Behaviour Research and Therapy: Arnaud Pictet, Anna E. Coughtrey, Andrew Mathews, Emily A. Holmes

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Fishing for happiness: The effects of generating positive imagery on mood


and behaviour
Arnaud Pictet a, Anna E. Coughtrey a, Andrew Mathews b, Emily A. Holmes a, *
a
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
b
Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Experimental evidence using pictureeword cues has shown that generating mental imagery has a causal
Received 11 May 2011 impact on emotion, at least for images prompted by negative or benign stimuli. It remains unclear
Received in revised form whether this finding extends to overtly positive stimuli and whether generating positive imagery can
4 October 2011
increase positive affect in people with dysphoria. Dysphoric participants were assigned to one of three
Accepted 4 October 2011
conditions, and given instructions to generate mental images in response to pictureeword cues which
were either positive, negative or mixed (control) in valence. Results showed that the positive picture
Keywords:
eword condition increased positive affect more than the control and negative conditions. Participants in
Mental imagery
Emotion
the positive condition also demonstrated enhanced performance on a behavioural task compared to the
Depression two other conditions. Compared to participants in the negative condition, participants in the positive
Behaviour condition provided more positive responses on a homophone task administered after 24 h to assess the
Evaluative learning durability of effects. These findings suggest that a positive pictureeword task used to evoke mental
imagery leads to improvements in positive mood, with transfer to later performance. Understanding the
mechanisms underlying mood change in dysphoria may hold implications for both theory and treatment
development.
Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY license.

Can we engage imagination to promote more positive mood? & Gotlib, 2007; MacLeod & Byrne, 1996), positive affect (Watson,
Recent evidence using pictureeword cues suggests that evoking Clark, & Carey, 1988) and willingness to pursue potentially
mental imagery has a more powerful impact on emotion than using rewarding goals (Dickson, Moberly, & Kinderman, 2011).
verbal language (Holmes, Mathews, Mackintosh, & Dalgleish, 2008), Research suggests that individuals with depression fail to benefit
at least for negative and benign stimuli. This is consistent with a line from the emotional lift provided by evoking past and future positive
of research suggesting that mental imagery has a special impact on personal events (Joorman et al., 2007; Joormann & Siemer, 2004).
emotion (Arntz, de Groot, & Kindt, 2005; Holmes & Mathews, 2005, Several mechanisms may contribute to this effect. With respect to
2010; Holmes, Mathews, Dalgleish, & Mackintosh, 2006). However, mental imagery, this lack of congruent emotional response might be
the impact of generating mental imagery from overtly positive explained by three characteristic features of depressive thinking.
pictureeword stimuli remains to be explored. We know little about First, depression has been shown to be associated with a deficit in
the promotion of overtly positive imagery, particularly when this is generating positive imagery about the future (Holmes, Lang, et al.,
most needed as in depressed mood. While the research is part of 2008; Stöber, 2000; Williams et al., 1996). Second, depression is
a broader endeavour to improve depressed mood, in this study we characterized by rumination which is predominantly verbal in
focused on the extent to which positive affect (rather than nature (Fresco, Frankel, Mennin, Turk, & Heimberg, 2002). The
depressed mood per se) improved as a result of generating positive verbal processing of positive information can have the paradoxical
imagery. Our focus on positive affect and imagery was driven by effect of worsening mood rather than improving it (Holmes, Lang, &
recent findings that depression is not just associated with an excess Shah, 2009). Finally, depressed individuals tend to recall positive
of negative cognitions and affect, but also a deficit in positive autobiographical events from an observer rather than a field
cognitions (Holmes, Lang, Moulds, & Steele, 2008; Joorman, Siemer, perspective (Lemogne et al., 2006). Imagining positive scenarios
from an observer perspective has been shown to lower mood,
possibly due to unfavourable comparisons with perceived reality
* Corresponding author. (Holmes, Coughtrey, & Connor, 2008). For all these reasons, we have
E-mail address: emily.holmes@psych.ox.ac.uk (E.A. Holmes). suggested the utility of promoting positive future-oriented, field

0 005-7967 Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY license.


doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.003
886 A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891

perspective mental imagery for improving depressed mood ambiguous homophones (Mathews, Richards, & Eysenck, 1989) was
(Blackwell & Holmes, 2010; Holmes, Lang, & Shah, 2009). completed 24 h later.
The benefit of generating images about positive future outcomes Our predictions were that, compared to the control condition,
may not be limited to mood and cognitive bias improvement but the positive condition would lead to a greater increase in positive
extend to subsequent behaviour. Indeed, a body of evidence mood. Conversely, we predicted that compared to control condi-
suggests that imagining one’s own future behaviour can increase tion, the negative condition would lead to a greater decrease in
the likelihood of that behaviour being enacted in reality (Carroll, positive mood. Furthermore, we predicted that generating mental
1978; Gregory, Cialdini, & Carpenter, 1982; Pham & Taylor, 1999). images in response to pictureeword cues would impact on later
Imagining a possible event outcome has been shown to increase behaviour, with participants in the positive condition performing
subjective probability that the same outcome will actually occur better on the fishing task than those in the control and negative
(Carroll, 1978). Depressed individuals’ attitudes towards the future conditions. Finally, we predicted a similar pattern of results on the
is marked with reduced expectancies for positive events (MacLeod, homophone test completed 24 h later.
Rose, & Williams, 1993; Pysczcynski & Greenberg, 1987) and a lack
of approach motivation towards future goals (Dickson & MacLeod, Method
2004). Moreover, a clinical feature of depression is a lack of
interest in almost all everyday activities that were previously Participants
perceived as rewarding and enjoyable (American Psychiatric
Association, 2000). Consequently, individuals with depression are Eighty-seven individuals (50 women; mean age ¼ 27.96,
less persistent and tend to engage less time in goal-directed SD ¼ 11.33) with scores in the dysphoric range on the Beck
behaviours than do healthy individuals (Hopko, Armento, Cantu, Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) were
Chambers, & Lejuez, 2003; Hopko & Mullane, 2008). recruited via advertisements in the local University town. When
Previous studies of dysphoric and depressed individuals have defining dysphoric samples it is generally recommended to use
shown that, although these groups are as able as non-dysphoric a multistage process (Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, & Ingram,
groups to identify desirable activities and goals, they are more 1987; Wenzlaff & Bates, 1998) to ensure that negative mood is
pessimistic about the outcome of any attempts to achieve these not merely elevated at the start of the experimental session. In this
goals (Dickson et al., 2011). Similarly, when actually required to study we defined dysphoric mood as a BDI-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, &
attempt experimental tasks, dysphoric individuals (and those Brown, 1996) score of 10 or above, at two time points, separated by
exposed to learned helplessness inductions) are more readily at least 5 days, following Aarts, Wegner, and Dijksterhuis (2006),
discouraged by anticipated loss, are more inclined to give up trying Dalgleish and Yiend (2006) and Sedek and von Hecker (2004).
after failures, and are less encouraged by expected rewards, result- Thus, of the 87 participants meeting criteria at recruitment, 81 still
ing in poorer performance when effort is required to achieve success met criteria at the beginning of the experimental session and were
(Henriques, Glowacki, & Davidson, 1994; Miller & Seligman, 1975; included in the study. The 6 people that no longer had a BDI-II score
Pizzagalli, Iosifescu, Hallett, Ratner, & Fava, 2008). On the basis of of 10 or above at the beginning of the experimental session
these results, we predicted that the performance of dysphoric (M ¼ 8.00, SD ¼ 1.55) did not differ from the sample in terms of age,
participants would be improved by positive imagery designed to t(85) ¼ 1.06, p ¼ .29, or gender c2 (2, N ¼ 87) ¼ 3.57, p ¼ .60. The
increase the anticipation of success under ambiguous conditions, mean BDI-II score for the final sample of 81 participants was 17.63
thus enhancing their effortful engagement in a difficult but poten- (SD ¼ 7.56) at recruitment, and 16.52 (SD ¼ 7.21) at the beginning of
tially rewarding task. A fishing game task (as in Reinecke, Deeprose, the experimental session (henceforth referred to as time 1 in
& Holmes, submitted for publication) was chosen as a simple Table 1).
behavioural performance measure assumed to tap behaviour
negatively associated with dysphoria, such as approach motivation Materials
and persistence, while being clearly different in nature from the
pictureeword task. Pictureeword cues to generate mental images
Exploring the transfer of positive mental imagery to behaviour is of Participants were required to generate mental images in
both clinical and theoretical relevance (Kavanagh, Andrade, & May, response to pictureeword cues as in earlier research (Holmes,
2005). The deficit in motivated behaviour is one of the most perni-
cious symptoms in depression because it cuts the individual off from
pleasant and rewarding experiences. This behavioural component of Table 1
depression has long been targeted in Cognitive Behavioural Therapy Characteristics of participants at baseline per condition.

(CBT) and has also given rise to the development of successful inter- Characteristic Positive Negative Control
ventions known as “behavioural activation” (Jacobson, Martell, & (n ¼ 27) (n ¼ 27) (n ¼ 27)
Dimidjian, 2001). The aim of behavioural activation is to increase M SD M SD M SD
the likelihood of engaging in rewarding behaviours. Therefore, the Age (years) 29.78 12.37 28 12 26 9.50
clinical relevance of using an experimental task designed to encourage
Gender (%)
positive mental imagery to alleviate depressed mood would be further
Female 59 67 59
supported if a behaviour-enhancing effect followed training. Male 41 33 41
In the current experiment, dysphoric participants were given training
BDI-II
and instructions to evoke mental imagery in line with our previous work At recruitment 18.15 7.96 17.00 6.98 17.74 7.94
on mental imagery generation (Holmes, Mathews, et al., 2008). Partici- At baseline (time 1) 16.67 6.85 16.30 7.14 16.59 7.87
pants were then asked to imagine and rate the vividness of imagery in
BHS 6.04 4.59 6.00 5.43 6.89 5.10
response to viewing 200 pictureeword combinations within one of three STAI-T 46.33 11.23 46.41 11.93 48.96 10.38
conditions: positive resolution, negative resolution or a mixed control SUIS 41.15 6.52 38.63 8.39 39.85 9.10
condition, before completing the behavioural task. To assess the dura- Note: BDI-II ¼ Beck Depression Inventory-II, BHS ¼ Beck Hopelessness, STAI ¼
bility of effects, a measure based on the perceived meaning of emotionally Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, SUIS ¼ Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale,
PANAS ¼ total positive affect score from the PANAS.
A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891 887

Mathews, et al., 2008), for more details see the Procedure. possesses high internal consistency with an alpha level of .9
Presentation of the pictureeword cue stimuli was controlled using (Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri, 1996). One week test retest reliability is
E-Prime software, version 1.1.4.1 (Schneider, Eschman, & Zucclotto, also high, r ¼ .93 (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). In addition, hope-
2002) running on a Dell PC (P791). The set of 200 digital pictures lessness was measured using the 20-item Beck Hopelessness Scale
used were photographed for the study by local art students given (BHS; Beck & Steer, 1998) rated on a dichotomous scale (trueefalse).
the remit of producing ambiguous pictures, or by the authors using Positive affective state was measured using the combined
a Sony digital camera, or downloaded from the internet (non- positive affect subscales (21-items) of the Positive and Negative
copyrighted). The pictures were 640  480 pixels saved as Bitmap Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellengen, 1988) as in
files. They were displayed, centred on the 1700 VDU such that their previous work (e.g. Holmes, Mathews, et al., 2008). Each word was
height took up 85% of the VDU height. rated on a 5-point scale (very slightly or not at all to extremely), using
Each picture was of common everyday objects and scenes from the short-term time instructions (“Indicate to what extent you feel
the local area that a student might typically encounter (e.g. this way now/in the past few minutes”).
students attending a lecture, partying with friends, sporting
activities). Each picture was paired with a word or short phrase Behavioural task: the fishing game
intended, when paired with the relevant picture, to create a posi- This simple task employed a toy measuring 1500 mm 
tive emotional resolution in the positive condition (e.g. a picture of 900 mm  250 mm, in which 12 brightly coloured plastic fish
students attending a lecture, and the word “understanding”) or moved round in a circle, opening and closing their mouths to reveal
negative emotional resolution in the negative condition (e.g. the a magnet. Participants were required to catch as many fish as they
same picture with the word “struggling”). In the control condition, could in 2.5 min by “hooking” them using a magnet on the end of
half of the pictures were paired with captions from the negative a 900 mm plastic fishing rod. The authors used 2 different sets of
condition, and the remaining 50% were captions from the positive fishing games of identical make and model.
condition (similar to the approach of Hirsch, Hayes, & Mathews,
2009). All participants saw the same pictures, but the captions Valence ratings
accompanying them differed accordingly to their assigned condi- Valence ratings for 50 pictures (in the absence of words) were
tion. During the imagery generation phase, each word was dis- obtained before and after the imagery generation task (as in
played in white against a black background, centrally beneath the Holmes, Mathews, et al., 2008). To sample across the 200 trials, the
relevant picture, using type font Arial at point size 30, for 3500 ms. 50 pictures consisted of a sub-selection of approximately 10 taken
During the liking ratings (see below), the pictures were displayed from each of the 5 pictureeword training blocks, presented in
without words against a black background. a randomized order. Participants rated the emotional valence of
each picture from 1(extremely unpleasant) to 9 (extremely pleasant)
Filler task to equalize mood using a standard keyboard.
An unrelated task of listening to a series of 40 s classical music
extracts was administered during a 10-min interval after the Subjective experience questions
imagery generation phase. This task has been successfully used in As a further manipulation check, participants were asked
previous studies (Holmes, Lang, & Shah, 2009; Holmes et al., 2006) a series of questions about their subjective experience of per-
to minimize the influence of mood differences across groups and forming the pictureeword task, both after the first and last blocks
return mood to baseline before post-training assessment tasks. of training. Questions assessed task difficulty (“How difficult or
Participants rated how pleasant they found each musical extract on easy did you find your task of combining the picture and the
a scale of 1 (extremely unpleasant) to 9 (extremely pleasant). word”), use of verbal processing and imagery during the training
phase (“How much were you verbally analyzing/imagining the
Homophone task pictureeword combinations?”) and use of field perspective (“How
A list of 14 homophones (e.g. dye/die) and 14 unambiguous much were you imagining the pictureeword trough your own
neutral words of similar word frequency was used (based on eyes”). Questions were rated on 9-point scales, ranging from 1 (not
Mathews et al., 1989; Mogg, Bradbury, & Bradley, 2006). The task was at all) to 9 (extremely/all the time).
chosen for reasons of convenience since it could be delivered by
telephone 24 h after the experimental session. The 24 h gap between Procedure
the experimental manipulation and the interpretation bias measure
was selected in line with previous investigation of the durability of Participants gave their written informed consent and were
interpretation bias modification procedure (Yiend, Mackintosh, & randomly assigned to positive, negative or control conditions
Mathews, 2005). The experimenter read the homophones over the (N ¼ 27 per condition). They completed the BDI-II (for the second
phone one word at a time. Participants were required to report the time to confirm dysphoric status), trait STAI, SUIS, BHS and PANAS
spelling of each word in turn. The total number of negative spellings (time 1). Then participants completed the valence ratings of 50
(e.g. die rather than dye) was calculated and converted into pictures presented on the computer.
a percentage of the number of all correctly spelled homophones. The mental imagery generation instructions, similar to those
used in Holmes, Mathews, et al. (2008) were then read out: In all
Questionnaire measures conditions, mental imagery was described as “the experience of
The trait tendency to use mental imagery was measured using seeing with the mind’s eye, hearing with the mind’s ear, and so
the 12-item Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS: Reisberg, on.”, using Kosslyn, Ganis, and Thompson’s (2001) definition i.e.
Pearson, & Kosslyn, 2003). Each item was rated on a 5-point scale in any sensory modality. All participants were given an imagery
(never appropriate to always completely appropriate). generation practice task of imagining cutting a lemon. The differ-
Depressive symptoms were measured using the 21-item Beck ence between field (first person) and observer (third person)
Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) in imagery was explained using two photographs of a person in the
which participants responded to a series of questions on a scale laboratory taken from either a field or observer perspective (as in
from 0 to 3. Possible scores range from 0 to 63, with 0e13 classified Holmes, Coughtrey, et al., 2008). Participants were asked to
as minimal depression and 14e19 as mild depression. The BDI-II imagine the combination of each picture and word from a field
888 A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891

perspective (i.e. seeing the situation through their own eyes, as if PANAS, F(2, 78) ¼ .10, p ¼ .91; or baseline liking ratings for the
they were actively involved). The experimenter also explained how pictures, F(2, 78) ¼ 2.32, p ¼ .11. Time 1 PANAS correlated signifi-
it was possible to imagine anything, even if it did not immediately cantly in our sample with BDI-II at baseline, r(81) ¼ .53, p < .001.
seem believable, using a picture of a flying elephant as an example.
In the next stage, practice was given in generating mental Positive affect change over the training phase
imagery in response to pictureeword cues. Two pictureeword
combinations (one positive and one negative) were given on paper PANAS scores were analyzed in a mixed model ANOVA with
as practice examples by the experimenter. Next, four pictureeword a within-subjects factor of time (pre- vs. post-training) and
items, consistent with condition assignment were presented on the a between-subjects factor of condition (positive vs. negative vs.
computer as full practice examples (as in Holmes, Mathews, et al., control). As predicted, there was a significant interaction of time
2008). Participants were told that they were to produce a field with condition, F(2, 78) ¼ 14.31, p < .001, h2 ¼ .27. There was no
perspective mental image combining the picture with the word. main effect of time F(2, 78) ¼ .20, p ¼ .66, h2 ¼ .03 or condition F(2,
Instructions on the computer screen for 1000 ms stated “imagine 78) ¼ 1.93, p ¼ .15, h2 ¼ .05.
the combination of the next picture and word as if you were actively To facilitate interpretation, we computed change scores for each
involved”. The picture and word then appeared for 3500 ms, fol- condition, and decomposed the interaction between time and
lowed by a 1000 ms beep. Participants were asked to shut their eyes condition using independent samples t-tests, comparing change
and imagine, and to describe their practice images aloud while the scores for each training group with the control condition. As pre-
experimenter checked that the description of the mental image dicted, there was a greater increase in positive affect for the positive
included reference to both picture and word. To promote compli- than the control condition (M ¼ 8.04, SD ¼ 6.65 vs. M ¼ .33,
ance with the imagery task instructions, after each pictureeword SD ¼ 12.42), t(52) ¼ 2.84, p ¼ .006, d ¼ .77. Conversely, there was
trial, participants had 10 s in which they made a rating of mental a greater decrease in positive affect for the negative than the control
image vividness on a scale from 1 (not at all vivid) to 9 (extremely condition (M ¼ 6.85, SD ¼ 10.74 vs. M ¼ .33, SD ¼ 12.42), t(52) ¼
vivid). Participants then continued to the remaining practice items. 2.27, p ¼ .027, d ¼ .62 (Fig. 1 illustrates the PANAS changes within
each condition).
Main experimental task Paired sample t-tests were used to test whether within each
Participants were asked to generate mental images (as defined group there had been the predicted change in affect from pre to
and practised above) in response to each of a series of pictureeword post-training (following Holmes et al., 2006). As predicted, the
cues. Two hundred pictureeword cues were presented in 5 positive condition showed a significant increase in positive affect,
randomized blocks of 40 according to condition. As in the practice t(26) ¼ 6.28, p < .001, d ¼ .50, whereas the negative group showed
trials, instructions, presented on the computer screen for 1000 ms, a significant decrease in positive affect t(26) ¼ 3.32, p ¼ .003,
stated “imagine the combination of the next picture and word as if d ¼ .51. There were no significant changes in PANAS scores across
you were actively involved.” The pictureeword cue was displayed training within the neutral condition, t(26) ¼ .14, p ¼ .89.
for 3500 ms, during which participants were asked to shut their
eyes and imagine the stimulus as soon as they saw it. This was Vividness ratings during the training phase
followed by a 1000 ms beep, at which point they had to open their
eyes and make a rating of image vividness on a scale from 1 (not at Vividness rating scores were analyzed in a mixed model ANOVA
all vivid) to 5 (extremely vivid) to promote task compliance in with a within-subjects factor of time (pre- vs. post-training) and
generating imagery. Participants were prompted to make their a between-subjects factor of condition (positive vs. negative vs.
ratings as quickly as possible without thinking too much about their control). There was no main effect of time F(2, 78) ¼ 2.83, p ¼ .096,
answers. Reminder instructions were given in between blocks. h2 ¼ .03 or condition F(2, 78) ¼ .77, p ¼ .47, h2 ¼ .02, but there was
The PANAS was re-administered at the end of the imagery a significant interaction of time with condition, F(2, 78) ¼ 3.45,
generation phase (time 2). Following this there was a 10 min filler p ¼ .037, h2 ¼ .08. This interaction was decomposed by comparing
task before the PANAS was re-administered (time 3). Participants the vividness means over time separately for each group using
then completed valence ratings for 50 pictures each displayed alone paired sample t-tests. Participants in the positive condition showed
for 1500 ms, in a separate random order for each participant, fol- significant increases in vividness ratings t(26) ¼ 2.87, p ¼ .008,
lowed by the behavioural task in which they were given the fishing d ¼ .35 but there were no significant changes in vividness ratings in
game and asked to catch as many fish as they could in 2.5 min. Finally
participants completed the manipulation check ratings and left the
laboratory. At 24 h after the experimental session, the experimenter 10
telephoned each participant and administered the homophone task. 8
Finally, all participants were debriefed and thanked.
6
Mean PANAS change

4
Results
2
An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests. 0

-2
Randomization checks
-4

Comparisons of participant characteristics assigned to each -6


condition revealed no significant differences in terms of gender (11 -8
men in the positive condition, 9 men in the negative condition, and
-10
11 men in the benign condition); age, F(2, 78) ¼ .72, p ¼ .49; BDI-II Positive Negative Control
condition condition condition
at recruitment, F(2, 78) ¼ .16, p ¼ .86; BDI-II at baseline Time 1, F(2,
78) ¼ .19, p ¼ .98; BHS, F(2, 78) ¼ .27, p ¼ .77; SUIS, F(2, 78) ¼ .66, Fig. 1. Mean changes in positive affect (PANAS) from pre to post-training in the each
p ¼ .52; STAI-trait, F(2, 78) ¼ .48, p ¼ .62 (see Table 1); Time 1 condition. Error bars show one standard error of the mean.
A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891 889

either the negative, t(26) ¼ .60, p ¼ .55, d ¼ .06 or the control p ¼ .006, d ¼ .79, and compared to the negative condition
condition t(26) ¼ .45, p ¼ .66, d ¼ .06. (M ¼ .37, SD ¼ .80), t(52) ¼ 3.83, p < .001, d ¼ 1.04. Any difference
between the negative and the control conditions did not reach
Affect after the filler task significance, t(52) ¼ 1.68, p ¼ .10, d ¼ .45.
Paired sample t-tests were used to test whether within each
For the PANAS administrated after the filler task, a one-way group there had been the predicted change in valence ratings from
ANOVA revealed no significant difference between conditions F(2, pre to post-training. As predicted, the positive condition showed
78) ¼ .12, p ¼ .89, thus indicating equivalence prior to administra- a significant increase in valence ratings t(26) ¼ 3.03, p ¼ .005,
tion of the behavioural task. d ¼ .65, whereas the negative group showed a significant decrease
in valence ratings t(26) ¼ 2.56, p ¼ .02, d ¼ .62. There were no
Behavioural task significant changes in valence ratings across training reported
within the control condition, t(26) ¼ .69, p ¼ .50, d ¼ .09.
As described above, there were two different sets of the fishing
game, with equal numbers of participants in each condition using Subjective experience questions
the two sets (15 participants in each condition used the first set and
12 used the second set). Unfortunately, there were unanticipated Subjective experience of the training was recorded at the end of
differences between scores achieved with the two sets (M ¼ 39.19, the first and last training block. Responses were compared between
SD ¼ 4.06 vs. M ¼ 21.62, SD ¼ 5.29), t(79) ¼ 16.44, p < .001, d ¼ 3.73, conditions using one-way ANOVAs. There was no difference
presumably due to physical differences between the two sets, such between groups in ratings for use of imagery, either at the end of
as a stronger fishing rod magnet in one set. After running an ANOVA the first block of training, (M ¼ 7.78, SD ¼ 1.05 in the positive
to check that game set did not interact with training condition condition: M ¼ 7.63, SD ¼ 1.50 in the negative condition; M ¼ 7.48,
F(2) ¼ 1.91, p ¼ .16, h2 ¼ .05, the game set was entered as a covariate SD ¼ 1.44 in the control condition), F(2, 78) ¼ .32, p ¼ .73, or at the
in an ANCOVA. end of the last block of training, (M ¼ 7.67, SD ¼ 1.04 in the positive
The total number of fish caught between the three conditions condition: M ¼ 7.48, SD ¼ 1.34 in the negative condition; M ¼ 7.66,
was compared using an ANCOVA with the game set entered as SD ¼ 1.44 in the control condition), F(2, 78) ¼ .19, p ¼ .83. The use of
a covariate. There was a significant effect of training condition, F(2, field imagery, M ¼ 7.63, SD ¼ 1.40, F(2, 78) ¼ .06, p ¼ .95, ease of the
77) ¼ 12.85, p < .001, h2 ¼ .25. Independent t-tests were used to run task, M ¼ 5.84, SD ¼ 1.73, F(2, 78) ¼ .67, p ¼ .52 and low use of
planned comparisons between the individual conditions, calcu- verbal processing, M ¼ 3.33, SD ¼ 1.92, F(2, 78) ¼ .13, p ¼ .88 was
lated using the estimated means and degrees of freedom adjusted also comparable at the end of the last training block.
appropriately for an ANCOVA (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996, p. 347). As
predicted, participants in the positive training condition caught Discussion
significantly more fish than participants in the control condition
(Madj ¼ 32.41, SD ¼ 4.19 vs. Madj ¼ 29.26, SD ¼ 4.19), t(77) ¼ 2.76, The present results suggest that systematic practice in gener-
p ¼ .007, d ¼ .75. Significantly more fish were caught in the positive ating overtly positive mental imagery from pictureeword cues can
than in the negative condition (M ¼ 26.63, SD ¼ 4.19), t(77) ¼ 5.06, help increase positive affect in people with dysphoria. This finding
p < .001, d ¼ 1.38. In line with predictions, participants in the is relevant to the suggestion that a deficit in positive future-
negative condition caught significantly less fish than those in the oriented imagery in depression may impede recovery from
control condition, t(77) ¼ 2.30, p ¼ .02, d ¼ .63. depressed mood (Holmes, Lang, & Deeprose, 2009; Williams, Healy,
& Ellis, 1999). Mood improved in the positive condition, deterio-
Homophone task rated in the negative condition and remained stable in the control
condition. These differences cannot be attributed to pre-training
A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference in the mood differences, as mood scores were equivalent between
percentage of negative spellings made between groups, F(2, 78) ¼ groups at baseline. Further, the impact of the imagery conditions
5.21, p ¼ .008, h2 ¼ .12. Independent sample t-tests did not show the was corroborated on a task in which participants rated valence for
predicted difference between positive and control conditions associated pictures presented without captions, perhaps due to
t(52) ¼ 1.34, p ¼ .19, d ¼ .36, although participants in the negative effects akin to evaluative conditioning (cf. Holmes, Mathews, et al.,
condition were significantly more negative than those in the 2008; Standage, Ashwin, & Fox, 2009).
control condition (M ¼ 68.79, SD ¼ 12.24 vs. M ¼ 62.70, SD ¼ 9.37), In addition, participants in the positive imagery condition per-
t(52) ¼ 2.05, p ¼ .045, d ¼ .56. Furthermore, as predicted, partici- formed better on a behavioural task (a fishing game) than did those
pants in the positive condition perceived fewer negative meanings in the two other conditions. Differences in performance were
than participants in the negative condition (M ¼ 58.47, SD ¼ 13.48 unlikely to be due to variations in state mood, since this was
vs. M ¼ 68.79, SD ¼ 12.24), t(52) ¼ 2.95, p ¼ .005, d ¼ .80. successfully equalized after the 10-min filler task. Finally, as pre-
dicted there were indications that the procedure influenced
Valence ratings subsequent emotional interpretation. The pictures alone can be
considered a form of ambiguous stimuli, in that the word captions
Valence ratings of the pictures before and after training were steer their interpretation as positive or negative. Forming mental
analyzed in a mixed model ANOVA that revealed a significant images in response to positive pictureeword cues led to congruent
interaction of time with condition F(2, 78) ¼ 9.69, p < .001, h2 ¼ .20. effects on interpretations of homophones made after a 24 h time
There was no main effect of time, F(2, 78) ¼ .02, p ¼ .89, h2 ¼ .00, but delay, with participants in the positive condition perceiving fewer
a main effect of condition, F(2, 78) ¼ 8.42, p < .001, h2 ¼ .18. Change negative meanings than those in the negative condition. However,
scores were computed for each condition and the interaction it should be noted that the difference between positive and control
between time and condition was decomposed using independent conditions did not reach significance.
samples t-tests. In line with expectations, there was a greater As highlighted by MacLeod, Koster, and Fox (2009) in a recent
increase in valence ratings in the positive compared to the control commentary on cognitive bias modification research (CBM), the
condition (M ¼ .43, SD ¼ .73 vs. M ¼ .07, SD ¼ .51), t(52) ¼ 2.89, consequences of most CBM studies have been assessed via self
890 A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891

report measures of state and trait mood, which may be prone to ability to generate positive imagery improved as a result of the
experimenter demand effects and contamination via priming of training. Future research could benefit from investigating this issue
congruent mood descriptors. The present study to some extent using measures such as the Prospective Imagery Task (Stöber,
addresses this methodological limitation. First, affect change 2000). It is possible that our findings of enhanced behavioural
following our procedure was accompanied by parallel changes on engagement and reduced negative interpretations of homophones
an indirect measure used as a manipulation check (e.g. valence following our procedure would have been strengthened by
ratings for associated stimuli). Second, we showed transfer of including a baseline assessment of these variables. However, in
effects to performance on a behavioural task completed within addition to the classical reasons for relying on random allocation to
a controlled experimental setting. Here again, demand effects an experimental administration rather than employing change
remain possible but seem unlikely given the lack of any obvious scores (see Cronbach & Furby, 1970) our use of post-manipulation
similarity between mental imagery generation and the fishing assessment was motivated by the likelihood that both critical
game task. Finally, there was evidence of some transfer of training outcome measures would be influenced by practice or the recall of
to a completely new task (i.e. interpretation of homophones) that previous responses. A challenge for future research is to develop
was quite different from the format of the experimental procedure. experimental tasks that would be more suitable for use in repeated
A concern that might be raised about these results is that the measure designs (e.g. Berna, Lang, Goodwin, & Holmes, 2011).
average scores on the fishing game were unexpectedly found to Despite these reservations, the finding of enhanced perfor-
differ depending on which set was used, probably due to one set mance in the positive mental imagery group may hold clinical
having a more powerful magnet that the other. However, equal relevance. One of the core symptoms of depression is anhedonia
numbers of participants in each condition were assigned to each (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), which refers to both
set, and this factor did not interact significantly with condition, so it a diminished ability to experience pleasure and reduced motivation
seems that set assignment did not influence the main finding of to seek reward. Our findings suggest that practice in generating
differences between conditions. positive mental imagery may be used not only to increase
The current study has a number of limitations. One important emotional responding to positive and rewarding stimuli, but could
limitation is that participants were only mildly depressed and also increase the likelihood of achieving these goals. Future
(beyond the limited data on the homophone task) only relatively research would profitably be extended to test transfer to a greater
immediate effects of imagery were assessed. Thus, we cannot be range of behavioural goals and tasks, and further examine the
certain that more severely depressed individuals would respond underlying mechanism(s) by which generating positive imagery
positively, nor how long any effects would persist. Also, in the improve later behavioural performance.
absence of any comparison between imagery and alternative forms Only one possible method of promoting positive mental
of positive representation we cannot be certain that the present imagery was studied here, and more needs to be known about the
method is the best method of achieving mood and performance comparative effectiveness of alternative methods. This is particu-
improvements; for example, other tasks involving “deep” levels of larly true in the case of individuals with depression, who can fail to
processing might have similar effects. Although this work follows benefit from the emotional lift provided by evoking positive
on other studies using the same imagery generation task in personal events (Joorman et al., 2007; Joormann & Siemer, 2004).
comparison to a verbal procedure (e.g. Holmes, Mathews, et al., Our findings are limited to showing that people with dysphoric
2008), the absence of such a comparison with a non-imagery mood benefit in both mood and behaviour from a task involving
condition in the current study does not allow us to conclude that practice in generating positive imagery. Consequently, it will be
mental imagery per se was responsible for the obtained effects. important to investigate the extent to which the present conclusion
However, several prior comparisons of imagery with verbal repre- can similarly be applied to depressive disorders. The effects of
sentations have consistently found imagery to have stronger effects generating positive mental imagery on increasing positive affect in
on emotional outcomes, with verbal methods sometimes having dysphoria may have potentially useful clinical implications and the
paradoxical negative effects. Further, our data on manipulation current study represents an initial step towards that end.
check questions suggest that our participants engaged in imagery
to a similar extent, irrespective of the condition they were allocated
Acknowledgements
to. For those in the positive condition, images became more vivid as
they went along with the training, which supports our assumption
This research was supported by a Wellcome Trust Intermediate
that the imagery task improved participant’s ability to generate
Clinical Fellowship to Emily Holmes (WT088217) and support from
positive images.
the Lupina Foundation.
Another limitation of the present study was while changes in
The authors would like to thank Abigail Reynolds and students
state positive affect were examined as a result of undertaking the
from the Ruskin School of Art in Oxford for their assistance
experimental task, negative affect was not assessed. Consistent
developing the experimental stimuli.
with previous findings (Watson et al., 1988), positive affect scores in
our sample were significantly correlated with baseline levels of
depression. However, given that positive and negative affect are References
commonly regarded as two separable and independent aspects of
emotional experience, future studies should include a state Aarts, H., Wegner, D. M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2006). On the feeling of doing:
dysphoria and the implicit modulation of authorship ascription. Behaviour
measure of negative as well as positive affect to encompass both Research and Therapy, 44(11), 1621e1627.
components of depressive experience. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
A further limitation is that we used a measure of lasting effects disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric
Association.
(the homophone task) that is potentially susceptible to response
Arntz, A., de Groot, C., & Kindt, M. (2005). Emotional memory is perceptual. Journal
bias effects (Mogg et al., 2006). This task was selected in part of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36(1), 19e34.
because it could be delivered over the telephone, but other Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1998). Beck hopelessness scale manual. San Antonio:
measures such as the Scrambled Sentence Test (Rude, Valdez, Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of Beck depression
Odom, & Ebrahimi, 2003) might have provided a convergent test inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality and
of interpretation bias. Also, we did not test whether participants’ Assessment, 67(3), 588e597.
A. Pictet et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 49 (2011) 885e891 891

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture:
inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112(2), 446e467.
Berna, C., Lang, T. J., Goodwin, G. M., & Holmes, E. A. (2011). Developing a measure of Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues
interpretation bias for depressed mood: an ambiguous scenarios test. Person- and recommendations regarding use of the Beck depression inventory. Cogni-
ality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 349e354. tive Therapy and Research, 11(3), 289e299.
Blackwell, S. E., & Holmes, E. A. (2010). Modifying interpretation and imagination in Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery.
clinical depression: a single case series using cognitive bias modification. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635e642.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 338e350. doi:10.1002/acp.1680. Lemogne, C., Piolino, P., Friszer, S., Astrid, C., Nathalie, G. B., Roland, J., et al. (2006).
Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effect of imagining an event on expectations for the event: Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: specificity, autonoetic
an interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic. Journal of Experimental consciousness, and self-perspective. Consciousness and Cognition,15(2), 258e268.
Social Psychology, 14, 88e96. MacLeod, A. K., & Byrne, A. (1996). Anxiety, depression, and the anticipation of future
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change” e or should positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 286e289.
we? Psychological Bulletin, 74(1), 68e80. MacLeod, A. K., Rose, G. S., & Williams, J. M. G. (1993). Components of hopelessness
Dalgleish, T., & Yiend, J. (2006). The effects of suppressing a negative autobio- about the future in parasuicide. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 441e455.
graphical memory on concurrent intrusions and subsequent autobiographical MacLeod, C., Koster, E. H. W., & Fox, E. (2009). Whither cognitive bias modification
recall in dysphoria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(3), 467e473. research? Commentary on the special section articles. Journal of Abnormal
Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2004). Approach and avoidance goals and plans: Psychology, 118(1), 89e99.
their relationship to anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Mathews, A., Richards, A., & Eysenck, M. (1989). Interpretation of homophones
28(3), 415e432. related to threat in anxiety-states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98(1), 31e34.
Dickson, J. M., Moberly, N. J., & Kinderman, P. (2011). Depressed people are not less Miller, W. R., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Depression and learned helplessness in
motivated by personal goals but are more pessimistic about attaining them. man. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 228e238.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0023665, Advance online Mogg, K., Bradbury, K. E., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Interpretation of ambiguous
publication. information in clinical depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(10),
Fresco, D. M., Frankel, A. N., Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). 1411e1419.
Distinct and overlapping features of rumination and worry: the relationship of Pham, L. B., & Taylor, S. E. (1999). From thought to action: effects of process- versus
cognitive production to negative affective states. Cognitive Therapy and outcome-based mental simulations on performance. Personality and Social
Research, 26(2), 179e188. Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 250e260.
Gregory, W. L., Cialdini, R. B., & Carpenter, K. M. (1982). Self-relevant scenarios as Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., & Fava, M. (2008). Reduced
mediators of likelihood estimates and compliance e does imagining make it so. hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probalistic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(1), 89e99. reward task. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(1), 76e87.
Henriques, J. B., Glowacki, J. M., & Davidson, R. (1994). Reward fails to alter response Pysczcynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the
bias in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 460e466. depressive self-focusing style: a self-awareness theory of reactive depression.
Hirsch, C. R., Hayes, S., & Mathews, A. (2009). Looking on the bright side: Accessing Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122e138.
benign meanings reduces worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 44e54. Reinecke, A., Deeprose, C., & Holmes, E. A. Is interpretation bias causal in depressed
Holmes, E. A., Coughtrey, A. E., & Connor, A. (2008). Looking at or through rose- mood? Cognitive bias modification and transfer to implicit priming and
tinted glasses? Imagery perspective and positive mood. Emotion, 8(6), 875e879. behaviour, submitted for publication.
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Mental imagery and emotion in Reisberg, D., Pearson, D. G., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2003). Intuitions and introspections
treatment across disorders: using the example of depression. Cognitive Behav- about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator’s
iour Therapy, 38(1), 21e28. theoretical views. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 147e160.
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., Moulds, M. L., & Steele, A. M. (2008). Prospective and Rude, S. S., Valdez, C. R., Odom, S., & Ebrahimi, A. (2003). Negative cognitive biases
positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, predict subsequent depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(4), 415e429.
46(8), 976e981. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zucclotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pitts-
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing interpretation bias burgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
modification as a ‘cognitive vaccine’ for depressed mood e imagining positive Sedek, G., & von Hecker, U. (2004). Effects of subclinical depression and aging on
events makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. Journal of generative reasoning about linear orders: same or different processing limita-
Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 76e88. tions? Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 133(2), 237e260, [Review].
Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: a special rela- Standage, H., Ashwin, C., & Fox, E. (2009). Comparing visual and auditory presen-
tionship? Emotion, 5(4), 489e497. tation for the modification of interpretation bias. Journal of Behavior Therapy
Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disor- and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 558e570.
ders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(3), 349e362. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001. Stöber, J. (2000). Prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression: replication and
Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Dalgleish, T., & Mackintosh, B. (2006). Positive inter- methodological extension. Cognition & Emotion, 14(5), 725e729.
pretation training: effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New
mood. Behavior Therapy, 37(3), 237e247. York: Harper Collins.
Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The causal effect Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their
of mental imagery on emotion assessed using pictureeword cues. Emotion, 8(3), relation to anxiety and depressive-disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
395e409. 97(3), 346e353.
Hopko, D. R., Armento, M. E. A., Cantu, M. S., Chambers, L. L., & Lejuez, C. W. (2003). Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellengen, A. (1988). Development and validations of brief
The use of daily diaries to assess the relations among mood state, overt measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality
behavior, and reward values of activities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063e1070.
1137e1148. Wenzlaff, R. M., & Bates, D. E. (1998). Unmasking a cognitive vulnerability to
Hopko, D. R., & Mullane, C. M. (2008). Exploring the relation of depression and overt depression: how lapses in mental control reveal depressive thinking. Journal of
behavior with daily diaries. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1085e1089. Personality and Social Psychology, 75(6), 1559e1571.
Jacobson, N. S., Martell, C. R., & Dimidjian, S. (2001). Behavioral activation treatment Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G., & MacLeod, A. K. (1996).
for depression: returning to contextual roots. Clinical Psychology: Science and The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future.
Practice, 8(3), 255e270. Memory & Cognition, 24(1), 116e125.
Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H. G., & Ellis, N. C. (1999). The effect of imageability and
dysphoria: difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of predicability of cues in autobiographical memory. Quarterly Journal of Experi-
Abnormal Psychology, 113(2), 179e188. mental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 555e579.
Joorman, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Mood regulation in depression: Yiend, J., Mackintosh, B., & Mathews, A. (2005). The enduring consequences of
differential effects of distraction and recall in happy memories on sad mood. experimentally induced biases in interpretation. Behaviour Research and
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 16, 484e490. Therapy, 43(6), 779e797.

You might also like