Retelling Lives. Narrative Style and Stability Og Highly Emotional Events Over Time

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Qualitative Psychology

© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 6, No. 2, 156 –166


2326-3601/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/qup0000150

Retelling Lives: Narrative Style and Stability of Highly Emotional


Events Over Time

Robyn Fivush Tilmann Habermas


Emory University Goethe University Frankfurt
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Elaine Reese
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University of Otago

Narrative forms are a tool for organizing lived experience, reflecting both the structure
of the experience in the world, and the individual’s reactions to that experience; Labov
(1972) termed these as referential and evaluative dimensions. We investigated how
four individuals narratively structure their stories across these formal narrative com-
ponents in their high- and low-point narratives in relation to stability of life story event
selection. Two participants showed a consistent, referentially elaborated and/or evalu-
atively dramatic narrative style, and also narrated the same culturally canonically
scripted events over time. In contrast, two participants displayed less referentially
elaborate, evaluative narrative styles which were specifically less consistent in the
low-point narratives, and each narrated different high and low points over time. We
posit that individual and cultural differences in the stability of the content of life stories
might be related to the consistency of narrative style used for the life story. We discuss
the possible roles of conventionality of life courses, canonicity of life stories, individ-
ualistic versus communal outlook, and rehearsal of a stock of life story anecdotes.

Keywords: narrative, autobiography, life story

The narrative turn in psychology focuses on dividuals narrate informs personality, traits,
the ways in which humans create meaning by motivations, and motifs (McAdams, 1990,
storying their lives. Human cognition carves the 2015). We argue that how people narrate is
flow of lived experience into meaningful tem- equally informative; how individuals structure
poral units with beginnings, middles, and ends, their stories and integrate referential and evalu-
stories that provide a narrative structure to hu- ative dimensions informs how humans use nar-
man understanding (Bruner, 1990; Ricoeur, rative to create meaning more broadly. We ex-
1980; Sarbin, 1986). If and how each individual plore how formally defined individual narrative
constructs a life story that is coherent and con- styles and their consistency across narrated
sistent is a pressing issue for exploring how events and time relates to the stability of events
individuals construct identity; deep qualitative selected as biographically salient, and how both
analyses allow more complex and nuanced un- express and inform identity, especially around
derstanding of this process. Certainly, what in- highly emotional pivotal events, such as high
points and low points.

Theoretical Frameworks
Robyn Fivush, Department of Psychology, Emory Uni-
versity; Tilmann Habermas, Department of Psychology, As cognitive developmentalists, our theoret-
Goethe University Frankfurt; Elaine Reese, Department of ical frame highlights how cognitive skills and
Psychology, University of Otago. tools are used to organize information about
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Robyn Fivush, Department of Psychology, Em-
personal experience, and how this process un-
ory University, 36 Kilgo Circle, Atlanta, GA 30030. E-mail: folds over developmental time (e.g., Fivush,
psyrf@emory.edu Booker, & Graci, 2017; Habermas & Reese,
156
NARRATIVE STYLE AND STABILITY 157

2015; Reese et al., 2011). Stemming from Vy- action, and resolution (Labov, 2008). This for-
gotsky’s (1978) sociocultural perspective, we mal description of narrative raises the question
argue that cultures provide tools for organizing of individual narrative style (McLean, Pasu-
thought, and narratives are one of the critical pathi, Greenhoot, & Fivush, 2017). Narrators
linguistic tools provided universally across cul- with a consistent narrative style provide similar
tures (Bruner, 1990; Fivush & Nelson, 2004; narrative forms, expressed as similar levels of
Gottschall, 2012). Narrative tools are learned referential and/or evaluative detail narrated
through guided participatory activity across de- within a similar macrostructure across their
velopment; as children are drawn into and de- event narratives. Narrators with a less consis-
velop these culturally mediated narrative forms, tent, more variable style provide different levels
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

they begin to understand their experiences and of referential and/or evaluative detail and dif-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

themselves in alignment with these forms (Fi- fering macrostructure across events and time.
vush, Habermas, Waters, & Zaman, 2011; Re- Obviously, the form of the narrative must
ese, Yan, Jack, & Hayne, 2010; Wang, 2013). partly rely on the narrative arc of the experi-
Thus, how individuals narrate their personal enced event. Experiences that are difficult or
experiences is constructed in sociocultural con- negative often have a canonical story structure
texts, and these narrative forms come to define of a problem besetting the protagonist that re-
individual experience. quires some action for resolution. Positive ex-
Our cognitive-developmental stance focuses periences, in contrast, are usually not centered
on processes of change over time. We under- on a problem that needs to be resolved, and the
stand how life stories originate from the rudi- resulting narrative structure is likely to be flat-
mentary personal narratives of early childhood, ter, with less of a narrative arc from problem, to
and continue to evolve as developmental abili- attempted solution, to resolution (Fivush, Haz-
ties, tasks, and challenges emerge across the life zard, Sales, Sarfati, & Brown, 2003; Fivush,
span (Fivush et al., 2017; Reese et al., 2011). Sales, & Bohanek, 2008). Thus, narrative style
Our sociocultural stance focuses on how coher- may vary as a function of event valence. To
ent life stories in adulthood arise from individ- probe these questions, we investigated how our
ual differences in the early reminiscing environ- four case studies expressed their individual nar-
ment with parents, and that narrators will vary rative style across two events of high emotional
in their styles in accordance with their cultural impact, the high-point and the low-point event,
backgrounds, life experiences, and identities. at the first and second life story interviews. We
Our focus is on individual differences in how acknowledge that limiting our analyses to these
cultural identities may inform narrative struc- narratives may limit our ability to generalize
ture across development. In addition, Haber- about these individuals. However, our goal is to
mas’ psychoanalytic training tuned us to the elucidate how narrative style might vary with
myriad unconscious meanings that surface the stability of life stories rather than to describe
forms of speech may implicate. At all points, specific individuals (e.g., Josselson, 2011). In
our focus is on how meaning is implied through line with our theoretical framework, we concep-
narrative form. tualized narrative style in three ways: (a) by
Narratives are a cultural tool that reflects both characterizing the macrostructure of each nar-
the structure of the experience in the world and rative in terms of canonical narrative compo-
the individual’s reactions to that experience in nents; (b) by considering microanalytic ele-
ways that create human meaning. Labov termed ments concerning the referential function; and,
these the referential and evaluative dimensions (c) by considering the evaluative function.
of narrative (Labov, 1972, 2008; Labov &
Waletzky, 1967/1997; Peterson & McCabe, Three Elements of Narrative Style
1983; see Reese et al., 2011 for a review). An
understandable and compelling narrative ad- Narrative Macrostructure
dresses the question of “what happened?” (ref-
erential information) and “why does it matter?” A personal narrative prototypically begins
(evaluative information), and relates this infor- with orienting information that introduces the
mation within a more holistic narrative macro- main characters and locates the event in time
structure that includes orientation, complicating and place (e.g., the who, when, and where of the
158 FIVUSH, HABERMAS, AND REESE

narrative; Labov, 2008). Sometimes this orien- as an internal monologue. These dramatizing
tation is preceded by an abstract or overview of devices draw listeners into the protagonist’s ex-
the event. The following complication section periential perspective. Finally, narratives that
details the complicating event that breaches include internal state language that describes
normality. Sometimes a series of actions lead up thoughts and emotions, desires and perceptions
to a climax that is followed by a resolution, help create a narrative that is both meaningful
which may be positive or negative, sometimes and compelling (Fivush & Baker-Ward, 2005).
ending with a coda that brings the temporal These various evaluative aspects of narratives
setting back into the present. Evaluating infor- mark the multiple subjective perspectives from
mation (emotions, emphasis, and insight) is es- which events are interpreted, and mark a basic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sential for conveying the importance of the level of self-reflection, a focus not just on what
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

event to the narrator, and why the event is happened but how one interprets, evaluates, and
reportable to others. understands the event. Narratives without any
subjective perspective may leave listeners won-
Referential Aspect dering about the narrator’s state of mind (Hab-
ermas & Diel, 2010).
The constitutive parts of narratives are narra-
Integrating referential and evaluative narra-
tive clauses, defined by Labov (2008) as inde-
tive aspects is an indication that the narrator
pendent clauses joined by a temporal juncture
“owns” the experience, that the experience is
that together answer the implicit question “And
singular and unique to the self (Fivush, 2001,
then what happened?” Generally, the order of
2012). Adding others’ subjective perspectives
narrative clauses reflects the order of the actual
renders the narrative more balanced and com-
events. Typically, these clauses begin with “and
plete (Habermas, 2006). However, subjective
then . . .”. The sequence of clauses can be
perspectives may be represented in narrative but
moved only to a limited degree without chang-
turned impersonal by using impersonal subjects
ing the meaning. Narrative clauses render nar-
like one or a generic you and by using an
ratives vivid by simulating the sequence of
atemporal present tense, placing the narrator’s
events, drawing listeners into the storyworld.
reactions in a more communal, shared context
Lengthier narratives are often more referentially
that describes a universal experience (e.g., “You
dense and elaborated.
are just so proud.”). Such impersonal and atem-
Evaluative Aspect poral perspectives may also serve defensive
ends (Habermas, 2006) or they may simply re-
In addition to what happened, good narratives flect the narrator’s emotional distance from the
include why it matters. This information is car- events, which tends to increase with time (Hab-
ried through multiple forms of evaluative de- ermas & Berger, 2011).
vices that provide the narrators’ and others’
subjective perspectives onto the narrated events. Narrative Style in the Narratives of High
At the structural level, the simple use of evalu- and Low Points
ative clauses in the narrative (e.g., “And here’s
the worst part”), provides narrative description We examined the high-point and low-point
of why this matters. In addition, casting events narratives of two low repeaters and two high
in the historical present is a way to create a repeaters at the first and second life story inter-
sense of immediacy by moving the narrative out views, as described in the introduction to this
of the past and into the present moment (Schif- special issue, in order to investigate individual
frin, 1981). The use of dialogue in personal narrative styles in terms of narrative structure,
narratives lends an air of objectivity to the nar- referential and evaluative aspects, and of their
rative and is an important form of self- consistency. These participants were drawn
presentation (Bakhtin, 1981; Labov, 2008; Wal- from a larger study conducted at Northwestern
ton & Walton-Wetzel, 2013). Direct speech is a University (Dan McAdams, PI), and approved
more vivid form of dialogue than indirect by that institutions’ internal review board (IRB
speech and creates a dramatic telling by replay- STU00001801-MODCR0002). We began our
ing the speakers’ voices (Tannen, 1986). Direct analysis by discussing at length what each of
speech can also be used to dramatize thoughts these narrative dimensions were, and how they
NARRATIVE STYLE AND STABILITY 159

could be identified in the narratives. We then defeating, [not] going anywhere relationship. ‘Cause
independently identified specific instances he, he doesn’t know how to commit, and he doesn’t
know how to tell himself the truth. It’s my responsi-
within the narratives, shared our descriptions bility for me to tell myself the truth.”
with each other, discussed and refined our def-
initions, and repeated this process. Through it- In Year 5, evaluations in the high-point story
erative readings and discussions, we mutually focused on the bad character of her male col-
refined our definitions and descriptions of what leagues, none of whom had attended her award
counted as elements of the narrative macro- ceremony. The Year 5 low-point narrative about
structure, referential and evaluative clauses. Ul- the voluntary entering of a psychiatric ward and
timately, we described each narrative through the process of discovering her addiction prob-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mutual consensus of each of these elements. We lem stands out, because it creates immediacy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

present the analyses in two phases. We first and perspective. Renee dramatized an episode
describe each participant individually, describ- with the medical staff in which she explained
ing the use of these narrative devices across that she was mentally sane but addicted and
events and time, and summarizing each partic- therefore needed rehab by using the historical
ipant’s narrative style. We present brief extracts present and direct speech and internal mono-
to convey the flavor of each narrator’s style. In logue. She reported only her own direct speech
the second stage, we compare across partici- and thought, while the other characters were
pants to provide a larger interpretative frame- depicted as stupid. Also in the Year 1 low-point
work for understanding consistency and vari- and the Year 5 high-point stories Renee used
ability in narrative style. some mental verbs, but only from her own
perspective. She mostly expressed negative
Renee (Low Repeater) emotions which were linked to a contrast be-
tween her own insight and the ignorance of the
Narrative macrostructure. At the first life
men around her.
story interview, Renee’s hig and lowpoint nar-
Renee’s narrative style. Across events and
ratives were very short (200 words each) and
time, Renee displayed very little narrativity;
were still quite short at the second life story
most of the stories consist of her own reflec-
interview (500 words). Other than the Year 5
low point, which was narrated more extensively tions, which document a struggle to understand
after a probe, responses contain no orientation better how to lead a good life. The few others’
section, and, at most, very brief complication perspectives were only mentioned to contrast
sections. Overall, Renee’s narratives do not ex- their incompetence (husband) and ignorance
hibit a compelling canonical narrative structure. (colleagues, medical personnel) to the protago-
Referential and evaluative aspects. All nist’s knowing better. This style conveys her
four narratives contained few narrative clauses, sense of having to fight her way through life by
but all contained lengthy evaluative compo- relying only on herself and not the family, hus-
nents. In Year 1, the high-point story contained band, or health professionals; she could rely
only a brief evaluation of the AA sponsor. Eval- only on her AA sponsor, who, however, re-
uations in the low-point narrative focused on the mains extremely undernarrated. Overall, her
shortcomings of the husband’s character and her narratives display little narrative structure or
self-criticism for not having been realistic about drama, with the exception of the Year 5 low
them: point when she was actively probed and did
provide a dramatic narrative.
When my husband decided to walk out and called me
a nag. This was 2013 in our home but my house, and
I was quite devastated that he had no value on what I Abigail (Low Repeater)
had done for him. It meant absolutely nothing. I think
I had put unrealistic expectations on him and myself Narrative macrostructure. Abigail’s
believing that he would step up to the plate and be a high-point narratives are somewhat longer
man. I was quite disillusional in believing that, not (500 – 600 words) than her low-point stories
actually putting things in their proper perspective. I (400 words). This is highly atypical because
found that I wasn’t being honest about who he really
was and, you know. I found that it was my fault, later,
usually more effort is invested in narrating neg-
as much as it was his [ . . . ]. And it’s time for me to ative than positive events (Baumeister, Brat-
smarten up and get out of this vicious cycle of self- slavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). All stories
160 FIVUSH, HABERMAS, AND REESE

are about specific events as requested, and all being a good person who acts appropriately. By
contain narrative clauses. They have very brief narrating specific events, Abigail fulfilled the
orienting sections, brief complication sections interviewer’s expectations, but she did not in-
with few narrative clauses, mainly regarding the dulge in the past experience by dramatizing, or
actions of others, no resolutions, and relatively by delving into emotions and thoughts of the
long final evaluation sections. past.
Referential and evaluative aspects. The
narrative clauses in the high-point stories nar- Dennis (High Repeater)
rate a detailed sequence of events at each of her
sons’ celebrations, and the evaluations highlight Narrative macrostructure. All of Dennis’
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the significance of her sons’ achievements, narratives are longer (800 words) compared to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

whereas the low-point stories contain very few Renee’s and Abigail’s, and the low-point story
narrative clauses. There is no use of the histor- in Year 5 is even longer (1,500 words). All
ical present or direct speech to create drama in narratives contain an orientation, complication,
any of the stories. The high-point stories have and resolution section, some even a coda. In the
an impersonal quality, because Abigail used high-point story of the first kiss, the sequence of
mental state language mostly to present others’ obstacles creates an increasing tension, with a
evaluative perspectives: those of her sons, a climax before the resolution (the kiss), resem-
hypothetical perspective of her grandparents, bling the quest genre (Propp, 1968). The low-
and a generalized parents’ perspective (how point narratives pile one bad episode unto the
parents generally feel). She used a generic sec- next. The Year 5 story of Dennis’s arrest con-
ond person “you” to express her own emotions tains a series of five increasingly severe com-
as those of a generalized other (“You feel part plicating events. We use italics for the words
of your country”). The low-point stories, in that were repeated verbatim from the first inter-
contrast, do contain first-person perspectives, view. This is the narrative of the second com-
mostly emotions. They are evenly mixed with plication of being involved in a theft:
her brother’s and mother’s perspectives in the We were driving around in my mom’s station wagon
low-point story at Year 1 about the death of her one night, and one of these guys had had his mini bike
brother: being worked at in a motorcycle shop right on U.S.
Route 6 [ . . . ]. And we drive by there, and he goes,
The moment in time that I’d like to talk about is the last “Oh, that fucker, you know, he did this and that,” and
time that my husband and I went to visit him and he there’s a mini bike out leaning against the wall. He
was bedridden, very deteriorated. And we were feeding goes “Pull over,” and, you know, I was not one of the
him small amounts of liquid, when he was lying in bed. dominant guys in this groups, more quite to the other
It was a very difficult memory. I think what I was end. So I did. This guy runs across the street, steals the
feeling as a loss of— of the— of the person that was my mini bike, and throws it in the back of my mom’s
brother and of the unrealized dreams that he will— station wagon, and we take off.
would never—would never have in his life, and also
the sadness, because my mother was still living. I knew Referential and evaluative aspects. All
that he was going to pass away and that my mother was narratives abound with narrative clauses, detail-
still living and still aware of how much he had suffered ing the sequence of complicating events step by
and was going to be burying her child.
step. They use the dramatizing devices of direct
The low-point narrative at Year 5 stands out speech and internal monologue, as well as his-
because it was much longer and elaborated on torical present, which marks the climax. The
Abigail’s own emotions. low-point story at Year 5 uses historical present
Abigail’s narrative style. All the narra- as well as direct speech by gang members (see
tives purvey a strong sense of conforming to extract above) and later by the authorities to
norms and others’ expectations, behind which mark the most intense complications. Some-
the narrator’s individuality, and even more so, what surprisingly, the abundance of narrative
her subjective experiences are mostly hidden. and dramatic elements is counterbalanced by a
This impression is conveyed by the stress on relative dearth of internal states across narra-
positive events through narrative clauses, the tives.
focus on how everybody else feels (both her Dennis’ narrative style. Dennis’ narrative
family members and the more generalized com- style differs starkly from Renee’s and Abigail’s
munity), and the narrator’s defense of herself as styles. Dennis told dramatic stories and built a
NARRATIVE STYLE AND STABILITY 161

narrative arc. He repeated the events by restag- of just sleepy. And my favorite show happened to be
ing them, leaving to the listener to infer his on. So it’s like 8:30 on Thursday which is Soap and I
loved Soap. And Tom [her husband] loved Soap too, so
subjective perspective. This is facilitated by the he would have understood this. And, you know, I just
dramatic narrative style and the good narrative looked at her, I said, “Okay, you know, we have all the
composition. rest of our lives to talk so I’m going to just watch this
In addition to consistency of narrative style, show for a half hour and when I’m done we’ll have a
there is also consistency of referential informa- great time together. So if you could just, you know, do
your sleeping thing or whatever you do, you babies,”
tion across these stories. Comparing the first and I just held her. And, you know, the world was
kiss and the arrest story (Year 1 negative child- perfect. I was hysterically laughing at whatever was on
hood memory, Year 5 low point) across the two TV. I knew Tom was happy.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tellings, the most central narrative clauses con-


Referential and evaluative aspects. The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tain verbatim repetitions between retellings


(marked in italics). This suggests that the stories main episode of the birth story contains no
have been told over and over again, which may narrative clauses, although in Year 1 several
have contributed to their elaboration (cf. Nor- internal monologues dramatize thoughts about
rick, 1998). Still there is change over time. The the future and advice by her mother in direct
second tellings appear to be a little less drama- speech. The second episode of watching the
tized and to be narrated with a little more hind- show (cf. extract above) was told with few
sight, indicated by the addition of subsequent narrative clauses and stretches of a blend of
narrative episodes, such as that Dennis reacted direct speech and internal monologue directed
differently from his father when confronted at the baby. The cancer story, in contrast, is
with a comparable situation by his daughter. replete with narrative clauses, most of which
introduce direct speech of both protagonist and
Leah (High Repeater) doctor/husband in “I said— he said” dialogues,
with some internal monologue in between. The
Narrative macrostructure. Similar to first two episodes of the low point are highly
Dennis, Leah’s low point was narrated at dramatized.
greater length than the high point (1,400 vs. All four of Leah’s narratives are rich in in-
1,000 words), and both shrank by over a third in ternal state language, especially in emotions,
Year 5. All narratives have a brief orienting and the low-point stories also in cognitions. The
section (except for Year 1 low point) and a representation of subjective experiencing is fo-
complication section, but no resolution. The cused on the protagonist, but also includes the
low-point cancer diagnosis story is complex, family (and the daughter) in the birth story and
consisting of three episodes. The birth narrative, the doctor in the cancer diagnosis story. The
in contrast, consists only of the positive com- focus is on the past experience, not on hind-
plication episode of the birth except for a brief sight.
orienting section, and of a second episode of Leah’s narrative style. Like Dennis, Leah
taking a break watching a sitcom: narrated at length, but her style is different,
So everyone left and I’m in the hospital, which I had especially in the high-point story. She focused
never really been before that I could remember. And I on her own past thoughts and accompanying
wasn’t scared or anything but I knew that life had emotions, but less on the evolving action, which
changed now forever. I was 24 and someone was going remains without resolution. The thoughts
to call me Mom and it [crying], geez. I mean who
would do that? Who would call me Mom, you know? mostly elaborate possible futures. The birth
What was she in for, this poor child? But I never had story main episode lacks a temporal sequence.
any problem with it. My mom even said to me like This is reinforced by a peculiar stylistic feature,
“You just bathe her and feed her, and you’ll see.” I, I which is the use of present tense together with
was not freaked out by her at all. I, you know, I had to
go through the whole breast feeding thing. [ . . . ] And, generic syntactic subjects like “the nurses” or a
okay, so this was a couple hours later, and they bring generic “you”: “in that situation nurses always
her to me and they, you know, she’s sleeping or some- say, doctors always do, mothers always feel
thing or she was about to sleep. They’re always kind of . . .” This way of speaking distances from a
sleeping and, you know, they want you to start trying
to nurse which, you know, was not my biggest talent
specific past sequence of events, and creates a
but I didn’t know that at the time. And I was going to kind of impressionistic painting of an emotional
do that and but she was kind of—I think she was sort state of mind. Like Dennis’ narratives, there are
162 FIVUSH, HABERMAS, AND REESE

several instances of verbatim or near verbatim Thus, for Dennis and Leah the differentiation
repetitions across both retellings (e.g., the between high- and low-point narratives is con-
speech to her daughter), probably indicating sistent over time, and within this differentiation
that both stories had been told repeatedly. In by event valence, each also showed a consistent
fact, the positive event is not really narrated, but elaborated and dramatic narrative style across
told as a long internal monologue. In contrast, events and time.
the negative event is narrated and dramatized, In contrast, both Renee and Abigail narrated
which only exaggerates the general difference the low-point event at the second interview
between narratives of positive and negative ex- quite differently than the other three narratives,
periences (e.g., Fivush & Baker-Ward, 2005). which did not substantially differ from each
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

other. Thus, there is both less differentiation


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Narrative Style and the Stability of the between high-point and low-point events and
Events Selected for the Life Story Interview less consistency over time in narrative style,
especially for the low-point events. In general,
Deep descriptions of each of the four narra- Renee consistently undernarrated, telling short
tor’s styles provide insight into the consistency narratives that barely tell what happened and
of individual narrative style in relation to the contain few dramatic devices, but focus on her
stability of choosing events from the life story. more general evaluative stance toward her life.
Dennis and Leah, the two high repeaters, told There was a high focus on negative emotion
narratives that are longer and more elaborate in across all narratives even though different
terms of narrative structure and drama than Re- events were selected to narrate. Abigail, in con-
nee and Abigail, the two low repeaters. But trast, provided elaborated referential details for
Dennis and Leah differ from each other as well; the positive events, with almost no self-
Dennis focused on the landscape of action, and evaluations. The mental states mentioned are
Leah on the landscape of consciousness those of others or impersonal. This was also true
(Bruner, 1990). Dennis consistently used an ac- for the first interview low point; only the second
tion-centered, dramatizing narrative style, in- interview low point differs in being focused on
ducing the protagonist’s perspective rather than her own emotions due to her good name being
explicitly naming it in terms of mental states, maligned by her sister-in-law. Thus, Dennis and
whereas Leah consistently focused on naming Leah showed high levels of narrativity overall,
the protagonist’s subjective experience via her consistency across time in how they narrated
own inner monologue, adding dramatic narra- positive and negative events, but also consis-
tive elements mostly in the negative stories. tently differentiated at each time point how they
Thus, both narrated in an elaborate, complex, narrated positive and negative events. In con-
and dramatic way, but differed in their individ- trast, Renee and Abigail showed lower narrativ-
ual narrative forms. ity overall than Dennis and Leah, and less con-
Of note, Dennis and Leah also differentiated sistency over time in how they narrated positive
between narratives of positive and negative ex- and negative events, as well as less consistent
periences. Their narratives of negative experi- differentiation between positive and negative
ence are longer and more complex than narra- narratives.
tives of positive experiences, both in terms of Why might the two narrators who selected
number of episodes included, and use of dra- the same events show a more dramatic and
matic narrative devices, supporting previous complex—as well as more consistent—narra-
theory and research on differences in event nar- tive style than the two narrators who did not
ratives as a function of valence (Fivush, Bohanek, select the same events? Of note, the events
Zaman, & Grapin, 2012; Fivush & Baker-Ward, chosen repeatedly were older, stemming from
2005; Fivush et al., 2008). Because negative ex- adolescence or early adulthood. Events which
periences create complications, they lend them- occur in emerging and early adulthood are iden-
selves to a more dramatic narration than posi- tity-defining (Erikson, 1968) and often conform
tive events. Narrative variability between highly to a normative cultural life script (Rubin &
positive and negative experiences may render Berntsen, 2003). Indeed, the consistently nar-
the life story more emotionally differentiated, rated events were meeting a romantic partner
creating a sense of emotional ups and downs. and having a first child, clearly life-defining and
NARRATIVE STYLE AND STABILITY 163

life-changing events that are part of the norma- consequence may be, first, that Renee may have
tive cultural life script. The exception was Le- less of a cultural template to guide a stable
ah’s cancer narrative, which occurred 7 year selection of events for her life story, and, sec-
earlier. Diagnosis of a severe illness is a major ond, that there is more focus on the present and
non-normative event; being a cancer survivor is future than on a cultivated past which is difficult
self-defining for most people and is expected to to adapt to the ever-changing fortunes of her
be included in life narratives (Rubin & Bern- life. Thus, her narrative style about specific
tsen, 2003). These two narrators are very much highly emotional experiences is sparse, with
narrating culturally defining moments within little drama or tension (although we note that
normative lives. her narratives contain many generalized evalu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Indeed, looking at the overarching life narra- ative statements about the events and people in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive, Leah and Dennis tell a typical life story, her life). She does not seem to “collect” specific
focusing on their individual development in ad- stories that define a stable sense of self over
olescence and early adulthood as the formative time. Her narratives are similar to life narratives
years, followed by a stable marriage, children, told by young Black women living highly chal-
and profession. Their lives are very much in lenging lives in the southeast United States;
conformity with the general middle-class Amer- their personal narratives are underdeveloped
ican life story of success. They follow the cul- (Sales, Merrill, & Fivush, 2013, in contrast to
tural norm of modern Western societies to fash- young Black children’s superior oral narrative
ion and cultivate the self by binding together skills in fictional narrative tasks, Curenton,
diverse life events into an individualized and yet 2011; Gee, 1989; Reese, Leyva, Sparks, &
coherent and continuous developmental story of Grolnick, 2010). Thus, we speculate that spec-
one’s life (Eakin, 2005; Giddens, 1991). Main- ificity and consistency in telling life stories
taining self-continuity across change requires might be difficult for those who have experi-
integrating biographical breaks into the life enced many adverse life events.
story by explicating and motivating transitions In contrast, Abigail appears to have a middle-
(Habermas & Köber, 2015). Thus, these narra- class background, but differs from Leah and
tives of life-changing events become pivot Dennis in conceiving herself not in an individ-
points in the life story. The narratives convey a ualized fashion, but primarily as a part of her
sense that they have been told many times be- family and community. Others and relationships
fore, having been elaborated to a highly tellable are more important to her than cultivating the
form (cf. Norrick, 1998). Dennis and Leah pro- self, which shows in the choice of events and of
vide both stable events and a dramatic narrative subjective perspectives. If the life story is made
style, perhaps because their lives conform to up by events of significant others, it fits less
cultural norms—they are living the culturally easily into a canonical Western life script, and
normative life script. events are less integrated into a stable life story.
This contrasts with both Renee and Abigail. Abigail’s life story is reminiscent of the life
Renee appears to have led a less scripted life. stories collected by Hatiboğlu Altunnar in Tur-
Indeed, Black Americans tend to have fewer key, which contained more events of family
resources and experience structural and endur- members and were less globally coherent than
ing discrimination, which is a recurring topic in those of Germans (Altunnar & Habermas, 2018;
Renee’s life story. Black Americans have less cf. Wang, 2013). Overall, Abigail’s stories are
standardized normative expectations about the characterized by a communal, less individualis-
life course (Coleman, 2014; see McLean & tic orientation and of less scripted events, with a
Syed, 2015). Renee’s life may even be atypical good deal less focus on her own subjective
for a Black woman from an apparently lower- perspective. When a life is defined through oth-
income background, because she excelled at ers, one’s own subjective perspective and nor-
school and succeeded in a White male- mative life events—which render a life consis-
dominated, well-paying technical job. Her life tent and coherent over time—may be lessened,
did not resemble a middle-class life script be- leading to a more variable selection of events in
cause of her premature familial responsibilities, the life story. This is supported by the finding
her addiction, the chaotic family relations, vio- that if life narratives contain fewer life script
lence, and unreliable love relationships. The events and are overall less coherent, the selec-
164 FIVUSH, HABERMAS, AND REESE

tion of life story events is less stable over time cultural expectations both give voice to some
(Köber & Habermas, 2017). forms of experiences, and silence others (Fi-
vush, 2010).
Concluding Comments Of course, we must be careful in drawing
universal implications of these differing styles.
Our analyses focused on the relation between We examined only high-point and low-point
narrative styles and their consistency across narratives within the context of a life story
events and time with the stability of selecting interview; thus, we were privileging a life story
the same high- and low-point events. High life schema as an organizational framework. Fur-
story event repeaters expressed more structur- thermore, as we ourselves are members of mid-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ally complex and dramatic narratives than did dle-class European cultures, we may resonate to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

low repeaters, and high repeaters differentiated the more dramatic narrative style of the high
between high-point and low-point events to a repeaters and argue that this style is somehow
greater extent and more consistently than did superior in terms of creating identity and stabil-
low repeaters. Intriguingly, the two high repeat- ity in a life, but the low repeaters in this study
ers were living the more typical American cul- belie this interpretation. The less consistent nar-
tural life script, and narrated repeated events rating of the low points across tellings could, for
that are culturally defined as prototypically piv- example, indicate that in the second life story
otal in a life. The two individuals who were not interview Renee and Abigail opened up more
stable in the events selected lived somewhat and told more emotionally pressing events in a
different lives—a Black American woman who more authentic way, whereas Dennis and Leah
overcame addiction and lived a quite unpredict- repeated the ever-same stories. Also, given the
able life, and a White woman from an immi- exigencies of their lives, Renee’s and Abigail’s
grant family who expressed a less individualis- narrative styles may be better adapted to their
tic, more communal life perspective. These circumstances, namely to unpredictably chang-
women expressed narrative styles with less ing life circumstances and to an other-centered
structure, less drama, and less subjective per- lifestyle. As our sociocultural theoretical frame
spective. Thus, stability of event selection and demands, we must interpret individual lives in
consistency of narrative style may be facilitated relation to the cultural contexts in which those
by conformity to a White Western canonical life lives unfold; Renee and Abigail’s less complex
structure that invites narrators to tell the same narratives conveyed as much about themselves
self-defining events over and over again, and through their narrative evaluations as did the
prizes individualism through elaborating and two individuals telling highly structured dra-
dramatizing, perhaps stabilizing the narratives matic stories. All of these possibilities call for a
in the process of repeated telling. more nuanced understanding of how identities
Our analyses reveal the deep interleaving of are created within cultural contexts that facili-
narrative structure with culturally mediated ca- tate and constrain how lives are lived and ex-
nonical expectations of what a life is and how it periences are narrated.
should be lived. Individuals conforming to a
typical life script have both the life experiences
and the culturally available narrative forms for References
expressing these experiences in complex narra-
tive ways that rely on a certain form of shared Altunnar, N. H., & Habermas, T. (2018). Life narra-
understanding of what a meaningful life is. In tives are more other-centered, more negative, and
contrast, individuals whose lives do not con- less coherent in Turkey than in Germany: Com-
form to the dominant cultural life script cannot paring provincial-Turkish, metropolitan-Turkish,
rely on shared narrative forms to express the Turkish-German, and native German educated
young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2466.
creation of meaning in less typical lives. Rather, http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02466
they struggle with expressing meaning in ways Bakhtin, M. (1981). The Dialogic Imagination: Four
that can be shared across cultural divides, yield- Essays by MM Bakhtin. Austin, TX: Speech Genre
ing less consistent narrations across time and and other late essays.
events. Thus, structural narrative analysis pro- Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., &
vides one way of understanding how dominant Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good.
NARRATIVE STYLE AND STABILITY 165

Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. http:// chology, 17, 1–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp


dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 .854
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning (Vol. 3). Cam- Fivush, R., Sales, J. M., & Bohanek, J. G. (2008).
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Meaning making in mothers’ and children’s nar-
Coleman, J. T. (2014). Examining the life script of ratives of emotional events. Memory, 16, 579–594.
African-Americans: A test of the cultural life http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210802150681
script. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 419– Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and lan-
426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3000 guage in the emergence of autobiographical mem-
Curenton, S. M. (2011). Understanding the land- ory. Psychological Science, 15, 573–577. http://dx
scapes of stories: The association between pre- .doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00722.x
schoolers’ narrative comprehension and produc- Gee, J. P. (1989). Two styles of narrative construc-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion skills and cognitive abilities. Early Child tion and their linguistic and educational implica-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Development and Care, 181, 791– 808. http://dx tions. Discourse Processes, 12, 287–307. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2010.490946 .doi.org/10.1080/01638538909544732
Eakin, P. J. (2005). Living autobiographically. Biog- Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self
raphy, 28, 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/bio and society in the late modern age. Stanford, CA:
.2005.0025 Stanford University Press.
Erikson, E. (1968). Youth: Identity and crisis. New Gottschall, J. (2012). The storytelling animal: How
York, NY: Norton. stories make us human. Boston, MA: Houghton
Fivush, R. (2001). Owning experience: Developing Mifflin Harcourt.
subjective perspective in autobiographical narra- Habermas, T. (2006). Who speaks? Who looks? Who
tives. In C. Moore & K. Skene (Eds.), The self in feels? Point of view in autobiographical narratives.
time: Developmental issues (pp. 35–52). Hillsdale, The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 87,
NJ: Erlbaum. 497–518. http://dx.doi.org/10.1516/AXWM-
Fivush, R. (2010). Speaking silence: The social con- QRNF-H69K-9K3C
struction of silence in autobiographical and cul-
Habermas, T., & Berger, N. (2011). Retelling every-
tural narratives. Memory, 18, 88–98.
day emotional events: Condensation, distancing,
Fivush, R. (2012). Subjective perspective and per-
and closure. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 206–219.
sonal timeline in the development of autobiograph-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931003783568
ical memory. In D. Bernsten & D. Rubin (Eds.),
Habermas, T., & Diel, V. (2010). The emotional
Theories of autobiographical memory (pp. 226–
impact of loss narratives: Event severity and nar-
245). New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO97811 rative perspectives. Emotion, 10, 312–323. http://
39021937.017 dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018001
Fivush, R., & Baker-Ward, L. (2005). The search for Habermas, T., & Köber, C. (2015). Autobiographical
meaning: Developmental perspectives on internal reasoning is constitutive for narrative identity: The
state language in autobiographical memory. Jour- role of the life story for personal continuity. In
nal of Cognition and Development, 6, 455– 462. K. C. McLean & M. Syed (Eds.), The Oxford
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0604_1 handbook of identity development (pp. 149–165).
Fivush, R., Bohanek, J. G., Zaman, W., & Grapin, S. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2012). Gender differences in adolescents’ autobi- Habermas, T., & Reese, E. (2015). Getting a life
ographical narratives. Journal of Cognition and takes time: The development of the life story in
Development, 13, 295–319. adolescence, its precursors and consequences. Hu-
Fivush, R., Booker, J. A., & Graci, M. E. (2017). man Development, 58, 172–201. http://dx.doi.org/
Ongoing narrative meaning-making within events 10.1159/000437245
and across the life span. Imagination, Cognition Josselson, R. (2011). “Bet you think this song is
and Personality, 37, 127–152. http://dx.doi.org/10 about you”: Whose narrative is it in narrative re-
.1177/0276236617733824 search? Narrative Works; Issues, Investigations, &
Fivush, R., Habermas, T., Waters, T. E. A., & Za- Interventions, 1, 33–51.
man, W. (2011). The making of autobiographical Köber, C., & Habermas, T. (2017). How stable is the
memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and personal past? Stability of most important autobi-
identity. International Journal of Psychology, 46, ographical memories and life narratives across
321–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594 eight years in a life span sample. Journal of Per-
.2011.596541 sonality and Social Psychology, 113, 608– 626.
Fivush, R., Hazzard, A., Sales, J. M., Sarfati, D., & http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000145
Brown, T. (2003). Creating coherence out of Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Phil-
chaos? Children’s narratives of emotionally posi- adelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
tive and negative events. Applied Cognitive Psy- Labov, W. (2008). Oral narratives of personal expe-
166 FIVUSH, HABERMAS, AND REESE

rience. In P. C. Hogan (Ed.), Cambridge encyclo- ment, 21, 318–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/


pedia of the language sciences (pp. 546–548). 10409289.2010.481552
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Reese, E., Yan, C., Jack, F., & Hayne, H. (2010).
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analy- Emerging identities: Narrative and self from early
sis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. childhood to early adolescence. In K. C. McLean
Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts & M. Pasupathi (Eds.), Advancing responsible ad-
(pp. 12– 44). Seattle, WA: University of Washing- olescent development. Narrative development in
ton Press. (Reprinted in Journal of Narrative and adolescence: Creating the storied self (pp. 23– 43).
Life History, 7, 3–38, 1997). New York, NY: Springer Science. http://dx.doi
McAdams, D. P. (1990). Unity and purpose in human .org/10.1007/978-0-387-89825-4_2
lives: The emergence of identity as a life story. In Ricoeur, P. (1980). Narrative time. Critical Inquiry,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff, & S. Frank (Eds.), Per- 7, 169–190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/448093


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sonality structure in the life course: Essays on Rubin, D. C., & Berntsen, D. (2003). Life scripts help
personology in the Murray tradition (pp. 148– to maintain autobiographical memories of highly
200). New York, NY: Springer. positive, but not highly negative, events. Memory
McAdams, D. P. (2015). The art and science of & Cognition, 31, 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/
personality development. New York, NY: Guilford BF03196077
Press. Sales, J. M., Merrill, N. A., & Fivush, R. (2013).
McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M., Greenhoot, A. F., & Does making meaning make it better? Narrative
Fivush, R. (2017). Does intra-individual variability meaning making and well-being in at-risk African-
American adolescent females. Memory, 21, 97–
in narration matter and for what? Journal of Re-
110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012
search in Personality, 69, 55– 66. http://dx.doi.org/
.706614
10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.003
Sarbin, T. R. (1986). Narrative psychology: The sto-
McLean, K. C., & Syed, M. (2015). Personal, master,
ried nature of human conduct. Westport, CT: Prae-
and alternative narratives: An integrative frame-
ger/Greenwood Publishing Group.
work for understanding identity development in Schiffrin, D. (1981). Tense variation in narrative.
context. Human Development, 58, 318–349. Language, 57, 45– 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/
Norrick, N. R. (1998). Retelling stories in spontane- lan.1981.0011
ous conversation. Discourse Processes, 25, 75–97. Tannen, D. (1986). Introducing constructed dialogue
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638539809545021 in Greek and American conversational and literary
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental narrative. Direct and indirect speech, 31, 311–332.
psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: Develop-
child’s narratives. New York, NY: Springer. ment of higher psychological processes (M. Cole,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-0608-6 Trans.). Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
Propp, V. (1968). Morphology of the folktale. Austin, Walton, M. D., & Walton-Wetzel, J. (2013). Re-
TX: University of Texas Press (Original 1928). ported speech and the development of authorial
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P., voice in middle childhood. Narrative Inquiry, 23,
Fivush, R., & Ornstein, P. A. (2011). Coherence of 388– 404.
personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidi- Wang, Q. (2013). The autobiographical self in time
mensional model and coding method. Journal of and culture. New York, NY: Oxford University
Cognition and Development, 12, 424– 462. http:// Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/978
dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.587854 0199737833.001.0001
Reese, E., Leyva, D., Sparks, A., & Grolnick, W.
(2010). Maternal elaborative reminiscing increases Received September 18, 2018
low-income children’s narrative skills relative to Revision received March 15, 2019
dialogic reading. Early Education and Develop- Accepted March 21, 2019 䡲

You might also like