Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Correlates of Anxiety, Hostility,

Depression, and Psychosocial


Adjustment in Parents of NICU Infants
Lynn V. Doering, RN, DNSc
Debra K. Moser, RN, DNSc
Kathleen Dracup, RN, DNSc

T HE BIRTH OF AN INFANT IS A DIFFICULT TIME FOR ALL


parents. When an infant is born with health problems
and requires placement in a neonatal intensive care unit
father), sociodemographic characteristics (age, marital status,
ethnicity, employment status, and education), and psychoso-
cial factors (social support, family functioning, and perceived
(NICU), parents experience intense emotions. Fears regard- control) (Figure 1).
ing the infant’s future and the
parents’ ability to care for him,1,2 BACKGROUND
ABSTRACT
feelings of grief surrounding the The bir th of a premature
loss of a fully healthy child,3 and Purpose: The birth of a premature or critically ill infant can and/or critically ill infant is a
result in debilitating parental responses. This study identi-
stress from unfamiliar sights and time of emotional difficulty for
4 fies correlates of parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and
sounds in the NICU have all psychosocial adjustment so that nurses can identify parents
parents.7,8 Anxiety and depres-
been documented. likely to need special attention or intervention. sion, particularly in mothers of
Neonatal clinicians consider Design: An explanatory, correlational design was used. premature infants, have been
both the infant and the infant’s Sample: The study involved 469 parents (mothers = 299, studied as manifestations of
parents as a family unit requiring 65 percent; mean age 29.1 ± 6.8 years) of infants hospital- parental emotional distress. Not
nursing care. Nurses need to ized in five Level III NICUs. surprisingly, mothers of prema-
understand the forces that con- Main Outcome Variable: The main outcomes were those ture low birth weight infants are
tribute to emotional distress variables which correlated with the independent variables more anxious and depressed
(anxiety, hostility, and depres- and included: parent status (mother or father), ethnicity, than mothers of term infants.9
sion) and poor psychosocial employment status, and education. These negative emotional states
Results: Parents experienced high levels of anxiety, hostili-
adjustment in parents whose appear to be heightened just
ty, and depression. Poorer family functioning, lower levels
infants require NICU care. of social support, and lower perceived control were associ-
prior to the infant’s hospital
Although parental distress has ated with higher levels of anxiety, hostility, and depression discharge.1
been reported, 1,4–6 factors associ- Little data have been report-
and with poorer adjustment. Parental status (mother or
ated with its presence or absence father), ethnicity, employment status, and education were ed regarding hostility and psy-
remain unclear. Clinicians caring significantly related to parental responses. chosocial adjustment in this
for infants and their parents in population. Regarding hostility,
the NICU may therefore fail to researchers using the Multiple
identify parents at risk. Affect Adjective Checklist have reported equivocal data. In a
The purpose of the current study was to identify correlates sample of mothers of low-weight preterm infants, Brooten
of anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment in parents of
infants in the NICU. We hypothesized that correlates in a This research was funded by National Institute of Nursing Research, National
multivariate model would be parental relationship (mother or Institutes of Health (R01 NR02434).

Accepted for publication January 1999. Revised September 1999.

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
VOL. 19, NO. 5, AUGUST 2000 15
FIGURE 1 ■ Hypothesized relationships among variables. well as in parents of low birth weight infants.16
For example, Prudhoe and Peters found that par-
Parental Relationship
ents and grandparents of infants in the NICU
Mother/Father
used support networks, including social resources
and emotional support from others, to cope with
Sociodemographics
their infants’ illnesses.17 Increased emotional dis-
Age Anxiety
tress has been associated with decreased closeness
Marital Status Hostility
in important personal relationships, a reflection
Ethnicity Depression
of inadequate social support.18
Employment Status Adjustment
Education
Family Functioning
The effects of prior family functioning on
Psychosocial Factors parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjust-
Social Support ment in response to the birth of a low birth
Family Functioning weight infant have rarely been studied.19,20 Many
Perceived Control investigators have reported on the effects of the
birth of a healthy infant on family functioning,
however—particularly on the quality of the mari-
and colleagues found means for hostility that tal relationship. These investigators have found
were comparable to normative populations (e.g., that marital satisfaction diminishes following the
adolescent mothers).1 Others have found levels birth of a healthy infant.21 The birth of a prema-
of hostility above normative data. Regarding ture or critically ill infant affects the couple even
psychosocial adjustment, Doering and associates more negatively, particularly related to financial
reported poorer adjustment in mothers com- burden, family and social disruption, and person-
pared to fathers. 10 Although little is known al strain.22,23 For a couple with poor communica-
about these variables in parents of high-risk tion patterns or inadequate problem-solving
infants, research in related populations (e.g., skills, such a birth may be a crushing burden.
families of high-risk cardiac patients) has shown
hostility and adjustment to be important vari- Perceived Control
ables of interest.11,12 Therefore, these variables Perceived control as a moderator of parental
have been included in the current study. anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment in
the NICU has not been studied, but differences
Parental Relationship to the Infant in perceived control between mothers and
When both parents have been studied, moth- fathers have been documented. Mothers report-
ers have been found to be more distressed than ed having greater perceived control over the
fathers4,5,10,13,14 and to use different styles of infant’s recovery than fathers, although a moder-
coping.15 Mothers sought more social support ately high correlation between spouses’ percep-
and used escapist coping strategies more than tions of control was documented.15 One mother
fathers did, although the two groups of parents of a critically ill infant described eloquently her
used overall efforts to cope with the situation feelings of powerlessness while her infant was in
with the same frequency. Fathers tended to cope the NICU. This mother, who also happened to
by using minimization, an emotion-focused be an NICU nurse, described lack of control as
strategy involving the avoidance of anxiety.15 intimidating and associated it strongly with feel-
Differing sources of anxiety in mothers and ings of failure.3
fathers also have been reported.5 Mothers’ anxi-
ety was diffuse, while fathers were more anxious Sociodemographic Characteristics
about uncertain outcomes regarding the infant’s The relationship of sociodemographic charac-
health and about adopting the parental role. teristics to parental anxiety, hostility, depression,
and adjustment is less clear. Sociodemographic
Social Support characteristics were included in the current study
Social support has been identified as a media- because they are easily assessed by clinicians and
tor of anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjust- because previous work has indicated that factors
ment in the normal transition to parenthood, as such as ethnicity, parental age, income, marital

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
16 AUGUST 2000, VOL. 19, NO. 5
status, and education may mediate the buffering effects of ing in the household at the time of study enrollment was 4.1
social support, family functioning, or perceived control in (± 1.6). Participants constituted an ethnically diverse group.
related populations. 18,23–27 Older parents may be more One hundred ninety-nine (43 percent) identified themselves
mature and may have developed a greater repertoire of cop- as Hispanic-Latino, 155 (34 percent) as Caucasian, 45 (10
ing skills and/or material resources, for example. Of particu- percent) as African American, 42 (9 percent) as Asian, and 20
lar importance, little research has been done regarding the (4 percent) as other. Many reported that they did not work
relationship of ethnicity to parental anxiety, hostility, depres- outside the home (n = 193, 42 percent). Forty-seven percent
sion, and adjustment in the NICU setting. Most previous had annual household incomes <$40,000. Fourteen percent
work has been done in homogeneous, Anglo American sam- of participants reported incomes between $40,000 and
ples. Although ethnic differences in parental adjustment have $59,999, and 16 percent reported incomes ≥$60,000.
not been evaluated, Koniak-Griffin and colleagues have Twenty-three percent of participants declined to report
reported differences in social support across ethnic groups income level.
among pregnant adolescents, with African American mothers
reporting significantly less total functional social support than Procedures
either Caucasian or Hispanic women.28 The diversity and size Prior to the infant’s discharge from the NICU, parents
of the sample in the current study made it possible to consid- completed questionnaire packets in a private conference room
er the role of ethnicity as a factor potentially influencing adjacent to the NICU. They were instructed to complete
parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment in the questionnaires without consulting anyone else, including
NICU. spouses. Parents were allowed to choose English or Spanish
language packets. Most parents spent approximately 45 min-
METHODS utes completing the questionnaires. Bilingual registered
The current research was conducted as part of a prospec- nurses, serving as research assistants, were available to answer
tive, multisite study designed to compare four types of car- questions during testing.
diopulmonary resuscitation training for parents of high-risk
infants in the NICU.29 Institutional review board approval Instruments
was obtained at participating hospitals. Parents from five To ensure equivalency between languages, study instru-
NICUs in two university-affiliated and three community hos- ments were translated from English to Spanish and back-
pitals volunteered and were enrolled when they responded to translated by a second interpreter. Two instruments were used
signs posted in each unit inviting participation. All parents to measure parental emotional distress (anxiety, hostility, and
signed consent forms prior to the study. depression) and parental adjustment to the infant’s illness: the
Parents were enrolled if they lived in the infant’s home, Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist and the Psychosocial
they were literate in English or Spanish, and their infant was Adjustment to Illness Scale. Hypothesized correlates of
identified as being at high risk for a cardiac or respiratory parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment were
arrest. High-risk infant criteria were prematurity (<38 weeks); measured by three instruments. Social support was measured
low birth weight (<2,500 gm); documented episodes of by the Blumenthal Perceived Social Support Scale. Family
apnea or bradycardia; congenital cardiac, neurologic, or gas- functioning was measured by the general functioning subscale
trointestinal anomalies; respiratory distress syndrome; or of the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Perceived con-
bronchopulmonary dysplasia. trol was measured by participants’ self-rating of their degree
of control regarding their infant’s health.
Sample and Setting The Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist was used to
Four hundred and sixty-one parents volunteered and pro- assess emotional or psychological distress. It has demonstrat-
vided baseline data prior to receiving any intervention. These ed validity in the measures of anxiety, depression, and hostility
data constitute the subject of this article. They gave birth to associated with stressful or stress-alleviating conditions in a
349 infants, including 296 single births, 19 sets of twins, and wide range of populations.30,31 Higher scores correspond to
5 sets of triplets. The most common diagnoses requiring higher levels of distress. The highest possible scores for anxi-
NICU admission were low birth weight (n = 280, 80 per- ety, depression, and hostility are 21, 40, and 30, respectively.
cent) and prematurity (n = 249, 71 percent). Internal consistency scores using Cronbach’s alpha were 0.78
Most participants were mothers (n = 299, 65 percent) and (anxiety), 0.87 (depression), and 0.76 (hostility).
were married (n = 309, 67 percent). The sample included The Psychosocial Adjustment to Illness Scale (family
136 mother-father pairs (n = 272) as well as 163 mothers and version) is a forced-choice questionnaire of 46 items that
26 fathers who participated without their spouse or partner. assesses seven areas with relevance for adjustment to medical
Mean age was 29.1 (± 6.8) years, and mean years of educa- illness of a family member.32 Responses are rated on a four-
tion was 12.7 (± 3.6). The average number of individuals liv- point scale from zero, indicating no distress, to three, indicat-

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
VOL. 19, NO. 5, AUGUST 2000 17
ing extreme distress. Total scores range from 1 study. In contrast, locus of control instruments
to 138; lower scores reflect improved adjust- perform poorly in measuring control related to
ment. This scale has been validated in a variety specific health circumstances, probably because
of populations, including cardiac patients and of their general nature.38
their families and parents of children with can-
cer.30,33 Cronbach’s alpha was used to test relia- Analysis
bility in the current study. Internal consistency The demographic and psychosocial character-
scores for the subscales were (1) health care ori- istics of the sample were evaluated using statis-
entation, 0.53; (2) vocational environment, tics of central tendency. Significance was set at
0.67; (3) domestic environment, 0.63; (4) sexu- .05 for all analyses.
al relationships, 0.76; (5) extended family rela- To assess relationships among all the variables
tionships, 0.59; (6) social environment, 0.84; of interest, we constructed a correlation matrix
and (7) psychological distress, 0.85. Cronbach’s to test the relationship of each of the three
alpha for the total score was 0.89. potential correlates (social support, family func-
The Blumenthal Perceived Social Support tioning, and perceived control) with each of the
Scale was used to measure subjectively assessed four outcome measures of emotional and psy-
social support from three sources: family, friends, chosocial adjustment (anxiety, depression, hostil-
and significant others.34 It consists of 12 items ity, and adjustment to the infant’s illness). For
rated on a seven-point scale, with higher scores each outcome measure, a model of criterion
indicating greater support. Possible scores range variables was tested using stepwise multiple
from 12 to 84. Internal consistency and con- regression. Each model included parental rela-
struct validity have been established.35 Internal tionship to the infant (mother or father), psy-
consistency using Cronbach’s alpha in the cur- chosocial correlates, and sociodemographic vari-
rent study was 0.92. ables (age, marital status, ethnicity, employment
The McMaster Family Assessment Device outside the home, and years of education).
was used to evaluate six dimensions of family Variables were entered into each model equa-
functioning: (1) problem solving, (2) communi- tion in a hierarchical fashion, with parental rela-
cation, (3) roles, (4) affective involvement, (5) tionship and sociodemographic variables entered
affective responsiveness, and (6) behavior con- as a block first, followed by psychosocial vari-
trol.36 In addition to these subscales, a general ables. Parental relationship and sociodemo-
functioning scale assesses the overall graphic variables were entered first because they
health/pathology of the family. Respondents are not amenable to intervention and their early
indicate degree of agreement on a four-point entry into the model controls for their effect.
scale to 60 statements about families. Total Customary criteria for entry into and removal
scores range from one (healthy) to four from the model (.05 and .10, respectively) were
(unhealthy). Reliability and validity of the sub- used.39 Each model was assessed for collinearity
scales, as well as the general functioning scale, using a variance inflation factor (VIF) and condi-
have been established.37 For the general func- tion index. The VIF values for each model were
tioning scale, used in this study, internal consis- well below the critical value of 10. Likewise, the
tency using Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86. content index values were below the critical
Perceived control was measured by asking value of 30, which indicates that collinearity was
each parent to rate agreement with the following not a significant threat to the four models
statement: “In regard to my infant’s health tested.40 Finally, the use of a stepwise procedure
problem, I feel in control.” A five-point scale guards against collinearity because variables
was used, so that feelings of being “totally in already in the model are tested for removal at
control” received a value of one and feelings of each step.
being “not in control at all” were weighted with The fact that 272 of the 461 participants
a value of five. Responses to this single question were part of a pair in which both partners
have been shown to be related to feelings of per- participated in the study raised concern that this
ceived control regarding health in adult cardiac subsample would violate the assumption of inde-
patients and their spouses.30 This approach was pendence among subjects. A test of the correla-
selected because the question posed is directly tion between paired scores on the four outcome
related to the specific health circumstance under variables (e.g., maternal and paternal anxiety) for

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
18 AUGUST 2000, VOL. 19, NO. 5
TABLE 1 ■ Description of Variables 10.50 (± 4.11) were well above the normal range of 7 or less,
Variable Mean Standard Deviation Range with higher scores indicating worse anxiety and hostility.31
Anxiety 9.74 4.30 0.00–21.00 Mean depression scores of 15.87 (± 6.62) reflected moderate
Hostility 10.50 4.11 0.00–26.00 to severe clinical depression.41 Anxiety, hostility, and depres-
Depression 15.87 6.62 1.00–34.00 sion were more pervasive in these parents than in families of
Adjustment 32.64 16.00 0.00–81.00 high-risk cardiac patients42 or in earlier reports of mothers of
Social support 68.63 13.93 12.00–84.00 preterm infants.1 The measure of psychosocial adjustment
Family functioning 1.87 0.49 1.00–3.67 used in this study was designed so that lower scores reflect
Perceived control 2.62 1.10 1.00–5.00 better adjustment. Adjustment to the infant’s illness averaged
32.64 (± 16.00), with a possible range of 0 (best) to 138
(worst). These adjustment scores were better than those doc-
these 136 couples, however, demonstrated weak correlations umented in adult cardiac patients.42
(i.e., <.30) not powerful enough to justify use of a nested The relationships between the identified independent and
analysis. To further assure that the inclusion of parental pairs dependent variables were assessed using Pearson Product
did not violate the assumption of independence, we conduct- Moment Correlations (Table 2). The correlations were mod-
ed multivariate analyses for the paired and nonpaired partici- erate at best. Nonetheless, because of the complexity of the
pants separately. In these separate analyses, the independent relationships and because partial correlations in regression
correlates were identical to those of the whole sample, with may be different from first-order correlations, a conservative
one exception. In the nonpaired subsample, the parental rela- approach of including even modestly correlated variables in
tionship to infant (mother or father) was not correlated inde- the multivariate analysis was applied.39 This approach was
pendently with anxiety and adjustment, as it was for the used so that as many significant relationships as possible could
whole sample. This was probably because the whole sample be identified.
included more fathers (35 percent) than the nonpaired only The degree of correlation between social support and fam-
sample did (14 percent). The amounts of variance explained ily functioning (–0.458) reflected possible conceptual overlap
by the models for the nonpaired and paired subsamples were between the two measures. In fact, one of the three subscales
similar (i.e., <5 percent difference) to those for the whole of the social support scale measures support from family (in
sample. Therefore, only results for the whole sample are contrast to friends and significant others). In an attempt to
reported here. reduce overlap, the family subscale was eliminated from the
Because previous work suggests that factors influencing total score, and the two variables (social support and family
responses to the NICU hospitalization of an infant may differ functioning) were compared again using Pearson Product
between mothers and fathers,4,5,10,13,14 the interaction of the Moment Correlations. The result was similar to the first anal-
mother/father relationship with ethnicity and levels of social ysis using the total social support instrument (–0.425); there-
support, family functioning, and perceived control were also fore, the entire questionnaire was used in the multivariate
examined. The General Linear Model was used, and parents analyses.
were divided into high or low groups via media splits for The multivariate relationships of criterion variables to anxi-
social support, family functioning, and perceived control. Age ety, hostility, depression, and adjustment were modeled
and educational level were used as covariates. through stepwise multiple regression (Table 3). Variables
explained 17 to 29 percent of the total variance. Parental rela-
RESULTS tionship to the infant (mother or father) had no significant
Key variables of interest are summarized in Table 1. Mean interaction effects with social support, family functioning,
anxiety scores of 9.74 (± 4.30) and mean hostility scores of perceived control, or ethnicity.

TABLE 2 ■ Intercorrelation of Variables


Anxiety Hostility Depression Adjustment Social Support Family Functioning Perceived Control
Anxiety 1.0
Hostility 0.668* 1.0
Depression 0.807* 0.749* 1.0
Adjustment 0.538* 0.501* 0.606* 1.0
Social support –0.187* –0.276* –0.195* –0.343* 1.0
Family functioning 0.223* 0.307* 0.219* 0.297* 0.458* 1.0
Perceived control 0.289* 0.220* 0.281* 0.298* –0.072 0.25 1.0

* p <.01

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
VOL. 19, NO. 5, AUGUST 2000 19
TABLE 3 ■ Multiple Regression of Anxiety, Hostility, Depression, and Adjustment of Several sociodemographic correlates of
Parents with Infants in the NICU*
parental response were identified, and they were
(Step) Variable Change in R2 F Change Final beta somewhat different for the four outcome vari-
Anxietya ables studied. (1) Ethnicity was important in
(1) Mother vs father .023 10.6e .15e anxiety, hostility, depression, and psychosocial
(2) Hispanic vs all others .017 8.2e –.13e adjustment. Hispanic parents were less likely to
(3) African American vs all others .012 6.0f –.12f report high levels of anxiety, hostility, and
(4) Perceived control .062 32.1g .26g depression and poor psychosocial adjustment;
(5) Family functioning .053 28.8g .23g African Americans were less likely to report feel-
(6) Social support .010 5.4f –.11f ing anxious or depressed. Caucasian parents
Hostilityb were less likely to report feeling hostile. (2)
(1) Not employed vs employed .022 10.4e .15e Parents who were not working outside the
(2) Hispanic vs all others .013 6.0f –.11f home were more likely to report feeling hostile
(3) Caucasian vs all others .011 5.3f –.14f than parents who were employed. (3) Parents
(4) Family functioning .087 45.8g .30g with higher levels of education were more likely
(5) Perceived control .040 22.2g .21g to describe poorer psychosocial adjustment. Age
(6) Social support .019 10.9e –.16e did not enter into any of the multivariate equa-
Depressionc tions as a correlate of parental distress.
(1) Mother vs father .063 30.6g .25g
(2) Hispanic vs all others .037 18.7g –.29g DISCUSSION
(3) African American vs all others .008 4.1f –.09f Although our models included influences
(4) Family functioning .059 32.4g .25g commonly hypothesized to contribute to
(5) Perceived control .044 25.6g .22g parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjust-
(6) Social support .014 8.4e –.14e ment, the amount of variance explained by each
Adjustmentd model was relatively small (i.e., <30 percent).
(1) Mother vs father .046 21.9g .21g No single variable accounted for a large propor-
(2) Hispanic vs all others .034 16.7g –.18g tion of the variance. These results indicate the
(3) Years of education .012 6.0f .12f complexity of the emotional and psychosocial
(4) Social support .142 84.5g –.38g states for parents with an infant in the NICU
(5) Perceived control .038 23.9g .21g and support the need for further research.
(6) Family functioning .029 18.9g .20g
Parental Relationship to the Infant
* Degrees of freedom for analyses ranged from 1, 454 to 1, 459. Compared to fathers, mothers were more
a Total R2 = .166, F(6, 454) = 16.30, p <.001. likely to report higher levels of anxiety and
b Total R2 = .182, F(6, 454) = 18.09, p <.001. depression, along with poor adjustment, a find-
c Total R2 = .215, F(6, 454) = 22.02, p <.001. ing noted by other investigators.10,14 Miles and
d Total R2 = .291, F(6, 454) = 32.5, p <.001.
colleagues reported that, although both parents
e p <.005.
had significant anxiety, the source of anxiety was
f p <.05.
different for fathers and mothers, and mothers
g p <.001.
found the entire NICU experience and its after-
Parental relationship to the infant (mother or math more stressful than fathers. 5 Similarly,
father) was associated with anxiety, depression, Shields-Poe and Pinelli used the Parental
and adjustment. Specifically, mothers were likely Stressor Scale: NICU, to identify associations
to demonstrate higher anxiety, more depression, between situational, parental, and infant vari-
and poorer psychosocial adjustment than fathers. ables and sources of parental stress in the NICU.
All the hypothesized psychosocial factors of A mother’s stress regarding her interactions with
parental response across the four outcome vari- her infant was correlated with her age and with
ables were significant, although their individual the location where she first saw her infant, while
contribution to the explained variance was small. her stress regarding the infant’s appearance was
Lower levels of social support and perceived correlated with her marital status. For fathers,
control and poor family functioning were associ- unique correlates of stress related to interactions
ated with anxiety, hostility, depression, and poor with the infant were the gap between perceived
psychosocial adjustment in parents. and measured morbidity and frequency of atten-

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
20 AUGUST 2000, VOL. 19, NO. 5
dance at religious services. Paternal stress related to sights and related to hostility; ethnicity and educational attainment were
sounds of the NICU were correlated with timing of the first related to parental adjustment. These findings reinforce the
visit and discussion with a social worker.14 These differences need to identify sociodemographic factors that characterize a
in the reasons for parental stress may account for the differ- group of parents at high risk for negative emotional response
ences in parental anxiety, depression, and adjustment found in and poor adjustment after the birth of a premature or critical-
the current study. ly ill infant.
Ethnicity was associated with anxiety, hostility, depression,
Social Support and adjustment, but the relationships varied by ethnic group.
Lack of social support was associated with anxiety, hostility, Hispanic ethnicity was associated with more positive parental
depression, and poor adjustment. This finding corresponds to responses, specifically lower levels of anxiety, hostility, and
other reports that higher stress is associated with decreased depression and with higher levels of parental psychosocial
closeness in personal relationships.43 The finding is particular- adjustment. This finding differs from other reports in clinical
ly important for mothers. Women count on close personal populations outside the NICU,28,43 in which non-Caucasian
support from sources outside the marriage.43 When the hos- ethnicity was associated with more negative parental respons-
pitalization of a critically ill infant disrupts those sources of es. However, this is the first large study of Hispanic parents,
support or when mothers lack extramarital support, these many of whom did not speak English. The results of previous
mothers may be at greater risk for negative responses to their investigators who have studied English-speaking Hispanics
infant’s NICU hospitalization. may have been influenced by the acculturation that occurs as
immigrants adapt to a new country and face the challenges of
Family Functioning integrating two different cultures. Fluency in the dominant
Family functioning also explained a percentage of the vari- language, in this case English, is a well-accepted surrogate for
ance in anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment. acculturation.44 The role of social support, family function-
Although this finding is consistent with reports of diminished ing, and perceived control in unacculturated, non-English-
marital satisfaction during the transition to parenthood,21 it speaking Hispanics requires further study.
also highlights the need to consider other dimensions of fami- Although there were no correlations between any of the
ly functioning when assessing parents’ risk for negative emo- outcomes measures (anxiety, hostility, depression, or adjust-
tional or psychosocial responses. In addition to marital satis- ment) and age or marital status in the current study, other
faction, the family’s problem-solving abilities, communication investigators have reported that age and marital status may be
patterns, and role or behavior expectations may influence associated with maternal stress. In particular, age and marital
parental distress in the NICU. status have been reported as correlates of maternal stress
related to infant-mother interactions and infant’s appearance,
Perceived Control respectively.14 Differences in the samples of the two studies
An important contribution of the current study is the eval- may explain the difference in findings. The sample of the ear-
uation of perceived control as an aspect of the parental experi- lier study was homogeneous, including only English speakers,
ence in the NICU. The evaluation of perceived control is and greater percentages were married and college educated.
important because it has received little attention from clini- The difference in findings may also arise from the difference
cians and researchers to date and because, unlike locus of con- in outcome measures. Shields-Poe and Pinelli measured
trol, which is personality driven, perceived control is parental stress.14 The current study measured emotional states
amenable to nursing intervention.30 In the current study, low often considered to be responses to stress.
perceived control was associated with higher anxiety, hostility,
and depression and poorer adjustment. These findings high- Overlapping Nature of Dependent Variables
light the need for nurses to assess manifestations of low per- As reflected by the correlation matrix (see Table 2),
ceived control in parents and seek opportunities to minimize parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment are
it. Future research needs to focus on design and testing of related concepts. It is not surprising, therefore, that factors
interventions, such as parental involvement in decision mak- related to one variable are also related to others. Because each
ing, to enhance parents’ sense of control while their infants variable was considered as the dependent variable in separate
are in the NICU. analyses, however, separate sociodemographic and psychoso-
cial influences have been identified. Such information may be
Sociodemographics helpful for clinicians, who must identify parents who need
Several sociodemographic variables accounted for a por- special support or intervention while their infant is in the
tion of the explained variance in parental anxiety, hostility, NICU. Given the relatively modest nature of our findings,
depression, and adjustment. Ethnicity was related to anxiety however, clinicians must remember that individuals vary
and depression. Both ethnicity and employment status were greatly and that the prognostic value of specific correlates

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
VOL. 19, NO. 5, AUGUST 2000 21
must be considered in the context of each individual family or 3. Maroney D. 1994. Helping parents survive the emotional “roller
parental situation. coaster ride” in the newborn intensive care unit. Journal of
Perinatology 14(2): 131–133.
Limitations 4. Miles MS, Funk SG, and Carlson J. 1993. Parental stressor scale:
The study is limited by the use of a volunteer, convenience Neonatal intensive care unit. Nursing Research 42(3): 148–152.
sample. Parents were enrolled when they came to a meeting 5. Miles MS, Funk SG, and Kasper MA. 1992. The stress response
in response to signs posted in the NICU. Although no par- of mothers and fathers of preterm infants. Research in Nursing
and Health 15(4): 261–269.
ents who volunteered decided not to participate after coming
to the posted meeting, we cannot verify the representativeness 6. Redshaw ME, and Harris A. 1995. Maternal perceptions of
neonatal care. Acta Paediatrica 84(6): 593–598. (Published
of the sample. Parents who volunteered may have had differ-
erratum appears in Acta Paediatrica, 1995, 84[8]: 858.)
ent responses to their infants’ NICU hospitalization or had a
7. Harper R, et al. 1976. Observation on unrestricted prenatal con-
different set of resources than parents who did not volunteer.
tact with infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of
Given the nature of the study and the requirements of the
Pediatrics 89(3): 441–445.
institutional review boards, however, it was not possible to
8. Kaplan F, and Mason E. 1960. Maternal reactions to premature
recruit parents more purposefully or to obtain data on parents
birth viewed as an acute emotional disorder. Journal of
who elected not to volunteer. Orthopsychiatry 30: 539–552.
9. Gennaro S. 1996. Leave and employment in families of preterm
CONCLUSION low birthweight infants. Journal of Nursing Scholarship 28(3):
Further research is needed to distinguish among ethnic 193–198.
groups (especially Hispanic) characteristics that influence 10. Doering L, Dracup K, and Moser DK. 1999. Comparison of
parental anxiety, hostility, depression, and adjustment. Within psychosocial adjustment of mothers and fathers of high-risk
large ethnic groups, the role of acculturation in promoting or infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of Perinatology
hindering parental adjustment has not been explored and may 19(2): 132–137.
explain apparently conflicting findings to date. Also, further 11. Moser DK, Dracup KA, and Marsden C. 1993. Needs of recov-
research is required to understand the role of perceived con- ering cardiac patients and their spouses: Compared views.
trol as it influences parental adjustment in the NICU setting. International Journal of Nursing Studies 30(2): 105–114.
Perceived control may be an important variable mediating the 12. Dracup K, et al. 1994. Is cardiopulmonary resuscitation training
psychosocial response of parents with a critically ill infant in deleterious for family members of cardiac patients? American
the NICU, but it is not clear if such perceived control is a Journal of Public Health 84(1): 116–118.
personality characteristic or if it can be enhanced by interven- 13. Miles MS. 1989. Parents of critically ill premature infants:
tions employed by NICU staff. Sources of stress. Critical Care Nursing Quarterly 12(3): 69–74.
These findings suggest an initial set of variables that clini- 14. Shields-Poe D, and Pinelli J. 1997. Variables associated with
cians may find useful in assessing parents in the NICU. parental stress in neonatal intensive care units. Neonatal Network
Nurses should consider both sociodemographic characteristics 16(1): 29–37.
and psychosocial resources in assessing parents’ vulnerability 15. Affleck G, Tennen H, and Rowe J. 1990. Mothers, fathers, and
to anxiety, hostility, depression, and poor adjustment. the crisis of newborn intensive care. Infant Mental Health
Psychosocial resources and sociodemographic characteristics Journal 11(1): 12–25.
may prove useful in identifying individuals or subgroups that 16. Crnic K, and Greenberg M. 1987. Maternal stress, social support
need more intensive follow-up and in developing interven- and coping: Influences on the early mother-infant relationship.
tions to facilitate adjustment. Parents may need assistance in In Research on Support for Parents and Infants in the Postnatal
Period, Boukydis CFZ, ed. Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex, 25–40.
identifying and using supportive relationships and in finding
ways in which they can feel more in control during the period 17. Prudhoe CM, and Peters DL. 1995. Social support of parents
and grandparents in the neonatal intensive care unit. Pediatric
when their infant is hospitalized in the NICU. Finally, clini-
Nursing 21(2): 140–146.
cians should be aware that there are great differences in indi-
18. Yogman MW, Kindlon D, and Earls F. 1995. Father involvement
vidual responses to the NICU experience and should conduct
and cognitive/behavioral outcomes of preterm infants. Journal
well-rounded individualized assessments of parental needs.
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
34(1): 58–66.
REFERENCES 19. Ahmann E, Wulff L, and Meny RG. 1992. Home apnea moni-
1. Brooten D, et al. 1988. Anxiety, depression, and hostility in
toring and disruptions in family life: A multidimensional con-
mothers of preterm infants. Nursing Research 37(4): 213–216.
trolled study. American Journal of Public Health 82(5):
2. Gennaro S. 1988. Postpartal anxiety and depression in mothers 719–722.
of term and preterm infants. Nursing Research 37(2): 82–85.

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
22 AUGUST 2000, VOL. 19, NO. 5
20. Crnic K, et al. 1984. Maternal stress and social support: Effects 38. Mahler HIM, and Kulick JA. 1990. Preferences for health care
on the mother-infant relationship from birth to eighteen involvement, perceived control and surgical recovery: A prospec-
months. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 54(2): 224–235. tive study. Social Science Medicine 31(7): 743–751.
21. Tomlinson PS. 1987. Spousal differences in marital satisfaction 39. Munro BH. 1997. Regression. In Statistical Methods for Health
during transition to parenthood. Nursing Research 36(4): Care Research, 3rd ed., Munro BH, ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott
239–243. Williams & Wilkins, 246–286.
22. Cronin CMG, et al. 1995. The impact of very low-birth-weight 40. von Eye A, and Schuster C. 1998. Regression Analysis for Social
infants on the family is long lasting. A matched control study. Sciences. San Diego, California: Academic Press, 133–142.
Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine 149(2): 151–158. 41. Vernon SW, et al. 1997. Correlates of psychologic distress in
23. Vaux A. 1985. Variations in social support associated with gen- colorectal cancer patients undergoing genetic testing for
der, ethnicity/race, and age. Journal of Social Issues 41: 89–110. hereditary colon cancer. Health Psychology 16(1): 73–86.
24. Eaton WW, and Kessler LG. 1981. Rates of symptoms of depres- 42. Dracup K, et al. 1997. The psychological consequences of car-
sion in a national sample. American Journal of Epidemiology diopulmonary resuscitation training for family members of
114(4): 528–538. patients at risk for sudden death. American Journal of Public
25. Hall LA. 1990. Prevalence and correlates of depressive symp- Health 87(9): 1434–1439.
toms in mothers of young children. Public Health Nursing 7(2): 43. Coffman S, Levitt MJ, and Guacci-Franco N. 1993. Mothers’
71–79. stress and close relationships: Correlates with infant health status.
26. Weinraub M, and Wolf B. 1983. Effects of stress and social sup- Pediatric Nursing 19(2): 135–140.
ports on mother-child interactions in single- and two-parent 44. Jones EG, and Kay M. 1992. Instrumentation in cross-cultural
families. Child Development 54(5): 1297–1311. research. Nursing Research 41(3): 186–188.
27. Monset-Couchard M, de Bethmann O, and Kastler B. 1996.
Mid- and long-term outcome of 89 premature infants weighing About the Author
less than 1,000 g at birth, all appropriate for gestational age. Lynn Doering holds an MN and DNSc from UCLA, where she is
Biology of the Neonate 70(6): 328–338. currently an assistant professor in acute care. Her research program
28. Koniak-Griffin D, Lominska S, and Brecht ML. 1993. Social focuses on physiologic and psychosocial adaptation of high-risk patients
support during adolescent pregnancy: A comparison of three with cardiac disease. As both a clinician and a researcher, she has worked
ethnic groups. Journal of Adolescence 16(1): 43–56. extensively with patients at high risk for cardiac events and their families,
29. Dracup K, et al. 1998. A comparison of cardiopulmonary resus- including both adults and infants. Her current work involves the study
citation training methods for parents of infants at high risk for of depression in women recovering from cardiac events and the role of
cardiopulmonary arrest. Annals of Emergency Medicine 32(2): family members in treatment-seeking behavior for cardiac symptoms.
170–177. Debra Moser holds an MN and DNSc from UCLA and is an associ-
30. Moser DK, and Dracup K. 1995. Psychosocial recovery from a ate professor at the College of Nursing, Ohio State University. Her
cardiac event: The influence of perceived control. Heart & Lung research program includes biobehavioral study of cardiac patients, espe-
24(4): 273–280. cially those with acute myocardial infarction and those with heart fail-
ure. Her current work focuses on testing nurse-managed community care
31. Zuckerman M, Lubin B, and Rinck CM. 1983. Construction of
in heart failure patients.
new scales for the multiple affect adjective check list. Behavioral
Kathleen Dracup holds an MN from UCLA and a DNSc from
Assessment 5: 119–129.
UCSF, where she is currently professor and dean of the School of Nursing.
32. Morrow GR, Chiarello RJ, and Derogatis LR. 1978. A new scale Dr. Dracup is internationally known for her work in psychosocial adjust-
for assessing patients’ psychosocial adjustment to medical illness. ment in cardiac patients. Her current research includes a multidisci-
Psychological Medicine 8(4): 605–610. plinary study of psychosocial, immunologic, autonomic, costs and quality
33. Dracup K, et al. 1997. The psychosocial consequences of CPR of life outcomes of exercise in heart failure patients, and an international
training for parents of infants at risk for sudden death. study of a nurse-directed intervention to decrease delay in treatment-seek-
Circulation 96(8): I-365. ing behavior in patients with symptoms of acute myocardial infarction.
34. Blumenthal JA, et al. 1987. Social support type A behavior and The authors wish to thank Mary Lynn Brecht, PhD, for her sugges-
coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine 49(4): 331–340. tions related to statistical analysis.
35. Zimet GD, et al. 1988. The multidimensional scale of perceived For further information, please contact:
social support. Journal of Personality Assessment 52(1): 30–41. Lynn V. Doering, RN, DNSc
36. Epstein NB, Baldwin LM, and Bishop DS. 1983. The McMaster Assistant Professor
family assessment device. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy UCLA School of Nursing
9(2): 171–180. PO Box 956918
37. Byles J, et al. 1988. Ontario Child Health Study: Reliability and Los Angeles, CA 90095-6918
validity of the general functioning subscale of the McMaster fam- (310) 825-4890
ily assessment device. Family Process 27(1): 97–104. FAX (310) 206-7433
E-mail: ldoering@sonnet.ucla.edu

N E O N ATA L N E T WO R K
VOL. 19, NO. 5, AUGUST 2000 23

You might also like