Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Clinical Social Work Journal

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10615-019-00721-3

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Process of Integrating Animal‑Assisted Therapy into Clinical Social


Work Practice
Katherine Compitus1 

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Animal-assisted therapy (AAT) is considered an effective treatment model to use with a variety of populations and condi-
tions, from autism to substance abuse, due to its flexible nature. As an integrative model, it often incorporates core modalities
such as psychodynamic psychotherapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, psychodynamic psychotherapy and Gestalt therapy.
Although there is a growing body of research addressing the efficacy of AAT, there is limited information on the actual
process of utilizing AAT in clinical social work practice. Research shows that many clinicians support the use of AAT with
clients but fail to integrate it into their own practice due to a lack of knowledge on the process of utilizing the model. This
paper explores the process of integrating AAT into direct clinical practice and the theoretical basis for utilizing AAT as an
adjunctive treatment model.

Keywords  Animal-assisted therapy · Attachment · Object relations · Integrative treatment · Canine-assisted psychotherapy ·
Mindfulness · Psychotherapy · Psychodynamic · Process · Implementation · Therapeutic alliance

Introduction the human–animal relationship has long been established


(Freedman et al. 2014). Humans are social beings and caring
Research indicates that the majority of clinicians (91.7%) for, or working cooperatively with, animals may be instinc-
have heard of Animal Assisted Therapy (AAT), the integra- tively gratifying (Compitus 2019). The inclusion of animals
tion of an animal into the clinical treatment of clients, and in clinical treatment planning may tap into that long-estab-
view it as a legitimate therapeutic modality (Hartwig and lished reciprocal relationship in a way that enhances thera-
Smelser 2018). There is a growing body of research on the peutic healing in humans.
benefits of AAT in clinical practice, but many clinicians are Animal-assisted therapy is multifaceted and works as
unsure how the process works or how to implement AAT both a skill-building and emotionally corrective interven-
with their own clients (Hartwig and Smelser 2018). One tion. The inclusion of a therapy animal in clinical practice
of the core values of social work includes the importance provides a number of benefits to improve patient outcomes.
of human relationships (Workers 2008), and animals often The primary benefit is within the therapeutic alliance; a
help to build, establish and enhance interpersonal relation- therapy animal may facilitate engagement and strengthen
ships among humans (Compitus 2019). From an evolution- the r apport between the therapist and the client. The therapy
ary standpoint, humans have been working cooperatively animal may be perceived by the client as reinforcing the
with animals for 11-16,000 years and the mutual benefits of notion of a safe holding environment and, therefore, may
help to alleviate patient anxiety about receiving treatment
and increase patient adherence to the treatment protocol
Special thanks to Dr. Carol Tosone, Dr. Aminda Heckman- (Winnicott 1986). The therapy animal may serve to illus-
Chomanczuk, and Dr. Rise VanFleet for their helpful comments trate the parallel process between the animal and therapist
when reviewing this manuscript.
and the therapist and client. If the patient observes that the
* Katherine Compitus animal trusts the therapist, the client may feel more safe and
kog206@nyu.edu secure in treatment with that therapist. This may be espe-
cially helpful in trauma therapy (Germain et al. 2018) or
1
Silver School of Social Work, New York University, with substance use disorders, where the patients are more
New York, NY, USA

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Clinical Social Work Journal

likely to follow through on their treatment plan when there goal directed, documented and evaluated; it usually involves
is a positive therapeutic alliance (Waite et al. 2018; Wesley one client at a time (Pet Partners n.d.).
et al. 2009). Animals may be a critical source of social support for
Since AAT is such an adaptive model, research has people of all ages; the incorporation of an animal into direct
focused on the outcomes of AAT (Maujean et al. 2015; practice may provide a reparative experience for clients. In
O’haire et al. 2015), rather than the process of implement- a randomized control trial, Berget et al. (2008) found that
ing AAT or integrating it into existing models. This paper when animals are integrated into psychiatric treatment, cli-
explores whether the direct integration of AAT in clinical ents showed distinct improvements in self-efficacy, coping
practice can accelerate the therapeutic relationship and be ability and quality of life. Jones et al. (2019) systematically
used to teach behavioral skills, which may facilitate the heal- reviewed the incorporation of canines in mental health treat-
ing process. A case study is used to illustrate the direct appli- ments and found that there was not only an improvement
cation of AAT. Since not all clients would benefit from AAT, in the symptomatology but in the overall well-being of the
recommendations will be provided to assess which clients client. They reported that the benefits of canine-assisted psy-
would be appropriate for an integrated model utilizing AAT. chotherapy include an improvement in both internalizing and
externalizing disorders (including trauma, stress and anxiety
and anger), an improvement in engagement and socialization
What is Animal‑Assisted Therapy (AAT)? and, finally, a significant improvement in global functioning
(Jones et al. 2019). Research on the efficacy of AAT is still
Animal-assisted therapy (AAT) is an adaptive treatment ongoing and Jones et al. (2019) did not find that there was
model that has been used to treat a multitude of conditions sufficient evidence in this study to prove that AAT helped
from substance abuse to autism spectrum disorder to pain improve self-esteem, subjective well-being or coping.
and anxiety (Fine 2011; Waite et al. 2018; Wesley et al. As a flexible treatment model, AAT has been adapted
2009). The goal of AAT is to improve the social, emotional for use with a variety of evidence-based practices to treat
or cognitive functioning of a client, as developed in a for- a variety of mental illnesses. AAT can be adapted to work
mal treatment plan with set goals and objectives. The treat- in conjunction with Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT),
ment plan must be developed and implemented by a licensed Adlerian Counseling, Gestalt Therapy, and Solution-
professional; the directing clinician may be a mental health Focused Therapy, among other modalities (Chandler 2017;
therapist, occupational therapist, physical therapist or other Hunt and Chizkov 2014). Wesley et al. (2009) examined the
type of clinician depending on the specific needs of the use of AAT with dual-diagnosed patients (co-morbid diag-
client. AAT is flexible and adaptive by nature and can be nosis of mental illness diagnosis and substance abuse). They
implemented in a variety of ways in a multitude of settings found that regardless of the substance used (polysubstance,
(Stewart 2014). Although the sessions must be directed by cannabis, methamphetamine or alcohol) or demographic
a licensed professional, the clinician may choose to include of the client (male/female, voluntary/mandated), there was
a separate animal handler in the process, if they do not have a significant increase in the success of the treatment with
access to their own therapy animal (Stewart 2014). the experimental group (therapy animal) versus the control
It is important to note the difference between animal- group (no therapy animal). They hypothesized that treatment
assisted therapy (AAT) and animal-assisted activities would be more successful if a therapy animal was involved
(AAA). Although the interaction between a human and ani- because the animal helped build a stronger therapeutic alli-
mal almost always has some benefit, AAA may be described ance between the client and clinician.
as any social interaction with a therapy animal, while AAT
is the direct and purposeful involvement of an animal in the
therapeutic process (Chandler 2017). AAAs are, therefore, Research into the Efficacy of AAT​
more informal in nature, and are designed to build upon the
human–animal bond to improve the quality of life for the One of the benefits of AAT is that it can be adapted for a
target population. Animal-assisted activities tend to be short- variety of populations and conditions. The effect of AAT
term in nature; an example of an AAA is a therapy animal has been studied on pediatric oncology patients and their
visiting people in a hospital or nursing home. AAAs can be caregivers (Silva and Osório 2018). They found a decrease in
implemented with multiple clients at the same time, since pain, irritability, and stress, with an improvement in depres-
the visits are more social in nature (Pet Partners n.d.). AAT sive symptoms in the pediatric patients. They also found that
is a more structured intervention which must be done by there was a significant reduction in the anxiety and stress of
a licensed health professional (social worker, occupational the caregivers. AAT provides important social support for
therapist, etc.). AAT requires specific treatment plans, is children, aiding them in the development of empathy (Fine

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

2011) and helps prevent depression and isolation in older measured (Herzog 2014). Friedmann and Son (2009) ana-
adults (Peretti 1990). lyzed 28 AAT research projects conducted with people with
The benefits of AAT have been studied with schizo- a variety of conditions including schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s
phrenic clients, children with learning disorders, and people disease and developmental disabilities. Each of these studies
with depression, anxiety and a multitude of other illnesses found that the subjects involved received additional benefits
and conditions (Chandler 2017; Virués-Ortega et al. 2012; from the inclusion of animals in the therapeutic process that
Waite et al. 2018). Jasperson (2010) is one of the few clini- the control group did not. Herzog (2014) reports that AAT
cians who studied the efficacy of the integration of a therapy may be an effective treatment model but the majority of AAT
animal in treatment with female inmates diagnosed with studies thus far have been significantly flawed. He states that
BPD and concluded that many inmates found the inclusion the lack of manualization of the AAT protocol, small sample
of a therapy animal beneficial to their treatment, including sizes, and the “file drawer effect” were the primary reasons
an increased motivation to attend regular therapy sessions. In for the invalidation of AAT as an accepted treatment model.
fact, the inclusion of a therapy animal in direct treatment can He suggests that future AAT research should focus more
help “strengthen social skills, reduce anxiety and encour- on objective, rather than subjective, outcomes in order to
age interpersonal growth” (Jasperson 2010, p. 422) among bolster the claims made by researchers. This is not to say
clients. that AAT is not an effective treatment model but, rather,
O’Haire (2013) examined 14 clinical trials that consid- that a lot of past research into AAT has been flawed and our
ered the integration of AAT in treatment with children with examination of the efficacy of AAT is ongoing.
autism spectrum disorders. She found that the children who
underwent treatment involving AAT showed a significant
improvement in 27 out of the 30 the treatment outcomes that The Benefits of AAT Integration
were measured (Herzog 2014). Friedmann and Son (2009)
analyzed 28 AAT research projects conducted with people The primary function of AAT is to facilitate the therapeu-
with a variety of conditions including schizophrenia, Alzhei- tic alliance but AAT may also be used to initiate an analy-
mer’s disease and developmental disabilities. Each of these sis of the client’s psyche, address cognitive distortions, or
studies found that the subjects involved received additional teach behavioral skills (Chandler 2012). At first, the inclu-
benefits from the inclusion of animals in the therapeutic pro- sion of a therapy animal in sessions may help a therapist
cess that the control group did not. to more quickly establish a rapport with their client, since
Hunt and Chizkov (2014) also found a direct benefit in the initial verbal communication may involve a discussion
the integration of a therapy animal in treatment. Hunt and of the shared positive attitude towards the therapy animal.
Chizkov (2014) examined the impact of having a dog present The therapeutic alliance is a complex part of the therapeutic
when working with trauma survivors. They divided partici- practice but one which most often predicts the quality of the
pants into an experimental group which had a therapy dog overall treatment and the motivation of the client to engage
present during an exposure therapy treatment (via an expres- in treatment. The therapeutic alliance involves the mutual
sive writing paradigm) and a control group which received and shared development of goals and objectives for each
the same treatment but did not have a therapy animal present session and establishes an emotional connection between
in the room. They found that, despite no difference in cogni- the therapist and client (VanFleet and Faa-Thompson 2015).
tive processing, the experimental group showed a significant The inclusion of a therapy animal in treatment may help
reduction in depressive symptoms at the end of the exercise the client in trusting the therapist more quickly, due to cli-
(Hunt and Chizkov 2014). ent’s observations of the trusting nature of the relationship
However, not every study on AAT has found it to be an between the therapist and the therapy animal. Since the ther-
effective model. Charry-Sanchez et al. (2018) found that, apy animal trusts the therapist, and the client observes the
since many examinations into the efficacy of AAT use a positive way that the therapist interacts with the animal, the
variety of different scales to measure outcomes, it is difficult client may feel that the therapy animal is a good predictor
to determine whether the results are truly effective. Virués- of character and if the animal trusts the therapist, so should
Ortega et al. (2012) also struggled with identifying the effi- the client.
cacy of AAT in their study of AAT with older adults for Direct benefits include a reduction in anxiety (for both
the same reason (variety of scales used in studies). O’Haire the therapist and the client, see Frued, below), and a reduc-
(2013) examined 14 clinical trials that considered the inte- tion in the physiological symptoms of stress such as lowered
gration of AAT in treatment with children with autism spec- cortisol levels (Baun et al. 1984; Beetz et al. 2012). The tac-
trum disorders. She found that the children who underwent tile stimulation provided when a client touches the therapy
treatment involving AAT showed a significant improve- animal also helps to reduce cortisol levels, lowers blood
ment in 27 out of the 30 the treatment outcomes that were pressure and even reduces cholesterol (Beetz et al. 2012).

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

Stroking the animal may also provide a sense of reassur- Chandler (2012) recommends best practices in AAT
ance, as a child sometimes strokes their favorite blanket or a which include basic training for the therapy animal, an
stuffed toy, to provide a tactile sense of nurturance. In addi- evaluation of the agency or environment for the appropri-
tion, the client may feel comfortable hugging the therapy ateness for AAT, networking with colleagues regarding the
animal for support, in a situation where hugging the therapist implementation of AAT in that agency, adhering to agency
may not be appropriate. In this sense the therapy animal regulations (or establishing policies if none exist) regarding
may, therefore, function either as a transitional object (Win- bringing an animal into an agency, and the evaluation of
nicott 1986) or as a reparative relationship for the client. client preparedness for AAT. Just as not all animals have
The other benefits of animal-assisted therapy include the temperament for AAT (some may be too hyperactive,
the development of empathic skills, facilitating the learn- distracted, fearful), not all clients may be appropriate for
ing of interpersonal relationships, modeling skills such as AAT and the evaluation of clients and client preparedness
distress tolerance and mindfulness, and boosting the client’s are important steps in the implementation process.
confidence. Clients may feel more comfortable discussing Pet Partners and TDI have strict regulations regarding
stressful situations, such as a trauma history, when they feel the evaluation and certification of animals’ appropriateness
the holding environment is safe for them (Winnicott 1986); for AAT work. Although no laws require the certification of
a therapy animal can increase a client’s sense of safety and therapy animals, there are numerous benefits to certifica-
security (VanFleet and Faa-Thompson 2015). Oxytocin has tion, including the liability insurance for AAT work and the
been established as a feel-good hormone that increases a knowledge that an objective outside agency judge the ani-
person’s sense of well-being (Thielke and Udell 2017) and is mal safe to work with humans. The author is also an animal
well-known as the hormone that bonds people to each other, behaviorist and, therefore, works both as the handler of the
and mothers to their children. Research shows that oxytocin therapy animal and the clinician. However, a therapy animal
also increases in both humans and non-human animals when does not need to belong to the clinician and the animal’s
they interact, providing people with a biopsychological basis handler may be an outside agent who is also integrated into
for their increased sense of well-being when spending time the therapeutic process. This comes with other concerns
with animals (Thielke and Udell 2017). (such as confidentiality), so when clinical work is done with
Social isolation is considered one of social work’s “grand clients it is suggested that the clinician work with their own
challenges” because of the dangers involved in isolation therapy animal.
(potentially leading to death) (Johnson and Bibbo 2015; Although the author has a background in animal behavior,
Peretti 1990). Animals, as social lubricants, help people it is not required to implement AAT. There are currently few
overcome social isolation by providing an immediate com- AAT training programs but more are being developed and
mon interest and a subject of discussion for people (Peretti several agencies such as NASW are now offering training
1990). Animals, as “social lubricants” help people connect classes in AAT. There are also many books on AAT that
to each other and their communities; veterans often report can help a clinician get started in AAT, such as Cynthia
that a PTSD service dog is comforting because they are Chandler’s Animal Assisted Therapy in Counseling (2005),
never alone and always have someone looking out for them the Handbook of Animal Assisted Therapy by Aubrey Fine,
(older adults report similar feelings) (Bleiberg et al. 2005). and Animal-Assisted Play Therapy by Rise VanFleet and
Tracie Faa-Thompson. There is also an associated Animal-
Assisted Play Therapy certification course available to cli-
Getting Started in AAT​ nicians. Many social work schools are now offering AAT
or Human–Animal Bond courses to social work graduate
Despite the therapeutic benefit involved in any contact students.
between humans and non-human animals, AAT is not as
simple as bringing an animal into a session to play with a
client. A therapy animal should be trained and evaluated by The Implementation of AAT into Treatment
an established therapy animal organization such as Pet Part-
ners or Therapy Dogs International (TDI). A therapy animal Since animals have the appearance of providing uncondi-
does not have to be trained to do specific tasks (as a service tional acceptance without judgement (VanFleet 2018), the
animal) but a therapy animal should be trained to have basic client may find it easier to trust the animal first and, later,
manners such as housebreaking, not biting, sitting, staying form an alliance with the therapist based on the shared trust
and ignoring distractions. A therapy animal should be well of the therapy animal (if the client trusts the dog and the dog
socialized and comfortable in a variety of settings, should trusts the therapist, then the client may feel more comfort-
be comfortable being touched and brushed by people and able trusting the therapist). The paired association of the
should not startle easily. clinician with the animal, may help the client transfer their

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

positive feelings about the animal onto the clinician. This condition. The integration of a therapy animal in treat-
may aid the therapist to position themselves as both a safe ment may help clients reflect upon, understand and adapt
object (Scharff 1996) and a secure base (Ainsworth 1991) their social interaction style for optimal success (Chandler
for their client, since the therapy animal has deemed them 2017). When the therapy dog shows stress signals (licking
trustworthy. The client may see that the clinician takes good lips, yawning, ears pinned back), the clinician may pause
care of the therapy animal and may then experience a form the session to point out these stress signals to the client to
of transference which includes viewing the clinician as the discuss and process this interaction. For example, one client
“good enough” parent that they never had (Winnicott 1986). noted that the therapy dog would move away from her when
The positive relationship as created between the therapist, she became dysregulated, but would return when she was
therapy animal and client may, therefore, develop into a calm. This may also illustrate to the client that the method
reparative relationship for the client (Blum 2004; Bowlby of interacting with others (tone of voice, body language,
1982). A skilled clinician can help their client to internalize etc.) can help identify and process the way she interacts with
these experiences and use them to build up the client’s ego others that can help or harm her interpersonal relationships.
strength (Scharff 1996). In addition to a psychological sense The integration of AAT into clinical practice does not
of safety, therapy animals may also provide a sense of physi- only involve a psychodynamic aspect but also skill learning;
cal safety to patients, which is especially crucial in clients the establishment of new behaviors and habits allows the cli-
with a trauma history. ent to self-validate, self-regulate and self-soothe when not
When formally integrating AAT into therapy, it may be in the therapist’s office. Finally, clients may see the animal
useful for the clinician to promote the therapy animal as a as a vulnerable being that they want to protect, in a way that
model of appropriate behavior (such as emotional regulation they, themselves, did not feel protected as a child. Even if
or interpersonal skill building), as suggested by Hunt and they do not feel like attending therapy on a regular basis, a
Chizkov (2014) (which has been described in the review sec- client may return each week to check if the therapy animal is
tion). Although they did not specify a cause for the reduction well. Once in session, the client may then be encouraged to
of symptoms, it is possible that participants may have looked do the work since the core skills are modelled by the therapy
to the therapy animal as a model for distress tolerance since animal that they care so much about.
the therapy animal was a model of resiliency. Similarly,
González-Ramírez et al. (2013) found that the presence of
a therapy animal in the room during a therapeutic interven- Case Study
tion “reduced the physiological indicators of stress” (p. 275).
This would also suggest that there is something in the ani- The following case study is a composite of multiple cases
mal’s behavior that is leading the participants to behave in involving AAT. The clinician’s own therapy dog, Chompy,
a similar fashion. who is registered with Pet Partners, was involved in each
The therapist can also encourage interactions with the session.
animal which teach emotional regulation and interpersonal Jane is a 16 year old girl who purposely hurt herself for
skills. Chandler (2017) discusses the way a therapy dog many years; she would use a knife to cut her skin, which
models emotional regulation to clients by waiting to receive she said would help stop any emotional pain she was feel-
food or a toy when asked to wait by the therapist. Clients ing. Jane can sometimes be very insightful about her own
may learn self-control and self-regulation by reflection on emotions and at other times appears unable to see past the
the behavior modelled by the therapy animal. The thera- surface. She reports a very troublesome and upsetting child-
pist can ask the client to interact with the therapy animal in hood, explaining that, for several years, she was molested by
a way that alternates between exciting and calming (Van- an uncle that was only slightly older than herself. She only
Fleet and Faa-Thompson 2017). This provides them with recently reported the sexual assault to her family, since she
the experience of practicing self-regulation and afterward was worried that they would not believe her.
they can process the experience verbally with the therapist. Since reporting the assault to her family, her family
The physiological piece of AAT is important because it is has been very supportive, but the assault was not the only
phenomenological and the client learns physiologically what stressor in Jane’s childhood. Jane’s mother is emotionally
each internal state feels like as they develop a sense of con- volatile; she can be loving and kind one moment and angry
trol (VanFleet and Faa-Thompson 2017). and emotionally abusive the next. Jane never knows what to
In addition, people are motivated by social relatedness expect. Her mother is often emotionally invalidating, which
and many clients in therapy report having interpersonal is why Jane waited so long to report the sexual assault. Jane
problems and may have difficulty with social interactions. laments that she has grown numb as a result of her mother’s
This may leave the clients feeling lonely and isolated and mood swings. Jane’s father is a drug addict and has been in
may feed into the stigma of mental illness, worsening their prison. Although Jane reports that he is the more nurturing

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

of the two, she reports that, due to his drug use, he was an uncertain future. We were able to discuss the benefits of
absent and neglectful parent. Jane stated several times that mindfulness and analyze what situations in her past may
she feels alone in life. have caused her to dissociate, rather than be mindful; we
Chompy is a Staffordshire Terrier and Bulldog mixed also discussed the reasons why mindfulness may be helpful
breed dog who was found, starved and near death, on the to her in the present. We discussed the causes of her psychic
street in a nearby city. Chompy was rehabilitated, social- pain, how certain maladaptive coping techniques were useful
ized and trained by the clinician to work as a therapy dog. in the past but now cause her more distress than help and,
The clinician has other dogs but Chompy’s temperament and finally, how mindfulness brings and increased awareness and
personality made him an exceptional candidate for therapy enjoyment of life. We then closed each session teaching the
work. therapy dog new tricks, to improve Jane’s feelings of mas-
In addition to his innate desire to help people, Chompy’s tery and self-efficacy, so that she would not leave the session
own story of trauma and resilience is often helpful when feeling raw but rather feeling in control of both her emotions
working with clients. Jane was very hesitant to start therapy and her environment.
and, at first, behaved as a mandated client would, stating The spontaneous nature of animals is a helpful aspect of
that she didn’t want therapy. However, Jane would return to AAT which was utilized in sessions. Whenever the therapy
therapy each week and would report that she was there to dog had any (minor) behavioral problems (such as pulling
see Chompy. While in session, she did valuable therapeutic on the leash), Jane and I were able to discuss possible causes
work and she reported that Chompy’s own trauma narrative for the behavior and possible resolutions that involved a non-
helped her feel connected to him from the first session. Jane judgemental evaluation of the situation. This often helped
showed insight in recognizing that during the initial ses- Jane pay more attention to her own body language and
sions she felt more comfortable with the therapy dog present behavioral challenges in a way that felt non-threatening to
because trusting the dog and trusting the therapist became a her. We discussed her childhood growing up in an abusive
parallel process. She reported that she had difficulty trusting environment and that the judgements expressed by her nar-
previous clinicians and she never felt that she established cissistic father were not usually accurate assessments of the
a lasting connection with them. Since a rapport was more world. Jane often felt extreme shame and guilt for everything
quickly established with Jane, we were able to move into that she did that she decided was “wrong”. We built on this
the integrative model using AAT after only a few sessions. analysis when I explained to Jane that if Chompy pulled on
Some core skills that we work on when integrating AAT the leash, he wasn’t a “bad dog”, but he was excited to smell
with behavioral therapies are mindfulness skills, interper- something and it was my job to work with him to understand
sonal effectiveness skills, emotional regulation skills and what he needed and how to redirect his attention. Jane used
distress tolerance skills (Linehan 2014). However, theses this example and applied it to her own childhood, realizing
subjects may also be discussed when using a psychodynamic that her father’s actions were inappropriate and that when
approach, since the psychic basis for the client’s emotional she was a child, and acted as a child, she was not being
difficulties can be analyzed as they come up in the client’s “bad”, as her father told her. She learned that the judge-
interactions with the therapy animal. The most important ments of her father did not have to linger in her psyche as
(and often forgotten) aspect about integrating AAT into self-criticisms in her adulthood.
clinical treatment is the development of goals and objec- Jane and I discussed how she related to the therapy dog
tives that integrate the therapy animal into the treatment (and how he interacted with her) during her various moods
model (Compitus 2019; Chandler 2017). Keeping in mind (angry, sad, happy), to help illustrate and practice interper-
the goal for each session, I developed individual objectives sonal effectiveness skills. As stated above, she learned that
for each of the skills that included the therapy animal in the patience and kindness would bring Chompy closer to her,
skill-based lessons. as would a happy mood and playing with him. She learned
In learning about mindfulness skills, I encouraged Jane to that the dog would disengage in their interactions and move
identify ways in which the dog utilized mindfulness skills. away from her when she would become anxious or (mildly)
For example, I pointed out that when Chompy was eating, dysregulated but would return when she would become calm
he was only focused on eating, when he was playing, he again. Chompy would not play with her when she would
was very interested in his toys and when he was out on a demand that he played, but he would happily play with her
walk, he was utilizing all of her senses to listen, smell, see when she kindly engaged him in play or called to him in a
(and sometimes taste) the world around him. I built upon happy voice. He also would move away from her when she
the dog’s focused attention on his food or toy to open up a became overly affectionate, but would enjoy interacting with
conversation with Jane about the concept of mindfulness her when she was just affectionate enough. We discussed
and the benefits of living in the moment, rather than feel- the interactions with Chompy and compared them to her
ing depressed about past mistakes or worrying about an relationships with friends and family. She reported that she

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

found it easier to understand the limitations and expectations with the therapy dog could serve as a model for healthy rela-
of other people when illustrated by her interactions with the tionships with other humans, she began to embrace therapy in
therapy dog. a way she previously had not. She began to thrive and prac-
Working with AAT also helped Jane to learn and practice tice her new found skills daily and, over time, her relationship
the skill of emotional regulation. She reported that she felt with her family and friends slowly began to improve and she
more motivated to regulate her emotions because she did not reported an increased sense of peace.
want to scare the dog when she was angry or upset. Jane and
I established goals and objectives that integrated the dog into
the development of both emotional regulation and distress Limitations and Future Directions
tolerance skills. When she would start to feel dysregulated
(or when she would see the therapy dog showing signs of Clinicians should pay special attention to the limitations in
her dysregulation, such as moving away from her), she could implementing AAT with select clients; not all clients are
speak softly to the dog, stroke the dog’s fur, hug or play with suitable for the integration of a therapy animal in treat-
the dog in order to calm herself. In addition, she brought ment. Some clients may have a fear of animals, others may
a fluffy stuffed animal that looked like Chompy and would have traits of antisocial personality disorder and may pose
keep it at her bedside, so that when she felt in distress at times a potential risk to the physical or emotional welfare of the
when she was away from the therapy dog, she could use the therapy animal. The client or therapist may eventually ter-
toy as a transition object by stroking it (Winnicott 1986). The minate therapy and it is important that the client develop
stroking of the toy reminded her of petting the dog and the strong emotional regulation skills in order to separate from
secure attachment and bond that she formed with the therapy the dog without maladaptive feelings of loss (VanFleet and
dog (Bowlby 1982); this helped her return to a more calm, Faa-Thompson 2017). Clinicians should also be cautious
less distressed state of mind. When she returned to session, of utilizing AAT with clients who have severe attachment
we were able to discuss the types of attachment, the reason disruptions. The client may form a bond so strong to the
why she had developed a disorganized attachment style, and therapy animal that when service is terminated or the animal
discuss ways of repairing her method of attaching to others dies, it may trigger feelings of abandonment. It is important,
(Ainsworth 1991; Bowlby 1982). when utilizing AAT with clients with insecure attachment
Jane and I practiced increased empathy and understand- styles that the clinician continually encourage the client’s
ing for others through her interactions with the therapy dog self-reliance rather than dependence on a therapy animal.
by teaching her how to pet, massage and groom the therapy The key to AAT with clients with an insecure or disorgan-
dog. The physical interactions with her dog, and focusing ized attachment style, is to transfer the relational abilities
on the dog’s feelings of wanting or disliking to pet touched that the client developed in therapy to the real world (Van-
in certain ways, helped Jane understand that others may Fleet and Faa-Thompson 2017). These clients may still be
have limitations and feelings that are different from what candidates for AAT, but should be guided through the termi-
she expected. The physical interactions with the dog was an nation process in order to therapeutically handle the endings
opportune moment to discuss the importance of boundaries. and loss and generalize these skills to the real world.
We were able to discuss the emotional limitations of indi-
viduals and that Chompy also had days where he felt more
playful and days when he just wanted to sleep. We discussed Therapy Animal Welfare
that the dog’s unwillingness to play did not mean that he did
not love her, but that sometimes for self-preservation we Finally, the welfare of the therapy animal must always be an
must ask for space and time to rest. important consideration in the implementation of AAT with
Finally, the dog provided Jane with a physical sense of any client (Fine 2015; Glenk 2017). Serpell et al. (2010)
safety. Due to her physical, emotional and sexual abuse history, explain that a concept of animal welfare should be multi-
she often had difficulty sleeping and rarely felt physically safe. factorial and include negative feelings that the animal may
Jane reported that before working with the therapy dog, she experience, the ability to express species-specific behaviors,
had frequently slept with the lights on and never slept soundly. the animal’s resiliency, adaptability and capacity to cope, as
After starting the integrated model with AAT, she began to well as their overall well-being.
sleep with the lights off, slept restfully and felt the safest she There is some concern that clinicians often do not receive
had ever felt in her life. It is important to note that a positive adequate training in animal behavior to understand the spe-
relational experience with an animal can then be more easily cific welfare requirements of the animals that they work
generalize to more positive relationships with other humans. with (Serpell et al. 2010). Therapy animals must have time
Jane had reported that in the past she had a strong reluctance to to habituate themselves to the therapeutic process (Serpell
participate in therapy. When Jane understood her relationship et al. 2010) and should not be involved in direct clinical

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

practice without sufficient preparation. Every clinician uti- does not qualify as AAT. Goals and objectives involving the
lizing AAT should be knowledgeable about the “Five Free- therapy animal must be included when developing a treat-
doms” establish guidelines for working with animals that ment plan. More research must be done on the efficacy of
protect the animal from harm. These include the right of AAT in order to establish it as an evidence-based practice,
the therapy animal to determine when it wants to work or but there appears to be a lot of promise in utilizing AAT as
when it needs a break (Barber and Mellen 2013). Not only a treatment model in the treatment of various mental and
should the therapy animal be allowed to choose their level physical illnesses.
of interaction with the client (which can be encouraged by
the clinician), the clinician must be aware of signs that the
animal is uncomfortable or in distress (such as yawning, ears
back, or trying to leave the room) and must have a plan for References
situations when the therapy animal needs to leave the ses-
sion before the session is over (VanFleet and Faa-Thompson Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1991). Attachments and other affectional bonds
across the life cycle. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P.
2015). In this case a covered dog crate or cat carrier may be Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 33–51). New
particularly useful in a clinician’s office. The clinician must York: Routledge.
make sure to explain to the client that the animal needs a Barber, J. C., & Mellen, J. D. (2013). Animal ethics and welfare. Zoo-
break and that it is no reflection on the client but that the keeping: An introduction to the science and technology (p. 53).
Chicago : The University of Chicago Press.
animal simply requires a break to self-regulate. In addition to Baun, M., Bergastrom, N., Langston, N., & Thoma, L. (1984). Physi-
establishing a safe environment for both the animal and cli- ological effects of human/companion animal bonding. Nursing
ent, paying attention to the therapy animal’s needs can easily Research, 15, 126–129.
be turned into a teachable moment where the clinician and Beetz, A., Uvnäs-Moberg, K., Julius, H., & Kotrschal, K. (2012).
Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human–animal
client discuss effective interpersonal skills such as assertive- interactions: The possible role of oxytocin. Frontiers in psychol-
ness and emotional regulation skills, including walking away ogy, 3, 234.
from a stressful situation without becoming dysregulated. Berget, B., Ekeberg, Ø., & Braastad, B. O. (2008). Animal-assisted
therapy with farm animals for persons with psychiatric disorders:
Effects on self-efficacy, coping ability and quality of life, a ran-
domized controlled trial. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in
Conclusion Mental Health, 4(1), 9.
Bleiberg, J., Prout, M., Debiak, D., Lefkowitz, C., & Paharia, I. (2005).
Although research on the efficacy of AAT is varied, it is Animal-assisted prolonged exposure: A treatment for survivors of
sexual assault suffering posttraumatic stress disorder. Society &
potentially a very effective adjunctive treatment model. Animals, 13(4), 275–296.
Often clinicians who want to implement AAT are unaware Blum, H. P. (2004). Separation-individuation theory and attachment
of how to start the process. This paper has outlined the pro- theory. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association,
cess of implementing AAT as an adjunctive model that can 52(2), 535–553.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment & loss. Attachment (2nd ed., Vol. 1).
be used in conjunction with CBT, DBT, Psychodynamic New York: Basic Books. Original work published 1969.
Psychotherapy, or other models. The primary reason to inte- Chandler, C. K. (2012). Animal assisted therapy in counseling. New
grate AAT into clinical practice is to build a faster rapport, York: Routledge.
to encourage the parallel process, to increase the client’s Chandler, C. K. (2017). Animal assisted therapy in counseling. New
York: Routledge.
sense of safety and security, and to build a stronger holding Charry-Sanchez, J., Pradilla, I., & Talero-Gutierrez, C. (2018). Effec-
environment. Other reasons may be that the therapy animal tiveness of animal-assisted therapy in the pediatric population:
serves as a motivation for the client to return to therapy each Systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled studies. Jour-
week, interactions with the therapy animal may service as a nal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 39(7), 580–590.
Compitus, K. (2019). Traumatic pet loss and the integration of attach-
discussion point for psychoanalytic work, may help illustrate ment-based animal assisted therapy. Journal of Psychotherapy
behavioral skill building, or help the client more quickly Integration. https​://doi.org/10.1037/int00​00143​.
alter cognitive distortions. The presence of a therapy animal Fine, A. H. (2011). Understanding the AAT Rx: Applications of AAI
may be more fun for clients and clients may feel less stigma- in clinical practice. In C. Blazina, G. Boyraz, & D. Shen-Miller
(Eds.), The psychology of the human–animal bond (pp. 125–136).
tized if they return each week to visit the animal, rather than New York: Springer.
admit early in the process that they may need psychotherapy. Fine, A. H. (Ed.). (2015). Handbook on animal-assisted therapy: Foun-
In addition, clients may feel overwhelmed or threatened dations and guidelines for animal-assisted interventions. London:
when asked to practice the skills for themselves (due to their Academic Press.
Freedman, A. H., Gronau, I., Schweizer, R. M., Ortega-Del Vecchyo,
feelings of low self-worth) but may be more inclined to learn D., Han, E., Silva, P. M., … & Beale, H. (2014). Genome sequenc-
the skills for the benefit of an animal. Clinicians must, how- ing highlights the dynamic early history of dogs. PLoS Genetics,
ever, be aware of the welfare implications of the animal and 10(1), e1004016.
they need to be reminded that simply playing with an animal

13
Clinical Social Work Journal

Friedmann, E., & Son, H. (2009). The human–companion animal bond: Silva, N. B., & Osório, F. L. (2018). Impact of an animal-assisted therapy
How humans benefit. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small programme on physiological and psychosocial variables of paediat-
Animal Practice, 39(2), 293–326. ric oncology patients. PLoS ONE, 13(4), e0194731.
Germain, S. M., Wilkie, K. D., Milbourne, V. M., & Theule, J. (2018). Stewart, L. A. (2014). Competencies in animal assisted therapy in coun-
Animal-assisted psychotherapy and trauma: A meta-analysis. seling: A qualitative investigation of the knowledge, skills and atti-
Anthrozoös, 31(2), 141–164. tudes required of competent animal assisted therapy practitioners,
Glenk, L. M. (2017). Current perspectives on therapy dog welfare in Ph.D. dissertation. Altanta, Georgia: Georgia State University.
animal-assisted interventions. Animals, 7(2), 7. Thielke, L. E., & Udell, M. A. (2017). The role of oxytocin in relation-
González-Ramírez, M. T., Ortiz-Jiménez, X. A., & Landero-Hernández, ships between dogs and humans and potential applications for the
R. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy and animal-assisted ther- treatment of separation anxiety in dogs. Biological Reviews of
apy: Stress management for adults. Alternative and Complementary the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 92, 378–388. https​://doi.
Therapies, 19(5), 270–275. org/10.1111/brv.1223.
Hartwig, E., & Smelser, Q. K. (2018). Practitioner perspectives on ani- VanFleet, R. (2018). Are dogs really unconditional? International Insti-
mal-assisted counseling. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 40, tute for Animal Assisted Play Therapy. Retrieved from http://www.
43–57. https​://doi.org/10.17744​/mehc.40.1.04. iiaap​t.org.
Herzog, H. (2014). Does animal-assisted therapy really work. What clinical VanFleet, R., & Faa-Thompson, T. (2015). Animal assisted play therapy.
trials reveal about the effectiveness of four-legged therapists). https​:// Play therapy: A comprehensive guide to theory and practice (pp.
www.psych​ology​today​.com/blog/anima​ls-and-us/20141​1/does-anima​ 201–214). New York: Guilford Publications.
l-assis​ted-thera​py-reall​y-work. Accessed on 26 February 2017. VanFleet, R., & Faa-Thompson, T. (2017). Animal assisted play therapy.
Hunt, M. G., & Chizkov, R. R. (2014). Are therapy dogs like Xanax? Sarasota: Professional Resource Press.
Does animal-assisted therapy impact processes relevant to cognitive Virués-Ortega, J., Pastor-Barriuso, R., Castellote, J. M., Población, A.,
behavioral psychotherapy? Anthrozoös, 27(3), 457–469. & de Pedro-Cuesta, J. (2012). Effect of animal-assisted therapy on
Jasperson, R. A. (2010). Animal-assisted therapy with female inmates the psychological and functional status of elderly populations and
with mental illness: A case example from a pilot program. Journal patients with psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. Health Psy-
of Offender Rehabilitation, 49(6), 417–433. chology Review, 6(2), 197–221.
Johnson, R., & Bibbo, J. (2015). Human–animal interaction in the aging Waite, T. C., Hamilton, L., & O’Brien, W. (2018). A meta-analysis of
boom. Handbook on animal-assisted therapy (pp. 249–260). New animal assisted interventions targeting pain, anxiety and distress in
York: Academic Press. medical settings. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice,
Jones, M. G., Rice, S. M., & Cotton, S. M. (2019). Incorporating ani- 33, 49–55.
mal-assisted therapy in mental health treatments for adolescents: Wesley, M. C., Minatrea, N. B., & Watson, J. C. (2009). Animal-assisted
A systematic review of canine assisted psychotherapy. PLoS ONE, therapy in the treatment of substance dependence. Anthrozoös,
14(1), e0210761. 22(2), 137–148.
Linehan, M. (2014). DBT skills training manual. New Yok: Guilford. Winnicott, D. W. (1986). Transitional objects and transitional phenom-
Maujean, A., Pepping, C. A., & Kendall, E. (2015). A systematic review ena. In P. Buckley (Ed.), Essential papers on object relations (pp.
of randomized controlled trials of animal-assisted therapy on psy- 254–271). New York: New York University Press. https​://doi.
chosocial outcomes. Anthrozoös, 28(1), 23–36. org/10.1093/med:psych​/97801​90271​350.001.0001.
O’Haire, M. E. (2013). Animal-assisted intervention for autism spec- Workers, N. A. (2008). NASW code of ethics (guide to the everyday pro-
trum disorder: A systematic literature review. Journal of Autism and fessional conduct of social workers). Washington, DC: NASW.
Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1606–1622.
O’Haire, M. E., Guérin, N. A., & Kirkham, A. C. (2015). Animal-assisted Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
intervention for trauma: A systematic literature review. Frontiers in jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Psychology, 6, 1121.
Peretti, P. O. (1990). Elderly-animal friendship bonds. Social Behavior
and Personality: An International Journal, 18(1), 151–156.
Pet Partners. (n.d.). Terminology. Retrieved August 3, 2018, from https​:// Katherine Compitus  LCSW, MSEd, MA is a DSW Candidate and
petpa​rtner​s.org/learn​/termi​nolog​y/. adjunct lecturer at the Silver School of Social Work, New York Uni-
Scharff, D. E. (1996). Object relations theory and practice: An introduc- versity. Katherine specializes in the human–animal bond and clinical
tion. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield. social work practice.
Serpell, J. A., Coppinger, R., Fine, A. H., & Peralta, J. M. (2010). Wel-
fare considerations in therapy and assistance animals. Handbook on
animal-assisted therapy (pp. 481–503). New York: Academic Press.

13

You might also like