Gershon (2002) - ADHD Gender-Diff

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

A Meta-Analytic Review of

Gender Differences in ADHD


J. Gershon

The present study examined gender differences in ADHD through a meta-analysis. Effect size estimates for the primary symptoms
and correlates of ADHD were calculated in an attempt to replicate and extend a previous meta-analysis on gender differences in the
disorder. Relatively lenient inclusion criteria were used in order to maximize the number of studies included in the effect sizes.

The results indicated that in comparison to ADHD boys, ADHD girls had lower ratings on hyperactivity, inattention, impulsivity, and
externalizing problems. In addition, ADHD girls had greater intellectual impairments and more internalizing problems than ADHD
boys. Overall, the results of the current meta-analysis indicated general agreement with the previous meta-analysis. The clinical
implications of these gender differences and future research considerations are discussed.

With prevalence estimates ofADHD usually ranging from ADHD females represent a ‘‘silent minority&dquo; as a result of
3% to 5% (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), ADHD their manifestation of the disordcr (Berry, Shaywitz, &
clearly affects a large segment of the general population. Shaywitz, 1985). Specifically, since ADHD females display
The study ofADHD has generated considerable literature less disruptive behavior than ADHD males (Achenbach,
on almost every facet of the disorder, including possible 1991) they arc more likely to be ignored if they only display
causes, diagnosis, and treatment. Despite its reputation as inattentive bchaviors (Gaub & Carlson,1997). ADHD clinics
one of the most comprehensively studied childhood mental typically receive a higher number of referrals for ADHD
disorders (Arnold, 1996; Barkley, 1998), much of the males due to iheir greater likelihood of disruption in settings
research on ADHD has tended to focus on an overly such as school. Thus, clinically referred females likely
homogeneous sample of subjects: clinically-referred exhibit particularly disruptive behavior but may not
Caucasian males. The ability of ADHD researchers to represent most felnales with the disorder. An understanding
generalize findings to females and the largcr population is of gender differences in ADHD is critical for disseminating
severcly restricted by this limited subject pool. the manifestation and identification of the disorder.
Ftirtlieniiore, the lack of information on ADHD females has
led to problems with the identification and treatment of In an cffort to summarize the existing literature and enhance
these individuals. the understanding of gender differences in ADHD, Gaub
and Carlson (1997) conducted the first meta-analysis on
The recruitment of ADHD females has proven to be a gender differences in ADHD. They found that in
foriuidable task since far fewer females than males are comparison to ADHD bo3~s, ADHD girls had greater
evaluated at clinics for ADHD (Arnold, 1996). intellectual impairments, lower ratings on li)pcmcti%i(3,, and
Epidemiological studies of ADHD have estimated that lower ratings of externalizing and internalizing problems.
gender differences in the disorder range from 3:1 (Szatmari, Gaub and Carlson discovered, however, that some of these
Offord, & Boylc, 1989), whereas reports from clinically- gender differences could be accounted for by moderator
referred samples have estimated a ratio closer to 9:11 variables such as referral source (clinic ~-s. community
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994). This discrepancy samples) or tlle diagnostic system used by the researcher.
in prevalence suggests that clinical settings treat far fewer
ADHD females than males (Gomez, Harvey, Quick, Scharer, The meta-analysis by Gaub and Carlson (1997) provided
& Harris, 1999). Based on studies of community samples, a tlie first quantitative summary on gender differences in
ADHD. A number of methodological problems, however,
large number of females and males should meet criteria for
ADHD, yet females are rarely identified in clinical studies, may have affected their conclusions. First, the authors used
This lack of information on ADHD females has poteniially stringent inclusion criteria that resulted in a total of only 18
serious public health implications since long-tenn problems possible studies for the analysis. Furthermore, w ithin each
for the disorder include social, academic, and emotional of the dependent variables that they examined for sex
difficulties (Arnold, 1996; McGee & Feehan, 1991). differences, their analyses contained even fewer studies

143
(range from 1-12 studies). Second, Gaub and Carlson ADHD researchers by e-mail to locate additional
included only one unpublished study in their anal3-scs. unpublished material.
The exclusion of unpublished data may have resulted in a
&dquo;file-draNN-ef’ problem (Roscnthal, 1991). Since negative Inclusion Criteria
findings are published less often than positive findings, Studies in the current analysis had to meet all of the
inclusion of unpublished data might alter the magnitude following inclusion critcria:
and/or direction of the effect size. Finally, although the
meta-analysis was published in 1997, all of the included 1. Subjects had to receive a diagnosis of ADHD with an
studies were conducted prior to 1992. Since that time, the explanation of how the diagnosis was made.
diagnostic critcria have changed with the publication of
DS_l t IT ; and enhanced efforts have been made to include 2. The study had to directly compare ADHD males and
females in studies ofADHD (Anioid, 1996). females on relevant variables.

The current analysis was thus undertaken for two primary 3. Sufficient data were provided in the text or by the
reasons. First, while relatively little data exist on gender autlior(s) to calculate an effect size.
differences in ADHD, an updated quantitative sununay
ia-otild aid ADHD researchers and clinicians in their effort In order to determine if inclusion criteria influenced the
to understand, diagnose, and treat affected individuals. A previous results, the current analysis contained specifically
less stringent criteria than those in the Gaub and Carlson
seconday rationale for thc current analysis was to compare
the results to the previous meta-analysis on gender (1997) paper. The inclusion criteria from the previous mcta-
diffcrcnccs in ADHD. In particular, the currcnt meta- analysis not used in this study consisted of a minimum
analysis would assess methodological changes, such as sample size of 10 subjects per group, subjects aged 13 and
modifications in inclusion criteria. Also, the addition of younger, and a measured IQ greater than 80. The current
more recent studies on gender differences in ADHD might analysis treated the Gaub and Carlson inclusion criteria in
have implications regarding the findings and conclusions two nianners. First, the current analysis corrected all effect

of the Gaub and Carlson (1997) study. Anumbcr of potential sizes by a weighting factor for sample size. Second, whether
or not IQ was assessed and the age of subjects where
moderators were selected for examination in order to
determine if these variables affected tlie magnitude and/or analyzed as potential moderator variables.
direction of the effect sizes for gender differences in ADHD
The search strategy yielded a total of 120 potential articles
(Baron & Kenny, i 98G). These moderator variables include for the analyses. Thirt3--eight articles provided sufficient
sex of the first author, publication status, referral source,
data for inclusion in thc analyses. From these articles, 13
rater, whether IQ was assessed, age of subjects, diagnostic
were included in the previous review (Barkley, 1989; Befera
system used, and inclusion in the previous meta-analysis.
These moderator variables are discussed in more detail & Barhle3; 1985; Bcrry et al., 1985; Breen, 1989; Breen &
bclow. Altepeter, 1990; Breen & Barkley, 1988; Brown, Madan-
Swain, & Baldwin, 1991; deHaas, 1986; Horn, NVigiier, &

Ialoligo, 1989; Kashani, Chapel, Ellis, & Shekim, 1979;


Method Pelham & Bender, 1982; Pelham, Walker, Sturges, & Hoza,
1989; Thomeer, 1996), and 25 were additional studies,

Search Strategy
The author used several means to identify relevant reports (Ackerman, Dykman, & Oglcsby, 1983; Arcia & Conners,
that examined ADHD and gender differences. First, 1998; Bicdcnnan et al., 1994; Carlson, TanU1l, & Gaub, 1997;
Dunn & Shapiro, 1999; Gabel, Schmitz, & Fillkcr, 1 996 ; HaBB 11,
computerized databases such as PsychINFO, Medline, and
Dissertation Abstracts International were searched using 1979; James & Taylor,1990; Katz, Goldstein, & Gcckle,1998;
the keywords .4.M/D./1DD, and /~~/’~!e~ crossed w ith Mannuzza & Gittelman, 1984; McGee, fVilliams, & Silva,
the keywords Ser Differences and Gender Differences. 1987; Miller, Pales, & Stewart,1973; Nada-Raja et al., 1997;
Second, the author obtained and examined all articles used Pascualvaca, 1989; Ramircz & Shapiro, 1998; Schaughcncy,
in the Gaub and Carlson (1997) meta-analysis. In order to McGee, Raja, Feehan, & Silva, 1994; Schireson, 1989;
find potentially relevant research omitted from the computer Scltuerltolz, Singer, & Dencl:Ia, 1998; Sharp et al., 1999;
search, the author also examined all reference sections from Shenbcrgcr,199~; Silverthom, Frick, Kupcr, & Ott, 1996;
the obtained studies. Finally, the author contacted major Wiedenhoff, 1993).

144
In addition, two research reports conducted multiple Tests for Homogeneity of Effect Sizes: The assumption of
studies under the mbric of a larger study. Specifically, homogeneity underlying the effect size estimate suggests
Pascualvaca (1989) and Ramirez and Shapiro (1998) that all iiidinidtial effect sizes derivc from a single population.
examined gender differences in ADHD with several This assumption was tested for all significant cffect sizcs
independent samples within a larger study. Tlierefore, the with the 0 statistic. If this value excecded the a priori critical
current analysis treated these data as separate studies, as value (a = .05), then the effect size was considered
deemed appropriate by Hedges and Becker (1986). All other homogeneous and the samples were examined for possible
studies contributed only one cffcct size to each dependent moderator variables. The potential moderator variables
variable. Further study characteristics are available upon examined included sex of the first author, publication status,
request. referral source, rater, whether IQ was assessed, age of
subjects, diagnostic system used, and inclusion in the
Statistical Analyses previous meta-analysis. These moderator variables were
chosen because they reflect methodological differences
Calculation of Effect Sizes between the two meta-analyses. Unfortunatel3~, other
In this
study, the effect sizes examined the magnitude and potential moderator variables such as comorbid diagnoscs
direction of the differences between ADHD macs and or racial characteristics of subjects could not be analyzed
females. For these analyses, the unbiased estin~ator d because such infonnation was rarely avalable in the existing
(Hedges & Olkin, 1985) was chosen for the calculation of literature. The effect sizes were regressed onto each
effect sizes in the current analysis since it corrects for a contrast using weighted least squares regression analyses
discrepancy between the sample and population effect (Hedges & Olkin, 1985), and the equations were weighted
sizes. For the present analyses, positive effect sizes by sample size. The moderator variables are described in
represented higlrer mean scorcs for males. Thus, positive more detail below. -

effect sizes on primary symptoms represented greater


impairment for males, whereas positive effect sizes on Sex of First Author
variables such as Full Scale IQ represented stronger
The purpose of examining sex of first author as a moderator
pcrfonnancc for ADHD males. variable was to mle out any potcntial gender biases of then
researchers. Eagley (I98G) has noted that gcnder differences
Effect sizes were calculated using a number of software
researcli is vulnerable to a number of potential biases,
programs and by hand, according to formulas provided by
including author biases.
-

Hedges and Olkin (1985), Hedges and Bcckcr (1986), and


Rosenthal (1991). If means and standard deviations were
not provided in thc text or by the author(s), effect sizes Publication Status
were estimated from t-N,alues, /?-~~alues, or F-values The meta-analysis included combinations ofpublished and
according to fonnulas provided by Rosenthal (1991,1994). unpublished studies. All of the unpublished data in tlic
In order to determine the significance of the unbiased effect analyses were either dissertations or theses not published
sizes, a 95% confidence internal was calculated. If a for unknown reasons. Smith (1980) has reported that
confidence interval did not contain zero, ten the effcct publislicd studies tend to have higher effcct sizes than
size was considered significant. unpublished studies. Therefore, exclusion of unpublished
data can lead to a potential bias in effect sizes.
Combining Effect Sizes: The current analysis calculated
unweighted and weighted grand effect sizes for each Referral Source
dependent variable of interest, which included primary Gaub and Carlson ( 199’7) found that some of the significant
ADHD symptoms, intcrnalizing/extcrnalizing problems, gender differences in their study could be accounted for
intelligence, academic achieveinent, neurops5-chological by referral source (i.e., clinic or community samples), In
fiunctioning, and social functioning. The w-eighted effect general, children who were assessed from clinical samples
sizes provided greater value to studies that used larger tended to have much more impairment than community
sample sizes, whicli tends to decrease the variance of the samples. Referral source allowed for the examination of
effect size estimator (Hedgcs & Olkin, 1985) and provides gender differences based on ascertainment site of the
more reliable and valid results. As a result, only the weighted
subjects.
effect sizes estimates are reported.

145
Rater and impulsmty (d+ .22, p < .05). Significant gender
p < .0~),
In many of the studics on ADHD, both parents and teachers differences in ADHD also emerged for measures of
rated subjects on symptoms of ADHD. Since differences comorbid conditions. Specifically, ADHD females
were expected in thesc ratings, the rater (parent or teaclier) manifested significantly less externalizing problems
was examined as a potential moderator variable. (d+= .21, p < .05) and significantly more internalizing
problems (d+ = -.12, p < .05) than ADHD males.
IQAssessed
The previous meta-analysis (Gaub & Carlson, 1997) used Significant gender differences in ADHD also emerged for
measures of intellectual functioning. ADHD females
an IQ of greater than 80 as an inclusion criterion. Since cut-
off scores were specifically removed for the current performed worse than ADHD males on Full Scale IQ
analysis, it was important to determine if differences existed (d+ .27, p < .Oa) and Verbal IQ (d+ .37,p < .0~). No gender
difference found for Performance IQ. In addition, no
was
between those studies that assessed for and those that
did not assess for IQ. significant gender differences emerged on measures of
academic achievement, ncuropsychological or social

functioning.
Age L

The previous analysis (Gaub & Carlson, 1997) only Ratings of ADHD primary symptoms and comorbid
examined children. For the current analysis, no age conditions were analyzed separately by parent and teachers
restrictions were placed on the studies. This moderator (see Table 2). ADHD females were rated as less hyperactive
variable compared children to adolescents and adults. than ADHD males based on the weighted mean effect size
by parents (d+ .1G, p < .05) and by teacliers (d,= .36,
Diagnostic System ’

p < .05). On ratings of inattention, only teacher ratings


In order to determine if the diagnostic criterion moderated showed a significant gender difference, rating ADHD
the findings, the data were analyzed for diagnostic system females as less inattentive than ADHD males (d~= .18,
as a potential moderator. Specifically, the data were coded p < .OS). Teachers also rated ADHD females as manifesting
for DSII III, DS.BI-IlI-R, or DS.1I I1’sysfems, if reported. fewer externalizing problems than ADHD males (d+= .19,
None of the studies from the previous meta-anal3~sis (Gaub p < .05).
& Carlson, 1997) were based on D£1 f-Ii ’ crileria.
Tests of Homogeneity and Analyses of
Previous Meta-Analysis Moderator Variables
Another important methodological question is w hether the Referral Source: Tlvc results of the moderator analyses for
studies included in the current analysis were somehow referral source are shown in Table 3. Thc findings indicated
different from those included in the previous mcta-analysis that referral source moderated hyperactivity (b -.30, =

(Gaub & Carlson,1997). CI = .03, p < ,05), reading achievement (b .25, C7= .23,
=

p < .05), parent ratings of hyperactivity (b -.33, CI = .07,


=

Estimation of the &dquo;Fail-Safe N&dquo; p < .0~), teacher ratings of li3-peractiN,it3~ (b -I ~, CI = .03,
=

An extensive search was conducted to obtain published p < .05), parent ratings of inattentiveness (b = -.38, CI = .25,
p < .05), and teacher ratings of inattentin-cness (b -.36,
=
and unpublished studies for inclusion in the current
analysis. Despite this effort, it is possible that additional CI = .03, p < .0~). Effect sizes were separated by clinic and
data, particularly unpublished, exist in the field, and, community sample. Using community samples only, ADHD
therefore, included studies may not represent the entire females were rated as significantly less impaired tltanADHD
population of studies conducted (Rosenthal, 1991). Thus, males on all of the dependent measures above. On parent
the ‘‘Fail-Safe N&dquo; statistic (01BB1n, 1983) was calculated to ratings of inattentiveness, however, clinic samples showed
represent the number of studies that would be necessary significantly more impairment for ADHD females than
to make a significant estimation of effect size insignificant. ADHD males.

Previous Meta-Analysis: The results of the moderator


Results analyses whether studies were included in the previous
for
The results of the current meta-anatysis are shown in meta-anal3-sis are shown in Table 4. Previous meta-anal3-sis
Table 1. Significant gender differences emerged for all moderated hyperactivity (b = .22, CI = .08, p < .OS), reading
primary symptoms ofADHD. ADHD females were rated as achievement (b = .23, CI = ,15, p < .05), parent ratings of
significantly less impaired than ADHD males on hyperactivity (b = .24, CI = .12, p < .05), parent ratings of
hyperactivity (d+= .29, p < .05), inattention (d.= .23,

146
Table 1. Effect Sizes of Primary Symptoms and Correlates of Gender Differences in ADHD

Note: ~, weighted mean effect size; CI confidence interval. k number of studies; Q


= = = =
test of homogeneity; df =
degrees of freedom.
*p < .0~..

Table 2. Effect Sizes of Primary Symptoms and Correlates of Gender Differences in ADHD for Parent andTeacher Ratings

Note: d =
weighted mean effect size; C/ =
confidence interval; k =
number of studies, 0 =
test of f~omogeneity; df =
degrees of freedom.
’&dquo;p < .05.

147
Table 3. Investigation of Referral Source as a Moderator

Note: N =
number of studies: b =
regression coeffcient; da =
weighted mean effect size; CI =
confidence interval; G =
test of homogeneity; *p < .05.

and teacher ratings


extertializing (b = .34, C/= .08,~ < .05), of externalizing problems (b = .09, CI = .07, p < .05). Effect
of inattentiveness (b = .1 ~, CI = .11, p .05). Effect sizes
< sizes were separated by publication status. Based on the
were separated by whether studies were included in the unpublished studies, ADHD females demonstrated
previous meta-analysis. Based on studies not included in significantly less impairment than ADHD males on
the previous meta-analysis, ADHD females were rated as hyperactivity and teacher ratings of hyperactivity.
significantly less impaired than ADHD males on
hyperactivity, parent ratings of hyperactivity, and parent IQ Assessed: Whether or not IQ was assessed moderated
ratings of inattentiveness. hyperactivity (b = -.24, CI = .03, p < ,05), parent ratings of
lypcractivity (b -.27, CI = .07, p < .05), teacher ratings of
=

Sex of First Author: The findings indicated that sex of first hyperactivity (b = -.33, CI = .03, p < .05), and teacher ratings
author significantly moderated parent ratings of of inattentiveness (b -.15, CI = .03, p < .05). Effect sizes
=

hyperactivity (b = .26, CI = .09, p < .05), teacher ratings of were separated by whether or not IQ was assessed. BVhen

hyperactivity (b -.19, C7=. .13, p < .05), and parent ratings


=
IQ was assessed, ADHD females were rated as significantly
of externalizing problems (b .46, (;1 = .08, p < ,05), Effect
=
less impaired than ADHD males on hyperactivity, teaclier
sizes were calculated separately for male and female authors. ratings of hyperactivity, and teacher ratings of
When the first author was female, teacher’ ratings on iiiatteiitin-ciiess.
hyperactivity were significantly lower for ADHD females
than males. Diagnostic System: The diagnostic system moderated math
achievement (b = -.27, CI = .18, p < ,05), teacher ratings of
Publication Status: Thc findings indicated that publication inattention (b -.09, CI = .07, p < .05), and teaclier ratings
=

status moderated hyperactivity (b = , I7, CI = .03, p < .05), of externalizing (b =.12, CV= .05,/? < .OS). Effect sizes were
math achievement (b -.30, CI = .20, p < .05), reading
=
separated by the diagnostic system used. Based on the
achievement (b -.22, Cl .21, p < .0~), teacher ratings of
= =
DS.1I Il ; ADHD females were rated as significantly less
hyperactivity (b ~ 19, CI = .05, p < .0~), and tcacher ratings impaired than ADHD males on teacher ratings of
inattentiveness.

148
Table 4. Investigation of Inclusion in Previous Meta-Analysis as a Moderator

Note: N -- number of studies: b =


regression coefficient; d, =
weighted mean effect size: Cl =
confidence interval; 0 =
test of homogeneity; ‘p~.05.

Age of Subjects: Age of subjects moderated lyperactivity Discussion .

(b .08, CI = ,04, p < .05), math achievement (b = .22,


=
The results of the current analysis indicated general
CI = .18, p < .05), reading achievement (b = .28, CI = ,11,
agreement with the Gaub and Carlson (1997) meta-analysis
p < .05), parent ratings of hyperactivity (b -.22, CI = ,05,
=
and demonstrated that the effect sizes were not influenced
p < .05), tcacher ratings of hyperactivity (b = .29, CI = .05, by methodological changes in the inclusion criteria. The
p < .05), and parent ratings of cxtcrnalizing problems rcsults suggested that ADHD females manifested fewer
(b -.20, CI = .05, p < .05). Effect sizes werc separated by
=

primary symptoms and extcnializitig problems than male.


the age of subjects (child YS, adolcscent/adult). For child In contrast, on related problems, such as intellectual
subjects, ADHD females were rated as significantly less functioning, ADHD females fared worse than their male
impaired than ADHD males on hyperactivity and teacher counterparts. Of particular interest, and the primary
ratings of hyperactivity. In addition, when the subjects difference between the current analysis and the Gaub and
were adolescents or adults, ADHD females were also rated Carlson study, was the finding tliat ADHD females were
as significantly less impaired than ADHD males on parent rated as higher on internalizing problems than males,
ratings of hyperactivity. suggesting that comorbid conditions such as depression
and anxiety may be more problematic for ADHD females.
Rater: Rater moderated only lypcractivity (b = .11, CI = .07,
p < .05). Effect sizes were separated by parent or teacher ‘Vhen effect sizes were separated by teacher and parent
ratings. Based on teacher ratings, ADHD females were rated ratings, a number of other gender differences emerged.
as significantly less impaired than ADHD males on
ADHD females were rated as less lyperactive according
hyperactivity. to parent and teacher reports. In contrast, ADHD females
were rated as less inattentive and displayed fewer

externalizing problems according to teachers only, The


results suggest that parent and teacher ratings generally

149
disagree on some of the primary symptoms and comorbid Unfortunately, the data did not permit the
existing
conditions, and teachers rate ADHD males as significantly examination of reason for referral or comorbid conditions
more impaired than ADHD females in comparison to parent such as LD.
reports. Such differences in ratings may indicate a ‘’halo
effect&dquo; whereby teachers overly attend to disruptive The moderator analyses indicated that a number of
behaviors and ignorc inattentive behaviors (Abikoff, variables might account for significant gender differences
for some of the dependent variables. As expected, rcferral
Courtncy, Pelham, & Koplewicz, 1993). This potential bias
source moderated a number of variables, which suggests
might contribute to the underidcntification of affected
females, particularly in the classroom. that clinically referred samples tended to manifest more
scvere symptomatology than community samples. Many

Alternatively, the data could also suggest that teachers of the variables examined significantly moderated
over-recognize ADHD males and correctly identify their hyperactivity, parent or teacher ratings of hy-peracti~-it5; or
female counterparts. Tlis interpretation would suggest that parent and teacher ratings of externalizing problems. These
there is bias by parents and not teachers. Based on the ratings were primarily conducted by rating scales and may
available data, it is not clear which interpretation is more indicate a gender bias for disruptive behaviors on these
justified. Since a number of variables significantly measures.
moderated all of these significant gender differences,
indicating that these dependent measures are complex and An unexpected finding einerged from the moderator
influenced by numerous factors, these results nuist be analyses. Sex offirst author significantly moderated parent
interpreted with caution. and teacher ratings of hyperactivity and parent ratings of
extcrnalizing problems. This finding may actually reflect
Since potential ratings biases exist in the disorder, and the clinic versus community samples as opposed to a bias in
symptoms of ADHD in females tend to be subtler than in the sex of the first author. Upon examining the studies used
males, it is likely ihat many ADHD females are unrecognized. to calculate moderator variables, most of the female authors
Since the classroom is often the first place in which these utilized community or school samples, whereas male authors
symptoms become problematic, it might be helpful to tended to use clinical samples. As a result, the ratings of
provide elementary school teachers and parents with ADHD girls from community samples might reflect lowerr
training on recognizing ADHD symptoms in both males ratings on hyperactivity than clinical samples.
and females and the differential manifestation of the
disorder by gender. Particularl5; teachers and parents need Limitations .

to be aware that difficulty in the ability/IQ domain may be


indicative of other problems besides intellectual impairment.
The current analysis suffers from some of the same
In addition, although the data from this review cannot
methodological shortcomings as thc Gaub and Carlson
(1997) study. Although the current analysis enlarged the
disseminate subtypes, ADHD boys and girls who meet
criteria for the inattentive subtype arc probably more
sample size for a number of important variables, otlier
difficult to recognize. Therefore, an enhanced
dependent variables such as neuropsychological and social
functioning remained limited by a small number of studies.
understanding of this subtype needs to be communicated Due to this lack of statistical power, it is unclear if gender
to both parents and teachers.
differences did not exist on these variables or if null findings
The findings also indicated that ADHD females perfonned
resulted from insufficient data. The lack of data on many of
the correlates ofADHD indicates a need for a more tuuversal
worse than males on measures of intellectual functioning.

Nadeau, Littma, and Quinn (1999) suggested that ADHD protocol in the assessment of ADHD since a lack of
.

females often receive referrals for scliool-related difficulties consistency significantly limits the ability to summarize
the findings quantitatiyc1y,
or potential learning disabilities (LD) and that gender
differences in intellectual functioning might reflect a referral
Another limitation of the gender differences literature on
bias. Previous research has indicated that ADHD subjects
ADHD and this data set is the lack of data on comorbid
in general arc likely to have both intellectual and academic
disorders. Conlorbid conditions potentially play a major
difficulties in comparison to non-ADHD counterparts
role in the manifestation, severity, and treatment of the
(Faraone et al., 1993). Barkley (1998) has suggested that disorder, as indicated by high levels of externalizing
these differences may not indicate true intellectual
differences but rather manifestations of a poor test-taking
problems for ADHD males and high levels of internalizing
problems for ADHD females. In fact, the current analysis
style characterized by inattention and impulsivity.
might actually be comparing ADHD males with comorbid

150
conduct disorder (CD) or oppositional dcfiant disorder consideration. Since gender differences emerged on
(ODD) to ADHD females with anxiety or mood disorders. internalizing and externalizing problems, differences in
Since numerous ADHD researchers have reported high comorbid conditions would also be expected. Thus,
rates ofcomorbidity (Biedcnnan, Newcom, & Sprich, 1991; assessment of comorbid conditions is essential. Finally,
Carlson et al., 1997; Pliszka, Carlson, & Swanson, 1999), the developmental course of the disorder and gender
the lack of information on comorbidity in this data set is differences within this trajectory are poorly understood.
particularly troubling. These high rates of comorbidity Additional longitudinal studies of adult and adolescent
make it unlikely that the current analysis provides males and females with ADHD would be especially
infonnation that characterizes a &dquo;pure&dquo; form of the disorder. beneficial.
Future research must assess comorbid conditions,
including, at a minimum, CD, ODD, depressive disorders, References
anxiety disorders, and LD. Abikoff, H., Courtney, M., Pelham, W. E., & Koplewiez, H.
S. (1993). Teachers’ ratings of disruptive behaviors:
The examination of gender differences in the subtypes of
The influence of halo effects. Journal of Abnormal
.
ADHD was also limited by the fact that little data exist on
this topic. Unfortunatel5; the current data set could not Child Psychology, 21
(5), 519-533.
provide additional insight into gender differences in Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior
subtypes of the disorder. Instead, the current analysis relied Checklist for Ages 4-18 and 1991 Profile.
on a unidimensional constmct of ADHD that has changed
in terminology and diagnostic criteria over the years. In
Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of
order to assess this question, the diagnostic system used
Psychiatry.
was examined as a potential moderator variable, In fact, the
Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A., & Oglesby, D. M. (1983).
diagnostic system was found to moderate only several Sex and group differences in reading and attention
dependent measures. Therefore, although the current disordered children with and without hyperkinesis.
analysis used different diagnostic systems, these system Journal of learning Disabilities, 16 (7), 407-415.
did not appear to play a major role in how the symptoms
and correlates of ADHD varied by gender. Future American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
conceptualizations of the disorder would benefit from the statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
examination of gender differences in subtypes, particularly Washington, DC: Author.
since gender differences in prevalence among clinical
samples have emerged in previous research. Arcia, E., & Conners, C. K. (1998). Gender differences in
ADHD? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral

Conclusion Pediatrics, 19 (2), 77-83.


The current and the Gaub and Carlson (1997) meta-analyses
Arnold, L. E. (1996). Sex differences inADHD: Conference
indicated that a number of changes are necessary in the Child
summary. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
study of gender differences in ADHD. First, more females 24
(5), 555-569.
are needed in ADHD research studies, and these

investigations should include sections on gender Barkley, R. A. (1989). Hyperactive girls and boys: Stimulant
differences. Second, ADHD studies need to move away drug effects on mother-child interactions. Journal of
from the clinic and address a wider community-based Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
population. White recruitment of females for studies remains Disciplines, 30(3), 379-390.
challenging, epidemiological evidence suggests that many
affected females should be present in these samples. Third, Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity
future research efforts should include comparisons of the disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment
subtypes of the DSAf-IV in the examination of gender (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
differences. Assessment of these subtypes is imperative
since the current diagnostic system does not view ADHD Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-
as a unidimensional constmct and since gender differences mediator variable distinction in social psychological
have been reported in the subtypes of ADHD. Fourth, the research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
diagnostic heterogeneity of ADHD needs further considerations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 51 (6), 1173-1182.

151
Befera, M. S., & Barkley, R. A.
(1985). Hyperactive and Eagley, A. H. (1986). Some meta-analytic approaches to
normal girls and
boys: Mother-child interaction, examining the validity of gender-difference research.
parent psychiatric status and child psychopathology. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp.
Allied Disciplines, 26 (3), 439-452. 159-177). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Berry, C. A., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1985). Girls Faraone, S. V, Biederman, J., Lehman, B. K., Spencer, T.,
with attention deficit disorder: A silent minority? A Norman, D., Seidman, L. J., Kraus, I., Perrin, J., Chen,
report on behavioral and cognitive characteristics. W. J., Tsuang, M. T. (1993). Intellectual performance
Pediatrics, 76 (5), 801-809. and school failure in children with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder and in their siblings. Journal
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V, Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Mick, of Abnormal Psychology, 102
(4), 616-623.
E., Lapey, K. A. (1994). Gender differences in a sample
of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. D. W. (1996). Comorbidity in
Gabel, S., Schmitz, S., & Fulker,
Psychiatry Research, 53 (1), 13-29. hyperactive children: Issues related to selection bias,
gender, severity, and internalizing symptoms. Child
Biederman, J., Newcom, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity Psychiatry and Human Development, 27 (1), 15-28.
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with
conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorders. Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Gender differences in
American Journal of Psychiatry, 148 (5), 564-577. ADHD: A meta-analysis and critical review. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Breen, M. J. (1989). Cognitive and behavioral differences Pschiatry, 36 (8), 1036-1045.
in ADHD boys and girls. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Gomez, R., Harvey, J., Quick, C., Scharer, I., & Harris, G.
30
(5), 711-716. (1999). DSM-IV AD/HD: Confirmatory factor models,
prevalence, and gender and age differences based on
Brccn, M. J., & Altepeter, T. S. (1990). Situational variability parent and teacher ratings of Australian primary
in boys and girls identified as ADHD. Journal of school children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Clinical Psychology, 46
(4), 486-490. Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40 (2), 265-274.
Breen, M. J., & Barkley, R A. (1988). Child pychopatholoy A study of the relationship between the
Hawn, C. S. (1979).
and parenting stress in girls and boys having behaviors used in the identification of hyperactive
attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Journal girls and hyperactive boys. Unpublished doctoral
of Pediatric Psychology, 13 (2), 265-280. dissertation, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale,
IL.
Brown, R T., Madan-Swain, A., & Baldwin, K. (1991). Gender
differences ina clinic-referred sample of attention-
Hedges, L. V, & Becker, B. J. (1986). Statistical methods in
deficit-disordered children. Child Psychiatry and the meta-analysis of research on gender differences.
Human Development, 22 (2), 111-128. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of
gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp.
Carlson, C. L., Tamm, L., & Gaub, M. (1997). Gender 14-50). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
differences in children with ADHD, ODD, and co-
occurring ADHD/ODD identified in a school Hedges, L. W., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods of
population. Journal of the American Academy of meta-analysis. San Diego: Academic Press.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36
(12), 1706-1714.
Horn, W. F., Wagner, A. E., & Ialongo, N. (1989). Sex
deHaas, P. A. (1986). Attention styles and peer relationships differences in school-aged children with pervasive
of hyperactive and normal boys and girls. Journal of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 14
(3), 457-467. Abnormal Child Psychology, 17
(1), 109-125.
Dunn, P. B., & Shapiro, S. K. (1999). Gender differences in James, A., & Taylor, E. (1990). Sex differences in the
the achievement goal orientations ofADHD children. hyperkinetic syndrome Journal
of childhood.
of Child
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23 (3), 327-344. Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
31
(3), 437-446.

152
Kashani, J., Chapel, J. L., Ellis, J., & Shekim, W. (1979). Pelham, W. E., Walker, J. L., Sturges, J., & Hoza, J. (1989).
Hyperactive girls. ,
Journal of Operational Psychiatry Comparative effects of methylphenidate on ADD girls
10
(2), 145-148. and ADD boys. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28 (5), 773-776.
Katz, L. J., Goldstein, G., & Geckle, M. (1998).
Neuropsychological and differences
personality Pliszka, S. R, Carlson, C. L., & Swanson, J. M. (1999).
ADHD
between men and women with ADHD. Journal of with comorbid disorders: Clinical assessment and
Attention Disorders, 2 (4), 239-247. management. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Mannuzza, S., & Gittelman, R. (1984). The adolescent Ramirez, R, & Shapiro, E. (1998). Teacher ratings of attention
outcome of hyperactive girls. Psychiatry Research, deficit hyperactivity disorder in Hispanic children.
, 19 29.
13 Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 20 (4), 275-293.
McGee, R., & Feehan, M. (1991). Are girls with problems
of attention underrecognized? Journal of Rosenthal, R. (1991).
Meta-analytic procedures for social
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13 (3), research (Vol. 6). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
187-198.
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. In
McGee, R., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1987). A comparison H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of
of girls and boys with teacher-identified problems of research synthesis (pp. 23 1-244). New York: Russell
attention. Journal of the American Academy of Child Sage Foundation.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26
(5), 711-717.
Schaugheney, E., McGee, R., Raja, S. N., Feehan, M., &
Miller, R. G., Palkes, H. S., & Stewart, M. A. (1973). Silva, P. A. (1994). Self-reported inattention,
Hyperactive children in suburban elementary schools. impulsivity, and hyperactivity at ages 15 and 18 years
Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 4 (2), in the general population. Journal of the American
121-127. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33 (2),
173-184.
Nada-Raja, S., Langley, J. D., McGee, R., Williams, S. M.,
Begg, D. T., Reeder, A. I. (1997). Inattentive and Schireson, J. (1989). Girls with attention deficit disorder:
hyperactive behaviors and driving offenses in A comparison with their male counterparts.
adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Wright
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36 (4), 515-522. Institute, Los Angeles, CA.

Nadeau, K. G., Littma, E. B., & Quinn, P. O. (1999). Schuerholz, L. J., Singer, H. S., & Denckla, M. B. (1998).
Understanding girls with AD/HD. Silver Spring, MD: Gender study of neuropsychological and neuromotor
Advantage Books. function in children with Tourette Syndrome with and
without attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Orwin, R. G (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta- Journal of Child Neurology, 13 (6), 277-282.
Journal of Educational Statistics, 8
analysis. (2),
157-159. Sharp, W. S., Walter, J. M., Marsh, W. L., Ritchie, G F.,
Hambuger, S. D., Castellanos, F. X. (1999). ADHD in
Pascualvaca, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in the girls: Clinical comparability of a research sample.
manifestation of attention deficit-hyperactivity Journal of the American Academy, of Child and
disorder. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City Adolescent Psychiatry, 38 (1), 40-47.
University of New York, New York, NY
Shenberger, M. (1995). Gender differences in prevalence,
Pelham, W. E., & Bender, M. E. (1982). Peer relationships in identification, and referral of attention-deficit/
hyperactive children: Description and treatment. In hyperactivity disorder in a school-based population.
K. Gadow & I. Bialer (Eds.), Advances in learning Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Houston,
and behavioral disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 365-436). Houston, TX.
Greenwich, CT: JAI.

153
Silverthorn, P., Frick, P. J., Kuper, K., & Ott, J. (1996). Use of two behavior rating scales
Wiedenhoff, A. (1993).
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and sex: A with mothers and teachers in the evaluation of
test of two etiological models to explain the male children with attention-deficit hyperactivity
predominance. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, disorder. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas
25
(1), 52-59. Woman’s University, Denton, TX.

Smith, M. L. (1980). Publication bias and meta-analysis. Jonathan Gershon, M. A., is a doctoral candidate in clinical
Evaluation in Education, 4, 22-24. psychology at Emory University in Atlanta.
Szatmari, P., Offord, D. R., & Boyle, M. H. (1989). Ontario Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
child health study: Prevalence of attention deficit to Jonathan Gershon, M.A., Department of Psychology,
disorder with hyperactivity. Journal of Child Emoy University, 532 North Kilgo Circle, Atlanta, GA
Psychology and Psychiatry, 30 (2), 219-230. 30322-2470. Electronic mail: jgers01@emory.edu

Thomeer, M. L. (1996). The social knowledge and peer


relationships of girls with ADHD in comparison to
boys with ADHD and non-disordered children.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Texas, Austin, TX.

154

You might also like