San
differences across grades one through 12. Child Development, Diego: Academic Press.
73, 509–527. Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school cultures
Jencks, C. L., & Brown, M. (1975). The effects of high schools on to enhance student motivation and learning. Boulder, CO:
their students. Harvard Educational Review, 45, 273–324. Westview Press.

c12.indd 432 11/3/08 8:09:22 PM


References 433

Mahoney, J. L., & Cairns, R. B. (1997). Do extracurricular activi- Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple pathways, multiple goals: The role
ties protect against early school dropout? Developmental of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of
Psychology, 33, 241–253. Educational Psychology, 92, 554–555.
Mahoney, J. L., Harris, A.L., & Eccles, J.S. (2006). Organized Renninger, K. A., Ewen, E., & Lasher, A. K. (2002). Individual
activity participation, positive youth development, and the over- interest as context in expository text and mathematical word
scheduling hypothesis. Social Policy Report, 20, 1–30. problems. Learning and Instruction, 12, 467–491.
Mahoney, J. L., Larson, R. W., & Eccles. J. S. (Eds.) (2005). Roderick, M. (1993). The path to dropping out: Evidence for inter-
Organized activities as contexts of development: Extracurricular vention. Westport, CT: Auburn House.
activities, after-school and community programs. Mahwah, NJ: Roderick, M., & Camburn, E. (1999). Risk and recovery from course
Lawrence Erlbaum. failure in the early years of high school. American Educational
McNeal, R. B. (1995). Extracurricular activities and high school Research Journal, 36, 303–344.
dropouts. Sociology of Education, 68, 62–81. Roeser, R. W. (2004). Competing schools of thought in achievement
Midgley, C. (2002). Goals, goal structures, and patterns of adaptive goal theory? In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances
learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. in motivation and achievement, vol. 13: Motivating students,
Midgley, C., Anderman, E., & Hicks, L. (1995). Differences between improving schools (pp. 265–299). New York: Elsevier.
elementary and middle school teachers and students: A goal Roeser, R. W. (2004, July). The diversity of selfways in school dur-
theory approach. Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 90–113. ing adolescence project. Paper presented at the annual meeting
Midgley, C., & Edelin, K. C. (1998). Middle school reform and early of William T. Grant Faculty Scholars Program, Vail, CO.
adolescent well-being: The good news and the bad. Educational Roeser, R. W., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Adolescents’ perceptions
Psychologist, 33, 195–206. of middle school: Relation to longitudinal changes in aca-
Midgley, C., & Feldlaufer, H. (1987). Students’ and teachers’ deci- demic and psychological adjustment. Journal of Research on
sion-making fit before and after the transition to junior high Adolescence, 88, 123–158.
school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 225–241. Roeser, R. W., & Eccles, J. S., (2000). Schooling and mental health.
Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). The transition to In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Handbook
junior high school: Beliefs of pre- and post-transition teachers. of developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.; (pp. 135–156).
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 543–562. New York: Plenum.
Midgley, C. M., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1989a). Changes in Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Freedman-Doan, C. (1999). Academic
teacher efficacy and student self- and task-related beliefs dur- functioning and mental health in adolescence: Patterns, pro-
ing the transition to junior high school. Journal of Educational gressions, and routes from childhood. Journal of Adolescent
Psychology, 81, 247–258. Research, 14, 135–174.
Midgley, C. M., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1989b). Student/ Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Academic and
teacher relations and attitudes toward mathematics before and emotional functioning in early adolescence: Longitudinal rela-
after the transition to junior high school. Child Development, tions, patterns, and prediction by experience in middle school.
60, 981–992. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 321–352.
National Research Council (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (2000). School as
high school students’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC: a context of social–emotional development: A summary of
National Academies Press. research findings. Elementary School Journal, 100, 443–471.
Noguera, P. A., & Wing, J. Y. (Eds.). (2006). Unfinished busi- Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S. & Strobel, K. (1998). Linking the study
ness: Closing the achievement in our schools. San Francisco: of schooling and mental health: Selected issues and empiri-
Jossey-Bass. cal illustrations at the level of the individual. Educational
Oakes, J. (2005). Keeping track: How schools structure inequality Psychologist, 33, 153–176.
(2nd ed.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Roeser, R. W., & Gonzalez, R. (1997). Research on transitions into
Oakes, J., Gamoran, A., & Page, R.N. (1992). Curriculum differen- and out of the middle school years. Working paper for the National
tiation: Opportunities, outcomes, and meanings. In P. Jackson Institute on the Education of At-Risk Students of the Office of
(Ed.), Handbook of Research on Curriculum (pp. 570–608). Educational Research and Improvement, Washington, DC.
New York: MacMillan. Roeser, R. W., & Midgley, C. M. (1997). Teachers’ views of aspects
Ogbu, J. (1992). Understanding cultural diversity and learning. of student mental health. Elementary School Journal, 98(2),
Educational Researcher, 21, 5–14. 115–133.
Okagaki, L. (2001). Triarchic model of minority children’s school Roeser, R. W., Marachi, R., & Gelhbach, H. (2002). A goal theory
achievement. Educational Psychologist, 36, 9–20. perspective on teachers’ professional identities and the contexts
of teaching. In C. M. Midgley (Ed.), Goals, goal structures,
Olsen. L. (1997). Made in America: Immigrant students in our pub- and patterns of adaptive learning (pp. 205–241). Mahwah, NJ:
lic schools. New York: New Press. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Oyserman, D., Gant, L., Ager, J. (1995). A socially contextualized Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C. M., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions
model of African American identity: Possible selves and school of the school psychological environment and early adoles-
persistence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, cents’ psychological and behavioral functioning in school: The
1216–1232. mediating role of goals and belonging. Journal of Educational
Oyserman, D., Terry, K., & Bybee, D., (2002). A possible selves Psychology, 88, 408–422.
intervention to enhance school involvement. Journal of Roeser, R. W., & Peck, S. C. (2003). Patterns and pathways of
Adolescence, 24, 313–326. educational achievement across adolescence: A holistic–
Padilla, A.M., & Gonzalez, R. (2001). Academic performance developmental perspective. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & S. C.
of immigrant and U.S. born Mexican-heritage students: Peck&R. W.Roeser (Vol. Eds.), New directions for child and
Effects of schooling in Mexico and Bilingual/English lan- adolescent development, vol. 101: Person-centered approaches
guage instruction. American Educational Research Journal, 38, to studying development in context (pp. 39–62). San Francisco:
727–742. Jossey-Bass.
Peck, S., Roeser, R.W., Zarrett, N.R. & Eccles, J.S. (2007). Roeser, R. W., Peck, S. C. & Nasir, N. S. (2006). Self and identity
Exploring the roles of extracurricular quantity and quality in processes in school motivation, learning, and achievement. In
the educational resilience of vulnerable adolescents: Variable- P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne, (Eds.), Handbook of edu-
and pattern-centered approaches. Journal of Social Issues, 64, cational psychology, 2nd ed.; (pp. 391–424 ). Mahwah, NJ:
135–155. Lawrence Erlbaum.

c12.indd 433 11/3/08 8:09:22 PM


434 Schools, Academic Motivation, and Stage–Environment Fit

Rosenholtz, S. J., & Simpson, C. (1984). The formation of ability Suarez-Orozco, C. & Suarez-Orozco, M. (2001). Children of
conceptions: Developmental trend or social construction? Immigration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Review of Educational Research, 54, 31–63. Suárez-Orozco, C., & Suárez-Orozco, M. (1995). Transformations:
Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1995). Coping with discrimina- Immigration, family life, and achievement motivation among
tion: How disadvantaged group members perceive the discrimi- Latino adolescents. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
nation that confronts them. Journal of Personality and Social Taylor, R. D., Casten, R., Flickinger, S., Roberts, D., &
Psychology, 68, 826–838. Fulmore, C. D. (1994). Explaining the school performance
Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: A mul- of African-American adolescents. Journal of Research on
tilevel analysis of students and schools. American Educational Adolescence, 4, 21–44.
Research Journal, 32, 583–625. United Way. (2008). Seizing the middle ground: Why middle school
Rumberger, R. W., & Thomas, S. L. (2000). The distribution of creates the pathway to college and the workforce. Los Angeles:
dropout and turnover rates among urban and suburban high United Way of Greater Los Angeles.
schools. Sociology of Education, 73, 39–67. Valdez, G. (2001). Learning and not learning English: Latino stu-
Rutter, M. (1983). School effects on pupil progress: Research find- dents in American schools. New York: Teachers College Press.
ings and policy implications. Child Development, 54, 1–29. Valencia, R. R. (Ed.) (1991). Chicano school failure and success:
Rutter, M.&Maughan, B. (2002). School effectiveness findings Research and policy agendas for the 1990s. London: Falmer
1979–2002. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 451–475. Press.
Rutter, M., Maughan, B., Mortimore, P., & Ouston, J. (1979). Wehlage, G., Rutter, R., Smith, G., Lesko, N., & Fernandez, R.
Fifteen thousand hours: Secondary schools and their effects on (1989). Reducing the risk: Schools as communities of support.
children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Philadelphia: Falmer Press.
Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social environ- Weinstein, R. (1989). Perceptions of classroom processes and stu-
ment and changes in adolescents’ motivation and engagement dent motivation: Children’s views of self-fulfilling prophe-
during middle school. American Educational Research Journal, cies. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation
38, 437–460. in Education, vol. 3: Goals and cognitions (pp. 13–44). New
Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evo- York: Academic Press.
lution. In W. Kessen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. Wentzel, K. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents?
1: History, theory, and methods (pp. 237–294). New York: John Teaching styles and student adjustment in early adolescence.
Wiley & Sons. Child Development, 73, 287–301.
Sellers, R. M., Caldwell, C. H., Schmeelk-Cone, K. H., & Wigfield, A., Byrnes, J. B., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Adolescent
Zimmerman, M. A. (2003). Racial identity, racial discrimina- development. In P. A. Alexander & P.Winne (Eds.), Handbook
tion, perceived stress, and psychological distress among African of educational psychology (2nd ed.; pp. 87–113). Mahwah, NJ:
American young adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Lawrence Erlbaum.
44(3), 302–317. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., MacIver, D., Reuman, D., & Midgley, C.
Sheets, R. H., & Hollins, E. R. (Eds.). (1999). Racial and ethnic (1991). Transitions at early adolescence: Changes in children’s
identity in school practices: Aspects of human development. domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem across
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. the transition to junior high school. Developmental Psychology,
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype suscep- 27, 552–565.
tibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R., Davis-Kean, P.
Psychological Science, 10, 80–83. (2006). Motivation. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child
Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence: psychology, vol. 3 (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
The impact of pubertal change and school context. Hawthorn, Wilson, J. Q., & Kelling, G. L. (1982). Broken windows, Atlantic
NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Monthly, March.[Available at www.TheAtlantic.com.]
Slavin, R. E. (1990). Achievement effects of ability grouping Wong, C. A., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (2003). The influence
in secondary schools: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of of ethnic discrimination and ethnic identification on African-
Educational Research, 60, 471–499. Americans adolescents’ school and socioemotional adjustment.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intel- Journal of Personality, 71, 1197–1232.
lectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., & Yates, M. (1997). What we know
Personality and Social Psychology,.69, 797–811. about engendering civic identity. American Behavioral Scientist,
Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S., & Brown, B. (1992). Ethnic differ- 40, 619–630.
ences in adolescents achievements: An ecological perspective. Youniss, J., Yates, M., & Su, Y. (1997). Social integration:
American Psychologist, 47, 723–729. Community service and marijuana use in high school.

c12.indd 434 11/3/08 8:09:23 PM

You might also like