Autoeficacia, Exito Competitivo, Autoestima y Cuidado Corporal en Patinaje Artistico de Elite

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Behavioral Health and Medicine 2010, 1, 43-51 Number 1 (Spring 2010)

THE RELATIONS BETWEEN SELF-EFFICACY, COMPETITION


SUCCESS, DELIGHT, SELF-ESTEEM, AND BODY-CARE AMONG
ELITE ARTISTIC ROLLER SKATERS
Harald Barkhoff
University of Hawai’i at Hilo

Elaine M. Heiby
University of Hawai’i at Manoa
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

This study explored whether elite artistic roller skaters grouped as low or high
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self-efficacy differed in terms of competition success. Correlates of self-


efficacy, the emotional state delight, trait self-esteem, and sensory-motor skills
were also inspected. Self-report measures were obtained from 29 athletes before
an Artistic Roller Skating World Championship competition. Successful skaters
who won a medal at this competition reported a higher level of self-efficacy be-
fore competition compared to skaters who did not. Compared to artistic roller
skaters with low self-efficacy ratings, those with high self-efficacy ratings re-
ported more delight, higher self-esteem, and more body-care before competition.
Results suggest that future studies investigate the impact of variables posited to
increase self-efficacy.
Keywords: Artistic roller skating, World championship competition, Self-
efficacy, Delight, Self-esteem, Body-care
_____________________
Artistic roller skating is seen as a creative scoring takes performance efficiency into
and aesthetic individual sport, in which the account but also assesses the appearance,
athletes are requested to present their pro- body shape, and individual charisma of the
grams alone in the spotlight of the audience athlete. That is why the athletes’ bodies serve
and the judges. This sport demands certain as both “instruments” to succeed in the tech-
types of athletic performance, including nical accomplishments, such as the triple
jumps, spins, and artistic expression (Bark- jumps, and also as aesthetic objects used to
hoff, 2000). While the efficiency of artistic elicit an emotional reaction from the audience
roller skating is scored by more objective and the judges during competition (Barkhoff,
measures such as performance norms (e.g., .4 2001; Gabler, 1999). The aesthetic reaction is
deduction for falling down), it is difficult to scored via the judges’ marks for artistic ex-
quantify artistic expression precisely. In aes- pression. Criteria for artistic expression are
thetic sports, such as artistic roller skating, the athlete’s: (a) attitude, (b) expression and
___________ combination of movements, (c) specific cho-
Address Correspondence to: reography used, and (d) interpretation of the
Harald Barkhoff, Ph.D. music (Barkhoff, 2000). Prior research (Bark-
Kinesiology & Exercise Sciences
Social Sciences
hoff, 2000; Barkhoff & Heiby, 2004a) found
University of Hawaii at Hilo that competitive artistic roller skating inher-
200 W. Kawili St ently has barriers to optimistic self-efficacy
Hilo, Hawaii 96720-4091 expectations, eliciting delight, maintaining
Phone: +1- 808-974-7604 self-esteem, and exhibiting body-care.
Fax: +1-808-974-7737
E-Mail: harald@hawaii.edu
Self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977) is defined as
expectations regarding one’s ability to suc-

43
44 HARALD BARKHOFF AND ELAINE M. HEIBY

cessfully perform a given behavior. Self- mental health model of performance (Morgan,
efficacy can be viewed as expectations re- 1985; Morgan et al., 1988; Raglin & Morgan,
garding the feasibility of adjusting behavior in 1994) and the mood-model by Thayer (1996)
relation to contingencies of externally- and suggests that emotional state, emotional
controlled reinforcement. This would include trait, and sensory-motor correlates of self-
one’s perceived ability to exhibit the perform- efficacy may elucidate its mechanisms.
ance required to receive a medal at an elite Studies have found a relation between self-
athletic competition. Bandura (1977) suggests efficacy ratings and various positive emo-
that a person with a high level of perceived tional states among a range of types of
self-efficacy is more likely to successfully athletes (Abele & Brehm, 1984; Feltz, 1982;
perform a particular behavior than a person Feltz & Mugno, 1983; Kavanagh & Hausfeld,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

with a lower level of perceived self-efficacy. 1986; LeUnes & Burger, 2000; Skinner &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

There is a large body of research supporting Brewer, 2004; Stevens & Lane, 2001; Terry,
the effect of optimistic self-efficacy cogni- 1995; Terry & Lane, 2000; Thayer, 1996). In
tions upon successfully performing a range of addition, Barkhoff and Heiby (2004b) found
voluntary behaviors (Bandura, 1990, 1997). that elite artistic roller skaters reported more
In addition, self-efficacy skills are trainable delight before and after competition compared
and can be enhanced through the use of a to elite inline speed skaters. However, the
guided mastery intervention (Scott & Cer- relation between the positive emotional state
vone, 2003). of delight and self-efficacy among artistic
The relation between high self-efficacy rat- roller skaters remains unexplored.
ings and performance has been supported in Self-esteem as a positive emotional trait has
sporting activities, such as amount of exercise been shown to be related to some sports per-
(McAuley, 1992; McAuley & Blissmer, 2002; formance abilities and competencies,
Mortiz, Feltz, Mack, & Fahrbach, 2000) and including the ability to deal with the demands
quality of sports performance (Cleary & in training and competition (Barkhoff, 2000;
Zimmerman, 2001; Feltz, 1992; Feltz & Gould & Damarjian, 1996; Gould, Finch &
Lirgg, 1998; Kistsantas & Zimmerman, Jackson, 1993). Barkhoff and Heiby (2004a)
2002). However, the relation between self- found that successful elite artistic roller and
efficacy ratings and performance by artistic figure skaters reported more positive self-
roller skaters has not yet been established. esteem compared to unsuccessful skaters.
Staats (1996) has interpreted the positive However, the relation between self-efficacy
effect of optimistic self-efficacy upon per- and self-esteem was not inspected.
formance in terms of the verbal-emotional Body-care is a complex sensory-motor skill
nature of the cognitions involved. Effective that involves hygiene, fitness, and other
self-efficacy cognitions elicit positive emo- health habits that affect the appearance re-
tional states and help maintain a positive quired in aesthetic sports. Body-care has been
mood. Positive emotions function as dis- shown to play an important role in competi-
criminative stimuli for sensory-motor tive sports performance (Franzoi & Herzog,
behavior and are reinforcing events. Staats 1986; Mrazek, 1987; Späth & Schlicht, 2000).
(1996) has shown that the affective compo- In addition, Barkhoff and Heiby (2004a)
nent of self-efficacy influences the activities found that successful artistic roller and figure
that a person chooses to approach, the effort skaters reported a higher level of body-care
expended on such activities, and the degree of compared to unsuccessful artistic roller and
persistence under challenging contingencies. figure skaters. The relation between self-
This research is also consistent with Morgan’s
SELF-EFFICACY AND SPORTS PERFORMANCE 45

efficacy and body-care among artistic skaters, “precision skating.” Seven participants re-
however, has not been inspected. ceived a medal whereas 22 did not.
To date, the generalizability of the relation
of self-efficacy and its emotional and sensory- Measures
motor correlates (Bandura, 1997; Morgan, Performance. The judges at the competition
1985; Staats 1996) has not yet been tested determined performance outcome. Perform-
among elite artistic roller skaters. This re- ance was measured in terms of whether the
search addressed four hypotheses. skater won a medal at the Artistic Roller Skat-
Hypothesis 1: Elite artistic roller skaters who ing World Champion competition
win a medal at a World Championship com- investigated in this study.
petition will report higher self-efficacy ratings Self-efficacy, delight, self-esteem, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

than skaters who do not win a medal. body-care were measured by a 23-item Likert
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Hypothesis 2: Skaters with higher self- scaled, English language questionnaire that
efficacy ratings will report more delight prior was a composite of one item developed for
to competition than skaters with lower self- this study and three subscales that were trans-
efficacy ratings. lated from German to English using the
Hypothesis 3: Skaters with high self-efficacy standard back-translation method (Brislin,
ratings will report more positive general self- 2000). These German measures were em-
esteem than skaters with lower self-efficacy ployed because they were successfully used in
ratings. prior studies with elite figure and artistic
Hypothesis 4: Skaters with high self-efficacy roller skaters as well as inline speed skaters
will report more general body-care skills than (Barkhoff, 2000; Barkhoff & Heiby, 2004b;
skaters with low self-efficacy. Barkhoff, Pagano & Heiby, 2007).
Self-efficacy. Self-efficacy regarding per-
METHOD formance at the competition was assessed
Subjects with the item “I am certain of my ability to
Participants were a convenience sample of perform my best at this World Champion-
31 top-level elite artistic roller skaters from ship”. The item was rated on a four point
eight different countries including Croatia (n Likert-scale providing the options “not at all”,
= 2), France (n = 2), Germany (n = 4), Great “somewhat”, “quite”, and “very much” with
Britain (n = 4), the Netherlands (n = 1), Slo- low scores reflecting less self-efficacy. Meas-
venia (n = 2), Switzerland (n = 1) and the urement of self-efficacy by using one self-
USA (n = 15). Skaters ranged in age from 15- report item has been shown in other studies to
25 years (M = 18.28; SD = 2.91). Twelve yield acceptable stability reliability and con-
skaters were male and 19 were female. To be struct validity estimates (e.g., Frank, Heiby, &
proficient in English reading and speaking Lee, 2007; Gayton, Matthews and
skills was an inclusion criterion for participa- Burchstead, 1986; Kitsantas & Zimmermann,
tion in this study. To be included in the study 2002; Weiss, Wiese and Klint, 1989).
skaters had to mark “very much” on the item Delight. The positive emotional state delight
“I fully understood the above questions”. Us- was assessed with a subscale of the Be-
ing this criterion two subjects had to be findlichkeitsfragebogen (BEF; Kuhl, 1997),
excluded from the study (N = 29). which is designed to measure situational
Nine athletes were competing in “dance states, such as delight. This inventory was
skating” and 10 in “figure skating.” Six were developed using Thayer’s (1989) “activation-
competing in “free skating,” one in “inline deactivation adjective check list”. The delight
artistic,” one in “pair skating,” and two in subscale consists of the three adjective items
46 HARALD BARKHOFF AND ELAINE M. HEIBY

“happy”, “pleased”, and “cheerful” rated on a Data Analysis


four point Likert scale. Higher scores reflect Analysis of differences in self-efficacy, de-
greater intensity of the mood state of feeling light, self-esteem, and body-care due to sex
delighted. The subscale has excellent internal and age was conducted using eight
consistency with coefficient alpha estimated ANOVA’s. Participants were classified into
to be .91. Construct validity of the instrument two groups in regard to the self-efficacy
is good (Barkhoff & Heiby, 2004b; Kazen, scores. No participant marked either “not at
2006; Kazen, Baumann & Kuhl, 2005). all” or “somewhat” on the four-point self-
Self-esteem. The 10-item subscale of the efficacy item. One group included partici-
Frankfurter Selbstkonzept Skalen (FSKN; pants indicating “quite” on the item and were
Deusinger, 1986) was used to measure self- classified as “low self-efficacy” group (n =
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

esteem on a six point Likert scale. Higher 17). The other group included participants
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

scores indicate more positive self-esteem. A indicating “very much” on the item and were
sample item is "I am content with myself". classified as “high self-efficacy” (n = 12)
Internal consistency and construct validity of group.
the measure are good (Eysenck & Eysenck, To determine if the assumptions of normal-
1964; Fahrenberg, Hampel, & Selg, 1989). ity had been met, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov
Body-care. Body-care was measured by a test was conducted on each variable.
subscale of the body-concept inventory To test if medal winners (n = 7) differed
Frankfurter Koerperkonzept Skalen (FKKS; from no medal winners (n = 22), an ANOVA
Deusinger, 1998). The subscale body-care of was conducted with medal as the independent
the FKKS consists of eight items rated on a variable and self-efficacy as the dependent
six point Likert scale with higher scores indi- variable (Hypothesis 1).
cating more positive body-care. A sample Because delight, self-esteem, and body-care
item is "I do a lot to look my best". Internal might correlate significantly, Pearson correla-
consistency reliability and test-retest reliabil- tions (2-tailed) among these variables were
ity are good. Construct validity has also been computed. Significant correlations indicated
established (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964; the use of MANOVA for hypothesis testing of
Fahrenberg, Hampel, & Selg, 1989). effects of self-efficacy upon delight (Hy-
pothesis 2), self-esteem (Hypothesis 3), and
Procedure body-care (Hypothesis 4) before competition.
Permission to conduct the study during the
Artistic Roller Skating World Championship
competition was obtained from the President RESULTS
of the International Artistic Rollerskating Female and male athletes did not differ sig-
Federation (CIPA) as well as the President of nificantly on ratings of self-efficacy (F(1,28)
the International Roller skating Trainer Asso- = .52, p = .478), delight (F(1,28) = .23, p =
ciation (IRSTA). Informed consent was .617), self-esteem (F(1,28) = .16, p = .690),
obtained from subjects prior to the beginning and body-care (F(1,28) = 3.29, p = .081).
of the study. Assessment took place on one Also, no significant differences were found
occasion three to eight minutes before their regarding age and self-efficacy ratings
performance in competition. Questionnaire (F(1,28) = 2.97, p = .096), delight (F(1,28) =
completion shortly before competition is im- .45, p = .507), self-esteem (F(1,28) = .26, p =
portant considering emotional states by .612), and body-care (F(1,28) = .01, p = .947).
definition change over time (Hill & Hill, Therefore, subsequent analyses combined
1991). participants in terms of these demographics.
SELF-EFFICACY AND SPORTS PERFORMANCE 47

Table 1: Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha’s of Delight, Self-Esteem, and
Body-care for Athletes with Low and High Self-Efficacy

Low Self-efficacy (n High Self-efficacy (n All


= 17) = 12) (n = 29)
Subscales alpha M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Delight .71 8.65 (1.69) 10.42 (1.44) 9.38 (1.80)
Self-esteem .97 42.88 (12.64) 53.92 (5.79) 47.45 (11.62)
Body-care .87 34.65 (5.42) 39.83 (3.41) 36.79 (5.30)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Results of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test indi- DISCUSSION


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cated no significant deviation from normality One purpose of this study (Hypothesis 1)
as all p values were greater than .05 (delight p was to explore whether elite artistic skating
= .091, self-esteem p = .397, and body-care p athletes with low and high self-efficacy rat-
= .244), indicating that use of ANOVA and ings differed in terms of success during an
MANOVA meets this diagnostic criterion. Artistic Roller Skating World Championship
The medal winner group differed signifi- competition. Self-efficacy was measured in
cantly compared to the no medal winner terms of the athletes’ reported certainty of
group regarding self-efficacy (F(1,28) = being able to exhibit peak performance at the
22.16, p = .000). The medal winner group (M competition. Success was equated with win-
= 4.00; SD = .00) reported higher levels of ning a medal at the competition under
self-efficacy going into the competition com- investigation. The three other purposes were
pared to the no medal winner group (M = to explore whether high self-efficacy was
3.23; SD = .43). The mean, standard devia- related to a higher level of the emotional state
tion, and Cronbach’s alpha on scale scores of delight (Hypothesis 2), trait self-esteem (Hy-
delight, self-esteem and body-care are re- pothesis 3), and the sensory-motor skills
ported in Table 1. involved in body-care (Hypothesis 4). These
Table 2 shows significant positive correla- correlates of self-efficacy follow from theo-
tions between delight and self-esteem and ries stating that self-efficacy expectations of
between self-esteem and body-care. The reinforcement contingencies are more effec-
MANOVA yielded a significant main effect tive if they elicit positive emotional states,
of self-efficacy based on Pillai's Trace esti- maintain positive emotional traits, serve as
mate (F = 870.87, p = .000). Univariate F’s discriminative stimuli for sensory-motor be-
for delight (F(1,28) = 9.95, p = .004), self- havior, and contribute to the reinforcing
esteem (F(1,28) = 4.45, p = .044), and body- effectiveness of consequences (Bandura,
care (F(1.28) = 5.96, p = 021) were also sig- 1997; Morgan et al., 1988; Staats, 1996).
nificant. It was found that the skaters who won a
Table 2: Results of Pearson Correlations Among Delight, Self-esteem, and Body-care

N delight delight self-esteem


X X X
self-esteem body-care body-care
29 0.389* 0.248 0.728**

**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)


* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
48 HARALD BARKHOFF AND ELAINE M. HEIBY

medal at an Artistic Roller Skating World self-efficacy, which is consistent with the
Championship competition reported a higher third hypothesis. The findings for self-esteem
level of self-efficacy compared to the skaters are consistent with those reported in a study
who did not win a medal, supporting the first involving elite artistic roller and figure skaters
hypothesis. Greater certainty of one’s ability (Barkhoff & Heiby, 2004a) as well as studies
several minutes prior to competition seems to involving other sports (Gould et al., 1993;
be related to exhibiting better performance Gould & Damarjian, 1996).
during competition. This finding is consistent Finally, the high self-efficacy group re-
with prior studies investigating the relation ported a higher level of the sensory-motor
between self-efficacy and successful perform- skills involved in general body-care, support-
ance in sports other than artistic skating ing the fourth hypothesis. The finding that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Cleary & Zimmermann, 2001; Feltz, 1992: skaters with high self-efficacy reported
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Kitsantas & Zimmermann, 2002), including a greater body-care compared to the skaters
marathon (Gayton et al., 1986) and competi- with low self-efficacy also is consistent with
tive gymnastics (Weiss et al., 1989). findings in regard to body-care reported by
Compared to skaters with low self-efficacy, athletes in artistic roller and figure skating
artistic skaters with high self-efficacy re- (Barkhoff & Heiby, 2004a) and other sports
ported more delight, supporting the second (Franzoi & Herzog, 1986; Mrazek, 1987;
hypothesis. This result is consistent with the Späth & Schlicht, 2000). Body-care may be
hypothesis that a positive emotional state particularly important for the aesthetic sport
(e.g., delight) is associated with self-efficacy of artistic roller skating where not only fitness
(Bandura, 1997; Staats, 1996). The finding but also attractiveness affects success.
also converges with Abele and Brehm's These findings must be viewed within the
(1984) postulate that in a situation which de- limitations of this study. The small sample
mands more creativity, a positive emotional size (n = 29) limited generalizability of the
state such as delight is closely related to suc- findings. Small samples sizes are inherent to
cessful performance. In addition, the finding investigations with elite athletes at top level
regarding delight converges with Morgan's performance as so few athletes by definition
(1985) postulate that vigour and Thayer's can reach this level. Replication across events
(1996) postulate that calm energy emotional is critical to evaluate whether the findings are
states are associated with successful sports robust with elite artistic roller skaters as well
performance. This result is also consistent as generalize to other sports. Assessment of
with other findings regarding the association correlates of self-efficacy was restricted to
of positive mood and successful performance measures of delight, self-esteem, and body-
in other sports (Lowther & Lane, 2002; Mor- care. Psychometrics properties of these meas-
gan, 1985; Morgan et al., 1988). Furthermore, ures have not been established with elite
this result is consistent with findings by Bark- athletes. Furthermore, self-efficacy was as-
hoff and Heiby (2004b) that successful sessed with only one item. However, prior
performance in the creative sport of artistic studies used one item successfully (Cleary &
roller and figure skating may be associated Zimmermann, 2001; Kitsantas &
with more positive emotional states (including Zimmermann, 2002). Finally, competition
delight) before competition. success was equated with winning a medal at
The results of the study also revealed that the competition under investigation, neglect-
artistic skaters with high self-efficacy re- ing the possibility that someone might have
ported higher trait self-esteem before shown his/her personal best, but did not win a
competition compared to skaters with low medal.
SELF-EFFICACY AND SPORTS PERFORMANCE 49

The present study provides preliminary Athlete in Artistic Roller Skating. Journal of
support for theories positing that high self- the American Board of Sport Psychology, 1,
1 – 14,
efficacy is related to successful performance, http://www.americanboardofsportpsychology.
feeling a sense of delight, having positive org/Portals/24/ABSP-JournalBarkhoff1.pdf
self-esteem, and exhibiting body-care (Ban- (1), 69-92.
dura, 1997; Morgan et al., 1988; Staats, Brislin, R.W. (2000). Back-translation. In A.E. Kazdin
1996). Findings suggest generalizability of (Ed.),Encyclopedia of psychology, Volume 1
(pp.359-360). Washington, D.C.: American
the effects of self-efficacy to elite artistic Psychological Association.
roller skaters during a World Championship Cleary, T.J., & Zimmermann, B.J., (2001). Self-
competition. Regulation Differences during Athletic Prac-
tice by Experts, Non-Experts, and Novices.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

REFERENCES Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13 (2),


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Abele, A., & Brehm, W. (1984). 185-206.


Befindlichkeitsveränderungen im Sport Deusinger, I.M. (1986). Die Frankfurter
[Changes of Mood in Sport]. Sportwissen- Selbstkonzeptskalen (FSKN) [The Frankfurter
schaft, 3, 252-275. Self-Concept Scales]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying Deusinger, I.M. (1998). Die Frankfurter
theory of behavioral change. Psychological Körperkonzeptskalen(FKKS): Handanweisung
Review, 84, 191-215. [The Frankfurter Body-Concept Scales:
Bandura, A. (1990). Perceived self-efficacy in the Manual). Göttingen: Hogrefe
exercise of personal agency. Journal of Ap- Eysenck, H. M., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual
plied Sport Psychology, 2, 128-163. of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. London:
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of con- London University Press.
trol. New York: Freeman. Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R., & Selg, H. (1989). Das
Barkhoff, H. (2000). Handlungskontrolle und Freiburger Persoenlichkeitsinventar FPI (5th
Selbstkonzept(e) von Hochleistungssportlern ed.) [The Freiburger Personality Inventory].
im Roll- und Eiskunstlauf in Trainings- und Goettingen: Hogrefe.
Wettkampfsituationen [Action Control and Feltz, D.L. (1982). The effect of age and number of
Self Concept(s) of top level Roller and Figure demonstrations on modelling of form and per-
Skaters in Training and Competition]. Egels- formance. Research Quarterly for Exercise
bach: Hänsel-Hohenhausen. and Sport, 52, 291-196.
Barkhoff, H. (2001). In Search of the Art in Roller- and Feltz, D.L. (1992). Understanding motivation in sport:
Figure-Skating. Sports Efficiency and the A self-efficacy perspective. In G.C. Roberts
Ability of Artistic Expression. IRSTA- News- (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp.
letter, 2, 3-4. 93-106). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Barkhoff, H., & Heiby, E.M. (2004a). Differences in Feltz, D.L., & Lirgg, C.D. (1998). Perceived team and
Self-concept, Body-concept, and Mood be- player efficacy in hockey. Journal of Applied
tween Training Champion and Competitor Psychology, 83, 557-564.
Type Athletes in Artistic Roller and Figure Feltz, D.L., & Mugno, D. (1983). A replication of path
Skating. Athletic Insight, 6. analysis of the causal elements in Bandura’s
www.athleticinsight.com/Vol6Iss1/Differences theory of self efficacy and the influence of
inSelf-coneptBody- autonomic perception. Journal of Sport Psy-
conceptMoodRollerFigureSkating.htm chology, 5, 263-277.
Barkhoff, H., & Heiby, E.M. (2004b). Differences in Frank, M.R, Heiby, E.M., & Lee, J.H. (2007). Assess-
Mood Among Elite Athletes in Artistic Roller, ment of determinants of compliance to twelve
Artistic Figure and Inline Speed Skating Be- health behaviors: Psychometric evaluation of
fore and After Competition. European Journal the Health Behavior Schedule-II. Psychologi-
of Sport Science, 4, 22-32. cal Reports,100, 1281-1297.
Barkhoff , H., Pagano, I. & Heiby, E. (2007). Longitu- Franzoi, S.L. & Herzog, M.E. (1986). The body esteem
dinal Analyses of Mood in Highlight Sport scale: A convergent and discriminant validity
Competitions: A Season Long Case Study of a study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50,
Training Champion vs. a Competitor Type 24-31.
50 HARALD BARKHOFF AND ELAINE M. HEIBY

Gabler, H. (1999). Charismatische Persönlichkeiten im McAuley, E., & Blissmer, B. (2002). Self-efficacy and
Sport [Charimatic Personalities in Sports]. Attributional Processes in Physical Activity.
Sportwissenschaft, 4, 412-426. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in Sport Psy-
Gayton, W.F., Matthews, G.R., & Burchstead, G.N. chology (pp. 185-205). Champaign, IL:
(1986). An investigation of the validity of the Human Kinetics.
physical self-efficacy in predicting marathon Morgan, W.P. (1985). Affective beneficence of vigor-
performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, ous physical activity. Medicine and Science in
752-754. Sport and Exercise, 17, 94-100.
Gould, D., & Damarjian, N. (1996). Imagery Training Morgan, W.P., Costill, D.L., Flynn, M.G., Raglin, J.S.
for Peak Performance. In J. L. Van Raalte & & O`Connor, P.J. (1988). Mood disturbance
B. W. Brewer (ed.). Exploring sport and exer- following increased training in swimmers.
cise psychology (pp. 25-50).Washington, DC: Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise,
American Psychological Association. 20, 408-414.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Gould, D., Finch, L. M. & Jackson, S. A. (1993). Cop- Mortiz, S.E., Feltz, D.L., Mack, D.E., & Fahrbach,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ing strategies used by champion figure K.R. (2000). The relation of self-efficacy
skaters. Research quarterly for exercise and measures to sport performance: a meta-
sport, 64, 453-468. analytic review. Research Quarterly for Exer-
Hill, C.M., & Hill, D.W. (1991). Influence of time of cise and Sport, 71, 280-294.
day on responses to the Profile of Mood Mrazek, J. (1987). Struktur und Entwicklung des Kör-
States. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 434- perkonzepts im Jugendalter [Structure and
439. Development of Body-concept in Youth].
Kazen, M. (2006). Problemessen vs. Spassessen: In- Zeitschrift für Enwicklungspsychologie und
teraktive Wirkungen von Diaetintention und Pädagogische Psychologie, 1, 1 -13
Selbstaktivierung auf das Essverhalten [Prob- Raglin, J. S., & Morgan, W. P., (1994). Development
lems vs. Fun when Eating: Interaction of of a scale for use in monitoring training-
following a diet regime and self-activation]. induced distress in athletes. International
Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie, 14 (2), journal sports medicine, 15 (2), 84-88.
34-35. Scott, W.E. & Cervone, D. (2003). Enhancing per-
Kazen, M., Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Self- ceived self-efficacy: Guided mastery therapy.
Regulation After Mortality Salience. Euro- In W. O'Donohue, J.E. Fisher, & S.C. Hayes
pean Psychologist, 10, 1-11. (Eds.) Cognitive behavior therapy: Applying
Kavanagh, D., & Hausfeld, S. (1986). Physical per- empiricically supported techniques in your
formance and self efficacy under happy and practice. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons,
sad mood. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, Inc. (pp. 288- 293).
112-123. Skinner, N. & Brewer, N. (2004). Adaptive Ap-
Kitsantas, A., & Zimmermann, B.J. (2002). Comparing proaches to Competition: Challenge
Self-Regulatory Processes Among Novice, Appraisals and Positive Emotion. Journal of
Non-Expert, and Expert Volleyball Players: A Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 283-305.
Microanalytic Study. Journal of Applied Sport Späth, U. & Schlicht, W. (2000). Sportliche Aktivität
Psychology, 14 (2), 91-105. und Selbst- und Körperkonzept in der Phase
Kuhl, J. (1997). BEF – Befindlichkeitsfragebogen. der Pubeszenz [Sport Actitivities and Self-
Unpublished. and Body-concept during Pubeszenz]. Psy-
LeUnes, A., & Burger, J., (2000). Profile of mood chologie und Sport, 2, 51-65.
states research in sport and exercise psychol- Staats, A.W. (1996). Behavior and personality. New
ogy: Past, present, and future. Journal of York: Springer
applied sport psychology, 12 (1), 5-15. Stevens, M.J., & Lane, A.M. (2001). Mood-regulating
Lowther, J., & Lane, A.M. (2002). Relationships be- strategies used by athletes. Athletic Insight, 3.
tween mood, cohesion and satisfaction with www.athleticinsight.com/Vol3Iss3/MoodRegul
performance among soccer players. Athletic ation.htm
Insight, 4. Terry, P. (1995). The efficacy of mood state profiling
www.athleticinsight.com/Vol4Iss3/MoodandP with elite performers: A review and synthesis.
erformance.htm The Sport Psychologist, 9, 309-324.
McAuley, E. (1992). Exercise and motivation: A self- Terry, P. & Lane, A. M., (2000). Normative values for
efficacy perspective. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), the profile of mood states for use with athletic
Motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 107- samples. Journal of applied sport psychology,
128). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. 12 (1), 93-109.
SELF-EFFICACY AND SPORTS PERFORMANCE 51

Thayer, R-E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and


arousal. New York: Oxford University Press.
Thayer, R.-E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods.
Managing energy, tension, and stress. New
York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
Weiss, M.R., Wiese, D.M., & Klint, K.A. (1989). Head
over heels with success: The relationship be-
tween self-efficacy and performance in
competitive youth gymnastics. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 444-451.

Action Editor: Mark R. Dixon


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

You might also like